Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 13

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/327639199

Don’t Get Too Excited: Assessing Individual Differences in the Down-


Regulation of Positive Emotions

Article in Journal of Personality Assessment · September 2018


DOI: 10.1080/00223891.2018.1501247

CITATIONS READS

0 9,970

3 authors:

Christopher Zou Jason E. Plaks


Ryerson University University of Toronto
16 PUBLICATIONS 362 CITATIONS 60 PUBLICATIONS 2,165 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Jordan B Peterson
University of Toronto
143 PUBLICATIONS 13,868 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Jordan B Peterson on 04 August 2021.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Journal of Personality Assessment

ISSN: 0022-3891 (Print) 1532-7752 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hjpa20

Don't Get Too Excited: Assessing Individual


Differences in the Down-Regulation of Positive
Emotions

Christopher Zou, Jason E. Plaks & Jordan B. Peterson

To cite this article: Christopher Zou, Jason E. Plaks & Jordan B. Peterson (2017): Don't Get Too
Excited: Assessing Individual Differences in the Down-Regulation of Positive Emotions, Journal of
Personality Assessment

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2017.1339711

Published online: 05 Jul 2017.

Submit your article to this journal

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=hjpa20

Download by: [72.142.16.190] Date: 05 July 2017, At: 13:28


JOURNAL OF PERSONALITY ASSESSMENT
https://doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2017.1339711

Don’t Get Too Excited: Assessing Individual Differences in the Down-Regulation


of Positive Emotions
Christopher Zou, Jason E. Plaks, and Jordan B. Peterson
Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Ontario, Canada

ABSTRACT ARTICLE HISTORY


A voluminous literature has documented the importance of emotion regulation for health and well-being. Received 14 July 2015
The studies in this literature, however, have generally focused on the down-regulation of negative affect. Revised 5 May 2017
Few studies have examined the down-regulation of positive affect. In Study 1, we constructed a scale, the
revised Regulatory Emotional Self-Efficacy Scale (r–RESE), which assesses both the down- and up-
regulation of positive affect, in addition to the traditional down-regulation of negative affect. In Study 2,
we conducted an extensive validation of the r–RESE scale, using a multimethod approach with informant
ratings, to illustrate that the down-regulation of positive affect represents a process independent of each
of the other forms of emotion regulation. In Study 3, we provided evidence that the ability to down-
regulate positive emotions provides added predictive utility when predicting indexes of impulsivity and
adjustment. Across the studies, we illustrate the potential importance of the down-regulation of positive
emotions as a topic of study for the field of emotion regulation.

Emotion regulation processes influence the experience of posi- Barbaranelli, Gerbino, & Pastorelli, 2003; Caprara et al., 2008),
tive and negative emotions (Gross & John, 2003), interpersonal better adjustment (Caprara et al., 2008), and less delinquency
relationships (Gross & John, 2003), and even physical health (Bandura et al., 2003).
(John & Gross, 2004). Much of the literature on emotion regu- As helpful as the RESE has been, a significant limitation is
lation has focused on the effects of different emotion regulation that it does not address a potentially useful element of emotion
strategies (i.e., reappraisal, suppression). Fewer studies have regulation: the down-regulation of positive emotions. Imagine
examined individual differences in the ability to implement two friends, A and B. Both are being considered for the same
such strategies. job. While having dinner with B, A receives a call offering her
Several scales have been constructed to assess emotion the job. Under such circumstances (i.e., an “outperformance
regulation, such as the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation situation”; Exline & Lobel, 1999), it is socially appropriate for A
Scale (DERS; Gratz & Roemer, 2004) and the Implicit The- to attenuate her expression of positive emotion until a more
ories of Emotion Scale (ITES; Tamir, John, Srivastava, & opportune moment. Recent studies have demonstrated that
Gross, 2007). However, such scales suffer from a number of individuals who suppressed, rather than expressed, their posi-
limitations. For instance, they typically do not distinguish tive emotions when outperforming others were evaluated more
between the up-regulation and the down-regulation of emo- positively (Schall, Martiny, Goetz, & Hall, 2016).
tions, which are distinct processes (Kim & Hamann, 2007). Comparatively few studies have focused on the potentially
More important, they rarely assess the regulation of positive detrimental consequences of positive emotions (e.g., Tugade &
emotions, which has been associated with well-being (e.g., Fredrickson, 2007). Even fewer have concentrated on the
Tugade & Fredrickson, 2007). potential usefulness of its down-regulation. For instance, the
One emotion regulation scale that does assess self-perceived Emotional Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ) by Gross and John
ability to regulate negative and positive emotions is the Regula- (2003) includes just one item assessing the down-regulation of
tory Emotional Self-Efficacy (RESE) scale (Caprara et al., positive emotions (“When I am feeling positive emotions, I am
2008). It contains two subfactors for regulating negative affect: careful not to express them”). Moreover, during scoring, that
managing despondency and controlling anger. It also includes item is aggregated with the negative emotion items. Ochsner
items tapping the up-regulation of positive emotions (e.g., and colleagues (2004), however, reported that up-regulation
“How well can you rejoice over successes?”). Higher scores on and down-regulation of emotions invoke unique neural cir-
both negative and positive emotion subscales, indicating higher cuits. Such results suggest the potential utility of assessing emo-
belief in the capacity to control personal emotions, have been tional down-regulation separately from up-regulation, for both
linked to more prosocial behavior (Bandura, Caprara, negative and positive emotions.

CONTACT Christopher Zou christopher.zou@alum.utoronto.ca Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, 100 St. George St., 4th Floor, Sidney Smith
Hall, Toronto, ON M5S 3G3, Canada.

