The Process and Mechanisms of Personality Change

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 14

nature reviews psychology https://doi.org/10.

1038/s44159-024-00295-z

Review article Check for updates

The process and mechanisms


of personality change
Joshua J. Jackson 1
& Amanda J. Wright 2

Abstract Sections

Although personality is relatively stable across the lifespan, there is Introduction

also ample evidence that it is malleable. This potential for change Mechanisms of personality
is important because many individuals want to change aspects of change

their personality and because personality influences important life Processes that prevent change
outcomes. In this Review, we examine the mechanisms responsible Evidence for change
for intentional and naturally occurring changes in personality. We mechanisms

discuss four mechanisms — preconditions, triggers, reinforcers and Summary and future
directions
integrators — that are theorized to produce effective change, as well
as the forces that promote stability, thereby thwarting enduring
changes. Although these mechanisms are common across theories
of personality development, the empirical evidence is mixed and
inconclusive. Personality change is most likely to occur gradually over
long timescales but abrupt, transformative changes are possible when
change is deliberately attempted or as a result of biologically mediated
mechanisms. When change does occur, it is often modest in scale.
Ultimately, it is difficult to cultivate a completely different personality,
but small changes are possible.

Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO, USA.
1

Department of Psychology, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland.


2
e-mail: j.jackson@wustl.edu

Nature Reviews Psychology


Review article

Introduction to occur. Broad latent traits reflect a complex pattern of functioning


Almost everyone wishes to modify their personality in some way1, per- comprised of inherited factors, learned processes, self-perception and
haps to be funnier, more outgoing or less anxious. The ability to change self-evaluation, and environmental influences accrued over decades.
one’s personality could also be exceptionally beneficial because per- Owing to the difficulty in directly targeting latent traits for change,
sonality is related to important life outcomes, such as life expectancy, most clinical models of intentional change instead focus on lower lev-
success at work, social interactions and wealth2,3. els of abstraction17,35,36. Thus, environmentally induced changes at the
Over the past three decades, longitudinal studies have demon- latent trait level are thought to result from bottom-up processes origi-
strated that personality never completely solidifies despite being nating from habits or state-level manifestations of affect, behaviour,
relatively stable over long timescales, and is instead malleable across cognition and desires. By contrast, biological factors, such as psycho-
the lifespan2–5. The ability to change personality, combined with its tropic substances (for example, selective serotonin reuptake inhibi-
long-term predictive validity, have prompted calls to focus research tors), that bypass the other levels might directly impact personality
efforts on what causes changes in personality and how to implement at the latent trait level32,37.
interventions to change personality on purpose6. However, most work Habitual behaviours can be considered the building blocks of
to date has examined the effects of broad macroprocesses responsi- broader personality traits. For example, a conscientious individual
ble for change (such as experiencing major life events7,8), rather than is habitually reliable and organized. Whereas efforts to change broad
testing specific mechanisms in intervention studies9. traits are rarely tested outside the clinical realm, habitual reactions to
In this Review, we examine the process of personality change and stimuli are the target of many interventions25,36,38–41. The key to effective
underlying mechanisms. First, we review mechanisms proposed to habit change is that there must eventually be a shift from goal-directed
underlie personality change that are common among many theoretical behaviour to automatic behaviour through repetition or conditional
frameworks10–21. We take a broad view and include general change mech- learning processes17,35,42,43. That is, behaviour that once required a cue
anisms (which we define as distinct components within the general or trigger should become automatic, effortless and an inherent part
temporal process of personality change that guides and impacts indi- of an individual44,45.
vidual differences14) applicable to a wide range of constructs rather Modifications at the lowest level of analysis are often needed to
than exclusively focusing on any one aspect of this process. Our focus create conditional habits that could ultimately lead to broad change
is primarily on environmental change mechanisms, but genetic and bio- in latent traits11,13,15. State-level manifestations of affect, behaviour,
logical mechanisms are also capable of and potentially responsible for cognition and desires are time-specific or situation-specific building
change22. Next, we discuss mechanisms that promote stability, thereby blocks by which habits and traits are modified. State manifestations
limiting potential change. We then review the empirical evidence for are variable46–48: people often behave ‘out of character’ (for example,
these change processes from studies of life events and interventions, an introverted person might act extraverted on occasion46). Changes
and conclude with unanswered questions for future research. in the latent trait arise from shifts in the typical behaviour one exhibits
The term ‘personality’ encompasses many individual differences (for example, an introvert having fewer daily instances of introverted
such as personality traits, values, life goals, interests, life satisfac- behaviour and instead spending more time acting extraverted). Given
tion and self-esteem23, each of which are relatively enduring over dec- that most people already have routine experiences of acting out of
ades and influence state-level affect, behaviour, cognition and desires24. character, it is probably easier to focus change efforts at the state level
We focus on personality traits but note when other individual differ- as opposed to at the level of habits they do not yet have. Thus, we focus
ences evidence different processes. ‘Change’ refers to changes in the on the mechanisms responsible for causing changes in lower-level
latent personality construct that persist over a moderate timescale and state manifestations that then result in broader changes to personality.
are evident across different situations. Personality is not changed if one Numerous theories of personality development identify potential
develops a novel habit or acts ‘out of character’ in an isolated instance15. mechanisms and processes responsible for change that vary in scope
Change also cannot be due to external forces only; behaviour might and depth10–13,15,35,49 (Table 1). Although many hypothesized mechanisms
initially change for external reasons, but change must eventually exist can contribute to change, few — if any — are able to change personality
independently of any external factors. alone. Thus, a multi-mechanism process is needed to fully account for
the process of personality change. Drawing on a large empirical and
Mechanisms of personality change theoretical literature10,13,14,16,18,20,50–57, we categorized each proposed
Mechanisms of personality change can target latent traits (for exam- mechanism into one of four mechanism categories. We view these as
ple, conscientiousness) directly, specific ‘if–then’ habits that reoccur being necessary to engender change: preconditions, triggers, rein-
and are contextualized within a particular setting (for example, using forcers and integrators. Together, these four mechanism categories
a planner at the start of each week), or narrower affect, behaviour, describe the general process of change across the different levels of
cognition and desire states that are time-specific or situation-specific personality abstraction10,26–28 (Fig. 2).
(for example, arriving late to a meeting)9,10,25. These three levels of per-
sonality abstraction10,26–28 (Fig. 1) are interrelated such that changes in Preconditions
one level can have top-down or bottom-up effects on another level. Preconditions are circumstances or contexts that make it possible for
Direct changes to broad and complex latent traits occur relatively change to occur12. For instance, in the context of therapy, patients can
infrequently and are difficult to elicit outside biologically mediated impact the therapeutic process by resisting treatment. Similarly, in their
pathways such as physical injury, cognitive decline or pharmacologi- daily lives people might — intentionally or not — close themselves off
cal mechanisms15,29–34. For example, education about the importance such that the process of change cannot be completed or even begin.
of certain characteristics (for example, the value of conscientious- For intentional personality change, people often need to have a desire
ness for a job) does not lead to prolonged change by itself because or motivation to change who they are either as an end goal itself or to
corresponding changes in trait-relevant habits or states also need achieve other goals12. In other words, they must want to change state-level

Nature Reviews Psychology


Review article

Latent trait Trait-relevant habits State-level expression


High
Trait level

Low
0.5 1.0 1.5 2.0 2.5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5
Time (years) Time (weeks) Time (days)

Levels of a trait change at the Levels of a trait begin to shift Levels of a trait fluctuate based on
latent level once habits are through successful (and bouts of state-level expressions of relevant
routinized and their effects unsuccessful) repetition of new affect, behaviour, cognition and
integrated into one’s identity habits desires

Fig. 1 | Timescales of change. Changes at long, medium and short timescales with longer timeframes reflects the fact that it is trivial to change behaviour
are associated with latent traits, trait-relevant habits and state-level for a few moments or even a few weeks of time, but this does not meet the
expression, respectively. However, each level of personality abstraction is not standard defining a change in the underlying trait that transcends context
necessarily restricted to this single timeframe. The pairing of the trait level and time.

manifestations of affect, behaviour, cognition and desires. If someone as incarceration65. Thus, a circumstance in which changes cannot
changes their state-level manifestations in a manner akin to just ‘going be opposed by an individual might be a sufficient precondition for
through the motions’ or for external reasons instead of enacting changes change. These circumstances do not lead to desired or goal-directed
they desire and welcome, they are unlikely to experience broad and changes on behalf of the individual, but rather prohibit the stability and
lasting personality change as a result58. For example, someone working maintenance of typical affect, behaviour, cognition and desire states.
in the service industry might act especially nice and warm in order to Importantly, even if personality change is intentional or desired,
receive a tip. However, changes in these state expressions will not result the potential changes in state-level manifestations of affect, behaviour,
in lasting, self-perceived changes to the broader trait of agreeableness cognition and desires must be doable12,66 for subsequent changes to
if they feel that smiling is merely part of the job and restricted to that actually occur. Whether state-level changes are possible is, in part,
context, have no intrinsic desire to be nice or warm, and subsequently contingent upon an individual’s capacity to enact new affect, behav-
fail to incorporate this behaviour into their self-concept. iour, cognition or desires and it might not be feasible for a person to
The motivation for change is often not specifically to elicit broad actively change these building blocks of who they are. For instance,
personality or behavioural change itself. Consequently, the desire for it might be too mentally taxing to act in an atypical fashion67 or a person
change is so far removed from perceiving or purposefully enacting might lack knowledge of when to modify their behaviour to obtain an
trait-relevant, state-level behaviour change that a person might not be appropriate response68, resulting in few or failed attempts to do so.
aware they have a change goal. This type of unintended change prob- Notably, there is an important distinction between believing that one
ably accounts for most of the changes that occur across the lifespan4. can change and the practical feasibility of enacting these changes.
For example, instead of seeking to change their behaviour, a person It might not be necessary for a person to believe they are capable of
might want to ‘fit in’ in a new social role. Commitment to certain broad change69–71; however, the corresponding affect, behaviour, cognition
social roles (such as establishing a career or becoming a parent) is a and desire changes must be feasible. No one can change their affect,
primary promoter of change19,59–62. Committing to these roles indicates behaviour, cognition and desires in all situations, so the ability to
an implicit desire to change behaviour because these roles are associ- effectively enact novel affect, behaviour, cognition and desires in the
ated with different expectations of how to think, feel and behave59,62–64. correct situation is crucial.
Thus, a person committing to a new role might not explicitly want to
change their personality, but at some level is open to changing some Triggers
aspects of themselves. Triggers are experiences that result in state-level affect, behaviour,
The desire or motivation to change is explicit when there are cognition and desire changes13,17. Anything that elicits a state-level
intentional efforts to change or clear change goals. However, this expression can be thought of as a trigger, including external factors
precondition of wanting to change some part of one’s personality such as situations or tasks and internal factors such as physiological
is not necessary for change to occur; change can occur due to cir- or stress responses54,56,57,72–74. The trigger’s corresponding state-level
cumstances outside one’s control. For example, change might occur manifestation is often not sufficient to constitute broad, lasting change
through biologically mediated processes such as dementia or trau- if the novel personality state occurs in isolation and is not repeated
matic brain injury29,34, or through environmental circumstances such over time. For instance, an introvert can on occasion act extraverted

Nature Reviews Psychology


Review article

Table 1 | Proposed mechanisms for changing personality

Category Component Example Refs.

