Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 13

Vision Res., Vol. 37, No. 19, pp.

2661-2673, 1997
© 1997 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved
Pergamon Printed in Great Britain
PII: S0042-6989(97)00114-4
0042-6989/97 $17.00 + 0.00

Flicker Parameters are Different for Suppression


of Myopia and Hyperopia
HARTMUT N. SCHWAHN,*t FRANK SCHAEFFEL*
Received 22 August 1996; in revised form 29 January 1997

Axial eye growth rates in the chicken are controlled by local retinal image-processing circuits.
These circuits quantify the loss of contrast for different spatial frequencies and promote axial
eye growth rates in correlation with the amount of retinal image degradation ("deprivation
myopia"). They also distinguish whether the plane of focus lies in front of or behind the retina. How
the sign of defocus is detected still remains unclear. Cues from chromatic aberration are not
important.
In an attempt to isolate retinal circuits controlling the development of myopia or hyperopia,
young chickens were raised in flickering fight of different frequencies (12 and 6 Hz) and duty cycles
(4-75 %) produced by rotating chopper disks. The effects of flickering light on refractive errors and
change in axial growth rates induced by translucent occluders or defocnsing lenses were measured
by infrared retinoscopy and A-scan ultrasound, respectively. Retinal electrical activity was
evaluated by flicker ERG after matching flicker parameters and stimulation brightness at retinal
surface. Changes in retinal and vitreal dopamine content caused by flicker in occluded and normal
eyes were determined by HPLC-ECD.
Strikingly, suppression of myopia occurred for similar flicker parameters, whether induced by
translucent occluders ("deprivation") or negative lenses ("defocns"). The degree to which myopia
was suppressed was correlated with the duration of flicker dark phase and with the ERG amplitude.
In contrast, suppression of hyperopia did not correlate with these parameters. We conclude that
two different retinal circuits with different temporal characteristics are involved in the processing
of hyperopic defocus/deprivation and of myopic defocus, the first one dependent on flicker ERG
amplitude. However, we did not find any correlation between the rate of dopamine release and the
degree of inhibition of deprivation myopia in flickering light. © 1997 Elsevier Science Ltd

Flicker light Myopia Hyperopia Electroretinogram Chicken

INTRODUCTION minimum. Apparently, however, the proposed image


processor in the retina not only quantifies the amount of
Axial eye growth and refractive development in higher
deprivation, but also determines the relative position of
vertebrates are controlled by signals derived from retinal
the plane of focus, including the sign of defocus imposed
image processing; the role of the brain is unclear and
by spectacle lenses (Schaeffel et al., 1988; Irving et al.,
possibly marginal (Diether & Schaeffel, 1997; Wildsoet 1992). Input from the accommodation feedback loop is
& Wallman, 1995). There is experimental evidence that not necessary (Schaeffel et al., 1990; Troilo & Wallman,
the retina can compile information on the average image 1991; Schwahn & Schaeffel, 1994; Diether & Schaeffel,
quality (i.e. the high spatial frequency content) because 1997; Schmid & Wildsoet, 1996a). There is even
translucent eye occluders produce myopia ("deprivation evidence that the sign of defocus is detected locally
myopia"; Wallman et al., 1978) in correlation with the within the eye. This is suggested by experiments showing
loss of image contrast (Bartmann & Schaeffel, 1994). It a local compensation for local refractive errors imposed
is tempting to speculate that a role of deprivation by lens segments that defocus only parts of the visual
myopia is to reduce hyperopia in neonates and to tune field (Diether & Schaeffel, 1997). How can the sign of
refractive state so that the average "deprivation" is at a defocus be determined locally within the eye without
input from the brain? This is still a puzzling enigma.
Although it would have been a plausible explanation,
*Department of Experimental Ophthalmology, University Eye Hospi- chromatic aberration occurring in the eye's optical
tal, Rrntgenweg 11, D-72076 Ttibingen, Germany. system does not provide cues for the sign of defocus
JTo whom all correspondence should be addressed [Tel (+49 7071) 29
85926; Fax (+49 7071) 29 5777; Email schwahn@uni-tuebin- (Schaeffel & Howland, 1991; Rohrer et al., 1992;
gen.de]. Wildsoet et al., 1993).
2661
2662 H.N. SCHWAHN and F. SCHAEFFEL

13ms
4%
!10 ms

12%
~1 ms

12 HZ 25 %
~42 ms i
5oo m
•3ms
75%

~-- :
1 rotation(1/6 sec)
~42 ms i

6 Hz f 25%: 813 ms

50 %

FIGURE I. Angular extent of the black and transparent sections of the rotating chopper disc for the generation of flickering
light. Duty cycles reflect the ratio of light to dark periods and are given in percent. Durations are given in msec and are valid for
the disk's speed of 6 rotations per second.

A possible approach to learn more about the retinal Rather than just varying the flicker frequency as in
image-processing involved is to study the effects of previous studies with xenon-lamp stroboscopes (extre-
flickering light stimulation on both deprivation myopia mely short light pulses), we have applied flickering light
and lens-induced refractive errors. There is experimental of defined brightness, frequency and duty cycle to
evidence (Schaeffel et al., 1995) that the underlying chickens treated with image-degrading occluders or
image processing occurs, at least in part, by pharmaco- defocusing lenses. To characterize retinal responses to
logically different mechanisms; they may also have flicker, electroretinograms (ERGs) were recorded at
different sensitivity to spatial and temporal stimulation. comparable duty cycles and frequencies with the retinal
Both, defocus-induced and deprivation-induced myopia brightness carefully matched to those in the lens- and
share some common features; they can both be occluder experiments.
suppressed by stroboscopic light (Gottlieb et al., 1987; Dopamine released from retinal amacrine cells has
Rohrer et al., 1995; Vingrys et al., 1991) or by reserpine been proposed to be a messenger linking retinal image
(Schaeffel et al., 1995), a neurotoxin that depletes retinal processing and eye growth, since its release and retinal
dopamine stores. Intermittent periods of clear vision content drop during lens-induced defocus and deprivation
reduce both occluder- and negative lens-induced myopia. (Stone et al., ! 989; Rohrer et al., 1993; Ohngemach et al.,
"Clear vision periods" of about the same duration are 1997). Dopamine release, however, can also be triggered
necessary for a 95% suppression of myopia: 130 min for in some species by stimulation with flickering light
deprivation myopia (Napper et al., 1995) and less than (Dong & McReynolds, 1992; Kirsch & Wagner, 1989;
180 min for lens-induced myopia (Schmid & Wildsoet, Boelen et al., 1996) and, in a frequency-dependent
1996a). On the other hand, both types of induced myopia fashion, by electrical stimulation (Dubocovich & Hens-
have been claimed to show different frequency tuning for let, 1986). Possibly the loss of high spatial contrast
suppression by stroboscopic light (Schmid & Wildsoet, during lens and occluder treatment might cause a
1996b). Also, deprivation myopia develops normally decrease in the activity of retinal cells leading to the
after optic nerve section (Troilo et al., 1987) whereas reported drop in the rate of dopamine release. However,
lens-induced myopia is reduced by this intervention in eyes deprived from clear vision, amacrine cells might
(Wildsoet & Wallman, 1995). Hyperopia induced by be stimulated by flickering light to release dopamine,
positive lenses, on the other hand, does not require an which in turn could compensate the drop in dopamine
intact optic nerve (Wildsoet & Wallman, 1995) and is not levels, mimicking a period of clear vision. We have,
suppressed by reserpine (Schaeffel et al., 1995). therefore, measured retinal dopamine content and
MYOPIA AND HYPEROPIA SUPPRESSEDBY DIFFERENTFLICKER LIGHT 2663

