Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 6

J Autism Dev Disord (2008) 38:1161–1165

DOI 10.1007/s10803-007-0474-1

BRIEF REPORT

Brief Report: Cognitive Flexibility and Focused Attention


in Children and Adolescents with Asperger Syndrome or
High-Functioning Autism as Measured on the Computerized
Version of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test
Nils Kaland Æ Lars Smith Æ Erik Lykke Mortensen

Published online: 27 October 2007


Ó Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2007

Abstract The aim of the present study was to assess mental Health Organization (WHO) 1993) and the DSM-IV-TR
flexibility and set maintenance of a group of individuals with (American Psychiatric Association (APA) 2000), charac-
Asperger syndrome (AS) or high-functioning autism (HFA) terized by deficits in reciprocal social interaction of the
(N = 13; mean age 16,4), as compared with a matched group autistic kind, subtle impairment of verbal and non-verbal
of typically developing children and adolescents (N = 13; communication and the presence of idiosyncratic isolated
mean age 15,6) on the computerized version of the Wis- interests. On the basis of accumulated research evidence, it
consin Card Sorting Test (WCST). The participants in the appears that there are very few qualitative distinctions
AS/HFA group performed less well than the controls on all between AS and high-functioning autism (HFA), and that
categories of the WCST, but the differences did not reach most symptoms, associated features, and biological indices
conventional statistical significance on most categories of are shared or overlapping (Mcintosh and Dissanayke 2004;
the WCST. On the category failure to maintain set, however, Ozonoff and McMahon Griffith 2000). In the present
the AS/HFA participants performed significantly less well context, the two terms, AS and HFA, will be used
than the controls, suggesting a deficit of focused attention. interchangeably.
There is now a considerable body of evidence showing
Keywords Asperger syndrome  that a majority of individuals with an autism spectrum
High-functioning autism  Executive function  disorder (ASD) are subject to a social-cognitive dysfunc-
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test  Attention problems tion (Kaland et al. 2002; Wellman et al. 2001). Another
cognitive deficit in autism spectrum disorder is executive
dysfunction (Pennington and Ozonoff 1996) ‘Executive
Introduction functions’ (EF) is an umbrella term for functions such as
mental flexibility, planning, set maintenance, working
Asperger syndrome (AS), termed after the Austrian phy- memory, impulse control, and inhibition, as well as the
sician, Hans Asperger, (1944/91), is a type of pervasive initiation and monitoring of such activities (Hill 2004).
development disorder, included in the ICD-10 (World Empirical research on the executive functions of indi-
viduals with autism spectrum disorders has been
extensively conducted with a variety of tests, and different
N. Kaland (&) components of these functions have been investigated
Faculty of Social Science, Lillehammer University College, (Hughes et al. 1994; Ozonoff et al. 1991; Ozonoff and
Gudbrandsdalsvegen 350, Lillehammer 2624, Norway
McEvoy 1994; Ozonoff et al. 2004; Pennington and Ozo-
e-mail: nils@kaland.net
noff 1996). Executive functions are presumably linked to
L. Smith frontal structures of the brain and to the prefrontal cortex in
Institute of Psychology, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway particular (Pennington and Ozonoff 1996; Ozonoff et al.
2004; Roberts et al. 1998; Stuss and Knight 2002).
E. L. Mortensen
Department of Health Psychology, Institute of Public Health, The present study of cognitive flexibility and focused
University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark attention employed the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test

