Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 20

Psychological Bulletin Copyright 1997 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.

1997, Vol. 121, No. 3, 417-436 0033-2909/97/S3.00

A Stitch in Time: Self-Regulation and Proactive Coping


Lisa G. Aspinwall Shelley E. Taylor
University of Maryland, College Park University of California, Los Angeles

In a conceptual and temporal framework, derived from research on social cognition, social interaction,
and stress and coping, the authors analyze the processes through which people anticipate or detect
potential stressors and act in advance to prevent them or to mute their impact (proactive coping).
The framework specifies five stages in proactive coping: (1) resource accumulation, (2) recognition
of potential stressors, (3) initial appraisal, (4) preliminary coping efforts, and (5) elicitation and
use of feedback concerning initial efforts. The authors detail the role of individual differences, skills,
and resources at each stage. They highlight the unique predictions afforded by a focus on proactive
coping and the importance of understanding how people avoid and offset potential stressors.

Anticipating that he may grow lonely over the holidays, a before it occurs. As such, proactive coping differs from coping
recently bereaved widower makes plans to join friends for Christ- with stressful events and from anticipatory coping in important
mas. Believing that job opportunities may be shrinking in her ways. In the stress and coping literature, coping is defined as
chosen field of work, a woman begins to acquire job skills in a activities undertaken to master, tolerate, reduce, or minimize
related field in which employment opportunities are still plentiful. environmental or intrapsychic demands perceived to represent
After a bad day at work, a man excuses himself from dinner to potential threats, existing harm, or losses (Fblkman & Lazarus,
avoid being grouchy around his family. Recognizing that lab 1985; Lazarus & Fblkman, 1984). Anticipatory coping involves
courses are especially challenging, a student arranges to take only preparation for the stressful consequences of an upcoming event
one lab course each semester to maintain good grades. whose occurrence is likely or certain (Breznitz, 1983a; Fblk-
The English language has many colorful expressions that re- man & Lazarus, 1985). Proactive coping may be distinguished
flect the idea that people engage in proactive coping. People are from coping and anticipatory coping on three grounds.
variously advised to recognize cues suggesting that trouble is First, proactive coping is temporally prior to coping and antic-
imminent (' 'Read the writing on the wall") and to act to forestall ipatory coping. It involves the accumulation of resources and
or to minimize an adverse event ("Head it off at the pass"). the acquisition of skills that are not designed to address any
Indeed, in many life domains, people experience good outcomes particular stressor but to prepare in general, given the recogni-
or avoid bad ones because of their proactive efforts. All four
tion that stressors do occur and that to be forearmed is to be
individuals in the examples above have in common that they
well prepared. Second, proactive coping requires different skills
anticipated stressful events before the events occurred and took
than does coping with extant stressors. For example, because
steps to avoid or minimize them. As such, all four are ' 'proactive
the activities of proactive coping are not directed to a particular
copers.''
stressor, skills relating to the ability to identify potential sources
Proactive Coping of stress before they occur assume importance in the activities
Definition of proactive coping. Third, as compared with coping with extant
Proactive coping consists of efforts undertaken in advance stressors, different skills and activities are likely to be successful
of a potentially stressful event to prevent it or to modify its form for proactive coping.
Two examples illustrate this last point. First, the social sup-
port literature suggests that the emotional benefits of social
Lisa G. Aspinwall, Department of Psychology, University of Maryland, support are especially important in the effective management of
College Park; Shelley E. Taylor, Department of Psychology, University
extant stressors (see S. Cohen & Wills, 1985, for a review). In
of California, Los Angeles.
This article was supported by the University of Maryland's Depart-
contrast, as we argue later, during the proactive stage, informa-
ment of Psychology and in part by National Institute of Mental Health tion and appraisal support may be more important and emotional
Grant MH 42152 and National Science Foundation Grant SBR-9507642. support may be less so and, under certain circumstances, unhelp-
We thank Nancy Adler, Susanne Brunhart, Christine Dunkel-Schetter, ful. Similarly, whereas coping strategies related to emotional
Stevan Hobfoll, Arie Kruglanski, Thane Pittman, Rena Repetti, Linda regulation, such as positive reappraisal, may be an effective
Richter, Carol Sansone, and Ellen Scholnick for helpful comments on way of coping with an extant stressor (e.g., Dunkel-Schetter,
an earlier version of this article. We also thank the MacArthur Foundation Feinstein, Taylor, & Falke, 1992), such emotionally soothing
Task Force on SES and Health for providing a stimulating forum that
reappraisals are unlikely to be helpful during the proactive stage.
contributed to the development of these ideas.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Lisa
Instead, as we argue, effective proactive coping is virtually al-
G. Aspinwall, Department of Psychology, University of Maryland, Col- ways active. To summarize, because proactive coping is tempo-
lege Park, Maryland 20742-4411. Electronic mail may be sent via In- rally prior, addresses nonexistent or nebulous stressors, requires
ternet to Ia28@umail.umd.edu. different skills, and is successfully accomplished through differ-
417
418 ASPINWALL AND TAYLOR

ent activities, it merits a conceptual and empirical focus that is be lessened or averted by proactive efforts and thus never be
distinct from existing work on stress and coping. felt. Second, the ratio of coping resources to the magnitude of
a stressor is likely to be favorable when the stressor is tackled
Has Proactive Coping Been Ignored? in its early stages rather than in its full-blown state. Specifically,
an incipient stressor is likely to be modest, and because re-
As a result of these characteristics, proactive coping is largely sources have not yet been expended to address it, coping re-
unstudied in the stress and coping literature. In a typical investi- sources are likely to be greater. Third, when a stressful event is
gation of stress and coping, the point of departure for researchers on the horizon, there may be a range of options available to
who are studying efforts to solve problems and to regulate the manage it; after the stressor has occurred or has developed,
emotional responses to them is a looming or fully developed options may be more constrained. Fourth, to the extent that
stressful event, such as an illness, financial strain, interpersonal stressful events can be averted or minimized, the burden of
discord, or academic failure. Activities undertaken in advance chronic stress carried by an individual is likely to be relatively
of the stressful event may go unstudied because the event itself low. Because many stressful events are of long-term duration
defines the point of departure. and add to the cumulative burden of obligations in an individu-
This focus on coping activities that take place after the event al's daily life, proactive efforts that avoid or offset stressful
has occurred may create several gaps in the understanding of events may keep chronic stress at a low level.
proactive processes. First, if proactive coping is successful, indi- There are some important potential disadvantages of proactive
viduals who demonstrated it may be eliminated from studies of coping as well. One is that, if a stressful event has not occurred,
stress and coping altogether. To a degree, illness, financial strain, then there is a chance that it may not occur at all. Thus, the
interpersonal discord, and academic failure—the examples just proactive coper may make offsetting plans and undertake activi-
noted—are preventable occurrences. If an individual prevents ties that would have been unnecessary had conditions been al-
them, he or she would not be eligible for an investigation of lowed to unfold without intervention (Schonpflug, 1986). A
such stressors. Second, even if proactive copers were included second potential liability stems from the inherent ambiguity of
in an outcome study of some kind, they may show up as well potential stressors. When a stressor has not yet happened, its
adjusted, successful, or healthy, but the researchers would not form may be nebulous and its attributes, ambiguous. Conse-
assess proactive efforts made before the stressful event had ma- quently, exactly what kinds of preliminary coping efforts and
terialized that contributed to these good outcomes. resources should be brought to bear on the problem may be
Proactive coping may also go unstudied because it is difficult unclear. Initial coping efforts, then, may be ineffective, or they
to detect. When people act to avert or minimize a potential may actually exacerbate the problem.
stressor, relatively little happens (cf. McGrath & Beehr, 1990). These two liabilities lead, in turn, to a third, namely, that
For example, an administrator who acts to avert a financial one may invest coping efforts and resources in a problem that
shortfall that would jeopardize the payroll and discredit his subsequently turns out to be different from the one initially
company may receive little notice for his work because the net envisioned. As a result, coping resources may be drained at
result, as seen by outside observers, is that nothing happened. precisely the moment they are most needed. It is important,
Nonevents are rarely selected for scientific investigation. In- therefore, to understand what factors influence the quality and
stead, our attention is drawn to those who lose such gambles. accuracy of appraisals of potential and incipient stressors, to
Therefore, proactive coping may go largely unrecognized. identify how people respond to information that might qualify
Of course, not all stressful events can be averted. For example, these initial assessments, and to ascertain how people are able
bereavement, certain chronic illnesses, and natural disasters sim- to distinguish soluble problems from insoluble ones and allocate
ply happen. Nonetheless, people may differ in the degree to their resources accordingly. These are the tasks of this review.
which they have undertaken proactive efforts to manage such
events. For example, a bereaved widow who has taken the time Overview of the Framework
to understand the family finances and to ensure that her benefits
will continue after the death of her spouse is better prepared for In this article, we develop a conceptual and temporal frame-
her spouse's death than her counterpart who did not take such work to analyze the proactive coping literature and review the
precautionary steps. A Califomian who has earthquake insur- research that addresses the individual differences and resources
ance and a store of earthquake-related supplies will be better that may promote effective proactive coping. A second purpose
prepared for an earthquake than an individual who does not of our article is to highlight the skills that enhance proactive
have such preparations. Thus, even in the case of stressful events coping. This position makes the explicit assumption that proac-
that cannot be avoided, the person who engages in proactive tive coping is generally beneficial to a person and that the poten-
coping will typically experience better adjustment than the per- tial liabilities of proactive coping either do not materialize or
son who did not undertake such preparatory activities. are offset by the benefits gained. Our review of the resources
and individual differences that may promote or inhibit effective
Potential Benefits and Liabilities proactive coping is necessarily selective, rather than exhaustive,
and is summarized in Table 1. We have included representative
Proactive coping has several important potential benefits. research on the role of each factor at each stage of the proactive
First, it may minimize the degree of stress experienced during model to illustrate its potential heuristic value.
a stressful encounter (Hobfoll, 1988, 1989). When a stressful A third purpose of this article is to develop explicit links
event is a possibility rather than an actuality, its full impact may between a large literature in social cognition on self-regulation
A STITCH IN TIME 419

Table 1
Influences on Proactive Coping
Stages of the proactive coping model

Resource Attention- Initial Preliminary Elicit and use


Influences accumulation recognition appraisal coping feedback

Individual differences
Anxiety 0
Control beliefs 0
Depression 0
Expertise and experience 0 + +
Future temporal orientation 0 0 7
Need for closure 0 ?
Neuroticism 0
Optimistic beliefs 0
Repression 0
Self-esteem 0 +
Tolerance for ambiguity 0 0
Vigilance, monitoring, and
sensitization 0
Types of social support
Appraisal 0 + 0
Emotional 0 0 +
Informational 0 + +
Tangible + + +
Environmental factors
Chronic burden —
Concurrent demands —
Environmental "busyness" —

Note. + = the influence facilitates proactive coping; — = the influence may compromise proactive coping;
? = the effects of the influence on this stage are unknown; 0 = the influence is not believed to affect this
stage.

and a voluminous literature in health psychology on coping. management of the chronic burden so that when, inevitably,
These two literatures have evolved largely independently, but stressors are detected, one is prepared as much as possible to
they address similar issues (Costa, Somerfield, & McCrae, manage them.
1996). Self-regulation is defined as the ways in which people Proactive coping also involves the recognition of potential
control and direct their own actions (Fiske & Taylor, 1991). stressors. Recognition refers to the ability to see a potential
Coping is defined as the process of managing internal or external stressful event coming. It depends on the ability to screen the
demands that are appraised as taxing or exceeding the resources environment for danger and to be sensitive to internal cues sug-
of a person (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). In proactive coping, gesting that threats may arise. After a potential stressor has been
the distinctions between the two sets of processes blur because detected, initial appraisal consists of preliminary assessments
stressful events have not yet occurred or have not progressed of the current ("What is this?") and potential ("What is this
to levels where resources are taxed. As we suggest later, self- likely to become?'') status of the potential stressor as well as
regulatory skills explain a great deal about how individuals related assessments, such as "Should I be worried about this?"
anticipate the occurrence of stressful events, enabling people to and ' 'Is this something I should keep an eye on?'' These apprais-
avoid stressful events or minimize their impact. Yet, because als may foster increased attention to the potential stressor and
the anticipation of a stressful event involves apprehension or a may also give rise to initial coping efforts. Initial coping efforts
sense of impending danger, activities more traditionally viewed are activities undertaken that are deemed likely to prevent or
as coping efforts also come into play. minimize a recognized or suspected stressor. We suggest that
successful proactive coping at this stage is virtually always ac-
Stages of Proactive Coping tive rather than avoidant, involving either cognitive activities,
We divide proactive coping into five stages. Figure 1 shows such as planning, or behavioral activities, such as seeking infor-
the critical tasks undertaken at each of the five stages of proactive mation from others and taking preliminary action.
coping and the interrelation and feedback among the stages. As The elicitation and use of feedback is the final step in the
shown in Figure 1, proactive coping starts with the building of proactive coping process. It centers around the acquisition and
resources and skills in advance of any specific anticipated stres- use of feedback about the development of the stressful event
sor (resource accumulation). Effective proactive coping in- itself ("Has it advanced, changed form, or improved?"), the
volves the mustering of time, money, planning and organiza- effects one's preliminary efforts have had so far on the stressful
tional skills, social support, and, to the extent possible, the event ("Was I successful in staving it off?"), and whether the
420 ASPINWALL AND TAYLOR

Resource Build a reserve


of temporal, financial,
accumulation and social resources

H:Atfention- Screen environment


Jrgcognition for danger

S Initial
appraisal
What will it become?

t Preliminary
* What can I do?
coping

Has the event developed?

Elicit and use Have preliminary efforts


feedback had an effect?

What has been learned about


the potential stressor?

