Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 21

Get Your Science Out of Here: When Does

Invoking Science in the Marketing of


Consumer Products Backfire?

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/49/5/721/6581083 by Jnls Cust Serv on 19 January 2023


AVIVA PHILIPP-MULLER
JOHN P. COSTELLO
REBECCA WALKER RECZEK

In this research, we propose that although consumers view the scientific process
as competent, they also perceive it as cold. Across 10 experimental studies, we
demonstrate that these lay beliefs impact consumers’ reactions to marketers tout-
ing the science behind their brands. Specifically, since hedonic attributes are as-
sociated with warmth, the coldness associated with science is conceptually disflu-
ent with the anticipated warmth of hedonic products and attributes, reducing
product valuation. In contrast, when products are positioned as utilitarian, invoking
science in marketing appeals has a positive effect, as the perceived competence
of the scientific process is more compatible with the competence associated with
utilitarian products. We further demonstrate that when the necessity of science to
create a hedonic product is made salient and thus more fluent, this backfire effect
is attenuated. Finally, we identify three theoretically and practically relevant indi-
vidual differences (endorsement of the lay belief, trust in scientists, and whether
the consumer works in a STEM field) that moderate the backfire effect of pairing
science with hedonically positioned products.

Keywords: perceptions of science, science appeals, lay belief, lay theory, hedonic
versus utilitarian, marketing communications

Aviva Philipp-Muller (aviva_philipp-muller@sfu.ca) is an assistant


professor in marketing at the Beedie School of Business, Simon Fraser
University, SFU Burnaby, 888 University Drive, Burnaby, BC V5A 1S6,
B rands often tout the scientific rigor used to develop
their products in their marketing communications.
For example, the chocolate company Cadbury frequently
Canada. John P. Costello (jcostel4@nd.edu) is an assistant professor in
marketing at the Mendoza College of Business, University of Notre emphasizes the scientific processes used to create its choc-
Dame, 381 Mendoza College of Business, Notre Dame, IN 46556, USA. olate, including a commercial depicting a scientist using a
Rebecca Walker Reczek (reczek.3@osu.edu) is the Berry Chair of New centrifuge to make chocolate (Cadbury Dairy Milk 2010).
Technologies in marketing at the Fisher College of Business, The Ohio
State University, 2100 Neil Avenue, 506A Fisher Hall, Columbus, OH Other brands (e.g., Aveeno, Haus Laboratories) across a
43210, USA. Please address correspondence to Aviva Philipp-Muller. The variety of product categories, including cosmetics, personal
authors would like to thank Kelly Haws and Jesse Walker for their helpful care, food, pharmaceuticals, and technology, highlight the
comments on this research, as well as Joe Reczek for providing both in-
sight and inspiration. Supplementary materials are included in the web ap- science used to bring their products to market (see web ap-
pendix accompanying the online version of this article. pendix A for additional examples of brands highlighting
the science behind their brands). Past research has exam-
ined a variety of issues at the intersection of marketing and
science (Andre, Chandon, and Haws 2019; Hingston and
Editor: Stacy Wood Noseworthy 2018; Marinova et al. 2017; Scott and Rozin
2017; Williams and Steffel 2014), but no work, to our
Associate Editor: Lisa E. Bolton knowledge, has examined how consumers’ lay beliefs
about the scientific process might impact when marketing
Advance Access publication May 5, 2022
appeals that explicitly reference science are effective at
C The Author(s) 2022. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of Journal of Consumer Research, Inc. All rights reserved.
V
For permissions, please e-mail: journals.permissions@oup.com  Vol. 49  2023
https://doi.org/10.1093/jcr/ucac020

721
722 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

increasing consumers’ product valuation and when they Raghunathan, Naylor, and Hoyer 2006; Zane, Smith, and
backfire. Reczek 2020) by introducing a novel lay belief about a do-
Over the past four decades, Americans have generally main that is becoming more relevant as consumers increas-
viewed science as beneficial (Krause et al. 2019), but, re- ingly rely on products produced by science even while
cently, a growing number of Americans report lower trust having mixed feelings about science itself. Finally, our
in science (American Academy of Arts and Sciences 2018; work also offers practical guidance to marketers on when
Gauchat 2012). There is therefore reason to believe that and why science appeals may backfire in the marketplace.
consumers may hold mixed beliefs about the use of the sci-
entific process in bringing products to market. In this work, SCIENCE APPEALS
we propose that although consumers view the scientific

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/49/5/721/6581083 by Jnls Cust Serv on 19 January 2023


process as competent, they also view it as cold (Aaker, Over the last half-century, the invocation of science in
Vohs, and Mogilner 2010; Cuddy, Fiske, and Glick 2008). marketing communications has proliferated (Lellis 2016).
Across 10 studies, we demonstrate that the lay belief Marketers often describe how a product has been scientifi-
that science is cold but competent impacts consumers’ cally developed in their promotions, on product packaging,
reactions to the invocation of science in the marketing of and on brand websites. Of course, invoking science does
consumer packaged goods (CPGs). We propose that be- not always prove effective, and past research in marketing
cause hedonic products are associated with warmth suggests it can backfire. For example, research has shown
(Chattalas and Takada 2013), the coldness of the scientific that reference to the use of genetic modification in food
process is conceptually disfluent with hedonic products products (including packaged food products) can elicit
specifically, reducing product valuation, consistent with moral opposition and consumer resistance (Hingston and
past research on conceptual fluency (Duhachek, Agrawal, Noseworthy 2018; Pham and Mandel 2019; Scott et al.
and Han 2012; Kidwell, Farmer, and Hardesty 2013; Lee 2018; Zheng et al. 2019). A related but distinct stream of
and Labroo 2004). We also identify a number of boundary research shows reduced purchase intentions for products
conditions for this effect. First, when products are posi- that consumers view as unnatural (Scott, Rozin, and Small
tioned as utilitarian, science appeals are more effective 2020; Scott et al. 2018). While both these streams of re-
than appeals that do not mention science. This occurs be-
search suggest that invoking science in marketing appeals
cause the scientific process is compatible with the compe-
can potentially be problematic for marketers, our work is
tence associated with utilitarian products. Second, we
distinct from this work in two ways.
introduce an intervention to eliminate the backfire effect:
First, while there is a rich literature in marketing that
By increasing the salience of science in a product category
explores consumer response to product claims that are ex-
where it is not naturally salient, science appeals are made
plicitly based on science, this literature explores consumer
more conceptually fluent and hence no longer backfire for
response to the specific information in the claim (e.g., that
hedonically positioned products. Finally, because the con-
ceptual disfluency of pairing science with hedonically po- a functional food offers a particular health benefit in
sitioned products stems from the lay belief that science is Naylor, Droms, and Haws 2009 or that a product can en-
cold, we show that, among those who do not subscribe to hance a central vs. peripheral individual trait in Riis,
the lay belief, science appeals do not backfire when paired Simmons, and Goodwin 2008) or to the specific scientific
with hedonic products. process used to make the product (e.g., genetic modifica-
From a theoretical perspective, our primary contribution tion in Hingston and Noseworthy 2018; Pham and Mandel
lies in documenting consumer response to the invocation 2019; Scott et al. 2018; Zheng et al. 2019). We are instead
of science in marketing appeals. Although prior literature interested in exploring the effectiveness of marketing
in marketing has explored reference to specific scientific appeals that do not necessarily make a specific scientific
processes in marketing appeals (e.g., genetic engineering; claim or reference a specific scientific process but instead
Hingston and Noseworthy 2018; Zheng, Bolton, and Alba associate the brand/product with science more broadly.
2019), no prior research, to our knowledge, has explored That is, we test the effect of making the claim that some as-
how consumers respond when brands reference the use of pect of the product was developed using science versus
science or the scientific process more broadly as part of a promoting the same aspect of the product with no reference
marketing appeal. Our research therefore addresses a novel to science. Second, in the work exploring consumer re-
question to the consumer behavior literature: How do con- sponse to natural versus unnatural products, a natural prod-
sumers respond to brands that invoke science in the mar- uct is defined as a product that has not had “any previous
keting of consumer products? We answer this question and human intervention and has no additives” (Scott et al.
reveal the consequences of consumers’ lay beliefs about 2020, 454). In our research, we focus exclusively on manu-
the scientific process for brands that do so. We also con- factured consumer products (i.e., CPGs that would not ex-
tribute to the literature on consumer lay theories (Cheng, ist without human intervention). In other words, we
Mukhopadhyay, and Schrift 2017; Deval et al. 2013; explore the effects of invoking science in marketing
PHILIPP-MULLER, COSTELLO, AND RECZEK 723

