The Role of Peer Relationships in Adjustment To College

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 18

The Role of Peer Relationships in Adjustment to College

Lisa M. Swenson, Alicia Nordstrom, Marnie Hiester

Journal of College Student Development, Volume 49, Number 6, November/December


2008, pp. 551-567 (Article)

Published by Johns Hopkins University Press


DOI: https://doi.org/10.1353/csd.0.0038

For additional information about this article


https://muse.jhu.edu/article/253955

Access provided by Arizona State University (12 Sep 2017 16:44 GMT)
The Role of Peer Relationships in
Adjustment to College
Lisa M. Swenson   Alicia Nordstrom   Marnie Hiester

According to developmental research, peer rela­ Terenzini, 1980; Tinto, 1975). Although many
tion­ships serve a positive function in children’s, factors come together to impact adjustment
adolescents’, and adults’ lives. We expected that to college, the purpose of this study was to
peer relationships would also benefit emerging examine one component of emerging adults’
adults as they transition into college. Using lives, peer relationships, to determine how
friendship quality and attachment measures, we these relationships affect adjustment.
examined the link between the closeness of peer In general, peers are central to adolescents’
relationships (with high school and college and emerging adults’ lives. Erikson’s theory of
friends) and adjustment outcomes (academic, psychosocial development posits that the major
social, emotional, and institutional attachment) developmental task of the early 20s is to
among 271 first-year college students. Results establish close intimate relationships (Erikson,
suggest that a close relationship with a high school 1963). Given the importance of peers during
friend is beneficial during the first weeks of these years, we expected that friends would
college, but later in the first semester there are play an important role in the transition into
more benefits to having a close relationship with college. Researchers have documented the
a new college friend. benefits of friendships among children and
adolescents (e.g., Hartup & Stevens, 1997;
When an adolescent transitions into college, New­comb & Bagwell, 1995; Rubin, Bukowski,
many changes take place. Adolescents enter & Parker, 1998), particularly in a school
the emerging adulthood age period, which by context (see Ladd & Kochenderfer, 1996, for
definition is a period of instability and a review). Research on older adults also has
exploration during which they must adjust to described the benefits of friends’ provisions of
an unfamiliar environment that consists of social support to well–being and health (e.g.,
different academic and social relationships, Adams & Blieszner, 1995; Antonucci &
identity explorations, and possible changes in Akiyama, 1995). The literature on the benefits
self–concept (Arnett, 2004). If these changes or role of friends during emerging adulthood
are negative stressors in a college student’s life, is not as extensive, but does indicate that peers
this may result in poor adjustment to the new often take over as primary attachment figures
environment, the consequences of which could (Fraley & Davis, 1997) and play a role in need
be poor performance in the classroom or even fulfillment (Carbery & Buhrmester, 1998). In
a loss of that student from the educational a school context, peer relationships can
institution (e.g., Bean, 1985; Pascarella & influence student development (Chickering &

Lisa Swenson is Assistant Professor of Psychology at Pennsylvania State University, Hazleton. Alicia Nordstrom is
Assistant Professor of Psychology and Marnie Hiester is Associate Professor of Psychology, both at Misericordia University.
This research was supported by a Faculty Research Development Grant obtained by the first author from her institution.
The authors would like to thank Drs. Peter Crabb, Christopher Goguen, Justin Nordstrom, Michael Polgar, and
Elizabeth Wright for their comments on versions of this manuscript.

November /December 2008 ◆ vol 49 no 6 551


Swenson, Nordstrom, & Hiester

Reisser, 1993) and can affect students’ satis­ (Mikulincer & Nachslon, 1991), report greater
faction with an institution (Astin, 1993). Yet, intimacy (Grabill & Kerns, 2000), and expect
as they transition from adolescence to emerging more support, trust, and acceptance from
adulthood and from high school into college, friends (You & Malley–Morrison, 2000).
many students’ friendships end or at least There is a significant link between the
change due to physical separations or contrast­ quality of peer relationships and adjustment
ing life goals (Paul & Brier, 2001; Rose, 1984). variables during the transition to high school
Moreover, there may be a lag in making close and throughout the high school years (Demir &
friends in their new college environment. In Urberg, 2004). Greater friendship quality was
this study, we expected that friends would play associated with greater emotional adjust­ment.
a role in adjustment to college. If friendships Furthermore, attachment styles were associated
themselves transition, though, we expected with adjustment such that secure attachments
that there would be differences in how older were associated with fewer internalizing
high school friendships versus new college problems and greater self–concept.
friendships were associated with adjustment. With a focus on attachment, researchers
found that attachment to one’s parents, but
Relationship Quality and Adjustment not to one’s peers, was beneficial to high school
Research examining relationship quality and students’ well–being following a stressful life
adjustment has focused mostly on transitions event (Greenberg, Siegel, & Leitch, 1983).
into or development through middle school Although peer relationships are important
and high school. Researchers have defined aspects of adolescents’ lives (e.g., see Hartup
school adjustment as “the degree to which & Stevens, 1997; Newcomb & Bagwell, 1995)
children become interested, engaged, comfort­ and adolescents spend an increasing amount
able, and successful in the school environment” of time with peers (Gavin & Furman, 1989),
(Ladd & Kochenderfer, 1996, p. 324). Assess­ perhaps when they are still living at home
ments of relationship quality include the use adolescents turn to their parents for help. Once
of friendship quality measures, which assess they transition into college and away from
variables such as the extent of intimacy, valida­ their family unit, they may start to seek
tion, or conflict resolution (e.g., Bukowski, support from friends to help them through big
Hoza, & Boivin, 1994; Parker & Asher, 1993; life changes, as is suggested by research on
Sharabany, Gershoni, & Hofman, 1981), and emerging adults (Fraley & Davis, 1997). When
measures of attachment style between partners assessing romantic relationships among high
in the relationship. Attachment relationships school students, adjustment was related to the
are close, emotional bonds between two attachment style between partners. As com­
people, and style can be defined by measuring pared to insecurely attached partners, securely
behaviors such as proximity seeking and attached partners were better adjusted, as
comfort (Goldberg, 2000). Research has indicated by lower anxiety and depression and
shown a connection between relationship higher perceived competence (Cooper, Shaver,
quality and attachment such that being & Collins, 1998).
securely attached to a partner is associated with Researchers studying emerging adults have
higher–quality relationships (Hazan & Shaver, described a significant link between the quality
1987; Markiewicz, Doyle, & Brendgen, 2001). of college students’ peer relationships and their
Individuals who are more securely attached are adjustment to college (Fass & Tubman, 2002;
more likely to self–disclose to friends Lapsley & Edgerton, 2002). When focusing