Figure 1. The five stages of proactive coping, their component tasks, and the potential feedback loops
among them.

event requires additional coping efforts ("Is there something feedback loops. Specifically, resource accumulation, attention,
more I can do, or should I wait to see if it's a problem?"). appraisal, and regulation of negative emotional arousal are con-
This feedback may be used to revise one's appraisals of the ceptualized as interrelated tasks in which appraisals that suggest
potential or incipient stressor and to modify one's strategies for a possible threat lead to increased attention to the potential
offsetting it. stressor, while prompting efforts to regulate emotions and to
Thus, from the standpoint of the widower in the opening shore up resources (cf. Lazarus & Folkman, 1984; Schonpflug,
paragraph, he must re-establish or firm up the bonds with his 1986). Specific kinds of initial appraisals influence the prelimi-
social support network (resource accumulation), realize in ad- nary coping efforts undertaken, which, in turn, influence the
vance that loneliness is a possible problem during the holidays kinds of information that may be extracted about the potential
(recognition), determine whether he can do something to avert
loneliness (initial appraisal), call friends or family to see who
would welcome a holiday visit (preliminary coping efforts), 1
Some researchers have identified two qualitatively distinct patterns
and recognize that his son's family seems a bit lukewarm about of physiological arousal, one corresponding to a positive state of energy
a possible visit from him but that his friends in Jamaica are mobilization to meet perceptions of challenge and the other representing
genuinely enthusiastic (elicitation and use of feedback). How- a negative state of perceived threat or stress (cf. Blascovich & Tomaka,
ever, as shown in Figure 1, the detection and appraisal of poten- 1996; Dienstbier, 1989; Manuck, Kamarck, Kasprowicz, & Waldstein,
tial stressors may generate negative emotional arousal.1 As we 1993). Although the former may facilitate proactive coping, the latter
may interfere with it. We use the term negative emotional arousal to
review later, the need to regulate negative emotional arousal
denote this latter state. Note that this term is certainly an oversimplifica-
may influence—and, in some cases, compromise—the tasks of tion of many sophisticated models concerning the links among physio-
each stage, either by limiting the person's ability to detect and logical arousal; cognitive appraisals; and the intrapsychic, social, and
offset potential stressors or by interfering with the ability to environmental factors that lead to the interpretation of physiological
engage in coping efforts. arousal as specific emotions (see Blascovich & Tomaka, 1996, for a
An important element of the model is the presence of several discussion).
A STITCH IN TIME 421

stressor, providing more information for the appraisal process circumstances (Hammen, 1992). Other families are more proac-
and the modification of coping efforts. tive. For example, in one family, children may be taken to a
store to get their school supplies or wardrobes; whereas in an-
Resource Accumulation: The Maintenance and other family, they may be asked to list in advance what they
Acquisition of Resources need. Such organizational skills generalize to new settings and
tasks. Similarly, schools differ in the degree to which they expect
An important first step in effective proactive coping is the their students to be involved in the planning and enactment of
preservation and accumulation of resources. Such resources in- activities. In large, chaotic classrooms in which teachers have
clude time, money, organizational and planning skills, and a many responsibilities, students may simply be given assignments
social network of family and friends as well as more individual- with due dates; whereas in smaller and more orderly classrooms,
ized resources. As Hobfoll (1989) noted, individuals who have children may be taught how to gather and organize information,
a surplus of resources and who are not responding to immediate plan a long-term project so that it will be done by a due date,
challenges can use them preventively to offset future net losses and enlist the cooperation of their parents in the completion of
of resources. certain assignments. Such training helps create general organiza-
As an example of how external resources may affect proactive tional skills, time management skills, and the ability to use a
efforts, consider the the importance of time. In the absence support network to achieve goals. Thus, differences in the per-
of time pressures, individuals may be attentive to subtle cues sonal environments to which an individual is exposed produce
suggesting that a potential problem may develop. The absence differences in the degree to which proactive coping skills are
of time pressures may also enable an individual to think through acquired and practiced.
likely future events, anticipate stressors, and take proactive mea- The establishment of a social network and social support are
sures to offset or minimize their occurrence. In contrast, con- valuable resources that not only enable individuals to cope with
sider an individual who works 70 hr a week at two jobs with a wide variety of extant stressors but may also facilitate proac-
little vacation time or sick leave benefits or a single parent who tive coping efforts. Social support is thought to be helpful for
works full time. Although such individuals may become expert coping with stressful events because it serves several explicit
at time management, they may have little time available for functions. Individuals in a social support network may provide
reflection. information germane to stressors (information support), enable
Lack of time and associated factors, such as fatigue, can have individuals to appraise situations effectively (appraisal sup-
several effects on proactive coping. First, they may minimize port), provide concrete aid or assistance during times of stress
the likelihood that an individual will recognize a warning sign (tangible aid), and provide emotional support (see S. Cohen &
that is at a relatively low level. Second, they may prevent the McKay, 1984; S. Cohen & Wills, 1985; and Thoits, 1986, for
individual from thinking through the future to the point that he reviews). Although analyses of social support often maintain
or she can anticipate likely future stressors. Third, time pressure that emotional support is the most important benefit of a social
or fatigue may lead an individual to premature closure, with support network (e.g., Dakof & Taylor, 1990), for proactive
accompanying deficits in the processing of subsequent informa- coping—as we note in subsequent sections—the ability of a
tion related to the stressor (Kruglanski & Webster, 1996; Web- support network to provide information and appraisals germane
ster, Richter, & Kruglanski, 1996). Finally, lack of time or en- to ambiguous or potential stressors may be especially valuable.
ergy may interfere with the enactment of proactive strategies. Resources also enable people to keep chronic stress under
For example, even if a worker correctly anticipates the need to control. Proactive coping is difficult, even impossible, in chroni-
acquire new job skills, he or she may not have the time or energy cally stressful environments. Chronic problems, such as finan-
to attend evening classes. Other finite resources, such as money, cial difficulties, domestic discord, or a troublesome car, or
may affect proactive coping in a similar fashion. chronic environmental stressors, such as crime, overcrowding,
Resource accumulation also involves the acquisition of proac- and noise, may diminish one's opportunities to learn and use
tive coping skills. From early childhood, children are exposed proactive coping skills. Because high-stress environments exac-
to others who influence the skills that they develop with respect erbate cognitive load, reduce perceptions of personal control,
to anticipating stress and coping with it (Bandura, 1986) and and reduce the opportunity to engage in proactive coping, they
to planning in general (McGillicuddy-De Lisi, De Lisi, favor reactive coping skills (cf. Pearlin, Menaghan, Lieber-
Flaugher, & Sigel, 1987). For example, research indicates that man, & Mullan, 1981). In addition, chronic stress is reliably
chief determinants of the practice of preventive health behaviors associated with negative affect, especially depression, and these
are whether parents set an effective example and encouraged affective consequences of chronic stress may reduce the likeli-
the practice of such proactive steps (R. Y. Cohen, Brownell, & hood that people will be able to respond proactively (Brown &
Felix, 1990; Maddux, Roberts, Sledden, & Wright, 1986). Such Harris, 1978; McGonagle & Kessler, 1990; Pearlin & Schooler,
health-relevant practices as a healthy diet, abstinence from 1978). To the extent that people are prevented from proactive
smoking, moderate or no alcohol consumption, exercise, seatbelt coping, the cumulative burden of stress that they face is likely
use, and other kinds of proactive behaviors depend heavily on to be great.
learning in the family (see S. E. Taylor, 1995). Moreover, the degree to which an individual is able to under-
Families also influence the development of specific coping take an activity free of distraction or simultaneous stressors
skills. Children living with mothers who are chronically de- may greatly moderate the quality of performance on that task.
pressed, for example, develop ways of coping that include hypo- Attention is a finite resource (Hasher & Zachs, 1979; Kahne-
responsivity to the environment and withdrawal from stressful man, 1973), and to the degree that it is compromised by other
422 ASPINWALL AND TAYLOR

demands, it is less available for tasks related to proactive coping. lez, 1984). Researchers studying temporal orientation and plan-
In particular, the ability to use information correctly and draw ning have indentified individual differences in the degree to
appropriate inferences may be compromised. Gilbert, Pelham, which participants think about and plan for their futures or use
and Krull (1988) have examined the effect of cognitive load, information about future outcomes in judging current outcomes
or what they term "busyness," on inference processes. To sum- (e.g., Strathman, Gleicher, Boninger, & Edwards, 1994). Such
marize a large and consistent literature, in a busy environment, distinctions are important in the study of proactive processes
inferences are crude: They are based on less information, poor because one must be able to anticipate the future and modify
consideration of the information available, high-probability in- one's current behavior to behave proactively. Temporal orienta-
terpretations that do not have the benefit of correction processes tion is a successful predictor of such future-oriented behaviors
or updating, premature closure, and heuristic-automatic ways as recycling and energy conservation and preventive health be-
of processing information that may be unresponsive to the data haviors, such as safe sex and decreased tobacco and alcohol use
(see Fiske & Taylor, 1991, for a review). (Rothspan & Read, 1996; Strathman et al., 1994).
The effects of cognitive load or busyness on proactive coping Several additional individual differences may facilitate atten-
are potentially important. A busy immediate environment may tion to threatening information, whether internally or externally
compromise people's ability to recognize the warning signs of generated. Such concepts as vigilance, sensitization, and moni-
an impending stressor. A busy environment may also compro- toring refer to chronic tendencies to scan the environment for
mise people's initial appraisals of a potential stressor if they potentially meaningful threatening information (Davidson &
consider information less thoroughly by relying on a salient, Bobey, 1970; Goldstein, Jones, Clemens, Flagg, & Alexander,
although not necessarily correct, initial interpretation of a situa- 1965; S. M. Miller, 1987; Neufeld & Davidson, 1971). Individu-
tion (cf. Kruglanski & Webster, 1996). Even if an individual als who respond to the possibility of threat by seeking informa-
recognizes a potential stressor and makes an accurate initial tion are more likely to approach and intellectualize threatening
appraisal, a busy environment may make it difficult to enact experiences than those who avoid information (Davidson &
initial coping efforts, to detect feedback about the success of Bobey, 1970; Goldstein et al., 1965; Neufeld & Davidson, 1971;
these efforts, and to gather and use new information about the Petrie, Collins, & Solomon, 1960; see S. E. Taylor & Clark,
unfolding stressor. Freedom from daily chronic distraction, then, 1986, for a review). In contrast, constructs such as repression
may maximize the degree to which proactive efforts may be (C. H. Hansen, Hansen, & Shantz, 1992; Weinberger, 1990)
undertaken. and blunting (S. M. Miller, 1987) refer to tendencies to ignore
As the preceding analysis suggests, proactive coping may threatening information. Whether such individual differences
be difficult to accomplish under conditions of low resources operate directly at encoding to reduce initial awareness of poten-
(Hobfoll, 1989). People without financial resources, without a tially negative stimuli or operate after information has been
support network, with little time, or with little opportunity to recognized to keep it from awareness or to distort it has yet to
learn proactive coping skills will be less able or likely to exert be determined.
proactive efforts than their more resource-wealthy counterparts.2 Intriguing evidence suggests that repressers may orient more
Thus, the ability to accumulate and preserve resources is essen- quickly to threatening information but process it less success-
tial to effective proactive coping. fully (C. H. Hansen et al., 1992). Specifically, C. H. Hansen et
al. suggested that, for repressers, threatening stimuli constrict
Recognition: Attention and Detection attention, resulting in impoverished memory for negative infor-
of Potential Stressors mation. If this is the case, then repressers may incompletely
process initial information about a problem or process it in a
The detection of a potential stressor requires attention to po- way that removes the threatening aspects (cf. Dunning & Story,
tentially threatening information. Sometimes, this task involves 1991; Foa & Kozak, 1986). As such, it may be difficult for
the interpretation of warning signs that come from the environ- chronic repressers or avoiders to act in advance to remediate a
ment. The tendency to orient to negative or unexpected stimuli potential stressor. Similarly, as research on coping with extant
appears to be innate. A large amount of literature shows that stressors suggests, people who cope through avoidance instead
humans and other species orient to negative stimuli with dra- of confrontation or vigilance may not make sufficient cognitive
matic but temporary physiological arousal, attentional en- or emotional efforts to anticipate and manage subsequent prob-
gulfment, and expenditure of cognitive and social resources (see lems (Suls & Fletcher, 1985; S. E. Taylor & Clark, 1986).
S. E. Taylor, 1991, for a review). At other times, information The role of dispositional optimism in the recognition of po-
about a potential stressor comes from one's internal processes
of reflection. By thinking through the tasks one must accomplish
2
within a given period of time, one may gain insights about those More interesting, highly demanding environments may make proac-
demands, such as the realization that one task must be completed tive behavior all the more valuable. The study of psychological resilience
before another can be started or that the amount of time available has identified certain key resources that seem to enable people to survive
and to adapt to extraordinarily stressful environments, such as living
may be insufficient, thereby dictating the need to establish prior-
with a parent who is abusive, alcoholic, or mentally ill. Some researchers
ities. Regardless of whether warning signs are internally or ex- have suggested that proactive behavior may be especially important
ternally generated, a number of individual differences may mod- under such demanding conditions. For example, children who have been
erate the tendency to detect potential stressors. abused may acquire knowledge about the situations that are likely to
The detection of a potential stressor may be facilitated by a anger the person who is abusive and make plans to avoid these situations
future temporal orientation (Jones, 1994; Zimbardo & Gonza- (Mirazek & Mrazek, 1987).
A STITCH IN TIME 423

