Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 36

HHS Public Access

Author manuscript
Hear Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2019 June 01.
Author Manuscript

Published in final edited form as:


Hear Res. 2018 June ; 363: 119–135. doi:10.1016/j.heares.2018.03.013.

Addressing variability in the acoustic startle reflex for accurate


gap detection assessment
Ryan J. Longeneckera, Inga Kristaponytea,b, Gregg L. Nelsona, Jesse W. Younga, and
Alexander V. Galazyuka
aNortheast Ohio Medical University, Department of Anatomy and Neurobiology, Rootstown, Ohio,
USA
Author Manuscript

bBiomedical Sciences Program, Kent State University, Kent, OH, USA

Abstract
The acoustic startle reflex (ASR) is subject to substantial variability. This inherent variability
consequently shapes the conclusions drawn from gap-induced prepulse inhibition of the acoustic
startle reflex (GPIAS) assessments. Recent studies have cast doubt as to the efficacy of this
methodology as it pertains to tinnitus assessment, partially, due to variability in and between data
sets. The goal of this study was to examine the variance associated with several common data
collection variables and data analyses with the aim to improve GPIAS reliability. To study this the
GPIAS tests were conducted in adult male and female CBA/CaJ mice. Factors such as inter-trial
interval, circadian rhythm, sex differences, and sensory adaptation were each evaluated. We then
examined various data analysis factors which influence GPIAS assessment. Gap-induced
Author Manuscript

facilitation, data processing options, and assessments of tinnitus were studied. We found that the
startle reflex is highly variable in CBA/CaJ mice, but that this can be minimized by certain data
collection factors. We also found that careful consideration of temporal fluctuations of the ASR
and controlling for facilitation can lead to drastically divergent GPIAS results. Finally, we show
that tinnitus decreases gap-induced inhibition of the acoustic startle reflex, but only for short gap
durations. This study provides a guide for reducing variance in the GPIAS methodology – thereby
improving the diagnostic power of the test.

Keywords
Gap-induced prepulse inhibition of the acoustic startle reflex; circadian rhythm; acoustic startle
response magnitude; prepulse facilitation; tinnitus
Author Manuscript

Correspondence to: Alexander Galazyuk, Department of Anatomy and Neurobiology, Northeast Ohio Medical University, 4209 State
Route 44, Rootstown, OH 44272, USA. Telephone: (330)325-6640; agalaz@neomed.edu.
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our
customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of
the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be
discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
Conflict of interest: The authors declare this research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that
could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Longenecker et al. Page 2

1. Introduction
Author Manuscript

A reliable animal model of tinnitus is a prerequisite for tinnitus related therapies. Due to its
relatively minor time commitment gap-induced prepulse inhibition of the acoustic startle
reflex (GPIAS) has quickly become one of the predominant behavioral assessments for
tinnitus (Turner et al., 2006) in several animal models (see Eggermont, 2013; Hayes et al.,
2014; Galazyuk and Hébert, 2015). Many groups have used GPIAS to behaviorally assess
tinnitus in rats (Turner et al., 2006; Lobarinas et al., 2013; Singer et al., 2013; Ropp et al.,
2014), mice (Longenecker and Galazyuk, 2011, Middleton et al., 2011; Hickox & Liberman,
2014; Lowe and Walton, 2015; Yu et al., 2016), guinea pigs (Dehmel et al., 2012; Berger et
al., 2013), and hamsters (Salloum et al., 2016). However, some uncertainty of this method is
rooted in a lack of consistency of methodologies and data assessment strategies across labs
(Galazyuk and Hébert, 2015). While the acoustic startle reflex (Landis and Hunt, 1939;
Fleshler, 1965), prepulse inhibition (Hoffman and Searle, 1965; Ison and Hammond, 1971;
Author Manuscript

Carlson and Willott, 1996; Swerdlow et al., 2001), and gap-induced prepulse inhibition
(Ison, 1982; Walton et al., 1997) have been studied for decades, many of the specifics of
stimuli, hardware, technical, and analytic information related to tinnitus detection have not
yet been solidified. For this reason, we have previously addressed some hardware and
stimulus presentation issues (Longenecker and Galazyuk, 2012), as well an in-depth analysis
of the startle response (Grimsley et al., 2015). Although these initial steps have improved the
confidence of GPIAS assessments, the finer details concerning data collection and data
analysis, as they specifically relate to tinnitus assessment, need further attention.

GPIAS studies have largely neglected to provide details on data collection when assessing
tinnitus in laboratory animals. However, many aspects of these approaches can dramatically
affect conclusions of GPIAS experiments. This is especially true for animals that have high
Author Manuscript

startle reflex variability (Berger et al., 2013; Longenecker and Galazyuk, 2016; Salloum et
al., 2016). Limiting ASR variability is critical in a repeated measure designs which compare
an animal’s performance before and after a tinnitus-inducing experimental treatment.
Several factors influencing the ASR should be considered in order to control the inherent
variability. These include issues that pertain to inter-trial intervals (ITI) (Ison and Hammond,
1971; Leitner et al., 1993; Willott and Carlson, 1995; Plappert et al., 2004), circadian rhythm
(Chabot and Taylor, 1992a; Chabot and Taylor, 1992b), sex differences (Plappert et al.,
2005; Koch, 1998), and sensory adaptation. A change to any of these factors can alter the
startle response magnitude and startle response variability. These issues could be magnified
when assessing the gap detection abilities in noise exposed animals, due to a suppressive
effect of acoustic over-exposure on startle magnitude (Longenecker and Galazyuk, 2011;
Lobarinas et al., 2013). Standardizing data collection efforts could decrease GPIAS data
Author Manuscript

variability between animals, experiments, and lab groups.

Detailed descriptions of GPIAS tinnitus analyses are also under-described in the literature.
One of the major confounds in GPIAS data analysis is gap-induced facilitation. A gap in a
continuous background noise usually inhibits or reduces the startle response magnitude (Stitt
et al., 1973; Ison, 1982). However, this is not always the case. Depending on stimulus
conditions, a prepulse, gap, or alterations of the background noise can alternatively act as a
facilitator of the startle reflex in mice (Plappert et al., 2004; Willott and Carlson, 1995), rats

Hear Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2019 June 01.


Longenecker et al. Page 3

(Stitt et al., 1974; Ison et al., 1997), and humans (Aasen et al., 2005). While it is not fully
Author Manuscript

understood why this dichotomy exists, both responses represent real sensory gating
phenomenon but likely have separate complimentary biological circuit (Schmajuk and
Larrauri, 2005). Thus, GPIAS assessments should be cognizant of this issue when
examining “how well” an animal can detect a gap. If not appropriately addressed during data
analysis, facilitation can limit the effectiveness of GPIAS as a tool to assess gap detection
performance because of unnecessary variability. The exact details of data processing can
also significantly affect GPIAS data interpretation.

Unfortunately, this part of data analysis is typically neglected in method sections of the
relevant papers. Nevertheless, details such as which data points are included/excluded, how
many testing sessions/days were used in the control and experimental conditions, how ratios
were calculated and/or averaged together, are critical to draw defendable conclusions based
on GPIAS data.
Author Manuscript

Our data indicate that ITI, circadian cycle, sex, and sensory adaptation all play roles in the
degree of variance present in GPIAS experiments. Our data also suggest that particular data
analyses are critical to minimize the effect of variance in GPIAS data. Taken together, these
findings suggest that the GPIAS method is more complicated than previously described, but
can provide accurate assessments of gap detection performance.

2. Materials and Methods


2.1. Subjects
A total of 62 CBA/CaJ mice were used in this study. Group A contained fourteen male mice
which were used in the majority of experiments. Group B contained an additional 48 mice
Author Manuscript

(24 males and 24 females) which were used in the sex-based variation experiments. In group
A, mice were divided into two groups of seven and were housed in separate rooms: seven of
these mice were housed in a regular light dark cycle (lights on 10 a.m. to 10 p.m.) (inactive
mice) and other seven were kept in a reverse light dark cycle (lights off 10 a.m. to 10 p.m.)
(active mice). Group B mice were housed in a regular 12-h light–dark cycle. Mice were
obtained from Jackson Laboratories and were approximately 10 weeks old. Mice were
housed in pairs within a colony room at 25°C. Procedures used in this study were approved
by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at the Northeast Ohio Medical
University.

2.2. Study design


Group A underwent baseline GPIAS testing for 21 days (one session per day) with 1-2 days
Author Manuscript

between sessions. We tested seven inactive Group A mice using long ITIs during three
sessions (ITI details are described in section 3.2). Following baseline assessment, all animals
were sound exposed to induce tinnitus, as described below. Three and five months post
exposure, behavioral evidence of tinnitus was assessed. Mice in Group B were not sound
exposed and were tested for 10 sessions in 21days for sex differences.

Hear Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2019 June 01.


Longenecker et al. Page 4

2.3. Acoustic trauma


Author Manuscript

Mice were at least five months old at the time of sound exposure. Mice were anesthetized
with an intramuscular injection of a ketamine/xylazine mixture (100/10 mg/kg). An
additional injection (50% of the initial dose) was given intramuscularly 30 min after the
initial injection. Mice were exposed to a one octave band noise centered at 12.5 kHz (~8-17
kHz) unilaterally for one hour. This noise was generated using a waveform generator
(Wavetek model 395), amplified (Sherwood RX-4109) to 116 dB SPL, and played through a
loudspeaker (Fostex FT17H). The output of the loudspeaker was calibrated with a 0.25-in.
microphone (Brüel and Kjaer 4135) and found to be ±4 dB between 10 and 60 kHz. The left
external ear canal was obstructed with a cotton plug and a Kwik-Sil silicone elastomer plug
(World Precision Instruments), a manipulation which reduces sound levels by 30 to 50 dB
SPL (Turner et al., 2006; Ropp et al., 2014).
Author Manuscript

2.4. Acoustic startle hardware/software


The equipment used to collect all acoustic startle reflex (ASR) data has been described in
detail previously (Longenecker and Galazyuk, 2012). Briefly, commercial hardware/
software equipment from Kinder Scientific, Inc. was used. Each behavioral testing station
was lined with anechoic foam to prevent sound reflection and wave cancelling sound echoes
(Sonex foam from Pinta Acoustics). Mice restrainers were open walled to allow for
maximum sound penetration (Fig. 3 in Longenecker and Galazyuk, 2012). Background
sound levels within each testing chamber were calibrated with a 0.25-in. microphone (Brüel
and Kjaer 4135) attached to a measuring amplifier (Brüel and Kjaer 2525) and found to be
less than 40 dB SPL between 4 and 40 kHz. Startle waveforms were recorded using load-cell
platforms which measure actual force changes during an animal’s startle. Each load cell was
calibrated with a 100g weight, corresponding approximately to 0.98 newton of force in
Author Manuscript

standard gravity.