552 Journal of College Student Development


Peer Relationships and Adjustment

on attachment between peers, poor attachment state. Although the negative effects of friend­
was associated with lower ratings of scholastic sickness do not necessarily indicate that
competence (Fass & Tubman, 2002). As students should end their old friendships, it
compared to insecure attachments, secure could be more evidence for the need to make
attachment styles were associated with better new friends in a new environment to help
social adjustment (Lapsley & Edgerton). When reduce feelings of loneliness and alienation.
focusing on social support, although students’ Intimacy with friends is negatively correlated
ratings of their closest friendships were not with loneliness (Wiseman, 1997), and thus we
significantly correlated with adjustment, when expected that lower intimacy between peers
students assessed support received from peers would be associated with poorer adjustment,
more generally, the greater the support particularly in a social and emotional/personal
received, the better was their emotional sense.
adjustment (e.g., less anxiety, better quality of
life; Abbey, Abramis, & Caplan, 1985). Fur­ Conflict and Adjustment
ther­more, researchers reported that increased In addition to the expectation of a positive
social support over the first two semesters of association between positive relationship
college predicted improved social and emo­ features and adjustment, negative aspects of
tional/personal adjustment (Friedlander, Reid, relationships may be associated with poorer
Shupak, & Cribbie, 2007). This leads to the adjustment. Although much research has been
expectation of positive associations between conducted to investigate the impact of peer
peer attachment and academic, social, and conflict (e.g., disagreements, arguing, fighting)
emotional/personal adjustment among emerg­ on the status or perceptions of relationships
ing adults, as is found between high school (e.g., see Collins & Laursen, 1992; Laursen,
romantic partners, college peers, and parents 1996), the literature on the link between
and teenage children. It is also reasonable to conflict and adjustment is not so extensive,
expect that more supportive friendships (i.e., even though such conflict could be damaging
better quality) would be associated with better to adjustment and success in a school environ­
adjustment. ment. Among adolescents, negative friendship
Another comparison of interest was the features such as conflict were associated with
quality of students’ relationships with their maladjustment, in terms of behavior problems
“old” high school best friend and their best and poor school grades (Berndt & Keefe, 1992;
new college friend to determine whether these Burk & Laursen, 2005). Thus, we would
relationships were associated with adjustment expect a significant negative association
in the same way. Bean (1985) described that between conflict and adjustment among
if students have greater attachments to emerging adults.
“outsiders” then they are not likely to be as
successfully socialized to the new institution, Purpose of the Present Study
thus suggesting the importance of making The main goal for this study was to describe
friends in the new school environment. the link between relationship quality and
Supporting the idea of moving on to new adjust­ment among first–year college students.
friendships, Paul and Brier (2001) used the Based on developmental research on children,
term “friendsickness” to describe the concept adolescents and older adults, positive friendship
of missing old friends and found that there qualities and better, more secure attachment
were negative implications of this emotional relationships are associated with better adjust­

November /December 2008 ◆ vol 49 no 6 553


Swenson, Nordstrom, & Hiester

ment. We expected to identify a significant waves of data collection. The average age of
positive association between relationship participants was 18.08 years (SD = .27). The
quality and adjustment among emerging majority of participants were Caucasian/White
adults. A second goal was to determine how (87%). Other racial and ethnic groups were
old high school friends and new college friends minimally represented, as matched the demo­
provided support. We expected that high graphics of the institutions and surrounding
school friendships could be beneficial when a communities.
student first starts college, but in order to Students who qualified for inclusion were
adjust and attach to the new environment, a recruited from two institutions in the north­
student must develop new intimate friendships eastern United States. Of the 271 total, 132
with college peers. A third goal of our study (66 females, 66 males) were enrolled at a
was to compare methods of assessing relation­ branch campus of a large state university and
ship quality among emerging adults. By using 139 (107 females, 32 males) were enrolled at
measures of attachment, we expected to a private liberal arts university. We recruited
confirm previous research showing that close students from two universities in order to
relationships are beneficial to adjustment increase our sample size, but not with the
during transitional periods. By using friendship intent of comparing samples. To ensure that
quality measures, we expected to explain how students at the two universities did not differ
and why these close relationships are beneficial significantly on demographic variables, we
by describing specific features that are asso­ conducted analyses to test for similarities. Age
ciated with adjustment. We expected to find and racial composition of the two universities
a positive association between relationship were similar and reflected population statistics
quality and adjustment when using both from the surrounding communities. Analyses
friendship quality and attachment measures, yielded a difference in sex composition, with
and we expected that the use of multiple the state university enrolling more males
measures would provide a more complete story relative to females and the private university
about this association. enrolling more females relative to males, which
also reflected university population character­
Method istics for these two institutions. Both univer­
sities were residential campuses with the
Participants
majority of students living on (73.7%) or near
A sample of 271 emerging adults in their first (3.8%) campus. The remainder of students
year of college (64% females) was selected from (22.5%) commuted from home, an average of
a larger sample of college students recruited 9 miles.
using convenience sampling techniques for a
short–term longitudinal study. Students were Measures
recruited during the first 2 weeks of fall The measures described below were part of a
semester classes (“Time 1” assessment) and larger packet of materials completed by
were asked to participate again during the 11th participants.
and 12th weeks of the same fall semester Friendship Quality. To indicate the quality
(“Time 2” assessment). Inclusion criteria for of their best friendships, participants completed
the present study were freshman (first–year) the Intimate Friendship Scale (IFS; Sharabany,
status, traditional age for college freshman (age 1994) regarding both their very best friend
18 or 19), and participation in both of the two from high school (Time 1 and 2 assessments)