tentially threatening information has been controversial. Al- In the presence of others who do not appear to regard a situation
though some have suggested that chronically optimistic beliefs as threatening, offsetting responses may be delayed.
may compromise attention to negative information because they
decrease worry (Perloff & Fetzer, 1986; Schwarzer, 1994; Ten- Initial Appraisals of Emerging Stressors
nen & Affleck, 1987; Weinstein, 1982, 1987), some empirical
studies suggest that the opposite may be true. In a study of Once a potential stressor has been detected, it must be ap-
attention to threatening health information (Aspinwall & Brun- praised. The task of initial appraisal centers around at least two
hart, 1996 ), young adults who were optimistic about their health interrelated tasks: definition of the problem and regulation of
paid more attention than their less optimistic counterparts to arousal. We consider how processes and factors, such as sche-
information about risks to their health. Trope and Neter (1994) mas, mental simulation, individual differences in appraisals, and
found that students who had recently received positive feedback the use of social information, affect these tasks.
showed greater interest in negative information about them- Definition of the problem. In its incipient stage, a potential
selves than did students who had not previously been given problem is likely to be low in magnitude and ambiguous in
positive feedback. Taken together, these studies suggest that pos- form. Thus, for an initial appraisal to lead to proactive coping,
itive expectations or experiences may bolster people's ability to the individual must make some sense of the nebulous negative
cope with the emotional effects of negative feedback, such that stimulus. Research from social cognition on schemas, especially
these expectations enhance rather than reduce attention to nega- on matching data to schemas, may elucidate the processes peo-
tive information (see also Baumeister & Cairns, 1992). ple go through in attempting to make sense of an incipient or
There may be liabilities associated with scanning the environ- ambiguous negative cue.
ment for potential danger as well. One potential risk is hypervig- Several factors influence what schema will be evoked. Salient
ilance. Hypervigilance may lead individuals to perseverate on cues—that is, cues that are dramatic, proximal, and personally
imagined problems or attend to so many potential threats that relevant—are likely to be noticed and to elicit relevant schemas
they are unable to manage any one of them (cf. McGrath & (Fiske & Taylor, 1991). Thus, for example, a pink slip delivered
Beehr, 1990). Moreover, people who are constantly on guard to one's next-door neighbor is more likely to elicit the scenario
against threats may pay a toll in emotional wear and tear and of potential job loss than a less proximal but perhaps more
cognitive fatigue (Perloff, 1983; Schonpflug, 1986). The poten- relevant layoff. Strong cues evoke schemas. Reports of a major
tial problems raised by hypervigilance suggest that, in addition layoff are more likely to trigger a job loss scenario than reports
to being vigilant to potential signs of danger, people may also of a minor one. Recently primed or accessible cues are likely
need to learn how to regulate their attention to be vigilant to to evoke powerful schemas (e.g., Bargh, Bond, Lombardi, &
some potential threats, but not to all, and to be able to orient Tola, 1986). If one's neighbor's layoff and his family's conse-
quent struggle with loss of income are fresh in one's mind, then
away from negative information (Derryberry & Reed, 1994).
a job loss scenario may loom especially large. Representative-
In addition, because people rarely have the luxury of attending
ness is an important criterion by which a schema or scenario is
to one threat at a time, understanding how they allocate attention
judged likely to occur (Tversky & Kahneman, 1982). A repre-
to multiple sources of negative information may yield insights
sentative scenario is one that matches the high-probability fea-
into self-regulatory efforts (Carver et al., 1993; McGrath &
tures of a person's pre-existing schemas. For example, if one's
Beehr, 1990; Pearlin, 1989).
company has a history of closing plants in response to an eco-
An area that has been largely ignored in studies of the recog- nomic downturn, then a plant closure is the representative sce-
nition of negative information is the important role that a social nario likely to come to mind when the danger signal of an
network can play in the detection of warning signs. A person impending recession is detected. In contrast, if one's company
may be alerted to the likelihood of a possible stressor by others has a history of cutting back evenly at all plants, especially in
in his or her network or the person may use the support network certain departments, then the scenario of selective layoffs is
to try to determine whether a potential stressor exists. Consider, most likely to be cued. Differences in the scenario evoked will
as an example, the function of rumor in the workplace when lead to consequent differences in the estimation of whether one
people share information about impending layoffs, possible will be affected by such cutbacks.
takeovers, financial shortfalls, and the like to be better prepared Characteristics of the perceiver also influence what schemas
for these eventualities should they occur. Although the informa- may be evoked. A match to past experience may cue a schema
tion provided by such social interaction may not always be (Andersen & Baum, 1994; Andersen & Cole, 1990; Scholnick,
accurate, it may serve the function of putting a person on notice 1994). For example, prior experience with a layoff and recogni-
so that he or she is vigilant to subsequent information relating tion that the present situation matches the situation that immedi-
to the potential stressor. Informational support from a social ately preceded the prior layoff may elicit a layoff scenario.
network, then, can be instrumental both in the detection of a Expert knowledge, or expertise, is also implicated in the match-
warning sign and in the determination of the extent of threat an ing of data to schemas. Specifically, the more one knows about
incipient stressor poses. Note that information from the social layoff situations, the more likely one is to match data to schemas
network can impede responsiveness to potential threats as well. easily and accurately (Fiske & Taylor, 1991; Hammond, 1990;
Even in potential emergencies, the mere presence of others and Scholnick, 1994).
the degree to which they appear to be responding to a potential These variables then—salience, strength, accessibility, repre-
threat influence the amount of time it takes people to notice the sentativeness, past experience, and expertise—influence the
threat and interpret it as dangerous (Latane & Darley, 1970). ways in which people interpret potential danger signals and
424 ASPINWALL AND TAYLOR

match them to available schemas or scenarios in long-term mem- differences. People vary in the ways in which they appraise
ory in a preliminary effort to understand what a warning sign potential stressful events, especially in whether they are predis-
may mean. posed to make favorable or unfavorable appraisals. Individuals
Use of mental simulation. For an initial appraisal to be use- who are high in optimism and in perceived self-efficacy appraise
ful in proactive coping efforts, it is insufficient to call forth a stressful events as less threatening or negative (Jerusalem,
static representation of what a warning signal is likely to mean. 1993). Wiebe (1991) found that participants high in hardiness
It is also essential to understand how it is likely to change. (Kobasa, 1977) appraised laboratory stressors as less threaten-
Essentially, the task facing the would-be proactive coper is to ing than did those low in hardiness (see also Allred & Smith,
run the incipient stressful event forward in time to project what 1989). Davey, Hampton, Farrell, and Davidson (1992) exposed
its likely implications or course will be or could be. Individuals participants to accounts of ambiguous situations, such as a
with a future temporal orientation may be more likely to engage friend waving from the water, and to situations crafted to be
in this process than those with a present or past orientation unambiguously threatening or benign. Regardless of the type
(Zimbardo & Gonzalez, 1984). In terms of cognitive skills, such of situation, participants high in anxiety interpreted all of the
forward-looking mental activity may take the form of mental situations as more threatening than did participants low in anxi-
simulation. Mental simulation is the imitative representation of ety. Amplification of threat appraisals by participants high in
real or hypothetical events, including rehearsals of likely future anxiety was also demonstrated for perceptions of their somatic
events and fantasies about the future (S. E. Taylor & Pham, symptoms (Steptoe & Vogele, 1992). Such results suggest that
1996; S. E. Taylor & Schneider, 1989). An important character- some individual differences, such as trait anxiety, are related to
istic of mental simulation is that it makes a course of action the amplification of threat appraisals, whereas others, such as
seem likely or true (e.g., Anderson & Sechler, 1986; Carroll, hardiness, optimism, and psychological control, are associated
1978; Gregory, Cialdini, & Carpenter, 1982; Hirt & Sherman, with lower threat appraisals.
1985). What one initially simulates, then, is likely to be an One might be concerned that appraisals that dampen threat
important determinant of one's interpretation of the meaning of would interfere with efforts to remediate potential stressors.
a danger cue and how it is likely to unfold. However, factors that result in lower threat appraisals may actu-
But what is a person likely to mentally simulate? Although ally facilitate proactive efforts. In particular, moderately favor-
the ambiguity of a potential stressor would seem to suggest able appraisals of potential stressors may allow people to make
many possible appraisals and simulations, research suggests that use of threatening information, whereas unfavorable appraisals
what is mentally simulated mirrors reality. An individual's per- may lead to avoidance or other deficits in the processing of
ception of reality is formed by a variety of factors, including threatening information. In support of this interpretation, Aspin-
personal experience, information from other people, social wall and Brunhart (1997) examined the relation between per-
norms, and cultural expectations. Taking these factors into con- ceptions of vulnerability to skin cancer and attention to informa-
sideration, people simulate the most representative scenario, tion about it. \bung adult women were told that the average
given the nature of a danger cue (Kahneman & Miller, 1986). patient with melanoma was either a 25-year-old woman (proxi-
When constructing mental simulations of events, people decide mal threat condition) or a 55-year-old woman (distal threat
what is "normal" and tend to exclude what is surprising; rarely, condition). Women who believed they were at lower risk than
if ever, do people interject improbable events into their simula- their peers paid more attention to information about ultraviolet
tions (Kahneman & Miller, 1986). Mental simulations of events exposure in the proximal threat condition than in the distal threat
tend to follow the same rules of plausibility that actual behavior condition. Mediational analyses suggest that decreased worry
does. To the extent that an individual interprets a danger sign in about vulnerability to skin cancer accounted for the greater
a specific way, that is, as indicative of a particular potential attention. These results raise the possibility that people with
threatening event, the mental simulation or scenario that unfolds favorable expectations may be better able to process threatening
is likely to be the most probable scenario that corresponds to information because they are less worried about their personal
the individual's representation.3 vulnerability (Trope & Neter, 1994).
In addition to their role in the identification of the meaning The preceding discussion suggests that the relationship be-
of a danger signal, mental simulations may also provide an tween threat appraisal and effective action may be curvilinear.
incipient plan to deal with the potential stressor. The process If no threat is perceived, which may be true for chronic avoiders,
of imagining how events are going to take place provides infor- then no action will be undertaken. At moderate levels of per-
mation about those events, such as their sequence or relation ceived threat, which may be found among optimists, effective
to each other. Consequently, simulations provide information action may be facilitated. At high levels of perceived threat,
rudimentary to a plan of action (Hayes-Roth & Hayes-Roth,
1979; G. A. Miller, Galanter, & Pribram, 1960). Mental simu-
3
lation, then, may not only enable an individual to define what As a basic cognitive process, mental simulation is used in a broad
a danger signal means but simulation may also bridge the gap array of tasks, not all of which are necessarily adaptive. For example,
sometimes people's mental simulations are inaccurate. Mental simulation
between initial appraisals and preliminary coping efforts by
may also be involved in intrinsically maladaptive activities, such as
providing information relevant to the plan of action (see John- cognitive rumination (Lyubomirsky & Nolen-Hoeksema, 1995). Differ-
son & Sherman, 1990; and S. E. Taylor & Pham, 1996, for ent kinds of mental simulation may be more or less adaptive for different
reviews). kinds of people (cf. Norem & Illingworth, 1993). The discussion above,
Individual differences in the favorability of appraisals. Ini- then, should not be taken to mean that mental simulation is inevitably
tial appraisals of warning signs are also influenced by individual accurate or productive.
A STITCH IN TIME 425

