Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 19

P1: GMX

Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review (CCFP) pp747-ccfp-460115 January 29, 2003 15:14 Style file version Nov. 07, 2000

Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, Vol. 6, No. 1, March 2003 (°
C 2003)

School Bonding in Children and Adolescents:


Conceptualization, Assessment, and Associated Variables

Samuel J. Maddox1,2 and Ronald J. Prinz1

School bonding refers to the “connections” that youth have with their schools and various
aspects of their academic lives. School bonding may be an important concept in prevention
because it has been linked to various developmental and adjustment outcomes. This paper
reviews conceptualizations, measurements, and theories of school bonding. Also considered
are empirical studies that have linked school bonding to a variety of outcomes (substance
use, delinquency, antisocial behavior, self-esteem). The review includes examination of how
school bonding serves as a mediator in these relations and, in turn, is moderated by other
variables. Despite inconsistencies in conceptualization and measurement, it is concluded that
school bonding is an important construct and an appropriate target for intervention. Recom-
mendations are offered regarding future research on school bonding, especially with respect
to positive developmental outcomes and examination of variables that might moderate school
bonding.
KEY WORDS: school bonding; children; adolescents; social development model; delinquency; substance
use.

This paper reviews theoretical and empirical Kleinman, Wish, Deren, & Rainone, 1986; Kumpfer
work on school bonding. Generally, school bond- & Turner, 1990–1991; Mcgee, 1992; Murguia, Chen, &
ing refers to the connection a student has with Kaplan, 1998; O’Donnell et al., 1995; Pilgrim, Abbey,
their school, the school personnel, and the aca- Hendrickson, & Lorenz, 1998; Simons-Morton et al.,
demic ideals espoused by the school. School bond- 1999; Valentine, Gottlieb, Keel, Griffith, & Ruthazer,
ing may be important in prevention for two signifi- 1998), school dropout (Cernkovich & Giordano, 1992;
cant reasons. First, theory and research have linked Crusto, 2000; Eggert et al., 1994; Keith, 1999), and teen
poor school bonding to a variety of life outcomes. pregnancy (Danziger, 1995).
Specifically, research has shown that students with A second consideration is that school bonding
poor school bonding have higher rates of delin- is a potentially malleable target for intervention. Re-
quency (Cernkovich & Giordano, 1992; Farnworth, search has shown that school environment and ex-
Schweinhart, & Beurueta-Clement, 1985; Figueira- periences are important factors in promoting or in-
McDonough, 1987; Free, 1994; Hirschi, 1969; Liska & hibiting school bonding (Eggert et al., 1994; Pilgrim
Reed, 1985; O’Donnell, Hawkins, & Abbott, 1995; et al., 1998). Programs that modify the school en-
Simons-Morton, Crump, Haynie, & Saylor, 1999; vironment may influence levels of school bonding
Wiatrowski & Anderson, 1987; Wiatrowski, Hansell, (Eggert & Kumpfer, 1997). Through modification of
Massey, & Wilson, 1982; Zhang & Messner, 1996), the school environment, the promotion of school
substance use (Eggert, Thompson, Herting, Nicholas, bonding has the potential to impact multiple prob-
& Dicker, 1994; Free, 1994; Hawkins et al., 1997; lem areas for an entire school population. Ide-
ally school bonding can be enhanced at a univer-
1 University sal level via school-wide programming (Berryhill &
of South Carolina, Columbia, South Carolina.
2 Address all correspondence to Samuel Maddox, Department Prinz, in press). Based on these reasons, a clear
of Psychology, University of South Carolina, Columbia, South understanding of the concept of school bonding,
Carolina 29208; e-mail: samjmaddox@aol.com. its links to other behaviors, and ways in which

31
1096-4037/03/0300-0031/0 °
C 2003 Plenum Publishing Corporation
P1: GMX
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review (CCFP) pp747-ccfp-460115 January 29, 2003 15:14 Style file version Nov. 07, 2000

32 Maddox and Prinz

it is influenced can contribute to the field of dimension include a sense of belonging, feeling pride
prevention. in the institution, and feeling safe and comfortable at
the institution.

WAYS SCHOOL BONDING IS DEFINED


Attachment to Personnel
Hirschi’s social control theory was one of the first
comprehensive conceptualizations of school bond- Another dimension is attachment to personnel,
ing (Hirschi, 1996). In the book entitled Causes of which refers to the interpersonal connections experi-
Delinquency, Hirschi (1969) discussed the different enced by youth as reflected in their respect, regard,
types of social bonds that individuals have with so- and admiration for the educators, staff, and adminis-
ciety including the family, peers, and school. He ini- tration (Cernkovich & Giordano, 1992). This dimen-
tially outlined four elements of social bonds: attach- sion encompasses the positive relationships students
ment, commitment, involvement, and belief (Hirschi, have at the institution. Cernkovich and Giordano
1969). This framework has been revised by others (1992) referred to this concept as attachment to teach-
(e.g., Cernkovich & Giordano, 1992; Wiatrowski & ers but it is expanded here to include administra-
Anderson, 1987) over time to fit theoretical paradigms tors, coaches, counselors, custodians, mentors, and
and to increase construct validity. other school personnel. Peers are not included be-
One change supported in the school bonding cause they represent a qualitatively different social
literature is the separation of attachment into two bond (Hirschi, 1969). Examples of attachment to per-
unique dimensions (Cernkovich & Giordano, 1992; sonnel include caring about school personnel, feel-
Murray & Greenberg, 2000, 2001), one focusing on ing supported by school personnel, and feeling un-
general school experiences and the other focusing on derstood by school personnel.
experiences with people in the school. Murray and
Greenberg (2000, 2001) conducted principal compo-
nent analyses and found that items referring to posi- School Commitment
tive school experiences loaded separately from items
referring to the teacher–student relationship. Sim- School commitment refers to personal invest-
ilarly, Cernkovich and Giordano (1992) conducted ment in school activities and the priority the school
factor analyses on their school bonding scale, which holds for youth. Highly committed students accept the
produced two unique attachment dimensions: attach- goals, norms, and morals of the school (Cernkovich
ment to school and attachment to teachers. These pa- & Giordano, 1992; Free, 1994; Hirschi, 1969; Mcgee,
pers provide empirical evidence that Hirschi’s orig- 1992; Simons-Morton et al., 1999; Wiatrowski &
inal dimension of attachment actually contains two Anderson, 1987). This dimension is a combination of
unique dimensions. Accordingly, this review focuses beliefs (Hirschi, 1969) and behaviors: beliefs about
on the dimensions attachment to school, attachment future achievement and getting good grades, and be-
to personnel, school commitment, and school involve- haviors such as homework completion and studying
ment. The construct of school belief is subsumed un- (Cernkovich & Giordano, 1992).
der school commitment because of low empirical sup-
port as an independent dimension (Cernkovich &
Giordano, 1992; Williams, 1994). School Involvement

School involvement is defined by behavioral


Attachment to School participation in school activities, which is usually
represented in terms of behavioral frequencies (e.g.,
One of the dimensions of school bonding is at- number of football games attended, or number of
tachment to school, which refers to feelings about the hours in extracurricular activities) and endorsement
school and the degree to which youth care about it of involvement (e.g., club membership). It repre-
(Cernkovich & Giordano, 1992). This dimension ap- sents a temporal component of school attachment
plies only to a youth’s feelings about the actual insti- (Cernkovich & Giordano, 1992). Although attach-
tution itself and not to feelings about the educators, ment refers to the affective connection a student
staff, or administrators, at the school. Feelings in this has to the institution, school involvement refers
P1: GMX
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review (CCFP) pp747-ccfp-460115 January 29, 2003 15:14 Style file version Nov. 07, 2000

School Bonding in Children 33

to a behavioral or demonstrable connection to the Attitude toward school is another construct that
institution. is similar to school bonding. Meier and McDaniel
(1974) developed a measure of attitude toward
school and divided the construct into attitude to-
Differentiation From Related Constructs ward schoolwork, attitude toward teachers, and at-
titude toward school in general. Similarly, the Be-
Related to school bonding are several concepts, havior Assessment System for Children (Reynolds
such as achievement motivation, motivation to learn & Kamphaus, 1998) contains two scales within its
and attitude toward school. Achievement motivation school maladjustment factors that are reflective of
is defined as an individual’s desire to excel (Balk, attitude toward school. The scales are called “at-
1995; Grotevant & Thorbecke, 1982; Santrock, 1996; titude toward school” and “attitude toward teach-
Woolfolk, 1995). It can be broken down into four ers.” The “attitude toward school” scale measures
components (Accordino, Accordino, & Slaney, 2000; the youth’s opinion about the utility of school and
Grotevant & Thorbecke, 1982). First is mastery, which his/her comfort with school matters (Reynolds &
is a preference for difficult and challenging tasks. Sec- Kamphaus, 1998). The “attitude toward teachers”
ond is competitiveness, which is the desire to prevail scale indicates the youth’s level of regard for teachers
over others. Third is work, which refers to the desire (Reynolds & Kamphaus, 1998). Based on both con-
to exert effort to achieve. Finally, personal unconcern ceptualizations of the construct (Meier & McDaniel,
is the lack of concern about the approval of others. 1974; Reynolds & Kamphaus, 1998), there are sig-
Although the last component might seem negative, it nificant points of overlap between school bonding
is important because it reduces fear of failure or em- and attitude toward school. Attitude toward school
barrassment, which means that the individual takes contains similar elements as attachment to school,
more risks and has more opportunities to achieve. school commitment, and attachment to personnel,
Although there are some similarities between but it lacks a behavioral component such as school
the concepts of school bonding and achievement mo- involvement.
tivation, they are distinct. School bonding is more
comprehensive focusing on the student’s affect (at-
tachment), cognition (commitment), and behavior THEORIES OF SCHOOL BONDING
(involvement), whereas achievement motivation fo-
cuses mainly on preferences and desires. The settings Control Theory
for the school bonding versus achievement motivation
are also different. Although achievement motivation Social control theory or social bonding theory
can apply to many different settings (i.e., work, school, was originally proposed by Hirschi (1969) to explain
sports, etc.), school bonding applies only to the school the causes of delinquency. The prevailing crimino-
itself. Thus, in regards to focus and settings, school logical theories at that time focused on what moti-
bonding and achievement motivation can be viewed vated individuals to violate laws. Social control the-
as distinct constructs. ory took an opposite approach by focusing on what
Another related concept is student motivation prevented individuals from violating laws. The first
to learn. This can be defined as a tendency to find approach assumes that individuals are moral beings
academic activities worthwhile and a desire to gain that are pushed into deviant acts, whereas the latter
knowledge from them (Brophy, 1988). Motivation to asserts that individuals are inherently immoral beings
learn includes components such as planning, focus- that are restrained from deviant acts by some method
ing, identifying the knowledge desired, active search of control. Hirschi (1969) conceptualized this method
for new information, pride in achievement, clear per- of control as bonds to conventional society. He ar-
ceptions of feedback, and an absence of fear of failure gued that the only reason people go against their in-
(Brophy, 1988). Motivation to learn is related perhaps herent immoral nature and conform to the rules of
to school commitment, but it does not adequately in- society is because of bonds they have with either a
corporate the other components of school bonding person or an institution that promotes conformity.
such as attachment to school and personnel and school When people are poorly bonded or bonded to per-
involvement. The inclusion of these other dimen- sons or institutions (gangs) that do not support con-
sions sets school bonding apart from motivation to formity then they will act upon their inherent immoral
learn. tendencies.
P1: GMX
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review (CCFP) pp747-ccfp-460115 January 29, 2003 15:14 Style file version Nov. 07, 2000