products that the research on perceived artificiality of prod- 2006). In fact, past research has shown that when a prod-
ucts would define as inherently “unnatural” because they uct’s attributes conflict with a lay belief about the relation-
are produced using human intervention. Thus, we are inter- ship between those attributes, consumers engage in
ested in the backfiring of science appeals beyond reasons increased information search because the information is
of unnaturalness (although we empirically rule out artifici- discrepant from what they expect (Haws et al. 2017). We
ality as an alternative explanation for the effects we ob- propose that the lay belief that science is cold yet compe-
serve in our studies). tent creates expectations that can either match or mismatch
with products that are positioned as hedonic or utilitarian.
CONSUMER LAY BELIEFS ABOUT THE Consumers tend to associate hedonic brands, products, and
SCIENTIFIC PROCESS attributes (which focus on positive sensory and affective

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/49/5/721/6581083 by Jnls Cust Serv on 19 January 2023


experiences) with higher levels of warmth, whereas they
To understand consumer response to marketing appeals associate utilitarian brands, products, and attributes (which
that explicitly invoke science, we explore consumers’ lay focus on instrumental outcomes and fulfilling a particular
beliefs regarding the scientific process. Lay beliefs (i.e., function) with higher levels of competence (Chattalas and
knowledge structures that reflect an individual’s naı̈ve un- Takada 2013; Dhar and Wertenbroch 2000; Hirschman and
derstanding of the world) have been shown to affect a vari- Holbrook 1982; Peter and Ponzi 2018; Strahilevitz and
ety of product judgments in past research (Cheng et al. Myers 1998). We therefore propose that products posi-
2017; Deval et al. 2013; Raghunathan et al. 2006; Wang, tioned as hedonic mismatch with consumers’ expectations
Tat Keh, and Bolton 2010), even when these beliefs are not that science is cold. It is important to note that this mis-
objectively accurate (Haws, Walker Reczek, and Sample match may not be obvious to marketers, as science is still
2017; Kramer and Block 2011). We focus on consumers’ commonly used to promote hedonic products in the mar-
lay beliefs about the scientific process. ketplace. Indeed, a pilot study we conducted with 208
Though no work, to our knowledge, has focused on lay MTurk workers (Mage: 38.4, 48.10% female) found that
beliefs regarding the scientific process, research examining 79% of participants had personally encountered science
stereotypes about scientists has found that scientists are appeals for hedonic products (see web appendix C for
perceived as high in agency but low in communion (Brown details).
et al. 2018; Diekman et al. 2011; McPherson, Park, and Ito This sense of feeling like there is a mismatch between
2018).1 Prior research in marketing has demonstrated that science appeals and products positioned as hedonic can be
warmth and competence, identified as the two broad cate- more formally labeled as a lack of conceptual fluency.
gories under which most stereotypes about people and Conceptual fluency2 has been defined in the consumer be-
groups fall (Cuddy et al. 2008), apply not only to person havior literature as the extent to which stimuli are consis-
perception but also to nonhuman entities like organizations tent with one’s expectations (e.g., using an image of people
(Aaker et al. 2010; Lee, Bolton, and Winterich 2017). We in a bar when promoting a beer is conceptually fluent be-
therefore suspect that the scientific process is likely per- cause we expect to see beer in a bar; Lee and Labroo 2004)
ceived similarly to the scientists working in that domain, and has been shown to increase a message’s persuasive
such that the scientific process will be perceived as cold, power (Duhachek et al. 2012; Kidwell et al. 2013;
yet competent. As such, we propose that because consum- Thompson and Hamilton 2006). In the present work, we
ers have the lay belief that science is cold, they will de- predict that since consumers have the lay belief that sci-
value products they expect to be warm when science is ence is competent but not warm, when they encounter sci-
invoked. ence paired with a hedonic product, it does not match their
expectations and is hence conceptually disfluent.
(MIS)MATCH BETWEEN SCIENCE Conceptual disfluency occurs when there is a mismatch
APPEALS AND HEDONIC PRODUCTS with expectations. For example, when an organic label is
applied to a vice food, that label is perceived to be at odds
Lay beliefs are a form of knowledge structure that can with the product category and is therefore less conceptually
serve as a source of product expectations that affect pur- fluent compared to when the same label is applied to a vir-
chase intentions (Luchs et al. 2010; Raghunathan et al. tue food (Parker et al. 2021). Just as conceptual fluency
increases product valuation, conceptual disfluency reduces
1 We note that stereotypes center on the trait associations individuals
have about people or entities and are hence a type of lay belief. The
product valuation (Lee and Labroo 2004; Parker et al.
term “lay belief” is therefore a broader term than stereotype since lay
beliefs can be about anything (e.g., products, processes, etc.) and not 2 Conceptual fluency is based on a stimulus’ meaning and the expect-
just about trait associations regarding people or entities. However, the ations one has about that stimulus. In contrast, perceptual fluency is
terms “lay belief” and “stereotype” are often used interchangeably in based on perceptual elements (such as appearance). For example, an
the psychology literature when discussing beliefs about people’s traits ad would be perceptually (but not conceptually) disfluent if it used a
(Krems et al. 2021). font that was difficult to read.
724 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

2021), similar to the dampening effects on product evalua- scientific process is cold yet competent and the expectation
tions when something does not “feel right” (e.g., a non- that hedonic products are warm. This theorizing suggests
rounded number in Wadhwa and Zhang 2015). two moderators for this backfire effect: (1) altering the
Thus, when science is invoked to market warm, hedonic associations consumers have with a specific hedonic prod-
products, we predict consumers should be less interested in uct and (2) altering belief in the science is cold yet compe-
those products.3 On the other hand, when science is in- tent lay belief. We first focus on altering associations
voked to market utilitarian products, conceptual fluency consumers have with hedonic products. In our experiments,
should be increased because the functional, competent ori- we focus on product categories where science is not natu-
entation of utilitarian products is consistent with the lay be- rally highly salient, as these are the products for which we
lief that science is competent, resulting in increased predict science appeals will backfire. Specifically, we fo-