© 2017 Taylor & Francis


2 ZOU, PLAKS, PETERSON

Indeed, extreme cases of mass unregulated positive emotion, Study 1


such as celebratory riots, might be rooted in situational cues that
We sought to create a revised measure of emotion regulation
loosen individuals’ ability to down-regulate their positive emo-
ability that possessed higher face validity and captured domains
tions. At the individual differences level, both mania and impul-
of emotion regulation not captured by the original RESE. We
sivity have been linked with excessive positive affectivity
examined whether scores on our measure would exhibit a four-
(Fredrickson, 1998; Revelle, Humphreys, Simon, & Gilliland,
factor structure: the three factors found in the original RESE
1980). Thus although there are numerous studies illustrating the
(up-regulation of positive emotions, down-regulation of
detrimental effects of negative emotions (Fredrickson, 1998),
despondency and distress, down-regulation of anger; Caprara
excessive positive emotions can also be socially disruptive.
et al., 2008), as well as a novel factor, the down-regulation of
positive emotions.
Validation of the revised RESE scale
The original RESE scale assesses individuals’ ability to up-regu- Method
late positive emotions and down-regulate negative emotions,
but neglects the down-regulation of positive emotions. For this Participants
reason, we generated and selected items for the revised RESE Four hundred and ninety-five participants (58.4% female) were
(r–RESE), adding items that focused specifically on positive recruited through Amazon’s Mechanical Turk (MTurk). The
emotion down-regulation. Additionally, given that neuroticism average age was 33.28 (SD D 12.51, range D 19–88). Given cer-
has been linked to emotion regulation (Shields & Cicchetti, tain limitations of MTurk samples (e.g., Chandler, Mueller, &
1997), we examined the discriminant validity of each dimen- Paolacci, 2014; Crump, McDonnell, & Gureckis, 2013), includ-
sion from neuroticism. We assessed convergent and discrimi- ing participation in related experiments and poor attention to
nant validity of scores on the r–RESE in Study 2 using a stimuli, we used an undergraduate sample in Study 2. In Study
multimethod approach (Campbell & Fiske, 1959), and evalu- 3 we collected an MTurk sample 3 years after Study 1. Addi-
ated its concurrent validity in Study 3. tionally, the total running time of the surveys on MTurk in this
One challenge we faced was that there is no reliable objective study and in Study 3 was kept to an average time of 15 min to
criterion by which to validate a person’s emotion regulation sustain participants’ attention.
ability. Although researchers have introduced a number of
emotion regulation tasks, such as the emotional Stroop task Materials and procedure
(Williams, Mathews, & MacLeod, 1996) and the emotional go– Participants first read a consent form detailing the nature of the
no-go task (Hare et al., 2008), the reliabilities of scores on such study. Then they completed the questionnaires listed next.
tasks have either come into question (Algom, Chajut, & Lev, After completing the questionnaires, participants were
2004) or are unknown. debriefed about the purpose of the study and compensated
Informant reports can be an effective method to obtain with $0.50.
meaningful personality information from an external source
(Vazire, 2006; Zou, Schimmack, & Gere, 2013). Well- r-RESE scale. A group of four graduate students and one fac-
acquainted informants are in a position to provide information ulty member (including authors of this article) generated a
about an individual’s behavior over an extended period of time. pool of items for the r–RESE scale and assessed all items’ face
Multiple informant reports increase validity by reducing mea- validity with dichotomous yes–no responses. We first examined
surement error (Connelly & Ones, 2010). In Study 2, we exam- the face validity of the 12 items of the original RESE scale using
ined the validity of scores on the r–RESE scale with a the widely accepted definition of emotion regulation by Gross
multitrait–multimethod (MTMM) approach, using self- and (1998): “the process by which individuals influence which emo-
informant reports from three informants per participant. tions they have, when they have them, and how they experience
In Study 3 we tested the concurrent validity of scores on the and express these emotions” (p. 275). All members of the group
r–RESE by examining how it would predict outcome variables unanimously agreed that the four items tapping the up-regula-
linked with emotion regulation. We paid particular attention to tion of positive affect in the original RESE scale do not capture
the link between emotion regulation and risky behaviors, as people’s ability to regulate their emotions, but rather people’s
there is evidence that the regulation of positive emotions exerts ability to react positively to specific events (“express joy when
unique influence on involvement in delinquency (Caprara & good things happen to you,” “feel gratified over achieving what
Gerbino, 2001). However, prior studies have tended to focus on you set out to do,” “rejoice over your successes,” “express
effective expression of positive emotions. We tested whether enjoyment freely at parties”). Therefore, these four items evalu-
ability to down-regulate positive emotions would be an impor- ating the up-regulation of positive emotions were not retained
tant predictor of risky behaviors. Previous research has demon- in the r–RESE.
strated that both extraversion and neuroticism uniquely predict However, the group unanimously agreed that the other eight
risky behavior (e.g., alcohol abuse, risky sex). Neurotic individu- items of the RESE scale were face valid and were thus retained.
als engage in risky behaviors to cope with negative mood states, We made slight modifications to the items so that they assessed
whereas extraverted individuals do so to enhance positive affect people’s general emotion regulation abilities, rather than pro-
(Cooper, Agocha, & Sheldon, 2000). Such work indirectly sug- cesses deployed in a specific context. For instance, the item
gests that the suppression of both positive and negative emotions “reduce your upset when you don’t get the appreciation you
might contribute to curbing involvement in risky behaviors. feel you deserve,” was changed to, “reduce your upset.”
DIFFERENCES IN DOWN-REGULATION OF POSITIVE EMOTIONS 3

Next, the group generated an additional 32 items by assess- the original RESE items (2 for D-DES and 1 for D-ANG) were
ing each potential item’s face validity. We followed the same retained in the final version of the r–RESE.
structure as the original RESE scale, in which participants rate The final r–RESE contained 12 items (Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin
how well they can perform the task on a scale from 1 (not very measure of sampling adequacy D .824, and Bartlett’s test of
well) to 7 (extremely well). sphericity was significant, x2(66) D 2374.25, p < .001). Follow-
To generate the items, we used a deductive approach (Bur- ing the recommendation by Fabrigar and colleagues (1999), we
isch, 1984), based on existing theories about emotions. conducted a parallel analysis along with a scree test to deter-
Informed by the circumplex model of emotion (Posner, Russell, mine the number of factors to retain. A parallel analysis (Horn,
& Peterson, 2005), we assessed the regulation of positive and 1965) was conducted using O’Connor’s (2000) syntax for SPSS
negative emotions on spectra of valence (pleasantness) and using a permutation test of 1,000 iterations with the same num-
arousal (activation). Additionally, given evidence that the up- ber of cases and variables as the original data set and an average
regulation and down-regulation of emotions are distinct pro- eigenvalue of 95% confidence interval (CI) for each factor.
cesses (Caprara et al., 2008; Kim & Hamann, 2007), we gener- Scree plots (Cattell, 1966) were also examined to supplement
ated items to tap both processes. The preliminary r–RESE scale our findings. Both the parallel analysis and the scree test sug-
consisted of 8 items assessing the up-regulation of positive gested a four-factor solution: D-DES (down-regulation of
affect (U-POS), 10 items assessing the down-regulation of despondency), D-ANG (down-regulation of anger), U-POS
despondency or distress (D-DES; 4 from the original RESE (up-regulation of positive emotions), and D-POS (down-regu-
scale), 5 items assessing the down-regulation of anger (D-ANG; lation of positive emotions). The communalities of all the varia-
4 from the original RESE scale), and 8 items assessing the bles were greater than .5, and all the factor loadings were
down-regulation of PA (D-POS). Finally, we included 9 items greater than .5.
assessing the up-regulation of negative affect (NA): despon-
dency or distress and anger.
Descriptive statistics
All analyses in Study 1 were conducted using SPSS Version 23.
Neuroticism. A possible confound of r–RESE is trait neuroti-
All items had a skew and kurtosis value of less than 1.0, sug-
cism. Neuroticism has been defined as emotionality or anxiety,
gesting an approximately normal distribution. Three items
and has sometimes been used interchangeably with emotional
assess beliefs about one’s ability to up-regulate positive emo-
(in)stability (John & Srivastava, 1999). Therefore, we consid-
tions (U-POS; a D .80), three assess managing despondency or
ered it necessary to demonstrate that the r–RESE is not another
distress (D-DES; a D .84), three assess managing anger (D-
measure of neuroticism. We assessed neuroticism using the
ANG; a D .81), and three items assess reducing positive emo-
eight neuroticism items in the Big Five Inventory (BFI; John &
tions (D-POS; a D .78). We averaged the three items across
Srivastava, 1999); for example, “I get nervous easily” (1 D
each factor to create mean composite scores for D-DES, U-
strongly agree to 7 D strongly disagree; a D .88).
POS, D-ANG, and D-POS. All factors demonstrated good reli-
ability. Descriptive scores for all factors and the full set of items
Results are provided in Table 1.
We performed Bonferroni-corrected pairwise t tests to
Exploratory factor analysis
examine differences in composite scores between the four fac-
We used an inductive approach by conducting an exploratory
tors. Of all the factors, the down-regulation of anger (D-ANG)
factor analysis (EFA) on the 40 items using SPSS Version 23.
had the highest score, D-ANG and U-POS, t(489) D 11.27, p <
Following Caprara and colleagues’ (2008) method of factor
.001, d D 1.02; D-ANG and D-DES, t(489) D 5.00, p < .001, d
analysis, we first conducted a maximum likelihood (ML) factor
D .45, although it did not differ significantly from D-POS. Sim-
analysis using a Promax rotation with the 40 items of the initial
ilarly, down-regulation of positive emotions scores (D-POS)
r–RESE. Although the authors of the original RESE conducted
and down-regulation of despondency or distress (D-DES)
a principal-axis factor analysis in the initial study, none of the
scores did not differ significantly from one another, but both
items in the current r–RESE were nonnormally distributed.
scores were significantly higher than the up-regulation of posi-
Thus, the ML estimation procedure was applied, as recom-
tive emotions (U-POS), D-POS and U-POS, t(489) D 8.14, p <
mended by Fabrigar, Wegener, MacCallum, and Strahan
.001, d D .74; D-POS and D-DES, t(489) D 7.62, p < .001, d D
(1999). Following the suggestions of Costello and Osborne
.69. These results suggest that people believe that they are better
(2005), we omitted items with low communality (< .40) and
at down-regulating than up-regulating their emotions, and
items that did not load > .30 on any of the factors. We also
believe they are best at down-regulating their anger.
omitted items that loaded > .30 on more than one factor. We
repeated the EFA until all the items maintained a communality
greater than .40, and all the items loaded >.30 on only one fac- Relationship of r–RESE to neuroticism
tor. Once the final EFA model was generated, a factor of up- Although neuroticism was strongly negatively correlated with
regulation of NA did not emerge. In the final EFA, there were D-DES (r D –.66) and D-ANG (r D –.46), it was not as strongly
three items that assessed the down-regulation of anger. Thus, related to the other factors (D-POS r D –.25; U-POS r D –.26).
we trimmed the number of items in each factor down to three This suggests that neuroticism more strongly reflects individu-
to keep the number consistent across each factor, while main- als’ beliefs about their inability to regulate negative emotions
taining the minimum number of items required to maintain a (»30% shared variance), rather than positive emotions (»6%
factor in a factor analysis (Costello & Osborne, 2005). Three of shared variance).
4 ZOU, PLAKS, PETERSON