34 Maddox and Prinz

Conceptualization of School Bonding control (Evans et al., 1997; Gottfredson & Hirschi,
1990). This difference has clear implications for pre-
As noted earlier, school bonding as conceptual- vention (Junger & Tremblay, 1999; Zhang, Welte, &
ized by Hirschi (1969) contained the dimensions of Wieczorek, 2002). Social control theory suggests that
attachment, commitment, beliefs, and involvement. interventions promoting social bonds are appropriate
More than in other theories, school bonding plays for the prevention of deviant behavior (Hirschi, 1969).
a central role in control theory. First, control theory In contrast, Gottfredson and Hirschi (1990), based on
offered the first comprehensive conceptualization of their general theory of crime, recommend preventive
school bonding. Secondly, control theory identified interventions that promote self-control or remove the
the lack of school bonding as a major cause for de- appeal of deviant behavior that easily tempts individ-
viant behavior. uals with low self-control.

Revision of Social Control Theory Reconciliation of Hirschi’s Two Theories

Control theory has been expanded over the years Although Hirschi abandoned his original social
and has enjoyed successful integrations with other control theory in favor of a general theory of crime
theories making it more inclusive and theoretically also referred to as self-control theory (Gottfredson &
heuristic (Akers & Cochran, 1985; Sommers, Fagan, Hirschi, 1990), the two theories are not completely in-
& Baskin, 1994). One major revision was the inclusion compatible (Pratt & Cullen, 2000; Taylor, 2001). Even
of self-control as a restraining force (Gottfredson & Gottfredson and Hirschi (1990) recognized that direct
Hirschi, 1990; Taylor, 2001). In this version of control social control (attachments to parents, teachers, etc.)
theory, called self-control theory, poor school bonding can promote self-control through monitoring, rein-
is not seen as a causal factor for delinquency, but as an forcement, and punishment (Pratt & Cullen, 2000).
outcome of low self-control (Gottfredson & Hirschi, Based on this reasoning external and internal con-
1990). Specifically, self-control theory asserts that low trols interact to promote or prevent deviant behavior
self-control, characterized by impulsivity, a lack of (Taylor, 2001).
sensitivity to others, risk-seeking, and poor decision-
making skills, causes an individual to engage in de-
viant behavior. In addition, Gottfredson and Hirschi Empirical Support for Control Theory
(1990) suggest that such individuals are unable to form
strong social bonds due to low self-control. There- There has been widespread empirical support
fore, a student with low self-control will not only ex- for social control theory. Cernkovich and Giordano
hibit deviant behavior but also have difficulty bond- (1992) examined how school bonding affected delin-
ing to school (Gottfredson & Hirschi, 1990; Nagin & quent behaviors of African American and Caucasian
Paternoster, 1993; Taylor, 2001). Therefore, the rela- youth in schools with different racial composition.
tion between poor school bonding and deviant behav- Their results were generally supportive of control
ior is said to be spurious—both are manifestations of theory and suggest that it is invariant across racial
low self-control. (Evans, Cullen, Burton, Dunaway, & boundaries. Wade and Brannigan (1998) tested con-
Benson, 1997; Gottfredson & Hirschi, 1990; Nagin & trol theory constructs on adolescent risk-taking be-
Paternoster, 1993; Paternoster & Brame, 2000; Pratt haviors and found support for the inhibitory effect of
& Cullen, 2000; Taylor, 2001). prosocial bonds. High levels of bonding to prosocial
others led to decreases in risk-taking behavior.

Difference in School Bonding


Integration With Other Theories
The two versions of control theory differ consid-
erably in their conceptualization of the role school Some studies have supported an integration of
bonding plays in deviant behavior. Social control the- control theory variables with variables from social
ory asserts that poor school bonding contributes to de- learning theory. According to social learning the-
viant behavior whereas self-control theory asserts that ory, deviant behavior is learned and maintained be-
poor school bonding is a manifestation of poor self- cause it is reinforced through rewards from the
P1: GMX
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review (CCFP) pp747-ccfp-460115 January 29, 2003 15:14 Style file version Nov. 07, 2000

School Bonding in Children 35

social environment (Williams, 1994). This process is cial others inhibit delinquency and problem behav-
referred to as socialization. Sommers et al. (1994) iors. It goes beyond social control theory by identi-
tested an integrated model of Puerto Rican delin- fying three factors that are essential in establishing
quency that included the tenets of control theory social bonds: opportunities for involvement, skills for
(i.e., social bonding), social learning theory (social- involvement, and reinforcement for the involvement
ization), and the sociocultural variables acculturation (Catalano & Hawkins, 1996; Hawkins et al., 1992;
and familism. Their control theory variables consisted Hawkins & Lishner, 1987; O’Donnell et al., 1995).
of family attachment, school commitment, and con- Opportunities for involvement refers to the youth’s
ventional beliefs. Association with delinquent peers potential to interact with others through the avenue
was their social learning theory variable. Their sam- of activities, clubs, etc. Skills for involvement refers
ple consisted of 1,077 Puerto Rican males between to social skills that allow the youth to establish and
the ages of 11 and 19 (mean = 15.6 years) residing maintain social relationships. Finally, reinforcement
in the South Bronx. They found significant effects for involvement refers to rewards and incentives pre-
on reports of delinquency for both control theory sented to the youth for initiating and maintaining
and social learning theory variables. Specifically fam- involvement in a particular social unit (Hawkins &
ily attachment, school commitment, and conventional Lishner, 1987). The assumption in the social develop-
beliefs were negatively associated with delinquency, ment model is that involvement is the key dimension
whereas delinquent peer association had a positive re- that influences bonding. In the model, involvement is
lationship to delinquency. Sommers et al. (1994) con- a prerequisite for establishing social bonds. Without
cluded that control and social learning theories were a sufficient level of involvement, the individual will
complementary. not be able to establish a bond within that social unit
Akers and Cochran (1985) also supported this (Hawkins & Lishner, 1987).
view in their study comparing social learning theory, In addition to identifying factors necessary for es-
control theory, and strain theory. Strain theory as- tablishing bonds, the social development model also
serts that delinquency occurs when youth are unable extends control theory by attempting to explain sub-
to achieve their goals through legitimate channels. stance use and other antisocial behaviors (Catalano
Akers and Cochran (1985) chose dependent variables & Hawkins, 1996; Catalano, Kosterman, Hawkins,
for each theory and modeled them for adolescent mar- Newcomb, & Abbott, 1996; Hawkins et al., 1992,
ijuana use. Their results showed moderate support for 1997; Hawkins & Lishner, 1987). The model also
control theory as an independent model (explaining offers a clear developmental perspective identifying
30% of variance). The unique variance of control the- unique processes for preschool, elementary, middle,
ory disappeared, however, when combined with the and high school youth (Catalano & Hawkins, 1996).
social learning model. They suggested that the sim- These unique processes are discussed in a later sec-
ilarities between the constructs in control and social tion. Finally, the social development model exam-
learning theories might explain these results. Minimal ines protective as well as risk factors (Catalano &
support was found for strain theory. Hawkins, 1996). These differences allow the social
development model to emerge as an integrated the-
ory that captures essential elements of past theories
Developmental Considerations in criminology (especially control and social learning
theories).
Control theory in its original conceptualization
was most applicable to understanding adolescent de-
viant behavior. It lacked developmental considera- Conceptualization of School Bonding
tions about how bonds progressed from childhood
through adolescence. These considerations were later Because of its linkages to control theory, school
addressed by another prominent theory. bonding also plays a major role in the social develop-
ment model, however, the conceptualization is differ-
ent from Hirschi (1969). In control theory, the social
Social Development Model bond consists of attachment, commitment, involve-
ment, and belief. In the social development model,
The social development model is an extension of involvement is no longer conceptualized as part of the
control theory that also asserts that bonds to proso- social bond but is seen as a mechanism for establishing
P1: GMX
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review (CCFP) pp747-ccfp-460115 January 29, 2003 15:14 Style file version Nov. 07, 2000