which may be found among people who are highly anxious, taken to offset potential stressors early in their course and re-
effective action may be impeded. A large literature on the impact quire sustained attention to the potential problem, denial should
of fear appeals on actions to reduce fear is generally consistent be detrimental to proactive coping. Specificially, if individuals
with this curvilinear hypothesis (Janis, 1967; H. Leventhal, engage in denial, then proactive efforts are unlikely to be under-
1970). Such findings also suggest that arousal is implicated in taken; if proactive efforts are undertaken, then they may not
the relation between perceptions of threat and effective action— correspond well to objective features of the potential stressor.
an issue to which we now turn. In contrast, activities undertaken to appraise the potential
Regulation of negative emotional arousal. The favorable problem may simultaneously lend clarity to negative emotional
appraisals of potential stressors that are conducive to the effec- arousal and, by doing so, actually reduce or moderate it (cf.
tive structuring of problems may also be conducive to the regula- Blascovich & Tomaka, 1996). The clarity afforded by success-
tion of negative emotional arousal (cf. Blascovich & Tomaka, ful problem definition may help define nebulous physiological
1996; Dienstbier, 1989). Blascovich and Tomaka suggested that arousal, yielding a more specific definition, such as fear, anger,
individual differences, such as self-esteem and control beliefs, anxiety, or excitement (Fiske, 1982; Gleicher et al., 1990; Ly-
may lead people to appraise their coping resources more favor- man, Bernardin, & Thomas, 1980; Markman, Gavanski, Sher-
ably and lead them to make challenge appraisals rather than man, & McMullen, 1993; D. T. Miller & McFarland, 1987; D. T.
threat appraisals. Challenge appraisals, in turn, may lead to the Miller, Turnbull, & McFarland, 1990). More specific processing
effective mobilization of energy to meet the demands of the of the meanings of negative emotional arousal may aid in its
task. The results of several studies provide evidence for such a regulation.
process (Katz & Epstein, 1991; Tomaka & Blascovich, 1994). Appraisal support. As in the case of recognition of a warn-
For example, Katz and Epstein found that participants high in ing sign, the social network can have profound influences on
constructive thinking (akin to optimism) were less debilitated the appraisal of emerging stressful events (e.g., Ashford, 1986).
by failure feedback and better able to complete a laboratory test When a potential stressor is nebulous in form, the social network
than were those low in constructive thinking. may be instrumental in lending clarity (Ashford & Cummings,
Dispositional tendencies to amplify threat appraisals also 1983). By talking with or simply observing individuals in simi-
have important implications for proactive coping. When initial lar circumstances and comparing his or her reactions with theirs,
appraisals are unfavorable, increased demands for emotional a person is able to test the appropriateness of his or her initial
management may impede both information processing (C. H. judgments and test out initial interpretations and plans for deal-
Hansen et al., 1992) and instrumental action (Fblkman, 1984; ing with an incipient stressor. That is, both verbal and nonverbal
Orr & Meyer, 1990).4 For example, in a study of repressers' forms of affiliation with others provide opportunities to increase
eye movements and physiological arousal during exposure to cognitive clarity regarding one's understanding of the situation
emotional stimuli, R. D. Hansen and Hansen (1994) found a and to determine whether one's emotional reactions are appro-
pattern of heightened initial attention to the stimuli, followed priate (Schachter, 1959; see Kulik & Mahler, in press, for a
by rapid subsequent decreases in responsiveness to them (see discussion). A large literature on social comparison processes
also Foa & Kozak, 1986). They speculated that repressers di- underscores the importance of these functions of the social sup-
rected much of their conscious processing capacity toward re- port network (see S. E. Taylor, Wayment, & Carrillo, 1995;.and
ducing the threat value of the stimulus rather than processing Wood, 1989).
the properties of the stimulus itself. Similarly, Baumeister and Additionally, social contacts may act as a sounding board for
Cairns (1992) found that repressers paid considerable attention initial interpretations and appraisals so that blatantly incorrect
to negative information about themselves (under some condi- or premature judgments can be moderated and appropriate judg-
tions) but showed low subsequent recall for this negative infor- ments can be reinforced (Ashford, 1986; S. Cohen & McKay,
mation. Similar findings concerning anxiety have been reported 1984). This is not to suggest that the social support network
(see Matthews, 1990, for a review). Thus, although people may inevitably provides a corrective or accurate influence. Because
successfully orient toward negative information, early increases individuals tend to draw support from others like themselves,
in negative emotional arousal in response to such information misinterpretations and biases may sometimes be shared.6 None-
may derail information processing and subsequent proactive ef-
forts. Studies by Carver and Scheier (1994) and by Lyon and 4
An exception to the pattern that high levels of arousal early in the
Bugental (1997) make similar points. process of managing a stressor may impair proactive efforts is found in
In addition to attentional narrowing and insufficient pro- research on defensive pessimism (Norem & Cantor, 1986; Norem &
cessing of threatening stimuli, people's perceptions of high lev- Illingworth, 1993; Showers, 1988, 1992; Showers & Ruben, 1990).
els of potential threat may prompt defensive processing as a Defensive pessimists enter situations expecting the worst and make use
means to regulate negative emotional arousal. At an extreme, of the high levels of arousal that result from such appraisals to prepare
people may simply deny or distort threatening information, re- for an upcoming event, thus decreasing the likelihopd that an adverse
event will occur. However, they do so at the expense of managing affect
sulting in less serious initial appraisals than may be warranted
effectively. For example, defensive pessimists are less happy and success-
(Croyle, 1992; Jemmott, Ditto, & Croyle, 1986).5 Although ful over time, despite initial academic success equivalent to their more
denial can be a useful strategy for well-bounded, short-term optimistic classmates (Cantor & Norem, 1989).
stressors, such as dental visits, it is known to interfere with 5
See Breznitz (1983b) and Lazarus (1983) for more extended discus-
long-term coping efforts that require one to monitor a situation, sions of different kinds of denial.
6
such as cardiac rehabilitation (see S. E. Taylor & Aspinwall, Ironically, however, the degree to which threatening experiences are
1990, for a review). Because proactive efforts involve actions shared in one's social network may give people a false sense of security
426 ASPINWALL AND TAYLOR

theless, by exchanging information and appraisals about poten- an abortion (Cozzarelli, 1993), and men at risk for AIDS (S.
tial stressors with one's support network, a person should be E. Taylor et al., 1992).
able to acquire new information and interpretations, which may Several provocative questions remain about the exact role
enable the person to clarify his or her understanding of a nebu- of optimism and control-related individual differences in such
lous but potentially threatening situation. appraisals. The more favorable appraisals reported by partici-
pants with high levels of optimism or perceived control may
From Initial Appraisals to Preliminary Coping Efforts reflect (a) greater coping resources (Schwarzer, 1994); (b)
response bias, positivity bias, or shared method variance; (c)
Preliminary coping efforts depend heavily on the initial defi- greater experience with the successful management of prior po-
nition of the stressful event. For example, to the extent that tential stressors; (d) differences in the cognitive processes un-
people project the potential stressor and its implications forward dertaken, such as the content of mental simulations that may
in time through the use of mental simulation, the content of lead a person to perceive events as controllable; (e) a general
their projections may ease them into an action plan. Nonetheless, propensity to take action; or ( f ) the presence of more domain-
at least three general questions merit attention: What kinds of specific self-efficacy beliefs. Which of these alternatives ac-
appraisals give rise to active coping efforts?, what factors predict counts for the role of these individual differences in appraisals
whether offsetting efforts will be undertaken?, and what specific is not yet known.
actions will be undertaken? Understanding appraisals of uncontrollability. We have
What kinds of appraisals give rise to active coping efforts? noted that appraisals conducive to action should favor successful
In general, to regard situations as amenable to change leads to proactive coping in many cases. However, an understudied but
favorable problem-solving appraisals and corresponding action. critical aspect of proactive coping is the ability to determine
The beliefs that one is capable of enacting, required behaviors early (and correctly) that some incipient stressors are not ame-
and that those behaviors will be successful in averting stress nable to preliminary coping efforts. The research evidence on
may have critical and distinct effects on efforts to offset a poten- participants' ability to do this is scant but intriguing. When a
tial stressor (Bandura, 1986; Weisz, 1983; see Skinner, in press, stressor is clearly insurmountable, participants seem to make
for a review). Skinner argued that people with high-perceived such determinations easily and to moderate their efforts
control see the world as a structured system; they see more accordingly.
means-end relations in a given situation and, therefore, see In a series of experiments by Brehm and his colleagues
more opportunities for action. In contrast, if people's appraisals (Brehm, Wright, Solomon, Silka, & Greenberg, 1983; Wright,
of controllability are unfavorable, their problem-solving efforts Brehm, Crutcher, Evans, & Jones, 1990), participants were told
may be compromised. For example, Sansone and Berg (1993) that they must memorize 2, 6, or 20 pairs of nonsense words to
asked children and adults about the degree to which they had avoid a burst of aversive noise. Participants in the 6-pairs condi-
been able to anticipate negative events in the past 6 months. tion showed the highest levels of arousal and reported the task
Although adults had correctly anticipated specific negative to be most unpleasant, whereas participants in the 20-pairs con-
events, 60% reported having done nothing to prevent them. The dition showed lower levels of physiologic activity and did not
most common reason for respondents' lack of proactive effort rate the task as especially unpleasant. Apparently, participants
was an underestimation of the severity of the consequences and in the 20-pairs condition readily identified the situation as un-
perceptions of low personal control over the event. Appraisals controllable, did not mobilize efforts to manage the experimen-
then did not prompt offsetting actions because the events were tal task, and did not perceive this failure to be aversive. More
not perceived as controllable (see also Vitaliano, Russo, & Mai- interesting, these effects occurred on the first trial; that is, parti-
uro, 1987). cipants did not need to try to memorize the 20 pairs to realize
What factors predict appraisals conducive to action? Con- that their efforts would be unsuccessful.
sistent links between individual differences and appraisals of These results suggest that timely perceptions of lack of con-
controllability have been uncovered. Depression has been linked tingency between potential actions and outcomes may save a
to unfavorable appraisals of one's problem-solving ability person from expending resources to manage tasks that are im-
(Blankenstein, Flett, & Johnston, 1992; C. J. Taylor & Scogin, possible. However, few naturalistic situations offer such unam-
1992); that is, participants with depression or dysphoria tended biguous and readily available information about their amenabil-
to respond to danger signs with the perception that they could ity to effort. Instead, one must usually engage in some problem
do nothing about them. Optimism, in contrast, has been linked solving to gain preliminary information about whether the situa-
to favorable appraisals in a variety of populations, including tion is amenable to one's efforts. The information generated by
patients who have had coronary artery bypass surgery (Fitzger- such preliminary efforts may inform subsequent appraisals and
ald, Tennen, Affleck, & Prantsky, 1993) or a heart attack (De- efforts, an issue addressed in Elicitation and Use of Feedback.
sharnais, Godin, Jobin, Valois, & Ross, 1990), women awaiting Influences on strategy selection: What specific actions will
be taken? Assuming that appraisals conducive to action have
been made, what specific actions will be undertaken? Obviously,
about what they are facing or will face. Several experimental studies an answer to this question depends heavily on the particular
show that participants who are told that others in their group share a nature of the problem, but there are also some general rules that
similar threatening diagnosis rate their condition as less serious than may determine the kinds of action sequences that people will
those who are told their diagnosis is unique to them (see Croyle, 1992, undertake with respect to problems in their preliminary stage.
for a review). People are likely to invest time, effort, and other resources to
A STITCH IN TIME 427

solve problems only to the degree that a problem is actually do not take the proactive measures that would have offset those
perceived to exist, and they are likely to conserve resources as events in advance or at least muted them before they occurred.
much and as long as possible (Hobfoll, 1989). Thus, on the In our review of the appraisals and individual differences that
whole, incipient or small problems should first be met with foster active coping, we suggest that there may be two advan-
small and convenient actions. tages of active coping for proaction. As in studies of extant
Consider the example of a child who has shown a small but stressors, active coping may be more likely than avoidant coping
discernible drop in grades. There are several actions a parent to eliminate or change the form of an incipient or potentially
could take. The parent could ignore the lower grades, provide stressful event. Moreover, active coping efforts are more likely
help with homework, arrange for a conference with the teacher, to yield information about the potential stressor that may pro-
or arrange for tutoring or counseling. After encountering three vide useful feedback. To the extent that such information is used
or four disappointing quiz grades, most parents would hesitate to revise appraisals and to modify coping efforts, an individual
to employ an expensive tutor when extra attention to homework may be more successful in subsequent efforts to prevent the
in the evening might suffice. If a scheduled parent-teacher con- stressor or reduce its effects. We discuss these processes in
ference were coming up, then the parent could discuss the recent greater detail in the next section.
grades with the teacher, but the parent might not make a special
appointment unless the problem were determined to be a stable Elicitation and Use of Feedback: Modification of
or deteriorating one.
Preliminary Coping Efforts
Representativeness criteria dictate the moving of one's efforts
from smaller steps to bigger ones: One deals with a small prob- Initial coping efforts reflect an educated guess about the na-
lem with a small amount of effort and expends a greater amount ture of a potential stressor and the strategies that will offset it.
of effort when a problem is greater (Fiske & Taylor, 1991). Many factors, however, contribute to the need to revise apprais-
Literature from attribution research, for example, suggests that als and efforts over time. By definition, a potentially stressful
participants tend to see causes and effects in these terms, judging situation is a situation that has the potential to deteriorate. More-
a small (or preliminary) effect to have been produced by a over, if initial appraisals are based on incomplete early informa-
small cause and needing remediation by a small solution and a tion or motives that lead to a distortion of the potential stressor,
large problem to have been caused by a major factor and needing then they may be incorrect. Even if initial appraisals are reason-
remediation by a strong intervention (see Einhorn & Hogarth, ably accurate, initial attempts to manage the stressor or to check
1986; Fiske & Taylor, 1991; Kassin & Pryor, 1985; Kelley & its development may be unsuccessful and may even aggravate
Michela, 1980; and S. E. Taylor, 1982). To the extent that incipi- the problem (see Schonpflug, 1986, for a discussion). Finally,
ent stressors are still small problems, the idea that people start the potential stressor may prove to be intractable. Thus, the
small derives viability from this related literature. Although this elicitation and use of feedback regarding how successful prelim-
point may seem obvious, it has considerable significance with inary coping efforts have been are critical to both the effective
respect to the potential liabilities of proactive coping discussed ongoing management of the potential stressor and the conserva-
earlier. If people typically respond to signs of danger with small tion of resources.
initial steps, then the risk that proactive efforts may deplete their Role of active and proactive coping to elicit feedback. A
coping resources may be modest. critical proposition of the proactive coping model is that active
Another set of predictors of strategy selection consists of forms of coping yield more information about a potential stres-
individual differences linked to active or avoidant forms of cop- sor than avoidant forms of coping. Simply put, even if prelimi-
ing. In the literature on coping with extant stressors, control nary coping efforts are unsuccessful or aggravate the problem,
beliefs and optimism have been found to be robust predictors they should yield information about the problem that may be
of active versus avoidant forms of coping (see S. E. Taylor & used in subsequent efforts to manage it. What evidence is there
Aspinwall, 1996, for a review). Dispositional optimism, for for this hypothesis? In a study by Knudson, Sommers, and Gol-
example, predicts greater active coping with stressors as diverse ding (1980), married couples were asked to discuss three differ-
as coronary artery bypass surgery (Scheier et al., 1989) and ent topics that had been previously identified as sources of con-
emigration from East to West Germany (Jerusalem, 1993). At flict for the couple. The discussions were then coded as to
least some of the benefits of optimism are mediated by opti- whether the partners had confronted the issues or avoided them.
mists' greater use of active coping and lower use of avoidant Individual members of the couple were then shown a videotape
coping (Aspinwall & Taylor, 1992; Carver et al., 1993; see S. E. of the interaction and asked to complete questionnaires concern-
Taylor & Aspinwall, 1996, for a review). These results are ing their beliefs about their partner's motives and how they
supported by studies in which researchers have randomly as- believed their partner perceived their own behavior at several
signed participants to engage in active coping (cf. E. A. Leven- different points in the interaction. Over the course of the interac-
thal, Leventhal, Shacham, & Easterling, 1989). In contrast, indi- tion, couples who addressed the conflictual issue moved in the
vidual differences, such as neuroticism, have been linked to direction of greater shared agreement about each other's per-
more avoidant forms of coping, such as self-blame and wishful spectives and motives, whereas couples who avoided the issue
thinking (Bolger, 1990). Avoidant coping, in turn, has been grew further apart in their assessments of the same interaction.
linked to adverse outcomes in longitudinal studies. Specifically, These findings suggest that active coping strategies are not only
participants who coped using chronic avoidance appeared to be more effective than avoidant ones for proactive coping but may
at greater risk for the occurrence of major life events (Holahan & also be more effective because they provide feedback that peo-
Moos, 1986, 1987). This may occur because avoidant copers ple must gain about the unfolding situation.
428 ASPINWALL AND TAYLOR