2.5. Startle waveform identification and measurement


All waveforms collected during testing sessions were analyzed offline using a recently
developed automatic method of startle waveform identification via a template matching
paradigm (Grimsley et al., 2015). In that study we used high-speed video recordings (1,000
frames/s) to visualize the animal’s startles in order to identify stereotyped waveforms
associated with a startle. This allowed us to develop custom software which automatically
separates data into either startles or non-startle-related movements. Based on this separation,
we only included trials that resulted in successful startle responses in our data analysis. We
also used a mathematical approach to convert the force generated on a load cell plate to
center of mass displacement (in mm) (Grimsley et al., 2015).
Author Manuscript

2.6. GPIAS data collection


The ability of mice to detect a gap of silence preceding the startle stimulus was determined
by comparing the startle magnitude in response to a startle stimulus presented alone (No-
Gap) and a startle stimulus paired with a gap (Gap). Both trial types had continuous
background noise. The gap had a 20 ms duration and 1 ms rise/fall time. Background for all
these trials was presented as a narrow band (1/3 octave) noise centered at six different

Hear Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2019 June 01.


Longenecker et al. Page 5

frequencies (8, 10, 12.5, 16, 20, and 25 kHz). This background noise level was constant (55
Author Manuscript

dB SPL) throughout each session. The startle stimulus was presented at 105 dB SPL (white
noise, 1 ms rise/fall, 20 ms duration). The gap was presented 100 ms before (onset to onset)
the startle stimulus. The testing session started with an acclimation period lasting 5 min. All
testing sessions were conducted in a lighted behavioral box, regardless of which circadian
cycle the animal was on, to avoid introducing light as an additional variable.

During a testing session, Gap and No-Gap trials for the same background frequency were
presented in blocks. Each block contained 5 Gap trials and 5 No-Gap trials presented for a
given frequency in pseudo-random fashion. After all six background frequencies were
completed, this stimulus sequence was repeated eight more times. Thus, during the entire
testing session 45 (9×5=45) Gap and 45 No-Gap trials were presented for each of six
background frequencies.
Author Manuscript

For best performance analysis (see Results), the total number of trials and testing parameters
were identical to the above description. A Segment was defined as 6 background frequency
blocks repeated 3 times (18 blocks total). Thus, for the purpose of best performance data
analysis, each session was subdivided into 3 Segments.

2.7. Inter-trial interval


Two inter-trial interval (ITI) arrangements were used in this study. Some experiments
utilized ITIs that were randomized between 4, 5 and 6 seconds, whereas other used ITIs
randomized between 7, 8, 9 …, and 15 seconds. The short and long ITI sessions had the
same number of trials and lasted about one and three hours, respectively. See Results to
determine which arrangement was used for what experimental design.
Author Manuscript

2.8. Estrus cycle assessment in female mice


To test whether the estrus cycle in female mice has an effect on their GPIAS performance
we visually assessed their external genitalia before each experiment (Byers et al., 2012).

2.9. GPIAS data and statistical analyses


Data obtained in GPIAS testing sessions were analyzed offline using several methodological
approaches (discussed below, along with the benefits and limitations of each method). All of
our analyses concern different strategies to manage GPIAS data directly following
individual startle identification and separation.

We used nonparametric Kolmogorov–Smirnov two-sample test (K–S test) to determine


whether two different absolute startle magnitude distributions were statistically different or
Author Manuscript

if they could be considered to be from the same distribution. The K–S test statistic D is
calculated by finding the maximum absolute vertical distance between the two distribution
functions. We choose the Fligner–Killeen test of homogeneity of variances because it does
not depend on the shape of the data distribution. To account for multiple comparisons p-
values were adjusted using the Benjamini and Hochberg’s false discovery rate (FDR)
procedure (see Table 1). Unequal variance t-tests were applied to ranked ratios to evaluate

Hear Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2019 June 01.


Longenecker et al. Page 6

the effect of noise exposure on gap detection at each background frequency. All data
Author Manuscript

analyses and statistical tests were carried out in R (R Core Team, 2016).

3. Results
Data collection
Mice are known to have a highly variable ASR. To minimize the impact of this variability on
experimental results, it is beneficial to identify experimental manipulations which might
contribute to this variability. Mice were tested over multiple sessions to assess the amount of
ASR variability caused by: ITI, circadian rhythm, sex differences, and sensory adaptation.

3.1 Inherent startle magnitude variance in CBA/CaJ mice


To determine the level of ASR variability in mice we first examined the startle variability in
Author Manuscript

14 individual mice during 21 individual testing sessions (Fig. 1). Four distinct types of ASR
behavior were observed: constant fluctuation (Fig. 1A), state change (Fig. 1B), habituation
(Fig. 1C), and minimal change (Fig. 1D). The amount of ASR variance seen in each of these
types was dramatically different ranging from 3.5 mm2 in “state change” to 1 mm2 in
“minimal change” (Fig. 1E; Table 1). These patterns of behavior were observed in all mice
over 21 days of testing but some were observed in far greater proportions. The constant
fluctuation type represented 75% of all sessions, making this pattern the most common (Fig.
1F). The other three types were observed in 15% or fewer of sessions (Fig. 1F). All mice
demonstrated a sampling of each type of ASR behaviors, which importantly demonstrates
that these effects were not mouse specific. Typically an individual mouse exhibited different
ASR behavior from one session to another. Interestingly, within-session habituation (Fig.
1C) is a highly conserved ASR behavior in rats (Davis, 1984), whereas it was only observed
in about 5% of all sessions in CBA/CaJ mice in this study (Fig. 1F).
Author Manuscript

3.2 Inter-trial interval variation


Here we examined how ITI affects ASR magnitude and probability in two groups of mice (7
animals in each group). Two-sample Kolmogorov–Smirnov test indicated statistically
significant difference between startle magnitudes in short (4 to 6 sec) versus long (7 to 15
sec) ITI trials (D = 0.18, p < 0.001; Fig. 2A). Although longer ITIs induce a greater startle
reflex magnitude, this also increases the dynamic range of startle and thus increases the
variance compared to shorter ITIs (Fig. 2B; Table 1). The probability that a valid startle was
recorded was significantly higher in the long ITI condition compared to the short ITI
condition (χ2 = 281.88, p < 0.001; Fig. 2C).

3.3 Circadian rhythm variation


Author Manuscript

The effect of circadian cycles on ASR magnitude was assessed for 14 mice across 21 testing
sessions (Fig. 3). Mice housed in a reverse light dark cycle (active mice, N=7) demonstrated
higher magnitude ASR than regular light dark cycle mice (inactive mice, N=7) (Fig. 3A).
Both Gap and No-Gap startle magnitudes were significantly different between inactive and
active mice (Gap trials: D = 0.12, p < 0.001; No-gap trials: D = 0.11, p < 0.001). The startle
magnitude variance was also greater in the active mice, (Fig. 3B; Table 1) with clear
fluctuations in startle reflex magnitude across sessions (Fig. 3A). Interestingly, both groups

Hear Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2019 June 01.


Longenecker et al. Page 7

had nearly identical initial ASR levels, but quickly differentiated on the second session of
Author Manuscript

testing with the inactive mice having dramatically lower ASR compared to active mice. The
magnitude and associated variance for both groups was subject to long term habituation. For
both groups the most dramatic changes were observed during the first 5 sessions of testing
(Fig. 3C; Table 1).

3.4 Variation due to sex differences


Previous studies have shown that startle magnitudes can fluctuate throughout the estrous
cycle in female mice (Plappert et al., 2005) and rats (Koch, 1998). Using long ITIs, ASRs
were assessed across 10 sessions in 24 male and 24 female mice (Fig 4A). A two-sample
Kolmogorov–Smirnov test indicated that both Gap and No-Gap startle magnitudes were
significantly different between male and female mice (Gap trials: D = 0.11, p < 0.001; No-
Gap trials: D = 0.12, p < 0.001). There was a significant difference in weight between males
Author Manuscript

(M = 34.05, SD = 3.64) and females (M = 24.00, SD = 4.03); t(433.89) = −31.46, p < 0.001.
A one-way Analysis of Covariance (ANCOVA) was applied to compare male and female
startle magnitudes, controlling for weight. Both weight and sex had a significant effect on
startle magnitudes (p < 0.001); startle magnitude decreased with increasing weight. After
adjusting for the effect of weight, post-hoc tests showed significant differences in startle
magnitudes between males (M = 2.68, SE = 0.11) and females (M = 2.07, SE = 0.11);
estimated adjusted mean difference 0.61 mm, p = 0.001. Using the Grand ratio method (Fig.
7) we found that females mice showed slightly but significantly better GPIAS compared to
males: males (M = 0.94, SD = 0.34), females (M = 0.87, SD = 0.29); t(2792) = −5.42, p <
0.001.

Females had far more startle magnitude variance than males (Fig. 4B; Table 1), which in part
can be explained by the shifting variance between parts of the estrus cycle, which was
Author Manuscript

assessed by visual observation of the external genitalia before each experiment (Byers et al.,
2012). The startle magnitude variance was relatively higher in the metestrus and proestrus
stages (Fig. 4C; Table 1). However, the female mice in this study did not spend equal time in
each cycle, but far more time in diestrus (supplementary Fig. 1), which had lower variance.

3.5 Sensory adaptation


Data collection under the GPIAS paradigm can be confounded by sensory adaptation, a
sensory learning process (Crofton et al., 1990). To test whether sensory adaptation occurs in
CBA/CaJ mice, we tested 14 mice over 21 sessions in a span of 50 days. Both the grand
ratio and modified ratio methods (see section 3.7 and 3.8 for method description) revealed
little changes in ratios between sessions (Fig. 5) indicating that gap detection performance in
CBA/CaJ mice was not a subject of sensory adaptation. The GPIAS ratios only slightly
Author Manuscript

fluctuated between sessions from 0.86 to 0.93. Solutions to the potential confound in GPIAS
data caused by sensory adaptation are presented in the data analysis section below.

Data analysis
Here we address issues of data analysis as they relate specifically to variance in gap
detection assessments. First, we demonstrate that gap-induced facilitation of the acoustic
startle reflex is a noteworthy phenomenon in CBA/CaJ mice, and that this facilitation can

Hear Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2019 June 01.


Longenecker et al. Page 8

significantly change the results of the study. Second, we analyze a single dataset from a
Author Manuscript

normal mouse with several different data processing strategies. Third, we show that sample
size is an important factor for the reliability of GPIAS data. Finally, we show that the
various data processing strategies result in different degrees of gap detection which can
impact evaluations of gap detection deficits associated with tinnitus.