36 Maddox and Prinz

social bonds. As a result, involvement is not viewed as As the children enter school (elementary school
a dimension of school bonding; rather, it is viewed as a period), both the family and the school are impor-
precursor. Thus, the school bond only includes attach- tant socializing forces that teach the youth skills nec-
ment, commitment, and belief (Catalano & Hawkins, essary for his/her next major social unit, peers. It is
1996). An additional distinction between control the- during the middle school period where the peer social
ory and the social development model is that control unit greatly increases. During this stage, several forces
theory only focused on adolescent behavior whereas (family, school, and peers) are exerting influences on
the social development model provides a framework the youth (Catalano & Hawkins, 1996). Although the
from preschool through adolescence. influence of one social unit will impact the others (e.g.,
prosocial family bonds could lead to prosocial school
bonds, which could both lead to prosocial peer bonds),
Empirical Support for the Social Development Model this relationship is not invariant.
Middle school is a major social transition in which
Like control theory, there is widespread support opportunities for both prosocial and antisocial in-
for the social development model. Catalano et al. volvement increases. There is a possibility that family,
(1996) examined the utility of the social development school, and peer bonds can conflict with each other.
model constructs measured at age 13–14 to predict A middle school student with prosocial family and
17–18-year-old substance use. Results confirmed the school bonds could still have antisocial peer bonds
hypothesized relationship between prosocial bond- due to the increased opportunities for interactions
ing and 17–18-year-old substance use. The analyses with antisocial peers. This suggests that relying upon
suggested that prosocial bonds might change slowly strong family and school bonds during earlier peri-
through experiences of opportunities, involvement, ods to lay the foundation to prevent later antisocial
and rewards (Catalano et al., 1996). These results sup- behavior without maintaining them through later de-
port the validity of social development model con- velopmental periods is not sufficient. The importance
structs in substance use. Similarly, Ayers et al. (1999) of family, school, and now, peer bonds continue to re-
evaluated the social development model constructs main important throughout high school (Catalano &
and found that they were significantly associated with Hawkins, 1996).
changes in delinquency and, O’Donnell et al. (1995) During the high school period, peers and com-
tested constructs from the social development model munity exert their strongest influence as a socializ-
on delinquency and substance use and confirmed its ing force. Opportunities for interaction with peers
utility in explaining both. Overall, there is strong sup- and community members increase significantly due
port emerging for the social development model as to increased independence during high school. High
an explanation of deviant behavior superior even to school students date, have access to cars, have a later
control theory (Williams, Ayer, Abbot, Hawkins, & curfew, and receive less supervision than they do in
Catalano, 1999). earlier stages of development, which all have the po-
tential to increase their exposure to and interaction
with antisocial peers and adults. However, family and
Developmental Considerations school bonds continue to serve as socializing forces
and it is important to promote them throughout high
Although control theory is unclear about de- school (Catalano & Hawkins, 1996).
velopmental issues, the social development model
explicitly states its developmental considerations.
The model asserts that each developmental period Other Theories and Perspectives
provides different opportunities for social involve-
ment (Catalano & Hawkins, 1996; Hawkins & Lish- Other theories recognizing the importance of
ner, 1987). Because of developmental limitations, school bonding have emerged in the literature.
preschool children have fewer opportunities to be in- Kumpfer and Turner (1990–91) developed the social
volved in school and peer social units (Catalano & ecology model to explain adolescent substance abuse.
Hawkins, 1996). During the preschool years, the fam- This model extends the social development model by
ily unit is the most important socializing force and including environmental factors such as family cli-
allows children to develop the skills necessary for in- mate and school climate and individual factors such
teraction in other social units. as self-esteem and self-efficacy (Kumpfer & Turner,
P1: GMX
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review (CCFP) pp747-ccfp-460115 January 29, 2003 15:14 Style file version Nov. 07, 2000

School Bonding in Children 37

1990–1991). Fraser (1996) outlined an ecological- Effective School Battery


developmental perspective that is similar to the
social development model, but emphasizes the recip- Gottfredson (1984) developed two standardized
rocal nature of social interaction skills and opportuni- scales that assess school bonding, the Effective School
ties for involvement. Fraser (1996) argued that social Battery and What About You scales. The Effective
skills are not only affected by external exogenous fac- School Battery (ESB; Gottfredson, 1984) is a 118-item
tors, hypothesized in the social development model, measure of school climate and its outcomes. The scales
but are also affected by opportunities for prosocial of the ESB fall into two categories: measures of psy-
involvement (Fraser, 1996). For example, elementary chosocial climate and measures of population char-
school students who are aggressive may not be al- acteristics. There is a separate inventory for teach-
lowed to play with certain peers, may be suspended ers and students. The scales of the ESB that contain
from school, and may be excluded from activities school bonding related items are located within the
with nonimmediate family members (e.g., not allowed student inventory and include Educational Expecta-
to stay overnight at a relative’s house). These youth tion, Social Integration, Attachment to School, Belief
will then lose opportunities to develop social skills in Rules, and School Effort. Attachment to School and
through their interactions with prosocial peers, teach- Social Integration capture the affective dimensions of
ers, and relatives. Based on this perspective, the posi- school bonding. Educational Expectation and Belief
tive, socializing forces available to these youths have in Rules capture the commitment dimension. School
been removed because of their antisocial behavior Effort captures both school involvement and commit-
(Fraser, 1996). ment. Thus, the ESB captures, at some level, all four
dimensions of school bonding.
The 1-year retest reliabilities of the ESB are
modest. Educational Expectation had reliabilities of
MEASUREMENT OF SCHOOL BONDING .48 and .41 for males and females, respectively. The
retest reliabilities for Social Integration were some-
Scales and Procedures what lower: .33 for males and .39 for females. Attach-
ment to School had a retest reliability of .53 for males
Hirschi (1969) originally conceptualized school and .46 for females. The retest reliabilities for Belief
bonding as a multidimensional construct. Cernkovich in Conventional Rules were .38 and .40 for males and
and Giordano (1992) operationalize school bond- females, respectively. Finally School Effort produced
ing as a combination of attachment, performance, a retest reliability of .46 for males and .40 for females
involvement, and commitment; however, many re- (Gottfredson, 1984).
searchers only use one or two variables to rep-
resent the construct. This practice creates a prob-
lem in school bonding research because inadequate What About You Scales
measurement can lead to erroneous, if not null
findings. The What About You (WAY) scales
A number of measurement methods have (Gottfredson & Gottfredson, 1999) measures
been used for school bonding including self-report risk and protective factors for problem behavior,
(Murray & Greenberg, 2000, 2001), teacher report substance use, and general well-being via student
(Pianta & Steinberg, 1992), and structured interviews report. The scales that capture school bonding
(Danziger, 1995). Self-report is the most often used dimensions are Attachment to School, Commit-
method in the assessment of school bonding. More ment to Education, Belief in Rules, Educational
common self-report measures include the Effective Self-efficacy Expectations, and Social Integration.
School Battery (Gottfrdson, 1984), the What About The WAY scales sufficiently assess attachment to
You scales (Gottfredson & Gottfedson, 1999), an school and personnel and commitment to educa-
adaptation of the People In My life Scale (Murray tion, but they do not adequately capture school
& Greenberg, 2000, 2001), and the Psychological involvement. Although school involvement is not
Sense of School Membership Scale (Goodenow, assessed, the WAY scales seem to do well in cap-
1993). Discussion of the scales will focus on the di- turing the richness and complexity of other school
mensions assessed and the depth and complexity of bonding dimensions. The WAY scales have shown
assessment. adequate internal consistency (see Gottfredson &
P1: GMX
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review (CCFP) pp747-ccfp-460115 January 29, 2003 15:14 Style file version Nov. 07, 2000

38 Maddox and Prinz

Gottfredson, 1999). Based on the conceptualization Goodenow (1993) found significant effects for rat-
and psychometric properties, the ESB and WAY ings of social standing on the PSSM, which offers
scales have potential as a measure of school bonding; some support for the construct validity of the mea-
however, this is not the major aim of these scales. sure. Like the adapted PIML measure, the PSSM lacks
items reflecting involvement and commitment. Be-
cause her purpose was to only measure belongingness,
Adapted People In My Life Scale Goodenow (1993) did not include such items. There-
fore, the psychometric properties of the PSSM suggest
Murray and Greenberg (2000, 2001) adapted that it is a reliable and valid measure of school belong-
the existing People In My Life scale (PIML; Cook, ing but its absence of key dimensions (involvement
Greenberg, & Kusche, 1995) to assess school bond- and commitment) limits its potential as a measure of
ing in fifth and sixth grade students. Their adapta- school bonding.
tion of the measure produced four factors: Affiliation
with Teachers (α = .88), Dissatisfaction with Teacher
(α = .66), Bonds with School (α = .80), and School Improving the Measurement of School Bonding
Dangerousness (α = .60). Their studies found that
the adapted measure had significant correlations with Wide variations in measurement of school bond-
outcomes hypothesized to be related to school bond- ing make it difficult to evaluate the research. A large
ing (i.e., antisocial behavior and delinquency). These part of these differences arises from the fact that there
relationships suggest good construct validity for the are few measures devoted exclusively to school bond-
adapted PIML. ing. Its measurement is usually derived from items
Based on its psychometric properties the adapted that are part of a larger measure. Consequently, the
PIML (Murray & Greenberg, 2000, 2001) is a poten- focus has been on assessing an aspect of school bond-
tially good measure of school bonding, however, there ing as part of a larger matrix of constructs rather than
is some concern about how it taps the full construct on fully assessing school bonding per se.
of school bonding. The four factors that emerged dur- A major improvement would be the creation and
ing the analyses of the scale only reflect the attach- validation of a measure devoted primarily to school
ment dimensions of school bonding. The involvement bonding. Although this task is large and most re-
and commitment dimensions, which have received searchers would rather use part of an existing mea-
both theoretical and empirical support (Cernkovich sure, the creation of a reliable and valid school bond-
& Giordano, 1992), are not included. The absence of ing measure would potentially improve the quality of
these dimensions reduces enthusiasm for the PIML research in this area through more comprehensive as-
as a comprehensive measure of school bonding. sessment.
Another way to improve school-bonding mea-
surement would be to develop age-related measures.
The Psychological Sense of School Membership Scale Presumably, preadolescent facets of school bonding
differ developmentally from school bonding in ado-
The final measure reviewed here is the Psycho- lescents. Ideally, measures would be robust across age
logical Sense of School Membership scale (PSSM; groups, but it is more likely that separate measures
Goodenow, 1993). The measure was created out of are needed to capture developmental/contextual dif-
the concern about the lack of psychometrically sound ferences.
measures of school belonging. School belonging is
defined as the degree to which a student feels ac-
cepted and included within the school (Goodenow, BEHAVIORAL OUTCOMES OF SCHOOL
1993). Goodenow (1993) piloted the PSSM on a sam- BONDING
ple of suburban middle school students. In a second
phase, she administered the measure to two urban Substance Use
junior high schools. Based on analyses from these
two phases the PSSM was reduced to 18 items re- School bonding, or lack thereof, may have a re-
flecting perceived liking, personal acceptance, inclu- lation to youth substance use (e.g., alcohol use, smok-
sion, and teacher and peer responses. Reliabilities ing, marijuana, LSD, cocaine). Based on control the-
of the PSSM ranged from .77 to .88 across samples. ory and the social development model, high levels
P1: GMX
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review (CCFP) pp747-ccfp-460115 January 29, 2003 15:14 Style file version Nov. 07, 2000