Table 1. Factor loadings and communalities based on a maximum-likelihood factor analysis with promax rotation for 12 items from the revised Regulatory Emotional Self-
Efficacy (r–RESE) scale.
D-DES U-POS D-POS D-ANG Com

1. Manage negative feelings? .72 .07 .02 .04 .63


2. Keep from getting discouraged in the face of difficulties? .93 ¡.07 ¡.08 ¡.10 .68
3. Reduce your upset? .74 .03 .00 .07 .63
4. Act excited for events you’re actually not excited about? ¡.09 .77 .03 ¡.01 .54
5. Be happy even when you’re not? .15 .68 .03 ¡.07 .58
6. Act cheerful even though you don’t feel that way? ¡.04 .83 ¡.07 .04 .64
7. Keep from getting overly excited? .03 ¡.04 .72 .01 .52
8. Contain your excitement? ¡.02 .05 .75 .00 .58
9. Avoid becoming overly energetic? ¡.05 ¡.02 .78 ¡.03 .55
10. Avoid losing your temper? ¡.04 ¡.06 .00 .91 .76
11. Avoid flying off the handle when you get angry? ¡.05 .01 ¡.06 .84 .64
12. Cool off when feeling angry? .23 .08 .10 .46 .50
Eigenvalue 4.53 1.70 1.44 1.10
Randomly generated eigenvalue 1.27 1.20 1.14 1.10
M 4.60 4.08 4.72 4.92
SD 1.38 1.40 1.38 1.42

Note. N D 484. D-DES D down-regulation of despondency or distress; U-POS D up-regulation of positive emotions; D-POS D down-regulation of positive emotions; DANG
D down-regulation of anger. The randomly generated eigenvalues are principal component eigenvalues from a parallel analysis, which is the recommended procedure
by Cattell (O’Connor, 2000). However, randomly generated common/principal factor eigenvalues using parallel analysis suggest a three-factor solution as the fourth fac-
tor eigenvalue is lower than the randomly generated eigenvalue (actual eigenvalue D 1.10 vs. randomly generated eigenvalue D 1.13) and this is the preferred method
among some experts. However, an additional analysis using Velicer’s minimum average partial (MAP) test confirms a four-factor solution and thus a four-factor solution
was retained. Bold values represent the items that assess the specific factor.

Discussion using an MTMM approach with informant ratings. As such,


Study 2 represented the first test (to our knowledge) of the
In Study 1, factor analyses revealed separate factors for the up-
validity of informant reports of emotion regulation. Given
regulation of positive emotion, the down-regulation of two neg-
recent evidence suggesting that use of emotional suppression
ative emotions, and the down-regulation of positive emotions
can be detected with some accuracy by informants (Impett
as its own distinct factor. Although neuroticism was strongly
et al., 2012), we expected to find high self–informant agree-
correlated with D-DES and D-ANG, it was weakly correlated
ment. By combining self- and informant ratings, we could
with D-POS and U-POS, demonstrating initial evidence of dis-
examine whether the factor structure would hold even when
criminant validity of scores on the r–RESE from neuroticism.
controlling for shared method variance (Campbell & Fiske,
This finding is consistent with Larsen and Ketelaar (1991), who
1959).
found that neurotic individuals are more susceptible to negative
emotions, but not to positive emotions.
We did not retain any items assessing the up-regulation of Method
negative emotions. Thus, the final r–RESE consisted of four
Participants
components: the up-regulation of positive emotions, the down-
Two hundred and twenty-five undergraduates (75.1% female)
regulation of positive emotions, the down-regulation of
were recruited from the University of Toronto introductory
despondency or distress, and the down-regulation of anger.
psychology pool. The average age was 18.89 (SD D 2.79, range
Given that up-regulation of negative emotion is rare, people
D 17–38). Each participant provided three informant reports.
might not hold firm beliefs about this skill. Future research
One hundred and ninety-one participants (84.9%) provided all
might reveal such a factor in certain subgroups (e.g., doctors,
three informant reports from individuals who have known the
social workers) who might be highly practiced at amplifying
target for over 2 years. Two participants returned two question-
negative emotional states (e.g., vicarious pain) to enhance
naires and one participant returned one questionnaire. The
empathic responding.
total analytic sample for Study 2 consisted of 803 reports (225
We were not able to assess gender differences in r–RESE in
self-reports, 578 informant reports).
this study as we did not examine measurement invariance
across genders. Previous studies have found some gender dif-
Materials and procedure
ferences in emotion regulation (e.g., McRae, Ochsner, Mauss,
Participants completed a booklet of questionnaires, which
Gabrieli, & Gross, 2008), but none have looked at potential gen-
included the r–RESE scale (U-POS, a D .69; D-POS, a D .81;
der differences in the down-regulation of positive emotions.
D-DES, a D .84; D-ANG, a D .81), and the neuroticism scale
This would be an interesting avenue for future research to
from the BFI (a D .82). Next, they were asked to use the enve-
explore.
lopes provided to distribute the questionnaires to three differ-
ent informants who have known them for more than 2 years.
They were asked to be absent when the informants were com-
Study 2
pleting the questionnaires. Informants were instructed to seal
The principal aim of Study 2 was to confirm the factor structure their responses in the envelope, sign the seal, and return it to
of Study 1 by conducting a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) the participant to submit to the lab. The informant
DIFFERENCES IN DOWN-REGULATION OF POSITIVE EMOTIONS 5