3.6 Gap-induced Inhibition and facilitation


Throughout each testing session we observed blocks of inhibition, in which a mean of Gap
trials was smaller than a mean of No-Gap trials, and blocks of facilitation, where a mean of
Gap trials was higher than a mean of No-Gap trials (Fig. 6A). In CBA/CaJ mice, the
occurrence of gap-induced facilitation is not a rare occurrence. To test for a relative
contribution of inhibition and facilitation a mean was calculated from all Gap values and a
mean from all No-Gap values at every background frequency for each Group A mouse
Author Manuscript

(N=14) in each 21 baseline testing session. Then, daily ratios (for each background
frequency, for each mouse) were obtained by dividing those means (Gap/No-Gap); x-axis in
Figure 6B. Each cumulative probability curve shows a separate baseline testing session (21
curves). About 70% of Gap/No-Gap ratios resulted in gap-induced inhibition (ratios < 1) and
about 30% of ratios resulted in gap-induced facilitation (ratios > 1). Similar proportions of
facilitation and inhibition at each background frequency were observed before and after
sound exposure (supplementary Fig. 2). Neither circadian cycle nor sex affected these
proportions (supplementary Fig. 3).

3.7 Options for GPIAS analysis


Here we present five unique methods for GPIAS data analyses, which include: standard
ratio, modified ratio, best performance, grand ratio, and cumulative probability (Fig. 7; Fig.
Author Manuscript

9). Each of these methods processes GPIAS data differently. Typically scientists collect
GPIAS data by blocks where several Gap and No-Gap trials are presented randomly or
pseudo-randomly at a given background frequency (Galazyuk and Hébert, 2015). Therefore
the first three methods take advantage of block data collection for data analysis. Other two
methods do not consider blocks and analyze all the data from each testing session as a
whole. We demonstrate how each analysis results in different outcomes (Fig. 7) when using
the same dataset shown in Figure 1A (one mouse; one session). In the discussion we suggest
some benefits and limitations of each method which should be considered when planning a
GPIAS experiment (Table 2).

Standard ratio (blocks)—Startle responses considered valid from the automated


classifier were counted in each block. Blocks were considered valid if they contained at least
Author Manuscript

1 valid Gap and 1 valid No-Gap from the maximum of 5 Gaps and 5 No-Gaps. Then, we
calculated a mean of all available Gap values and a mean of all No-Gap values. A single
ratio (mean of Gap/mean of No-Gap) was calculated for each valid block. For each
background frequency all available ratios (45 max) were averaged. Standard ratio
calculations included and averaged both the blocks showing inhibition (ratios <1) and those
showing facilitation (ratios >1). The contribution of facilitation led to variable and markedly
high ratio values ranging from ~0.8 to ~1.5 (Fig. 7A). Unsurprisingly, the associated
variance was 0.269 (Fig. 7E), a value much higher than other types of analyses (Table 1).

Hear Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2019 June 01.


Longenecker et al. Page 9

Modified ratio (blocks)—This type of GPIAS data analysis is a modification of the


Author Manuscript

standard ratio. The major difference between these two methods lays in the way of how gap-
induced facilitation is treated. This method considers both gap-induced inhibition and gap-
induced facilitation as an indication of gap detection. Both are expressed using the same
scale (ranging from 0 to 1) and averaged together. To accomplish this, we divided the Gap
by No-Gap for the blocks showing inhibition (mean of Gap < mean of No-Gap) and No-Gap
by Gap for the blocks showing facilitation (mean of Gap > mean of No-Gap). The resulting
ratios for this type of analysis ranged between ~0.6 and ~0.8 across different background
frequencies with relatively small standard errors (Fig. 7B). This analysis provides a very low
variance of 0.032 (Fig. 7E).

Best performance ratio (blocks)—This is another variation of the block analysis. As


we described above, our testing sessions were organized in blocks. These blocks were
repeated nine times for a given frequency, which can be further subdivided into three
Author Manuscript

identical Segments. Thus every background frequency was tested with 3 identical blocks per
segment and every testing session contained three identical segments. Such subdivision of
the testing session allowed assessment of gap detection performance in each of these three
segments independently. The best performance method compared the gap detection
performance in an individual mouse across three segments. The lowest ratio value (mean of
Gap/mean of No-Gap) or best performance was chosen among the three segments (Fig. 7C).
This approach approximates those used in sporting contests in which the best performance
out of several attempts is recorded. Similar to the modified ratio, this method minimizes the
contribution of facilitation to the overall gap detection performance assessment. The values
obtained in this analysis can have similar ranges of ratios as standard (Fig. 7A) or grand
ratio (Fig. 7D) within a day (Fig. 7C). For example, in Figure 7, ratios ranged from ~0.7 to
~1.3. Variance for this example was 0.148 (Fig. 7E).
Author Manuscript

Grand ratio—This method does not take blocks into consideration. Instead, the mean of all
values for Gap trials and the mean of No-Gap trials were calculated across an entire session
for each background frequency. Then, one ratio was determined by dividing the mean of the
Gap trials by the mean of the No-Gap trials for each background frequency tested. No
standard errors can be calculated for Grand ratio because there is a single datum for an entire
session. Additionally, by collapsing Gap and No-Gap trials into a single (mean) value, any
differences in variability among the trial types (i.e., heteroscedasticity) is effectively ignored.
Ratios in this dataset range from ~.8 to ~1.25 (Fig. 7D). The variance for this analysis was
0.022, which is low compared to other methods (Fig. 7E). However, this is not an accurate
way of representing variance as it will be discussed below.
Author Manuscript

Cumulative probability analysis—This method avoids the use of both blocks and
ratios. All trials resulting in a valid startle (Gap or No-Gap) were included into this analysis.
Absolute startle magnitudes were plotted as cumulative probability functions for each
background frequency. The Gap and No-Gap functions were then statistically compared with
two-sample Kolmogorov–Smirnov tests. The associated D value determines the degree of
difference between these two functions with the p values indicating the significance of this
difference (Fig. 7F).

Hear Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2019 June 01.


Longenecker et al. Page 10

3.8 GPIAS sample size considerations


Author Manuscript

The GPIAS method was introduced to the field as a tool to assess tinnitus in laboratory
animals (Turner et al., 2006). It was assumed that gap detection performance in the animals
with tinnitus should be significantly reduced compared to baseline. Comparison of the
performance before and after tinnitus induction in mice is challenging due to high variability
from one session to another collected from the same mouse. Figure 8 A–D shows that the
gap detection performance varies in a normal mouse (with no induction of tinnitus) across
different testing sessions. These four sessions were selected from 21 sessions conducted to
assess animal’s baseline gap detection. Such variability in gap detection was revealed when
either the Modified ratio or Grand ratio methods were used for analysis. It is possible that
this variability in gap detection performance is due to a low sample size collected during a
single session. To test for this possibility we tested gap detection performance in this mouse
when the sample size was increased by increasing the number of testing sessions (Fig. 8E).
Author Manuscript

The number of valid blocks for each background frequency increased with the number of
sessions combined for analysis. Modified and Grand ratio approaches were used here (Fig.
7). The first data set (one session) used in Figure 8E was the same as shown in Figure 8 A.
Including an increasing number of sessions reduced the variance in ratios across different
frequencies and also reduced standard error values. The combined data set from ten testing
sessions made the resulting gap detection ratios more consistent across frequencies and
reduced the standard errors (compare single sessions shown in Fig. 8 A-D with the
combined session shown in F).

3.9 GPIAS analyses for tinnitus assessment


As we showed above many factors can alter accurate GPIAS assessment which is crucial for
identification of tinnitus-positive animals. For example, GPIAS performance in mice can
Author Manuscript

vary due to ASR habituation (Fig. 3). To minimize this effect, we excluded the first five
testing sessions from the baseline assessment analysis. We compared the results from ten
combined GPIAS baseline sessions (sessions 6 - 15 were used with the results from five
combined sessions after sound exposure. The same data set collected from a representative
mouse was used for all five GPIAS data analysis methods shown in Figure 9. A frequency-
specific gap detection deficit was evident by all five methods following sound exposure (Fig.
9). Ranked unequal variance t-tests were applied to assess the effect of acoustic trauma at
each background frequency; we report p-values unadjusted for multiple comparisons.
Standard ratios were significantly different at both 12.5 kHz (t (82.46) = −2.12, p = 0.04)
and 25 kHz (t (91.39) = 2.27, p = 0.03; Figure 9A). Significant difference at 25 kHz was also
found with modified (t (91.91) = 2.67, p = 0.009), best performance (t (26.62) = 2.04, p =
0.05) and grand ratios (t (12.66) = 2.25, p = 0.04; Figure 9B–D). This deficit at 25 kHz was
Author Manuscript

also seen for the cumulative probability analysis: before noise exposure, Kolmogorov–
Smirnov D-value was 0.18 (p < 0.001), changing to 0.09 (p = 0.75) after exposure –
signifying that the Gap vs No-Gap separation was greater before exposure than after
exposure. Such change was only observed at 25 kHz. Overall, these data suggest that gap
detection deficits can be detected with several unique analyses.

Hear Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2019 June 01.


Longenecker et al. Page 11

4. Discussion
Author Manuscript

4.1 Current state of GPIAS for tinnitus assessment


Despite its popularity as a methodology for tinnitus assessment (Eggermont, 2013; Hayes et
al., 2014; Galazyuk and Hebert, 2015), the GPIAS method is not well developed for several
reasons. First, scientists who routinely use this method for tinnitus assessment rarely
describe specific details of data collection and data analysis. Second, the major stimulus
parameters used for GPIAS are not well justified. Criteria and statistical verification of the
presence of tinnitus-like behavior, even those used for the same animal tinnitus model by
different research groups, vary dramatically. Many of these details are critical and largely
determine whether GPIAS can assess gap detection deficits in laboratory animals or in
humans. Therefore, it is not surprising that some research groups have found that this
methodology is able to assess tinnitus whereas other groups have questioned the validity of
Author Manuscript

GPIAS for tinnitus assessment in animal models (Hickox and Liberman, 2014; Radziwon et
al., 2015) and humans (Campolo et al., 2013; Fournier and Hebert, 2013). Our recent work
attempted to shed light on some issues related to hardware and stimulus presentation issues
(Longenecker and Galazyuk, 2012), as well as startle identification and startle magnitude
measurement (Grimsley et al., 2015). While these initial steps increased our understanding
of ASR and GPIAS complexity, the present study provides further in-depth analyses of
GPIAS-related issues to help avoid obtaining inconclusive or even misleading results.

4.2 Considerations of data collection


The magnitude of the acoustic startle reflex fluctuates, and this fluctuation is especially
pronounced in mice (Valsamis and Schmid, 2011). In order to explore and minimize the
effect of these variations on GPIAS testing, we tested several parameters which have been
Author Manuscript

known to modulate the startle reflex (Koch, 1999; Swerdlow et al., 2001). Here we show that
inter-trial interval (Fig. 2), circadian stage (Fig. 3), sex (Fig. 4), and sensory adaptation (Fig.
5) contribute to ASR variability and affect GPIAS results. These results should be
considered as descriptive, rather than hardline suggestions for how to conduct GPIAS
experiments, especially when considering species differences.