Researchers who study psychological control have made a different types of information. Compared with controls, partici-
similar point. People with high levels of perceived control may pants exposed to the insoluble task showed impaired problem-
attempt to exert control more often and more forcefully than solving skills: They did not gather information that would allow
would those with low levels of perceived control. As people them to distinguish among alternatives and did not gather the
experiment with ways to exert control, they gain information most important information first. Although Sedek et al. interpre-
about the kinds of situation that are amenable to change as well ted these results as evidence of cognitive exhaustion, the litera-
as. important procedural knowledge about how and when to ture on helplessness suggests that such deficits in the acquisition
exert control in different situations (Skinner, in press; S. E. and use of information may result from motivational deficits,
Taylor, 1989). Such information may have general use for subse- cognitive deficits, or both (Maier & Seligman, 1976; Seligman,
quent coping with a wide range of stressors. 1975). Thus, a person's unsuccessful attempts to offset or pre-
Temporal factors in coping and the elicitation of feedback. vent stress may compromise his or her subsequent efforts to use
The preceding analysis suggests that the temporal pattern of feedback as the situation unfolds.
initial coping efforts and the degree to which these early efforts Do individual differences linked to active coping facilitate or
yield information about the stressor are ripe areas for future impede the use of feedback? Researchers are divided on the
research. Although no researcher of proactive coping has ex- question of whether such individual difference as optimism,
plored this issue, there is some suggestive evidence from re- perceived control, and self-esteem facilitate or impede respon-
search on coping with extant stressors. For example, studies of siveness to feedback. Baumeister (1989) hypothesized that peo-
adaptation to chronic illness suggest that patients hold differenti- ple with extremely favorable beliefs in their abilities do not
ated perceptions of control over different aspects of their illness, recognize their limits, enter situations for which they are unpre-
depending on how advanced their illness is (Affleck, Tennen, pared, and—ultimately—fail (e.g., Baumeister, Heatherton, &
Pfeiffer, & Fifield, 1987; Collins, Taylor, & Skokan, 1990; S. E. Tice, 1993). Consistent with this point, in several experimental
Taylor, Helgeson, Reed, & Skokan, 1991; Thompson, Nanni, & studies, researchers have linked self-esteem (Janoff-Bulman &
Levine, 1994; Thompson, Sobolew-Shubin, Galbraith, Schwan- Brickmah, 1982; McFarlin, Baumeister, & Blascovich, 1984)
kovsky, & Cruzen, 1993). Specifically, it appears that patients and perceived control (Sieber et al., 1992) to increased persis-
initially attempted to exert control over the course of their ill- tence on insoluble tasks. These studies suggest that the very
ness, discovered that such attempts were largely unsuccessful, beliefs that foster active coping may make people somewhat
and then transferred their coping efforts to more controllable resistant to the new information yielded by such attempts.
domains, such as the experience of daily symptoms and the A caveat must be raised about the literature on responses to
establishment of personal priorities. insoluble tasks, however. An experimental paradigm in which
Typically, coping inventories are not sensitive to this pattern the tasks are rigged can make a strategy that is usually successful
of coping efforts because the inventories require participants to appear to be a liability. If active copers gain better knowledge
report all of the coping strategies they used without regard to of situations that are amenable to control, then they may persist
the order. Thus, little is known about which strategies were in an experimental situation because the parameters of the situa-
tried first and what information was gained from these attempts. tion (e.g., an experiment on learning or a test of performance)
Similarly, studies of laboratory stressors rarely provide enough match the contingencies of situations in which they know the
trials to examine patterns of coping over time. (Studies by Ban- exercise of control is usually successful. They may persist at
dura and his colleagues [e.g., Bandura & Jourden, 1991] are insoluble tasks, not because they are more vulnerable to the
elegant exceptions.) Therefore, although there is some intriguing adverse effects of insolubility or impervious to unfavorable situ-
evidence that people develop differentiated beliefs about stress- ational contingencies but because the contingency information
ful events and their management, the role of early coping efforts that they are trying to use effectively is at odds with the structure
in this process merits additional investigation (cf. Carver & of the experimental paradigm (cf. Alloy, Albright, Abramson, &
Scheier, 1994; Ptacek, Smith, & Zanas, 1992). Dykman, 1990).
Cognitive and motivational factors in the use of feedback. A strikingly different relation of favorable beliefs to coping
After information about the nature of the potential stressor has with intractable stressors has been found in field research among
been elicited, people must be able and willing to process such participants coping with adverse conditions (Epstein & Meier,
information and to revise their appraisals and coping efforts 1989; Scheier & Carver, 1992; S. E. Taylor, 1989). Specifically,
accordingly (cf. Ashford & Cummings, 1983). In cases where participants with high levels of personal resources seemed to
the stressor proves intractable or at least very difficult, people be able to moderate their appraisals and coping efforts to match
must also be able to withstand the cognitive and motivational the characteristics of the stressor (Wiebe & Williams, 1992).
consequences of having tried to exert control and failed, at least For example, Vitaliano et al. (1987) found that medical students
initially. with an internal locus of control were more likely to report
The preservation of cognitive and motivational resources in having accepted relatively unchangeable stressors, such as hos-
the face of initial failure can be difficult. Researchers who have tile professors and an impersonal work environment, than would
exposed participants to insoluble tasks have uncovered debilitat- students with an external locus of control. Scheier, Weintraub,
ing effects of exposure to a lack of contingency, which may have and Carver (1986) found that optimists reported working ac-
implications for subsequent self-regulatory efforts. For example, tively to fix problems appraised as controllable but reported
Sedek, Kofta, and Tyszka (1993) exposed participants to five using strategies to manage their emotions in the face of problems
trials of an insoluble task and then asked them to participate appraised as uncontrollable. Neter, Taylor, and Kemeny (1997)
in an unrelated decision task that required attention to several found that initially optimistic men with HIV infection whose
A STITCH IN TIME 429

condition subsequently deteriorated over the following year re- 1. Many stressors are or can be avoided. An understanding
mained lower in psychological distress than their less optimistic of self-regulatory skills from social cognition, the functions of
counterparts whose situation also deteriorated. personal and social resources, and proclivities to use certain
Taken together, these findings suggest that individual differ- kinds of coping strategies illustrates how people may gain a
ences, such as control beliefs and optimism, do not necessarily proactive edge in many situations.
produce psychological distress or lead people to persist in efforts The realization that many stressors are or can be avoided or
to remediate stressors that are not amenable to their efforts. How minimized by effective proactive coping is important for several
can these findings be reconciled with the findings of laboratory reasons. First, it reverses the usual direction of causality in the
studies of nonproductive persistence? Our review suggests that stress-coping relationship, giving a primary and central status
people who hold optimistic or control beliefs may be better to coping. The emphasis on proaction encompasses events in
able to recognize the contingencies that influence which coping which the stress component is minimal but the coping compo-
strategies will be effective than would individuals who do not nent, potentially substantial. Although models of stress and cop-
make use of active coping strategies.7 In this vein, Pittman ing processes recognize the complex feedback relationships
(1993) suggested that, because initial failure to exert control among these processes (e.g., Lazarus & Fblkman, 1984), most
may cause people to question their understanding of the situa- researchers have examined the occurrence of a stressful event
tion, people with a high level of desire for control may respond and its subsequent moderation by coping strategies. To give
to difficult situations with increased and careful information coping a central status illustrates how proactive efforts set the
processing. Systematic processing, in turn, may serve to identify stage for the arrival of stressful events, substantially affecting
situational contingencies that would influence subsequent ac- whether they occur and what form they will take if they do.
tions with respect to the potential stressor. In contrast, as sug- The analysis of proactive coping also highlights the important
gested earlier, individual differences that result in poor subse- role that individual and social resources play in effective self-
quent information processing, such as repression and anxiety, regulation. In many stress and coping models, resources are
may make it difficult for people to respond to subsequent feed- regarded as moderators, such that less stress is experienced by
back and to changes in the nature of the task or problem (Jerusa- people with more time, money, and friends. Insofar as resource
lem, 1990; Matthews, 1990).8 accumulation precedes the recognition of any particular stressor
Social factors in the provision and interpretation of feedback. and proactive coping is virtually impossible in the absence of
As in the recognition and appraisal stages of proactive coping, resources, our analysis gives resources a more central and tem-
the social support network can be very important in the provi- porally prior status than is assumed within traditional stress and
sion and interpretation of feedback gained from preliminary coping frameworks (cf. Hobfoll, 1989). It suggests that, rather
coping efforts. When people attempt to cope with a stressor that than playing a moderating role, resources play a critical role in
is nebulous in form, the meaning and impact of their preliminary whether one experiences a stressor at all, what form it will take,
coping efforts are likely to be at least somewhat unclear. An- and how fully developed it is at the time one must deal with it.
swers to such questions as "How did I do?" and "Did I over- People who have few resources, such as those on the lower end
react?" can often only be provided by members of one's social of the sbcioeconomic scale, may experience more difficult and
network because the feedback one obtains in response to prelim- more severe stressors. For these reasons, the degree to which
inary efforts may be ambiguous. People may seek information reactive rather than proactive strategies must be used in the
from others about the effects of their actions or how their actions management of stress becomes evident.
may have appeared to others (Ashford & Cummings, 1983). This point leads to a third insight, namely, the recognition
Such information may be used to further interpret a potential that chronic and acute stress are integrally related (Brown &
stressor or to come up with new strategies to deal with an Harris, 1978). Freedom from chronic stress acts as a proactive
identified threatening event. Thus, the social support network
has an important role to play in clarifying the nature of feedback 7
in response to preliminary coping efforts (Ashford, 1986). Researchers of coping have examined participants' reactions to
strongly disconfirmatory evidence about their ability to manage stressful
events. How people respond to lesser indications of uncontrollability
Summary and Implications remains unclear. Such individual differences as the ability to tolerate
ambiguity (Andersen & Schwartz, 1992; Kruglanski & Webster, 1996)
We have argued that an important addition to stress and cop- may promote more favorable assessments in the face of early evidence
ing research is an understanding of how people cope proactively of a lack of control. Future research may address this issue.
8
to offset or eliminate stressors before they have a chance to The discussion above assumes that there are situational contingencies
develop. We proposed five interrelated tasks of proactive self- to detect. In the typical study of nonproductive persistence, participants
regulation, beginning with the accumulation of resources as a are presented with an insoluble task for which no variation of effort or
preventive measure and ending with the use of feedback from strategy is successful. In such cases, positive beliefs may foster nonpro-
preliminary coping efforts to discern whether an incipient stres- ductive persistence because there are no alternative ways to meet the
goal. In contrast, in naturalistic situations, such as the adjustment to
sor has emerged and whether preliminary efforts to avert it or chronic illness, some aspects of one's situation may be intractable,
change its form have been successful. We have detailed the whereas other aspects may be amenable to change. We suggest that the
role of individual differences, personal resources, and social latter set of circumstances presents the better test of whether individual
resources at each stage. We suggest that several implications differences linked to active coping predispose people to nonproductive
for the stress and coping literature may be derived from this persistence or are associated with greater ability to detect and respond
analysis. to situational contingencies.
430 ASPINWALL AND TAYLOR