Figure 1. Confirmatory factor analysis of revised Regulatory Emotional Self-Efficacy Scale (r–RESE) for Study 2. Note. Numbers are standardized. DDES D down-regulation
of despondency or distress; DANG D down-regulation of anger; DPOS D down-regulation of positive emotions; UPOS D up-regulation of positive emotions; S D self-
report; I D informant report. All the same methods were allowed to correlate with one another (i.e., self-reports with self-reports, Informant 1 reports with Informant 1
reports, Informant 2 reports with Informant 2 reports, Informant 3 reports with Informant 3 reports), which are not shown in the figure.

questionnaires included the r–RESE scale and the neuroticism and Akaike’s information criterion (AIC), with lower BIC and
scale from the BFI. Each question was rephrased so that it AIC values indicating better fit. Specifically, an AIC and BIC
referred to the target participant. When participants returned difference greater than 10 provides strong evidence to support
all responses to the lab, they were debriefed and compensated the model with lower value (Burnham & Anderson, 2002; Raf-
with partial course credit. tery, 1995).
We constructed the base CU model based on our findings to
Analytic approach. We examined the self-informant data in confirm the factor structure observed in the EFA in Study 1
two ways: the traditional MTMM approach (Campbell & Fiske, (Figure 1). Each r–RESE factor (D-POS, U-POS, D-DES, D-
1959), and CFA using structural equation modeling (SEM). To ANG) was extracted from its respective three items. We also
construct our MTMM matrix, we correlated mean composite allowed all factors of the r–RESE to correlate with one another
scores from each of the r–RESE factors from the self- and infor- as Study 1 demonstrated that they were not strictly indepen-
mant reports. To model the MTMM data using SEM, we mod- dent. Because we arbitrarily assigned informants as Informant
eled the correlated uniqueness (CU) model1 (Figure 1; Lance, 1, Informant 2, and Informant 3, informants were assumed to
Noble, & Scullen, 2002). The CU model is one way of modeling be equally valid by restricting the residual variances of all infor-
self-informant data that allows measures from the same mant ratings to be equal. We allowed all the same methods
method to correlate with one another (e.g., self-report with (i.e., self-reports with self-reports, Informant 1 reports with
self-report, informant report with informant report) to account Informant 1 reports, Informant 2 reports with Informant 2
for shared method variance. However, one limitation of the CU reports, Informant 3 reports with Informant 3 reports) to cor-
model is that it assumes that the independent methods are not relate with one another to account for shared method variance
correlated (e.g., self-report and informant report; Conway, (e.g., self-report of DERS was allowed to correlate with self-
Lievens, Scullen, & Lance, 2004). However, given that we report of POS).
obtained one self-report and three informant reports from We then compared the base model with alternative mod-
three independent informants, the CU model is appropriate for els that were similarly tested in Caprara and colleagues’
our data. We conducted the SEM analyses using Mplus 5.0 soft- (2008) study. First we tested an alternative model with an
ware (Muthen & Muthen, 2007). The remaining analyses were overarching general RESE factor extracted from the four
conducted on SPSS version 23. Because Mplus can account for factors, with the four factors becoming secondary factors.
missing data using a full information ML estimation, we Second, we tested for the potential existence of second-
included all participants in the analysis. Model fit was assessed order factors. As positive emotion regulation and negative
using a standard criteria of comparative fit index (CFI) > .95, emotion regulation have been shown to be distinct from
root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) < .06, and one another (e.g., Caprara et al., 2008), it might be possible
standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) < .08 (Hu & to observe a secondary factor that distinguishes between the
Bentler, 1999). Because chi-square difference tests are suscepti- regulation of positive emotions versus negative emotions.
ble to large sample sizes (Raftery, 1995), model comparisons We initially examined a model with a secondary factor
were assessed using the Bayesian information criterion (BIC) comprised of just the regulation of negative emotions,
which was found in Caprara and colleagues’ (2008) CFA
study of the original RESE. We then assessed a model with
1
We initially fit a correlated trait–correlated method (CTCM) model but the model
did not converge. Thus, as suggested by Lance et al. (2002), once the CTCM two secondary factors of managing positive emotions and
model failed, the CU model was chosen. managing negative emotions.
6 ZOU, PLAKS, PETERSON

Table 2. Multitrait–multimethod matrix of Regulatory Emotional Self-Efficacy Scale and neuroticism.

Self-report Informant report

U-POS D-POS D-DES D-ANG N iU-POS iD-POS iD-DES iD-ANG iN

Self
U-POS
D-POS .12
D-DES .16 .17
D-ANG .23 .18 .49
N ¡.06 ¡.06 ¡.60 ¡.45
Informant
iU-POS .30 ¡.02 .20 .23 ¡.20
iD-POS .07 .29 .17 .17 ¡.05 .27
iD-DES .13 .05 .34 .27 ¡.39 .58 .45
iD-ANG .11 .10 .21 .36 ¡.34 .59 .46 .76
iN ¡.10 ¡.04 ¡.37 ¡.33 .55 ¡.42 ¡.32 ¡.67 ¡.63
M 4.08 4.18 4.28 4.43 3.12 4.16 4.14 4.50 4.55 2.80
SD 1.29 1.41 1.37 1.39 0.77 0.88 0.80 0.99 0.99 0.61

Note. r > .16 is significant at p < .05; r > .13 is significant at p < .1. Convergent validity coefficients are shown in bold. U-POS D up-regulation of positive emotions; D-
POS D down-regulation of positive emotions; D-DES D down-regulation of despondency or distress; D-ANG D down-regulation of anger; N D neuroticism; i D infor-
mant report.