4.2.1 Inherent variance of CBA/CaJ mice—To examine the degree of ASR variability,
we tested mouse ASRs about three hundred of sessions and classified four types of behavior
(Fig. 1). The most prevalent behavioral classification was the “constant flux.” This behavior
was marked by continuously changing startle amplitudes for both No-Gap and Gap trials
(Fig. 1A). While continual fluctuations of startle magnitudes would be expected to have high
variance (Fig. 1E), interestingly “state change” showed even greater ASR variability. The
“state change” classification, characterized by 10-20 minute periods of consistently elevated
Author Manuscript

or decreased ASR, seems to represent distinct physiological or attentional state deviations


within a testing session (Fig. 1B). These well-defined states should be further studied,
perhaps with camera monitoring, corticosterone assessments, and possibly cortically
implanted electrodes. It is likely that each animal’s emotional and/or attentional responses to
the startle paradigm provide a basis for these “state changes.” It is well established that these
changes, stemming from neural input from the amygdala and cortical centers, can modify
the ASR magnitude in many species (Koch and Schnitzler, 1997; Grillon and Baas, 2003).

Hear Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2019 June 01.


Longenecker et al. Page 12

Furthermore, attentional declines have been directly linked to restraint stress (Pérez et al.,
Author Manuscript

2013), which is a necessary component of ASR testing in most rodents. Attentional and
stress factors are likely contributors to large intra- and inter-session ASR variation.
Regardless of the cause, highly variable ASR behavior (constant flux and state change; Fig.
1F) constitutes roughly 90% of all data collected in Group A mice, which implies that this
phenomenon should not be ignored during GPIAS data collection, analysis, and
interpretations.

Another finding from detailed analysis of startle behavior across a large data set was that
within-session habituation, which occurs in nearly all mammalian species (Davis, 1984),
does not seem to be a significant factor in CBA/CaJ mice (Longenecker et al., 2016). Clear
habituation, as noted by a decrease in absolute startle magnitude from the beginning to the
end of a testing session, was observed in only about 5% of all trials (Fig. 1C). This has
positive implications for this model because it reduces concerns that the animal is
Author Manuscript

responding with progressively weaker startle responses by the end of the testing session.
Although within-session habituation seems to be uniquely underrepresented in this model
(Fig. 1C), across-session habituation seems to be a universal phenomenon (Fig. 3; Fig. 4)
(further discussed below). To determine whether ASR data are effected by habituation with a
given animal model, a large number of trials should be collected within a single testing
session. This will allow to assessment of whether the behavioral state of the animal changes
over time (Fig.1). After such a careful review of the behavioral data, it would be easier to
determine typical variations in ASR data for a given animal model and also to identify and if
it is necessary to exclude “atypical” sessions or animals from further data analysis.

4.2.2 Inter-trial interval variation—Inter-trial interval, or the amount of time between


startle eliciting trials, is an important factor for planning ASR or GPIAS experiments. This
Author Manuscript

should not be confused with inter-stimulus interval (a.k.a. inter-pulse interval), which
represents the time between a gap of silence and the startle stimulus (Ison and Hammond,
1971; Leitner et al., 1993; Willott and Carlson, 1995; Plappert et al., 2004). Here we
assessed long vs. short ITIs to examine whether it affects ASR during GPIAS testing. We
found that the GPIAS experiments could be conducted in CBA/CaJ mice using both long
and short ITIs (Fig. 2). Resulting startle magnitudes were higher for the long ITIs compared
to short ITIs (Fig. 2A). This has also been shown for rats (Davis, 1970). Reductions of the
startle magnitude at shorter ITIs makes startle response identification and evaluation more
challenging (Grimsley et al., 2015). Mice in our study show little habituation to the short
ITIs (4-6 seconds) even during a session containing several hundred trials, which is
consistent with previous reports (Bullock et al., 1997). Both the startle magnitude and startle
probability decrease with shorter ITIs (Fig. 2C), which has been reported for mice
Author Manuscript

(Longenecker et al., 2016) and humans (Prokasy and Ebel, 1967; Blumenthal and Berg,
1986; Blumenthal, 1996). A decreased ASR due to short ITIs could be explained by basal,
non-neural factors, such as muscle fatigue or muscle refractory periods (Valsamis and
Schmid, 2011). The reduction in startle response magnitude and startle response probability
is important because it allows researchers to plan experiments based on predictable
quantities of valid ASR trials. The apparent tradeoff in study design is between relative
length of testing and the amount of valid startles. A quicker testing session with short ITIs

Hear Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2019 June 01.


Longenecker et al. Page 13

reduces startle variability and provides more startle trials in a given amount of time, but
Author Manuscript

reduces the number of valid startle responses. Perhaps the most important criterion in
determining an appropriate ITI is the specific animal model. While we describe that
CBA/CaJ mice startle rather consistently with very short ITIs, this might not be the case
with other animal models. Therefore any startle-related work with a new animal model
should probably begin with identification of optimal ITIs to minimize a possible effect of
both the short-term and/or long-term habituation on data collection and interpretation.

4.2.3 Circadian variation—It is not surprising that ASR would be dramatically altered by
testing at various epochs of the circadian cycle because nearly all modulatory
neurotransmitters and hormones are subject to circadian modulation (Saper et al., 2005).
Previous data in rats has shown that ASR magnitude is sensitive to a testing epoch (Chabot
and Taylor, 1992a; Chabot and Taylor, 1992b). While the exact neural substrate of this
modulation is still uncertain (Frankland and Ralph, 1995), an animal’s state of arousal is
Author Manuscript

likely a major factor in this ASR modulation (Samuels et al., 2007). Our data confirm
previous findings in rats (Horlington, 1970; Davis and Sollberger, 1971), by showing that
mice tested during their active time (dark) demonstrate a greater ASR (Fig. 3A). Three
distinct points highlight the importance of this data set. First, in both the initial testing (first
5 days) and later steady state phase, the active mice had a greater startle response (Fig. 3A).
Second, the variance was also higher for the active mice compared to inactive mice across
sessions (Fig. 3B, C). This suggests that using the inactive animals might provide more
consistent results over the long term, even if some percentage of maximum startle
magnitudes need to be sacrificed. Third, the amount of prepulse inhibition seems to be
similar between groups. Because gap detection is thought to be reliant on higher level
circuitry, including the cortex (Ison et al., 1991), this suggests that the neural substrates
influencing circadian states are acting at levels at or below the caudal pontine reticular
Author Manuscript

nucleus (Frankland and Ralph, 1995), the obligatory central output of the acoustic startle
reflex (Koch, 1999). When considering this dataset, the most important suggestion for
running ASR experiments is to test at a consistent time period each day, otherwise increases
in startle variability would be expected. A more in depth study should be done to assess
whether circadian cycles specifically impact prepulse inhibition.

4.2.4 Sex variation—It is well documented that female mice (Plappert et al., 2005), rats
(Koch, 1998) and humans show estrus cycle-dependent fluctuations of startle magnitude.
Here we observe that female mice have higher startle magnitudes compered to males, at least
through the first several days of testing (Fig. 4A). This is largely due to lower body mass
compared to their male cohorts. Towards the end of the experiment (10 testing sessions in
total), however, both groups of animals reached a habituated ASR state and seemed to have
Author Manuscript

similar startle magnitudes. This might imply that to compare sexes more precisely, the ASR
should be habituated first. However, gap detection performance between sexes was
significantly different but small, so using a ratio format for analyzing GPIAS testing would
eliminate habituation concerns. Female mice also showed an increased variance (Fig. 4B), as
might be expected with increased startle response magnitudes. However, increases in
variability cannot be simply attributed to body mass differences. A similar phenomenon is
also seen in humans, where the variability of the eye blink component of the startle response

Hear Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2019 June 01.


Longenecker et al. Page 14

(indexed by recording EMG activity of the orbicularis oculi muscle; Aasen et al., 2005) was
Author Manuscript

found to be more variable in females than males. With careful monitoring of the estrus cycle,
we found that ASR variance differed across stages (Fig. 4C). These results did not match a
previous study on mice in which ASR was tracked across the estrus cycle, however, that
study used C57BL/6J and C3H mice (Plappert et al., 2005). In that study, the two strains
differed in ASR-estrus modulation and thus inter-species differences should not be
unexpected. It should be noted that mice in this study spent the majority of time in the
diestrus phase (supplementary Fig. 1), which has also been shown previously (Byers et al.,
2012). Fortunately, for ASR experiments, this part of the cycle seems to have the least
variance. Importantly, even with the diestrus phase being the most common and associated
with the least variance, male mice always had less ASR variance. This suggests that GPIAS
studies should be planned with this variance difference in mind (Fig. 1). Estrogen levels can
impact prepulse inhibition in mice (Charitidi et al., 2012), rats (Koch, 1998), and humans
Author Manuscript

(Swerdlow et al., 1997; Van den Busse and Eikelis, 2001; Jovanovic et al., 2004). Thus, the
estrus cycle should be taken into account during GPIAS experiments. While an equal
representation between sexes is important for most disease models, excluding female
animals, if possible, can lower session-to-session ASR variance. If both sexes need to be
included, then data from female mice should be individualized, as the averaging between
sexes or even between females could alter data due to fluctuations in estrous cycles.

4.2.5 Habituation and sensory adaptation—Long-term habituation of the ASR is a


universal phenomenon across species and testing conditions (Davis, 1970; Davis and
Gendelman, 1977; Pilz and Schnitzler, 1996; Pilz et al., 2014). We also found that CBA/CaJ
mice, like other strains of mice (Bullock et al., 2007), exhibit long-term habituation, which
was mainly evident during the first 4 to 5 testing sessions (Fig. 4; Fig. 5). This habituation
should be taken into account during GPIAS studies for tinnitus assessment due to several
Author Manuscript

reasons. First, for a repeated measures design in which the same animal is tested over several
days, the ASR will decrease over sessions. If an animal is tested prior to an experimental
manipulation (i.e. acoustic overexposure or salicylate treatment), it would be important to be
certain that an animal’s ASR habituation has reached a steady state (Fig. 4; 5) (Jordan and
Poore, 1998; Blaszczyk, 2003; Plappert and Pilz, 2005). If not, the conclusions of the
experiment could be misinterpreted. Some recent publications have shown that after acoustic
over-exposure the ASR magnitude drops (Longenecker and Galazyuk, 2011; Longenecker et
al., 2014; Lobarinas et al., 2013; Salloum et al., 2016). However, this effect might partially
be due to the habituation process rather than noise-induced hearing loss. Second, even if
different groups of mice are used to compare a treatment vs. baseline effect via ASR
assessments, the ASR habituation of these animals might differ between groups as a result of
the treatment. Lastly, inter-animal, inter-strain, and inter-species differences of startle
Author Manuscript

magnitude, startle habituation, and prepulse inhibition have often been reported (Bullock et
al., 1997; Willott et al., 2003; Yee et al., 2005; Csomor et al., 2008). Thus, each animal, if
possible, should be tested as an individual in a repeated measures design. This would ensure
that all baseline and post treatment gap detection performances can be accurately assessed.