TABLE 1. Groups of chickens, treatment and number


Rearing condition
Treatment
Flicker Flicker Diurnal
Group frequency duty cycle fight cycle Treatment Age Number
1 12 Hz 4% 12F-12D occl. day 9--day 16 6
2 12 Hz 12% 12F-12D eccl. day 9--day 16 7
3 12 Hz 50% 12F-12D eccl. day 9--day 16 9
4 12 Hz 75% 12F-12D eccl. day 9--day 16 6
5 6 Hz 25% 12F-12D eccl. day 9--day 16 5
6 6 Hz 50% 12F-12D eccl. day 9-day 16 5
7 no flicker 12N-12D eccl. day 9---day16 9
8 12 Hz 12% 12F-12D +8 D day lO--day 15 6
9 12 Hz 12% 12F-12D -8 D day 10--day 15 6
10 12 Hz 50% 12F-12D +8 D day 10--day 15 8
11 12 Hz 50% 12F-12D -8 D day 10.-day 15 10
12 6 Hz 25% 12F-12D +8 D day 10--day 15 5
13 6 Hz 25% 12F-12D -8 D day 10-day 15 5
14 6 Hz 50% 12F-12D +8 D day 10--day 15 5
15 6 Hz 50% 12F-12D -8 D day 10--day 15 5
16 no flicker 12N-12D +8 D day 10-day 15 5
17 no flicker 12N-12D -8 D day 10-day 15 5
19 12 Hz 4% 9N-3F-12D eccl. day 10--day 17 7
20 12 Hz 50% 12F-12D eccl. day 10--day 17 6
21 12 Hz 50% 9N-3F-12D eccl. day 10--day 17 6
F, hours of flicker illumination; N, hours of normal incandescentlight; D, hours of darkness.

dopamine release in flickering light of different duration To obtain the different flicker parameters, the dark and
(12 and 3 hr) and its effect on deprivation myopia. light sectors of the chopper disc were varied in angular
extent, resulting in different duty cycles. Doubling the
number of light and dark sectors on the chopper disc
MATERIAL AND METHODS provided twice the flicker frequency (from 6 to 12 Hz)
but left the light--dark transition times (6 msec) un-
Animals changed. Only for the 4% duty cycle at 12 Hz, did the
Male white leghorn chickens were obtained from a rotation speed have to be doubled, resulting in only a
local chicken farm (Suppingen, Germany) at day 1 post- 3 msec, rather than a 6 msec transition time. The timing
hatching. All chicks were kept under a 12/12 hr light/dark of the different flicker stimuli used in this study is
cycle at 28°C for the first 10 days and at 22°C thereafter. schematically illustrated in Fig. 1.
They had access to food and water ad libitum. Chickens To compensate for the decrease in the total amount of
were raised and treated according with the ARVO light with decreasing duty cycles (e.g. at 4% duty cycle a
resolution for the use of laboratory animals and treatment one light flash would require 12.5-times as much light
was approved by the University commission for animal energy than a light flash at 50% duty cycle in order to
welfare (AK 2/91). deliver the same energy per cycle), we placed an
adjustable aperture on the optical bench to control the
Housing and light stimulation
intensity of the beam. To provide a similar average
On day 6, groups of 6 to 14 chickens were transferred illuminance in the dome, light was measured by a slowly
from standard cages illuminated by 60 Watt incandescent integrating photometer ( t = 2 . 5 s e c ) and the lamp's
light bulbs to a hemispherical plastic dome (diameter aperture was adjusted until the photometer showed
1 m). The dome was covered inside with highly reflective similar readings for all flicker protocols and for constant
white paint and was used as an integrating sphere to light for all flicker protocols. As a result, the shortest light
provide uniform illumination. A 1 5 0 W xenon-lamp, pulses (4% duty cycle at 12 Hz) with a peak illuminance
mounted on an optical bench, produced a light beam that of 1500 lux and about 150 lux for a 75% duty cycle at
was focused on a rotating chopper disc (diameter 15 cm) 12 Hz and 50% at 6 Hz produced an average illuminance
and was then projected via a convex lens and a mirror level (_+0.2 log units) equivalent to normal (constant)
onto a diffusing plate (5 × 5 cm) that covered a hole cut in illumination.
the top of the dome. The dome was set up in a darkroom
and shielded from the lamp's stray light so that, except
for the flickering light, no light from external sources Treatment protocols
could enter. A small electrical fan equipped with a light The treatment of the different experimental groups is
trap was installed at the dome to ensure sufficient summarized in Table 1. To exclude effects of adaptation
ventilation. to flicker during the treatment periods with occluders or
2664 H.N. SCHWAHNand F. SCHAEFFEL

lenses, all groups except 18 and 21 were exposed to at the same brightness in both the ERG recording sessions
flicker light 1 day before the treatment began. Control and in flicker-rearing. Although 150 W xenon-lamps with
groups 7, 16 and 17 were kept under continuous the same spectral composition were used in both
illumination during a 12 hr day (8 a.m. to 8 p.m.; below experiments, it was difficult to compare the intensity of
referred to as "normal illumination" or "normal light"). a light beam projected onto the eye's cornea in
Groups 1-6, 8-15 and 18 and 20 received flickering light Maxwellian view to the illuminance levels measured in
in a diurnal cycle of 12 hr light/dark. Groups 19 and 21 the dome (since different physical units apply). To bypass
were kept under continuous illumination during the day this problem, we employed the following procedure: a
for 9 hr and were then exposed to flicker light for the photometric photocell (United Detectors Technology w/
remaining 3 hr only (5 p.m. to 8 p.m.) before the light AP-10) acted as an "artificial retina" in an artificial eye of
was turned off. Occluders and lenses ( ___8 D power) were the chicken adjusted to emmetropia (focal length 15 ram,
used as previously described (Schaeffel et al., 1994a). pupil diameter 6 mm, f/# 2.5; lateral stray light in the
Only one eye was treated, whereas the contralateral eye artificial eye was excluded by making its walls from
served as individual control. During the day, occluders black cardboard). (1) If the "artificial chicken eye" was
and lenses were checked hourly for proper attachment moved through the dome (average illuminance level
and cleanness. If occluders or lenses were lost more than about 100 lux, see above), the readings of the photocell
once, the animals were excluded from the study. Previous ranged from 9.0 mV (pointed to ground, from 8 cm
studies in our laboratory showed that a single loss of a above) to 19.0 mV (pointed at illuminated diffuser at the
lens or a occluder of less than 1 hr during the whole ceiling of the dome). We considered this reading as
experiment (5-7 days) did not significantly reduce the equivalent to the amount of light that fell on the chickens'
effect on refractive development. However, we consid- retinas when they were foraging in the dome. (2) If the
ered a repeated loss as a condition of intermittent lens-/ same artificial eye was placed in the stimulation beam of
occluder-wear and expected a reduction in deprivation the ERG recording device and was illuminated in
myopia (Napper et al., 1995) or lens-induced myopia Maxwellian view, as in the respective ERG experiment,
(Schmid & Wildsoet, 1996a). the photocell gave a reading of 8.9 mV. Therefore, it
seems unlikely that the difference in retinal image
Electroretinograms brightness in the ERG-measurements and the experi-
The ERG recording device consisted of an optical ments in the dome would exceed a factor of 2
bench and a computer system which controlled stimula- (0.3 log units).
tion and data sampling. Contact lens electrodes (Henke's
type) specially designed for chickens were used (Medical Measurement of refractive state and ocular dimensions
Workshop, Gronningen, Holland). The recording device At the end of the occluder treatment (after 7 days) or
and procedures are described in detail in a previous paper lens treatment (after 5 days), refractive state and ocular
(Schaeffel et al., 1991). Brightness was controlled by dimensions were determined by automated infrared
neutral density wedges actuated by stepping motors. photoretinoscopy (Schaeffel et al., 1994b) and A-scan
White Ganzfeld stimuli (about 120 deg angular extent in ultrasound (Schaeffel & Howland, 1991), respectively. In
the central visual field of the chicken; Schaeffel et al., some experimental groups (2, 3, 7), corneal radius of
1991) were projected onto the eye of the anesthetized curvature was determined, in addition, by infrared
animal in Maxwellian view. Flickering light of different photokeratometry as previously described (Schaeffel &
duty cycles and frequency was produced by an electronic Howland, 1987).
shutter which was controlled by the computer. ERG-
responses were low-pass filtered (cut-off at 400 Hz, Preparation of tissue samples and measurement of
Bessel function, 10th order) and digitized to 12-bit catecholamines
precision. After flicker stimulus onset, recording was Retinal and vitreal catecholamine levels were deter-
delayed for 10 sec until a stable response amplitude to the mined in groups 18-21 (Table 1) by high pressure liquid
flicker stimulation was attained. Twenty sweeps were chromatography with electrochemical detection (HPLC-
averaged and the absolute maximal voltage difference ECD), as described earlier (Bartmann et al., 1994). On
between peaks and troughs was determined and plotted as the last day of the experiments, after the measurements of
"ERG-modulation" in Fig. 6. Flicker electroretinograms refractions and ocular dimensions, chicks were exposed
[Fig. 6(A)] were recorded from two chickens. Since the to 3-3.5 hr of flickering light. They were then removed
recordings were very consistent and reproducible from the dome one by one, killed by decapitation and
(Schaeffel et al., 1991), data from only one animal are their eyes instantly excised. The eyeballs were cut into
shown. halves in the equatorial plane. The gelatinous vitreous
could be readily removed from the posterior segment by a
Matching stimulus brightness for the ERG to the pair of tweezers. Subsequently, the retina was carefully
brightness in the dome scraped from the posterior eye cup with a blunt hook,
To compare the results of the flicker ERGs to results leaving the retinal pigment epithelium in place. Samples
obtained in the flicker-rearing experiments, it may be of vitreous and retina were frozen in liquid air and stored
very critical that flicker stimulation of the retina occurred at 80°C. Catecholamine content was referenced to wet
MYOPIA AND HYPEROPIA SUPPRESSED BY DIPFERENT FLICKER LIGHT 2665