School Bonding in Children 39

of school bonding are expected to delay the initi- been many studies that have continued to test this as-
ation and reduce the likelihood of substance use. sociation. Cernkovich and Giordano (1992) examined
Throughout the literature, there is consistent support how school bonding affected delinquent behaviors
for this notion (Catalano et al., 1996; Eggert et al., of African American youth in schools with different
1994; Eggert & Kumpfer, 1997; Hawkins et al., 1997; racial composition. The authors used a neighborhood
O’Donnell et al., 1995). School bonding has even been sample because students in school are more likely to
the focus of some drug prevention programs. have higher levels of school bonding than those who
A study by Eggert et al. (1994) confirmed do not attend school regularly, have dropped out, or
the hypothesized relation between school bonding who are in institutions. Based on a sample of 942 youth
and substance use. They attempted to prevent sub- ages 12–19 (51% female, 45% White) it was found that
stance use and school dropout through an intensive school commitment, school involvement, and attach-
school based program with 259 subjects from four ment to school were significant predictors of delin-
urban Northwest High Schools. The experimental quency (Cernkovich & Giordano, 1992).
group (n = 101) completed a one-semester personal Farnworth et al. (1985) examined data from the
growth program that promoted social network sup- Ypsilanti Perry Preschool Project (PPP) that was de-
port and school bonding. Data collected from students signed to reduce putative risk factors (e.g., academic
at preintervention, 5-month postintervention, and a failure, lack of school bonds, etc.) for African Amer-
10-month follow-up showed that school bonding ican children ages 3 and 4. The purpose of the study
steadily increased in the experimental group. Results was to test the assumption by Hirschi (1969) that low
also showed a reduction in drug use and a significant ability decreases school success and, in turn, school
decrease in drug control problems and consequences. bonding, which subsequently results in delinquency.
Such findings support the hypothesized relation be- IQ was assessed prior to entry in the preschool inter-
tween school bonding and substance use (Eggert vention. School experiences (academic achievement
et al., 1994). Further support for the relation of school and school bonding variables) were assessed through-
bonding to substance use comes from Murguia et al. out elementary school. Delinquency, assessed when
(1998) who found that perception of teacher’s accep- participants were 15, was combined into four factors:
tance had a significant, negative correlation with prior dishonesty, escape, aggression, and group/gang activ-
drug involvement, drug using peers, and recent drug ity. The results of the study revealed that attachment
involvement for non-Hispanic Whites. to school had its strongest relation to escape behavior
Evaluation of the social development model has whereas school commitment was related to dishon-
also produced support for a relation between school est and aggressive delinquency. Contrary to Hirschi’s
bonding and substance use (Catalano et al., 1996; hypothesis (Hirschi, 1969), this relationship was inde-
Hawkins et al., 1997; O’Donnell et al., 1995; Williams pendent of academic failure (Farnworth et al., 1985).
et al., 1999). Williams et al. (1999) found that school In addition, Farnworth et al. (1985) found that higher
bonding had an indirect effect on substance use preschool IQ was related to higher levels of delin-
through academic and social skills. Catalano et al. quency at age 15. These results suggest that school
(1996) and Hawkins et al. (1997) confirmed a rela- bonding is important in explaining delinquency inde-
tionship between school bonding and substance use at pendent of IQ.
age 17–18. O’Donnell et al. (1995) found that a higher Beyond substance abuse, the Seattle Social De-
level of school bonding was associated with lack of velopment Project also produced support for the
involvement in substance use. Other support for the relation between school bonding and delinquency
importance of school bonding in substance use comes (Ayers et al., 1999; O’Donnell et al., 1995). Ayers
from the social ecology model (Kumpfer & Turner, et al. (1999) found that school bonding was signifi-
1990–1991), the Family In Action program (Pilgrim cantly associated with changes in delinquency regard-
et al., 1998), and Monitoring the Future data (Bryant, less of gender. Their study consisted of 566 partici-
Schulenberg, Bachman, O’Malley, & Johnston, 2000). pants (male = 52%) including White (48%), African
American (23%), Asian American (18%), and other
ethnic groups (9%). Data collected at ages 12–13 and
Delinquency 14–15 showed that participants who desisted in crime
from Time 1 to Time 2 were more strongly bonded in
Since the relation of school bonding to delin- regards to attachment and commitment. O’Donnell,
quency was first specified (Hirschi, 1969), there have et al. (1995) found, as with substance use, higher
P1: GMX
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review (CCFP) pp747-ccfp-460115 January 29, 2003 15:14 Style file version Nov. 07, 2000

40 Maddox and Prinz

levels of school bonding were associated with a lack quency. Liska and Reed (1985) also examined the
of involvement in delinquent behavior. Support for Youth in Transition study (Time 1 and Time 2) using a
this relation has been found in other studies as well sample of 1,886 respondents (White = 82%, African
(Figueira-McDonough, 1983; Sommers et al., 1994; American = 12%). They found that, for Caucasians,
Zhang & Messner, 1996). parental attachment affected delinquency more than
In contrast to the substance abuse literature, the school bonding. Thus for Caucasians, school bonding
delinquency literature has more conflicting findings. appeared to be more of an outcome of delinquency
Hoffmann and Miller (1998) collected data via the than a predictor. For African Americans in the sam-
Family Health Study with 795 youth, ages 11–17, ple, delinquency was an outcome of school bonding
examining negative life events, family attachments, and parental attachment.
school performance, self-efficacy, self-esteem, and Despite the conflicting findings, the hypothesis
delinquency (property and violent offenses). Signif- about the importance of school bonding in delin-
icant paths between school bonding and delinquency quency might remain tenable for a few reasons.
did not emerge, nor did their exclusion change the First, the studies were not comparable in the way
parameters of the model challenging the hypothesis school bonding was conceptualized or measured,
that school bonding has a significant negative relation which might have accounted for inconsistent patterns.
with delinquency (Hoffmann & Miller, 1998). Second, although sometimes modest, a relationship
Data from the Youth in Transition study also between school bonding and delinquency appears to
challenge the expected relation. The Youth in Tran- be present. Third, the measures used to assess school
sition study is a nationally representative stratified bonding, often relying on a small number of items,
sample of high school males. Initial data (Time 1) may have insufficiently captured the breadth of school
was collected on 2,213 White and Black males en- bonding. A continued focus on the relation between
tering 10th grade. Four subsequent waves of inter- delinquency and school bonding with careful atten-
views were conducted at 11th grade (Time 2), prior tion to measurement and conceptualization is recom-
to graduation (Time 3), 1-year postgraduation (Time mended.
4), and 5 years after graduation (Time 5; Wiatrowski
et al., 1982). Data collected included academic and
ability tests, reports of delinquent behavior, measures Academic Performance
of school experiences, self-perception, values, expec-
tations, and demographic information. Academic performance is an important outcome
Using data from the first four waves of the study, to consider because of its predictive relations with
Wiatrowski et al. (1982) examined the effect of cur- other life outcomes (Farnworth et al., 1985; Figueira-
riculum tracking on delinquency. In their model self- McDonough, 1983; Fraser, 1996). Hirschi (1969) hy-
esteem, academic achievement, expectations, and at- pothesized that school bonding has a direct effect on
tachment to school at Time 1 were predicted to affect achievement. Testing of a substance use model by
curriculum placement at Time 2. Curriculum place- Williams et al. (1999) revealed that school bonding
ment at Time 2 should affect academic achievement, was directly related to academic skills. Wiatrowski
self-esteem, and attachment to school at Time 3, which et al. (1982) supported this hypothesis in their re-
should in turn affect Delinquency at Time 3 and Time search examining the effect of curriculum tracking in
4. Their results only marginally supported the rela- the Youth in Transition study. They tested a structural
tion between attachment to school and delinquency. model of curriculum influences on achievement using
One possible explanation offered is that many of the a variety of interview and survey instruments. School
observed variables may have had their effect before bonding and academic performance (grades) were as-
Time 1 or 10th grade (Wiatrowski et al., 1982). There- sessed in 10th grade and again in 12th grade. School
fore, the significant but modest relations that were bonding in 10th grade was significantly related to aca-
obtained might have been stronger if assessed earlier demic performance in 12th grade, which supports a
or perhaps with a cross-sequential design. predictive relation between school bonding and aca-
Wiatrowski and Anderson (1987) attempted to demic performance.
model social bonds and adolescent delinquency using Marchant, Paulson, and Rothlisberg (2001)
data from only the first wave of the Youth in Transition found that teacher responsiveness, teacher control,
study (Time 1; 2,213 males), but school bonding was and school responsiveness were related to aca-
again found to be minimally useful in predicting delin- demic competence, motivation, and achievement. The
P1: GMX
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review (CCFP) pp747-ccfp-460115 January 29, 2003 15:14 Style file version Nov. 07, 2000