resource, enabling people to engage in the task of anticipating or early on in the stress and coping process, these findings are
or recognizing stressful events before they occur. To the extent consistent with the prediction that usually beneficial emotion-
that an individual can accomplish this task, proactive efforts focused coping strategies, such as positive reappraisal, may be
may be undertaken. In an environment characterized by chronic maladaptive for proactive coping.
stress, proactive efforts are unlikely and often impossible, with The proactive coping framework also implicitly predicts that
acute stressors reaching a serious stage before they are dealt cognitive coping is ineffective unless accompanied by specific
with at all. When chronic burden reduces the degree to which actions. Mattlin et al. (1990) revealed support for this prediction
proactive coping can take place, the likelihood that acute stres- as well. They found that active cognitive coping was associated
sors will be exacerbated increases, thereby increasing the with poor emotional adjustment when a participant thought
amount of chronic stress that an individual experiences. about ways to make the situation better instead of acting. Active
The question arises as to whether proactive coping is always behavioral coping was consistently associated with positive
adaptive. Because it involves the mustering and allocation of emotional adjustment. Passive coping was associated with posi-
resources to offset potential future losses, the possibility that tive emotional adjustment only when stressors were chronic.
such efforts will be more costly than the problems they address These findings make explicit the need for such cognitive activi-
must be considered (Schonpflug, 1986). If a stressor does not ties as mental simulation, planning, and organization to be un-
occur or the wrong kinds of resources and actions are brought dertaken to assist behavioral coping and other self-regulatory
to bear on it, then one may drain coping efforts and resources activities rather than undertaken for their own sake (see also
so that, when the stressful event becomes full blown, one has Billings & Moos, 1984; Davey et al., 1992; and Lyubomirsky &
fewer resources with which to manage it. A propensity for proac- Nolen-Hoeksema, 1995).
tive coping may leave people constantly on guard or hypervigi- The proactive framework also provides some insight into the
lant about risks. To the extent that proactive coping efforts fail, question of why certain individual differences bear an inconsis-
cognitive and motivational resources may be further depleted. tent relation to coping and adaptational outcomes. Specifically,
Certainly these are legitimate risks, and they highlight the we have identified some individual differences that facilitate
possibility that proactive coping may not always be effective or some stages of proactive coping but inhibit others. A specific
adaptive (cf. Schonpflug, 1986). However, if individuals typi- example that we have highlighted in our review is the ironic
cally start small, then the initial actions undertaken in anticipa- relation between such concepts as anxiety and repression and
tion of a stressful event may be modest experimental efforts to the different stages of proactive coping. Although such factors
see if the stressor will be tractable in response. A major expendi- are associated with more rapid orientation to threatening stimuli
ture of resources may not be undertaken until a stressful event and a tendency to impute threat to ambiguous stimuli, they are
seems more likely. There may also be a trade-off for upfront also linked to incomplete processing of threatening information
costs and vigilance because such resource expenditures often (C. H. Hansen et al., 1992; Matthews, 1990) and to avoidant
provide information about a stressful event. Moreover, such coping (Davey et al., 1992). That is, the factors that make
feedback may enable people to further hone their proactive cop- people most likely to detect or perceive threats may also make
ing skills by providing general information about the kinds of them the least likely to respond effectively.
situations that may be tractable to proactive efforts. Finally, as This analysis highlights the distinction between proactive
just noted, to the extent that proactive efforts are successful, the coping and related activities, such as worrying and hypervigi-
total volume of stress to which an individual is exposed may lance. Specifically, the distinction rests on the observation that
be lower than that experienced by an individual who does not the detection of potential problems is not sufficient to ensure
engage in proactive coping efforts. On balance, then, we suggest proactive management of them. In addition to the detection of
that proactive coping efforts are typically more likely to be potential threats, proactive coping involves the interrelated activ-
beneficial than counterproductive. ities of maintaining attention to potential stressors, acting on
2. The proactive coping framework generates unique predic- them, and using feedback from such actions to modify one's
tions that have not been generated by stress and coping models. appraisals and actions. Activities, such as worry, may involve
First, we consider the effectiveness of emotion-focused cop- the identification of potential stressors and the simulation of
ing techniques, such as positive reappraisal from a proactive different negative outcomes, but worry bears a consistent posi-
perspective. Past analyses of stress and coping have often found tive relation to avoidant coping and a negative relation to active
positive emotion-focused strategies to be beneficial ways of coping. Our analysis suggests then that people who worry, are
coping with stressful events (e.g., Dunkel-Schetter et al., 1992). anxious, or are high in repression may be less successful at
Because we believe that effective proactive coping is virtually appraising potential stressors and may be less able to elicit new
always active, we maintain that positive reappraisal can often information about them than would individuals low in these
be an ineffective coping strategy at the stage of proaction. There qualities.
is some suggestive evidence for this hypothesis. Mattlin, Weth- The proactive coping framework explicitly predicts that active
ington, and Kessler (1990) found that positive reappraisal re- coping is superior to avoidant coping. Some reviews of the
duced the distress of extant stressors, such as the death of a coping literature have argued that active and avoidant coping
loved one, but actually interfered with the management of low- are merely different categories of coping strategies, each suited
level practical problems. They suggested that positive reap- to different types of stressors (Suls & Fletcher, 1985; S. E.
praisal in practical situations may be maladaptive because it Taylor & Clark, 1986). In such a viewpoint, active copers deal
inhibits active resolution of a problem. To the extent that the well with stressors amenable to action and avoidant copers deal
management of low-level practical problems occurs proactively well with stressful situations that must be tolerated. We suggest
A STITCH IN TIME 431

that, by virtue of ignoring proactive coping, the stress and coping Finally, the proactive coping framework generates specific
researchers have systematically underestimated the degree to predictions with respect to the benefits of social support. Re-
which active coping is adaptive. To the extent that proactive searchers have suggested that the emotional benefits provided
coping enables individuals to prevent, offset, eliminate, reduce, by a social support network constitute the main way in which
or modify impending stressful events, proactive copers eliminate social support is beneficial to people; the role of social support
much stress before it occurs. These successful efforts go largely in providing information or helping an individual appraise
unrecognized if extant stressors are the main focus of research. stressful events has received less empirical attention. We suggest
This analysis leads to the testable prediction that avoidant copers that the importance of information and appraisal support may
should typically experience more stress at any given point in be underestimated to the degree that researchers have ignored
time than active copers, a position for which there is emerging proactive coping. Although people surely benefit greatly from
evidence. For example, neuroticism (Aldwin, Levenson, emotional support when they face extant stressors, at the stage
Spiro, & Bosse, 1989; Bolger & Schilling, 1991; Headey & of proactive coping, they are especially likely to find information
Wearing, 1989; Magnus, Diener, Fujita, & Pavot, 1993), lack and appraisal support to be useful. Information may aid in the
of hardiness (Rhodewalt & Zone, 1989), and depression (Ham- recognition stage of proactive coping, and appraisal support may
men, 1992) have been linked to increased reports of the occur- help an individual to make preliminary appraisals of potential
rence of both major and minor stressors.9 Active coping, then, stressors. Both information and appraisal support may enable
may be more beneficial than previous analyses have implied.10 people to undertake effective preliminary coping strategies, and
The proactive coping framework also highlights the potential an individual may turn to others for help in interpreting the
value of conceptualizing active coping as a set of skills, not just feedback gained from preliminary coping efforts. To the extent
as a stylistic preference. This view of active coping renders that stress and coping researchers broaden their investigations
certain anomalies in the coping literature comprehensible. Spe- to include proactive coping, the important benefits of informa-
cifically, individual differences that predispose people toward tion and appraisal support from others should increasingly
active coping efforts in controllable situations also appear to emerge.
promote emotion-focused coping for uncontrollable stressors.
When active coping is viewed as a set of skills, this paradox Conclusion
becomes more comprehensible. When people learn any skill,
they learn both how to use the skill and when to use it. Thus, The perceptual, cognitive, and social processes that get people
individuals with active coping skills should understand when through the day also enable them to avoid or head off potentially
those skills are likely to serve effectively but also ineffectively stressful events. As such, basic processes of self-regulation and
and thus shift to emotion-focused or more passive coping strate- social interaction, as well as the more exceptional coping strate-
gies at this point. gies that people evolve for dealing with stress, come together
The focus on active coping as a set of skills alerts us to two in the stages of proactive coping. Continued examination of the
additional parameters that merit additional investigation. First, considerable efforts people undertake to avoid, detect, or man-
it suggests the need to move beyond general terminology, such
as problem solving, to focus on the specific skills of which such 9
As is the case with research on appraisals, an interpretational diffi-
problem solving may be composed. Such skills may include culty of these studies concerns whether individual differences are con-
goal setting, organization, planning, parsing problems into their founded with differences in the perception, memory, or reporting of
components, and mental simulation. Second, such a focus sug- negative events. Researchers who have reduced this bias by considering
gests that researchers might seek to identify environments that only "objective" events, such as marriage, promotion, death (cf.
foster the development of proactive coping skills and to study Headey & Wearing, 1989; Magnus et al., 1993), or arguments reported
the characteristics of those environments. As our analysis of by both spouses (Bolger & Schilling, 1991), have in general found
resources has suggested, such characteristics may include an evidence supporting the link between individual differences and reported
numbers of negative and positive life events.
emphasis on personal responsibility and control, opportunities 10
Note that most researchers of personality, coping, and life events
for reflection and the practice of specific skills, and the like.
have not examined the severity of the stressors in question, either because
Knowledge of the characteristics of environments that foster of space limitations in the assessment instruments or to avoid confound-
proactivity may be useful in the design of interventions that ing reported stress with appraisal processes. For example, Norris and
impart active coping skills in general or foster them with respect Murrell (1984), who found no relation between personal resources and
to specific problems.11 the occurrence of stressors, did not assess the intensity or severity of
This analysis suggests a range of human tasks on which re- the stressors (see also Ensel & Lin, 1991). It is possible that a focus
searchers might profitably focus to further understand proactive solely on the occurrence of events may further underestimate the contri-
coping skills. These include such widespread problems as per- bution of proactive coping to the reduction of stress over time. That is,
sonal retirement saving, disability or life insurance purchases, although people may not always be able to prevent the occurrence of
undesirable events, they may be able to reduce the severity of those
or borrowing and spending reductions. Similarly, standardized
events if they attempt to cope with them early in their development.
proactive efforts undertaken by individuals or families, such as 11
An excellent example of this approach is provided by relapse pre-
shopping with a list, keeping a checkbook, maintaining a car, and vention interventions (Brownell, Marlatt, Lichtenstein, & Wilson,
using a calendar effectively, merit consideration (Scholnick & 1986), in which participants are taught to recognize situations that may
Friedman, 1993). These topics have been largely ignored by prompt relapse, appraise them as controllable, develop specific skills
psychologists in the past but have intriguing potential for future and responses to avoid or control the situations, and use feedback to
study. evaluate if these efforts were successful.
432 ASPINWALL AND TAYLOR

age potential sources of stress in their environment may provide Billings, A. C., & Moos, R. H. (1984). Coping, stress, and social re-
a window to a wide range of social, cognitive, and behavioral sources among adults with unipolar depression. Journal of Personality
processes previously overlooked in stress and coping research and Social Psychology, 46, 877-891.
and may also point the way to interventions to identify and Blankenstein, K. R., Flett, G. L., & Johnston, M. E. (1992). Depression,
problem-solving ability, and problem-solving appraisals. Journal of
remediate potential sources of stress.
Clinical Psychology, 48, 749-759.
Blascovich, J., & Tomaka, J. (1996). The biopsychosocial model of
References arousal regulation. In M. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental
social psychology (Vol. 28, pp. 1-51). New \ark: Academic Press.
Affleck, G., Tennen, E., Pfeiffer, C., & Fifield, J. (1987). Appraisals of Bolger, N. (1990). Coping as a personality process: A prospective study.
control and predictability in adapting to a chronic disease. Journal Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 525—537.
of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 273—279. Bolger, N., & Schilling, E. A. (1991). Personality and the problems of
Aldwin, C. M., Levenson, M. R., Spiro, A., Ill, & Bosse, R. (1989). everyday life: The role of neuroticism in exposure and reactivity to
Does emotionality predict stress? Findings from the Normative Aging daily stressors. Journal of Personality, 59, 355-386.
Study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 618-624.
Brehm, J. W., Wright, R. A., Solomon, S., Silka, L., & Greenberg, J.
Alloy, L. B., Albright, J. S., Abramson, L. Y, & Dykman, B. M. (1990).
(1983). Perceived difficulty, energization, and the magnitude of goal
Depressive realism and nondepressive optimistic illusions: The role
valence. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 19, 21—48.
of the self. In R. E. Ingram (Ed.), Contemporary psychological ap-
Breznitz, S. (1983a). Anticipatory stress and denial. In S. Breznitz
proaches to depression (pp. 71-86). New "fork: Plenum.
(Ed.), The denial of stress (pp. 225-255). New York: International
Allred, K. D., & Smith, T. W. (1989). The hardy personality: Cognitive
Universities Press.
and physiological responses to evaluative threat. Journal of Personal-
ity and Social Psychology, 56, 257-266. Breznitz, S. (1983b). The seven kinds of denial. In S. Breznitz (Ed.),
Andersen, S. M., & Baum, A. (1994). Transference in interpersonal The denial of stress (pp. 257-280). New York: International Universi-
relations: Inferences and affect based on significant-other representa- ties Press.
tions. Journal of Personality, 62, 459-497. Brown, G. W, & Harris, T. (1978). Social origins of depression: A
Andersen, S. M., & Cole, S. W. (1990). "Do I know you?": The role study of psychiatric disorder in women. New \fork: Free Press.
of significant others in general social perception. Journal of Personal- Brownell, K. D., Marlatt, G. A., Lichtenstein, E., & Wilson, G. T.
ity and Social Psychology, 59, 384-399. (1986). Understanding and preventing relapse. American Psycholo-
Andersen, S. M., & Schwartz, A. H. (1992). Intolerance of ambiguity gist, 41, 765-782.
and depression: A cognitive vulnerability factor linked to hope- Cantor, N., & Norem, J. K. (1989). Defensive pessimism and stress and
lessness. Social Cognition, 10, 271-298. coping [Special issue]. Social Cognition, 7, 92-112.
Anderson, C. A., & Sechler, E. S. (1986). Effects of explanation and Carroll, J. S. (1978). The effect of imagining an event on expectations
counterexplanation on the development and use of social theories. for the event: An interpretation in terms of the availability heuristic.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 24-34. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 14, 88-96.
Ashford, S. J. (1986). Feedback-seeking in individual adaptation: A Carver, C. S., Pozo, C., Harris, S. D., Noriega, V, Scheier, M. E, Rob-
resource perspective. Academy of Management Journal, 29, 465-487. inson, D. S., Ketcham, A. S., Moffat, F. L., Jr., & Clark, K. C. (1993).
Ashford, S. J., & Cummings, L. L. (1983). Feedback as an individual How coping mediates the effect of optimism on distress: A study of
resource: Personal strategies of creating information. Organizational women with early stage breast cancer. Journal of Personality and
Behavior and Human Performance, 32, 379-398. Social Psychology, 65, 375-390.
Aspinwall, L. G., & Brunhart, S. M. (1996). Distinguishing optimism Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1994). Situational coping and coping
from denial: Optimistic beliefs predict attention to health threats. dispositions in a stressful transaction. Journal of Personality and
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 993-1003. Social Psychology, 66, 184-195.
Aspinwall, L. G., & Brunhart, S. M. (1997). Optimism and attention Cohen, R. Y, Brownell, K. D., & Felix, M. R. J. (1990). Age and sex
to proximal threats to health. Manuscript in preparation. differences in health habits and beliefs of school children. Health
Aspinwall, L. G., & Taylor, S. E. (1992). Modeling cognitive adaptation: Psychology, 9, 208-224.
A longitudinal investigation of the impact of individual differences Cohen, S., & McKay, G. (1984). Social support, stress, and the buffering
and coping on college adjustment and performance. Journal of Per- hypothesis: A theoretical analysis. In A. Baum, S. E. Taylor, & J.
sonality and Social Psychology, 63, 989-1003. Singer (Eds.), Handbook of psychology and health (Vol. 4, pp. 253-
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social-
268). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Cohen, S., & Wills, T. A. (1985). Stress, social support, and the buff-
Bandura, A., & Jourden, F. J. (1991). Self-regulatory mechanisms gov-
ering hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 310-357.
erning the impact of social comparison on complex decision making.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 941-951. Collins, R. L., Taylor, S. E., & Skokan, L. A. (1990). A better world or
Bargh, J. A., Bond, R. N., Lombard!, W. J., & Tola, M. E. (1986). The a shattered vision? Changes in perspectives following victimization.
additive nature of chronic and temporary sources of construct accessi- Social Cognition, 8, 263-285.
bility. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 869-878. Costa, P. T, Jr., Somerfield, M. R., & McCrae, R. R. (1996). Personality
Baumeister, R. F. (1989). The optimal margin of illusion. Journal of and coping: A reconceptualization. In M. Zeidner & N. S. Endler
Social and Clinical Psychology, 8, 176-189. (Eds.), Handbook of coping: Theory, research, applications (pp. 44—
Baumeister, R. F., & Cairns, K. J. (1992). Repression and self-presenta- 61). New York: Wiley.
tion: When audiences interfere with self-deceptive strategies. Journal Cozzarelli, C. (1993). Personality and self-efficacy as predictors of
of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 851-862. coping with abortion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Baumeister, R. F., Heatherton, T. E, & Tice, D. M. (1993). When ego 65, 1224-1236.
threats lead to self-regulation failure: Negative consequences of high Croyle, R. T. (1992). Appraisal of health threats: Cognition, motivation,
self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 141- and social comparison. Cognitive Theory and Research, 16, 165-182.
156.- Dakof, G. A., & Tayor, S. E. (1990). Victims' perceptions of social
A STITCH IN TIME 433