Another phenomenon that should be considered in GPIAS studies is sensory adaptation, a


sensory learning process which can enhance prepulse inhibition over time (Crofton et al.,

Hear Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2019 June 01.


Longenecker et al. Page 15

1990; Friedman et al., 2004; Plappert et al., 2006). This adaptation is plastic (Pietropaolo
Author Manuscript

and Crusio, 2009) and developmentally regulated (Willott et al., 1994; Green et al., 2016).
Our data indicate that mice (at least CBA/CaJ) show little signs of adaptation as revealed by
both the Grand and Modified ratios (Fig. 5).

4.3 Data analysis considerations


4.3.1 Addressing facilitation in GPIAS research—Prepulse inhibition of the acoustic
startle reflex is a well characterized phenomenon for many species (Hoffman and Ison,
1980; Koch, 1999). Prepulse facilitation is a far less studied phenomenon, but has been
reported for mice (Willott and Carlson, 1995; Plappert et al., 2004), rats (Davis, 1974;
Reijmers and Peeters, 1994; Ison et al., 1997), and humans (Blumenthal and Levey, 1989;
Ludewig et al., 2003). Although sometimes contradictory among studies, several stimulus
parameters that might trigger prepulse facilitation have been repeatedly reported. Inter
Author Manuscript

stimulus intervals to study PPI (prepulse stimulus to startle stimulus) of 10 ms or below have
been shown to cause prepulse facilitation (Willott and Carlson, 1995; Hoffman and Ison,
1980; Ison and Hammond, 1971). Intense background noise (70 dB SPL and above) was
also shown to contribute to the gap-induced facilitation (Hoffman and Searle, 1965; Davis,
1974; Gerrard and Ison, 1990). Neither short inter stimulus intervals nor loud background
noise could explain the facilitation in the present study because we used much longer
intervals (100 ms) and very mild (55 dB SPL) background narrowband noise.

Perhaps the most reasonable explanation for the occurrence of facilitation in our study (Fig.
6) is that the background noise varied in frequency every few minutes within the construct of
the block format. Some previous works have suggested that changes in background
frequency can lead to facilitation of the startle (Stitt et al., 1974; Gerrard and Ison, 1990).
Indeed, some data collected in this study seemed to have gap-induced inhibitory or
Author Manuscript

facilitatory periodicity (Fig. 1; Fig. 6A), although no specific background frequency was
more likely to produce facilitatory GPIAS responses (supplementary Fig. 2). Another
reasonable explanation, is that the facilitation may occur as a product of stochastic
fluctuations in startle magnitude. These fluctuations when it compared to the control startle
value may result in inhibition or facilitation.

A few points could explain why our data set consisted of roughly 25% facilitation (Fig. 6B).
The vast majority of ASR studies have been conducted on rats. While there are not many
direct ASR comparisons between mice and rats, it seems that under most stimulus
parameters, rats demonstrate robust inhibition (see Davis, 1984), whereas mice show a
mixture of inhibition and facilitation (Plappert et al., 2004; Kraus et al., 2011; Hickox and
Liberman, 2014). The issue is further complicated in mice by inter-strain variability
Author Manuscript

(Plappert et al., 2004). Mice don’t habituate to an acoustic startle within a session very often
(Bullock et al., 1997; Fig. 2C). This necessarily implies that the ASR in mice has some
fundamental differences in its underlying neural circuity, and it would not be surprising for
different circuitry to produce more facilitation compared to other laboratory animals. Lastly,
and perhaps most importantly, data presented here is the first to show gap-induced
facilitation as raw data on a block to block basis (Fig. 1; Fig. 6A), rather than an average
over a testing session (e.g. Willott and Carlson, 1995; Plappert et al., 2004). Decades of

Hear Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2019 June 01.


Longenecker et al. Page 16

study have shown that inhibition and facilitation are two independent processes (Hoffman
Author Manuscript

and Wible, 1969; Stitt et al., 1974; Plappert et al., 2004; Schmajuk and Larrauri, 2005).
However, more research should be done to understand the significance and the underlying
neuronal mechanism of facilitation.

The presence of facilitation can create a significant problem for GPIAS research. If
facilitation was an isolated phenomenon, its removal could be easily justified. However,
facilitation is quite common (Fig. 6B), and if not addressed appropriately with data analysis,
facilitation can mask the effects of inhibition. Until optimal testing parameters are
discovered which reduce the occurrence of gap-induced facilitation in mice, GPIAS
assessments which aim to identify tinnitus should objectively address this issue through
analytical means that are discussed below.

4.3.2 Block analysis—We clearly demonstrated that the ASR is inherently variable (Fig.
Author Manuscript

1), and this variability extends to fluctuations between gap-induced inhibition and
facilitation (Fig. 6A). To minimize the effect of these fluctuations on GPIAS assessment we
propose grouping trials into frequency blocks. Block data collection and analysis allows for
assessment of inhibition and facilitation in a temporally sensitive manner. The block analysis
would benefit from when the ASR and the gap-induced inhibition are stable throughout a
session. Thus, the need for using this method would depend on the particular animal model.
The present study used blocks containing 5 Gap and 5 No-Gap trials at each background
frequency. Although block approach in data analysis might be beneficial, it is still unclear
whether this arrangement is optimal. Further research is necessary to address this point.

4.3.3 GPIAS sample size—We showed that combining several testing sessions for gap
detection assessment has an obvious benefit for GPIAS studies because it reduces data
Author Manuscript

variability and standard errors (Fig. 8). Such approach increases GPIAS sensitivity or
statistical power to identify even small changes in gap detection performance which often
are critical for detecting changes associated with tinnitus (Fig. 9). The appropriate number of
combined sessions would largely depend on the variability in the gap detection performance
in the same animal across different testing sessions. Additionally, this variation may change
before and after treatment. For species in which the variability is minimal and the effect of a
treatment is considerable, data sets from fewer sessions may to be combined.

An alternative approach to increase the data set for GPIAS studies would be to increase the
duration of a single testing session. Unfortunately, this option has obvious restrictions, as
mice demonstrate a reduction in gap detection performance when a testing session lasts
more than one hour (Longenecker and Galazyuk, 2012). This would also be true for an
Author Manuscript

animal model such as rats, which demonstrate substantial habituation of the ASR (Koch,
1999).

4.3.4 From data processing to tinnitus assessments—Precise descriptions of


GPIAS data analysis are underrepresented in the literature. In this study we proposed, tested
and demonstrated benefits and limitations for 5 different methods. The majority of these
methods utilize the traditional ration of raw gap ASR divided by the raw no-gap ASR data.
These gap ratios are resistant to startle magnitude fluctuations which allows intra- and inter-

Hear Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2019 June 01.


Longenecker et al. Page 17

animal normalization. Another recently published method used A’ assessments to


Author Manuscript

accomplish the same task while utilizing the entire data set (Allen and Luebke, 2017), a
limitation of some of the ratio methods discussed below. Similarly to the A’ method, the
cumulative probability function is a novel way to represent both ASR and GPIAS data. It
utilizes all raw data and thus differs from ratio methods which plot rations of varying
degrees of raw datasets. While we are not declaring that one method is superior to another,
the major benefits and limitations of each of these five approaches are listed in Table 2 while
details are discussed below.

4.3.4.1 Standard ratio (blocks)—The standard ratio is the analysis (Fig. 7A) that
incorporates the concept of blocks in the data set. This minimizes the effect of ASR
fluctuations when Gap and No-Gap trials are changing together within a session (Fig. 1B).
The ratios are calculated in individual blocks which encompass short epochs within a testing
session. This method also requires very little data processing other than the definition of a
Author Manuscript

valid block as discussed above. The limitation of this analysis is that it will have a much
higher variance (Fig. 7E) due to the averaging of blocks exhibiting gap-induced facilitation
and gap-induced inhibition.

4.3.4.2 Modified ratio (blocks)—While the modified ratio analysis (Fig. 7B) might not
be the most traditional option, it does introduce a creative solution to assess gap detection
performance using both gap-induced inhibition and facilitation (Fig. 6). The major
assumption behind this method is that both inhibition and facilitation represent gap
detection. It is difficult to use both these measures for gap assessment due to the fact that the
scale for inhibition and facilitation ratios are fundamentally different. The ratio values for
inhibition are ranging from 0 to 1, whereas for facilitation from 1 to ∞ (practically, not more
than 10). When the GPIAS data using these two scales are combined, the gap-induced
Author Manuscript

inhibition and gap detection are underestimated because inhibition and facilitation are
cancelling each other. To address this problem we suggest expressing facilitation as a ratio of
no-gap/gap. In this case both inhibition and facilitation values are ranging from 0 to 1. Thus,
this method measures gap detection (both facilitation and inhibition) rather than gap-induced
inhibition only. This analysis does not generate a false positive result, because if an animal
shows poor gap detection, both the inhibition and facilitation ratios have values closer to 1
(Fig. 9). Additional assurance that this is a valid alternative method comes from the fact that
the other four methods of GPIAS data analysis detect the same frequency-specific deficit in
this example animal (Fig. 9).

4.3.4.3 Best performance ratio (blocks)—Best performance analysis (Fig. 7C) is also
an untraditional block analysis approach. This method naturally minimizes the effect of
Author Manuscript

facilitation by only selecting the highest amount of inhibition (the lowest ratio values)
throughout a testing session. Thus, it also removes a great deal of variance from the data
(Fig. 7E). On the other hand, this analysis has been applied to performance based behaviors
as an indicator of the maximum sensitivity an individual can achieve (Rosen et al., 2012;
Sarro and Sanes, 2014; Gay et al., 2014). Despite some benefits of this analysis, it does
suffer from higher variance. Although all data from each session is used to assess the best
performance, this method is considered only a subset of data the segment with the greatest

Hear Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2019 June 01.


Longenecker et al. Page 18

inhibition, is used for statistical analysis, which leads to higher variance. This method can
Author Manuscript

also be subject to facilitation if all portions of the session demonstrate gap-induced


facilitation. Even so, this suggests that facilitation can be a stable psychoacoustic reaction to
the background noise and/or gap.