Preconditions Motivation to change A person feels lonely and wishes they were more outgoing so that they could make 9,17,196
new friends

Belief that one can change Change is viewed as feasible 10,53,69,197

A goal that necessitates change A person wants to have more friends, therefore they must act in a more extraverted way 12,198–201

Feasibility of change A person’s life has enough flexibility to allow change; there are ample resources to 12,66,130,144
support change

Environmental disequilibrium A person moves away from their hometown for tertiary education and is prompted to 156,170,172
engage in new behaviours and adopt a new routine

Triggers Environment with expectations A new job comes with a set of standards and expectations for how one should behave 13,59,64,143

Modest (scaled) behavioural Purchasing and using a daily planner (modest change) versus dedicating several 12,41,202
change hours each week to a new exercise routine (drastic change)

Cued discrepancy between actual A person has a desired identity (for example, a good employee) and engages in 203–207
and ideal self relevant behaviours (for example, setting project reminders) when the discrepancy is
cued (for example, forgetting to do a project)

Biologically mediated processes Pre-programmed maturation; health or cognitive decline; hormonal processes; gene 15,16,20,21,29,
expression 101,208,209

Triggers and Social roles Psychological commitment to social roles (for example, a spouse) and expectations 19,59,60,64,
reinforcers to engage in role-typed behaviours (for example, warmth) 79,206,210

Situation selection A person who wants to be a better student selects into environments that provide 13,141,142,211
support for this change (for example, choosing to attend study hall)

Reinforcers Transfer or generalization A person changes trait-relevant behaviours in several aspects of their life as opposed 13,43,44,212
to in a single context

Self-regulation and reframing A person who finds it scary to meet new people shifts their perception of the 28,84,213
situation and views it as an opportunity to make friends instead of an opportunity to
be ignored, bullied or ostracized

Self-monitoring New behaviours are enacted until the desired end state is reached 12,13

Habit formation New affect, behaviours, cognitions and desires are integrated into one’s lifestyle 36,41,43,45,
through repetition 68,92,124

Adequate resources A person has the behavioural and/or cognitive capacity and the self-regulatory 9,67,130,
abilities necessary to maintain change 144,145

Sufficient support A person seeks out support from friends and family when facing difficulties enacting 10,130,144,214
change rather than giving up

Corresponding principle A person selects into environments that are consistent with their existing traits, 18,19,77,139,
reinforcing and deepening the expression of those traits 143,215

Societal norms A person moves to a new country where it is less usual to start conversations with 94–97
strangers, reinforcing a decreased tendency to exhibit extraverted behaviours
across time

Reinforcement The type of feedback received from others either encourages or discourages 13,125
continued engagement in the behaviour

Integrators Self-reflection Explicit perception and awareness of the trait-relevant changes in behaviour 113,125,216,217

Assimilation A person integrates behavioural changes into their identity 140,218–221

Internal attributions Behaviour change is recognized and attributed to personality change versus an 12,13,216
external factor

Feedback Other people react to an individual’s behaviour and provide information about the 125
behaviour’s utility, benefits and/or consequences

Reputation Other people discuss and come to conclusions about what a person’s personality is 222
like based on their behaviour

Reinforcement The information a person receives from others about their new behaviour reinforces 13,125
new changes to their self-concept

Implicit learning Repeated cognitive activation of two concepts that temporally coincide with 126,127
one another

Nature Reviews Psychology


Review article

Fig. 2 | Process of change across levels of


personality abstraction. Preconditions (red)
Latent trait
create circumstances that are conducive to initiating
Duration and/or degree of persistence

the process of change. Triggers (yellow) prompt


• Motivations, desires, goals
• Associative processes conditional state-level expressions of affect,
• Feasibility of change
• Reflective processes
• Open to change behaviour, cognition and desires. Reinforcers
(purple) ensure that state-level expressions continue
and persist over time. Integrators (blue) involve
Habits reflecting on newly formed behavioural repertoires
and the environmental or social reactions to
• Situation selection • Incorporation into routine these novel behaviours and making them a part of
• Situation-specific expectations • Repeated selection of
• Environmental affordances similar situations
self-concept. Change is not guaranteed to happen,
• Reinforcement or feedback because exogenous forces acting on any of the four
mechanisms could shift the change process.
State expression

Preconditions Triggers Reinforcers Integrators Exogenous factors

and talk to strangers at a party (the trigger), but this single instance of likely to elicit change. For example, a person can change how they
extraverted behaviour does not and will not make that person extra- view their job. Instead of simply considering their job a way to pass
verted. However, repeated instances of the trigger are not necessary time, which might encourage passive behaviour or ‘going through the
in some situations, such as when a trigger is internal or driven by bio- motions’, they could view it as a means of behaving in an industrious
logical mechanisms (such as severe injury34,75 or dementia29,76), because and efficient manner. Thus, similar to what is often done in therapy,
its effects independently perpetuate. Thus, trigger-induced changes successfully reframing a situation involves focusing on strategies for
require that exposure to the trigger is maintained: either a persistent selecting situations that yield the desired behaviour, by interpreting
trigger or repeated instances of a transient trigger. and evaluating each situation in accordance with goals, or by modifying
Environmental cues or triggers that facilitate state-level change perception of the situation89,90. Thus, triggering experiences range from
include broad life events77,78, social roles20,79, developmental tasks80, relatively straightforward behaviourism, whereby a trigger stimulus
guided experiences (such as therapy81), specific situations (such as induces a behaviour, to a more complex cue that triggers a person to
educational settings82), emotion regulation strategies (such as rumina- notice emotional responses they do not desire or actions that lead
tion or suppression)83 and cognitive reframing of thought processes84. to incongruent goals.
Triggering experiences influence the objective situation around a
person as well as their psychological interpretation of the situation85,86. Reinforcers
Ideally there would be a simple one-to-one correspondence Triggers change the expression of state-level affect, behaviour, cog-
between a trigger and a person’s subsequent state-level manifesta- nition and desires, but lasting change occurs only through repeated
tion. For instance, the waiter in the previous example wants to continue state-level changes13,15,44,68. It is relatively easy to shift and modify behav-
receiving tips (trigger), and therefore their physical and psychologi- iour in a single moment, whereas it is more difficult to establish an
cal situations remain the same — they continue to perform their job automatic, habitual change in affect, behaviour, cognition and desires
and have the goal to behave in a way that earns them tips. Thus, their that is sustained over time. Furthermore, triggers might be avoided
intentional affect, behaviour, cognition and desires continue to include because they are unpleasant or effortful. Thus, external or internal
being friendly and kind (state-level manifestations) to achieve their reinforcers are needed to ensure that exposure to triggers persists long
goal, which is activated by the trigger. However, situations can be inter- enough to produce the repeated changes in state-level manifestations
preted idiosyncratically87,88 and there is little reason to believe that all required for enduring bottom-up change17,35,41,43,91, such as through the
individuals respond to the same situations in a similar manner86. The formation of habits12,36,43,45,92.
environmental pressures for certain behaviours will differ substantially For example, a workplace might trigger industrious behaviours79,
between a waiter whose goal is to make more money and a waiter who and the guarantee of a paycheque might serve as a reinforcer that
is working primarily to spend time with their friends, for example. results in continued exposure to or engagement with the same trig-
Thus, many of the proposed mechanisms for personality change ger. In this example, the reinforcer might exert its effect through
focus not on explicit environmental cues but rather on broader ways person-centric responses to situations such as continued situation
of thinking that are applicable across many situations11. In line with selection (for example, choosing to continue to go to work in order
this view, changing personality is not accomplished by leading people to get paid) or the positive affective response a person has once in
into novel situations, but rather by having people change how they that situation (for example, satisfaction at knowing they are earning
view existing situations, their expectations for how others interpret money).
their actions, and their ability to carry out behaviours. Each type of In addition to person-centric responses to situations, expecta-
reframing can create cues that transcend the objective situation. For tions for how to behave appropriately in a given situation, outcomes
instance, although a job does not itself necessarily lead to personality that are contingent upon behaving in a certain manner, and overt
change, modifications can be made to this situation that make it more consequences for failing to act in accordance with expectations93–95