10 retinal catecholamine levels fluctuate over the year,


experiments with groups 18-21 were done within
1 month. Data in the text are mean values +__1 standard
8 ...............................................* ~ .................................................. deviation (SD). Flicker parameters are expressed as
"frequency/duty cycle", i.e. "12/4" for 12 Hz at a duty
7
cycle of 4%. In the figures, data are given as mean
value___standard error of the mean (SEM), except Fig.
2(A) and Fig. 2(B), where standard deviations are plotted.

p RESULTS
3
Effects of flickering light on the development of the
2 anterior segment of the eye
1 Previous work (Gottlieb & Wallman, 1987; Wildsoet
& Pettigrew, 1988; Vingrys et al., 1991; Squires et al.,
0 1992) has shown that the growth of anterior segment of
4% 12% 50% 75% 25% 50% no the eye is independently controlled from that of the
duty cycle flicker posterior segment. Therefore, we have compared the
FIGURE 2. Refractions of non-occluded fellow eyes of chickens kept corneal radius of curvature in groups 2 and 3, which were
in flickering light for 12 hr a day for 6-8 days. Significance levels refer kept under permanent flicker illumination during the day,
to comparisons with chicks that were kept in normal light during the and corneal curvature in the animals kept in normal light
day (black bar on the right). ( * * P < 0 . 0 1 ; * P < 0 . 0 5 ; Dunnett's
(group 7). Corneas were significantly flatter in the chicks
ANOVA).
raised under flickering light (Table 2). In normal
illumination, occluded eyes had significantly steeper
weight rather than protein content since the vitreous corneas than the open fellow eyes. Steepening of corneas
contained virtually no protein (Ohngemach et al., 1997). in occluded eyes has been reported previously (Gottlieb
Conversion of the retinal content per wet weight to et al., 1987; Hayes et al., 1986). Strikingly, this
content per mg protein can be readily done because difference vanished in animals kept under flickering
retinas in young chicks contain 103 + 8 mg protein per light (Table 2).
1000 mg wet weight (n = 169 eyes, unpublished observa- In all flicker groups, except for group 1 (12/4; Table 3)
tion). flicker-rearing alone had an inhibitory effect on axial
elongation, even if the eyes were not covered by
occluders or lenses (Tables 3 and 4). Interestingly, there
Data analysis and statistics were no significant differences in vitreous chamber depth
To evaluate the effects of flickering light on refractive to the groups kept under normal light (data not shown).
development with occluders or lenses, we used one-way Thus, the differences in axial eye length did not reflect
analysis of variance (ANOVA) after Dunnett. Paired two- changes in vitreous chamber depth but are rather due to
tailed t-tests were employed to assess the significance of flattening of corneas accompanied by shallower anterior
interocular differences in individual chicks, both for chambers. Indeed, except for group 4 (12/75; Table 3), all
ocular dimensions and measurements of biogenic amines. chickens raised in flickering light were more hyperopic in
Unpaired two-tailed t-tests were applied to compare axial their uncovered control eyes than the ones raised in
length and refraction data from different groups. Since normal light (Fig. 2; Tables 3 and 4).

TABLE 2. Mean values + SD of corneal curvature in flicker and non-flicker groups

Corneal radius of curvature (mm) Significance*


Rearing
condition Occluded (O) Control (C) O vs C F/O vs N/O F/C vs N/C F/M vs N/M

12 Hz/12% (F) 3.38 +__0.09 (7) 3.36 + 0.07 (7) n.s. P < 0.001 P < 0.01
12 Hz/50% (F) 3.32 -t-_0.03 (9) 3.31 ± 0.02 (9) n.s. P < 0.001 P < 0.01
mean (M)
3.34 +_ 0.08 (32) P < 0.01

no flicker (N) 3.09 -t- 0.03 (9) 3.18 ± 0.04 (9) P < 0.01
mean (M)
3.13 _ 0.06 (18)
See also Fig. 2.
*Significance levels for two-tailed t-tests. O vs C: occluded eyes vs uncovered fellow eyes, paired t-test. Unpaired t-tests in the following: F/O vs
N/O: occluded eyes, flicker group vs occluded eyes, non-flicker group; F/C vs N/C: uncovered eyes, flicker groups vs uncovered eyes, non-
flicker group; F/M vs N/M: all eyes, flicker group vs all eyes non-flicker group.)
2666 H . N . SCHWAHN and F. SCHAEFFEL

TABLE 3. Mean values + SD of axial length and refractive error of occluded and uncovered eyes of monocularly occluded chicks raised under
flicker of different frequency and duty cycles

Treatment Axial length (ram) Refractive error (D)

Occluder Occluded Control Difference Significance* Occluded Control Difference Significance*