Results there were also instances of overlap between particular r–RESE


factors. Additionally, informant reports of r–RESE had higher
Descriptive statistics
loadings than self-reports of r–RESE. This was expected because
For the descriptive statistics (Table 2), we only included partici-
the design included only one self-report compared to three
pants who returned all three questionnaires. To increase the
informant reports. Indeed, constraining the residual variances of
reliability and validity of the informant report data, we created
self-reports and informant reports to be equally valid worsened
a composite informant score by averaging across informant
the model fit, x2(100) D 167.67, CFI D 0.959, RMSEA D 0.055,
reports (Lucas, Diener, & Suh, 1996; Sandvik, Diener, & Seid-
SRMR D 0.103, AIC D 8708.96, BIC D 8886.37).2
litz, 1993; interrater reliability (k): U-POS D .54, D-POS D .39,
We then tested an alternative model with one general RESE
D-DES D .68, D-ANG D .50). The interrater reliability of
factor represented by the shared variance across all four r–
scores on the r–RESE was within the range of those found in
RESE factors as secondary factors. This worsened the model fit
well-being and affect measures (Zou et al., 2013). Full descrip-
when compared with the base model, x2(df D 92) D 145.81,
tive statistics are presented in Table 2.
CFI D 0.967, RMSEA D 0.051, SRMR D 0.076, AIC D 8703.11,
BIC D 8907.80. This suggests that the four factors of RESE do
Multitrait–multimethod matrix. Table 2 presents all correla- not share a common factor. We then fit a secondary factor
tions between the factors of r–RESE and neuroticism based on comprised of D-DES and D-ANG to replicate Caprara and col-
self- and informant reports. Correlations between the same leagues’ (2008) CFA model. This also worsened model fit, x2(df
construct assessed using different methods (i.e., convergent D 92) D 150.97, CFI D 0.964, RMSEA D 0.053, SRMR D
validity coefficients) are shown in bold. All convergent validity 0.085, AIC D 8708.26, BIC D 8912.96. We then fit a model
coefficients were greater than .29. Additionally, there was only with two oblique secondary factors with one consisting of the
a single instance where the self-informant convergent coeffi- positive emotions (D-POS, U-POS), and the other the negative
cient was weaker than any associated self-informant correlation emotions (D-DES, D-ANG). This model also displayed poorer
in the corresponding row and columns, which provides evi- fit, x2(df D 91) D 143.50, CFI D 0.968, RMSEA D 0.051, SRMR
dence for discriminant validity (Campbell & Fiske, 1959). The D 0.086, AIC D 8702.79, BIC D 8910.91. Taken together, the
correlation between D-DES self-ratings and D-DES informant four-factor model represented the best fit for the data.
ratings was .34, the correlation of D-DES self-ratings with neu-
roticism informant ratings was –.37, and the correlation of D-
DES informant ratings with neuroticism self-ratings was –.39. Discussion
Replicating Study 1, D-DES appears to overlap with neuroti- Using an MTMM approach provided evidence that there are
cism. Next, to provide further support for the findings pre- four related, but distinguishable components of the r–RESE.
sented in the MTMM matrix (Kenny & Kashy, 1992), we Replicating findings from Study 1, we found that neuroticism
conducted a CFA based on the MTMM matrix. was associated with the regulation of negative, but not positive,
emotions.
Confirmatory factor analysis. This base model (Figure 1) had These data provide the first evidence for the validity of
acceptable fit to the data, x2(90) D 120.29, CFI D 0.982, RMSEA informant reports of emotion regulation. There was moder-
D 0.039, SRMR D 0.066, AIC D 8681.58, BIC D 8893.10), and ate self-informant agreement in the r–RESE, which is
served as the base model to which we compared alternative
models. As depicted in Figure 1, all four factors of r–RESE 2
Whereas the BIC value was slightly lower than 10, model fit worsened on all
showed evidence of independence from one another, although other fit indexes.
DIFFERENCES IN DOWN-REGULATION OF POSITIVE EMOTIONS 7

consistent with findings from informant reports of other existing evidence that positive emotions play a specific role in
stable individual difference characteristics such as personal- impulsivity (Furnham & Thompson, 1991; Revelle et al., 1980).
ity (e.g., Vazire, 2006) and well-being (Zou et al., 2013).
Thus, these data suggest that future researchers might use
informant reports as an additional tool—beyond self- Participants
report—for assessing emotion regulation.
Four hundred and eighty-three participants (54.2% female)
Although the informant reports did converge with the
were recruited from Amazon’s MT. The average age was 36.67
self-reports, the MTMM matrix suggests that informants
(SD D 12.41, range D 19–77).
are less able to discriminate the r–RESE factors than partici-
pants themselves. For example, examining the correlations
in the informant monomethod triangle in Table 2, the cor- Materials and procedure
relation between D-POS and neuroticism was –.32, which is
markedly higher than the –.06 correlation between self- After completing the consent form, participants completed the
reported D-POS and self-reported neuroticism. In fact, all questionnaires listed next. Afterward, they were thanked and
correlations in the informant monomethod triangle were debriefed.
higher than the corresponding correlations in the self-report
monomethod triangle. A similar pattern has been observed r–RESE
in previous work examining the discriminant validity of The r–RESE was distributed to the participants. Scores on each
well-being measures using self- and informant reports dimension once again showed good reliabilities (U-POS,
(Lucas et al., 1996). This suggests a potential limitation a D .77; D-POS, a D .79; D-DES, a D .82; D-ANG, a D .84).
with using informant reports to assess internal feelings.
Based on this finding, researchers should be wary of using
only informant reports and should collect self-reports in Attachment style
conjunction with informant reports to increase validity. The Revised Adult Attachment Scale (RAAS) for close relation-
Although in Study 2 all four factors were correlated with ships (Collins, 1996) assesses people’s general attachment style
one another, the correlations observed in Study 2 were gen- with close others. The anxious dimension examines the extent
erally lower than in Study 1. One potential explanation for to which people worry about their close relationships (a D .93).
this might be that Study 1’s MTurk sample was evenly split The avoidant dimension assesses the extent to which people
between genders and possessed greater variability in age. In attempt to avoid becoming attached altogether (a D .91).
contrast, the undergraduate sample in Study 2 was dispro-
portionately female and highly restricted in age range. Self-esteem
Given the gender and age differences found in previous We assessed self-esteem using the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale
studies on emotion regulation (e.g., Urry & Gross, 2010), (Rosenberg, 1979), which is a 10-item scale that measures
these demographic differences could account for the differ- global self-worth (a D .93).
ences between the two samples.
These data did not reveal the factor comprised of DES and
Resilience
ANG reported by Caprara and colleagues (2008). However,
The Resilience Scale for Adults (RSA; Friborg, Hjemdal, Rose-
Caprara et al. (2008) found that the three-factor solution
nvinge, & Martinussen, 2003) is a 45-item measure of resilience
(where DES, ANG, and POS were modeled as three separate
with five dimensions: personal competence, social competence,
factors) fit equally as well as the two-factor solution (i.e., POS
family coherence, social support, and personal structure. Scores
and NEG) with a second-order negative factor (i.e., NEG !
on this scale demonstrated good reliability (a D .96).
DES & ANG). Although negative emotions are often combined
(e.g., Watson & Tellegen, 1985), there is growing behavioral
and neurophysiological evidence that anger is unique among Health
negative emotions in its association with approach motivation The SMU Health Questionnaire (Watson, 1988; Watson &
(Carver & Harmon-Jones, 2009). Future researchers might ben- Pennebaker, 1989) is a 63-item measure that assesses a broad
efit from distinguishing between the regulation of anger versus range of health problems including minor complaints (e.g.,
despondency. cold or flu) and major health complications (e.g., cancer) expe-
rienced in the past year. Higher scores indicate worse health.
The scale includes six items that assess the number of days
Study 3
spent in the hospital in the past year for the following prob-
Caprara and colleagues (2008) reported that all three factors of lems: surgery, childbirth, psychological problems, injury, ill-
the original RESE scale positively predicted indicators of adjust- ness, and drug or alcohol problems. For the purposes of this
ment (e.g., self-esteem, prosocial behavior) and negatively pre- study, our main interest was in the number of days hospitalized
dicted indicators of maladjustment (e.g., depression, due to injury and drug or alcohol problems, as these were taken
aggression). In Study 3, we examined whether beliefs regarding as proxy indicators of engagement in risky behaviors. We
the down-regulation of positive emotions would provide addi- hypothesized that there would be a negative association
tional predictive power to predict adjustment and impulsivity. between the ability to down-regulate positive emotions and
We focused specifically on measures of risky behaviors given risky behaviors.
8 ZOU, PLAKS, PETERSON

Table 3. Correlation matrix of Regulatory Emotional Self-Efficacy Scale (RESE) and life outcomes.