Nature Reviews Psychology


Review article

serve as reinforcers at the societal level93,94,96,97. These expectations, paying bills and in their social life. ‘Personality’ refers to a broad set
contingencies and consequences serve as a guide for how to behave of skills that permeates many aspects of life, so changing personality
(and how not to behave)19,97,98. For example, a new job is associated requires more than a single habit change in one domain111. Although
with certain behaviours that are deemed appropriate (such as arriving transfer from one situation to another is extremely difficult112, these
on time) as well as behaviours that are deemed inappropriate (such broad changes can be facilitated through reflective processes that
as stealing office supplies)79,99,100. Behaving in line with these expecta- integrate changes in affect, behaviour, cognition and desires into
tions has long-term implications for colleague relations and salary. self-concept or identity18,113. Such reflective processes might take the
Thus, there are strong incentives to act in accordance with the expec- following form: “If I am organized enough to make going to the gym
tations and norms of the situation. These incentives are particularly a habit, then I am organized enough to make sure my office is clean.
strong for many social roles, but — regardless of their domain — most Because I have an organized schedule and clean office, I consider myself
situations have expectations for standard behaviour (and punishments a conscientious person.” This type of integration is essential to engen-
for deviations from those expectations). dering long-term change, and is one of the main effective components
Within the context of intentional change, reinforcers are conscious of many types of therapy114,115.
and key to having individuals continue to engage in what was once a Reflective processes can be extended to other people. Reflection
challenging task1,10,17,81. For example, identifying strengths and avail- at the individual level should lead to changes in self-reported personal-
able resources enables people to reframe and overcome challenges in ity. However, a person might not always be the most accurate judge of
maintaining changes in affect, behaviour, cognition and desires101. This the extent to which they changed and/or rely on the most appropriate
type of explicit reinforcement is often done directly (such as through reference points to evaluate their personality116–119. Informants (such
a therapist), but is also a key component of habit formation, in which as friends, family members or romantic partners) can also provide per-
short-term goals scaffold the successful completion of long-term goals. sonality judgements to help to validate that measurable, observable or
For example, acknowledging and celebrating small wins in developing meaningful changes occurred following a sustained period of reinforced
an exercise habit (such as a person’s new personal record) facilitates state-level expressions120–122. Correspondence between self-rated and
the more long-term goal of establishing the habit41,43. informant-rated changes in personality confirms that some changes
In addition to environmental and affective reinforcers, reinforc- in a person’s personality have occurred25,50,117,119,123, and suggests that
ers might be more biological. For example, long-term periods of high the changes to that person’s personality were successfully integrated
inflammation might increase negative affectivity102. Smoking or the by informants as well. That is, informants attributing new personality
immediate high stress caused by unexpectedly losing one’s job can trig- characteristics to this person now ascribe what were once novel changes
ger increased inflammation103,104; the maintenance of such behaviours in affect, behaviour, cognition and desires to who this person ‘is’. Thus,
or circumstances can likewise maintain this physiological response, although personality is an aspect of an individual, aspects of the change
with both processes now serving as separate (albeit complementary) process can occur for and be detected by other people.
reinforcers. Cognitive decline is another biological example. One Reflective processes are an explicit way to integrate changes in
of the more common causes of cognitive decline is dementia105–107. affect, behaviour, cognition and desires into self-concept. Such integra-
Although behavioural and lifestyle factors play some part in cognitive tion can also occur implicitly through associative processes13,124, includ-
decline76,105,108,109, the continued buildup and spread of malfunctioning ing implicit learning, feedback and reinforcement in how someone
proteins such as beta-amyloid and tau in the brain107 serve as a dominant views themselves13,125–127. The lack of conscious awareness of associative
reinforcer for subsequent changes to how a person with dementia processes suggests that they are primarily involved in the integra-
thinks, feels and behaves. tion of naturally occurring as opposed to intentional changes. The
effects of associative processes often take longer to materialize than
Integrators intentional integration efforts via reflective processes owing to the
The prolonged state-level changes brought on by reinforcers need to incremental nature of associative processes13,124,128 and are contingent
be sustained over time. In habit formation, these reinforcers turn upon environmental factors that promote them13,124,128,129.
trigger-induced behaviour into automatic behaviour. Changes in states For example, a teenager starting their first job might acquire new
that lead to the adoption of habits might occur through reinforcers habits and skills, such as: time management to balance work and school
(for example, an alarm reminder and the cost of gym membership obligations; impulse control to not spend money because they are now
might lead to a habit of exercising). However, the transition from habits more aware of the time and effort required to obtain it; and organiza-
to trait-level personality change requires an integration of any novel tion abilities to keep track of when work tasks and school assignments
affect, behaviour, cognition and desire into one’s identity and general need to be completed. After several months of receiving better grades,
lifestyle in other life domains. That is, habits formed in one situation good performance reviews and praise from their parents, these exter-
need to be widely extended to other situations to constitute change nal indicators might eventually serve as cues that their behaviour has
in latent personality. A steady exercise schedule is associated with changed, and that these behaviours have come to, in part, define who
conscientiousness, but a newly implemented gym regimen does not they are across multiple domains. That is, rather than seeing themselves
constitute personality change, because this habit and its trait-relevant as someone who ‘does good work’ and ‘controls their spending urges’,
manifestations of affect, behaviour, cognition and desires are local these become implicit learned qualities of the self such that when a
in scope. new task is presented they feel capable of accomplishing it because
Borrowing from the cognitive training literature, the difference they ‘are a good worker’ and ‘have self-control’.
between habit formation and personality change is skill transfer from Reflective and associative processes are not mutually exclusive.
one domain to another110. For example, transfer would be evident if For example, it is possible that the teenager in the previous example
someone who became more organized after establishing an exercise was always aware of their behavioural changes and their consequences.
habit extended this skill to become also more organized at work, in However, these perceived changes might have been localized to the

Nature Reviews Psychology


Review article

work domain, such that they recognized that they were a skilled, organ- circumstances (such as becoming a new parent and job loss) do not
ized worker, yet failed to see how these learned skills transferred to lead to major changes in personality traits on average77,138. To com-
other domains. In this case, the feedback and acknowledgement of plement the previous discussion on the mechanisms of change, here
changes in other areas from external sources might be necessary for we describe mechanisms that contribute to consistency and thereby
learning that these qualities reflect more than just how they behave at prevent change (Table 2).
work. Thus, integration processes by both the person and the people First, acting out of character (or outside the standard range of
around them validate that changes have occurred. state-level manifestations of affect, behaviour, cognition and desires)
is an effortful process that can result in feeling inauthentic or ‘off’67. To
Summary avoid taxing behaviours and/or aversive affective reactions, people cre-
The processes underlying personality change can be summed up as a ate experiences that are in line with their personality by self-selecting
willingness to (take the time to) fake it until you make it (and believe into familiar experiences that are consistent with who they are19,139,140
it, across multiple domains). If someone is open to or willing to or manipulating the environment to make it more in line with their
change (preconditions), encounters situations that lead to changes in personality19,21. If people are not able to find or create a situation that
personality-relevant affect, behaviour, cognition and desires (triggers), aligns with who they are, they might opt out of the situation entirely141.
repeats and sustains these changes over time until they become autono- Moreover, according to the notion of ‘like attracts like’, people are often
mous (reinforcers), and incorporates them into their self-concept selected by others into experiences that fit well with their personality.
(integrators) by employing the new skillset across domains and time, The individuals that select a person into environments that already
then their personality has changed. Thus, personality change is pri- correspond and align with that person’s personality (for example, the
marily a bottom-up process such that changes in states shift over time highly conscientious person is invited to join a study group) thereby
within a context, become habitual and autonomous, and then extend act as gatekeepers of personality change because they help that person
across different contexts and other state-level manifestations to lead to maintain stability142. Collectively, these processes serve to reinforce
to changes in broader personality traits. consistency and prohibit change from occurring.
These steps apply to both naturally occurring and intentional For example, starting tertiary education comes with numerous
changes. Preconditions, triggers and reinforcers are more conscious expectations for how to behave, often related to industrious behaviour.
during intentional change than in naturally occurring change because However, a student can manipulate the environment by seeking out and
intentional change involves an acknowledgement of wanting to change self-selecting into a party crowd rather than finding a hard-working
(preconditions), explicitly seeking out situations that elicit desired study group. If a party crowd is similar to the one the student identi-
behaviours (triggers), and self-directed efforts to maintain desired fied with in high school, the crowd might likewise seek out the student
behaviours (reinforcers)9,10,12. Similarly, during intentional change because they fit in well. The student might initially get invited to a study
integrators more probably operate through reflective processes than group, but instead of studying they might change the atmosphere
associative processes because there is conscious awareness of the by gossiping and not paying attention. After some pushback by the
change process, such as cognitive reappraisals that recognize and study group, the student might opt out and find a situation that bet-
attribute new behaviours to one’s identity (integrators)10,130. It is likely ter coincides with who they are. Thus, despite ample opportunities
that being aware of, and directly focused on, the different components for triggers that promote industrious behaviour, the processes of
needed for personality change to occur accelerates the change process, selection, attraction, manipulation and attrition can all serve to keep
thereby making intentional change more efficacious than naturally personality consistent by influencing the situations in which a person
occurring change. Naturally occurring change is instead probably finds themselves141,143. If a person continuously creates and finds them-
driven by associative learning processes that foster change in a less selves in the same types of situation, such continuity will reinforce the
consistent and slower manner. In the absence of intentional or con- stability of their current personality18.
scious efforts to change some aspect of oneself, external factors and Furthermore, a person might enter a situation with a certain goal
cues become a major part of the change process. Irrespective of the in mind but have a subsequent aversive reaction to the situation that
motivation, intention or awareness of change, external sources can weakens (or even fully negates) its intended impact. In this instance,
trigger, reinforce, integrate and help to validate any changes that occur. situation selection serves to oppose change because of the person’s
reaction to the situation. For example, a person might decide to start
Processes that prevent change setting an early alarm to wake up and go to the gym before work. How-
Although most people want to change some aspect of their personality1, ever, when the alarm goes off the person is immediately annoyed at
not everyone has the right preconditions or triggers — nor maintains their past self for setting this alarm and trying to force a new routine.
exposure to triggers over long enough time periods — to do so. Simi- Because their sleep was disrupted, the person is now tired and irritable
larly, not every person can successfully integrate any changes into and is less productive at work and gets into small arguments with their
their self-concept. Merely wanting to change is not enough. In general, partner. The result of this sequence of events is that even though the
personality is a very consistent and stable system5,131,132, so it should be situation the person tried to select into was meant to have desirable,
no surprise that intervening in and changing personality is difficult12,133. positive consequences, their reaction to this situation had the opposite
For instance, people tend not to show average changes in their levels of effect: instead of catalysing a new routine that could lead to change, the
dispositional traits or life satisfaction following major life events and, likelihood of entering that situation again was reduced and consistency
if they do change, they often revert to their previous levels77,78,134–136. was reinforced.
This ‘bouncing back’ of levels of personality following life events is Second, even if people shift their state-level manifestations of
often studied and found in data that spans one to a few years between affect, behaviour, cognition and desires, the shift might not last long
waves77,78,135,137 but has also been observed within mere months136. enough for change to occur. Behaving in a way that is inconsistent with
Similarly, even those life events which lead to extreme changes in life one’s disposition is effortful and depletes self-regulatory abilities,

Nature Reviews Psychology


Review article

Table 2 | Proposed mechanisms that reinforce personality stability

Category of Component Example Refs.


mechanism
opposed

Preconditions No desire for change Most people view their current trait levels as relatively desirable; discount potential 223–227
outcomes that would follow any change; and want to avoid feelings of discomfort,
loss of sense of control and unexpected reactions from their environment
Change is not viewed as possible Believing that people inherently cannot change 10,12,53,69,197
Changes are not feasible A person might have a rigid lifestyle that offers no flexibility, or their desired changes 12,66
are too different from their current personality
Triggers Consistent environment A person has had the same job, friends, hobbies and partner for the past decade, and 18,59,206
none of these factors have changed
Biological mechanisms Stability in gene expression and phenotypic factors; genetic predispositions 20,21,209,215
Misattributing the need for change A person might misattribute negative experiences or failures to external causes and 228–230
fail to recognize their own role
Insufficient environmental pressures The external pressure that could elicit affective, behavioural or cognitive change is 17,77,148,149
either too similar to existing factors a person is exposed to or is too different for a
person to continue engaging with it
Reinforcers Inconsistent or short-term behavioural A person might try to set up a new exercise routine or diet plan, but then revert to 41,43,44
change their old lifestyle after a couple of weeks
Selecting into personality-congruent People seek out specific environments that will stabilize the traits that led them to 19,139–141
environments seek out those niches in the first place
Inadequate internal resources Behaving in a way that is inconsistent with dispositions is effortful and depletes 9,67,144,145
self-regulatory abilities, causing people to gravitate back to their original dispositions
Lack of external support Someone might not have friends and/or family to support them or they might be in 9,144,214
an environment that does not allow for long-term changes in behaviour
Lack of transfer or generalization Change is localized to a particular context (for example, a person might get better at 41,43,110,212
organizing their social plans but fail to be more organized at work)
Integrators External attributions A person might attribute change to external circumstances instead of themselves 228–230
(for example, “I haven’t become more organized, I just have to keep my desk clean so
I don’t get fired”)
Lack of self-insight or self-awareness People are usually not aware of how their personality traits have changed across time 117,231,232
Shift in reference group for After acquiring a new behavioural routine or being in a new environment, a person 116,118,163
self-evaluation shifts their reference group to be the people who also perform these behaviours,
thereby failing to see changes in themselves that were once evaluated against a
different population
Absence of environmental support A person might not regularly interact with people who can provide feedback on 124,126,222
or reward and/or punish new behaviour, thereby leading to a failure to form an
associative link between the new behaviours and self-concept