123
1162 J Autism Dev Disord (2008) 38:1161–1165

(WCST) (Heaton et al. 1993). This instrument is com- insignificant differences between the AS/HFA and control
monly regarded as ‘‘the gold standard executive function children on age as well as the three IQs (the range of Full
task’’ (Ozonoff et al. 2005, p. 532). It promises to be a Scale IQs was 94–125 and 92–122 in the AS/HFA and the
highly sensitive indicator of executive functions, especially control group, respectively). Eleven of the thirteen partic-
such as mental flexibility, planning, and set maintenance. ipants in the AS/HFA group had higher VIQ than PIQ.
The WCST has been used in studies of clinical groups Only males were included in the present study. All par-
comprising children with autism spectrum disorders, lan- ticipants were free of medication at the time of testing.
guage impairments, dyslexia, and ADHD, and individuals Analysis of questionnaires, completed by 11 of the 13
with psychiatric conditions (Liss et al. 2001; Nyden et al. pairs of parents, revealed no significant group differences
1999; Ozonoff 1997; Pennington and Ozonoff 1996; Ozo- in level of parental education (F (1,10) = 0,49, p = .50),
noff et al. 1991). and in parental education plus occupational experience (F
The present work was part of a more extensive inves- (1,10) = 0,01, p = .93).
tigation of cognitive functions of children and adolescents
with AS/HFA. As the less intelligent individuals with AS/
HFA who had participated in a previous study (Kaland The WCST
et al. 2002) had been excluded as participants, the aim of
the study was to assess whether the remaining 13 matched The computerized version of the WCST, consisting of four
10–20-years-old normally intelligent children and adoles- stimulus cards and 128 response cards, was employed
cents with AS would perform significantly less well on the (Heaton et al. 1993). The test proceeds through a number
central categories of the WCST́s computerized version as of shifts in set (sorting principles) that varies along three
compared with a matched group of normally developing dimensions (color, form and number). Successful perfor-
children and adolescents. mance on the WCST requires the participant first to
determine the correct sorting principle on the basis of
computer feedback, and then to maintain this sorting
Method principle or set. Scores were based on number of trials
administered, total number of correct responses, number of
Participants errors, number of perseverative responses, number of
perseverative errors, number of non-perseverative errors,
The participants in the AS/HFA group were recruited from number of categories completed, number of trials to com-
Kasperskolen in Copenhagen, which is a special school for plete the first category, conceptual level responses, failure
children and adolescents with Pervasive Developmental to maintain set, and learning to learn.
Disorders, and from the Centre for Autism in Copenhagen.
About 25 individuals were potential participants in the
study, but only those who were regarded as specifically Procedure
meeting the ICD-10 criteria for AS were included. The
participants in the clinical group had been independently The participants were told that this test was a little unusual
diagnosed in clinical settings by experienced psychologists in the sense that the examiner could not tell very much
and child psychiatrists and had received the diagnosis of about how to do it. Four ‘‘key cards’’ were displayed at the
AS. They had been assessed on ADI-R (Lord et al. 1994) top of the computer screen. The participant was asked to
and on ADOS (Lord et al. 1989). The controls were nor- match each of the cards that appeared on the screen to one
mally developing volunteers recruited from schools in of the four key cards. Four symbols on the keyboard in
Copenhagen. front of the participant resembled the four cards at the top
The present report presents data on 13 pairs of matched of the screen. In order to match the cards, the participant
index and control participants. The pairs were individually had to press the key with the symbol he thought would
matched on WISC-III IQ (Wechsler 1998) and chrono- match the card on the screen. Then a new card appeared
logical age. As shown in Table 1, there were small and from the bottom of the screen.
Table 1 Descriptive statistics
Variable Asperger group (N = 13) Control group (N = 13) Difference F (df 1,12)a p

a
Repeated measure ANOVA Chronological ageb 16.40 (2.84) 15.60 (3.07) 0.79 0.50 .50
corresponding to a matched Full scale IQ 109.00 (11.52) 109.62 (10.36) –0.62 0.18 .68
pairs t-test Verbal IQ 108.92 (14.55) 110.15 (12.25) –1.23 0.18 .68
b
Months transformed to Performance IQ 107.00 (8.80) 106.92 (12.87) 0.08 0.00 .98
decimal