RESE Adult attachment Health

D-POS U-POS D-ANG D-DES AX AV RES HOSP–D/A HOSP–INJ

D-POS
U-POS .19
D-ANG .36 .28
D-DES .31 .40 .65
SE .14 .18 .30 .42
AA–AX ¡.16 ¡.08 ¡.27 ¡.42
AA–AV ¡.05 ¡.24 ¡.31 ¡.40 .62
RES .15 .34 .38 .56 ¡.52 ¡.65
HOSP–D/A ¡.04 .03 .03 .02 .04 .04 ¡.02
HOSP–INJ ¡.15 .02 ¡.05 ¡.07 .05 .01 .00 .03
M 4.99 4.21 4.93 4.57 2.61 2.79 3.70 0.09 0.15
SD 1.20 1.24 1.31 1.29 1.12 0.84 0.67 0.87 0.92

Note. N D 483. All correlations above .14 are significant at p < .01. D-POS D down-regulation of positive emotions; U-POS D up-regulation of positive emotions; D-ANG D
down-regulation of anger; D-DES D down-regulation of despondency or distress; AX D anxiety; AV D avoidance; RES D resilience; HOSP D number of days hospitalized;
D/A D drug or alcohol problems; INJ D injury; SE D self-esteem.

Analytic approach injury. None of the r–RESE factors was significantly correlated
We first examined the associations between the r–RESE fac- with hospitalization due to drug or alcohol problems.
tors and outcome scores with a series of correlations. We When we entered all four factors of r–RESE simultaneously
then examined whether D-POS uniquely predicted any of into a regression analysis, the D-POS factor did not uniquely
the outcome variables by modeling a series of regression predict most of the outcomes,3 with the notable exception of
analyses with the four r–RESE factors as simultaneous pre- the hospitalization measures. In the first model, we used the
dictors to examine how they each independently predict the number of hospitalizations due to injury as the outcome vari-
different outcomes. able. We conducted a negative binomial regression to take into
account that the outcome measure (i.e., number of days of hos-
pitalization) is a count variable. Higher D-POS significantly
Results
predicted fewer days of hospitalizations due to injury (inci-
Table 3 provides correlations among the four mean com- dence rate ratio [IRR] D .47, 95% CI [.36, .60], p < .001). More-
posite scores of r–RESE factors and all outcomes. D-ANG over, higher U-POS predicted more hospitalization days due to
and D-DES were consistently associated with all the out- injury (IRR D 1.53, 95% CI [1.15, 2.02], p D .003). We also rep-
comes except the number of days hospitalized due to injury licated the same pattern with the number of hospitalizations
and drug or alcohol problems. Better up-regulation of posi- due to drugs or alcohol problems (D-POS: IRR D .58, 95% CI
tive emotions was related to higher self-esteem, lower avoid- [.42, .78], p < .001; U-POS D 1.40, 95% CI [1.01, 1.95], p D
ance, and higher resilience. Better down-regulation of .04).4 Both of these results held when controlling for age and
positive emotions was related to self-esteem and resilience gender.
and lower levels of relationship anxiety. In sum, these results suggest that the ability to curb one’s
Because the D-POS was a novel factor in the r–RESE, we exuberance uniquely predicts a lower likelihood of ending up
compared the correlations between D-POS and the four out- in the hospital due to injury, substance abuse, or both. More-
come variables (avoidance, anxiety, self-esteem, resilience) with over, the reverse appeared to be true: The tendency to amplify
the correlations among the other three factors (U-POS, D-DES, positive emotion predicted higher occurrence of the same nega-
D-ANG) and the outcome variables. To account for multiple tive outcomes.
comparisons, each p value was adjusted using Bonferroni correc-
tion. Comparing D-POS to U-POS, there was a significant differ- Discussion
ence in the correlations for avoidance dimension of attachment
(z D 3.02, p D .003) and resilience (z D 3.14, p D .002). Com- These data provide evidence that the down-regulation of posi-
paring D-POS with D-ANG, differences in the correlations were tive emotions and the up-regulation of positive emotions might
once again observed in the avoidance dimension of attachment be related to distinct outcomes. These data lend further support
(z D 4.19, p < .001) and resilience (z D 3.86, p < .001). In com- for the convergent validity of the D-POS factor. Specifically, D-
parisons between D-POS and D-DES, differences were observed POS was related to lower anxiety scores on the attachment
for all outcome measures: self-esteem (z D 4.75, p < .001), the measure, greater resilience, and fewer days of hospitalization
anxiety dimension of attachment (z D 4.44, p < .001), the avoid- due to injury. The correlation table also illustrates that the D-
ance dimension of attachment (z D 5.79, p < .001), and resil- POS factor behaves slightly differently from the other three
ience ( D 7.46, p < .001). The differences in patterns of
correlations between D-POS and the other factors of r–RESE 3
D-POS was uniquely predictive of the avoidance subscale of the attachment
provide further evidence that the D-POS is somewhat unique. scale, but in the opposite direction from what we expected, where higher D-POS
was significantly related to greater avoidance, b D .08, t(436) D 2.35, p D .02.
Focusing on risky behavior outcomes, as predicted, higher 4
D-ANG also uniquely predicted the number of days in the opposite direction (IRR
D-POS was related to fewer days of hospitalization due to D 1.51, 95% CI [1.02, 2.22], p D .04).
DIFFERENCES IN DOWN-REGULATION OF POSITIVE EMOTIONS 9