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/49/5/721/6581083 by Jnls Cust Serv on 19 January 2023


product valuation. Our predictions thus share some com- cus on CPGs.4 However, there are many hedonic, yet
monality with past work on persuasion that has found that highly technical product categories where we would expect
when promotional appeals use language that matches fea- the role of the physical sciences or engineering in produc-
tures of the product, consumers tend to be more persuaded tion to be highly salient. For example, most consumers pre-
and likely to purchase that product (e.g., utilitarian vs. he- sumably understand that finely engineered shock absorbers
donic appeals are more successful when promoting utilitar- are required for a smooth ride in a vehicle. We would
ian products; Chitturi, Raghunathan, and Mahajan 2008; therefore not expect consumers to balk at a hedonic sports
Klein and Melnyk 2016). This type of direct matching also car that was developed by engineers using a scientific pro-
likely enhances conceptual fluency. The primary difference cess: When it is highly salient to consumers that science is
in our conceptualization relative to this existing work is necessary to create a given hedonic product, science
that, while past work has shown dampened product valua- appeals should match consumers’ expectations.
tions from direct mismatches between the appeal and the However, even for a CPG where science is not naturally
product, we are proposing that expectations stemming salient, we predict that by increasing the salience of the ne-
from the lay belief that science is cold yet competent indi- cessity of science to create a given hedonic product, the
rectly produce the mismatch and sense of disfluency that conceptual fluency of science appeals can be increased.
drives our effect. Additionally, that past work has not Providing information indicating, for example, that a given
looked at when science mismatches with products. More hedonic CPG cannot be produced without science, should
formally, we predict the following: mitigate the backfire effect predicted in hypothesis 1a by
associating science with that product and hence making
H1a: When a product is positioned as hedonic, mentioning science appeals more expected and hence more conceptu-
the use of science in marketing appeals will reduce product ally fluent. Such findings would provide additional evi-
valuation relative to not invoking science. dence for our proposed mechanism by demonstrating
H1b: When a product is positioned as utilitarian, mention- process via moderation (Spencer, Zanna, and Fong 2005).
ing the use of science in marketing appeals will increase More formally, we therefore predict:
product valuation, relative to not invoking science.
H2: The effect of appeal type on product valuation proposed H3: If the need for science when creating a given hedonic
in hypothesis 1 will be mediated by the appeal’s conceptual product is made salient to consumers, the backfire effect for
fluency. hedonic products proposed in hypothesis 1a will be
attenuated.

MODERATING THE SCIENCE BACKFIRE


Individual Differences Moderating the Science
EFFECT
Backfire Effect
The Salience of Science in Developing In addition to altering the associations consumers have
Consumer Products with a hedonic product, the science backfire effect can be
According to our theorizing, the science backfire effect mitigated if consumers do not see science as cold.
occurs due to a mismatch between the lay belief that the Although it may be difficult for marketers to alter the lay
belief that science is cold yet competent, consumers can
3 To manipulate whether a product is perceived as hedonic or utilitar-
ian in our studies, we highlight either a hedonic or utilitarian attribute 4 Although science is widely used to develop many CPGs like pack-
of the target product while holding the product constant (e.g., for a aged foods, beverages, and personal care products (Goddard and
shampoo one could highlight its cleansing efficacy [utilitarian] vs. Gruber 1999; Singh and Singh 2005), we suspected (and showed in a
silky feel [hedonic]; Dhar and Wertenbroch 2000). While a given pilot study on MTurk; details in web appendix C) that the use of sci-
product may have both utilitarian and hedonic attributes, making a ence in these product categories is not as salient to consumers as it is
specific attribute focal in a marketing appeal influences consumers’ in product categories seen as more technical (e.g., cars, electronic
holistic evaluation of the product as hedonic or utilitarian. devices, prescription drugs).
PHILIPP-MULLER, COSTELLO, AND RECZEK 725

differ in the extent to which they naturally endorse a given share (study 1a) and willingness to pay (WTP) (study 1b)
lay belief (Cheng et al. 2017; Mukhopadhyay and Johar are reduced when a hedonic product is promoted with a sci-
2005; Raghunathan et al. 2006). We propose that individu- ence appeal (vs. a control appeal with no mention of sci-
als who do not explicitly endorse the lay belief that science ence). Study 1c further demonstrates that the backfire
is cold will not devalue hedonic products that invoke sci- effect we observe is uniquely driven by referencing sci-
ence since, without this belief, science appeals will not be ence, as an appeal referencing rigor (but not science) in a
conceptually disfluent. Such a finding would also provide product development process does not produce the same
further evidence for our proposed process. We therefore negative effect on product valuation. Study 2 provides evi-
predict: dence that the science backfire effect for hedonic products
is driven by conceptual disfluency (hypothesis 2). Studies
H4: Those who do not hold the lay belief that science is

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/49/5/721/6581083 by Jnls Cust Serv on 19 January 2023


3a and 3b test both hypotheses 1a and 1b in the context of
cold should not exhibit the effect described in hypothesis
1a.
personal lubricant (study 3a) and body wash (study 3b),
demonstrating that, although science (vs. control) appeals
In addition to explicitly measuring belief in the lay the- backfire when the product is positioned as hedonic, science
ory to test this hypothesis, we also test it indirectly with (vs. control) appeals lead to increased purchase intentions
two other factors that theoretically ought to impact the ex- when the product is positioned as utilitarian. The results of
tent to which individuals hold this lay belief. Specifically, studies 3a and 3b also show evidence of moderated media-
we explore whether participants’ trust in scientists5 and tion via conceptual fluency. Consistent with hypothesis 3,
whether the individual works in a STEM (Science, in study 4, we demonstrate that when the salience of the
Technology, Engineering, and Mathemathics) field moder- necessity of science to produce a hedonic product is in-
ate the science backfire effect described in hypothesis 1a. creased, a science appeal does not reduce purchase inten-
Past work finds that trust is highly related to perceptions of tions. Finally, we test hypothesis 4 in studies 5a–5c,
warmth (Fiske, Cuddy, and Glick 2007), and the warmth/ demonstrating that those who do not see science as cold
coldness stereotype derives from initial perceptions of (study 5a), who trust scientists a great deal (study 5b), or
trustworthiness (Williams and Bargh 2008). Therefore, we who work in a STEM field (study 5c) do not exhibit the
expect that the backfire effect will attenuate for consumers science backfire effect. Full stimuli for each study are
with high levels of trust in scientists, as they should be less reported in web appendix B, as are additional details
likely to believe that science is cold. We also examine around data exclusions. Inclusion of all responses does not
whether the effect attenuates for individuals who work in a change the significance of our findings in any studies (web
scientific industry sector (i.e., a STEM field), as these indi- appendix D).
viduals should ether be scientists themselves or have more
experience with science and scientists. Past work on con-
tact theory (Allport 1954) suggests that when people work LAY BELIEF PILOT STUDY
with members of different groups in a collaborative con- A total of 312 MTurk workers (Mage: 40.60, 51.60% fe-
text, they are less likely to stereotype them. We therefore male) completed this pilot study in exchange for monetary
predict that individuals working in STEM industries should compensation. Participants were presented with a brief de-
not hold the same stereotypes (or lay beliefs) about scien- scription of both warmth and competence adapted from
tists and the scientific process as lay people and thus existing warmth-competence research in marketing and
should not show the same backfire effect for hedonic prod- psychology (Aaker, Garbinsky, and Vohs 2012; Aaker
ucts that involve science. et al. 2010; Judd et al. 2005) and then rated the scientific
process in reference to the production of consumer goods
OVERVIEW OF STUDIES on two nine-point semantic differential scales anchored by
“4, Cold” and “þ4, Warm,” and “4, Incompetent” and
We first report the results of a pilot study directly assess-
“þ4, Competent,” respectively. They then provided demo-
ing consumers’ lay beliefs about the scientific process,
graphic information (e.g., age, gender) and answered an
which demonstrates that, among the general population, it
open-ended data validity indicator used to identify bots and
is viewed as competent, but cold. Studies 1a, 1b, and 1c
individuals using virtual private servers (Chmielewski and
then test hypothesis 1a, demonstrating that actual choice
Kucker 2020; Dennis, Goodson, and Pearson 2020). A re-
5 We measure trust in scientists (rather than trust in the scientific pro-
peated measures ANOVA revealed that, as predicted, the
cess) because it allows us to utilize established measures from a na- scientific process was more associated with competence
tionally administered Pew Research Center poll about Americans’ (M ¼ 3.03) than warmth (M ¼ 1.26; F(1, 311) ¼ 131.57,
trust in scientists. Though technically distinct constructs, we conjec- p < .001, gp2 ¼ 0.30). Further, the mean evaluation of the
ture that perceptions of scientists and the scientific process are related.
We elaborate on this issue and potential sources of the lay belief in scientific process on the competence/incompetence dimen-
more depth in the General Discussion. sion was higher than the midpoint of the scale (M ¼ 3.03,
726 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