support: What is helpful from whom? Journal of Personality and Does imagining make it so? Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
Social Psychology, 58, 80-89. chology, 43, 89-99.
Davey, G. C. L., Hampton, J., Farrell, J., & Davidson, S. (1992). Some Hammen, C. L. (1992). Cognitive, life stress, and interpersonal ap-
characteristics of worrying: Evidence for worrying and anxiety as proaches to a developmental psychopathology model of depression.
separate constructs. Personality and Individual Differences, 13, 133— Development and Psychopathology, 4, 189-206.
147. Hammond, K. J. (1990). Case-based planning: A framework for plan-
Davidson, P.P., & Bobey, M. J. (1970). Repressor-sensitizer differ- ning from experience. Cognitive Science, 14, 385-444.
ences on repeated exposures to pain. Perceptual and Motor Skills, Hansen, C. H., Hansen, R. D., & Shantz, D. W. (1992). Repression at
31, 711-714. encoding: Discrete appraisals of emotional stimuli. Journal of Person-
Derryberry, D., & Reed, M. A. (1994). Temperament and attention: ality and Social Psychology, 63, 1026-1035.
Orienting toward and away from positive and negative signals. Jour- Hansen, R. D., & Hansen, C. H. (1994, July.). Repression: Front precon-
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 1128-1139. scious facilitation to conscious inhibition. Paper presented at the
Desharnais, R., Godin, G., Jobin, J., Valois, P., & Ross, A. (1990). American Psychological Society's annual meetings, Washington, DC.
Optimism and health-relevant cognitions after a myocardial infarction. Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (1979). Automatic and effortful processes in
Psychological Reports, 67, 1131-1135. memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 108, 356-
Dienstbier, R. A. (1989). Arousal and physiological toughness: Implica- 388.
tions for mental and physical health. Psychological Review, 96, 84- Hayes-Roth, B., & Hayes-Roth, F. (1979). A cognitive model of plan-
100. ning. Cognitive Science, 3, 275-310.
Dunkel-Schetter, C., Feinstein, L. G., Taylor, S. E., & Falke, R. L. Headey, B., & Wearing, A. (1989). Personality, life events, and subjec-
(1992). Patterns of coping with cancer. Health Psychology, 11, 79- tive well-being: Toward a dynamic equilibrium model. Journal of
87. Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 731-739.
Dunning, D., & Story, A. (1991). Depression, realism, and the overcon- Hirt, E. R., & Sherman, S. J. (1985). The role of prior knowledge in
fidence effect: Are the sadder wiser when predicting future actions explaining hypothetical events. Journal of Experimental Social Psy-
and events? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 521 — chology, 21, 519-543.
532. Hobfoll, S. E. (1988). The ecology of stress. Washington, DC:
Einhorn, H. J., & Hogarth, R. M. (1986). Behavioral decision theory: Hemisphere.
Processes of judgment and choice. Annual Review of Psychology, 32, Hobfoll, S. E. (1989). Conservation of resources: A new attempt at
53-88. conceptualizing stress. American Psychologist, 44, 513—524.
Ensel, W. M., & Lin, N. (1991). The life stress paradigm and psycholog- Holahan, C. J., & Moos, R. H. (1986). Personality, coping, and family
ical distress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 32, 321-341. resources in stress resistance: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of
Epstein, S., & Meier, P. (1989). Constructive thinking: A broad coping Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 389—395.
variable with specific components. Journal of Personality and Social Holahan, C. J., & Moos, R. H. (1987). Risk, resistance, and psychologi-
Psychology, 57, 332-350. cal distress: A longitudinal analysis with adults and children. Journal
Fiske, S. T. (1982). Schema-triggered affect: Applications to social per- of Abnormal Psychology, 96, 3-13.
ception. In M. S. Clark & S. T. Fiske (Eds.), Affect and cognition: Janis, I. L. (1967). Effects of fear arousal on attitude change: Recent
The 17th Annual Carnegie Symposium on Cognition (pp. 55-78). developments in theory and experimental research. In L. Berkowitz
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 3, pp. 167-
Fiske, S. T, & Taylor, S. E. (1991). Social cognition (2nd ed.). New 225). New York: Academic Press.
York: McGraw-Hill. Janoff-Bulman, R., & Brickman, P. (1982). Expectations and what peo-
Fitzgerald, T. E., Tennen, H., Affleck, G., & Prantsky, G. S. (1993). The ple learn from failure. In N. T. Feather (Ed.), Expectations and ac-
relative importance of dispositional optimism and control appraisals tions: Expectancy-value models in psychology (pp. 207-237). Hills-
in quality of life after coronary artery bypass surgery. Journal of dale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Behavioral Medicine, 16, 25-43. Jemmott, J. B., Ill, Ditto, P. H., & Croyle, R. T. (1986). Judging health
Foa, E. B., & Kozak, M. J. (1986). Emotional processing of fear: Expo- status: Effects of perceived prevalence and personal relevance. Jour-
sure to corrective information. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 20-35. nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 899-905.
Folkman, S. (1984). Personal control and stress and coping processes: Jerusalem, M. (1990). Temporal patterns of stress appraisals for high-
A theoretical analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, and low-anxious individuals. Anxiety Research, 3, 113-129.
46, 839-852. Jerusalem, M. (1993). Personal resources, environmental constraints,
Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R. S. (1985). If it changes, it must be a process: and adaptational processes: The predictive power of a theoretical stress
Study of emotion and coping during three stages of a college examina- model. Personality and Individual Differences, 14, 15—24.
tion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 150-170. Johnson, M. K., & Sherman, S. J. (1990). Constructing and recon-
Gilbert, D. T, Pelham, B. W., & Krull, D. S. (1988). On cognitive busy- structing the past and the future in the present. In E. T. Higgins &
ness: When person perceivers meet persons perceived. Journal of R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and social cogni-
Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 733-740. tion: Foundations of social behavior (pp. 482-526). New "York: Guil-
Gleicher, F., Kost, K. A., Baker, S. M., Strathman, A. J., Richman, ford Press.
S. A., & Sherman, S. J. (1990). The role of counterfactual thinking Jones, J. M. (1994). An exploration of temporality in human behavior.
in judgments of affect. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, In R. C. Schank & E. Langer (Eds.), Beliefs, reasoning, and decision-
16, 284-295. making: Psycho-logic in honor of Bob Abelson (pp. 389-411). Hills-
Goldstein, M. J., Jones, R. B., Clemens, T. L., Flagg, G. W, & Alexander, dale, NJ: Erlbaum.
F. G. (1965). Coping style as a factor in psychophysiological response Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Pren-
to a tension-arousing film. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- tice-Hall.
ogy, 1, 290-302. Kahneman, D., & Miller, D. T. (1986). Norm theory: Comparing reality
Gregory, L. W, Cialdini, R.B., & Carpenter, K. M. (1982). Self- to its alternatives. Psychological Review, 93, 136-153.
relevant scenarios as mediator of likelihood estimates and compliance: Kassin, S. M., & Pryor, J. B. (1985). The development of attribution
434 ASPINWALL AND TAYLOR

processes. In J. Pryor & J. Day (Eds.), The development of social McFarlin, D. B., Baumeister, R. F., & Blascovich, J. (1984). On know-
cognition (pp. 3-34). New \brk: Springer-Verlag. ing when to quit: Task failure, self-esteem, advice, and nonproductive
Katz, L., & Epstein, S. (1991). Constructive thinking and coping with persistence. Journal of Personality, 52, 139—155.
laboratory-induced stress. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- McGillicuddy-De Lisi, A. V., De Lisi, R., Flaugher, J., & Sigel, I. E.
ogy, 61, 789-800. (1987). Familial influences on planning. In S. L. Friedman, E. K.
Kelley, H. H., & Michela, J. L. (1980). Attribution theory and research. Scholnick, & R. R. Cocking (Eds.), Blueprints for thinking: The role
Annual Review of Psychology, 31, 457—501. of planning in cognitive development (pp. 395-427). Cambridge,
Knudson, R. M., Sommers, A. A., & Golding, S. L. (1980). Interper- England: Cambridge University Press.
sonal perception and mode of resolution in marital conflict. Journal McGonagle, K. A., & Kessler, R. C. (1990). Chronic stress, acute stress,
of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 751-763. and depressive symptoms. American Journal of Community Psychol-
Kobasa, S. C. (1979). Stressful life events and health: An inquiry into ogy, 18, 681-706.
hardiness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1-11. McGrath, I.E., & Beehr, T. A. (1990). Time and the stress process:
Kruglanski, A. W., & Webster, D. M. (1996). Motivated closing of the Some temporal issues in the conceptualization and measurement of
mind: "Seizing" and "freezing." Psychological Review, 103, 263— stress. Stress Medicine, 6, 93—104.
283. Miller, D. T, & McFarland, C. (1987). Counterfactual thinking and
Kulik, J. A., & Mahler, H. I. M. (in press). Social comparison, affilia- victim compensation: A test of norm theory. Personality and Social
tion, and coping with acute medical threats. In B. P. Buunk & F. X. Psychology Bulletin, 12, 513-519.
Gibbons (Eds.), Health and coping: Perspectives from social com- Miller, D. T, Turnbull, W, & McFarland, C. (1990). Counterfactual
parison theory. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. thinking and social perception: Thinking about what might have been.
Latane, B., & Darley, J. M. (1970). The unresponsive bystander: Why In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology
doesn't he help? New \brk: Appleton-Century-Crofts. (Vol. 23, pp. 305-331). New York: Academic Press.
Lazarus, R. S. (1983). The costs and benefits of denial. In S. Breznitz Miller, G. A., Galanter, E., & Pribram, K. H. (1960). Plans and the
(Ed.), Denial of stress (pp. 1 —30). New %rk: International Universi- structure of behavior. New \brk: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.
ties Press. Miller, S. M. (1987). Monitoring and blunting: Validation of a question-
Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. naire to assess styles of information-seeking under threat. Journal of
New %rk: Springer. Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 345—353.
Leventhal, E. A., Leventhal, H., Shacham, S., & Easterling, D. V. Mrazek, P. J., & Mrazek, D. A. (1987). Resilience in child maltreatment
(1989). Active coping reduces reports of pain from childbirth. Jour- victims: A conceptual exploration. Child Abuse and Neglect, 11, 357-
nal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57, 365—371. 366.
Leventhal, H. (1970). Findings and theory in the study of fear communi- Neter, E., Taylor, S. E., & Kemeny, M. E. (1997). When the future gets
cations. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psy- worse: Does optimism undermine or sustain well-being during pro-
chology (Vol. 5, 120-186). New York: Academic Press. gression of HIV? Manuscript submitted for publication.
Lyman, B., Bernardin, S., & Thomas, S. (1980). Frequency of imagery Neufeld, R. W. J., & Davidson, P. O. (1971). The effects of vicarious
in emotional experience. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 50, 1159-1162. and cognitive rehearsal on pain tolerance. Journal of Psychosomatic
Lyon, J., & Bugental, D. (1997). Relationship power and risk for sexu- Research, 15, 329-335.
ally transmitted diseases: The role of attentional "capture" during Norem, J. K., & Cantor, N. (1986). Defensive pessimism: "Harnessing"
sexual encounters. Manuscript submitted for publication. anxiety as motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Lyubomirsky, S., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1995). Effects of self-focused 51, 1208-1217.
rumination on negative thinking and interpersonal problem-solving. Norem, J. K., & Illingworth, K. S. S. (1993). Strategy-dependent effects
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 176—190. of reflecting on self and tasks: Some implications of optimism and
Maddux, I.E., Roberts, M. C., Sledden, E. A., & Wright, L. (1986). defensive pessimism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Developmental issues in child health psychology. American Psycholo- 65, 822-835.
gist, 41, 25-34. Norris, F. H., & Murrell, S. A. (1984). Protective function of resources
Magnus, K., Diener, E., Fujita, F., & Pavot, W. (1993). Extraversion related to life events, global stress, and depression in older adults.
and neuroticism as predictors of objective life events: A longitudinal Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 25, 424-437.
analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 1046- Orr, E., & Meyer, J. (1990). Disease appraisals as a coping strategy
1053. with cancer threat. Israeli Journal of Psychiatry and Related Sciences,
Maier, S. F., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1976). Learned helplessness: Theory 27, 145-159.
and evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 195, 3- Pearlin, L. I. (1989). The sociological study of stress. Journal of Health
46. and Social Behavior, 30, 241-256.
Manuck, S. B., Kamarck, T. W, Kasprowicz, A. S., & Waldstein, S. R. Pearlin, L. I., Menaghan, E. G., Lieberman, M. A., & Mullan, J. T.
(1993). Stability and patterning of behaviorally evoked cardiovascular (1981). The stress process. Journal of Health and Social Behavior,
reactivity. In J. Blascovich & E. S. Katkin (Eds.), Cardiovascular 22, 337-356.
reactivity to psychological stress and disease: An examination of the Pearlin, L. I., & Schooler, C. (1978). The structure of coping. Journal
evidence (pp. 83-108). Washington, DC: American Psychological of Health and Social Behavior, 19, 2-21.
Association. Perloff, L. S. (1983). Perceptions of vulnerability to victimization. Jour-
Markman, K. D., Gavanski, I., Sherman, S. J., & McMullen, M. N. nal of Social Issues, 39, 41-61.
(1993). The mental simulation of better and worse possible worlds. Perloff, L. S., & Fetzer, B. K. (1986). Self-other judgments and per-
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 29, 87-109. ceived vulnerability to victimization. Journal of Personality and So-
Matthews, A. (1990). Why worry? The cognitive function of anxiety. cial Psychology, 50, 502-510.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 28, 455—468. Petrie, A., Collins, W, & Solomon, P. (1960). The tolerance for pain
Mattlin, J. A., Wethington, E., & Kessler, R. C. (1990). Situational deter- and sensory deprivation. American Journal of Psychology, 73, 80-
minants of coping and coping effectiveness. Journal of Health and 90.
Social Behavior, 31, 103-122. Pittman, T. S. (1993). Control motivation and attitude change. In G.
A STITCH IN TIME 435