School Bonding in Children 41

variables teacher responsiveness and teacher control academic performance, and cigarette use in 8th–12th
contained items that were reflective of the teacher– grade. Monitoring the Future is a nationally repre-
student relationship (i.e., attachment to teacher). sentative survey project that has collected data on
School responsiveness referred to the student’s per- 12th grade students since 1975. In the early 1990s the
ception of their school as a nurturing environment. project expanded to collect data from 8th and 10th
Their path model revealed that teacher responsive- grade students as well (Bryant et al., 2000).
ness had direct effects on academic competence Bryant et al. (2000) found that antisocial behav-
and perceived motivation and an indirect effect ior in eighth grade was the most significant predic-
on achievement through competence and motiva- tor leading to decreased school bonding and aca-
tion. School responsiveness had an indirect effect on demic performance, and increased cigarette use in
achievement through motivation. Teacher control had later grades. Similarly Blankmeyer et al. (2002),
no direct or indirect effects in their model (Marchant, found that poor school adjustment was associated
Paulson, & Rothlisberg, 2001). with more negative attachment to school personnel.
Overall, the research suggests that there is a re- These findings question the temporal relationship be-
lation between school bonding and academic per- tween school bonding and antisocial behavior. Al-
formance (Learner & Kruger, 1997; Lopez, Ehly, though poor school bonding should be a precursor
& Garcia-Vazquez, 2002; Marchant et al., 2001; to antisocial behavior, the social development model
Wiatrowski et al., 1982; Williams et al., 1999). This re- (Catalano & Hawkins, 1996; Fraser, 1996) has also
lation is important for intervention efforts. Academic acknowledged that this relation is reciprocal. Specif-
performance presumably could be increased through ically, poor school bonding could lead to antisocial
the promotion of school bonding. A drug prevention behavior, which could then lead to decreased school
program by Eggert et al. (1994), which strengthened bonding in later grades. Malecki and Demaray (2002)
school bonding, also increased GPA and perceptions and Demaray and Malecki (2002) support this propo-
of school performance and reduced absenteeism. sition in their findings of an inverse relationship be-
tween social support (including school bonding) and
antisocial behaviors. Overall there seems to be sup-
Antisocial Behaviors port for a relation between school bonding and anti-
social behavior (Demaray & Malecki, 2002; Malecki
Control theory originally conceptualized delin- & Demaray, 2002) but the direction of this relation is
quency as the primary adverse outcome of low bond- not always clear (Bryant et al., 2000).
ing. The social development model focused on the Welsh et al. (1999) also reported controversial
broader category, antisocial behaviors, as an outcome findings regarding school bonding and antisocial be-
variable (Catalano et al., 1996; Hawkins et al., 1992; havior. They used the ESB (Gottfredson, 1984) to as-
O’Donnell et al., 1995; Williams et al., 1999). Anti- sess the effect of individual and community factors
social behaviors can be described as those behaviors on school disorder (i.e., student antisocial behavior).
that have the potential to negatively affect the so- They evaluated positive peer associations, school re-
cial unit (e.g., aggression, noncompliance, disruptive- ward, and school involvement and commitment di-
ness). Although delinquency is logically subsumed mensions in 11 middle schools in Philadelphia. All in-
under antisocial behavior, there are studies that ex- dividual factors except school rewards were related to
amine antisocial behavior apart from delinquency school misconduct across all 11 middle schools and ex-
(Blankmeyer, Flannery, & Vazsonyi, 2002; Demaray plained a significant portion of the variance in student
& Malecki, 2002; Malecki & Demaray, 2002; Murray misconduct. Involvement and commitment were the
& Greenberg, 2000, 2001; Welsh, Greene, & Jenkins, strongest predictors of antisocial behavior. Ironically,
1999). higher levels of involvement were associated with
Theories have hypothesized that antisocial be- higher misconduct (Welsh et al., 1999). Cernkovich
havior is an outcome of poor school bonding; how- and Giordano (1992) also found this relation. The so-
ever, there are other studies that show that antisocial cial development model offers an explanation.
behavior can be a precursor to poor school bonding Based on the social development model, strong
(Blankmeyer et al., 2002; Bryant et al., 2000). Bryant bonds are not always positive ones. In contrast,
et al. (2000) used two national panel samples from bonds can be prosocial or antisocial depending upon
the Monitoring the Future project (n = 3,2056) to ex- the social unit. Involvement in school activities not
amine school bonding, antisocial behavior at school, only increases opportunities for association with
P1: GMX
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review (CCFP) pp747-ccfp-460115 January 29, 2003 15:14 Style file version Nov. 07, 2000

42 Maddox and Prinz

prosocial peers, but also creates opportunities for ment in school whereas 22 out of the 41 teen mothers
associations with antisocial peers (Hawdon, 1999). dropped out of school. This suggests that nonmothers
Therefore, the more involvement, the more exposure had higher levels of school bonding and that moth-
one would have to antisocial models who have the erhood negatively affects school bonding (Danziger,
potential to promote antisocial behaviors in the youth 1995).
(Dishion, McCord, & Poulin, 1999). These results sug- More studies are needed to examine the effect
gest that greater specification of school involvement is that strong school bonding has on sexual initiation,
necessary to distinguish between antisocial and proso- frequency of sexual contact, number of sexual part-
cial involvement. ners, and contraceptive use. Studies of this nature
should examine the hypothesis that strong school
bonding will lead to delayed sexual initiation, lower
Risky Behavior frequency of sexual contact and number of sexual
partners. The relation between school bonding and
Antisocial behaviors are behaviors that nega- higher contraceptive use should be more complex be-
tively affect the social unit whereas risky behaviors cause there should be lower sexual activity among
negatively affect the youth (i.e., produce psychologi- strongly bonded youth but a higher rate of contracep-
cal, emotional, or physical harm) including substance tive use proportionate to their sexual activity.
use, unsafe sexual practices, and teen pregnancy. High
levels of school bonding are hypothesized to be re-
lated to fewer risky behaviors. As with antisocial be- Self-Esteem
havior, most studies focus upon one or two behav-
iors instead of examining risky behaviors as a whole. Self-esteem and self-efficacy refer to the ways
Wade and Brannigan (1998) conducted a major study youth conceptualize themselves and their abilities.
with 1,075 seventh, ninth, eleventh, and twelfth grade Based on theories such as the social ecology model
students in Ontario, Canada. The variables studied (Kumpfer & Turner, 1990–1991), high levels of school
were demographic variables, family structure, fam- bonding should be closely related to high levels of
ily financial situation, family attachment, peer at- self-esteem and self-efficacy. The findings of Eggert
tachment, school bonding, and risk taking behavior. et al. (1994) support this relation. Their drug pre-
The results yielded a model in which school bond- vention program, which increased school bonding,
ing had a significant independent effect on risk tak- produced a steady increase in self-esteem scores.
ing behavior. Students with low family attachment Williams and Mcgee (1991) had similar findings
but high school bonding were less likely to engage when they examined self-perception of strengths in
in risk taking behaviors than students with both 960, 15-year-old males and females from the gen-
low family and school bonding (Wade & Brannigan, eral population of New Zealand. Based on their re-
1998). sults, school involvement was one of the most im-
Relatively little research has examined the di- portant factors related to self-perception of strengths
rect effects of school bonding on teen sexual be- for boys but not for girls (Williams & McGee,
havior and teen pregnancy. Theoretically youth who 1991).
have a well-developed sense of hope for their future Hunt and Hunt (1977) performed secondary
should avoid behaviors that might jeopardize that fu- analysis on existing data (Rosenberg & Simmons,
ture (e.g., unsafe sex and pregnancy). The reverse 1971) examining self-attitudes (efficacy and esteem),
can also be inferred in which adolescents who have specific self-description, and attachment to school
little hope for future success may turn to sex and and found an association between school attachment
pregnancy as an attempt to achieve status (Black, and self-image. These results generally showed a
1998). School bonding might provide a pathway for relation between school bonding and self-esteem—
a more hopeful future, which could reduce the like- however, contextual factors that affected this re-
lihood of early pregnancy and parenthood. Danziger lationship will be considered in detail at a later
(1995) supported this proposition in her study con- point. Malecki and Demaray (2002) found mod-
ducting life history interviews with 80 African Amer- erate relations between total social support (in-
ican women ages 15–20. The 39 respondents who were cluding school bonding) and self-concept but the
not mothers were more likely to have a positive sense unique contribution of school bonding was not
of themselves as a student and family encourage- clear.
P1: GMX
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review (CCFP) pp747-ccfp-460115 January 29, 2003 15:14 Style file version Nov. 07, 2000

School Bonding in Children 43

MEDIATING AND MODERATING EFFECTS significant variable in predicting delinquency among