4.3.4.4 Grand ratio—The grand ratio is the most straightforward approach for assessing
GPIAS data by excluding both block analysis and any adjustments for facilitation (Fig. 7D).
It is calculated by dividing the mean of all startle only No-Gap trials by the mean of all the
Gap trials at a given frequency. This approach provides one ratio per frequency tested per
session. The major downside of this method is that gap-induced facilitation shift the ratios to
higher values which are often referred as an absence of gap detection. Additionally, by
collapsing all Gap or No-Gap data from a session into a single (mean) value,) this method
effectively ignores all of the potential sources of startle magnitude variability discussed
above. The benefit of this method is that it uses the entire data set of valid startles.
Author Manuscript

Depending on the animal model, if ASR fluctuations and gap-induced facilitation are
minimal, this method could be a good choice to analyze GPIAS data.

4.3.4.5 Cumulative probability—The cumulative probability assessment (Fig. 7F) is an


alternative approach for assessing gap-induced changes in the ASR. The benefits of this
method are as follows: 1. All data are used and considered in the analysis. 2. No extra data
processing is required. 3. This method would be highly comparable between animal models
and different labs. However, the negatives of these analyses are that they do not consider the
temporal fluctuations of the ASR (Fig. 1) or gap-induced facilitation. Both of these factors
might alter the measurements of the actual gap detection performance. For animal models
without large ASR fluctuations such as “constant flux” and “state change” categories seen in
this study (Fig. 1), this method could be considered as the most appropriate.
Author Manuscript

Supplementary Material
Refer to Web version on PubMed Central for supplementary material.

Acknowledgments
We would like to acknowledge Dr. Merri Rosen for her comments on earlier versions of this manuscript. The
authors also thank Olga Galazyuk for developing software that allowed off-line data analysis. This research was
supported by grant R01 DC011330 to AVG from the National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication
Disorders of the U.S. Public Health Service.

Abbreviations
Author Manuscript

PPI prepulse inhibition

ASR acoustic startle reflex

ITI inter trail interval

Hear Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2019 June 01.


Longenecker et al. Page 19

References
Author Manuscript

Aasen I, Kolli L, Kumari V. Sex effects in prepulse inhibition and facilitation of the acoustic startle
response: implications for pharmacological and treatment studies. J Psychopharm. 2005; 19:39–45.
Allen PD, Luebke AE. Reflex modification audiometry reveals dual roles for olivocochlear
neurotransmission. Front Cell Neurosci. 2017; 11:361. [PubMed: 29213229]
Berger JI, Coomber B, Shackleton TM, Palmer AR, Wallace MN. A novel behavioural approach to
detecting tinnitus in the guinea pig. J Neurosci Meth. 2013; 213:188–195.
Blaszczyk JW. Startle response to short acoustic stimuli in rats. Acta Neurobiol Exp. 2003; 63:25–30.
Blumenthal TD, Berg WK. Stimulus rise time, intensity, and bandwidth effects on acoustic startle
amplitude and probability. Psychophysiology. 1986; 23:635–641. [PubMed: 3823338]
Blumenthal TD, Levey BJ. Prepulse rise time and startle reflex modification: Different effects for
discrete and continuous prepulses. Psychophys. 1989; 26:158–165.
Blumenthal TD. Inhibition of the human startle response is affected by both prepulse intensity and
eliciting stimulus intensity. Biological Psych. 1996; 44:85–104.
Bullock AE, Slobe BS, Vazquez V, Collins AC. Inbred mouse strains differ in the regulation of the
Author Manuscript

startle and prepulse inhibition of the startle response. Behav Neurosci. 1997; 111:1353–1360.
[PubMed: 9438803]
Byers SL, Wiles MV, Dunn SL, Taft RA. Mouse estrous cycle identification tool and images. PLoS
ONE. 2012; 7(4):e35538. [PubMed: 22514749]
Campolo J, Lobarinas E, Salvi R. Does tinnitus “fill in” the silent gaps? Noise Health. 2013; 15:1–14.
[PubMed: 23412573]
Carlson S, Willott JF. The behavioral salience of tones as indicated by prepulse inhibition of the startle
response: relationship to hearing loss and central neural plasticity in C57BL/6J mice. Hear Res.
1996; 99:168–175. [PubMed: 8970825]
Chabot CC, Taylor DH. Circadian Modulation of the rat acoustic startle response. Behav Neurosci.
1992; 106:846–852. [PubMed: 1445660]
Chabot CC, Taylor DH. Daily rhythmicity of the rat acoustic startle response. Physiol & Behav. 1992;
51:885–889. [PubMed: 1594689]
Charitidi K, Meltser I, Canlon B. Estradiol Treatment and Hormonal Fluctuations During the Estrous
Author Manuscript

Cycle Modulate the Expression of Estrogen Receptors in the Auditory System and the Prepulse
Inhibition of Acoustic Startle Response. Neuroendocrinology. 2012; 153:4412–4421.
Crofton KM, Dean KF, Sheets LP, Peele DP. Evidence for involvement of associative conditioning in
reflex modification of the acoustic startle response with gaps in background noise. Pyschobiology.
1990; 18:467–474.
Csomor PA, Yee BK, Vollenweider FX, Feldon J, Nicolet T, Quednow BB. On the influence of
baseline startle reactivity on the indexation of prepulse inhibition. Behav Neurosci. 2008;
122:885–900. [PubMed: 18729642]
Davis M. Effects of intersimulus interval length and variability on startle-response habituation in the
rat. J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1970; 72:177–192. [PubMed: 5489452]
Davis M, Sollberger A. Twenty-four-hour periodicity of the startle response in rats. Psychon Sci. 1971;
25:37–39.
Davis M. Signal-to-noise ratio as a predictor of startle amplitude and habituation in the rat. J
Comparative and Physiological Psychology. 1974; 86:812–825.
Author Manuscript

Davis M, Gendelman PM. Plasticity of the acoustic startle response in the acutely decerebrate rat. J
Comp Physiol Psychol. 1977; 91:549–563. [PubMed: 874121]
Davis, M. The mammalian startle response. In: Eaton, RC., editor. Neural Mechanisms of Startle
behavior. New York, NY: Springer Science & Plenum Press; 1984. p. 287-351.
Dehmel S, Eisinger D, Shore SE. Gap prepulse inhibition and auditory brainstem-evoked potentials as
objective measures for tinnitus in guinea pigs. Front Syst Neurosci. 2012; 6:1–15. [PubMed:
22291622]
Eggermont JJ. Hearing loss, hyperacusis, or tinnitus: what is modeled in animal research? Hear Res.
2013; 295:140–149. [PubMed: 22330978]

Hear Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2019 June 01.


Longenecker et al. Page 20

Fleshler M. Adequate acoustic stimulus for startle reaction in the rat. J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1965;
60:200–207. [PubMed: 5832345]
Author Manuscript

Fournier P, Hébert S. Gap detection deficits in humans with tinnitus as assessed with the acoustic
startle paradigm: Does tinnitus fill in the gap? Hear Res. 2013; 295:16–23. [PubMed: 22688322]
Frankland PW, Ralph MR. Circadian modulation in the rat acoustic startle circuit. Behav Neuro. 1995;
109:43–48.
Friedman JT, Peiffer AM, Clark MG, Benasich AA, Fitch RH. Age and experience-related
improvements in gap detection in the rat. Dev Brain Res. 2004; 152:83–91. [PubMed: 15351495]
Galazyuk AV, Hébert S. Gap-prepulse inhibition of the acoustic startle reflex (GPIAS) for tinnitus
assessment: current status and future directions. Front Neurol. 2015; 6:1–12. [PubMed: 25699006]
Gay JD, Voytenko SV, Galazyuk AV, Rosen MJ. Developmental hearing loss impairs signal detection
in noise: putative central mechanisms. Front Syst Neurosci. 2014; 8:162. [PubMed: 25249949]
Gerrard RL, Ison JR. Spectral frequency and the modulation of the acoustic startle reflex by
background noise. J Exp Psychol: Anim Behav Processes. 1990; 16:106–12.
Green DB, Ohlemacher J, Rosen MJ. Benefits of stimulus exposure: Developmental learning
independent of task performance. Front Neurosci. 2016; 10:1–13. [PubMed: 26858586]
Author Manuscript

Grillon C, Baas J. A review of the modulation of the startle reflex by affective states and its application
in psychiatry. Clinical Neurophys. 2003; 114:1557–1579.
Grimsley CA, Longenecker RJ, Rosen MJ, Young JW, Grimsley JM, Galazyuk AV. An improved
approach to separating startle data from noise. J Neurosci Methods. 2015; 253:206–217. [PubMed:
26165984]
Hayes SH, Radziwon KE, Stolzberg DJ, Salvi RJ. Behavioral models of tinnitus and hyperacusis in
animals. Front Neurology. 2014; 5:1–15.
Hickox AE, Liberman MC. Is noise-induced cochlear neuropathy key to the generation of hyperacusis
or tinnitus? J Neurophysiol. 2014; 111:552–564. [PubMed: 24198321]
Hoffman HS, Searle JL. Acoustic variables in the modification of startle reaction in the rat. J Comp
Physiol Psychol. 1965; 60:53–58. [PubMed: 14334243]
Hoffman HS, Wible BL. Temporal parameters in startle facilitation by steady background signals. J of
Acoustic Soc Am. 1969; 45:7–12.
Hoffman HS, Ison JR. Reflex modification in the domain of startle: I. Some empirical findings and
Author Manuscript

their implications for how the nervous system processes sensory input. Psychol Rev. 1980;
87:175–189. [PubMed: 7375610]
Horlington M. Startle response circadian rhythm in rats: Lack of correlation with motor activity.
Physiol Behav. 1970; 5:49–53. [PubMed: 5538403]
Ison JR, Hammond GR. Modification of the startle reflex in the rat by changes in the auditory and
visual environments. J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1971; 75:435–52. [PubMed: 5091224]
Ison JR. Temporal acuity in auditory function in the rat: reflex inhibition by brief gaps in noise. J
Comp Physiol Psychol. 1982; 96:945–54. [PubMed: 7153390]
Ison JR, Taylor MK, Bowen P, Schwarzkopf SB. Facilitation and inhibition of the acoustic startle
reflex in the rat after a momentary increase in background noise level. Behav Neurosci. 1997;
111:1335–1352. [PubMed: 9438802]
Ison JR, O’Connor K, Bowen GP, Bocirnea A. Temporal resolution of gaps in noise by the rat is lost
with functional decortication. Behav Neurosci. 1991; 105:33–40. [PubMed: 2025392]
Jordan WP, Poore LH. Long- and short-term habituation of acoustic startle is not frequency specific in
Author Manuscript

the rat. Physiol Behav. 1998; 63:643–649. [PubMed: 9523910]


Jovanovic T, Szilagyi S, Chakravorty S, Fiallos AM, Lewison BJ, Parwani A, Schwartz MP,
Gonzenbach S, Rotrosen JP, Duncan EJ. Menstrual cycle phase effects on prepulse inhibition of
acoustic startle. Psychophysiology. 2004; 41:401–406. [PubMed: 15102125]
Koch M, Schnitzler HU. The acoustic startle response in rats- circuits mediating evocation, inhibition
and potentiation. Behavioural Brain Res. 1997; 89:35–49.
Koch M. Sensorimotor gating changes across the estrous cycle in female rats. Psysiol & Behav. 1998;
64:625–628.
Koch M. The neurobiology of startle. Prog Neurobiol. 1999; 59:107–128. [PubMed: 10463792]

Hear Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2019 June 01.