components of r–RESE. This provides further support for the differently when predicting life outcomes; down-regulation of
discriminant validity of the D-POS construct.5 Furthermore, positive emotions uniquely predicts proxies for risky behaviors
D-POS was uniquely predictive of hospitalizations due to injury (Study 3). Such data suggest that a comprehensive model of
and substance abuse when simultaneously entered into a emotion regulation ability should include the down-regulation
regression equation with the other components of r–RESE. of positive emotions.
This represents evidence of incremental convergent validity
above and beyond the other r–RESE factors.
We acknowledge that the cross-sectional, correlational
Limitations and future directions
design does not permit a conclusive determination of the direc-
tion of causality. Although it is plausible that the inability to Future studies should examine how the regulations of specific
down-regulate positive emotions causes people to engage in emotions are related to one another. For instance, it is unclear
behaviors that result in hospitalization, it is also possible that whether the down-regulation of anger is related to the down-
engaging in these behaviors worsens people’s ability to down- regulation of sadness. It would also be useful to distinguish
regulate positive emotions. For instance, recent alcohol con- between the valence of emotions and the arousal of emotions to
sumption is common in serious offenses (Elliott, Huizinga, & observe whether the regulation of emotional valence might dif-
Menard, 1989). Future researchers could use manipulations or fer from the regulation of emotional arousal (Posner et al.,
longitudinal designs to help to disentangle questions of causal 2005).
direction. We did not find evidence for another potential compo-
The finding that the ability to down-regulate positive emo- nent of emotion regulation—the up-regulation of negative
tions independently predicts days of hospitalization due to emotions (U-NEG; Ochsner et al., 2004). For instance, a
injury and alcohol or drug use has direct clinical implications recent study suggests that faking anger in different ways
for the treatment of alcoholism and drug addiction. It might can lead to more favorable or unfavorable negotiation out-
also shed light on what drives delinquent behavior more gener- comes (C^ote, Hideg, & van Kleef, 2013). Although there
ally. Given the link between extraversion and positive emotions might be some circumstances in which the up-regulation of
(Watson & Clark, 1997), these data suggest that the link negative emotions is appropriate (e.g., a funeral), we suggest
between extraversion and delinquency is attributable in part to that these situations are generally rare. Our data indicate
individuals’ inability to down-regulate positive emotions rather that people do not hold firm beliefs about their ability to
than negative emotions. We acknowledge, however, that the up-regulate negative emotions.
dependent measures in this study were indirect indicators of Another possibility, however, is that that we did not
delinquency. Future studies should replicate our findings with phrase the questions properly to effectively distinguish emo-
more direct measures of delinquency. tional self-efficacy beliefs from actual emotion regulation
Another limitation is that it is unclear whether the self-effi- ability. This possibility is muted somewhat by the informant
cacy aspect of r–RESE or actual emotion regulation ability reports in Study 2; informants were not asked about the
drove the observed effects. In other words, does simply holding individual’s beliefs about his or her emotion regulation abil-
the belief that one is good at regulating emotions lead to these ities (i.e., “To what extent does the individual see him/her-
positive outcomes, or are these beliefs an indicator of one’s self as being good at reducing their upset?”), but rather the
actual emotion regulation ability? Future studies would benefit individual’s objective emotion regulation abilities (i.e., “To
from the inclusion of another indicator of emotion regulation what extent is the individual good at reducing their
abilities (e.g., informant reports) that might disentangle subjec- upset?”). Nevertheless, future researchers might consider
tive beliefs from actual emotion regulation ability. teasing apart the two constructs more explicitly.

General discussion
Conclusion
Although there is strong evidence demonstrating the positive
impact of successful emotion regulation (e.g., DeSteno, Gross, Using an MTMM approach, we present data supporting the use
& Kubzansky, 2013; Gross, 2013), prior measures of individual of a new tool for measuring trait-level ability to down-regulate
differences in self-reported emotion regulation ability have gen- negative emotions and both down-regulate and up-regulate
erally failed to assess regulation of both negative and positive positive emotions. Using this tool, these data suggest that future
emotions. One questionnaire that does address both forms of researchers would do well to consider the down-regulation of
regulation is the RESE scale. This study made improvements to positive affect as an independent psychological experience with
the RESE scale and more thoroughly examined the validity of meaningful behavioral consequences.
scores on the revised scale using self- and informant reports.
An EFA (Study 1) and a CFA using MTMM data (Study 2)
both suggested that there are four distinct but related subscales Acknowledgments
in the r–RESE. Each component of the r–RESE behaves
We would like to thank and acknowledge the members of the Plaks lab for
their assistance in the initial development of the scale and Quyet Dinh for
5
We could not conduct a formal test to compare the strength of all the correla- organizing the data. We would also like to thank the members of
tions as there would be too many tests to be conducted (4 RESE components Dr. Impett's Relationship and Well-Being Lab for their helpful comments
and 6 outcome measures D 45 comparisons). on the earlier version of the manuscript.
10 ZOU, PLAKS, PETERSON