which can cause people to gravitate back to their original dispositions that are challenging yet attainable is a well known process in exposure
and behavioural routines9,67,144,145. It is unclear how long state changes therapy, where progressively greater exposures are used to facilitate
need to last to engender trait change. Habit formation can take weeks greater change151.
to months to occur depending on the habit146. Changes in personality Fourth, people might successfully develop habits and new char-
that occur as part of an intervention or therapy have been found to acteristic ways of behaving but fail to perceive these changes or to
occur over a period of weeks81,147, but naturally occurring personality attribute any perceived changes to the more abstract concept of who
change is probably slower19. they are. Thus, no change develops owing to a failure in effective reflec-
Third, shifts in states must be calibrated to what is possible for tive or associative processes. Unsuccessful self-perception can be a
the person. People often regress back to their previous personality if barrier to measurable change but can potentially be compensated for
they are placed in situations that demand too much or have unspeci- by external sources of validation.
fied norms, rules or expectations for behaviour20,148,149. Too drastic a In sum, just as there are mechanisms that foster personality
change will lead to feelings of discomfort and subsequent avoidance change, there are also processes that reinforce stability and/or pre-
of the trigger, whereas too little change will not engage reinforcer vent change. People often prefer sameness; the drive to maintain a
and integrator processes. What is needed are changes in behaviour familiar environment and stable sense of self, accompanied by every-
that build on what someone is currently capable of, such as what has day obstacles that interfere with successful and sustained changes in
been termed a zone of proximal development150. This perpetuating, behaviour, places limitations on whether (and how much) change is
combined use of a person’s existing skills and capabilities with goals possible21,141,143,145,152.

Nature Reviews Psychology


Review article

Evidence for change mechanisms also explicitly directed to integrate novel components of personal-
The model of mechanisms responsible for personality change based ity into a broader trait within the incarceration rehabilitation pro-
on preconditions, triggers, reinforcers and integrators shares many cess and military training. However, despite the explicit existence
commonalities with other theories of personality development18–21,64. of all four mechanisms of change, there is still only modest evidence
Here we review the evidence for each of the proposed mechanisms that these experiences promote change and results are inconsistent
drawn from studies of key external factors believed to engender lasting across studies65,169–172.
change: life events and interventions.
Interventions
Life events Personality traits are often not the target of interventions, but many
Research has focused on life events as catalysts of change7,8,77,78,137,153. interventions target related constructs such as life satisfaction. In
Life events serve as a major source of all four change mechanisms, general, these interventions are efficacious, with modest effect sizes
especially triggers and reinforcers. For example, people starting a job (d = 0.20–0.40) that have meaningful impacts in that people report
typically have a goal to perform well (either for personal satisfaction notable changes in their well-being173,174. The most effective of these
or monetary gain), thereby satisfying many preconditions of change. well-being interventions focus on principles established via thera-
The job itself triggers novel experiences, such as completing new tasks. peutic techniques, such as cognitive reappraisal175 and mindfulness
People show up to their job day after day, ensuring adequate repetition interventions176. These interventions explicitly target reinforcers and
and reinforcement of these new behaviours, and people are motivated integration. Positive psychology interventions177, which aim to increase
to act in accordance with expectations to keep their job and be in good positive affect, meaning in life and engagement, rather than to decrease
standing with their colleagues and superiors. Finally, integration is negative symptoms, vary in content and target mechanism and show
likely because people tend to do many different (albeit related) tasks modest to limited effectiveness for improving life satisfaction (d = 0.20)
in their job, thereby developing generalizable skills, and they receive with evidence of small sample bias178.
feedback about their performance. Indeed, there is ample evidence Interventions that target self-control also show modest to lim-
that occupations are associated with changes in personality, well-being ited effects (d = 0.15–0.25; ref. 179). The nature of these interventions
and self-esteem49,154,155. vary, but they generally repeat triggers180–182. Many of the less effective
Although many studies find associations between life events and self-control interventions and positive psychology interventions are
personality development78,139,156–158, many do not137,138,159,160 and causal, short and vague in terms of which mechanisms should be responsible
directional effects are often not statistically significant161. For exam- for lasting change.
ple, starting a job is associated with increases in neuroticism in some Far fewer interventions for personality traits have been attempted.
studies77,154,156 but not in others78,79,162. Associations between life events The largest number of studies have looked at personality traits as a side
and personality are larger and are observed more frequently for affec- effect of interventions that target other constructs such as cognitive
tive and self-traits (such as self-esteem and life satisfaction) than for ability147,180,183, psychopathology184–188 or delinquent behaviour189–191.
dispositional traits (such as the Big Five)8,77. Even though these interventions attempt to directly influence the
The limited evidence that the broad category of life events pro- four change mechanisms, the evidence for personality trait change as
duces meaningful average changes in personality might be due to the a result of these interventions is mixed at best. Although some stud-
overreliance of self-reported traits rather than implicit measures or ies show that changing daily behaviours (such as openness-related
informant reports that might detect additional aspects of personal- behaviours147) are associated with trait change, other studies find null
ity that are not consciously accessible, were originally misjudged (for effects180. Similarly, successful studies tend to find that changes fade
example, use of inappropriate reference group163), or are not perceived over time, demonstrating the strength of processes that promote
or noticed by the person themselves56,117,119,121–123. Alternatively, life consistency192.
events could be too broad to serve as a meaningful proxy for change The best evidence that personality traits can be modified comes
mechanisms because it is not the broad event that matters but rather from studies that use psychotherapy methods. A meta-analysis
the specific new affect, behaviour, cognition or desires that a given of 207 studies found that personality traits changed over the course of
person expresses in the context of the event, which can further vary therapy81. Changes were evident regardless of the therapy modality,
across people. Moreover, people interpret life events differently156,164,165. indicating that type of therapy is less important than the process
Thus, life events might promote change mechanisms for some but not of attempting to change. Changes in personality were strongest for
all individuals166. Because interpretations of life events are varied, they neuroticism (d = 0.50), which is associated with disorders such as
probably result in idiosyncratic change, as opposed to a meaningful anxiety and depression2,3. However, there were changes in all other
and quantifiable average change137,165,167,168. Big Five traits as well, suggesting that therapy has broad effects on
‘Fish out of water’ experiences, such as when a person is incarcer- personality traits. Notably, these effects persisted up to one year
ated65, moves to a new country169,170 or joins the military171, might be after therapy stopped, underscoring the potential for persistent,
better suited for testing change mechanisms because there is more long-term change81.
homogeneity in the day-to-day experiences, especially for military Interventions that specifically target integration mechanisms,
and incarceration experiences. In these situations, a person’s entire particularly via reflective processes, are effective193. These findings
normative day-to-day experience shifts, and these experiences are have prompted development of self-directed personality interventions
designed with explicit change goals in mind (for example, rehabili- where therapeutic concepts are implemented without a therapist. For
tation is the goal of incarceration, and ‘be all that you can be’ was example, in a 15-week-long intervention individuals created if–then
the recruiting slogan for the US army). Triggering situations are plans (such as “if I feel stressed, then I will call my mom to talk about
constantly present and difficult to avoid, and there are major con- it”) and set themselves behavioural challenges (such as “introduce
sequences for not changing behaviour (reinforcement). People are yourself to someone new”)193 that directly invoke triggering situations.

Nature Reviews Psychology


Review article

Table 3 | Application of personality change mechanisms to interventions

Category Role in personality change Example Implementation scenario

Preconditions Prerequisite states that facilitate the opportunity for Actuate discrepancy awareness by Have people write down what they hope
change, such as by creating an intention or desire to explicitly considering the incongruity their personality looks like in five years and
change between actual and desired personality contrast this with their current personality
Triggers Factors that underlie state-level changes in Target change in localized behaviours by If a person reports a bad day at work,
construct-relevant affect, behaviour, cognition and creating a set of if–then contingencies remind them of their intention to create
desires via behavioural activation and cue-conditioned that are applicable in specific a to-do list when they get to work in the
responses circumstances morning
Reinforcers Factors that create favourable internal and external Identify the strengths and resources Remind people that their friends and
circumstances that enable a person to repeatedly a person possesses that can alleviate family can assist when they encounter
engage in and maintain state-level affect, behaviour, the burdensome and taxing nature of difficulties and help to hold them
cognition and desires continuously enacting novel affect, accountable
behaviour, cognition and desires
Integration The consolidation and automatization of previously Prompt self-reflection or explicit After a bout of successful adherence to an
trigger-induced changes in narrow contexts into awareness of the progress a person has intervention plan, send a summary of how
enduring changes that transcend domains are made and the period of time in which they reported behaviours have changed and
performed automatically, and are part of self-concept have upheld these changes the progress made towards the goal