554 Journal of College Student Development


Peer Relationships and Adjustment

and their very best new college friend (Time the experience with him/her); imposition
2). The IFS consists of 32 items which, (α = .70; e.g., If I want him/her to do
according to previous factor analyses (see something for me all I have to do is ask);
Sharabany) organize into eight subscales. Use common activities (α = .63; e.g., I like to do
of this scale in previous research with college things with him/her); and trust and loyalty
students showed high inter–item reliability (α = .67; e.g., I know that whatever I tell him/
among items on each subscale (Wiseman, her is kept secret between us). Participants
1997): frankness and spontaneity (α = .83; rated items using a 7–point scale to indicate
e.g., I feel free to talk to him/her about almost the degree to which each statement described
everything); sensitivity and knowing (α = .80; the relationship with their best friend (1 =
e.g., I know how he/she feels about things “This sentence does not describe your relationship
without him/her telling me); attachment at all ”; 7 = “This sentence describes your
(α = .81; e.g., I feel close to him/her); relationship very well ”). For data analyses, we
exclusiveness (α = .76; e.g., I do things with created eight subscale scores by averaging
him/her which are quite different from what ratings for the four items that composed each
others do); giving and sharing (α = .76; e.g., subscale (see Table 1 for mean ratings and
When something nice happens to me I share reliability statistics for subscales). We used the

Table 1.
Means, Standard Deviations, and Cronbach’s Alpha Reliability Statistics for
Relationship Quality Scales

T1 – High School T2 – High School T2 – College


Best Friend Best Friend Best Friend

α M SD α M SD α M SD

Intimate Friendship Scale


Frankness & Spontaneity .83 6.08 0.96 .89 5.94 1.20 .83 5.53 1.22
Sensitivity & Knowing .80 6.03 0.94 .89 5.89 1.19 .82 5.36 1.23
Attachment .74 5.53 1.16 .79 5.38 1.38 .78 4.75 1.36
Exclusiveness .54 4.61 1.06 .71 4.53 1.37 .61 3.91 1.20
Giving & Sharing .80 5.92 1.00 .85 5.83 1.17 .79 5.54 1.12
Imposition .74 5.56 1.06 .84 5.46 1.31 .76 4.98 1.26
Common Activities .66 5.50 1.05 .83 4.96 1.45 .72 4.89 1.23
Trust & Loyalty .76 6.17 0.94 .84 6.05 1.20 .76 5.78 1.14

Quality of Relationships Inventory


Conflict Subscale .85 1.47 0.36 .88 1.50 .47 .83 1.35 0.35

Inventory of Peer Attachment


Trust .90 4.37 0.58
Communication .86 3.94 0.69
Alienation .69 3.86 0.58

Note. Means represent the average rating of all subscale items.

November /December 2008 ◆ vol 49 no 6 555


Swenson, Nordstrom, & Hiester

eight subscale scores rather than an overall a fear of intimacy, which leads to avoidant
average or total score so that we could examine behaviors), preoccupied (overinvolvement or
the specific aspects of relationships that are preoccupation with an intimate relationship),
associated with adjustment. and dismissing–avoidant (dismissing of the
Peer Attachment. To indicate the quality importance of intimate relationships; Bartholo­
of the attachment with their best friends, mew & Horowitz). Validity of the attachment
participants completed the Inventory of Peer styles was established by comparing ratings of
Attachment, part of the larger Inventory of style with ratings of other self–report self–
Parent and Peer Attachment (Armsden & concept measures. For data analysis, attachment
Greenberg, 1987). The Inventory of Peer style was represented as a categorical variable.
Attachment is 25 items long, organized into Conflict. To indicate the extent to which
three subscales, which, according to previous conflict was present in their relationships with
factor analyses display high inter–item reli­ best friends, participants completed the
ability (see Armsden & Greenberg): trust conflict subscale of the Quality of Relationships
(α = .91; e.g., My friends accept me as I am.); Inventory (QRI, Pierce, Sarason, & Sarason,
communication (α = .87; e.g., My friends 1991). The QRI conflict subscale consists of
encourage me to talk about my difficulties.); 14 items for which participants rate the degree
and alienation (α = .72; e.g., My friends don’t to which the relationship is a source of conflict
understand what I am going through these (e.g., “How often do you have to work hard
days.). Items are rated on a scale from 1 (almost to avoid conflict with this person?”). Items are
never or never true) to 5 (almost always or always rated on a 4–point scale ranging from “not at
true) to indicate the extent to which each all” to “very much.” In previous research, the
statement is true about the relationship with conflict subscale displayed high reliability
one’s friends. For data analyses, we computed among item ratings (α = .91; Pierce et al.). For
three subscale scores to represent the average data analyses, we computed an average conflict
ratings of the 10 trust items (α = .90), 8 score by averaging the ratings of all 14 conflict
communication items (α = .86), and 7 aliena­ items (see Table 1 for mean ratings and
tion items (α = .69). We used subscale scores reliability statistics).
in analyses, rather than a total attachment Adjustment. We assessed four types of
score, so that we could examine how specific adjustment to college (academic, social, emo­
aspects of close friendships are associated with tional/personal, and institutional attach­ment)
adjustment (see Table 1 for mean ratings for using the Student Adaptation to College
each subscale). Questionnaire (SACQ; Baker & Siryk, 1984).
Attachment Style. To indicate general The SACQ consists of 67 items with 8 of the
attachment style with friends, participants items contributing to both the social adjust­
completed the Relationship Questionnaire ment and institutional attachment subscales.
(RQ; Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991). For Participants rate each item on a 9–point scale
this measure, participants read through four ranging from “applies very closely to me” to
paragraphs, each describing a different style, “doesn’t apply to me at all ” to indicate the
and they selected the paragraph that best degree to which the statement was true for
described themselves. The four attachment them at the time of assessment (Time 2). Baker
styles represented are: secure (characterized by and Siryk (1999) summarized the psycho­
comfort with both intimate relationships and metric properties of the SACQ and reported
autonomy), fearful–avoidant (characterized by high reliability for all subscales: academic