Weary, F. Gleicher, & K. L. Marsh (Eds.), Control motivation and Strathman, A., Gleicher, E, Boninger, D. S., & Edwards, C. S. (1994).
social cognition (pp. 157-175). New "fork: Springer-Verlag. The consideration of future consequences: Weighing immediate and
Ptacek, J. T., Smith, R. E., & Zanas, J. (1992). Gender, appraisal, and distant outcomes of behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
coping: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of Personality, 60, 747-770. chology, 66, 742-752.
Rhodewalt, R, & Zone, J. B. (1989). Appraisal of life change, depres- Suls, J., & Fletcher, B. (1985). The relative efficacy of avoidant and
sion, and illness in hardy and nonhardy women. Journal of Personality nonavoidant coping strategies: A meta-analysis. Health Psychology,
and Social Psychology, 56, 81-88. 4, 249-288.
Rothspan, S., & Read, S. J. (1996). Present versus future time perspec- Taylor, C. J., & Scogin, F. (1992). Dysphoria and coping in women:
tive and HIV risk among heterosexual college students. Health Psy- The effect of threat and challenge appraisals. Journal of Social and
chology, 15, 131-134. Clinical Psychology, 11, 26-42.
Sansone, C., & Berg, C. A. (1993). Adapting to the environment across Taylor, S. E. (1982). Social cognition and health. Personality and Social
the life span: Different process or different inputs? International Jour- Psychology Bulletin, 8, 549-562.
nal of Behavioral Development, 16, 215-241. Taylor, S. E. (1989). Positive illusions: Creative self-deception and the
Schachter, S. (1959). The psychology of affiliation. Stanford, CA: Stan- healthy mind. New \brk: Basic Books.
ford University Press. Taylor, S. E. (1991). The asymmetrical impact of positive and negative
Scheier, M. E, & Carver, C. S. (1992). Effects of optimism on psycho- events: The mobilization-minimization hypothesis. Psychological
logical and physical well-being: Theoretical overview and empirical Bulletin, 110, 67-85.
update. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 16, 201-228. Taylor, S. E. (1995). Health psychology (3rd ed.). New York: McGraw-
Scheier, M. E, Matthews, K. A., Owens, J., Magovern, G. T, Sr., Le- Hill.
febvre, R. C., Abbott, R. A., & Carver, C. S. (1989). Dispositional Taylor, S. E., & Aspinwall, L. G. (1990). Psychological aspects of
optimism and recovery from coronary artery bypass surgery: The chronic illness. In G. R. VandenBos & P. T. Costa, Jr. (Eds.), Psycho-
beneficial effects on physical and psychological well-being. Journal logical aspects of serious illness (pp. 3-60). Washington, DC: Ameri-
of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 1024-1040. can Psychological Association.
Scheier, M. E, Weintraub, J. K., & Carver, C. S. (1986). Coping with Taylor, S. E., & Aspinwall, L. G. (1996). Mediating and moderating
stress: Divergent strategies of optimists and pessimists. Journal of processes in psychosocial stress: Appraisal, coping, resistance, and
Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1257-1264. vulnerability. In H. B. Kaplan (Ed.), Psychosocial stress: Perspectives
Scholnick, E. K. (1994). Planning. In V. S. Ramachandran (Ed.), Ency- on structure, theory, life course, and methods (pp. 71-110). San
clopedia of human behavior (Vol. 3, pp. 525—534). New %rk: Aca- Diego, CA: Academic Press.
demic Press. Taylor, S. E., & Clark, L. F. (1986). Does information improve adjust-
Scholnick, E. K., & Friedman, S. A. (1993). Planning in context: Devel- ment to noxious events? In M. J. Saks & L. Saxe (Eds.), Advances in
opmental and situational considerations. International Journal of Be- applied social psychology (Vol. 3, pp. 1 -28). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
havioral Development, 16, 145-167. Taylor, S. E., Helgeson, V. S., Reed, G. M., & Skokan, L. A. (1991).
Schonpflug, W. (1986). Behavior economics as an approach to stress Self-generated feelings of control and adjustment to physical illness.
theory. In M. H. Appley & R. Trumbull (Eds.), Dynamics of stress: Journal of Social Issues, 47(4), 91-109.
Physiological, psychological, and social perspectives (pp. 81-98). Taylor, S. E., Kemeny, M. E., Aspinwall, L. G., Schneider, S. C., Rodri-
New 'fork: Plenum Press. guez, R., & Herbert, M. (1992). Optimism, coping, psychological
Schwarzer, R. (1994). Optimism, vulnerability, and self-beliefs in distress, and high-risk sexual behavior among men at risk for AIDS.
health-related cognitions: A systematic overview. Psychology and Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 460-473.
Health, 9, 161-180. Taylor, S. E., & Pham, L. B. (1996). Mental simulation, motivation, and
Sedek, G., Kofta, M., & Tyszka, T. (1993). Effects of uncontrollability action. In P. M. Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychology of
on subsequent decision making: Testing the cognitive exhaustion hy- action: Linking cognition and motivation to behavior (pp. 219-235).
pothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 1270- New York: Guilford Press.
1281. Taylor, S. E., & Schneider, S. K. (1989). Coping and the simulation of
Seligman, M. E. P. (1975). Helplessness: On depression, development events. Social Cognition, 7, 176-196.
and death. San Francisco: Freeman. Taylor, S. E., Wayment, H. A., & Carrillo, M. A. (1995). Social compar-
Showers, C. (1988). The effects of how and why thinking on perceptions ison and self-regulation. In R. M. Sorrentino & E. T. Higgins (Eds.),
of future negative events. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 12, 225- Handbook of motivation and cognition (pp. 3-27). New \fork:
240. Guilford.
Showers, C. (1992). The motivational and emotional consequences of Tennen, H., & Affleck, G. (1987). The costs and benefits of optimistic
considering positive or negative possibilities for an upcoming event. explanations and dispositional optimism. Journal of Personality, 55,
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 474-484. 378-393.
Showers, C., & Ruben, C. (1990). Distinguishing defensive pessimism Thoits, P. A. (1986). Social support as coping assistance. Journal of
from depression: Negative expectations and positive coping mecha- Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 54, 416-423.
nisms. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 14, 385-399. Thompson, S. C., Nanni, C., & Levine, A. (1994). Primary versus sec-
Sieber, W. J., Rodin, J., Larson, L., Ortega, S., Cummings, N., Levy, S., ondary and central versus consequence-related control in HIV-positive
Whiteside, T., & Herberman, R. (1992). Modulation of human natural men. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 540-547.
killer cell activity by exposure to uncontrollable stress. Brain, Behav- Thompson, S. C., Sobolew-Shubin, A., Galbraith, M. E., Schwankovsky,
ior, and Immunity, 6, 141-156. L., & Cruzen, D. (1993). Maintaining perceptions of control: Finding
Skinner, E. A. (in press). Planning and perceived control. In S. Fried- perceived control in low-control circumstances. Journal of Personal-
man & E. Scholnick (Eds.), Why, how, and when do we plan? The ity and Social Psychology, 64, 293-304.
developmental psychology of planning. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Tomaka, J., & Blascovich, J. (1994). Effects of justice beliefs on cogni-
Steptoe, A., & Vogele, C. (1992). Individual differences in the percep- tive appraisal of and subjective, physiological, and behavioral re-
tion of bodily sensations: The role of trait anxiety and coping style. sponses to potential stress. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
Behavior Research Therapy, 30, 597-607. ogy, 67, 732-740.
436 ASPINWALL AND TAYLOR

Trope, Y., & Neter, E. (1994). Reconciling competing motives in self- Weisz, J. R. (1983). Can I control it? The pursuit of veridical answers
evaluation: The role of self-control in feedback seeking. Journal of across the life span. In P. B. Baltes & O. G. Brim, Jr. (Eds.), Life-
Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 646-657. span development and behavior (pp. 233-300). New \brk: Academic
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1982). Judgments of and by representa- Press.
tiveness. In D. Kahneman, P. Slovic, & A. Tversky (Eds.), Judgment Wiebe, D. J. (1991). Hardiness and stress moderation: A test of pro-
under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases (pp. 84-100). Cambridge, posed mechanisms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
England: Cambridge University Press. 60, 89-99.
Vitaliano, P. O., Russo, J., & Maiuro, R. D. (1987). Locus of control, Wiebe, D. J., & Williams, P. G. (1992). Hardiness and health: A social
type of stressor, and appraisal within a cognitive-phenomenological psychophysiological perspective on stress and adaptation. Journal of
model of stress. Journal of Research in Personality, 21, 224-237. Social and Clinical Psychology, 11, 238-262.
Webster, D. M, Richter, L., & Kruglanski, A. W. (1996). On leaping Wood, J. V. (1989). Theory and research concerning social comparisons
to conclusions when feeling tired. Journal of Experimental Social of personal attributes. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 231-248.
Psychology, 32, 181-195. Wright, R. A., Brehm, J. W., Crutcher, W, Evans, M. T, & Jones, A.
(1990). Avoidant control difficulty and aversive incentive appraisals:
Weinberger, D. A. (1990). The construct validity of the repressive cop-
Additional evidence of an energization effect. Motivation and Emo-
ing style. In J. L. Singer (Ed.), Repression and disassociation (pp.
tion, 14, 45-73.
337-386). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Zimbardo, P. G., & Gonzalez, A. (1984, February). The times of your
Weinstein, N. D. (1982). Unrealistic optimism about susceptibility to life. Psychology Today, 18, 54.
health problems. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 5, 441-460.
Weinstein, N. D. (1987). Unrealistic optimism about susceptibility to Received June 19, 1995
health problems: Conclusions from a community-wide sample. Jour- Revision received August 1, 1996
nal of Behavioral Medicine, 10, 481-500. Accepted August 1, 1996 •

AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGICAL ASSOCIATION


SUBSCRIPTION CLAIMS INFORMATION Today's Date:_

We provide this form to assist members, institutions, and nonmember individuals with any subscription problems. With the
appropriate information we can begin a resolution. If you use the services of an agent, please do NOT duplicate claims through
them and directly to us. PLEASE PRINT CLEARLY AND IN INK IF POSSIBLE.

PRINT FULL NAME OR KEY NAME OF INSTITUTION MEMBER OR CUSTOMER NUMBER (MAYBE FOUND ON ANYPASTISSUE LABEL)

DATE YOUR ORDER WAS MAILED (OR PHONED)

PREPAID . CHECK __CHAROE


CHECK/CARD CLEARED DATE:_
crry STATE/COUNTRY
(If possible, send a copy, front and back, of yonr cancelled check to help us in our research
of your claim.)
YOUR NAME AND PHONE NUMBER ISSUES: __MISSINO DAMAGED

TITLE VOLUME OR YEAR NUMBER OR MONTH

Thank you. Once a claim is received and resolved, delivery of replacement issues routinely takes 4-6 weeks.

—————^—-^—-— (TO BE FILLED OUT BY APA STAFF) ——————


DATE RECEIVED:. DATE OF ACTION: _
ACTION TAKEN: _ INV.NO.&DATE:
STAFF NAME: LABEL NO. & DATE:

Send this form to APA Subscription Claims, 750 First Street, NE, Washington, DC 20002-4242

PLEASE DO NOT REMOVE. A PHOTOCOPY MAY BE USED.

You might also like