12 Hz/4% 9.60 __+0.55 9.46 _ 0.38 0.15 _+ 0.08 (6) P < 0.01 1,5 _+ 4.9 5.3 _+ 1.9 -3.8 ___ 2.4 (6) P < 0.01
12 Hz/12% 9.01 _+ 0.26 8.87 _ 0,14 0.16 ___ 0.11 (7) P < 0.01 2.2 __+ 5.6 5.5 ___ 2.1 -3.2 ___ 1.4 (7) P < 0.01
12 Hz/50% 9.44 __+ 0.44 9.00 ___ 0.20 0.43 _+ 0.09 (9) P < 0.01 -3.1 _+ 5.4 7.8 _ 3.0 -10.9 ___ 2.1 (9) P < 0.05
12 Hz/75% 9.93 _+ 0.21 9.09 + 0.11 0.84 + 0.08 (6) n.s, -12.3 -t- 2.1 2.9 _ 1.1 -15.2 _+ 1.2 (6) n.s.
6 Hz/25% 9.38 -t- 0.18 9.26 -t- 0.23 0.12 ___ 0.10 (5) P < 0.01 6.0 _ 0.9 6.7 _ 0.8 -0.7 _-4-0.5 (5) P < 0.01
6 Hz/50% 9.53 _+ 0.19 9.29 __. 0.25 0.24 + 0.03 (5) P < 0.01 0.8 + 0.9 4.8 __+0.8 -4.0 _ 0.3 (5) P < 0.01
No flicker 10.06 + 0.28 9.14 -t- 0.50 0.91 + 0,10 (9) -13.4 _ 2.5 3.1 + 1.0 -16.5 -t- 1.1 (9)
Interocular differences are given as mean values _+SEM.
*Significance levels of Dunnett's ANOVA on interocular differences: effect o f occluder deprivation on flicker groups vs occluder effect in the
non-flicker group.

TABLE 4. Mean values _+SD of axial length and refractive error of experimental eyes covered by negative ( - 8 D) or positive (+8 D) lenses and
the uncovered fellow eyes of chicks raised under flicker of different frequency and duty cycles

Treatment Axial length (mm) Refractive error (D)

Negative lenses Experimental Control Difference Significance* Experimental Control Difference Significance*

12 Hz/12% 8.91 _+ 0.07 8.85 _+ 0.10 0.06 _ 0.03 (5) P < 0.01 4.3 _+ 2.1 6.1 _ 1.0 -1.8 _ 0.7 (5) P < 0.05
12 Hz/50% 9.05 + 0.23 8.84 ___ 0.17 0.21 __+0.04 (10) P < 0.01 -0.2 + 3.3 6.4 -t- 1.8 -6.6 ___ 1.1 (10) n.s.
6 Hz/25% 9.02 _+ 0.16 9.04 _+ 0.11 -0.02 + 0.07 (5) P < 0.01 2.7 _+ 1.4 5.2 + 1.4 -2.5 ___ 0.9 (5) P < 0.05
6 Hz/50% 9.06 + 0.07 9,01 + 0.05 0.05 _+ 0.05 (5) P < 0.01 4.6 _+ 5.4 6.6 ___ 2.8 -1.9 + 1.7 (5) P < 0.05
No flicker 9,83 _+ 0.17 9.47 _+ 0.14 0.36 _ 0.02 (5) -4.8 _+ 3.0 3.3 _ 2.4 -8.1 _+ 1.5 (5)

Positive lenses

12Hz/12% 9.00+0.14 8.98__+0,16 0.02+0.07(6) P<0.01 2.9+2.9 6.1 + 1.4 -3.2 _+ 1.2(6) P<0.01
12 Hz/50% 8.70 _+ 0.20 8,82 _ 0.19 -0.11 + 0.04 (8) n.s. 8.5 ± 2.4 6,4 ___ 1.1 2.1 ___ 1.0 (8) P < 0.05
6 Hz/25% 8.77 + 0.13 8.89 _ 0.09 -0.12 + 0.04 (5) n.s. 9.0 + 2.4 6.0 + 2.9 3.1 + 0.8 (5) n.s.
6 Hz/50% 8.87 + 0.13 8.93 _+ 0.08 -0.06 + 0.03 (5) P < 0.05 12.2 + 0.8 8.7 + 2.1 3.5 ___ 1.0 (5) n.s.
No flicker 9.07 _+ 0.09 9.36 _ 0.07 -0.29 + 0.02 (5) 9.4 _ 1.7 3,0 _+ 1.4 6.4 + 0.9 (5)
Interocular differences are given as mean values _+SEM.
*Significance levels for Dunnett's ANOVA on interocular differences: effect of lens-imposed defocus on flicker groups vs lens effect in the non-
flicker group.

•-' 1.0
E ~'-2
£
~0.8
J
"~0.6
.E
~-10
~ 0.4
E ~-12
t~ 0.2
-16

0.0 -18
4% 12% 50% 75% 25% 50% no 4% 12% 50% 75% 25% 50% no
duty cycle flicker duty cycle flicker
FIGURE 3. (A) Interocular differences in axial length of monocularly occluded chicks that were raised under flicker during the
day, with different parameters (gray bars; frequency/duty cycle: 12/4, 12/12, 12/50, 12/75, 6/25, 6/50) and under normal light
during the day (black bar). Significance levels refer to the group raised in normal light (**P < 0.01; Dunnett's ANOVA). (B)
Interocular differences in the refractions of the same animals in (A) (**P < 0.01; *P < 0.05; Dunnett's ANOVA).
MYOPIA AND HYPEROPIA SUPPRESSED BY DIFFERENT FLICKER LIGHT 2667

0.35 B
~' 0.30
E
•~ 0.25
¢1l
I=
.tl 0.20
]
xa 0.'15

-- 0.10
Q

¢ 0.05

~ 0.00
-10
-0.05

-0.10 -12
12% 50% 25% 50% no 12% 50% 25% 50% no
duty cycle flicker duty cycle flicker
FIGURE4. (A) Interoculardifferencesin axial lengthin chicksraisedwithnegativelenses (-8 D) on one eye.Plot as in Fig. 3
(**P < 0.01; Dunnett's ANOVA). (B) Interocular differencesin refractions in the same animals as in (A) (**P < 0.01;
*P < 0.05; Dunnett'sANOVA).

To facilitate the separation of the effects of flicker- eyes (Dunnett, P < 0.01; except 12/50). Axial lengths
raising on deprivation myopia development or lens- were consistently shorter in the flickering light than in
induced refraction changes (i.e., the increase/inhibition of normal light (Dunnett, P < 0.01).
vitreous chamber growth) from the general hyperopic Positive lenses. Positive lens-treated eyes of flicker-
shift in flickering light due to flatter corneas, interocular reared chicks had absolute refractions that were not
differences between experimental and uncovered eyes different from lens-treated eyes of chick raised normal
rather than absolute axial lengths and refractions are light (except 12/12; Table 4). Considering a general
plotted in the subsequent figures. Absolute refractions hyperopic shift that was also found in the untreated
and axial lengths are given in Tables 2, 3 and 4. fellow eyes, flicker indeed reduced the effect of positive
lenses on refractive development [Fig. 5(A, B)]. How-
Effects of flicker light on the development of deprivation ever, in contrast to the negative lens experiments, the
myopia reduction was significant only in the 12 Hz flicker groups.
There was a significant suppressive effect of flickering In 12/12 flickering light, there was even a tendency to
light on deprivation myopia. The occluded eyes of groups reverse the sign of the change in refraction (interocular
raised in flickering light showed less myopic refractions difference: 12/12: -3.2 +_ 1.2D vs normal light:
than the ones raised in normal light (Table 3). This +6.4 _+ 0.9 D; Table 4). As in the negative lens-treated
suppressive effect on the development of deprivation groups, both positive lens-treated and open eyes had
myopia, however, was clearly dependent on the flicker significantly shorter axial lengths than the eyes of chicks
parameters [Fig. 3(A) and Fig. 3(B)]. Note that at both raised in normal light (Dunnett, P < 0.01).
6/25 and 6/50, which are equivalent to light pulse In summary, flickering light suppressed lens-induced
durations of 42 and 83 msec, the suppression of as well as deprivation-induced refractive errors. How-
deprivation myopia was significantly more effective than ever, there may be a tendency that 6 Hz flicker was more
in 12/50 flicker, which has the same light pulse duration effective on negative lens-induced myopia (Fig. 4),
as 6/25, and that the same duty cycle as 6/50. whereas 12Hz flicker seemed more competent in
suppressing the effect of positive lenses (Fig. 5). Note,
Effects offlickering light on the compensation of imposed that flicker of 12/12 was most effective in suppressing the
defocus by spectacle lenses effects on refractive development of both positive and
negative lenses, as well as occluders.
Negative lenses. Under flickering light, negative lenses
were less effective in producing axial myopia than in
normal light [compare gray columns in Fig. 4(A) and Fig. Global retinal activity under various duty cycles
4(B) with black columns). The reduction of the lens- Electroretinograms were recorded in response flicker-
effect was significant in all cases, except 12/50, for both ing light of different duty cycles and frequencies, with the
axial lengths and refractions (Table 4). Superimposed on stimulation intensities matched to retinal image bright-
the suppressive effect of flickering light, there was a ness. For technical reasons, stimulation was not possible
general shift towards more hyperopic refractions in both with exactly the same duty cycles and frequencies as in
2668 H• N. SCHWAHN and F. SCHAEFFEL