556 Journal of College Student Development


Peer Relationships and Adjustment

adjustment (24 items, α range = .81 to .90; second set of questionnaires. Of the 488, 321
e.g., “I am satisfied with the level at which I completed and returned the Time 2 booklets
am performing academically”); social adjust­ (66% participation rate). Of these participants,
ment (20 items, α range = .83 to .91; e.g., “I 271 (132 from the state university, 139 from
am meeting as many people and making as the liberal arts university) met criteria for
many friends as I would like at college.”); inclusion in our sample due to their age and
emotional/personal adjustment (15 items, class status. During Time 2, participants
α range = .77 to .86; e.g., “I have been feeling completed the IFS and QRI–Conflict regarding
tense or nervous lately.”); and institutional their best high school and best college friend,
attachment (15 items, α range = .85 to .91; as well as the SACQ.
e.g., “I expect to stay at college for a bachelor’s
degree.”). For data analyses, we created four Results
subscale scores by averaging the ratings for the
items that comprised each subscale. Data analyses addressed the following research
hypotheses: (a) The quality of peer relation­ships
Procedure would be positively associated with adjustment
We recruited participants using convenience to college (academic, social, emotional/
sampling techniques. During the first and personal, and institutional attach­ment) among
second weeks of the fall semester we visited 19 first–year college students; (b) The association
sections of English composition courses at one between friendship quality and adjustment
university and 13 sections of History courses would differ when assessing relationships with
at the second university because these courses best high school friends versus best new college
were taken by the majority of first–year friends; (c) Measures of friendship quality and
students and were general education core peer attachment among college friends would
requirements for all students. We explained consistently yield positive associations with all
the general purpose of the study and distributed forms of adjustment.
consent forms to review with the students. To
students who consented to participate, we gave Preliminary Analyses
a questionnaire booklet to complete on their Because data were collected from students at
own time outside of the classroom and asked two separate institutions, we conducted all
them to return it within 1 week. data analyses with institution (pubic, private)
In total, 488 students (257 from the state as a covariate. Results were identical to results
university, 231 from the liberal arts university) from analyses without the institution variable,
agreed to participate and completed the thus we report results from the aggregate
questionnaire booklets for the Time 1 assess­ sample.
ment, including completion of the IFS and Previous research has suggested that
QRI–Conflict in regards to their best high females report significantly closer peer relation­
school friend and the IPPA and RQ regarding ships than do males (e.g., Sharabany et al.,
their friendships in general. Ten weeks later, 1981), and one–way analyses of variance
we visited the same classes to follow up with confirm this sex difference with the IFS
the 488 students who completed the question­ subscales. We conducted all analyses with sex
naires at Time 1 and asked them to complete (male, female) as a covariate to test for an effect
a second booklet of questionnaires. We gave for sex in adjustment outcomes. Results were
students 1 week to complete and return the identical to results from analyses without the

November /December 2008 ◆ vol 49 no 6 557


Swenson, Nordstrom, & Hiester

Table 2.
Associations Among Friendship Quality with High School Best Friend
(Time 1 Assessment) and Adjustment to College

Student Adaptation to College Questionnaire

Emotional/
Academic Social Personal Institutional
Adjustment Adjustment Adjustment Attachment

Intimate Friendship Scale (T1)


Frankness & Spontaneity .11 .13 .15* .15*
Sensitivity & Knowing .00 –.00 –.08 –.06
Attachment –.06 –.11 –.08 –.10
Exclusiveness –.10 –.03 –.09 –.05
Giving & Sharing .07 .02 .06 .12
Imposition .10 –.01 .11 –.04
Common Activities –.12 .05 –.02 .02
Trust & Loyalty .08 .03 –.02 –.02
2
R for Overall Regression .12*** .07* .07* .07*
Model

Quality of Relationships Scale


(T1)
Conflict Subscale –.29*** –.18** –.18** –.26***

Note. Numbers in table represent unique contributions of predictors to R2 (semipartial correlations, sr2) in their
respective regression models.
*p < .03. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

sex variable, thus we report results without subscales as predictor variables and ratings for
discussion of sex. the SACQ as outcome or dependent variables.
Due to the high number of subscales of First, we conducted analyses involving ratings
the IFS, we examined multicollinearity of the relationship with one’s best high school
diagnostics within the regression models that friend at the beginning of the students’ first
follow to ensure that the variance accounted college semester (Time 1). Four linear regres­
for was not inflated due to a high degree of sion analyses were computed to assess associ­
overlap among the subscales. The variance ations among the eight IFS subscales with each
inflation factors (VIF) indicated that multi­ of the four SACQ subscales. Because of the
collinearity was not present among our data. high number of comparisons in the regression
analyses, we adopted a more conservative alpha
Friendship Quality and Adjustment value of .03.
We conducted standard multiple regression As expected, the total contribution of all
analyses to test the hypothesis of a positive IFS subscales were significant predictors of
association between friendship quality and each of the four types of adjustment (see Table
adjustment. We used ratings for the IFS 2 for R2 and associated values). When exam­

558 Journal of College Student Development


Peer Relationships and Adjustment

ining each predictor individually, ratings of of all IFS subscales were significant predictors
frankness and spontaneity items were positively of each of the four types of adjustment (see
associated with both emotional/personal Table 3 for R2 and associated values). Results
adjustment and institutional attachment (see differed, though, when we examined individ­
Table 2), meaning that greater frankness and ual friendship qualities in association with
spontaneity between friends was associated adjustment. Greater exclusivity with one’s best
with better adjustment. high school friend was significantly associated
We conducted another set of standard with poorer academic and social adjustment
multiple regression analyses to see whether and poorer institutional attachment (see Table
one’s best high school friend continued to 3). Higher ratings on the imposition subscale
benefit adjustment later in a student’s first (indicating a good and close relationship) were
semester. In four regression analyses, we associated with better emotional/personal
compared IFS subscale ratings of the quality adjustment (see Table 3). Finally, higher ratings
of the relationship with one’s high school best for the common activities subscale were
friend during Time 2 assessment to adjustment associated with greater institutional attachment
(SACQ subscales). Similar to analyses with (see Table 3).
Time 1 data, as expected the total contribution Next, we conducted standard multiple

Table 3.
Associations Among Friendship Quality with High School Best Friend
(Time 2 Assessment) and Adjustment to College

Student Adaptation to College Questionnaire

Emotional/
Academic Social personal Institutional
adjustment adjustment adjustment attachment

Intimate Friendship Scale (T2)


Frankness & Spontaneity –.01 .01 –.01 .02
Sensitivity & Knowing .03 .06 –.00 –.01
Attachment .03 –.04 –.10 –.00
Exclusiveness –.17** –.16* –.13 –.19**
Giving & Sharing –.07 –.11 .04 –.08
Imposition .08 .14* .14* .07
Common Activities .02 .12 .06 .19**
Trust & Loyalty .16* .09 .06 .11
R2 for Overall Regression .10** .12** .10* .10**
Model

Quality of Relationships Scale (T2)


Conflict Subscale –.23*** –.18** –.27*** –.23**

Note. Numbers in table represent unique contributions of predictors to R2 (semipartial correlations, sr2) in their
respective regression models.
* p < .03. ** p < .01. *** p < .001.