123
J Autism Dev Disord (2008) 38:1161–1165 1163

The examiner told the participants if they had placed a maintain set. This pattern of group differences suggests
card correctly or incorrectly and each correct response was that intelligent individuals with AS or HFA may be less
followed by a high-pitch beep, whereas a buzzer followed prone than individuals with a diagnosis of autism to per-
an incorrect trial. Once 10 consecutive cards had been severate on the WCST. The reported group difference on
categorized correctly, the sorting principle changed; all the category conceptual level responses, which almost
sorts according to the previously correct strategy now reached statistical significance (p = .06), suggests than the
received a negative feedback. The participants were then participants in the clinical group showed less reliable
expected to use the examiner’s feedback to shift to a new insight into the correct sorting principle than the typically
categorization principle until 128 response cards had been developed control persons.
completed. Some studies have found limited executive dysfunctions
in relatively able individuals with AS or HFA when
assessed on the WCST. Liss and coworkers (2001) found
Results
that children with HFA showed impairments relative to
children with developmental language only on the category
The results are listed in Table 2. As may be seen, the
perseverative errors. Hill and Bird (2006) did not find
participants in the AS group performed less well than those
significant differences between persons with AS and mat-
in the control group on all categories, but the differences
ched controls on ‘‘classical’’ tests of executive functions
did not reach statistical significance, except for the cate-
such as the Modified Card Sorting Test (Nelson 1976),
gory failure to maintain set (p \ .04). Table 2 also shows
whereas significant differences were demonstrated on
that the differences were approaching conventional statis-
newer and more sensitive tests of executive functions.
tical significance on conceptual level responses and
Minshew et al. (1992) detected no difference between the
categories completed. With only 13 participants in each
number of perseverative errors produced by a group of
group statistical power was necessarily low.
adolescents with HFA and the number of such errors pro-
No significant differences were observed between the
duced by normal controls. Schneider and Asarnow (1987),
AS and the control group with respect to the correlations
whose study may be methodologically questionable, also
between background variables (age and IQ) and WCST
found few significant differences on the WCST between
performance. Consequently, we calculated the mean of the
children with ASD and schizophrenia and typically
correlations in the two groups (using Fischer’s Z-trans-
developing control children.
formation). The mean correlations were generally low (in
There may be different pattern of group differences in
most cases \ .20) and insignificant.
participants with and without mental retardation. Turner
(1995, as cited in Turner 1997) found that whereas those
Discussion with mental retardation performed significantly less well
than the controls on a set-shifting task, there were no sig-
The participants in the AS group performed below the level nificant difference between the high-functioning group and
of the control group on all measures of the WCST. How- the comparison group on this task. Nydén et al. (1999)
ever, the differences between the groups were not found no specific marker of executive functions in children
statistically significant, except for the category failure to with AS and attention disorder.

Table 2 Results on the WCST for the AS group and in the control group
Category AS group (N = 13) Means (SD) Control group (N = 13) Means (SD) Difference F p

Trials administered 107.15 (20.14) 96.69 (18.07) 10.46 1.67 .22


Total number of correct responses 82.62 (12.09) 75.46 (9.15) 7.15 2.20 .16
Total numbers of errors 24.54 (11.39) 21.23 (11.40) 3.31 0.64 .44
Perseverative responses 12.31 (5.89) 10.77 (4.40) 1.54 0.76 .40
Perseverative errors 11.46 (5.21) 10.31 (4.37) 1.15 0.49 .50
Non-perseverative errors 13.08 (6.84) 10.92 (7.43) 2.15 0.68 .43
Conceptual level responses 76.69 (10.74) 69.08 (7.30) 7.62 4.16 .06
Number of categories completed 5.15 (1.28) 5.92 (0.28) –0.77 4.14 .06
Number of trials to complete 1. category 17.15 (8.24) 14.46 (5.29) 2.69 0.85 .37
Failure to maintain set 2.46 (2.30) 0.69 (0.85) 1.77 5.41 \.04
Learning to learn 3.17 (3.44) 2.30 (1.68) 0.87 0.48 .50

