Download as docx, pdf, or txt
Download as docx, pdf, or txt
You are on page 1of 4

Quasi-Experimental Design | Definition, Types & Examples

Published on July 31, 2020 by Lauren Thomas. Revised on January 22, 2024.
Like a true experiment, a quasi-experimental design aims to establish a cause-and-effect relationship
between an independent and dependent variable.
However, unlike a true experiment, a quasi-experiment does not rely on random assignment. Instead,
subjects are assigned to groups based on non-random criteria.
Quasi-experimental design is a useful tool in situations where true experiments cannot be used for ethical
or practical reasons.
Differences between quasi-experiments and true experiments
There are several common differences between true and quasi-experimental designs.

True experimental design Quasi-experimental design

Assignment to The researcher randomly assigns subjects to Some other, non-random method is used to
treatment control and treatment groups. assign subjects to groups.

Control over treatment The researcher usually designs the treatment. The researcher often does not have control ove
the treatment, but instead studies pre-existing
groups that received different treatments after th
fact.

Use of control groups Requires the use of control and treatment Control groups are not required (although they
groups. are commonly used).

Example of a true experiment vs a quasi-experiment


Let’s say you are interested in the impact of a new psychological therapy on patients with depression.
Example: True experimental design to run a true experiment, you randomly assign half the patients in a
mental health clinic to receive the new treatment. The other half—the control group—receives the
standard course of treatment for depression.
Every few months, patients fill out a sheet describing their symptoms to see if the new treatment produces
significantly better (or worse) effects than the standard one.
However, for ethical reasons, the directors of the mental health clinic may not give you permission to
randomly assign their patients to treatments. In this case, you cannot run a true experiment.
Instead, you can use a quasi-experimental design.
Example: Quasi-experimental design You discover that a few of the psychotherapists in the clinic have
decided to try out the new therapy, while others who treat similar patients have chosen to stick with the
normal protocol.
You can use these pre-existing groups to study the symptom progression of the patients treated with the
new therapy versus those receiving the standard course of treatment.
Although the groups were not randomly assigned, if you properly account for any systematic differences
between them, you can be reasonably confident any differences must arise from the treatment and not
other confounding variables.
Prevent plagiarism. Run a free check.
Types of quasi-experimental designs
Many types of quasi-experimental designs exist. Here we explain three of the most common types:
nonequivalent groups design, regression discontinuity, and natural experiments.
Nonequivalent groups design
In nonequivalent group design, the researcher chooses existing groups that appear similar, but where only
one of the groups experiences the treatment.
In a true experiment with random assignment, the control and treatment groups are considered equivalent
in every way other than the treatment. But in a quasi-experiment where the groups are not random, they
may differ in other ways—they are nonequivalent groups.
When using this kind of design, researchers try to account for any confounding variables by controlling
for them in their analysis or by choosing groups that are as similar as possible.
This is the most common type of quasi-experimental design.
Example: Nonequivalent groups designYou hypothesize that a new after-school program will lead to
higher grades. You choose two similar groups of children who attend different schools, one of which
implements the new program while the other does not.
By comparing the children who attend the program with those who do not, you can find out whether it has
an impact on grades.
Regression discontinuity
Many potential treatments that researchers wish to study are designed around an essentially arbitrary
cutoff, where those above the threshold receive the treatment and those below it do not.
Near this threshold, the differences between the two groups are often so minimal as to be nearly
nonexistent. Therefore, researchers can use individuals just below the threshold as a control group and
those just above as a treatment group.
Example: Regression discontinuity Some high schools in the United States are set aside for high-
achieving students, who must exceed a certain score on a test to be allowed to attend. Those who pass this
test most likely differ systematically from those who do not.
However, since the exact cutoff score is arbitrary, the students near the threshold—those who just barely
pass the exam and those who fail by a very small margin—tend to be very similar, with the small
differences in their scores mostly due to random chance. You can therefore conclude that any outcome
differences must come from the school they attended.
To test the impact of attending a selective school, you can study the long-term outcomes of these two
groups of students (those who barely passed and those who barely failed).
Natural experiments
In both laboratory and field experiments, researchers normally control which group the subjects are
assigned to. In a natural experiment, an external event or situation (“nature”) result in the random or
random-like assignment of subjects to the treatment group.
Even though some use random assignments, natural experiments are not considered to be true
experiments because they are observational in nature.
Although the researchers have no control over the independent variable, they can exploit this event after
the fact to study the effect of the treatment.
Example: Natural experiment The Oregon Health Study is one of the most famous natural experiments. In
2008, the state of Oregon decided to expand enrollment in Medicaid, America’s low-income public health
insurance program, to more low-income adults.
However, as they could not afford to cover everyone who they deemed eligible for the program, they
instead allocated spots in the program based on a random lottery.
Researchers were able to study the impact of the program by using the enrolled individuals as a randomly
assigned treatment group, and the others who were eligible but did not succeed in the lottery as a control
group.
When to use quasi-experimental design
Although true experiments have higher internal validity, you might choose to use a quasi-experimental
design for ethical or practical reasons.
Ethical
Sometimes it would be unethical to provide or withhold a treatment on a random basis, so a true
experiment is not feasible. In this case, a quasi-experiment can allow you to study the
same causal relationship without the ethical issues.
The Oregon Health Study is a good example. It would be unethical to randomly provide some people with
health insurance but purposely prevent others from receiving it solely for the purposes of research.
However, since the Oregon government faced financial constraints and decided to provide health
insurance via lottery, studying this event after the fact is a much more ethical approach to studying the
same problem.
Practical
True experimental design may be infeasible to implement or simply too expensive, particularly for
researchers without access to large funding streams.
At other times, too much work is involved in recruiting and properly designing an experimental
intervention for an adequate number of subjects to justify a true experiment.
In either case, quasi-experimental designs allow you to study the question by taking advantage of data
that has previously been paid for or collected by others (often the government).
Advantages and disadvantages
Quasi-experimental designs have various pros and cons compared to other types of studies.
 Higher external validity than most true experiments, because they often involve real-world
interventions instead of artificial laboratory settings.
 Higher internal validity than other non-experimental types of research, because they allow you to
better control for confounding variables than other types of studies do.
 Lower internal validity than true experiments—without randomization, it can be difficult to verify
that all confounding variables have been accounted for.
 The use of retrospective data that has already been collected for other purposes can be inaccurate,
incomplete or difficult to access.

You might also like