RELATED TO SCHOOL BONDING White males (β = −.213, p < .05). Perceived risk of
arrest, which was included in the study’s definition
Parental Influences Mediated by School Bonding of school bonding, and age were significant predic-
tors of delinquency for White females (β = −.239,
Control theory, the social development model, p < .001 and β = .162, p < .05, respectively). School
and the social ecology model all allude to the possibil- commitment, attachment, and involvement were sig-
ity that school bonding might serve as a mediator be- nificant correlates with delinquency in African Amer-
tween family influences and life outcomes (Catalano ican males (β = −.211, p < .05; β = −.196, p < .05;
& Hawkins, 1996; Hirschi, 1969; Kumpfer & Turner, and β = .152, p < .05, respectively). School bond-
1990–1991). The social ecology model (Kumpfer & ing factors were not significant predictors for African
Turner, 1990–1991) revealed that family climate had American females (Cernkovich & Giordano, 1992).
an effect on substance use indirectly through its ef- These results suggest that the impact of school bond-
fect on school bonding/self-efficacy and peer effects. ing on delinquency is not as effective for females but
Williams et al. (1999) also found that family rela- caution should be taken because females had signifi-
tionships had both a direct effect on substance use cantly lower rates of reported delinquency.
and an indirect effect through the mediators, school Eggert et al. (1994) found gender differences in
bonding and academic and social skills. There were that females receiving intervention decreased in de-
similar results for the role of school bonding as a viant peer bonding but there was no shift for treated
mediating factor for familial bonds in risky behavior males even though school bonding steadily increased
(Wade & Brannigan, 1998) and delinquency research for both sexes. Williams and Mcgee (1991) found
(Sommers et al., 1994). Although there is some evi- that high levels of school involvement was one of the
dence that contradicts school bonding as mediator of most important factors related to greater perception
parental influences (Liska & Reed, 1985), the stud- of strength for boys but that it was not as important
ies that support the relationship provides prevention for girls. However, they only used school involvement
workers with a potential alternative to family inter- items as an indicator of school bonding, which could
vention as a means to offset the adverse impact of explain the lack of significance for school bonding in
family influences. females. Based on these studies it seems as if the ef-
fect of school bonding dimensions are moderated by
gender with involvement being most important for
Gender as a Moderator of School Bonding males.

There are gender differences in the way school


bonding affects life outcomes, which is expected be- SES as a Moderator of School Bonding
cause there are gender differences in life outcomes
in general (e.g., delinquency, substance use). Because McGee (1992) used data from the Monitoring the
males are more likely to engage in certain forms of Future study to examine interactive effects of social
delinquency (violent forms such as assault and homi- class, parental influence, and delinquent peers on ado-
cide) and engage in polysubstance use (Bachman lescent drug use. The sample consisted of 16,502 high
& Peralta, 2002; Hickman & Piquero, 2001; Ludwig school seniors in 115 public and 17 private schools.
& Pittman, 1999; Mazerolle, Brame, Paternoster, The variables in the study were substance use, SES,
Piquero, & Dean, 2000), differences in the impact parental attachment, religious attachment, beliefs in
of school bonding on these variables are suspected. societal laws, and peer influence. School bonding vari-
Cernkovich and Giordano (1992) revealed both gen- ables were attachment to school and school commit-
der and racial differences. African American females ment that both had a weak negative effect on overall
reported higher levels of attachment to school, com- drug use. Interactions with social class variables re-
mitment, and perceived risk of arrest than males and vealed that attachment to school had an effect only on
White females. African American males reported the substance use in participants from the middle-income
highest level of school involvement, with White males background whereas commitment had a significant
second and African American females third. effect on substance use only on participants in the
Regarding the relations between school bond- lower-income background. Neither had an effect on
ing and delinquency, school commitment was the only upper class substance use, which suggests that SES
P1: GMX
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review (CCFP) pp747-ccfp-460115 January 29, 2003 15:14 Style file version Nov. 07, 2000

44 Maddox and Prinz

moderates the effect school bonding, has on substance tary school but the trend was reversed by senior high
use (Mcgee, 1992). where low attachment to school produced negative
self-attitudes. These racial differences could be the
result of a process identified as “the burden of acting
Culture as a Moderator of School Bonding white.” Theory and research (Fordham, 1985) have
suggested that minority youth often shy away from
Expectations of culture as a moderator are academic achievement because it is perceived as “act-
prevalent throughout the school bonding literature ing white.” According to the theory, it can be assumed
(Demaray & Malecki, 2002; Liska & Reed, 1985; that African American youth who are less attached to
Lopez et al., 2002; Morrison, Robertson, & Harding, their school (thereby reducing the “burden of acting
1998; Murguia et al., 1998). Although Hirschi (1969) white”) will have higher self-esteem.
argued that his theory should explain delinquency Research by Zhang and Messner (1996) with 143
across racial groups, the racial differences in life out- (delinquent = 57) 15–18-year-olds in the People’s Re-
comes suggest that these variables might produce dif- public of China suggests that school bonding operates
ferential effects across cultures. Liska and Reed’s re- the same in China as in Western countries. Their re-
sults produced two separate causal models for White sults for school bonding and delinquency were consis-
and African American respondents (Liska & Reed, tent with research in the United States. The similari-
1985). Parental attachment affected school attach- ties between attitudes toward education in China and
ment, which then affected delinquency for African the United States might account for the similarities in
Americans, but school attachment affected parental results. Overall, however, there is evidence that cul-
attachment that then affected delinquency for Whites. ture moderates the effects of school bonding on life
This suggests that school attachment effects differ outcomes (Liska & Reed, 1985; see also Cernkovich
across culture but because the sample of African & Giordano, 1992; Hunt & Hunt, 1977; Murguia
Americans was so small, the authors urge against et al., 1998). Examination of cultural context will be
drawing premature conclusions. However, the results important when trying to use school bonding as part
do suggest possible differences in processes by which of an intervention.
parental attachment, school attachment and delin-
quency are related.
School Context as a Moderator of School Bonding
Murguia et al. (1998) also found cultural dif-
ferences in the effect of attachment to parents and
In theory, school context should moderate the
schools on deviant peers and illicit drug use. The sam-
effects of school bonding. Cernkovich and Giordano
ple included 1,807 Whites and 236 Mexican Amer-
(1992) revealed an effect of school context on school
icans who participated in all three waves of a lon-
bonding’s association with delinquency. Although dif-
gitudinal intervention. Path analyses revealed two
ferences according to school context were not signif-
different models for non-Hispanic Whites and Mex-
icant, they suggest that school bonding may not be
ican Americans. For non-Hispanic Whites positive
consistent across contexts. In contrast, Welsh et al.
school experiences, the school bonding related vari-
(1999) examined 11 middle schools and did not find
able, had both a direct effect on drug involvement
any significant differences in school bonding across
and an indirect effect mediated by drug using peers.
school context.
Positive school experiences only had a direct effect
on drug involvement for Mexican Americans. These
models suggests that school bonding may not have Age Group as a Moderator of School Bonding
as strong as an effect on drug using peers for Mexi-
can Americans as it did for non-Hispanic Whites. As Most school bonding studies examined life out-
with Liska and Reed (1985), the sample sizes were comes such as delinquency and substance use so their
small. focus has been on adolescents due to the high fre-
Hunt and Hunt (1977) found racial differences quency in this group. This leaves little empirical in-
in their study of 690 male students in elementary formation about how school bonding might affect
(245), junior (259), and senior (186) high school. life outcomes differently due to stage of develop-
Their results showed that African Americans with ment. Simons-Morton et al. (1999) studied 4,263 stu-
lower attachment to school had higher self-esteem dents in Grades 6–8 and found differences in levels of
and self-efficacy. This was true for Whites in elemen- school bonding across grades, but they did not report
P1: GMX
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review (CCFP) pp747-ccfp-460115 January 29, 2003 15:14 Style file version Nov. 07, 2000

School Bonding in Children 45

differential effects of school bonding for these grades. differential effects on the relationship such as gender,
A firmer conclusion was made by Hunt and Hunt culture, and SES.
(1977). They found that Whites with low attach-
ment in elementary school had higher self-esteem,
but that this trend reversed by the time they were Model of School Bonding
in senior high. By senior high, when the awareness
of the link between academic and economic success The social development model offers a good
was higher, White youth with high levels of school conceptualization of the association between school
bonding tended to feel better about themselves and bonding and various behavioral outcomes for the
their future. Differences in school bonding effects youth (see Fig. 1). Based on the model, external
for elementary versus middle or high schools have constraints (e.g., family practices, classroom manage-
been suggested by other researchers (Liska & Reed, ment, school policies), demographic factors (e.g., SES,
1985). race, age, gender), and individual factors (e.g., level of
self-control) directly affect the youth’s opportunities
and skills for involvement. Once there are sufficient
CONCLUSIONS opportunities for involvement, skills for involvement
directly affect the amount of reinforcement the youth
Major Findings receives for being involved in that particular social
unit. This reinforcement promotes continued involve-
School bonding is a multidimensional concept ment, which subsequently is hypothesized to lead to
that has been linked to a variety of outcomes and be- attachment and commitment to people within that so-
haviors. Unfortunately, many studies do not capture cial unit. This commitment and attachment socializes
the breadth of this construct in their assessment. De- the youth’s belief system, which has a direct effect on
spite the lack of consistency in assessment, some con- subsequent behavior.
sistent findings about school bonding are apparent.
In control theory, school bonding was initially con-
ceptualized as a constraining force against delinquent Policy and Intervention
behaviors (Hirschi, 1969). More comprehensive the-
ories such as the social development model have es- The social development model suggests that
tablished school bonding as a protective factor that school bonding might be a significant target for in-
inhibits negative and promotes positive life outcomes tervention as a way of reducing negative life out-
through its effects on opportunities for prosocial in- comes and promoting positive outcomes. Interven-
teractions. tions can be designed that promote both the op-
Among the outcomes that have been shown to be portunities and skills for school involvement, which
affected by school bonding are substance use, delin- should increase school attachment, teacher attach-
quency, antisocial behavior, academic performance, ment, and school commitment. However, a couple of
and self-esteem/self-efficacy. Higher levels of school factors must be considered when attempting to mod-
bonding are associated with higher self-esteem and ify school bonding. First, a defensible and consistent
academic performance, but lower levels of antiso- definition of school bonding is important to assess
cial behavior, substance use, and delinquency. De- how well the program is reaching its goal. Hirschi’s
spite this pattern of direct effects, construed relations original dimensions (Hirschi, 1969) have been re-
with school bonding are not always straightforward or vised through the years because of differential em-
simple. Research has shown that the school bonding pirical and theoretical support (Elliott, Huizinga, &
dimensions can have differential effects on life out- Ageton, 1985; Krohn & Massey, 1980) so his orig-
comes moderated by gender, culture, and SES. For ex- inal conceptualization does not offer much help.
ample, school involvement seems to be more strongly The Cernkovich and Giordano (1992) study offers
implicated in self-esteem for males than for females. some suggestions for an operational definition. Their
In addition, school involvement has the potential study was relatively well controlled and sophisti-
to both inhibit and promote negative life outcomes cated. Based on their results the conceptualization
depending upon the type of involvement (Hawdon, of school bonding should include the dimensions of
1999). When examining school bonding relations, it is attachment to school, school commitment,and school
important to consider other factors that could have involvement because these dimensions have had
P1: GMX