123
1164 J Autism Dev Disord (2008) 38:1161–1165

In the present study the participants in the AS group Acknowledgments Colleagues and mentors contributed to the work
seemed to recognize the sorting principle, but did not presented in this paper with ideas and discussions. Special thanks are
given to Kirsten Callesen, Dorte Gottlieb and Annette Moller Nielsen
manage to keep the strategy of sorting in mind during the for their enthusiastic and indefatigable effort in testing the children
whole test session. The category failure to maintain set and adolescents. The kind involvement of the participants and their
appeared to be the one that demonstrated the AS persons’ families is thankfully acknowledged. This work was supported by
impairment on the WCST most persuasively. This finding, grants from the Norwegian Council for Research (to the first author).
however, contrasts with other studies showing that persons
with the diagnosis of high-functioning autism and AS are References
impaired on several categories of the WCST (Ozonoff et
American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and statistical
al. 1991; Shu et al. 2001).
manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text rev.). Washington, DC:
Did the AS participants in the present study have Author.
problems with attention, making them unable to control Asperger, H. (1944/1991). Autistic Psychopathy. In Childhood (Frith,
their impulses, thus missing the sorting principle during U., translation & annotation). In U. Frith (Ed.), Autism and
Asperger syndrome (pp. 37–92). New York: Cambridge Univer-
the WCST test session? There may be seeds of evidence
sity Press (Original work published 1944).
for this assumption. On the WISC-III’s subtest digit Garretson, H. B., Fein, D., & Waterhouse, L. (1990). Sustained
span, which is a measure of focused attention, the AS attention in children with autism. Journal of Autism and
participants performed significantly less well than the Developmental Disorders, 20, 101–114.
Heaton, R. K., Chelune, G. J., Talley, J. L., Kay, G. G., & Curtiss, G.
controls (p = .0276), and this was also the case for the
(1993). Wisconsin Card Sorting Test manual: Revised and
subtest symbols (p = .0151, which is a measure of sus- Expanded. Odessa. F. L. Psychological Assessment Resources.
tained attention. These findings are in accordance with Hill, E. (2004). Evaluating the theory of executive function in autism.
those of Garretson et al. (1990) and Nyden et al. (1999), Developmental Review, 24, 189–233.
Hill, E., & Bird, C. M. (2006). Executive processes in Asperger
who found that children and adolescents with high-
syndrome: Patterns of performance in a multiple case series.
functioning autism or AS showed attention problems Neuropsychologia, 44, 2822–2835.
when assessed on diverse measures of executive Hughes, C., Russell, J., & Robbins, T. W. (1994). Evidence for
functions. executive dysfunction in autism. Neuropsychologia, 32, 477–492.
Kaland, N., Møller-Nielsen A., Callesen, K., Mortensen, E. L.,
Individuals with ASD do not seem to perform poorly on
Gottlieb D., & Smith, L. (2002). A new ‘advanced’ test of theory
the WCST across the board. It is also important to recog- of mind: Evidence from children and adolescents with Asperger
nize that there can be considerable within-group syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43,
heterogeneity (Liss et al. 2001), as was the case in the 517–528.
Liss, M., Harel, B., Fein, D., Allen, D., Dunn, M., Feinstein, C.,
present study. Moreover, detailed WCST studies by per-
Morris, R., Waterhouse, L., & Rapin, I. (2001). Predictors and
sons with focal frontal lobe lesions have revealed varying correlates of adaptive functioning in children with developmen-
performance profiles according to the site of the lesion tal disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
(Ozonoff et al. 2004; Stuss et al. 2000). 31, 219–230.
Lord, C., Rutter, M., Goode, S., Heemsbergen, J., Jordan, H.,
Studies that include participants from across the autism
Manhood L., & Schopler, E. (1989). Autism Diagnostic Obser-
spectrum are needed in answering basic questions about the vation Schedule: A standardized observation of communicative
universality of executive function impairments in this and social behavior. Journal of Autism and Developmental
spectrum and also the external validity of subtypes, for Disorders, 19, 185–212.
Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism Diagnostic
example Asperger syndrome (Ozonoff et al. 2004). Ozo-
Interview-Revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview
noff (1997) suggested that executive functions should be for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive develop-
understood in a multidimensional perspective. There is still mental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental
uncertainty about which executive and non-executive Disorders, 24, 659–685.
Mcintosh, K. E., & Dissanayake, C. (2004). Annotation: The
functions the WCST measures; while generally considered
similarities and differences between autistic disorder and
to be a test of cognitive flexibility, a variety of other Aspergerés disorder: A review of the empirical evidence.
cognitive operations appear to be required for successful Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 421–434.
performance. Administration of a more extensive and Minshew, N. J., Goldstein, G., Muenz L. R., & Payton, L. R. (1992).
Neuropsychological functioning in non-mentally retarded autis-
sensitive battery of executive function measures for indi-
tic individuals. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
viduals with ASD may permit identification of a unique Neuropsychology 14, 749–761.
executive profile, with disorder-specific patterns of Nelson, H. E. (1976). A modified card sorting test sensitive to frontal
strengths and weaknesses. Such executive ‘‘fingerprints’’ lobe defects. Cortex, 12, 313–324.
Nyden, A., Gillberg, C., Hjelmquist, E., & Heimann, M. (1999).
(Ozonoff and Jensen 1999, p. 175) may be a useful guide in
Executive/attention deficits in boys with Asperger syndrome,
the attempt to identify profiles in AS/HFA and other attention disorder, and reading/writing disorder. Autism, 3, 213–
aberrant conditions. 228.