0.05
A B
E o.oo
tS -o.os
~D
I=
!l -o.lo
~ -o.15
w
c
--
3
.0.20
:!i!iii!!iiiiiiiiiiiiiiiiiiil;i"
:
= -0.25 iiiiiiiiiiiiii!iiiiiiiiiiiiiiiiiiii

~
' u -0.30

-0.35

-0.40
12% 50% 25% 50% no 12% 50% 25% 50% no
duty cycle flicker duty cycle flicker
FIGURE 5. (A) Interocular differences in axial length in chicks raised with positive lenses (+8 D) in one eye. Plot as in Fig. 3
(**P < 0.01; *P < 0.05; Dunnett's ANOVA). (B) Interocular differences in refractions in the same animals as in (A)
(**P < 0.01; *P < 0.05: Dunnett's ANOVA).

150
.~ ...... ~6t25
A. 140
B ,~ a duration of light stimulus
• duration of dark phase 90
"'l. ,
125 x ~x x flicker duty cycle
• "e" - ~ 1 6 / 5 0 [] __ Ka= 0.8~3 80
f- ,\ ,•
> 120 X X
• i 0
i" \ 70
•=1~.100 • e t~Jt.2 \
i:: r '~100
0
om E 6o ~"
0s
75 ~ 8o
"O
[] o x
0
E ~ 60 40 ~
t~ 50
rr 30
40 =o • •o x o
W
20
25
i l / I ~ X 0
20 x [] o
o° o 10
interpolated
I I i P 0 0
20 40 60 80 100 0 50 100 150

duty cycle [%1 E R G - r n o d u l a t i o n [t~V]

FIGURE 6. (A) Amplitudes in the flicker electroretinogram (ERG) during white Ganzfeld stimulation for different stimulus
frequencies and duty cycles (filled symbols). Since for technical reasons exactly the same stimulus parameters as in the previous
experiments could not be delivered by the ERG recording apparatus, the respective flicker ERG amplitudes were obtained from
the amplitude traces by linear interpolation (open squares). In the boxed sample traces of original ERG responses to flicker
stimulation of 6.5, l0 or 15 Hz are shown for different duty cycles, with the shortest light pulse duration (smallest duty cycle)
the uppermost traces. (B) IfERG data from different flicker frequencies were pooled, the inter-stimulus intervals (filled squares)
remained correlated to the flicker ERG amplitudes but not duty cycles (crosses) or light pulse durations (open squares)•

the occluder and lens experiments. However, the required Comparable ERG amplitudes were generated for a duty
flicker ERG amplitude data could be determined by linear cycle of 25% at 15 Hz and a duty cycle of 65% at 6.5 Hz.
interpolation of the actually measured ERG data [Fig. Which flicker stimulus parameter was the most
6(A)]. Flicker modulation of ERG amplitude increased important factor to determine the amplitude in the
with decreasing duty cycle (shorter light pulses) for all ERG? For that, we plotted duty cycle, light pulse
flicker frequencies tested [Fig. 6(A), see insets]. Given duration and its complement, the duration of the dark
the same duty cycle, ERG modulation was about twice as phase between two light pulses, further denominated as
high at 6.5 Hz stimulation than at 15 Hz stimulation• dark phase (DP), against the ERG amplitude [Fig. 6(B)].
MYOPIA AND HYPEROPIA SUPPRESSED BY DIFFERENT FLICKER LIGHT 2669

TABLE 5. Mean values _ SD of axial length and refractive error of occluded and uncovered fellow eyes of monocularly occluded chicks raised
under flicker of different parameters and different duration (12 and 3 hr)
Treatment Axial length (mrn) Refractive error (D)
Occluder Occluded Control Difference Significance* Occluded Control Difference Significance*

2 Hz/4% 12 hr 9.65 ___0.45 9.50 -t- 0.12 0.15 +__0.07 (7) P < 0.001 -1.9 _+ 3.6 2.6 __ 1.5 -4.5 _ 1.6 (6) P < 0.01
12 Hz/50% 12 hr 9.83 ___0.27 9.43 + 0.18 0.40 _+ 0.06 (6) P < 0.01 -5.2 ___6.9 4.6 ___2.1 -9.8 _+ 2.1 (6) P < 0.05
12 Hz/4% 3 hr 10.36 ___0.42 9.61 + 0.14 0.75 _ 0.15 (7) n.s, -10.3 ___3.0 3.2 +__1.7 -13.5 ___ 1.3 (7) n.s.
12 Hz/50% 3 hr 10.05 ___0.37 9.42 ___0.14 0.63 + 0.08 (6) P < 0.05 -9.4 _ 2.9 2.8 + 1.6 -12.2 ___ 1.2 (7) n.s.
no flicker 10.06 _ 0.28 9.14 __ 0.50 0.91 + 0.10 (9) -13.4 ___2.5 3.1 + 1.0 -16.5 __+0.9 (9)
Interocular differences are given as mean values _ SEM.
*Significance levels of Dunnett's ANOVA on interocular differences: effect of occluder deprivation on flicker groups vs occluder effect in the
non-flicker group.

1.0

0.9

E 0.8 ,a" , - 2
£ ¢0
•~ 0.7 =..4
¢11
_e 0.6 -6

N 0.5
tl .E
t=
-o- 0.4 o-10
u
o g[
¢=
0.3
~ -12

~0.2 "1:;-14

0.1 -16

0.0 -18
4% 50% 4% 50% no 4% 50% 4% 50% no
d u t y cycle flicker d u t y cycle flicker
FIGURE 7. (A) lnterocular differences in axial eye length of monocularly deprived chicks raised under different flicker
treatment: 12/4 and 12/50 flicker, either 12 hr a day (light bars) or for only 3 hr with normal illumination for the remaining 9 hr
(dark bars). Significance levels refer to comparison of the flicker groups with the group raised in normal light during the day
(**P < 0.01; *P < 0.05; Durmett's ANOVA). (B) Interocular differences in refractions in the same animals as in (A). Only
12 hr of flicker could inhibit the effect of occluders on refractive development (compared with the respective differences found
in normal illumination (black bar) **P< 0.01; *P < 0.05; Dunnett's ANOVA).