F(1, 311) ¼ 1753.10, p < .001, gp2 ¼ 0.58), while the condition where the description of Option A included a ref-
warmth/coldness of the scientific process was significantly erence to science, Option A was chosen significantly less
below the midpoint (M ¼ 1.26, F(1, 311) ¼ 76.74, p < often compared to the control condition (21.18% vs.
.001, gp2 ¼ 0.06). Consistent with our theorizing, these 30.47%; b ¼ .25, 2(1) ¼ 5.71, p ¼ .017, u ¼ 0.11). In
results suggest that consumers view the scientific process terms of percentage change [(Science  Control)/Control],
as competent but cold. adding a science appeal decreased the likelihood of choos-
ing cookie Option A by 30.48%. Thus, study 1a shows that
STUDIES 1A–1C a science appeal significantly reduces consumers’ choice
of the cookie.
Study 1a

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/49/5/721/6581083 by Jnls Cust Serv on 19 January 2023


In this study, we tested hypothesis 1a by examining par- Study 1b
ticipants’ real choice of a hedonic chocolate chip cookie. A Study 1b (preregistration available at https://aspredicted.
total of 511 undergraduate students (Mage: 19.95, 51.72% org/blind.php?x=c6hy7w) conceptually replicated study 1a
female) completed study 1a in exchange for course credit.6 using another measure of product valuation, incentive com-
Participants were assigned to one of two conditions in patible WTP. A total of 809 MTurk workers (Mage: 40.51,
which they were told that the researchers had partnered 53.40% female) completed study 1b in exchange for mone-
with a baked goods company to examine interest in differ- tary compensation. Participants were assigned to one of
ent cookies. Participants were told they would receive the two conditions (Appeal Type: Control vs. Science) in
cookie they selected. All participants received a pen and a which they read a slogan for an indulgent chocolate chip
paper menu describing three cookie options that varied cookie. Participants were told that the cookies were from a
depending on condition. In the control condition, the three real cookie brand that we dubbed “Zoza” due to the brand’s
options were: “Option A—Luscious chocolatey taste,” purported request to remain anonymous. All participants
“Option B—Our Most scrumptious cookie,” and “Option saw an image of an indulgent cookie and then read a slogan
C—Loads of ooey-gooey chocolate chunks.” In the science that varied by condition. In the science appeal condition,
condition, the options were identical except the first cookie participants read, “Our rigorous scientific development
was listed as “Option A—Scientifically developed to have process ensures that Zoza cookies taste delicious, indul-
a luscious chocolatey taste.” A pretest (n ¼ 202 undergrad- gent, and gooey,” and, in the control condition, participants
uates, Mage: 21.56, 40.87% female) revealed that partici- read, “We ensure that Zoza cookies taste delicious, indul-
pants predominantly see chocolate chip cookies described gent, and gooey.”
as having a “luscious chocolatey taste” (the target attribute After viewing the slogan, participants reported their
across both conditions) as a hedonic (vs. utilitarian) prod- WTP for a case of cookies from Zoza on a sliding scale
uct (M ¼ 1.90, significantly different from the midpoint of ranging from $0 to $20. We made this WTP measure in-
the seven-point scale where 1 ¼ predominantly hedonic centive compatible by employing a procedure used in prior
and 7 ¼ predominantly utilitarian; F(1, 201) ¼ 483.56, p < research (Costello and Walker Reczek 2020; Fuchs,
.001, see web appendix C for reporting of manipulation Schreier, and van Osselaer 2015) that involves an initial
checks and pretests for all other studies). Participants then lottery followed by the standard BDM procedure (Becker,
selected which cookie they wanted and reported their de- DeGroot, and Marschak 1964). Participants were told that
mographic information. At the end of the study session, if chosen from the lottery, they would get the opportunity
participants went to a separate room where they presented to purchase a case of 30 packs of cookies (each pack was
a research assistant with the menu on which they had cir- described as containing three cookies, for a total of 90
cled their choice of cookie. They then received a cookie cookies per case) and that the winner of this lottery would
from a labeled container that corresponded with their be subject to the BDM procedure based on their stated
selection. WTP. Participants next provided demographic information
To test hypothesis 1a, we conducted binary logistic re- and answered two open-ended data validity indicators.
gression with condition (coded as control ¼ 1; scien- Finally, participants provided contact information in case
ce ¼ 1) predicting whether participants selected cookie they were selected as winners. The selected winners then
Option A or not (Option A ¼ 1; Option B or C ¼ 0). In the received their prizes based on the outlined procedure.
A one-way ANOVA predicting incentive-compatible
6 The study was conducted simultaneously at two large, Midwestern
WTP from appeal type revealed that participants were will-
universities in the United States using similar populations (undergrad- ing to pay less for the cookies in the science appeal (M ¼
uate business students). We made the choice to collect data at both $9.04) than the control condition (M ¼ $9.97; F (1, 807) ¼
institutions over the same time period to achieve adequate statistical 6.04, p ¼ .014, gp2 ¼ 0.01). In percentage change terms,
power despite ongoing challenges around participant recruitment due
to the COVID-19 pandemic. We do not observe any interaction be- including a science appeal decreased WTP by 9.33%.
tween experimental condition and data collection location (p ¼ .870). Thus, study 1b conceptually replicated the findings of
PHILIPP-MULLER, COSTELLO, AND RECZEK 727

study 1a with a different operationalization of product val- appeal referencing science does so. Additionally, this study
uation, demonstrating that consumers are willing to pay provides evidence that the science backfire effect goes be-
less for a case of hedonic cookies when they are promoted yond cognitive/affective matching because simply appeal-
using a science appeal. ing to a cognitive attribute (i.e., a rigorous development
process) was insufficient to yield a backfire effect com-
Study 1c pared to the control condition.
The goal of study 1c was to demonstrate that the back-
fire effect for hedonic positioning is unique to explicitly STUDY 2
referencing science (vs. a rigorous development process
In study 2 (preregistration https://aspredicted.org/blind.
that does not invoke science) because it is driven by