123
J Autism Dev Disord (2008) 38:1161–1165 1165

Ozonoff, S. (1997). Components of executive function in autism and Roberts, A. C., Robbins, T. W., & Weiskrantz, L. (1998). The
other disorders. In J. Russell (Ed.), Autism as an executive prefrontal cortex. Executive and cognitive functions. Oxford:
disorder (pp. 179–211). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Oxford University Press.
Ozonoff, S., Cook, I., Coon, H., Dawson, G., Joseph, R. M., Klin, A., Schneider, S. G., & Asarnow, R. F. (1987). A comparison of
McMahoon, W. M., Minshew, N., Munson, J. A., Pennington, B. cognitive-neuropsychlogical impairment of nonretarded autistic
F., Rogers, S. J., Spence, M. A., Tager-Flusberg, H., Volkmar, F. and schizophrenic children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psych-
R., & Wrathall, D. (2004). Performance on Cambridge neuro- logy, 15, 29–46.
psychological test automated battery subtests sensitive to frontal Shu, C., Lung, W., Tien, A. Y., & Chen, C. (2001). Executive
lobe function in people with autistic disorder: Evidence from the function deficits in non-retarded autistic children. Autism, 5,
collaborative programs of excellence in autism network. Journal 165–174.
of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 139–150. Stuss, D. T., & Knight, R. T. (2002). Principles of frontal lobe
Ozonoff, S., Goodlin-Jones, B. L., & Solomon, M. (2005). Evidence- function. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
based assessment of autism spectrum disorder in children and Stuss, D. T., Levine, B., Alexander, M. P., Hong, J., Palumbo, C.,
adolescents. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychol- Hamer, L., Murphy, K. J., & Izukawa, D. (2000). Wisconsin
ogy, 34, 523–540. Card Sorting Test performance in patients with focal frontal and
Ozonoff, S., & Jensen, J. (1999). Brief report: Specific executive posterior brain damage: Effects of lesion location and test
function profiles in three neurodevelopmental disorders. Journal structure on separable cognitive processes. Neuropsychologia,
of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 171–177. 38, 388–402.
Ozonoff, S., & McEvoy, R. E. (1994). A longitudinal study of Turner, M. (1997). Towards an executive dysfunction account of
executive function and theory of mind development in autism. repetitive behaviour in autism. In J. Russell (Ed.), Autism as an
Development and Psychopathology, 6, 415–431. executive disorder (pp. 57–100). New York: Oxford University
Ozonoff, S., & McMahon Griffith, E. (2000). Neuropsychological Press.
function and the external validity of Asperger syndrome. In A. Wechsler, D. (1998). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children.
Klin, F. R. Volkmar, & S. S. Sparrow (Eds.), Asperger syndrome Danish translation and adaptation. Kopenhagen: Danish Psy-
(pp. 72–96). New York: Guildford Press. chological Publishers.
Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (1991). Executive Wellman, H., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of
function deficits in high-functioning autistic individuals: Rela- theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child
tionship to theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology and Development, 72, 655–684.
Psychiatry, 32, 1081–1105. World Health Organization (1993). The ICD-10 classification of
Pennington, B. F., & Ozonoff, S. (1996). Executive functions and mental and behavioral disorders. Diagnostic criteria for research.
developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology Geneva: Author.
and Psychiatry, 37, 51–87.

123

You might also like