The results showed that those that desired to change their personality Summary and future directions
changed more than those who desired no change (d = 0.2). In this Review, we outlined four categories of mechanisms necessary
Another successful self-guided intervention involved a per- for personality change — preconditions, triggers, reinforcers and
sonalized app that delivered interventions based on psychotherapy integrators — their role in fostering lasting change, and empirical evi-
principles50. Researchers explicitly specified the mechanisms tar- dence validating their importance. We also described the processes
geted in the intervention, which correspond well with the four change that promote stability and prevent enduring changes in personality.
mechanisms described here10 (Table 3). In the three-month-long ran- Finally, we discussed two of the major external factors believed to cause
domized controlled trial participants interacted with a digital coach personality change — life events and interventions — and the mixed
within the app, which provided specific tools and techniques to help evidence regarding their effectiveness in engendering lasting change.
and maintain modifications in affect, behaviours, cognitions and Despite decades of research on personality change, there remain
desires. Both self-reports (d = 0.50) and observer reports (d = 0.35) many open questions. First, little is known about which specific mecha-
of personality indicated that those who underwent the intervention nisms are sufficient and/or necessary to lead to change. Although post
changed more than the control group, with the changes lasting at least hoc examination across different types of intervention to find analo-
three months after the intervention. Thus, drawing from empirically gous mechanisms is possible, it is difficult to identify commonalities
supported change mechanisms and developing an individualized meaningfully. Future interventions should be rigorously and iteratively
intervention protocol that targeted each component fostered enduring developed to identify the necessary components. For example, when an
personality change9,10,50. intervention is deemed successful the researcher should then change
different aspects to test which components are essential.
Summary Future research should also identify whether change processes
The findings described in this section have three broad takeaways. are general or personalized, especially for naturally occurring changes.
First, life events are too broad to offer much insight into the mecha- One way to interpret current evidence is that many environments
nisms that drive change. Life events probably engender change for engender change for some people, but many people do not change as
some people, some of the time, but differences in the ways that people a consequence of any specific experience. It might be that some people
interpret, react and experience life events are probably so varied that are more malleable or resilient than others, or that people are more
general conclusions cannot be made. Although people change across amenable to change at specific times in the lifespan. Better assessment
the lifespan, the vast majority do not seek therapy or attempt to change of environments is needed to understand how environments ‘get under
their personality; it is unclear what drives these changes4,137,159,160,167,194. the skin’ to influence people195.
Second, interventions show promise in changing broad constructs, In summary, there is no standard set of steps for personality
such as life satisfaction and personality traits. The most effective inter- change; instead, there are many potential pathways by which change
ventions mirror psychotherapy. However, many interventions are inef- can occur. However, change is not easy because many processes work
fective, probably because they focus on simple models of change and to prevent change from occurring and change processes themselves
do not address multiple mechanisms necessary for change to occur. take time. Although interventions can accelerate the change process,
Third, few interventions specify and rigorously test which mecha- the experiences that lead to personality change probably differ in
nisms are responsible for change. Instead, interventions are often effectiveness across people. Importantly, change, when it occurs, is
planned, tested and then left unmodified if successful. Thus, there is modest50,178,182. Other than in extenuating circumstances such as bio-
a lack of iterative testing for the necessity and existence of boundary logically mediated change, a person is not able to become someone
conditions for individual intervention components. Although there are completely different.
exceptions9, this lack of rigorous testing makes it difficult to identify
the mechanisms underlying effective interventions. Published online: xx xx xxxx

Nature Reviews Psychology


Review article

References 28. Hooker, K. & McAdams, D. P. Personality reconsidered: a new agenda for aging research.
1. Hudson, N. W. & Roberts, B. W. Goals to change personality traits: concurrent links J. Gerontol. B 58, 296–304 (2003).
between personality traits, daily behavior, and goals to change oneself. J. Res. Personal. 29. Terracciano, A., Stephan, Y., Luchetti, M. & Sutin, A. R. Cognitive impairment, dementia,
53, 68–83 (2014). and personality stability among older adults. Assessment 25, 336–347 (2018).
2. Beck, E. D. & Jackson, J. J. A mega-analysis of personality prediction: robustness and 30. Caselli, R. J. et al. Personality changes during the transition from cognitive health to mild
boundary conditions. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 122, 523–553 (2022). cognitive impairment. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 66, 671–678 (2018).
3. Soto, C. J. How replicable are links between personality traits and consequential life 31. Robins Wahlin, T.-B. & Byrne, G. J. Personality changes in Alzheimer’s disease: a
outcomes? The life outcomes of personality replication project. Psychol. Sci. 30, 711–727 systematic review. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiat. 26, 1019–1029 (2011).
(2019). 32. Tang, T. Z. et al. Personality change during depression treatment: a placebo-controlled
4. Bleidorn, W. et al. Personality stability and change: a meta-analysis of longitudinal trial. Arch. Gen. Psychiat. 66, 1322–1330 (2009).
studies. Psychol. Bull. 148, 588–619 (2022). 33. Quilty, L. C., Meusel, L.-A. C. & Bagby, R. M. Neuroticism as a mediator of treatment
5. Wright, A. J. & Jackson, J. J. Are some people more consistent? Examining the stability response to SSRIs in major depressive disorder. J. Affect. Disord. 111, 67–73 (2008).
and underlying processes of personality profile consistency. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 124, 34. Max, J. E. et al. Predictors of personality change due to traumatic brain injury in children
1314–1337 (2023). and adolescents in the first six months after injury. J. Am. Acad. Child. Adolesc. Psychiat.
6. Bleidorn, W. et al. The policy relevance of personality traits. Am. Psychol. 74, 1056–1067 44, 434–442 (2005).
(2019). 35. Chapman, B. P., Hampson, S. & Clarkin, J. Personality-informed interventions for healthy
This paper highlights the importance of intervening on personality traits. aging: conclusions from a national institute on aging workgroup. Dev. Psychol. 50,
7. Bleidorn, W., Hopwood, C. J. & Lucas, R. E. Life events and personality trait change. 1426–1441 (2014).
J. Pers. 86, 83–96 (2018). 36. Wood, W. & Rünger, D. Psychology of habit. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 67, 289–314 (2016).
8. Bühler, J. L. et al. Life events and personality change: a systematic review and 37. Weiss, B., Miller, J. D., Carter, N. T. & Keith Campbell, W. Examining changes in personality
meta-analysis. Eur. J. Personal. https://doi.org/10.1177/08902070231190219 following shamanic ceremonial use of ayahuasca. Sci. Rep. 11, 6653 (2021).
(2023). 38. Owens, M. et al. Habitual behavior as a mediator between food-related behavioral
9. Allemand, M. & Flückiger, C. Personality change through digital-coaching interventions. activation and change in symptoms of depression in the MooDFOOD trial. Clin. Psychol.
Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 31, 41–48 (2022). Sci. 9, 649–665 (2021).
This paper provides an overview of a successful digital coaching intervention. 39. Martell, C. R., Addis, M. E. & Jacobson, N. S. Depression in Context: Strategies for Guided
10. Allemand, M. & Flückiger, C. Changing personality traits: some considerations from Action (W W Norton & Co, 2001).
psychotherapy process–outcome research for intervention efforts on intentional 40. Watkins, E. R. & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. A habit–goal framework of depressive rumination.
personality change. J. Psychother. Integr. 27, 476–494 (2017). J. Abnorm. Psychol. 123, 24–34 (2014).
This broad theoretical overview on how to intervene to change personality borrows 41. Lally, P. & Gardner, B. Promoting habit formation. Health Psychol. Rev. 7, 137–158 (2013).
from ideas developed in psychotherapy. 42. Denissen, J. J. A., van Aken, M. A. G., Penke, L. & Wood, D. Self‐regulation underlies
11. Geukes, K., Zalk, M. & Back, M. D. Understanding personality development: an integrative temperament and personality: an integrative developmental framework. Child. Dev.
state process model. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 42, 43–51 (2018). Perspect. 7, 255–260 (2013).
This paper presents an innovative theoretical model on how personality develops. 43. Lally, P., van Jaarsveld, C. H. M., Potts, H. W. W. & Wardle, J. How are habits formed:
12. Hennecke, M., Bleidorn, W., Denissen, J. J. A. & Wood, D. A three-part framework for modelling habit formation in the real world. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 40, 998–1009
self-regulated personality development across adulthood. Eur. J. Personal. 28, 289–299 (2010).
(2014). 44. Danner, U. N., Aarts, H. & de Vries, N. K. Habit vs. intention in the prediction of future
This article presents a theoretical model of how personality develops through the lens behaviour: the role of frequency, context stability and mental accessibility of past
of self-regulation. behaviour. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 47, 245–265 (2008).
13. Wrzus, C. & Roberts, B. W. Processes of personality development in adulthood: the 45. Wood, W. & Neal, D. T. A new look at habits and the habit–goal interface. Psychol. Rev.
TESSERA framework. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 21, 253–277 (2017). 114, 843–863 (2007).
This article provides a detailed process view of the many potential intervening 46. Fleeson, W. Toward a structure- and process-integrated view of personality: traits as
processes that result in personality change. density distributions of states. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 80, 1011–1027 (2001).
14. Baumert, A. et al. Integrating personality structure, personality process, and personality 47. Eid, M. & Diener, E. Global judgments of subjective well-being: situational variability and
development. Eur. J. Personal. 31, 503–528 (2017). long-term stability. Soc. Indic. Res. 65, 245–277 (2004).
15. Roberts, B. W. & Jackson, J. J. Sociogenomic personality psychology. J. Pers. 76, 48. Geukes, K., Nestler, S., Hutteman, R., Küfner, A. C. P. & Back, M. D. Trait personality
1523–1544 (2008). and state variability: predicting individual differences in within- and cross-context
16. Roberts, B. W. A revised sociogenomic model of personality traits. J. Pers. 86, 23–35 fluctuations in affect, self-evaluations, and behavior in everyday life. J. Res. Personal. 69,
(2018). 124–138 (2017).
17. Magidson, J. F., Roberts, B. W., Collado-Rodriguez, A. & Lejuez, C. W. Theory-driven 49. Reitz, A. K. Self‐esteem development and life events: a review and integrative process
intervention for changing personality: expectancy value theory, behavioral activation, framework. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 16, 12709 (2022).
and conscientiousness. Dev. Psychol. 50, 1442 (2014). 50. Stieger, M. et al. Changing personality traits with the help of a digital personality change
This article presents a therapy-informed theoretical account of the personality change intervention. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 118, (2021).
process. 51. Hampson, S. E. Personality processes: mechanisms by which personality traits ‘get
18. Roberts, B. W. & Caspi, A. in Understanding Human Development (eds Staudinger, U. M. & outside the skin’. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 63, 315–339 (2012).
Lindenberger, U.) 183–214 (Springer, 2003). 52. Specht, J., Egloff, B. & Schmukle, S. C. Examining mechanisms of personality maturation:
This chapter reviews the types of process that result in change and consistency in the impact of life satisfaction on the development of the big five personality traits. Soc.
passive longitudinal studies. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 4, 181–189 (2013).
19. Roberts, B. W. & Nickel, L. B. in Handbook of Personality Theory and Research (eds John, 53. Hudson, N. W. Does successfully changing personality traits via intervention require
O. & Robins, R. W.) 259–283 (Guilford, 2021). that participants be autonomously motivated to change? J. Res. Personal. 95, 104160
20. Specht, J. et al. What drives adult personality development? A comparison of theoretical (2021).
perspectives and empirical evidence. Eur. J. Personal. 28, 216–230 (2014). 54. Borghuis, J. et al. Longitudinal associations between trait neuroticism and negative daily
This article provides an overview of the predominant theoretical models of personality experiences in adolescence. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 118, 348–363 (2020).
development. 55. van Zalk, M. H. W., Nestler, S., Geukes, K., Hutteman, R. & Back, M. D. The codevelopment
21. Scarr, S. & McCartney, K. How people make their own environments: a theory of of extraversion and friendships: bonding and behavioral interaction mechanisms in
genotype → environment effects. Child. Dev. 54, 424–435 (1983). friendship networks. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 118, 1269–1290 (2020).
22. Briley, D. A. & Tucker-Drob, E. M. Genetic and environmental continuity in personality 56. Quintus, M., Egloff, B. & Wrzus, C. Daily life processes predict long-term development
development: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 140, 1303–1331 (2014). in explicit and implicit representations of Big Five traits: testing predictions from the
23. Roberts, B. W. & Yoon, H. J. Personality psychology. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 73, 489–516 TESSERA (Triggering situations, Expectancies, States and State Expressions, and
(2022). ReActions) framework. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 120, 1049–1073 (2021).
24. Wilt, J. & Revelle, W. Affect, behaviour, cognition and desire in the Big Five: an analysis of 57. Wrzus, C., Luong, G., Wagner, G. G. & Riediger, M. Longitudinal coupling of momentary
item content and structure. Eur. J. Personal. 29, 478–497 (2015). stress reactivity and trait neuroticism: specificity of states, traits, and age period.
25. Stieger, M. et al. Becoming more conscientious or more open to experience? Effects of J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 121, 691–706 (2021).
a two‐week smartphone‐based intervention for personality change. Eur. J. Personal. 34, 58. Hudson, N. W., Briley, D. A., Chopik, W. J. & Derringer, J. You have to follow through:
345–366 (2020). attaining behavioral change goals predicts volitional personality change. J. Pers. Soc.
26. Rosenberg, E. L. Levels of analysis and the organization of affect. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 2, Psychol. 117, 839–857 (2019).
247–270 (1998). 59. Roberts, B. W. & Wood, D. in Handbook of Personality Development (eds Mroczek, D. K. &
27. Roberts, B. W. & Pomerantz, E. M. On traits, situations, and their integration: a Little, T. D.) 11–39 (Pyschology Press, 2006).
developmental perspective. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 8, 402–416 (2004). 60. Lehnart, J., Neyer, F. J. & Eccles, J. Long-term effects of social investment: the case of
This article presents one of the most successful personality interventions. partnering in young adulthood. J. Pers. 78, 639–670 (2010).