Longenecker et al. Page 21

Kraus KS, Ding D, Jiang H, Lobarinas E, Sun W, Salvi RJ. Relationship between noise-induced
hearing loss, persistent tinnitus and growth-associated protein-43 expression in the rat cochlear
Author Manuscript

nucleus: Does synaptic plasticity in ventral cochlear nucleus suppress tinnitus? Neurosci. 2011;
194:309–325.
Landis, C., Hunt, WA. The startle pattern. New York, NY: Farrar & Rinehart; 1939.
Leitner DS, Hammond GR, Springer CP, Ingham KM, Mekilo AM, Bodison PR, Aranda MT,
Shawaryn MA. Parameters affecting gap detection in the rat. Perception & Psychophysics. 1993;
54:395–405. [PubMed: 8414898]
Lobarinas E, Hayes SH, Allman BL. The gap-startle paradigm for tinnitus screening in animal models:
limitations and optimization. Hear Res. 2013; 295:150–160. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.heares.
2012.06.001. [PubMed: 22728305]
Longenecker RJ, Galazyuk AV. Development of tinnitus in CBA/CaJ mice following sound exposure. J
Assoc Res Otolaryngol. 2011; 12:647–658. [PubMed: 21667173]
Longenecker RJ, Galazyuk AV. Methodological optimization of tinnitus assessment using prepulse
inhibition of the acoustic startle reflex. Brain Res. 2012; 1485:54–62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
j.brainres.2012.02.067. [PubMed: 22513102]
Author Manuscript

Longenecker RJ, Galazyuk AV. Variable effects of acoustic trauma on behavioral and neural correlates
of tinnitus in individual animals. Front Behav Neurosci. 2016; 10:1–14. [PubMed: 26834590]
Longenecker RJ, Chonko KT, Maricich SM, Galazyuk AV. Age effects on tinnitus and hearing loss in
CBA/CaJ mice following sound exposure. SpringerPlus. 2014; 3:542. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1186/2193-1801-3-542. [PubMed: 25279331]
Longenecker RJ, Alghamdi F, Rosen MJ, Galazyuk AV. Prepulse inhibition of the acoustic startle
reflex vs. auditory brainstem response for hearing assessment. Hear Res. 2016; 339:80–93.
[PubMed: 27349914]
Lowe AS, Walton JP. Alterations in peripheral and central components of the auditory brainstem
response: a neural assay of tinnitus. Plus One. 2015; 10:1–23.
Ludewig K, Ludewig S, Seitz A, Obrist M, Geyer MA, Vollenweider FX. The acoustic startle reflex
and its modulation: effects of age and gender in humans. Biological Psychology. 2003; 63:311–
323. [PubMed: 12853174]
Middleton JW, Kiritani T, Pedersen C, Turner J, Shepherd GMG, Tzounopoulos T. Mice with
Author Manuscript

behavioral evidence of tinnitus exhibit dorsal cochlear nucleus hyperactivity because of decreased
GABAergic inhibition. PNAS. 2011; 108:7601–7606. [PubMed: 21502491]
Pérez MÁ, Pérez-Valenzuela C, Rojas-Thomas F, Ahumada J, Fuenzalida M, Dagnino-Subiabre A.
Repeated restraint stress impairs auditory attention and GABAergic synaptic efficacy in the rat
auditory cortex. Neuroscience. 2013; 246:94–107. [PubMed: 23639878]
Pietropaolo S, Crusio WE. Strain-dependent changes in acoustic startle response and its plasticity
across adolescence in mice. Behav Genet. 2009; 39:623–631. [PubMed: 19641985]
Pilz KD, Schnitzler HU. Habituation and sensitization of the acoustic startle response in rats:
Amplitude, threshold, and latency measures. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 1996; 66:67–
79. [PubMed: 8661252]
Pilz PKD, Arnold SW, Rischawy AT, Plappert CF. Longterm-habituation of the startle response in
mice is stimulus modality, but not context specific. Front Integrative Neurosci. 2014; 7:1–10.
Plappert CF, Pilz PKD, Schnitzler HU. Factors governing prepulse inhibition and prepulse facilitation
of the acoustic startle response in mice. Behav Brain Res. 2004; 152:403–412. [PubMed:
15196809]
Author Manuscript

Plappert CF, Pilz PKD. Long-term habituation of the startle response in mice evoked by acoustic and
tactile stimuli. Behavioural Brain Res. 2005; 162:307–310.
Plappert CF, Rodenbücher AM, Pilz PK. Effects of sex and estrous cycle on modulation of the acoustic
startle response in mice. Physiol Behav. 2005; 84:585–594. [PubMed: 15811394]
Plappert CF, Kuhn S, Schnitzler HU, Pilz PKD. Experience increases the prepulse inhibition of the
acoustic startle response in mice. Behav Neuro. 2006; 120:16–23.
Prokasy WF, Ebel HC. Three components of the classically conditioned GSR in human subjects. J
Exper Psych. 1967; 73:247–256.

Hear Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2019 June 01.


Longenecker et al. Page 22

Radziwon KE, Stolzberg DJ, Urban ME, Bowler RA, Salvi RJ. Salicylate-induced hearing loss and
gap detection deficits in rats. Front in Neurology. 2015; 6:1–10.
Author Manuscript

Reijmers LG, Peeters BW. Effects of acoustic prepulses on the startle reflex in rats: a parametric
analysis. Brain Res. 1994; 661:174–180. [PubMed: 7834367]
Ropp TJ, Tiedemann KL, Young ED, May BJ. Effects of unilateral acoustic trauma on tinnitus-related
spontaneous activity in the inferior colliculus. JARO. 2014; 15:1007–1022. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1007/s10162-014-0488-2. [PubMed: 25255865]
Rosen MJ, Sarro EC, Kelly JB, Sanes DH. Diminished behavioral and neural sensitivity to sound
modulation is associated with moderate developmental hearing loss. PLoS One. 2012;
7(7):e41514. [PubMed: 22848517]
Salloum RH, Sandridge S, Patton DJ, Stillitano G, Dawson G, Niforatos J, Santiago L, Kaltenbach JA.
Untangling the effects of tinnitus and hypersensitivity to sound (hyperacusis) in the gap detection
test. Hear Res. 2016; 331:92–100. [PubMed: 26520582]
Samuels ER, Hou RH, Langley RW, Szabadi E, Bradshaw CM. Modulation of the acoustic startle
response by the level of arousal: Comparison of Clonidine and Modafinil in healthy volunteers.
Neuropsychopharmacology. 2007; 32:2405–2421. [PubMed: 17342169]
Author Manuscript

Saper CB, Scammell TE, Lu J. Hypothalamic regulation of sleep and circadian rhythems. Nature.
2005; 437:1257–1263. [PubMed: 16251950]
Sarro EC, Sanes DH. Few juvenile auditory perceptual skills correlate with adult performance. Behav
Neurosci. 2014; 128:29–41. [PubMed: 24512063]
Schmajuk NA, Larrauri JA. Neural network model of prepulse inhibition. Behav Neurosci. 2005;
119:1546–1562. [PubMed: 16420158]
Singer W, Zuccotti A, Jaumann M, Lee SC, Panford-Walsh R, Xiong H, Zimmermann U, Franz C,
Geisler HS, Kopschall I, Rohbock K, Varakina K, Verpoorten S, Reinbothe T, Schimmang T,
Ruttiger L, Knipper M. Noise-induced inner hair cell ribbon loss disturbs central arc mobilization:
a novel molecular paradigm for understanding tinnitus. Mol Neurobiol. 2013; 47:261–279.
[PubMed: 23154938]
Stitt CL, Hoffman HS, Marsh R. Modification of the rat’s startle reaction by termination of antecedent
acoustic signals. J Comp Physiol Psych. 1973; 84:207–215.
Stitt CL, Hoffman HS, Marsh R, Boskoff KJ. Modification of the rat’s startle reaction by an antecedent
Author Manuscript

change in the acoustic environment. J Comp and Physiol Psych. 1974; 86:826–836.
Swerdlow NR, Geyer MA, Braff DL. Neural circuit regulation of prepulse inhibition of startle in the
rat: current knowledge and future challenges. Psychopharmacology. 2001; 156:194–215.
[PubMed: 11549223]
Swerdlow NR, Hartman PL, Auerbach PP. Changes in sensorimotor inhibition across the menstrual
cycle: Implications for neuropsychiatric disorders. Biol Psychiatry. 1997; 41:452–460. [PubMed:
9034539]
Turner JG, Brozoski TJ, Bauer CA, Parrish JL, Myers K, Hughes LF, Caspary DM. Gap detection
deficits in rats with tinnitus: a potential novel screening tool. Behav Neurosci. 2006; 120:188–195.
[PubMed: 16492129]
Valsamis B, Schmid S. Habituation and Prepulse Inhibition of Acoustic Startle in Rodents. J Vis Exp.
2011; 55:e3446.
Van den Busse M, Eikelis N. Estrogen increases prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle in rats.
European J of Pharmacol. 2001; 425:33–41. [PubMed: 11672572]
Willott JF, Carlson S, Chen H. Prepulse inhibition of the startle response in mice: Relationship to
Author Manuscript

hearing loss and auditory system plasticity. Behav Neurosci. 1994; 108:703–713. [PubMed:
7986364]
Willott JF, Carlson S. Modification of the acoustic startle response in hearing-impaired C57BL/6J
Mice: prepulse augmentation and prolongation of prepulse inhibition. Behav Neurosci. 1995;
109:396–403. [PubMed: 7662150]
Willott JF, Tanner L, O’Steen J, Johnson KR, Bogue MA, Gagnon L. Acoustic startle and prepulse
inhibition in 40 inbred strains of mice. Behav Neurosci. 2003; 117:716–727. [PubMed: 12931957]

Hear Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2019 June 01.


Longenecker et al. Page 23

Yu H, Patil KV, Han C, Fabella B, Canlon B, Someya S, Cederroth CR. GLAST deficiency in mice
exacerbates gap detection deficits in a model of salicylate-induced tinnitus. Front Behav Neuro.
Author Manuscript

2016; 10:1–12.
Yee BK, Chang T, Pietropaolo S, Feldon J. The expression of prepulse inhibition of the acoustic startle
reflex as a function of three pulse stimulus intensities, three prepulse stimulus intensities, and three
levels of startle responsiveness in C57BL6/J mice. Behav Brain Res. 2005; 163:265–276.
[PubMed: 16039728]
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript

Hear Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2019 June 01.