F o r p o o l e d E R G d a t a f r o m all flicker frequencies, there d e v e l o p e d , to a slightly lesser extent at 12/50 (signifi-


was a high correlation o f the E R G a m p l i t u d e to the D P cance level: P < 0.05).
(R = 0 . 9 4 8 , n = 2 5 , P < 0 . 0 0 1 ) but not to the other Retinal d o p a m i n e content was d e c r e a s e d in the
parameters. o c c l u d e d e y e s o f the groups raised in n o r m a l i l l u m i n a -
tion, as well as in the o c c l u d e d e y e s o f the groups that
e x p e r i e n c e d flicker light o f different duration (12 hr/3 hr)
Dopamine release during flicker stimulation [Fig. 8(B)]. B e c a u s e the d e v e l o p m e n t o f d e p r i v a t i o n
T o study the r e l a t i o n s h i p b e t w e e n flicker, retinal m y o p i a m a y be influenced b y the release o f d o p a m i n e
d o p a m i n e release, and rescue f r o m refractive errors, w e into the interstitial space (as m e a s u r e d in the vitreous)
used t w o p a r a d i g m s : flicker for 12 hr p e r d a y and for only rather than the overall retinal d o p a m i n e content (Ohnge-
3 hr a d a y (see M e t h o d s ) . W e r e p e a t e d e x p e r i m e n t s with m a c h et al., 1997), w e d e t e r m i n e d also the vitreal
t w o flicker parameters, 12/50 and 12/4, that s u p p r e s s e d d o p a m i n e levels. E x c e p t for 12/4 in the 3 hr flicker
d e p r i v a t i o n m y o p i a to different degrees. A s f o u n d in the r e g i m e n there was a d e c r e a s e in d o p a m i n e release [Fig.
p r e v i o u s e x p e r i m e n t s (groups 1, 3; Fig. 3), d e p r i v a t i o n 8(A)]. A p p a r e n t l y , d e p r i v a t i o n m y o p i a c o u l d b e sup-
m y o p i a was s u p p r e s s e d b y 12 hr flicker p e r d a y with 12/4 p r e s s e d b y 12/4 flickering light (12 hr a d a y ) without
and 12/50 [Table 5; Fig. 7 ( A ) and (B)]. H o w e v e r , in p r e v e n t i n g the d e c l i n e in retinal d o p a m i n e content and
groups 19 and 21, w h i c h w e r e treated with flickering light d o p a m i n e release, M o r e o v e r , in the groups treated with
for o n l y 3 hr at the e n d o f the day, d e p r i v a t i o n m y o p i a 3 hr o f 12/4 flicker a d a y there was no significant d r o p in
2670 H.N. SCHWAHN and F. SCHAEFFEL

100%

80%
A .
• 300h

¢11 60% | non-occluded fellow eye i


5.5 o~ 200A
40% • 3.5 - .¢_
I
10%
20%
i 12hours | " l ~
0% 0% .............. , , ! i i
70 . "s6 • II 90 i
-20% 7O
-10%

-40% a

0
~ -20%
-60% " "1~ ,#i;'~!Fi'0~i
-30%
-80%
vitreous retina
- 100% - - -40%
4% 50% 4% 50% no 4% 50% 4% 50% no
duty cycle flicker duty cycle flicker

FIGURE 8. (A) Vitreal dopamine content in monocularly deprived chicks raised under different flickering lights (12/4 or 12/50
flicker, applied either for 12 or 3 hr a day). Changes in vitreal dopamine content are expressed as percentages of the vitreal
content in the non-occluded fellow eyes. Boxed values represent the absolute content of dopamine [ng/1 g wet weight]: upper
value: occluded eye; lower value: non-occluded fellow eye. Significance levels refer to the effect of occluder deprivation on the
experimental eyes vs contralateral control eyes (*P < 0.05, paired t-test). (B) Retinal dopamine contents. Plots as in (A)
(*P < 0.05, paired t-test).

retinal dopamine content [Fig. 8(B)], despite the fact that vision at a distance. If the image is defocused or blurred
deprivation myopia developed (Fig. 7). In fact, there was over a certain coherent time period, a "blur detector"
a considerable increase in vitreal dopamine content in the within the retina might trigger increased growth in the
occluded eye in the 12/4 group (P < 0.01), whereas in the sclera in case of occluder deprivation, as well as under
12/50 (3 hr) flicker group, as in all other groups, vitreal negative lens treatment, as suggested by Diether &
dopamine was decreased in the occluded eye [Fig. 8(A)]. Schaeffel (1997). A quite trivial explanation could then
account for the suppression of experimental refractive
DISCUSSION errors in flicker: with short light pulses there might be
less integration time for a presumed retinal image
We have found that the development of both depriva- processor ("blur detector") to detect changes in image
tion-induced and defocus-induced refractive errors can be quality and shifts in the focal plane. Since flicker of 6/25
suppressed by flickering light. Flicker-rearing also
has the same exposure time per light pulse as 12/50 thus,
caused a hyperopic shift, which was mainly due to
one could expect similar suppression of refractive errors
corneal flattening, even in uncovered eyes. However, this
under both flicker conditions. However, in contrast to
effect did not account for the suppression of deprivation
lens-induced hyperopia, where 6/25 and 12/50 flicker
myopia and lens-induced myopia but was rather super-
indeed lead to a similar reduction, 6/25 flicker was
imposed upon all refractive changes observed in this
study. In order to compare effects of flicker on the significantly more competent in reducing both lens-
outcome of deprivation myopia and lens-induced myopia induced and deprivation myopia treatment than 12/50
or hyperopia, we calculated a measure of "suppression of (both P < 0.01, t-test). Hence, light pulse duration does
refractive errors" which was given as the ratio of not seem to be the major critical parameter for the
experimentally induced refractive errors (interocular suppression of myopia.
differences) in normal light (N) to those in flicker (F): What else might be the nature of this blur detector?
suppression = (1 - F ) . 100%. (Negative percentages re- Since most retinal ganglion cells show transient
fer to an enhancement of refractive errors and suppres- responses to light stimuli, the retina is considered as a
sion by more than 100% represents a reversal of the sign sensory organ that is best adapted to detect changes in the
of the induced refractive error.) visual environment, Thus, no neural activity will be
generated if the retinal image does not change, which is
What parameters of flicker light might account for the the case with a constantly blurred image caused by
suppression of refractive errors? negative lenses or occluders. With clear vision, retinal
In a normal visual environment (with a wide variety of activity returns to normal levels and inhibits exaggerated
viewing distances) hyperopic eyes without accommoda- scleral growth (Gottlieb & Wallman, 1987; Wallman,
tion experience a blurred image all the time, whereas in 1990). In other words, the "signal" within the retina that
myopic eyes the retinal image is defocused only for finally leads to myopia might just be reduced retinal
MYOPIA AND HYPEROPIA SUPPRESSED BY DIFFERENT FLICKER LIGHT 2671

200%

A Ill : ,i

,o0% ................................. .............................. ,00%

J
¢ o i
0% e, • • , • = 0% -- i & "1 J ~ o, ,
A &
A O A o
0 0

-50% -50%
0 25 50 75 100 125 150 0 20 40 60 80 100 120 140 160
dark phase duration [m~c] ERG-Modulation [pV]
FIGURE 9. Suppression of both deprivation- and lens-induced myopia correlates with the flicker ERG amplitudes. In contrast,
suppression of lens-induced hyperopia is not correlated with the ERG, suggesting different retinal processing for myopia and
hyperopia. Suppression of refractive errors due to flicker: (1 - ~) • 100% (F refractive errors (interocular differences) in flicker,
N under normal light). Significance levels of correlation for deprivation myopia (n = 38): r = 0.709, P < 0.001; for lens-induced
myopia (n = 25): r = 0.512, P < 0.01; for lens-induced hyperopia (n -- 24): r = 0.162, n.s.)