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/49/5/721/6581083 by Jnls Cust Serv on 19 January 2023


php?x=n4te2g), we test hypotheses 1a and 2, this time in
expectations stemming from the lay belief that science is
the context of a hedonic smoothie rather than cookies. We
cold. Going beyond the existing affective/cognitive match-
measured conceptual fluency in addition to several other
ing literature (Fabrigar and Petty 1999; Klein and Melnyk
potential mediators to rule out alternative explanations.
2016), we therefore sought to demonstrate that it is not
Specifically, because hedonic products that are scientifi-
simply the case that any cognitive appeal would backfire
cally developed may be perceived to be more artificial
for a hedonically positioned product simply because the
(Scott and Rozin 2017), we measured perceived artificial-
product is seen as more affective in nature. Thus, we tested
ity using established measures from the literature. Further,
whether a cognitive appeal that referenced the product’s
because scientifically developed food products may be
rigorous development process would backfire or whether
seen as healthier, more conducive to weight loss, less tasty
there was something unique about the coldness that scien-
(Andre et al. 2019), or less plausible, we also measured
tific cognitive appeals confer.
these perceptions.
A total of 587 MTurk workers (Mage: 35.38, 48.04% fe-
male) were presented with a description of an indulgent
cookie brand. Participants were presented with one of three
Participants and Procedure
appeals promoting the brand’s cookies: control (“Our prod- A total of 402 participants (Mage ¼ 41.02, 50.00% fe-
uct creation process ensures that Zoza cookies taste deli- male) completed study 2 on MTurk in exchange for pay-
cious, indulgent, and gooey”), nonscience rigor (“Our ment. All participants saw an image of an indulgent
rigorous development process ensures that Zoza cookies smoothie brand (named “JTB smoothies”) prior to seeing
taste delicious, indulgent, and gooey”), and science (“Our the ad copy. A pretest (n ¼ 101 MTurk workers) confirmed
science-based rigorous development process ensures that that the smoothie was viewed as hedonic (see web appen-
Zoza cookies taste delicious, indulgent, and gooey”). dix C for details). In the science appeal condition, partici-
Participants then reported their purchase intentions using pants read “Our rigorous scientific development process
the following scale: “Based on the slogan above, how ensures that JTB smoothies taste delicious, indulgent, and
likely would you be to purchase Zoza cookies?” (1— creamy,” and, in the control condition, participants read,
Extremely Unlikely; 7 ¼ Extremely Likely). Finally, par- “We ensure that JTB smoothies taste delicious, indulgent,
ticipants reported demographic information. and creamy.”
To test whether appealing to rigor would yield the same All participants then reported their intentions to pur-
backfire effect as appealing to scientific rigor specifically, chase the smoothie and the conceptual fluency of the slo-
we employed orthogonal contrast coding, which allowed gan on the following five items (a ¼ 0.93) adapted from
us to test our focal predictions directly (Rosenthal, past work (Kidwell et al. 2013; Lee and Aaker 2004;
Rosnow, and Rubin 2000). We compared the science con- Rocklage and Fazio 2020): “The slogan seemed to flow
dition (coded as þ2) to the rigorous and control conditions well,” “The slogan seemed disjointed,” “Something
(both coded as 1). We also examined the rigorous condi- seemed weird about the slogan (1 ¼ completely disagree,
tion (coded as 1) compared to the control condition (coded 7 ¼ completely agree, reverse coded for the latter two
as 1). As predicted, purchase intentions were signifi- items), and the extent to which the slogan was fitting for
cantly lower in the science appeal (M ¼ 4.02) compared to the product (1 ¼ not fitting at all, 7 ¼ completely fitting,)
the control (M ¼ 4.58) and nonscience rigor conditions and a good match for the product (1 ¼ The slogan was not
(M ¼ 4.34; F (1, 584) ¼ 10.60, p ¼ .001, gp2 ¼ 0.02). a good match at all, 7 ¼ The slogan was a very good
Purchase intentions were not significantly different be- match). Unless otherwise specified, all measures in subse-
tween the nonscience rigor and control conditions (F (1, quent studies used seven-point scales where higher num-
584) ¼ 2.23, p ¼ .136, gp2 ¼ 0.004). Thus, study 1c dem- bers indicated greater agreement with the item.
onstrates that it is not the case that referencing any type of Participants also rated perceived artificiality using three
rigorous development process in an appeal for a hedonic items taken from existing research (Rozin 2005; Scott and
product produces a backfire effect, but, rather, only an Rozin 2017; a ¼ 0.89): “To what extent do you think JTB
728 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

smoothies are artificial?,”7 “How natural do you think JTB 0.01]). To test the relative indirect effect size of taste ver-
smoothies are?” (reverse coded), and “To what extent do sus fluency in our parallel mediation model, we also con-
you think JTB smoothies have additives?”). Finally, we ducted a pairwise comparison using the “contrast”
measured the extent to which participants thought the command through the PROCESS macro (Hayes 2017).
smoothie was tasty, healthy, and conducive to weight loss When the indirect effects of taste and fluency were directly
(from Andre et al. 2019), as well as whether the slogan was contrasted, fluency mediated to a significantly greater de-
plausible, that is it “seem[ed] like a real slogan companies gree (95% CI: [0.53, 0.24]). See web appendix D for a
might use” (1 ¼ “Not at all”; 7 ¼ “Very much so”). We full reporting of means, ANOVAs, and mediation analysis
measured plausibility because we propose that the effect of for these additional variables.
invoking science on product valuation is driven by the con-
Discussion. Study 2 provides support for both hypothe-

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/49/5/721/6581083 by Jnls Cust Serv on 19 January 2023