Funding Fabrigar, L. R., Wegener, D. T., MacCallum, R. C., & Strahan, E. J. (1999).
Evaluating the use of exploratory factor analysis in psychological
This research project was supported by a standard research grant awarded research. Psychological Methods, 4, 272–299.
to Jason Plaks and Jordan B. Peterson by the Canadian Social Sciences and Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). What good are positive emotions? Review of
Humanities Research Council. General Psychology, 2, 300–319.
Friborg, O., Hjemdal, O., Rosenvinge, J. H., & Martinussen, M. (2003). A
new rating scale for adult resilience: What are the central protective
References resources behind healthy adjustment? International Journal of Methods
in Psychiatric Research, 12(2), 65–76.
Algom, D., Chajut, E., & Lev, S. (2004). A rational look at the emotional Furnham, A., & Thompson, J. (1991). Personality and self-reported delin-
Stroop phenomenon: A generic slowdown, not a Stroop effect. Journal quency. Personality and Individual Differences, 12, 585–593.
of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 323–338. Gratz, K. L., & Roemer, L. (2004). Multidimensional assessment of emo-
Bandura, A., Caprara, G. V., Barbaranelli, C., Gerbino, M., & Pastor- tion regulation and dysregulation: Development, factor structure, and
elli, C. (2003). Role of affective self-regulatory efficacy in diverse initial validation of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale. Journal
spheres of psychosocial functioning. Child Development, 74, 769– of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 26, 41–54.
782. Gross, J. J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative
Burisch, M. (1984). Approaches to personality inventory construction: A review. Review of General Psychology, 2, 271–299.
comparison of merits. American Psychologist, 39, 214–227. Gross, J. J. (2013). Emotion regulation: Taking stock and moving forward.
Burnham, K. P., & Anderson, D. R. (2002). Model selection and multimodel Emotion, 13, 359–365.
inference: A practical information-theoretic approach. New York, NY: Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion reg-
Springer Science & Business Media. ulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being.
Campbell, D. T., & Fiske, D. W. (1959). Convergent and discriminant vali- Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 348–362.
dation by the multitrait–multimethod matrix. Psychological Bulletin, 56 Hare, T. A., Tottenham, N., Galvan, A., Voss, H. U., Glover, G. H., &
(2), 81–105. Casey, B. J. (2008). Biological substrates of emotional reactivity and
Caprara, G. V., Di Giunta, L., Eisenberg, N., Gerbino, M., Pastorelli, C., & regulation in adolescence during an emotional go-nogo task. Biological
Tramontano, C. (2008). Assessing regulatory emotional self-efficacy in Psychiatry, 63, 927–934.
three countries. Psychological Assessment, 20, 227–237. Horn, J. L. (1965). A rationale and test for the number of factors in factor
Caprara, G. V., & Gerbino, M. (2001). Autoefficacia Emotiva Percepita: La analysis. Psychometrika, 30, 179–185.
capacita di regolare l’affettivita negativa e di esprimere quella positiva Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance
[Affective perceived self-efficacy: The capacity to regulate negative structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struc-
affect and to express positive affect]. In G. V. Caprara (Ed.), La valuta- tural Equation Modeling, 6, 1–55.
zione dell’autoefficacia [Self-efficacy assessment] (pp. 35–50). Trento, Impett, E. A., Kogan, A., English, T., John, O., Oveis, C., Gordon, A. M., &
Italy: Edizioni Erickson. Keltner, D. (2012). Suppression sours sacrifice: Emotional and rela-
Carver, C. S., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2009). Anger is an approach-related tional costs of suppressing emotions in romantic relationships. Person-
affect: Evidence and implications. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 183–204. ality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38, 707–720.
Cattell, R. B. (1966). The scree test for the number of factors. Multivariate John, O. P., & Gross, J. J. (2004). Healthy and unhealthy emotion regula-
Behavioral Research, 1, 245–276. tion: Personality processes, individual differences, and life span devel-
Chandler, J., Mueller, P., & Paolacci, G. (2014). Nonna€ıvete among Ama- opment. Journal of Personality, 72, 1301–1334.
zon Mechanical Turk workers: Consequences and solutions for behav- John, O. P., & Srivastava, S. (1999). The Big Five trait taxonomy: History,
ioral researchers. Behavior Research Methods, 46, 112–130. measurement, and theoretical perspectives. Handbook of Personality:
Collins, N. L. (1996). Working models of attachment: Implications for Theory and Research, 2, 102–138.
explanation, emotion, and behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Kenny, D. A., & Kashy, D. A. (1992). Analysis of the multitrait–multime-
Psychology, 71, 810–832. thod matrix by confirmatory factor analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 112,
Connelly, B. S., & Ones, D. S. (2010). An other perspective on personality: 165–172.
Meta-analytic integration of observers’ accuracy and predictive validity. Kim, S., & Hamann, S. B. (2007). Neural correlates of positive and negative
Psychological Bulletin, 136, 1092–1122. emotion regulation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 776–798.
Conway, J. M., Lievens, F., Scullen, S. E., & Lance, C. E. (2004). Bias in the Lance, C. E., Noble, C. L., & Scullen, S. E. (2002). A critique of the corre-
correlated uniqueness model for MTMM data. Structural Equation lated trait–correlated method and correlated uniqueness models for
Modeling, 11, 535–559. multitrait–multimethod data. Psychological Methods, 7, 228–244.
Cooper, M. L., Agocha, V. B., & Sheldon, M. S. (2000). A motivational per- Larsen, R. J., & Ketelaar, T. (1991). Personality and susceptibility to posi-
spective on risky behaviors: The role of personality and affect regula- tive and negative emotional states. Journal of Personality and Social
tory processes. Journal of Personality, 68, 1059–1088. Psychology, 61, 132–140.
Costello, A. B., & Osborne, J. W. (2005). Best practices in exploratory fac- Lucas, R. E., Diener, E., & Suh, E. (1996). Discriminant validity of well-
tor analysis: Four recommendations for getting the most from your being measures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 616–
analysis. Practical Assessment, Research, & Evaluation, 10, 1–9. 628.
C^ote, S., Hideg, I., & van Kleef, G. A. (2013). The consequences of faking McRae, K., Ochsner, K. N., Mauss, I. B., Gabrieli, J. J., & Gross, J. J. (2008).
anger in negotiations. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49, Gender differences in emotion regulation: An fMRI study of cognitive
453–463. reappraisal. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 11, 143–162.
Crump, M. J., McDonnell, J. V., & Gureckis, T. M. (2013). Evaluating Muthen, L. K., & Muthen, B. O. (2007). Mplus user’s guide (5th ed.). Los
Amazon’s Mechanical Turk as a tool for experimental behavioral Angeles, CA: Muthen & Muthen.
research. PloS ONE, 8(3), e57410. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0057410 Ochsner, K. N., Ray, R. D., Cooper, J. C., Robertson, E. R., Chopra, S.,
DeSteno, D., Gross, J. J., & Kubzansky, L. (2013). Affective science and Gabrieli, J. D., & Gross, J. J. (2004). For better or for worse: Neural sys-
health: The importance of emotion and emotion regulation. Health tems supporting the cognitive down- and up-regulation of negative
Psychology, 32, 474–486. emotion. Neuroimage, 23, 483–499.
Elliott, D. S., Huizinga, D., & Menard, S. (1989). Multiple problem youth: O’Connor, B. P. (2000). SPSS and SAS programs for determining the
Delinquency, substance use, and mental health problems. New York, number of components using parallel analysis and Velicer’s MAP
NY: Springer-Verlag. test. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 32,
Exline, J. J., & Lobel, M. (1999). The perils of outperformance: Sensitivity 396–402.
about being the target of a threatening upward comparison. Psychologi- Posner, J., Russell, J. A., & Peterson, B. S. (2005). The circumplex model of
cal Bulletin, 125, 307–337. affect: An integrative approach to affective neuroscience, cognitive
DIFFERENCES IN DOWN-REGULATION OF POSITIVE EMOTIONS 11

development, and psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, Urry, H. L., & Gross, J. J. (2010). Emotion regulation in older age. Current
17, 715–734. Directions in Psychological Science, 19, 352–357.
Raftery, A. E. (1995). Bayesian model selection in social research. Sociologi- Vazire, S. (2006). Informant reports: A cheap, fast, and easy method
cal Methodology, 25, 111–164. for personality assessment. Journal of Research in Personality, 40,
Revelle, W., Humphreys, M. S., Simon, L., & Gilliland, K. (1980). The 472–481.
interactive effect of personality, time of day, and caffeine: A test of Vohs, K. D., & Heatherton, T. F. (2000). Self-regulatory failure: A resource-
the arousal model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, depletion approach. Psychological Science, 11, 249–254.
109(1), 1–31. Watson, D. (1988). Intraindividual and interindividual analyses of positive
Rosenberg, M. (1979). Conceiving the self. New York, NY: Basic Books. and negative affect: Their relation to health complaints, perceived
Sandvik, E., Diener, E., & Seidlitz, L. (1993). Subjective well-being: The stress, and daily activities. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
convergence and stability of self-report and non-self-report measures. 54, 1020–1030.
Journal of Personality, 61, 317–342. Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1997). Extraversion and its positive emotional
Schall, M., Martiny, S. E., Goetz, T., & Hall, N. C. (2016). Smiling on the core. In R. Hogan, J. A. Johnson, & S. R. Briggs (Eds.), Handbook of
inside: The social benefits of suppressing positive emotions in outperform- personality psychology (pp. 767–793). San Diego, CA: Academic.
ance situations. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 42, 559–571. Watson, D., & Pennebaker, J. W. (1989). Health complaints, stress, and
Shields, A., & Cicchetti, D. (1997). Emotion regulation among school-age distress: Exploring the central role of negative affectivity. Psychological
children: The development and validation of a new criterion Q-sort Review, 96, 234–254.
scale. Developmental Psychology, 33, 906–916. Watson, D., & Tellegen, A. (1985). Toward a consensual structure of
Tamir, M., John, O. P., Srivastava, S., & Gross, J. J. (2007). Implicit theories mood. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 219–235.
of emotion: Affective and social outcomes across a major life transition. Williams, J. M. G., Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1996). The emotional
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 731–744. Stroop task and psychopathology. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 3–24.
Tugade, M. M., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2007). Regulation of positive emo- Zou, C., Schimmack, U., & Gere, J. (2013). The validity of well-being meas-
tions: Emotion regulation strategies that promote resilience. Journal of ures: A multiple-indicator–multiple-rater model. Psychological Assess-
Happiness Studies, 8, 311–333. ment, 25, 1247–1254.

View publication stats

You might also like