activity. Considering the result shown in Fig. 6(B), where focal plane, whereas with negative lenses scleral growth
the amplitude of flicker ERG modulation, that serves as a is stimulated. Our data suggest that the separation of both
crude measure of overall retinal activity, correlates with processes occurs at a very early stage, i.e. at different
the dark phase duration of flickering light but not with neuronal circuits in the retina, and that both circuits may
light pulse duration, we found it appropriate to look for release different factors that control choroid or sclera,
correlation of this parameter to the flicker-effects. respectively. The experiments with reserpine and apo-
Indeed, we found correlation of the suppression of both morphine suggest that only the mechanism that controls
deprivation myopia and negative lens-induced myopia scleral growth in deprivation and lens-induced myopia
with dark phase duration and therefore with the involves dopamine (Schaeffel et al., 1995; Schmid et al.,
interpolated flicker ERG amplitudes of the corresponding 1997).
flicker parameters (Fig. 9A). In contrast, the suppression Unfortunately, we can only speculate about the cellular
of positive lens-induced hyperopia does not correlate origin of the flicker ERG responses. In the single-flash
with the dark phase duration and flicker ERG amplitude. ERG the amplitude of the positive going b-wave of the
This suggests that "deprivation" and "hyperopic defocus" light ON-response is believed to reflect retinal activity
might trigger similar retinal processes, supporting the postsynaptic to the photoreceptors: a change in the
"retinal activity model" of myopia. membrane potential of the MUller cells, as a consequence
of depolarizing bipolar cells which extrude potassium
Possible retinal mechanisms for deprivation myopia and into the extracellular space (Dick & Miller, 1978). In the
lens-induced myopia or hyperopia single-flash ERG, the amplitude of the b-wave decreased
while the amplitude of the d-wave, which is the light-
Although we still do not know how the sign of defocus
OFF response, increased with increasing flash duration
is detected, we do know that there are probably two
(Sieving, 1993). In recent models (Bush & Sieving, 1996)
different retinal circuits. The data agree with earlier
the superimposition of depolarizing and hyperpolarizing
findings that reserpine, a dopamine uptake blocker,
activity (resulting in b-wave and parts of the d-wave) also
suppressed negative lens-induced and deprivation myopia
resembles the amplitude of the periodic flicker ERG. We
but not positive lens-induced hyperopia (Schaeffel et al.,
must consider that this sum electrical activity, as
1995). The two different circuits may, therefore, have a
measured on the cornea, is determined mostly by retinal
different accessibility to reserpine treatment, but may also
cells reaching perpendicular to the retinal surface (i.e.
have a different pharmacology. Apomorphine, a non-
bipolar cells, photoreceptors and Mtlller cells); cells that
selective dopamine agonist, which suppressed deprivation
extend horizontally within the retinal plane, like the
myopia (Rohrer et al., 1993) also inhibited lens-induced
amacrine cells, do not contribute to the ERG.
myopia, but not lens-induced hyperopia (Schmid et al.,
1997). Wallman et al. (1995) have shown that the target
tissues for compensation of positive and negative lenses Frequency tuning of the suppression of deprivation
are also different: with positive lenses, the choroid myopia and lens-induced myopia
increases in thickness to move the retina towards the We did not find significant differences in the suppres-
2672 H.N. SCHWAHN and F. SCHAEFFEL

sion of deprivation myopia and negative lens-induced CONCLUSIONS


myopia with the flicker frequencies tested (6 and 12 Hz). How the sign of imposed defocus is detected by local
Schmid & Wildsoet (1996b) reported slightly different retinal circuits is still not clear. Because the refractive
suppression functions using stroboscopic flicker at 10, 15 errors induced by positive and negative lenses are
and 20 Hz. In their experiments, deprivation myopia suppressed most effectively by different flicker para-
declined linearly with increasing frequency but lens- meters, we propose that two different neural circuits are
induced myopia was significantly suppressed only at involved at the retinal level. Deprivation myopia and
20 Hz. The latter result is different from ours, since we negative lens-induced myopia both are suppressed by
obtained suppression already at 6 Hz. It is possible that flicker with a similar dark phase duration (DP). The
our experimental conditions are not comparable, since suppression of myopia correlates with the DP and flicker
Schmid & Wildsoet (1996b) added the flickering light to ERG amplitude. Suppression of hyperopia does not
an ambient background illumination of 105 lux. ERG correlate with DP or flicker ERG amplitude. Taken
recordings under these conditions are not available. They together, the finding that reserpine did not prevent
would have been most useful to determine whether there compensation of positive lenses and choroidal thicken-
was a similar correlation of "retinal activity" to ing, and our observations on flicker-reared chicks we
suppression of myopia, as in our study. suggest that the development of myopia and hyperopia
probably utilizes different retinal circuits with different
pharmacology.
Role o f dopamine
Flickering light releases dopamine from retinal sources.
REFERENCES
This was measured in various species (Kramer, 1971;
Kirsch & Wagner, 1989), but this has not been Bartmann, M. & Schaeffel, F. (1994). A simple mechanism for
emmetropizationwithoutcues from accommodationor color. Vision
demonstrated previously in chick. Rohrer et al. (1995) Research, 34, 873-876.
hypothesized that deprivation myopia might be sup- Bartmann, M., Schaeffel,F., Hagel, G. & Zrenner, E. (1994). Constant
pressed by flicker, because intermittent illumination at light affects retinal dopamine levels and blocks deprivationmyopia
certain frequencies may counteract the drop in dopamine but not lens-inducedretractiveerrors. Visual Neuroscience, 11, 199-
208.
release which occurs normally during deprivation and
Boelen, M. K., Boelen, M. G. & Marshak, D. W. (1996). Light-
lens treatment (Ohngemach et al., 1997). However, our stimulated release of dopaminefrom the primateretina is blockedby
measurements show that retinal dopamine content APB. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science (suppl.), 37,
(intracellular DA stores) does not necessarily correlate 951.
with the amount of deprivation myopia [Fig. 7, Fig. 8(B)]. Bush, R. A. & Sieving, P. A. (1996). Inner retinal contributionsto the
primate photopic fast flicker electroretinogram. Journal of the
The occluded eyes of the group treated with only 3 hr of Optical Society of America A, 13, 557-565.
flicker per day showed no decrease in retinal dopamine Dick, E. & Miller, R. F. (1978). Light evoked potassium activity in
but still developed deprivation myopia. Even with the mudpuppy: its relationship to the b-wave of the electroretinogram.
most severe decrease of retinal dopamine from occlusion, Brain Research, 154, 388-394.
Diether, S. & Schaeffel, F. (1997). Local changes in eye growth
no myopia developed in the 12/4 12 hr flicker group. If the induced by imposed local refractive error despite active
release of dopamine from the retina, rather than retinal accommodation. Vision Research, 37, 659-668.
content were important for eye growth control (Ohnge- Dong, C. J. & McReynolds,J. S. (1992). Comparisonof the effects of
mach et al., 1997), we would expect that vitreal content is flickering light and steady light on dopamine release and horizontal
cell coupling in the mudpuppy retina. Journal of Neurophysiology,
increased under flickering light in the occluded eyes when 67, 364-372.
deprivation myopia is suppressed. An increase in Dubocovich, M. L. & Hensler, J. G. (1986). Modulation of [3H]-
dopamine release was only found in the 12/4 3 hr flicker dopamine released by different frequencies of stimulation from
group [Fig. 8(A)]. In this group, however, deprivation rabbit retina. British Journal of Pharmacology, 88, 51--61.
Gottlieb, M. D., Fugate-Wentzek, L. A. & Wallman, J. (1987).
myopia developed. Taken together, the available data still Different visual deprivations produce different ametropias and
reveal no clear picture of how dopamine is involved in different eye shapes. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual
experimentally induced myopia. Probably signals carried Science, 28, 1225-1235.
by other retinal neurotransmitters are also important (e.g. Gottlieb, M. D. & Wallman, J. (1987). Retinal activity modulates eye
growth: evidence from rearing in stroboscopicillumination. Society
Seltner & Stell, 1995; Seko et al., 1995).
of Neuroscience Abstracts, 13, 1297.
Rohrer et al. (1993) found that intravitreal injection of Hayes, B. P., Fitzke, F. W., Hodos, W. & Holden, A. L. (1986). A
apomorphine, a non-selective dopamine agonist, was morphological analysis of experimental myopia in young chickens.
competent to suppress deprivation myopia in chickens. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 27, 981-991.
The effect of apomorphine was blocked by spiperone, an Irving, E. L., Sivak, J. G. & Callender, M. G. (1992). Refractive
plasticity of the developing chick eye. Ophthalmic and Physiolo-
antagonist of D2-receptors. If the "dopamine release"- gical Optics, 12, 448-456.
hypothesis of flicker was correct, our observations Kirsch, M. & Wagner, H.-J. (1989). Release pattern of endogenous
suggest further experiments with flicker together with dopamine in teleost retinae during light adaptation and pharmaco-
simultaneous administration of a D2-antagonist, whereby logical stimulation. Vision Research, 29, 147-154.
Kramer, S. G. (1971). Dopamine: a retinal neurotransmitter.I. Retinal
the rescuing effect of flickering light on myopia should be uptake, storage, and light-stimulated release of H3-dopamine in
reduced or blocked. vivo. Investigative Ophthalmology, 10, 438--452.
MYOPIA AND HYPEROPIA SUPPRESSED BY DIFFERENT FLICKER LIGHT 2673