November /December 2008 ◆ vol 49 no 6 559


Swenson, Nordstrom, & Hiester

Table 4.
Associations Among Friendship Quality with New Best College Friend and
Adjustment to College

Student Adaptation to College Questionnaire

Emotional/
Academic Social Personal Institutional
Adjustment Adjustment Adjustment Attachment

Intimate Friendship Scale


Frankness & Spontaneity .07 .03 .03 .03
Sensitivity & Knowing .02 .09 .06 –.02
Attachment –.11 –.03 –.17* –.08
Exclusiveness –.02 .00 –.01 –.07
Giving & Sharing –.06 –.07 .03 –.05
Imposition .02 –.02 .04 .03
Common Activities .02 .15* .03 .21**
Trust & Loyalty .16* .10 .01 .11
2
R for Overall Regression .10** .17*** .05 .14***
Model

Quality of Relationships Scale


Conflict Subscale –.27*** –.20** –.17* –.26***

Note. Numbers in table represent unique contributions of predictors to R2 (semipartial correlations, sr2) in their
respective regression models.
*p < .03. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

regression analyses to compare ratings of the ment and institutional attachment (see Table
relationship with one’s best college friend to 4), meaning that having more activities and
ratings of adjustment. We conducted four interests in common was associated with better
regression analyses with the eight IFS subscales adjustment. Moreover, greater trust and loyalty
(college friend, Time 2) and each of the four between friends was associated with better
SACQ subscales. Supporting the hypothesis academic adjustment (see Table 4).
of a positive association between friendship
quality and adjustment, three of the four Attachment and Adjustment
regression models revealed that friendship To test the hypothesis of a consistent positive
quality was significantly associated with association with adjustment when using either
adjustment (see Table 4), the exception being friendship quality or attachment measures, we
the model predicting emotional/personal conducted another set of standard multiple
adjustment. Upon examining the individual regression analyses to compare ratings of the
contributions of each IFS subscale, ratings of Peer Attachment scale of the IPPA and ratings
common activities shared between friends were of items on the SACQ. We conducted four
positively associated with both social adjust­ regression analyses with the three peer attach­

560 Journal of College Student Development


Peer Relationships and Adjustment

ment subscales as predictor variables and each η2 = .04, and emotional/personal adjustment,
of the four SACQ subscales as outcome or F(3,206) = 4.82, p < .01, η 2 = .07. Upon
dependent variables. Results revealed that the examining post–hoc follow–up analyses, we
total contribution of all the peer attachment dis­covered that individuals who rated them­
subscales was a significant predictor of each of selves as securely attached had significantly
the four types of adjustment (see Table 5). better emotional/personal adjustment than did
Upon examining the individual contributions individuals who were “disorganized” in their
of each peer attachment subscale, greater attachments. Individuals who were disorganized
alienation from peers was associated with also differed significantly in their emotional/
poorer adjustment (all four forms; see Table 5). personal adjustment from those who were
Furthermore, ratings of trust were signifi­cantly avoidant (see Table 6).
associated with social adjustment and insti­
tutional attachment such that greater trust of Conflict and Adjustment
peers was associated with better adjustment Based on our hypothesis of a positive association
(see Table 5). between friendship quality (positive aspects of
As another test of the association between relationships) and adjustment, we expected
attachment and adjustment, we conducted a ratings of conflict to be negatively associated
general linear model analysis to assess the with adjustment. We conducted correlational
association between self–ratings of attachment analyses to compare ratings of the QRI–
style (from the RQ) and the subscales of the Conflict subscale regarding one’s best high
SACQ. In the overall model, attachment style school friend (Time 1 and Time 2) and one’s
was associated with adjustment at the multi­ best college friend (Time 2) to ratings of the
variate level, Wilks’ Λ = .88, F(12, 537) = 2.29, four SACQ subscales. Supporting our hypoth­
p < .01, η 2 = .04, and univariate level for esis, results of these analyses revealed that
social adjustment, F(3, 206) = 2.66, p < .05, conflict with both high school and college

Table 5.
Associations Among Attachment Behaviors and Adjustment to College

Student Adaptation to College Questionnaire

Emotional/
Academic Social Personal Institutional
Adjustment Adjustment Adjustment Attachment

Inventory Of Peer Attachment


Peer Trust .07 .13* .11 .13*
Peer Communication .00 .02 –.12 –.04
Peer Alienationa .22*** .17** .29*** .19**
R2 for Overall Regression .12*** .15*** .15*** .12***
Model
Note. Numbers in table represent unique contributions of predictors to R2 (semipartial correlations, sr2) in their
respective regression models.
a Alienation items were reverse–scored. Thus, higher scores indicate lesser alienation.
*p < .03. **p < .01. *** p < .001.

November /December 2008 ◆ vol 49 no 6 561


Swenson, Nordstrom, & Hiester

Table 6.
Attachment Style and Adjustment to College

Adjustment

Emotional/
Academic Social Personal Institutional

Attachment Style M SD M SD M SD M SD

Secure 51.61 11.02 50.74 8.87 50.91a 9.65 49.44 10.65

Anxious–Resistant 51.68 8.03 47.23 10.57 48.13 7.88 48.23 9.48

Avoidant 51.59 11.60 49.44 8.56 49.78b 10.71 49.53 7.68

Disorganized 48.52 7.05 46.04 7.21 43.26a,b 4.67 47.57 7.72

Note. Means in a column sharing subscripts are significantly different, p < .05, according to the Tukey honestly
significant difference comparison. For all adjustment categories, higher means indicate better adjustment.