46
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review (CCFP)
pp747-ccfp-460115
January 29, 2003
15:14
Style file version Nov. 07, 2000

Fig. 1. The social developmental model of antisocial behavior: general model. [Reprinted from Catalano, R., & Hawkins, J. D. (1996). The social development model: A theory of
antisocial behavior. In J. D. Hawkins (Ed.), Delinquency and crime: Current theories (pp. 149–197). Cambridge, England Cambridge University Press.]
Maddox and Prinz
P1: GMX
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review (CCFP) pp747-ccfp-460115 January 29, 2003 15:14 Style file version Nov. 07, 2000

School Bonding in Children 47

empirical support and are theoretically valid (Cata- are established for conceptualization and measure-
lano et al., 1996; Cernkovich & Giordano, 1992; ment. Criteria should involve the inclusion of mul-
Fraser, 1996; Simons-Morton et al., 1999; Wiatrowski tiple dimensions and depth of dimension assessment.
& Anderson, 1987). The dimension attachment to The development of a standardized measure of school
teachers was not significant in the Cernkovich and bonding will be helpful in establishing and enforcing
Giordano (1992) study but was significant in other these criteria.
studies (Murray & Greenberg, 2000, 2001) and has The lack of empirical studies that focus specif-
high theoretical value as a dimension of school bond- ically on positive outcomes is another area for im-
ing. Thus, attachment to teachers should also be in- provement. Despite the tendency of research to fo-
cluded in the conceptualization of school bonding. cus on negative life outcomes for prevention efforts,
To make this dimension more sensitive to the attach- more studies looking at the ability of school bonding
ments students can form with persons in the school to promote and strengthen positive life outcomes are
system it is suggested that this dimension be expanded needed. Outcomes such as academic excellence, social
to include guidance counselors, coaches, school nurse, competence, and resilience should arguably be associ-
etc. The resulting dimension will be more accurately ated with school bonding. As with negative outcomes,
referred to as attachment to school personnel to re- there are factors (e.g., development, culture, and gen-
flect the inclusion of these other adults. In conclu- der) that will moderate the effect of school bonding.
sion, an accurate conceptualization of school bond- This line of research could offer new opportunities for
ing should include the domains attachment to school, prevention.
attachment to personnel, school commitment, and Finally, more information regarding develop-
school involvement. This conceptualization is useful mental moderators of school bonding is clearly
to the prevention scientist in that it captures the mul- needed. Although there are good theoretical argu-
tidimensional nature of school bonding by including ments, empirical studies comparing school bonding
dimensions that have been supported throughout the across developmental stages would add to the knowl-
literature (Cernkovich & Giordano, 1992; Murray & edge base. Improving what is known about the de-
Greenberg, 2000, 2001). velopmental variables associated with increases, de-
Once a defensible conceptualization is achieved, creases, and stability in school bonding is likely to
the next step is the consideration of contextual moder- provide clues about how to enhance quality of life for
ators and predictors. As the social development model youth.
suggests, demographic and family factors have a di-
rect effect on the youth’s opportunities and skills for
involvement. Although school bonding might com-
pensate for low familial bonds (Wade & Brannigan, REFERENCES
1998), the influence of family factors should be ex-
amined with any intervention targeting school bond- Accordino, D. B., Accordino, M. P., & Slaney, R. B. (2000). An
investigation of perfectionism, mental health, achievement,
ing. Likewise, programs may need to be altered to and achievement motivation in adolescents. Psychology in the
better accommodate gender, cultural, SES, and de- Schools, 37, 535–545.
velopmental effects to avoid the assumption that a Akers, R. L., & Cochran, J. K. (1985). Adolescent marijuana use: A
test of three theories of deviant behavior. Deviant Behavior,
singular or narrow approach would enhance school 6, 323–346.
bonding. With these considerations included, a well- Ayers, C. D., Williams, J. H., Hawkins, J. D., Peterson, P. L.,
designed prevention program has the opportunity to Catalano, R. F., & Abbott, R. D. (1999). Assessing correlates
of onset, escalation, deescalation, and desistance of delinquent
affect school wide changes and promote positive life behavior. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 15, 277–307.
outcomes. Bachman, R., & Peralta, R. (2002). The relationship between drink-
ing and violence in an adolescent population: Does gender
matter. Deviant Behavior: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 23, 1–
Directions for Future Research 19.
Balk, D. E. (1995). Adolescent development: Early through late ado-
Although a fairly consistent pattern emerged for lescence. Pacific Grove: Brooks/Cole.
Berryhill, J. C., & Prinz, R. J. (in press). Environmental interven-
most of the expected relations in school bonding, the tions to enhance student adjustment: Implication for preven-
research in this area can be further strengthened. The tion. Prevention Science.
first major area is in the conceptualization of school Black, S. (1998). Facts of life. American School Board Journal, 185,
33–36.
bonding. It is not suggested that there be a uniform Blankmeyer, M., Flannery, D. J., & Vazsonyi, A. (2002). The role of
concept of school bonding, but that certain criteria aggression and social competence in children’s perceptions of
P1: GMX
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review (CCFP) pp747-ccfp-460115 January 29, 2003 15:14 Style file version Nov. 07, 2000