Napper, A. N., Brennan, N. A., Barrington, M., Squires, M. A., Vessey, Seltner, R. L. & Stell, W. K. (1995). The effect of vasoactive intestinal
G. A. & Vingrys, A. J. (1995). The duration of normal visual peptide on development of form deprivation myopia in the chick: a
exposure necessary to prevent form deprivation myopia in chicks. pharmacological and immunocytochemical study. Vision Research,
Vision Research, 35, 1337-1344. 35, 1265-1270.
Ohngemach, S., Hagel, G. & Schaeffel, F. (1997). Concentrations of Sieving, P. A. (1993). Photopic ON- and OFF-pathway abnormalities
biogenic amines in fundal layers in chickens with normal visual in retinal dystrophies. Transactions of the American Ophthalmolo-
experience, deprivation and after reserpine application. Visual gical Society, 91, 701-773.
Neuroscience, 14, 493-507. Squires, M. A., Brennan, N. A., Napper, G. A., Vessey, G. A.,
Rohrer, B., Iuvone, P. M. & Stell, W. K. (1995). Stimulation of Barrington, M. & Vingrys, A. J. (1992). An association between
dopaminergic amacrine cells by stroboscopic illumination or emmetropization and anterior segment structure. Investigative
fibroblast growth factor (bFGF, FGF-2) injections: possible roles Ophthalmology and Visual Science (suppl.), 33, 97.
in prevention of form-deprivation myopia in the chick. Brain Stone, R. A., Lin, T., Laties, A. M. & Iuvone, P. M. (1989). Retinal
Research, 686, 169-181. dopamine and form-deprivation myopia. Proceedings of the
Rohrer, B., Schaeffel, F. & Zrenner, E. (1992). Longitudinal chromatic National Academy of Sciences USA, 86, 704-706.
aberration and emmetropization: results from the chicken eye. Troilo, D., Gottlieb, M. D. & Wallman, J. (1987). Visual deprivation
Journal of Physiology, 449, 363-376. causes myopia in chicks with opitc nerve section. Current Eye
Rohrer, B., Spira, A. W. & Stell, W. K. (1993). Apomorphine blocks Research, 6, 993-999.
form-deprivation myopia in chickens by a dopamine D2-receptor Troilo, D. & Wallman, J. (1991). The regulation of eye growth and
mechanism acting in retina or pigmented epithelium. Visual refractive state: an experimental study of emmetropization. Vision
Neuroscience, 10, 447-453. Research, 31, 1237-1250.
Schaeffel, F., Bartmarm, M., Hagel, G. & Zrenner, E. (1995). Studies Vingrys, A. J., Squires, M. A., Napper, G. A., Barrington, M., Vessey,
on the role of the retinal dopamine/melatonin system in experi- G. A. & Brennan, N. A. (1991). Prevention of form deprivation
mental refractive errors in chickens. Vision Research, 35, 1247- myopia in post-hatch chickens. Investigative Ophthalmology and
1264. Visual Science (suppl.), 32, 1203.
Schaeffel, F., Glasser, A. & Howland, H. C. (1988). Accommodation, Wallman, J. (1990). Retinal influences on sclera underlie visual
refractive error and eye growth in chickens. Vision Research, 28, deprivation myopia. In Bock, G. R. & Widdows, E. (Eds), Myopia
639-657. and the control of eye growth. Ciba Foundation Symposium (Vol.
Schaeffel, F. & Howland, H. C. (1991). Properties of the feedback 155, pp. 126-141). Chichester, U.K.: Wiley and Sons.
loops controlling eye growth and refractive state in the chicken. Wallman, J., Turkel, J. & Trachtman, J. (1978). Extreme myopia
Vision Research, 31,717-734. produced by modest changes in early visual experience. Science,
Schaeffel, F. & Howland, H. C. (1987). Corneal accommodation in 201, 1249-1251.
chick and pigeon. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 160, 375- Wallman, J., Wildsoet, C., Xu, A., Gottlieb, M. D., Nickla, D. L.,
384. Marran, L., Krebs, W. & Christensen, A. M. (1995). Moving the
Schaeffel, F., Rohrer, B., Lemrner, T. & Zrenner, E. (1991). Diurnal retina: choroidal modulation of refractive state. Vision Research, 35,
control of rod function in the chicken. Visual Neuroscience, 6, 641- 37-50.
653. Wildsoet, C. F. & Wallman, J. (1995). Choroidal and scleral
Schaeffel, F., Troilo, D., Wallman, J. & Howland, H. C. (1990). mechanisms of compensation for spectacle lenses in chicks. Vision
Developing eyes that lack accommodation grow to compensate for Research, 35, 1175-1195.
imposed defocus. Visual Neuroscience, 4, 177-183. Wildsoet, C. F., Howland, H. C., Falconer, S. & Dick, K. (1993).
Schmid, K. L., Nevin, S. T. & Wildsoet, C. F. (1997). Apomorphine Chromatic aberration and accommodation: their role in emmetro-
inhibits lens-induced myopia but not lens-induced hyperopia in pization in the chick. Vision Research, 33, 1593-1603.
chick. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science (suppl.), 38, Wildsoet, C. F. & Pettigrew, J. D. (1988). Kainic acid-induced eye
$758. enlargement in chickens: differential effects on anterior and
Schwahn, H. N. & Schaeffel, F. (1994). Chick eyes under cycloplegia posterior segments. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual
compensate for spectacle lenses despite six-hydroxy dopamine Science, 29, 311-319.
treatment. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 35,
3516-3524.
Seko, Y., Shimokawa, H., Shimizu, M. & Tokoro, T. (1995). In vivo Acknowledgements~This study was supported by the German
and in vitro association of retinoic acid with form-deprivation Research Council (SFB 307, TP A7). We thank Dr. Norbert Benda
myopia in chicks. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science from the Institute of Medical Biometry, Tiibingen, for advice on the
(suppl.), 36, 759. statistics and Prof. Dr. E. Zrenner for general support.

You might also like