friends was significantly associated with all and adolescence (e.g., see Hartup & Stevens,
forms of adjustment, such that a greater 1997; Newcomb & Bagwell, 1995; Rubin et
presence of conflict was associated with poorer al., 1998) and older adulthood (e.g., see Adams
adjustment to college (see Tables 2, 3, and 4). & Blieszner, 1995; Antonucci & Akiyama,
1995), we found that, during the first few
Discussion weeks of a college student’s first semester, the
relationship with one’s best high school friend
The major goal of this study was to describe is important because this friend is someone
the association between relationship quality with whom the new college student can be
and adjustment among college students. The open or frank. Having an old familiar friend
literature on this topic is small, and with to turn to when in need of peer support was
growing concern about retention of students related to both emotional/personal adjustment
on college campuses we sought to provide and institutional attachment. These results are
some useful information for campus personnel similar to other research on college students
who can assist students as they adjust to this that describes a positive association between
new environment and the emerging adulthood social support and emotional well–being
phase of life. (Abbey et al., 1985). Moreover, results are
con­sistent with research on younger adolescents
Relationship Quality and Adjustment describing that better–quality friendships are
We began with an assessment of friendship associated with fewer internalizing problems
quality and compared ratings of positive or better emotional adjustment (Cooper et al.,
aspects of relationships to several types of 1998; Demir & Urberg, 2004; Rubin et al.,
college adjustment, including academic, social, 2004). Perhaps it is the case that venting about
emotional/personal, and institutional attach­ stressors helps the new college student to not
ment. Consistent with research documenting only lesson the likelihood of poor emotional
the benefits of friendships during childhood coping but also to feel better about being in

562 Journal of College Student Development


Peer Relationships and Adjustment

the new college environment and, thus, more common interests. These results are similar to
attached to the institution itself. research that shows that having friends to
Later into a college student’s first semester, spend time with is positively correlated with
the role of the relationship with one’s best high satisfaction with student life (Astin, 1993).
school friend changed with regard to adjust­ Our data expand on this by revealing that
ment. Our results suggest that it is important knowledge that there is a friend available when
for a new college student to no longer be in needed may be enough to help students adjust
an exclusive relationship with his/her best high and commit to this new environment. The
school friend. It still may be helpful to know importance of loyal peers is characteristic of
this friend will be loyal and would be there if the adolescent age period (see Hartup, 1993;
needed without it being an imposition. Laursen, 1996; Sullivan, 1953) and continues
However, in order to really attach to the new to be important among emerging adults, as
environment and adjust socially, as is suggested suggested by our data. That trust and loyalty
by results of analyses of ratings of college of new college friends was significantly related
friendships, making new college friends is to academic adjustment could suggest that if
important to adjustment. a college student can successfully make new
These results support Bean’s (1985) friends who will be loyal and trustworthy, this
findings that socialization is not as successful can help them maintain some focus on aca­
if students are more attached to individuals demics and thus experience better adjustment
outside the institution as opposed to those in this area.
within. The different role of high school In addition to assessing the link between
friends in the life of college students who are friendship quality and adjustment for high
into the third month in their new environment school and college best friendships, we expected
also seems similar to literature describing the that adjustment would be positively associated
functions of friendships in older adulthood. with measures of attachment. In measuring
Carstensen’s (1987, 1992) socioemotional attachment with friends in general rather than
selectivity theory describes that as adults age attachment to one’s specific best friend, greater
into their later years they become more feelings of alienation from peers was associated
selective about the relationships that they with poor academic and emotional/personal
maintain. Older adults retain the relationships adjustment. This suggests that an insufficient
that provide them with the most support, number of friends may be related to a dimin­
whether it is perceived or enacted support. ished ability to concentrate on academic work.
Perhaps this filtering out process also takes Moreover, our results are consistent with
place with high school friends, especially as research describing a link between loneliness,
college students encounter new peers in their social anxiety, or insecure attachment styles
new environment from whom they may be and internalizing problems, such as depression
able to connect to draw support. (e.g., La Greca & Harrison, 2005; Lopez,
As expected, the new college friendships Mitchell, & Gormley, 2002). However, if a
that are forming play a different role than do new college student has a friend who will be
high school friendships. The pattern of results there for support or to show understanding,
regarding best college friendships suggests that this is associated with better social adjustment
academic and social adjustment and institu­ and attachment to the institution.
tional attachment are associated with finding Our assessment of attachment also in­
a person who will be loyal and who shares cluded a comparison of attachment style with

November /December 2008 ◆ vol 49 no 6 563


Swenson, Nordstrom, & Hiester

adjustment outcomes. Results were consistent Given the specific friendship qualities that we
with previous findings described that closer identified in relation to the specific types of
relationships are associated with better emo­ adjustment, this study should be useful to
tional/personal adjustment and that less college personnel who may wish to intervene
security in relationships is associated with to help new students adjust to the college
poorer adjustment. These results are consistent environment. For example, providing opportu­
with previous research focusing on children nities for peer interaction and friendship
and adolescents (e.g., Cooper et al., 1998; formation seems crucial to helping students
Greenberg et al., 1983), and college students adjust to their new environment. It also could
(Fass & Tubman, 2002; Lapsley & Edgerton, be beneficial to recognize the importance of
2002). high school friendships during the initial weeks
of a student’s first semester. Counselors could
Conflict and Adjustment help new students learn how to balance time
Another assessment was of negative friendship with “old” friends back home and time with
quality, specifically conflict. When we examined new peers in the new college setting.
the link between conflict and adjustment, we Another strategy for intervention could be
found that if there was conflict, this was to use assessments of relationship quality to
associated with poorer adjustment in all areas, identify at–risk students who have the potential
which is consistent with results from previous to be lost from the system. Although it seems
research on young children (Ladd, Kochen­ that the measures are consistent in describing
derfer, & Coleman, 1996). Although our data a positive association between relationship
do not allow for conclusions about a causal quality and adjustment, we recommend that
relationship between conflict and adjustment, researchers or college personnel who are
a possibility for future research would be to interested in studying this link utilize a
determine whether the conflict occurs first. If it measure that can provide a more detailed
does, then perhaps this is related to dimin­ished analysis of peer relationships, such as the
ability to concentrate on adjusting to new friendship quality measure utilized in this
academic demands, to make new friends, or to study (Intimate Friendship Scale, Sharabany,
attach oneself to the new college environment. 1994).
Research on social support suggests that a good
support network can serve as a buffer against Limitations and Recommendations
stressors and is associated with better mental for Future Research
and emotional health (e.g., Abbey et al., 1985; We hope that this study may inspire the
Adams & Blieszner, 1995; Buhrmester, 1990). growth of research on the role of peer relation­
If a student is in conflict with peers, this could ships in the lives of emerging adults. Although
limit the size of his/her support network and, our results show only small amounts of
thus, adjustment may suffer. variance in adjustment accounted for by
relationship quality, the fact that the contribu­
Applications of this Research tions were significant and supported our
Taking all of these findings into consideration, hypotheses based on previous research suggests
the picture is clear: Relationship quality is that peer relationships do have a significant
positively and significantly associated with effect on adjustment and should be studied
adjustment among first–year college students further. Given all the transitions that these
who are in their emerging adulthood years. individuals experience, having a supportive