48 Maddox and Prinz

the child–teacher relationship. Psychology in the Schools, 39, Free, M. D., Jr. (1994). Religiosity, religious conservatism, bonds
293–304. to school, and juvenile delinquency among three categories of
Brophy, J. E. (1988). On motivating students. In D. Berliner & B. drug users. Deviant Behavior, 15, 151–170.
Rosenshine (Eds.), Talks to teachers (pp. 201–245). New York: Goodenow, C. (1993). The psychological sense of school member-
Random House. ship among adolescents: Scale development and educational
Bryant, A. L., Schulenberg, J., Bachman, J. G., O’Malley, P. M., & correlates. Psychology in the Schools, 30, 79–90.
Johnston, L. D. (2000). Acting out and lighting up: Understand- Gottfredson, G. D. (1984). The effective school battery. Odessa, FL:
ing the links among school misbehavior, academic achievement, Psychological Assessment Resources.
and cigarette use Monitoring the Future Occasional Paper No. Gottfredson, G. D., & Gottfedson, D. C. (1999). Technical manual
46). Ann Arbor, MI : The University of Michigan Institute for for research editions of What About You (WAY). Ellicott City,
Social Research. MD: Gottfredson Associates.
Catalano, R., & Hawkins, J. D. (1996). The social development Gottfredson, M., & Hirschi, T. (1990). A general theory of crime.
model: A theory of antisocial behavior. In J. D. Hawkins Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
(Ed.), Delinquency and crime: Current theories (pp. 149–197). Grotevant, H. D., & Thorbecke, W. L. (1982). Sex differences in
Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. styles of occupational identity formation in late adolescence.
Catalano, R., Kosterman, R., Hawkins, J. D., Newcomb, M. D., & Developmental Psychology, 18, 396–405.
Abbott, R. D. (1996). Modeling the etiology of adolescent sub- Hawdon, J. E. (1999). Daily routines and crime: Using routine activ-
stance use: A test of the social development model. Journal of ities as measures of Hirschi’s involvement. Youth and Society,
Drug Issues, 26, 429–455. 30, 395–415.
Cernkovich, S. A., & Giordano, P. C. (1992). School bonding, race, Hawkins, J. D., Catalano, R. F., Morrison, D. M., O’Donnell, J.,
and delinquency. Criminology, 30, 261–291. Abbott, R. D., & Day, L. E. (1992). The Seattle social de-
Cook, E. T., Greenberg, M., & Kusche, C. (1995). People in my velopment project: Effects of the first four years on protec-
life: Attachment relationships in middle childhood. Paper pre- tive factors and problem behaviors. In J. McCord & R. E.
sented at the meeting of the Society for Research in Child Tremblay (Eds.), Preventing antisocial behavior: Interventions
Development, Indianapolis, IN. from birth through adolescence (pp. 139–206). New York: Guil-
Crusto, C. (2000, June 1–4). Developing successful strategies to ford Press.
prevent dropping out of school: Linking correlates and devel- Hawkins, J. D., Graham, J. W., Maguin, E., Abbott, R., Hill, K. G., &
opmental processes to interventions. Paper presented at the Catalano, R. (1997). Exploring the effects of age of alcohol use
Society for Prevention Reseaerch, Montreal, Canada. initiation and psychosocial risk factors on subsequent alcohol
Danziger, S. K. (1995). Family life and teenage pregnancy in the misuse. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 58, 280–290.
inner-city: Experiences of African-American youth. Children Hawkins, J. D., & Lishner, D. (1987). Etiology and prevention of an-
and Youth Services Review, 17, 183–202. tisocial behavior in children and adolescents. In D. H. Crowell,
Demaray, M. K., & Malecki, C. K. (2002). The relationship between I. M. Evans, & C. R. O’Donnell (Eds.), Childhood aggres-
percieved social support and maladjustment for students at sion and violence: Sources of influence, prevention and control
risk. Psychology in the Schools, 39, 305–316. (pp. 263–282). New York: Plenum.
Dishion, T. J., McCord, J., & Poulin, F. (1999). When interventions Hickman, M., & Piquero, A. (2001). Exploring the relationship
harm: Peer groups and problem behavior. American Psychol- between gender, control balance, and deviance. Deviant Be-
ogist, 54, 755–764. havior: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 22, 323–351.
Eggert, L., & Kumpfer, K. L. (1997). Drug abuse prevention for Hirschi, T. (1969). Causes of delinquency. Berkeley, CA: University
at-risk individuals. Bethesda, MD: Abt Associates. of California Press.
Eggert, L. L., Thompson, E. A., Herting, J. R., Nicholas, L. J., & Hoffmann, J. P., & Miller, A. S. (1998). A latent variable analysis
Dicker, B. G. (1994). Preventing adolescent drug abuse and of general strain theory. Journal of Quantitative Criminology,
high school dropout through an intensive school-based social 14, 83–110.
network development program. American Journal of Health Hunt, J. G., & Hunt, L. L. (1977). Racial inequality and self-image:
Promotion, 8, 202–215. Identity maintenance as identity diffusion. Sociology and So-
Elliott, D. S., Huizinga, D., & Ageton, S. S. (1985). Explaining delin- cial Research, 61, 539–559.
quency and drug use. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Junger, M., & Tremblay, R. E. (1999). Self-control, accidents, and
Evans, T. D., Cullen, F. T., Burton, V. S., Jr., Dunaway, R. G., & crime. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 26, 485–501.
Benson, M. L. (1997). The social consequences of self-control: Keith, P. (1999). Parental involvement and attendance. Columbia,
Testing the genreal theory of crime. Criminology, 35, 475– SC: University of South Carolina, College of Liberal Arts, De-
501. partment of Psychology.
Farnworth, M., Schweinhart, L. J., & Beurueta-Clement, J. R. Kleinman, P. H., Wish, E. D., Deren, S., & Rainone, G. A. (1986).
(1985). Preschool intervention, school success and delinquency Multiple drug use: A symptomatic behavior. Journal of Psy-
in a high-risk sample of youth. American Educational Research choactive Drugs, 18, 77–86.
Journal, 22, 445–464. Krohn, M., & Massey, J. (1980). Social control and delinquent be-
Figueira-McDonough, J. (1983). On the usefulness of Merton’s havior: An examination of the elements of the social bond.
Anomie Theory: Academic failure and deviance among high The Sociological Quarterly, 21, 529–543.
school students. Youth and Society, 14, 259–279. Kumpfer, K. L., & Turner, C. W. (1990–1991). The social ecology
Figueira-McDonough, J. (1987). Discrimination or sex differences? model of adolescent substance abuse: Implications for pre-
Criteria for evaluating the juvenile justice system’s han- vention. The International Journal of the Addictions, 25(4A),
dling of minor offenses. Crime and Delinquency, 33, 403– 435–463.
424. Learner, D. G., & Kruger, L. J. (1997). Attachment, self-concept,
Fordham, S. (1985). Black students’ school success: Coping with and academic motivation in high-school students. American
the “burden of ‘acting white’.” Paper presented at the annual Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 67, 485–492.
meeting of the American Anthropological Association, Wash- Liska, A., & Reed, M. D. (1985, August). Ties to conventional insti-
ington, DC. tutions and delinquency: Estimating reciprocal effects. Amer-
Fraser, M. W. (1996). Aggressive behavior in childhood and ican Sociological Review, 50, 547–560.
early adolescence: An ecological-developmental perspective Lopez, E. J., Ehly, S., & Garcia-Vazquez, E. (2002). Ac-
on youth violence. Social Work, 41, 347–361. culturation, social support and academic achievement of
P1: GMX
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review (CCFP) pp747-ccfp-460115 January 29, 2003 15:14 Style file version Nov. 07, 2000

School Bonding in Children 49

Mexican and Mexican American high school students: An prevention program in rural communities. The Journal of Pri-
exploratory study. Psychology in the Schools, 39, 245– mary Prevention, 18, 341–361.
257. Pratt, T. C., & Cullen, F. T. (2000). The empirical status of
Ludwig, K. B., & Pittman, J. F. (1999). Adolescent prosocial val- Gottfredson and Hirschi’s general theory of crime: A meta-
ues and self-efficacy in relation to delinquency, risky sex- analysis. Criminology, 38, 931–964.
ual behavior, and substance use. Youth and Society, 30, 461– Reynolds, C. R., & Kamphaus, R. W. (1998). BASC; Behavior As-
482. sessment System for Children manual. Circle Pines, MN: Amer-
Malecki, C. K., & Demaray, M. K. (2002). Measuring percieved ican Guidance Service.
social support: Development of the child and adolescent so- Rosenberg, M., & Simmons, R. G. (1971). Functions of children’s
cial support scale (CASSS). Psychology in the Schools, 39, 1– perception of the stratification system. American Sociological
18. Review, 36, 235–249.
Marchant, G. J., Paulson, S., & Rothlisberg, B. A. (2001). Relations Santrock, J. W. (1996). Adolescence (6th ed.). Madison: Brown &
of middle school students perceptions of famliy and school con- Benchmark.
text with academic achievement. Psychology in the Schools, 38, Simons-Morton, B. G., Crump, A. D., Haynie, D. L., & Saylor,
505–519. K. E. (1999). Student–school bonding and adolescent prob-
Mazerolle, P., Brame, R., Paternoster, R., Piquero, A., & Dean, lem behavior. Health Education Research, 14, 99–107.
C. (2000). Onset age, persistence, and offending versatility: Sommers, I., Fagan, J., & Baskin, D. (1994). The influence of ac-
Comparisons across gender. Criminology, 38, 1143–1172. culturation and familism on Puerto Rican delinquency. Justice
Mcgee, Z. T. (1992). Social class differences in the parental and Quarterly, 11, 207–228.
peer influence on adolescent drug use. Deviant Behavior, 13, Taylor, C. (2001). The relationship between social and self-control:
349–372. Tracing Hirschi’s criminological career. Theoretical Criminol-
Meier, R., & McDaniel, E. (1974). A measure of attitude toward ogy, 5, 369–388.
school. Educational and Psychological Measurment, 34, 997– Valentine, J., Gottlieb, B., Keel, S., Griffith, J., & Ruthazer, R.
998. (1998). Measuring the effectiveness of the urban youth con-
Morrison, G. M., Robertson, L., & Harding, M. (1998). Resilience nection: The case for dose-response modeling to demonstrate
factors that support the classroom functioning of acting out the impact of an adolescent substance abuse prevention pro-
and aggressive students. Psychology in the Schools, 35, 217– gram. The Journal of Primary Prevention, 18, 363–387.
227. Wade, T. J., & Brannigan, A. (1998). The genesis of adolescent risk-
Murguia, E., Chen, Z.-Y., & Kaplan, H. B. (1998). A compari- taking: Pathways through family, school, and peers. Canadian
son of causal factors in drug use among Mexican Americans Journal of Sociology, 23, 1–19.
and non-Hispanic Whites. Social Science Quarterly, 79, 341– Welsh, W. N., Greene, J. R., & Jenkins, P. H. (1999). School disor-
360. der: The influence of individual, institutional and community
Murray, C., & Greenberg, M. (2000). Children’s relationship with factors. Criminology, 37, 73–115.
teachers and bonds with school: An investigation of patterns Wiatrowski, M., & Anderson, K. L. (1987). The dimensionality of
and correlates in middle childhood. Journal of School Psychol- the social bond. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 3, 65–81.
ogy, 38, 423–445. Wiatrowski, M. D., Hansell, S., Massey, C. R., & Wilson, D. L.
Murray, C., & Greenberg, M. (2001). Relationships with teachers (1982). Curriculum tracking and delinquency. American So-
and bonds with school: Social emotional adjustment correlates ciological Review, 47, 151–160.
for children with and without disabilities. Psychology in the Williams, J. H. (1994). Understanding substance use, delinquency
Schools, 38, 25–41. involvement, and juvenile justice system involvement among
Nagin, D. S., & Paternoster, R. (1993). Enduring individual differ- African American and European American adolescents. Un-
ences and rationale choice theories of crime. Law and Scoiety published doctoral dissertation, University of Washington.
Review, 27, 467–496. Williams, J. H., Ayer, C. D., Abbot, R. D., Hawkins, J. D., &
O’Donnell, J., Hawkins, J. D., & Abbott, R. D. (1995). Predict- Catalano, R. F. (1999). Racial differences in risk factors for
ing serious delinquency and substance use among aggressive delinquency and substance use among adolescents. Social
boys. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 63, 529– Work Research, 23, 241–256.
537. Williams, S., & McGee, R. (1991). Adolescent’s self-perceptions of
Paternoster, R., & Brame, R. (2000). On the association among their strengths. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 20, 325–337.
self-control, crime, and analogous behaviors. Criminology, 38, Woolfolk, A. E. (1995). Educational psychology (6th ed.). Boston:
971–982. Allyn and Bacon.
Pianta, R. C., & Steinberg, M. (1992). Teacher-child relationships Zhang, L., & Messner, S. F. (1996). School attachment and official
and the process of adjusting to school. In R. C. Pianta (Ed.), delinquency status in the People’s Republic of China. Socio-
Beyond the parent: The role of other adults in children’s lives logical Forum, 11, 285–303.
(Vol. 57, pp. 61–80). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. Zhang, L., Welte, J. W., & Wieczorek, W. F. (2002). Underlying
Pilgrim, C., Abbey, A., Hendrickson, P., & Lorenz, S. (1998). Im- common factors of adolescent problem behaviors. Criminal
plementation and impact of a family-based substance abuse Justice and Behavior, 29, 161–182.

You might also like