Nature Reviews Psychology


Review article

61. Helson, R., Kwan, V. S. Y., John, O. P. & Jones, C. The growing evidence for personality 95. Fehr, E. & Schurtenberger, I. Normative foundations of human cooperation. Nat. Hum.
change in adulthood: findings from research with personality inventories. J. Res. Behav. 2, 458–468 (2018).
Personal. 36, 287–306 (2002). 96. Morris, M. W., Hong, Y., Chiu, C. & Liu, Z. Normology: integrating insights about social
62. Caspi, A. & Roberts, B. W. in Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research 2nd edn (eds norms to understand cultural dynamics. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 129, 1–13
John, O. P. & Robins, R. W.) 300–326 (Guilford Press, 1999). (2015).
63. Sarbin, T. R. The dangerous individual: an outcome of social identity transformations. Br. 97. Cialdini, R. B. & Trost, M. R. Social influence: social norms, conformity and compliance.
J. Criminol. 7, 285–295 (1967). Annu. Rev. Psychol. 55, 591–621 (1998).
64. Roberts, B. W., Wood, D. & Smith, J. L. Evaluating five factor theory and social investment 98. Dempsey, R. C., McAlaney, J. & Bewick, B. M. A critical appraisal of the social norms
perspectives on personality trait development. J. Res. Personal. 39, 166–184 (2005). approach as an interventional strategy for health-related behavior and attitude change.
65. Bollich-Ziegler, K. L., Beck, E. D., Hill, P. L. & Jackson, J. J. Do correctional facilities correct Front. Psychol. 9, 2180 (2018).
our youth?: effects of incarceration and court-ordered community service on personality 99. Hoff, K. A., Einarsdóttir, S., Chu, C., Briley, D. A. & Rounds, J. Personality changes predict
development. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 121, 894–913 (2021). early career outcomes: discovery and replication in 12-year longitudinal studies. Psychol.
66. Lücke, A. J., Quintus, M., Egloff, B. & Wrzus, C. You can’t always get what you want: Sci. 32, 64–79 (2021).
the role of change goal importance, goal feasibility and momentary experiences for 100. Sutin, A. R., Costa, P. T., Miech, R. & Eaton, W. W. Personality and career success:
volitional personality development. Eur. J. Personal. 35, 690–709 (2021). concurrent and longitudinal relations. Eur. J. Personal. 23, 71–84 (2009).
67. Gallagher, P., Fleeson, W. & Hoyle, R. A self-regulatory mechanism for personality trait 101. Bleidorn, W. & Hopwood, C. J. A motivational framework of personality development in
stability: contra-trait effort. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 2, 333–342 (2011). late adulthood. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 55, 101731 (2024).
68. Verplanken, B. & Orbell, S. Attitudes, habits, and behavior change. Annu. Rev. Psychol. This theoretical account of personality development focuses on motivational aspects
73, 327–352 (2022). to explain normative ageing.
69. Hudson, N. W., Fraley, R. C., Briley, D. A. & Chopik, W. J. Your personality does not care 102. Raison, C. L., Capuron, L. & Miller, A. H. Cytokines sing the blues: inflammation and the
whether you believe it can change: beliefs about whether personality can change do not pathogenesis of depression. Trends Immunol. 27, 24–31 (2006).
predict trait change among emerging adults. Eur. J. Personal. 35, 340–357 (2021). 103. Miller, G. E., Rohleder, N. & Cole, S. W. Chronic interpersonal stress predicts activation
70. Macnamara, B. N. & Burgoyne, A. P. Do growth mindset interventions impact students’ of pro- and anti-inflammatory signaling pathways 6 months later. Psychosom. Med. 71,
academic achievement? A systematic review and meta-analysis with recommendations 57–62 (2009).
for best practices. Psychol. Bull. 149, 133–173 (2023). 104. McEvoy, J. W. et al. Relationship of cigarette smoking with inflammation and subclinical
71. Quoidbach, J., Gilbert, D. T. & Wilson, T. D. The end of history illusion. Science 339, 96–98 vascular disease. Arterioscler. Thromb. Vasc. Biol. 35, 1002–1010 (2015).
(2013). 105. Deary, I. J. et al. Age-associated cognitive decline. Br. Med. Bull. 92, 135–152 (2009).
72. Williams, P. G., Smith, T. W., Gunn, H. E. & Uchino, B. N. in The Handbook of Stress 106. Salthouse, T. A. When does age-related cognitive decline begin? Neurobiol. Aging 30,
Science: Biology, Psychology, and Health 231–245 (Springer, 2011). 507–514 (2009).
73. Bolger, N., DeLongis, A., Kessler, R. C. & Schilling, E. A. Effects of daily stress on negative 107. Boyle, P. A. et al. Much of late life cognitive decline is not due to common
mood. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 57, 808–818 (1989). neurodegenerative pathologies. Ann. Neurol. 74, 478–489 (2013).
74. Mroczek, D. K. & Almeida, D. M. The effect of daily stress, personality, and age on daily 108. Balsis, S., Carpenter, B. D. & Storandt, M. Personality change precedes clinical diagnosis
negative affect. J. Pers. 72, 355–378 (2004). of dementia of the Alzheimer type. J. Gerontol. B 60, P98–P101 (2005).
75. Cattran, C. J., Oddy, M., Wood, R. L. & Moir, J. F. Post-injury personality in the prediction of 109. Terracciano, A., Stephan, Y., Luchetti, M., Albanese, E. & Sutin, A. R. Personality traits and
outcome following severe acquired brain injury. Brain Inj. 25, 1035–1046 (2011). risk of cognitive impairment and dementia. J. Psychiat. Res. 89, 22–27 (2017).
76. James, B. D. & Bennett, D. A. Causes and patterns of dementia: an update in the era of 110. Sala, G. et al. Near and far transfer in cognitive training: a second-order meta-analysis.
redefining Alzheimer’s disease. Annu. Rev. Public. Health 40, 65–84 (2019). Collabra Psychol. 5, https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/9efqd (2019).
77. Denissen, J. J. A., Luhmann, M., Chung, J. M. & Bleidorn, W. Transactions between life 111. Olaru, G. et al. Personality change through a digital-coaching intervention: using
events and personality traits across the adult lifespan. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 116, 612–633 measurement invariance testing to distinguish between trait domain, facet, and nuance
(2019). change. Eur. J. Personal. 38, https://doi.org/10.1177/08902070221145088 (2022).
78. Specht, J., Egloff, B. & Schmukle, S. C. Stability and change of personality across the life 112. Moreau, D. How malleable are cognitive abilities? A critical perspective on popular brief
course: the impact of age and major life events on mean-level and rank-order stability of interventions. Am. Psychol. 77, 409–423 (2022).
the Big Five. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 101, 862–882 (2011). 113. Boyd, E. M. & Fales, A. W. Reflective learning: key to learning from experience.
79. Hudson, N. W. & Roberts, B. W. Social investment in work reliably predicts change in J. Humanist. Psychol. 23, 99–117 (1983).
conscientiousness and agreeableness: a direct replication and extension of Hudson, 114. Stedmon, J. & Dallos, R. Reflective Practice in Psychotherapy and Counselling
Roberts, and Lodi-Smith (2012). J. Res. Personal. 60, 12–23 (2016). (McGraw-Hill Education, 2009).
80. Hutteman, R., Hennecke, M., Orth, U., Reitz, A. K. & Specht, J. Developmental tasks as a 115. Keefe, J. R. et al. Reflective functioning and its potential to moderate the efficacy of
framework to study personality development in adulthood and old age. Eur. J. Personal. manualized psychodynamic therapies versus other treatments for borderline personality
28, 267–278 (2014). disorder. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 91, 50–56 (2023).
81. Roberts, B. W. et al. A systematic review of personality trait change through intervention. 116. Lenhausen, M. R., Bleidorn, W. & Hopwood, C. J. Effects of reference group instructions
Psychol. Bull. 143, 117–141 (2017). on big five trait scores. Assessment 31, https://doi.org/10.1177/10731911231175850 (2023).
82. West, P. & Sweeting, H. Fifteen, female and stressed: changing patterns of psychological 117. Oltmanns, J. R., Jackson, J. J. & Oltmanns, T. F. Personality change: longitudinal self–
distress over time. J. Child. Psychol. Psychiat. 44, 399–411 (2003). other agreement and convergence with retrospective reports. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 118,
83. Koval, P. et al. Emotion regulation in everyday life: mapping global self-reports to daily 1065–1079 (2020).
processes. Emotion 23, 357–374 (2023). 118. Credé, M., Bashshur, M. & Niehorster, S. Reference group effects in the measurement of
84. Brockman, R., Ciarrochi, J., Parker, P. & Kashdan, T. Emotion regulation strategies in daily personality and attitudes. J. Pers. Assess. 92, 390–399 (2010).
life: mindfulness, cognitive reappraisal and emotion suppression. Cogn. Behav. Ther. 46, 119. Wrzus, C., Quintus, M. & Egloff, B. Age and context effects in personality development: a
91–113 (2017). multimethod perspective. Psychol. Aging 38, 1–16 (2023).
85. Rauthmann, J. F., Sherman, R. A. & Funder, D. C. Principles of situation research: towards 120. Jackson, J. J., Connolly, J. J., Garrison, S. M., Leveille, M. M. & Connolly, S. L. Your friends
a better understanding of psychological situations. Eur. J. Personal. 29, 363–381 (2015). know how long you will live: a 75-year study of peer-rated personality traits. Psychol. Sci.
86. Kuper, N. et al. Individual differences in contingencies between situation characteristics 26, 335–340 (2015).
and personality states. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 123, 1166–1198 (2022). 121. Wright, A. J. et al. Prospective self- and informant-personality associations with
87. Mischel, W. Toward an integrative science of the person. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 55, 1–22 inflammation, health behaviors, and health indicators. Health Psychol. 41, 121–133 (2022).
(2004). 122. Smith, T. W. et al. Associations of self-reports versus spouse ratings of negative
88. Beck, E. D. & Jackson, J. J. Personalized prediction of behaviors and experiences: an affectivity, dominance, and affiliation with coronary artery disease: where should we
idiographic person–situation test. Psychol. Sci. 33, 1767–1782 (2022). look and who should we ask when studying personality and health? Health Psychol. 27,
89. Barlow, D. H. et al. The unified protocol for transdiagnostic treatment of emotional 676–684 (2008).
disorders compared with diagnosis-specific protocols for anxiety disorders: a 123. Lenhausen, M., van Scheppingen, M. A. & Bleidorn, W. Self–other agreement in
randomized clinical trial. JAMA Psychiat. 74, 875–884 (2017). personality development in romantic couples. Eur. J. Personal. 35, 797–813 (2021).
90. Sauer-Zavala, S. et al. Countering emotional behaviors in the treatment of borderline 124. Rothman, A. J., Sheeran, P. & Wood, W. Reflective and automatic processes in the
personality disorder. Personal. Disord. Theory Res. Treat. 11, 328–338 (2020). initiation and maintenance of dietary change. Ann. Behav. Med. 38, s4–s17 (2009).
91. Aarts, H. & Dijksterhuis, A. Habits as knowledge structures: automaticity in goal-directed 125. Caspi, A. & Roberts, B. W. Personality development across the life course: the argument
behavior. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 78, 53–63 (2000). for change and continuity. Psychol. Inq. 12, 49–66 (2001).
92. Neal, D. T., Wood, W., Labrecque, J. S. & Lally, P. How do habits guide behavior? Perceived 126. Seger, C. A. Implicit learning. Psychol. Bull. 115, 163–196 (1994).
and actual triggers of habits in daily life. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 48, 492–498 (2012). 127. Reber, P. J. The neural basis of implicit learning and memory: a review of
93. Fehr, E. & Fischbacher, U. Social norms and human cooperation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, neuropsychological and neuroimaging research. Neuropsychologia 51, 2026–2042
185–190 (2004). (2013).
94. Bicchieri, C. The Grammar of Society: The Nature and Dynamics of Social Norms xvi, 260 128. Back, M. D. & Nestler, S. in Reflective and Impulsive Determinants of Human Behavior
(Cambridge Univ. Press, 2006). 137–154 (Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group, 2017).