564 Journal of College Student Development


Peer Relationships and Adjustment

network of peers should benefit them in many peer relationships affect adjustment. For
ways. example, examining verbal communications
Future research could explore other between peers could provide insight into how
outcome variables that are associated with the peers provide support, or interviews with
success or failure of close peer relationships. students might uncover what makes friends
In this study we only assessed types of trustworthy.
adjustment. We encourage researchers to move In addition to considering different
forward to examine how peer relationships and measurement techniques, researchers may also
adjustment are associated with additional wish to replicate this research with different
outcomes such as academic achievement and samples to determine the generalizability of
overall retention in college, or whether other results. We used convenience sampling to
variables such as self–esteem, mental health, recruit students for this study. It may be infor­
or high school academic achievement might ma­tive to use random sampling techniques to
medi­ate the association between relationship generate a sample from different types of
quality and adjustment or college universities and colleges or to compare students
achievement. at different institutions (e.g., community col­
We also encourage researchers to consider leges vs. large research universities; campuses
other methods of assessing peer relationships in urban areas vs. rural).
to understand better the links between specific
relationship qualities and outcomes such as Conclusions
adjustment. Although the self–report measures Peer relationships are an integral part of
that we selected for this study were previously adolescents’ and emerging adults’ lives. In this
reported to be reliable and valid, we did study, we identified specific ways in which
discover several low Cronbach’s alpha statistics close peer relationships are associated with
when assessing reliability of subscales (r’s adjustment to college. Maintaining ties with
ranged from .54 to . 90). We chose to report high school friends can help a new college
results despite lower reliabilities in order to student adjust during the initial transition
make comparisons with previous research period, but it is also important for these college
using these measures. Given that we found students to make new friends in their new
some consistent patterns of results across environment if they want to improve their
measures, this could suggest that the low chances of success. Given the serious implica­
reliabilities did not have an impact. However, tions of failure in college, this study provides
we recommend that future research consider empirical evidence for the importance of
different self–report measures of friendship friendships in the transition to college.
quality to determine whether a similar pattern
of results still emerges. Moreover, we suggest Correspondence concerning this article should be
extending the literature by using different addressed to Lisa Swenson, Penn State Hazleton, 76 Uni­
forms of measurement to further analyze how ver­sity Drive, Hazleton, PA 18202; Lms42@psu.edu

November /December 2008 ◆ vol 49 no 6 565


Swenson, Nordstrom, & Hiester

References
Abbey, A., Abramis, D. J., & Caplan, R. D. (1985). Effects of Chickering, A. W., & Reisser, L. (1993). Education and identity
different sources of social support and social conflict on (2nd ed.). San Francisco: Jossey–Bass.
emotional well–being. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 6, Collins, W. A., & Laursen, B. (1992). Conflict and relationships
111–129. during adolescence. In C. U. Shantz & W. W. Hartup (Eds.),
Adams, R. G., & Blieszner, R. (1995). Aging well with friends Conflict in child and adolescent development (pp. 216–241).
and family. American Behavioral Scientist, 39, 209–224. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Antonucci, T. C., & Akiyama, H. (1995). Convoys of social Cooper, M. L., Shaver, P. R., & Collins, N. L. (1998).
relations: Family and friendships within a life span context. Attachment styles, emotion regulation, and adjustment in
In R. Blieszner & V.H. Bedford (Eds.), Handbook of aging adolescence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74,
and the family (pp. 355–371). Westport, CT: Greenwood 1380–1397.
Press. Demir, M., & Urberg, K. A. (2004). Friendship and adjustment
Armsden, G. C., & Greenberg, M. T. (1987). The Inventory among adolescents. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,
of Parent and Peer Attachment: Individual differences and 88, 68–82.
their relationship to psychological well–being in adolescence. Erikson, E. H. (1963). Childhood and society, (2nd ed.). New
Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 16, 427–454. York: W.W. Norton.
Arnett, J. J. (2004). Emerging adulthood: The winding road from Fass, M. E., & Tubman, J. G. (2002). The influence of parental
the late teens through the twenties. New York: Oxford and peer attachment on college students’ academic
University Press. achievement. Psychology in the Schools, 39, 561–573.
Astin, A. W. (1993). What matters in college? Four critical years Fraley, R. C., & Davis, K. E. (1997). Attachment formation
revisited. San Francisco: Jossey–Bass. and transfer in young adults’ close friendships and romantic
Baker, R. W., & Siryk, B. (1984). Measuring adjustment to relationships. Personal Relationships, 4, 131–144.
college. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 31, 179–189. Friedlander, L. J., Reid, G. J., Shupak, N., & Cribbie, R. (2007).
Baker, R. W., & Siryk, B. (1999). Student Adaptation to College Social support, self–esteem, and stress as predictors of
Questionnaire manual. Los Angeles: Western Psychological adjustment to university among first–year undergraduates.
Services. Journal of College Student Development, 48, 259–274.
Bartholomew, K., & Horowitz, L. M. (1991). Attachment styles Gavin, L. A., & Furman, W. (1989). Age differences in
among young adults: A test of a four–category model. Journal adolescents’ perceptions of their peer groups. Developmental
of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 226–244. Psychology, 25, 827–834.
Bean, J. P. (1985). Interaction effects based on class level in an Goldberg, S. (2000). Attachment and development. London:
explanatory model of college student dropout syndrome. Arnold.
American Educational Research Journal, 22, 35–64. Grabill, C. M., & Kerns, K. A. (2000). Attachment style and
Berndt, T. J., & Keefe, K. (1992). Friends’ influence on intimacy in friendship. Personal Relationships, 7, 363–378.
adolescents’ perceptions of themselves at school. In D. G., Greenberg, M. T., Siegel, J. M., & Leitch, C. J. (1983). The
Schunk & J. L. Meece (Eds.). Student perceptions in the nature and importance of attachment relationships to parents
classroom. Hillsdale, NJ: Laurence Erlbaum. and peers during adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence,
Buhrmester, D. (1990). Intimacy of friendship, interpersonal 12, 373–386.
competence, and adjustment during preadolescence and Hartup, W. W. (1993). Adolescents and their friends. In B.
adolescence. Child Development, 61, 1101–1111. Laursen (Ed.), Close friendships in adolescence (pp. 3–22). San
Bukowski, W. M., Hoza, B. & Boivin, M. (1994). Measuring Francisco: Jossey–Bass.
friendship quality during pre– and early adolescence: The Hartup, W. W., & Stevens, N. (1997). Friendship and
development of psychometric properties of the Friendship adaptation in the life course. Psychological Bulletin, 121,
Qualities Scale. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 355–370.
11, 471–484. Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. R. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized
Burk, W. J., & Laursen, B. (2005). Adolescent perceptions of as an attachment process. Journal of Personality and Social
friendship and their associations with individual adjustment. Psychology, 52, 511–524.
International Journal of Behavioral Development, 29, Ladd, G. W., & Kochenderfer, B. J. (1996). Linkages between
156–164. friendship and adjustment during early school transitions.
Carbery, J., & Buhrmester, D. (1998). Friendship and need In W. M. Bukowski, A. F. Newcomb, & W. W. Hartup (Eds.),
fulfillment during three phases of young adulthood. Journal The company they keep: Friendship in childhood and adolescence
of Social and Personal Relationships, 15(3), 393–409. (pp. 322–345). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Carstensen, L. L. (1987). Age–related changes in social activity. Ladd, G. W., Kochenderfer, B. J., & Coleman, C. C. (1996).
In L.L . Carstensen & B. A. Edelstein (Eds.), Handbook of Friendship quality as a predictor of young children’s early
clinical gerontology (pp. 222–237). Elmsford, NY: Pergamon school adjustment. Child Development, 67, 1103–1118.
Press. La Greca, A. M., & Harrison, H. M. (2005). Adolescent peer
Carstensen, L. L. (1992). Social and emotional patterns in relations, friendships, and romantic relationships: Do they
adulthood: Support for socioemotional selectivity theory. predict social anxiety and depression? Journal of Clinical Child
Psychology and Aging, 7, 331–338. and Adolescent Psychology, 34, 49–61.