ceptual fluency of the appeal (i.e., how much the appeal
ses 1a and 2. The results of study 2 also demonstrated that
matches consumers’ expectations) and not the extent to
fluency significantly mediated the effect of appeal type on
which participants thought a brand would never use a cer-
purchase intentions even when controlling for artificiality
tain type of appeal. Participants then answered demo-
and other potential mediators, thus ruling out alternative
graphic questions and two data validity indicators. No
explanations for the effect.
participants failed our data validity indicators, leaving 402
valid responses.
STUDY 3A
Results and Discussion Study 3a tested hypotheses 1a, 1b, and 2 in a new non-
Purchase Intentions. We subjected the data to a one- consumable product category, personal lubricant, and ex-
way ANOVA comparing the two conditions (science vs. tended the conceptual fluency mediation findings of study
control) on purchase intentions. Conceptually replicating 2 to a moderated mediation context. This study examined
the findings of studies 1a–1c we found that, compared to the impact of appeal type (science vs. control) and product
the control condition (M ¼ 5.09), those in the science con- positioning (hedonic vs. utilitarian) on purchase intentions
dition (M ¼ 4.37) reported reduced purchase intentions for and measured perceived brand trustworthiness and brand
the smoothie (F(1, 400) ¼ 19.06, p < .001, gp2 ¼ 0.05). competence as alternative explanations.
Conceptual Fluency Mediation. We ran a parallel me-
diation analysis using model 4 of PROCESS (Hayes 2017) Participants and Procedure
to test the mechanisms driving the effect of appeal type on Participants in study 3a were MTurk workers who com-
purchase intentions. We set appeal type as the independent pleted the study in exchange for monetary compensation.
variable (control ¼ 1, science ¼ 1), purchase intentions as In one of the conditions, the appeal described the lubricant
the dependent variable, and fluency, plausibility, artificial- as possessing a utilitarian attribute (adding a layer of birth
ity, tastiness, healthiness, and conduciveness to weight loss control), which may not be desired or necessary for a siz-
as parallel mediators8 (figure 1). The indirect effect of ap- able proportion of people. Therefore, participants were
peal type on purchase intentions through fluency was sig- only invited to complete the main survey if they reported
nificant (indirect effect ¼ 0.42, 95% CI: [0.57, 0.29]), that they used birth control (“Do you or your romantic part-
as predicted in hypothesis 2, even while controlling for the ner ever use any kind of product that provides birth control
alternative mediators. The indirect effects were not signifi- (e.g., condoms, birth control pill etc.)?”) on a brief screener
cant for perceived artificiality (indirect effect ¼ 0.03, 95% survey in which the birth control question was embedded
CI: [0.01, 0.07]); weight loss potential (indirect effect ¼ among other decoy questions (see web appendix B for
0.02, 95% CI: [0.001, 0.05]); healthiness (indirect effect complete screener item wordings). Participants determined
¼ 0.001, 95% CI: [0.02, 0.03]); or appeal plausibility (in- to be eligible based on their responses were invited to the
direct effect ¼ 0.05, 95% CI: [0.13, 0.03]). Taste signif- main study. In total, 900 participants (Mage: 38.11, 61.09%
icantly mediated, though the effect was much weaker than female) completed the study. Participants were excluded if
that of fluency (indirect effect ¼ 0.04, 95% CI: [0.09, they failed a data validity indicator (31 participants) and/or
stated that they did not actually use birth control in the
7 This item was anchored on a scale from 0 to 6 to remain consistent main study despite stating that they or their romantic part-
with the original item from Rozin (2005). However, we recoded the
responses from 1 to 7 prior analysis and combining with the other two ner used birth control in the original screener (49 partici-
artificiality measures. pants). An additional participant’s data were excluded
8 The results are robust to parallel analyses wherein each alternative because they took the survey twice. The final sample size
explanation is individually contrasted to fluency (i.e., two mediators in was therefore 823.
each analysis). Fluency mediates to a significantly greater degree than
each of the alternative mediators (see web appendix D for these Participants were presented with an appeal promoting a
analyses). personal lubricant (named “Noma”) in a 2 (Appeal:
PHILIPP-MULLER, COSTELLO, AND RECZEK 729

FIGURE 1

PARALLEL MEDIATION MODEL.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/49/5/721/6581083 by Jnls Cust Serv on 19 January 2023


*p < .10, **p < .05

Science vs. Control)  2 (Positioning: Hedonic vs. they or their romantic partner used birth control, completed
Utilitarian) between-subjects design. In one between- a data validity indicator, and responded to demographic
subjects factor, half the participants were told to imagine questions and several exploratory measures (reported in
they had the goal of finding a lubricant that enhanced sex- web appendix B).
ual pleasure (the hedonic condition), and the other half
were told to imagine they were searching for a lubricant Results and Discussion
that would add an additional layer of birth control protec-
tion (the utilitarian condition). Participants in the hedonic Purchase Intentions. We subjected the data to a two-
condition then saw an appeal that positioned the lubricant way ANOVA predicting purchase intentions from appeal
as hedonic (increasing sexual pleasure), whereas partici- type (control ¼ 1, science ¼ 1), product positioning
pants in the utilitarian condition saw an appeal that posi- (hedonic ¼ 1, utilitarian ¼ 1), and the two-way interac-
tioned the lubricant as utilitarian (increasing birth control tion. There was no main effect of appeal type (F(1, 819) ¼
protection). In the other between-subjects factor, half the 0.70, p ¼ .402, gp2 ¼ 0.001), but there was a main effect
participants saw an appeal that invoked science, and the of product positioning (F(1, 819) ¼ 6.69, p ¼.01, gp2 ¼
other half saw a control appeal that did not mention sci- 0.01), such that when the lubricant was positioned as utili-
ence. Participants then reported their likelihood of purchas- tarian, purchase intentions were higher (M ¼ 4.61) com-
ing this lubricant using the same purchase intentions item pared to when it was positioned as hedonic (M ¼ 4.33).
and conceptual fluency measures (a ¼ 0.88) as study 2. Most relevant to our hypotheses, there was a significant
Participants also reported the extent to which they trusted two-way interaction (F(1, 819) ¼ 13.54, p < .001, gp2 ¼
the lubricant brand using two items: (r ¼ 0.87; “How trust- 0.02; figure 2). Simple effects revealed that when the lubri-
worthy do you think the makers of Noma lubricant are?” cant was positioned as enhancing sexual pleasure, the con-
and “How honest do you think the makers of Noma lubri- trol appeal (M ¼ 4.48) led to higher purchase intentions
cant are?”), as well as how competent they thought the than the science appeal (M ¼ 4.17; F(1, 819) ¼ 4.05, p ¼
brand was (“How competent do you think the makers of .045, gp2 ¼ 0.01). In contrast, when the lubricant was posi-
Noma lubricant are?”). Participants next reported whether tioned as providing birth control, the science appeal
730 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

FIGURE 2 hedonic but was effective for a product positioned as utili-


tarian. Consistent with hypothesis 2, these effects of appeal
SCIENCE APPEALS INCREASE PURCHASE INTENTIONS FOR type on purchase intentions were mediated by the concep-
LUBRICANT POSITIONED AS UTILITARIAN BUT DECREASE tual fluency of the appeal. One potential limitation of this
PURCHASE INTENTIONS FOR LUBRICANT POSITIONED AS
HEDONIC study is that a birth control attribute in a lubricant may be
seen as novel by some consumers who have never used a
spermicide. In study 3b, we conceptually replicate the find-
ings of study 3a using a product that has commonly en-
countered attributes in all conditions.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/49/5/721/6581083 by Jnls Cust Serv on 19 January 2023