Nature Reviews Psychology


Review article

129. Hofmann, W., Friese, M. & Roefs, A. Three ways to resist temptation: the independent 162. Asselmann, E. & Specht, J. Personality maturation and personality relaxation: differences
contributions of executive attention, inhibitory control, and affect regulation to the of the Big Five personality traits in the years around the beginning and ending of working
impulse control of eating behavior. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 45, 431–435 (2009). life. J. Pers. 89, 1126–1142 (2021).
130. Gassmann, D. & Grawe, K. General change mechanisms: the relation between problem 163. Lenhausen, M. R., Hopwood, C. J. & Bleidorn, W. Nature and impact of reference group
activation and resource activation in successful and unsuccessful therapeutic effects in personality assessment data. J. Pers. Assess. 105, 581–589 (2023).
interactions. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 13, 1–11 (2006). 164. Vaidya, J. G., Gray, E. K., Haig, J. R., Mroczek, D. K. & Watson, D. Differential stability and
131. Anusic, I. & Schimmack, U. Stability and change of personality traits, self-esteem, individual growth trajectories of big five and affective traits during young adulthood.
and well-being: introducing the meta-analytic stability and change model of retest J. Pers. 76, 267–304 (2008).
correlations. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 110, 766–781 (2016). 165. Haehner, P. et al. Perception of major life events and personality trait change. Eur. J.
132. Terracciano, A., McCrae, R. R. & Costa, P. T. Jr Intra-individual change in personality Personal. 37, 524–542 (2022).
stability and age. J. Res. Personal. 44, 31–37 (2010). 166. Beck, E. D. & Jackson, J. J. Detecting idiographic personality change. J. Pers. Assess. 104,
133. Jackson, J. J., Beck, E. D. & Mike, A. in Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research 467–483 (2022).
4th edn, 793–805 (The Guilford Press, 2021). 167. Jackson, J. J. & Beck, E. D. Personality development beyond the mean: do life events
This article provides an overview of interventions that attempt to change constructs shape personality variability, structure, and ipsative continuity? J. Gerontol. B 76, 20–30
related to personality. (2021).
134. Headey, B. Life goals matter to happiness: a revision of set-point theory. Soc. Indic. Res. 168. Haehner, P., Pfeifer, L. S., Fassbender, I. & Luhmann, M. Are changes in the perception of
86, 213–231 (2007). major life events associated with changes in subjective well-being? J. Res. Personal. 102,
135. Clark, A. E., Diener, E., Georgellis, Y. & Lucas, R. E. Lags and leads in life satisfaction: a test 104321 (2023).
of the baseline hypothesis. Econ. J. 118, F222–F243 (2008). 169. Goodwin, R., Polek, E. & Bardi, A. The temporal reciprocity of values and beliefs:
136. Schwaba, T. & Bleidorn, W. Personality trait development across the transition to a longitudinal study within a major life transition. Eur. J. Personal. 26, 360–370 (2012).
retirement. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 116, 651–665 (2019). 170. Zimmermann, J. & Neyer, F. J. Do we become a different person when hitting the road?
137. Wright, A. J. & Jackson, J. J. The associations between life events and person-centered Personality development of sojourners. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 105, 515–530 (2013).
personality consistency. J. Pers. 92, 162–179 (2024). 171. Jackson, J. J., Thoemmes, F., Jonkmann, K., Lüdtke, O. & Trautwein, U. Military training and
138. Asselmann, E. & Specht, J. Testing the social investment principle around childbirth: little personality trait development: does the military make the man, or does the man make
evidence for personality maturation before and after becoming a parent. Eur. J. Personal. the military? Psychol. Sci. 23, 270–277 (2012).
35, 85–102 (2020). 172. Nissen, A. T., Bleidorn, W., Ericson, S. & Hopwood, C. J. Selection and socialization effects
139. Denissen, J. J. A., Ulferts, H., Lüdtke, O., Muck, P. M. & Gerstorf, D. Longitudinal of studying abroad. J. Pers. 90, 1021–1038 (2022).
transactions between personality and occupational roles: a large and heterogeneous 173. van Agteren, J. et al. A systematic review and meta-analysis of psychological
study of job beginners, stayers, and changers. Dev. Psychol. 50, 1931–1942 (2014). interventions to improve mental wellbeing. Nat. Hum. Behav. 5, 631–652 (2021).
140. Roberts, B. W., Wood, D. & Caspi, A. in Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research 3rd 174. Weiss, L. A., Westerhof, G. J. & Bohlmeijer, E. T. Can we increase psychological
edn, 375–398 (The Guilford Press, 2008). well-being? The effects of interventions on psychological well-being: a meta-analysis of
141. Roberts, B. W. Personality development and organizational behavior. Res. Organ. Behav. randomized controlled trials. PLoS ONE 11, e0158092 (2016).
27, 1–40 (2006). 175. Jensen, S. E. et al. Cognitive–behavioral stress management and psychological
142. Schneider, B., Smith, D. B., Taylor, S. & Fleenor, J. Personality and organizations: a test of well-being in HIV+ racial/ethnic-minority women with human papillomavirus. Health
the homogeneity of personality hypothesis. J. Appl. Psychol. 83, 462–470 (1998). Psychol. 32, 227–230 (2013).
143. Caspi, A., Roberts, B. W. & Shiner, R. L. Personality development: stability and change. 176. Howells, A., Ivtzan, I. & Eiroa-Orosa, F. J. Putting the ‘app’ in happiness: a randomised
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 56, 453–484 (2005). controlled trial of a smartphone-based mindfulness intervention to enhance wellbeing.
144. Mühlig-Versen, A., Bowen, C. E. & Staudinger, U. M. Personality plasticity in later J. Happiness Stud. 17, 163–185 (2016).
adulthood: contextual and personal resources are needed to increase openness to new 177. Dwyer, R. J. & Dunn, E. W. Wealth redistribution promotes happiness. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci.
experiences. Psychol. Aging 27, 855–866 (2012). USA 119, 2211123119 (2022).
145. Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D. & Tice, D. M. The strength model of self-control. Curr. Dir. 178. White, C. A., Uttl, B. & Holder, M. D. Meta-analyses of positive psychology interventions:
Psychol. Sci. 16, 351–355 (2007). the effects are much smaller than previously reported. PLoS ONE 14, 0216588 (2019).
146. Buyalskaya, A. et al. What can machine learning teach us about habit formation? 179. Friese, M., Frankenbach, J., Job, V. & Loschelder, D. D. Does self-control training improve
Evidence from exercise and hygiene. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 120, e2216115120 self-control? A meta-analysis. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. J. Assoc. Psychol. Sci. 12, 1077–1099
(2023). (2017).
147. Jackson, J. J., Hill, P. L., Payne, B. R., Roberts, B. W. & Stine-Morrow, E. A. Can an old dog 180. Sander, J., Schmiedek, F., Brose, A., Wagner, G. G. & Specht, J. Long-term effects of an
learn (and want to experience) new tricks? Cognitive training increases openness to extensive cognitive training on personality development. J. Pers. 85, 454–463 (2017).
experience in older adults. Psychol. Aging 27, 286–292 (2012). 181. Hyun, M.-S., Chung, H.-I. C., De Gagne, J. C. & Kang, H. S. The effects of
148. Beck, E. D. & Jackson, J. J. Idiographic personality coherence: a quasi experimental cognitive-behavioral therapy on depression, anger, and self-control for Korean soldiers.
longitudinal ESM study. Eur. J. Personal. 36, 391–412 (2022). J. Psychosoc. Nurs. Ment. Health Serv. 52, 22–28 (2014).
149. Caspi, A. & Moffitt, T. E. When do individual differences matter? A paradoxical theory of 182. Stieger, M., Allemand, M. & Lachman, M. E. Effects of a digital self-control intervention
personality coherence. Psychol. Inq. 4, 247–271 (1993). to increase physical activity in middle-aged adults. J. Health Psychol. 28, 984–996
150. Vygotsky, L. S. Mind in Society: Development of Higher Psychological Processes (Harvard (2023).
Univ. Press, 1978). 183. Cerino, E. S., Hooker, K., Goodrich, E. & Dodge, H. H. Personality moderates intervention
151. Foa, E. B. & McLean, C. P. The efficacy of exposure therapy for anxiety-related disorders effects on cognitive function: a 6-week conversation-based intervention. Gerontologist
and its underlying mechanisms: the case of OCD and PTSD. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 12, 60, 958–967 (2020).
1–28 (2016). 184. LeBouthillier, D. M. & Asmundson, G. J. G. The efficacy of aerobic exercise and resistance
152. Swann Jr, W. B., Rentfrow, P. J. & Guinn, J. S. in Handbook of Self and Identity, 367–383 training as transdiagnostic interventions for anxiety-related disorders and constructs: a
(The Guilford Press, 2003). randomized controlled trial. J. Anxiety Disord. 52, 43–52 (2017).
153. Headey, B. & Wearing, A. Personality, life events, and subjective well-being: toward a 185. Barrett, E. L., Newton, N. C., Teesson, M., Slade, T. & Conrod, P. J. Adapting the
dynamic equilibrium model. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 57, 731–739 (1989). personality‐targeted preventure program to prevent substance use and associated
154. Roberts, B. W., Caspi, A. & Moffitt, T. E. Work experiences and personality development in harms among high‐risk Australian adolescents. Early Interv. Psychiat. 9, 308–315
young adulthood. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 84, 582–593 (2003). (2015).
155. Scollon, C. N. & Diener, E. Love, work, and changes in extraversion and neuroticism over 186. Fishbein, J. N., Haslbeck, J. & Arch, J. J. Network intervention analysis of anxiety-related
time. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 91, 1152–1165 (2006). outcomes and processes of acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT) for anxious
156. Lüdtke, O., Roberts, B. W., Trautwein, U. & Nagy, G. A random walk down university cancer survivors. Behav. Res. Ther. 162, (2023).
avenue: life paths, life events, and personality trait change at the transition to university 187. Bateman, A. & Fonagy, P. A randomized controlled trial of a mentalization-based
life. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 101, 620–637 (2011). intervention (MBT-FACTS) for families of people with borderline personality disorder.
157. Asselmann, E. & Specht, J. Till death do us part: transactions between losing one’s Personal. Disord. Theory Res. Treat. 10, 70–79 (2019).
spouse and the Big Five personality traits. J. Pers. 88, 659–675 (2020). 188. Sauer-Zavala, S., Wilner, J. G. & Barlow, D. H. Addressing neuroticism in psychological
158. Boyce, C. J., Wood, A. M., Daly, M. & Sedikides, C. Personality change following treatment. Personal. Disord. Theory Res. Treat. 8, 191–198 (2017).
unemployment. J. Appl. Psychol. 100, 991–1011 (2015). 189. McMurran, M., Charlesworth, P., Duggan, C. & McCarthy, L. Controlling angry aggression:
159. van Scheppingen, M. A. et al. Personality trait development during the transition to a pilot group intervention with personality disordered offenders. Behav. Cogn.
parenthood. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 7, 452–462 (2016). Psychother. 29, 473–483 (2001).
160. Gnambs, T. & Stiglbauer, B. No personality change following unemployment: a registered 190. Wells-Parker, E., Dill, P., Williams, M. & Stoduto, G. Are depressed drinking/driving
replication of Boyce, Wood, Daly, and Sedikides (2015). J. Res. Personal. 81, 195–206 offenders more receptive to brief intervention? Addict. Behav. 31, 339–350 (2006).
(2019). 191. Fisher, B. M. The mediating role of self-concept and personality dimensions on factors
161. Kammeyer-Mueller, J. D., Judge, T. A. & Piccolo, R. F. Self-esteem and extrinsic career influencing the rehabilitative treatment of violent male youthful offenders. (ProQuest
success: test of a dynamic model. Appl. Psychol. Int. Rev. 57, 204–224 (2008). Information & Learning, 2002).