566 Journal of College Student Development


Peer Relationships and Adjustment

Lapsley, D. K., & Edgerton, J. (2002). Separation–individuation, Pierce, G. R., Sarason, I. G., & Sarason, B. R. (1991). General
adult attachment style, and college adjustment. Journal of and relationship–based perceptions of social support: Are
Counseling and Development, 80, 484–492. two constructs better than one? Journal of Personality and
Laursen, B. (1996). Closeness and conflict in adolescent peer Social Psychology, 61, 1028–1039.
relationships: Interdependence with friends and romantic Rose, S. M. (1984). How friendships end: Patterns among young
partners. In W. M. Bukowski, A. F. Newcomb, & W. W. adults. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 1,
Hartup (Eds.), The company they keep: Friendship in childhood 267–277.
and adolescence (pp. 186–212). New York: Cambridge Rubin, K. H., Bukowski, W., & Parker, J. G. (1998). Peer
University Press. interactions, relationships, and groups. In W. Damon & N.
Lopez, F. G., Mitchell, P., & Gormley, B. (2002) Adult Eisenberg (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3 Social,
attachment orientations and college student distress: Test of emotional, and personality development (5th ed., pp. 619–683).
a mediational model. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 49, New York: John Wiley & Sons.
460–467. Rubin, K. H., Dwyer, K. M., Booth–LaForce, C., Kim, A. H.,
Markiewicz, D., Doyle, A. B., & Brendgen, M. (2001). The Burgess, K. B., & Rose–Krasnor, L. (2004). Attachment,
quality of adolescents’ friendships: Associations with mothers’ friendship, and psychosocial functioning in early adolescence.
interpersonal relationships, attachments to parents and Journal of Early Adolescence, 24, 326–356.
friends, and prosocial behaviors. Journal of Adolescence, 24, Sharabany, R. (1994). Intimate Friendship Scale: Conceptual
429–445. underpinnings, psychometric properties and construct valid­
Mikulincer, M., & Nachslon, O. (1991). Attachment styles and ity. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 11,
patterns of self–disclosure. Journal of Personality and Social 449–469.
Psychology, 61, 321–331. Sharabany, R., Gershoni, R., & Hofman, J. E. (1981).
Newcomb, A. F., & Bagwell, C. L. (1995). Children’s friendship Girlfriend, boyfriend: Age and sex differences in intimate
relations: A meta–analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 117, friendship. Developmental Psychology, 17, 800–808.
306–347. Sullivan, H. S. (1953). The interpersonal theory of psychiatry.
Parker, J. G., & Asher, S. R. (1993). Friendship and friendship New York: W.W. Norton.
quality in middle childhood: Links with peer group Tinto, V. (1975). Dropout from higher education: A theoretical
acceptance and feelings of loneliness and social dissatisfaction. synthesis of recent research. Review of Educational Research,
Developmental Psychology, 29, 611–621. 45, 89–125.
Pascarella, E. T., & Terenzini, P. T. (1980). Predicting freshman Wiseman, H. (1997). Interpersonal relatedness and self–
persistence and voluntary dropout decisions form a definition in the experience of loneliness during the transition
theoretical model. Journal of Higher Education, 51, 61–95. to university. Personal Relationships, 4, 285–299.
Paul, E. L., & Brier, S. (2001). Friendsickness in the transition You, H. S., & Malley–Morrison, K. (2000). Young adult
to college: Precollege predictors and college adjustment cor­ attachment styles and intimate relationships with close
relates. Journal of Counseling and Development, 79, 77–89. friends. Journal of Cross–Cultural Psychology, 31, 528–534.

November /December 2008 ◆ vol 49 no 6 567

You might also like