STUDY 3B
In study 3b, we used a body wash as the focal product.
We selected this product because it possesses commonly
encountered hedonic and utilitarian attributes and thus may
be positioned as either. For example, body wash brands
sold on Walmart’s website are described using both he-
donic terms (e.g., “immerses all your senses in its lush lath-
er,” “rich lather leaves skin feeling silky”) and utilitarian
terms (e.g., “effectively washes away dirt and bacteria,”
(M ¼ 4.86) led to higher purchase intentions compared to “formulated to wash away bacteria”). In this study, we
the control appeal (M ¼ 4.36; F(1, 819) ¼ 10.17, p ¼ .001, model our product descriptions after these real examples.
gp2 ¼ 0.01). We additionally measured perceived novelty of the product
Conceptual Fluency Moderated Mediation. We next as an alternative explanation, in addition to artificiality,
tested the indirect effect of product positioning by appeal brand trustworthiness, and brand competence.
type on purchase intentions, mediated by conceptual flu-
ency of the appeal using PROCESS model 7 (Hayes 2017), Participants and Procedure
which tests for moderated mediation. We set appeal type as In study 3b, 1,015 MTurk workers completed the study
the independent variable, product positioning as the moder- in exchange for monetary compensation (Mage: 39.84,
ator, purchase intentions as the dependent variable, and 55.68% female). Participants were excluded if they failed a
conceptual fluency as the mediator (figure 3). The confi- data validity indicator (two participants). The final sample
dence interval for the index of moderated mediation did size was 1,013.
not cross zero (index ¼ 0.40, 95% CI: [0.26, 0.53]), indi- Participants were presented with an appeal promoting a
cating significant moderated mediation via conceptual flu- body wash (named “Ely”) using the same 2 (Appeal Type)
ency. We also tested whether the fluency moderated  2 (Product Positioning) design as in study 3a (see web
mediation would remain when controlling for the alterna- appendix C for a pretest of 204 MTurk workers demon-
tive mediators of brand trustworthiness and competence. It strating that the hedonic appeal was perceived as hedonic
did, as the confidence interval for the index of moderated and the utilitarian appeal was perceived as utilitarian).
mediation of conceptual fluency did not cross zero in this Participants were told that they would view a “product de-
analysis (index ¼ 0.19. 95% CI: [0.10, 0.29]).9 scription displayed on the brand’s product packaging and
website.” Those in the hedonic condition saw an appeal
Discussion. Consistent with hypotheses 1a and 1b, the that described the lather as something that will “immerse
science appeal backfired for a product positioned as your senses in an indulgent experience,” whereas partici-
pants in the utilitarian condition saw an appeal that de-
9 Additional details around this moderated mediation analysis and al- scribed the lather as something that will “wash away odor
ternative mediation models are available in web appendix D. In partic-
ular, there is a significant serial moderated mediation (PROCESS causing bacteria.”
model 83) in which fluency is the proximal mediator and brand trust All participants then answered the same purchase inten-
the distal mediator, consistent with work suggesting that more fluently tions item and measures of conceptual fluency (a ¼ .092),
processed marketer-generated information may result in higher brand
trust (Kostyk, Leonhardt, and Niculescu 2017). Although this model
artificiality (a ¼ 0.86), brand trust (r ¼ 0.83), and brand
provides further insight into the mechanism of the science backfire ef- competence used in prior studies, as well as new measures
fect, we note that the index of moderated mediation was substantially of perceived novelty measured on six-point scales (r ¼
smaller for this model compared to the more parsimonious model in 0.70): “How novel is Ely Body Wash compared to other
which conceptual fluency is the sole mediator (.06 for serial vs. .40 for
nonserial). We therefore discuss this analysis in the web appendix but body washes on the market?” and “How new is Ely Body
do not discuss this further in the main paper. Wash compared to other body washes on the market?”.
PHILIPP-MULLER, COSTELLO, AND RECZEK 731

FIGURE 3

CONCEPTUAL FLUENCY MEDIATES THE APPEAL TYPE BY PRODUCT POSITIONING INTERACTION ON PURCHASE INTENTIONS

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/49/5/721/6581083 by Jnls Cust Serv on 19 January 2023


The order in which participants reported fluency, artificial- conceptual fluency as the mediator. This analysis indicated
ity, novelty, brand competence, and brand trust was ran- significant moderated mediation through conceptual flu-
domized. Finally, participants completed a data validity ency (index ¼ 0.24, 95% CI: [0.14, 0.34]). We next ran the
indicator and responded to demographic questions. same model but also included artificiality, novelty, brand
trustworthiness, and competence as parallel mediators
Results and Discussion along with conceptual fluency. The confidence interval for
Purchase Intentions. We subjected the data to a two- the index of moderated mediation continued to remain sig-
way ANOVA predicting purchase intentions from appeal nificant for conceptual fluency (index ¼ 0.16, 95% CI:
type (control ¼ 1, science ¼ 1), product positioning [0.09, 0.23]), even when controlling for artificiality, nov-
(hedonic ¼ 1, utilitarian ¼ 1), and the two-way interac- elty, trustworthiness, and competence. However, neither
tion. There was no main effect of appeal type (F(1, 1,009) artificiality (index < 0.001, 95% CI: [0.01, 0.02]), nov-
¼ 0.01, p ¼.927, gp2 < 0.001), but there was a main effect elty (index ¼ 0.02, 95% CI: [0.002, 0.05]), trustworthi-
of product positioning (F(1, 1,009) ¼ 13.60, p < .001, gp2 ness (index ¼ 0.04, 95% CI: [0.02, 0.10]) nor
¼ 0.01), such that when the body wash was positioned as competence (index ¼ 0.004, 95% CI: [0.02, 0.03]) medi-
utilitarian, purchase intentions were higher (M ¼ 4.69) ated the effect. Although the overall moderated mediation
compared to when the body wash was positioned as he- analyses for studies 3a and 3b were both significant, results
donic (M ¼ 4.35). Most relevant to our hypotheses, there around the specific indirect effects in this study were
was a significant two-way interaction (F(1, 1,009) ¼ slightly different than in study 3a. A theoretical discussion
14.58, p < .001, gp2 ¼ 0.01; figure 4). Simple effects for why this might be the case and additional details and
revealed that when the body wash was positioned as he- analyses are available in web appendix D. Supplementary
donic, the control appeal (M ¼ 4.52) led to higher purchase analysis (additional mediation analysis, including the serial
intentions than the science appeal (M ¼ 4.19; F(1, 1,009) ¼ mediation with brand trust tested in study 3a, ANOVAs,
6.95, p ¼ .009, gp2 ¼ 0.01). In contrast, when the body and means for mediator variables) are also available in
wash was positioned as utilitarian, the science appeal web appendix D.
(M ¼ 4.86) led to higher purchase intentions compared to Discussion. We once again found evidence that science
the control appeal (M ¼ 4.51; F(1, 1,009) ¼ 7.64, p ¼ .006, appeals backfire for products positioned as hedonic but not
gp2 ¼ 0.01). utilitarian. We also found evidence of significant moder-
Conceptual Fluency Moderated Mediation. We first ated mediation through conceptual fluency while ruling out
tested for moderated mediation via conceptual fluency. a number of alternative explanations. Having found sup-
Using PROCESS model 7, we set appeal type as the inde- port for our first two hypotheses, we now shift our focus to
pendent variable, product positioning as the moderator, understanding the moderators of the backfire effect of sci-
purchase intentions as the dependent variable, and ence appeals for hedonic products.
732 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

FIGURE 4 In the other between-subjects factor, half the participants


saw a science slogan, and the other half of the participants
SCIENCE APPEALS INCREASE PURCHASE INTENTIONS FOR saw a control slogan, using the same language as study 1b.
BODY WASH POSITIONED AS UTILITARIAN BUT DECREASE
PURCHASE INTENTIONS FOR BODY WASH POSITIONED AS
Participants next reported their purchase intentions using
HEDONIC the same measure used in prior studies. Finally, partici-
pants completed a data validity indicator and responded to
demographic and psychographic questions (e.g., “how
much do you like cookies?”).