Nature Reviews Psychology


Review article

192. Bailey, D. H., Duncan, G. J., Cunha, F., Foorman, B. R. & Yeager, D. S. Persistence and 218. Appenzeller, Z. Dialectical behavior therapy and schema therapy for borderline
fade-out of educational-intervention effects: mechanisms and potential solutions. personality disorder: mechanisms of change and assimilative integration. PhD thesis
Psychol. Sci. Public. Interest. 21, 55–97 (2020). (ProQuest Information & Learning, 2022).
193. Hudson, N. W. & Fraley, R. C. Volitional personality trait change: can people choose to 219. Back, M. D., Schmukle, S. C. & Egloff, B. Predicting actual behavior from the explicit and
change their personality traits? J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 109, 490–507 (2015). implicit self-concept of personality. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 97, 533–548 (2009).
194. Allemand, M. & Martin, M. On correlated change in personality. Eur. Psychol. 21, 237–253 220. Brandtstädter, J. Personal self-regulation of development: cross-sequential analyses of
(2016). development-related control beliefs and emotions. Dev. Psychol. 25, 96–108 (1989).
195. Luhmann, M., Fassbender, I., Alcock, M. & Haehner, P. A dimensional taxonomy of 221. Krampen, G. Toward an action‐theoretical model of personality. Eur. J. Personal. 2, 39–55
perceived characteristics of major life events. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 121, 633–668 (1988).
(2021). 222. Hogan, R. & Roberts, B. W. A socioanalytic model of maturity. J. Career Assess. 12,
196. Boyatzis, R. E. & Akrivou, K. The ideal self as the driver of intentional change. J. Manag. 207–217 (2004).
Dev. 25, 624–642 (2006). 223. Arkowitz, H. Toward an integrative perspective on resistance to change. J. Clin. Psychol.
197. Dweck, C. S. Can personality be changed? The role of beliefs in personality and change. 58, 219–227 (2002).
Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 17, 391–394 (2008). 224. Fleeson, W., Malanos, A. B. & Achille, N. M. An intraindividual process approach to the
198. Baumeister, R. F. in Can Personality Change? (eds Heatherton, T. F. & Weinberger, J. L.) relationship between extraversion and positive affect: is acting extraverted as ‘good’ as
281–297 (American Psychological Association, 1994). being extraverted? J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 83, 1409–1422 (2002).
199. Hudson, N. W. & Fraley, R. C. in Personality Development Across the Lifespan (ed. Specht, 225. Swann, W. B., Stein-Seroussi, A. & Giesler, R. B. Why people self-verify. J. Pers. Soc.
J.) 555–571 (Academic Press, 2017). Psychol. 62, 392–401 (1992).
200. Roberts, B. W., O’Donnell, M. & Robins, R. W. Goal and personality trait development in 226. Wood, D. & Wortman, J. Trait means and desirabilities as artifactual and real sources of
emerging adulthood. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 87, 541–550 (2004). differential stability of personality traits. J. Personality 80, 665–701 (2012).
201. Winter, D. G., John, O. P., Stewart, A. J., Klohnen, E. C. & Duncan, L. E. Traits and motives: 227. Zelenski, J. M. et al. Personality and affective forecasting: trait introverts underpredict the
toward an integration of two traditions in personality research. Psychol. Rev. 105, hedonic benefits of acting extraverted. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 104, 1092–1108 (2013).
230–250 (1998). 228. Bradley, G. W. Self-serving biases in the attribution process: a reexamination of the fact or
202. Barrick, M. R. & Mount, M. K. The big five personality dimensions and job performance: fiction question. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 36, 56–71 (1978).
a meta-analysis. Pers. Psychol. 44, 1–26 (1991). 229. Levenson, H. Differentiating among internality, powerful others, and chance. In
203. Burke, P. J. Identity change. Soc. Psychol. Q. 69, 81–96 (2006). Research with the Locus of Control Construct (ed. Lefcourt, H. M.) (Academic Press, 1981).
204. Funder, D. C. in Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research (eds John, O. P., Robins, R. W. 230. Miller, D. T. Ego involvement and attributions for success and failure. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol.
& Pervin, L. A.) 568–580 (The Guilford Press, 2008). 34, 901–906 (1976).
205. Kandler, C. & Zapko-Willmes, A. in Personality Development Across the Lifespan (ed. 231. Gunty, A. L. et al. Moderators of the relation between perceived and actual posttraumatic
Specht, J.) 101–115 (Academic Press, 2017). growth. Psychol. Trauma Theory Res. Pract. Policy 3, 61–66 (2011).
206. Lodi-Smith, J. & Roberts, B. W. Social investment and personality: a meta-analysis of the 232. Robins, R. W., Noftle, E. E., Trzesniewski, K. H. & Roberts, B. W. Do people know how their
relationship of personality traits to investment in work, family, religion, and volunteerism. personality has changed? Correlates of perceived and actual personality change in
Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 11, 68–86 (2007). young adulthood. J. Pers. 73, 489–522 (2005).
207. McAdams, D. P. & Pals, J. L. A new big five: fundamental principles for an integrative
science of personality. Am. Psychol. 61, 204–217 (2006). Author contributions
208. Kandler, C. Nature and nurture in personality development: the case of neuroticism and The authors contributed equally to all aspects of the article.
extraversion. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 21, 290–296 (2012).
209. McCrae, R. R. & Costa, P. T. in Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research Competing interests
(eds John, O. P. et al.) 150–181 (The Guilford Press, 2008). The authors declare no competing interests.
210. Bleidorn, W. et al. Personality maturation around the world: a cross-cultural examination
of social-investment theory. Psychol. Sci. 24, 2530–2540 (2013). Additional information
211. Ardichvili, A., Cardozo, R. & Ray, S. A theory of entrepreneurial opportunity identification Peer review information Nature Reviews Psychology thanks Jaap Denissen, Ted Schwaba and
and development. J. Bus. Ventur. 18, 105–123 (2003). Cornelia Wrzus for their contribution to the peer review of this work.
212. Kazdin, A. E. Behavior Modification in Applied Settings 7th edn (Waveland Press, 2012).
213. King, L. A. The hard road to the good life: the happy, mature person. J. Humanist Psychol. Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in
41, 51–72 (2001). published maps and institutional affiliations.
214. Ozbay, F. et al. Social support and resilience to stress. Psychiat. Edgmont 4, 35–40
(2007). Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this
215. Kandler, C. et al. Sources of cumulative continuity in personality: a longitudinal article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author
multiple-rater twin study. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 98, 995–1008 (2010). self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the
216. Bem, D. J. in Advances in Experimental Social Psychology Vol. 6 (ed. Berkowitz, L.) 1–62 terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.
(Academic Press, 1972).
217. Staudinger, U. M. Life reflection: a social–cognitive analysis of life review. Rev. Gen. © Springer Nature America, Inc. 2024
Psychol. 5, 148–160 (2001).

Nature Reviews Psychology

You might also like