Longenecker et al. Page 24

Highlights
Author Manuscript

• The ASR is variable, and this variation can contribute to “messy” GPIAS
data.

• Careful consideration of ITI, sex, circadian, and sensory adaptation factors


can reduce ASR variation.

• Preceding gaps can inhibit and facilitate the startle response which needs to
be appropriately addressed in data analysis.

• The benefits and limitations of several GPIAS analyses are considered for
tinnitus assessment.
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript

Hear Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2019 June 01.


Longenecker et al. Page 25
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript

Figure 1. Within Session ASR variability


A–D. Four representative examples of ASR variability plotted as absolute startle magnitude
(mm) for Gap (grey) and No-gap (black) trials within one testing session. A. Constant Flux:
rapid and continual ASR fluctuations. B. State Change: distinct epochs of elevated or
Author Manuscript

depressed ASR. C. Habituation: ASR declines as a function of trial number. D. Minimal


Change: minimal ASR fluctuations. E. Variance (mm2) of each type of ASR behavior as
shown in A–D. F. Occurrence (% of all sessions analyzed; 294 sessions total from 14 Group
A mice) of each type of ASR behavior.
Author Manuscript

Hear Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2019 June 01.


Longenecker et al. Page 26
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript

Figure 2. ASR changes resulting from inter-trial interval


A. Absolute startle magnitude (mm) for long and short ITI GPIAS trials (seven mice in each
group) across three GPIAS sessions. Error bars represent SEM. B. Startle magnitude
Author Manuscript

variance (mm2) for long and short ITI GPIAS trials. C. Startle probability (% total trials) for
long and short ITI GPIAS trials. ** represents statistical significance at p ≤ 0.001.
Author Manuscript

Hear Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2019 June 01.


Longenecker et al. Page 27
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript

Figure 3. ASR changes resulting from circadian cycle


Author Manuscript

A. Absolute startle magnitude (mm) of both Gap (dashed lines) and No-gap- (solid lines) of
active (grey) and inactive (black) mice (7 mice in each group) across 21 GPIAS sessions.
Error bars represent SEM. B. Startle magnitude variance (mm2) for inactive and active mice.
C. Startle magnitude variance (mm2) for binned sessions across the 21 session experiment
for both inactive and active mice.

Hear Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2019 June 01.


Longenecker et al. Page 28
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript

Figure 4. ASR changes relating to sex variation


Author Manuscript

A. Absolute startle magnitude (mm) of both Gap (dashed lines) and No-gap (solid lines) of
male (black) and female (grey) mice (24 mice in each group) across ten GPIAS sessions.
Error bars represent SEM. B. Startle magnitude variance (mm2) for male and female mice.
C. Startle magnitude variance (mm2) for various stages of the estrus cycle for female mice.

Hear Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2019 June 01.


Longenecker et al. Page 29
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript

Figure 5. Gap-induced inhibition changes related to sensory adaptation


GPIAS ratio (Grand ratio and Modified ratio analysis) assessed across 21 sessions in 14
mice. Error bars represent SEM.
Author Manuscript

Hear Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2019 June 01.


Longenecker et al. Page 30
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript

Figure 6. Contributions of gap-induced inhibition and gap-induced facilitation


Author Manuscript

A. Absolute startle magnitude (mm) of Gap (orange) and No-gap trials (black) within a
segment GPIAS session. Blue box highlights the blocks with inhibition (mean of Gap trials
< mean No-Gap trials) and the orange box highlights the blocks with facilitation (mean of
Gap trials > mean No-Gap trials). B. Cumulative probability of daily GPIAS ratios (Gap/No-
gap) across all 21 sessions from 14 mice. Solid vertical line indicates the ratio one where the
gap detection was not evident. Values below one represent gap-induced inhibition, whereas

Hear Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2019 June 01.


Longenecker et al. Page 31

values above one gap-induced facilitation. Vertical black lines indicate borders of blocks
Author Manuscript

used for data collection.


Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript

Hear Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2019 June 01.


Longenecker et al. Page 32
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript

Figure 7. Five different analyses for GPIAS dataset


Data from a single GPIAS session obtained from an individual mouse, analyzed with five
different methods. A–D. Results of four different methods of data analysis utilizing ratios.
Dotted black horizontal line represents ratios of one where the gap detection was not
Author Manuscript

evident. Error bars represent SEM. E. Corresponding variance of ratios shown in A–D. F.
Cumulative probability of absolute startle magnitudes (mm) in response to Gap (green) and
No-gap (pink) trials. Separate panels show data from six background frequencies (8, 10,
12.5, 16, 20, and 25 kHz).

Hear Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2019 June 01.


Longenecker et al. Page 33
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript

Figure 8. Increasing size of GPIAS data set improves accuracy of gap detection assessment
Author Manuscript

GPIAS data analyzed with the Modified ratio and Grand ratio approaches, collected from a
representative mouse in multiple testing sessions and analyzed individually (A–D; Modified
ratio - black, Grand ratio - orange) or combined across multiple testing sessions (E). F for
Modified ratio and (G). H for Grand ratio. For both methods of analysis the combining data
from ten sessions reduces GPIAS ratio variability across different background frequencies
and reduces SEM values.

Hear Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2019 June 01.


Longenecker et al. Page 34
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript

Figure 9. GPIAS data analyses for tinnitus assessment


GPIAS data combined from ten baseline sessions and 5 months after sound exposure in a
representative mouse exhibiting a gap detection deficit at 25 kHz. A–D. GPIAS baseline
ratios (colored) and after (black) sound exposure obtained with four different ratio-based
data analyses. Stars indicate the frequencies where a significant gap detection deficit was
Author Manuscript

identified (p ≤ 0.05, unadjusted for multiple comparisons). E. Variances of ratios for


different methods shown in A–D (color matches A–D analyses). F. Changes in gap detection
performance caused by exposure assessed with the cumulative probability approach. Gap
detection performance was reduced at 25 kHz following sound exposure. The Gap and No-
gap distributions were significantly different at baseline (p < 0.001) but not after sound
exposure (p = 0.75) indicating gap detection deficit at 25 kHz (outlined by a rectangle).

Hear Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2019 June 01.


Longenecker et al. Page 35

Table 1

Startle magnitude and startle magnitude ratio variances.


Author Manuscript

Figure # Variable Comparisons Variances p-value


Figure 1 E Startle magnitude A vs B 2.253 vs 3.493 <0.001*
A vs C 2.252 vs 1.166 <0.001*
A vs D 2.252 vs 0.997 <0.001*
B vs C 3.493 vs 1.166 <0.001*
B vs D 3.493 vs 0.997
<0.001*
C vs D 1.166 vs 0.997
<0.001*

Figure 2 B Startle magnitude Long vs Short 2.881 vs 2.004 <0.001*

Figure 3 B Startle magnitude Inactive vs Active 1.973 vs 2.908 <0.001*

Figure 3 C Startle magnitude 1-5 inactive vs 6-10 inactive 2.965 vs 1.725 <0.001*
1-5 inactive vs 11-15 inactive 2.965 vs 0.970 <0.001*
1-5 inactive vs 16-21 inactive 2.965 vs 1.340 <0.001*
6-10 inactive vs 11-15inactive 1.725 vs 0.970 <0.001*
6-10 inactive vs 16-21 inactive 1.725 vs 1.340
<0.001*
11-15 inactive vs 16-21 inactive 0.970 vs 1.340
<0.001*
Author Manuscript

1-5 active vs 6-10 active 4.163 vs 2.267


1-5 active vs 11-15 active 4.163 vs 2.160 <0.001*
1-5 active vs 16-21 active 4.163 vs 2.098 <0.001*
6-10 active vs 11-15 active 2.267 vs 2.160 <0.001*
6-10 active vs 16-21 active 2.267 vs 2.098 0.001*
11-15 active vs 16-21 active 2.160 vs 2.098 0.027*
6-10 active vs 11-15 active 2.267 vs 2.160 0.213
6-10 active vs 16-21 active 2.267 vs 2.098 0.001*
11-15 active vs 16-21 active 2.160 vs 2.098 0.027*
0.213
Figure 4 B Startle magnitude Male vs Female 3.138 vs 5.116 <0.001*

Figure 4 C Startle magnitude P vs E 5.394 vs 3.755 <0.001*


P vs M 5.394 vs 5.803 <0.001*
P vs D 5.394 vs 4.406 <0.001*
E vs M 3.755 vs 5.803 <0.001*
E vs D 3.755 vs 4.406
<0.001*
M vs D 5.803 vs 4.406
<0.001*

Figure 7 E Ratio Standard ratio vs Modified ratio 0.269 vs 0.033 <0.001*


Author Manuscript

Standard ratio vs Best performance 0.269 vs 0.149 0.549


Standard ratio vs Grand ratio 0.269 vs 0.022 0.040*
Modified ratio vs Best performance 0.033 vs 0.149 0.003*
Modified ratio vs Grand ratio 0.033 vs 0.022 0.303
Best performance vs Grand ratio 0.149 vs 0.022 0.040*

Figure 9 E (baseline) Ratio Standard ratio vs Modified ratio 0.507 vs 0.051 <0.001*
Standard ratio vs Best performance 0.507 vs 0.209 0.002*
Standard ratio vs Grand ratio 0.507 vs 0.05 <0.001*
Modified ratio vs Best performance 0.051 vs 0.209 <0.001*
Modified ratio vs Grand ratio 0.051 vs 0.05 0.230
Best performance vs Grand ratio 0.209 vs 0.05
<0.001*

Pairwise variance comparisons were conducted with the Fligner–Killeen test of homogeneity of variances. Benjamini and Hochberg’s FDR
procedure was used to adjust the p-values where multiple comparisons were made.
Author Manuscript

Hear Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2019 June 01.


Longenecker et al. Page 36

Table 2

Major benefits and limitations of five different methods of GPIAS data analysis.
Author Manuscript

Analysis Type Benefits Limitations


May reduce an impact of ASR fluctuations Increased ratio due to averaging of blocks with
Standard Ratio (blocks)
inhibition and facilitation

May reduce an impact of ASR fluctuations, considers Overestimates gap-induced inhibition. Little is
Modified Ratio (blocks)
both inhibition and facilitation as gap detection understood about mechanism of facilitation

Best Performance Ratio May reduce an impact of ASR fluctuations, reduces the Smaller data set
(blocks) effect of facilitation

Does not depend on data collection paradigm, all raw Increased ratio due to facilitation inclusion
Grand Ratio
data are used

Includes all raw data, minimal data processing Does not account for temporal ASR
Cumulative Probability
fluctuations or facilitation
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript

Hear Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2019 June 01.

You might also like