Results

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/49/5/721/6581083 by Jnls Cust Serv on 19 January 2023


Purchase Intentions. We subjected the data to a two-
way ANOVA predicting purchase intentions from appeal
type (control ¼ 1, science ¼ 1), salience of science’s role
in production (control ¼ 1, increased science sali-
ence ¼ 1), and the two-way interaction. There was a main
effect of appeal type (F(1, 803) ¼ 6.10, p ¼.014, gp2 ¼
0.01), such that purchase intentions were higher in the con-
trol (M ¼ 5.22) compared to the science (M ¼ 4.97) condi-
tion. There was also a main effect of science salience
STUDY 4 condition (F(1, 803) ¼ 8.69, p ¼.003, gp2 ¼ 0.01), such
that when participants read about chemistry being a neces-
To test hypothesis 3, we examined whether reading an
sary part of baking (M ¼ 5.24), purchase intentions were
article about the importance of chemistry in baking (vs. a
higher compared to the control article (M ¼ 4.94). These
control article) on a cookie brand’s website eliminates the
backfire effect of using a science appeal for a hedonic main effects were, however, qualified by a two-way inter-
cookie by making it salient that science is a necessary part action (F(1, 803) ¼ 10.71, p ¼ .001, gp2 ¼ 0.01; figure 5).
of cookie production. This study therefore used a 2 Probing the simple effects revealed that when participants
(Appeal Type: Science vs. Control)  2 (Salience of read the control article, the control appeal (M ¼ 5.23) led
Science’s Role in Production: Increased Salience vs. to higher purchase intentions than the science appeal
Control) between-subjects design. (M ¼ 4.67; F(1, 803) ¼ 16.34, p < .001, gp2 ¼ 0.02), repli-
cating prior studies. However, when participants read that
Participants and Procedure chemistry is necessary for baking, there was no difference
in purchase intentions between the science appeal
A total of 814 participants (Mage: 40.68, 56.10% female) (M ¼ 5.28) compared to the control appeal (M ¼ 5.20; F(1,
completed study 4 on MTurk. Participants were excluded if 803) ¼ 0.32, p ¼ .569, gp2 < 0.001). Thus, the backfire ef-
they failed the data validity indicator (seven participants). fect of science appeals was attenuated when it was made
In one between-subjects factor, half the participants read salient to consumers that science is a necessary part of a
an article explaining that chemistry is necessary for high hedonic product’s production process.
quality baking (increased science salience condition), and
the other half read an article explaining that knowing what Discussion. Consistent with hypothesis 3, when con-
ingredients are in season is necessary for high quality bak- sumers were informed that science is necessary to create
ing (control condition). These articles were pretested on baked goods, invoking science in a marketing appeal no
MTurk (n ¼ 209) to ensure they led to significantly differ- longer reduced purchase intentions. By making the science
ent levels of salience about the necessity of science to cre- behind the production of a hedonic food product salient,
ate cookies. We also conducted a separate pretest on the science appeal became more conceptually fluent (be-
MTurk (n ¼ 201) indicating that the use of science is not cause it was more expected). Rather than measuring the
naturally salient in this product category, as we expected fluency of a science appeal, we manipulated how conceptu-
(see web appendix C for details on both pretests). ally fluent science was perceived to be in producing he-
After reading the articles, participants in the main study donic products, thus manipulating the mediator of our
answered two filler measures about the article (e.g., “How proposed process (Spencer et al. 2005).
appropriate would you say the length of this article was for In studies 5a–5c, we now test hypothesis 4, which pre-
an online format?”) before moving on to the next part of dicts that those who do not endorse the lay belief that sci-
the study. Participants were then asked to “evaluate and ence is cold should not exhibit the effect described in
make decisions about a real chocolate chip cookie brand.” hypothesis 1a.
PHILIPP-MULLER, COSTELLO, AND RECZEK 733

FIGURE 5 regressed purchase intentions on appeal type (con-


trol ¼ 1, science ¼ 1), the mean-centered endorsement of
SCIENCE APPEALS DO NOT DECREASE PURCHASE the science is cold lay belief, and the two-way interaction.
INTENTIONS FOR COOKIES WHEN SALIENCE OF SCIENCE IN There was a main effect of both appeal type (bunstandardized
BAKING IS HIGH
¼ 0.19, t(499) ¼ 3.09, p ¼ .002, gp2 ¼ 0.13) and en-
dorsement of the lay belief (bunstandardized ¼ 0.19, t(499) ¼
8.04, p < .001, gp2 ¼ 0.33). Importantly, these two main
effects were also qualified by our predicted interaction
(bunstandardized ¼ 0.07, t(499) ¼ 2.78, p ¼ .006, gp2 ¼
0.11). Probing the interaction using floodlight analysis

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/49/5/721/6581083 by Jnls Cust Serv on 19 January 2023


(Spiller et al. 2013) via PROCESS model 1 (Hayes 2017)
indicated that, among those whose lay belief endorsement
is at or below 0.27 (on a scale where 4 ¼ cold and
þ4 ¼ warm; 65% of the sample), there was a significant re-
duction in purchase intentions for the body wash with a sci-
ence (vs. control) label. However, there is no difference
across the science and control conditions among those who
view science as relatively warm (figure 6A).
Our theory predicts that the backfire effect occurs for he-
STUDY 5A–5C donically positioned products because the coldness of sci-
ence conflicts with consumers’ beliefs about the warmth
Study 5a stereotype dimension rather than the competence stereo-
In this study, we test whether the backfire effect we ob- type dimension. We therefore repeated the same analysis
serve for hedonically positioned products in past studies is using appeal type, mean-centered endorsement of the sci-
moderated by participants’ endorsement of the lay belief ence is competent stereotype, and the interaction of these
that science is cold. Because our theorizing posits that the variables. Consistent with our theorizing, we do not find an
science backfire effect is driven by this lay belief, we ex- interaction between appeal type and endorsement of the
pect that among those who do not see science as cold, the science is competent lay belief (bunstandardized ¼ 0.006,
backfire effect will attenuate. t(499) ¼ .15, p ¼ .884, gp2 < 0.001). Thus, study 5a pro-
A total of 503 participants (Mage: 39.78, 59.00% female) vides evidence that endorsement of the lay belief that sci-
completed study 5a on MTurk. Participants were assigned ence is cold moderates the backfire effect of science
to one of two conditions (Appeal Type: Control vs. appeals, supporting our proposed theoretical account via
Science) in which they read the product description for the moderation.
label of a hedonically positioned body wash (see web ap-
pendix C for a pretest of n ¼ 264 MTurk workers demon- Study 5b
strating that it was seen as hedonic). In the control
condition, the description was “Ely: Wash with a rich, In study 5b, our goal was to conceptually replicate the
creamy lather,” while in the science condition the descrip- findings of study 5a using a moderator that is theoretically
tion was “Ely: Scientifically engineered to wash with a consistent with our proposed process, trust in scientists, but
rich, creamy lather.” Participants indicated their purchase also that is relevant to managers due to its known correlates
intentions for the body wash on the same scale used in with various demographic variables identified via a poll
prior studies and then reported the extent to which they ex- conducted by the Pew Research Center (an American
plicitly endorsed the lay belief that science is competent thinktank that conducts public opinion polling on a variety
but cold. Specifically, consistent with the lay belief pilot of public interest topics; Funk 2020). Participants in study
study, they were given definitions of the warmth/coldness 5b (n ¼ 609, Mage: 40.44, 55.3% female) therefore partici-
stereotype and competence/incompetence stereotype pated in a study that was almost identical to study 5a, ex-
(Aaker et al. 2010, 2012; Judd et al. 2005) and indicated cept, rather than reporting their endorsement of science lay
how warm (4 cold to þ4 warm) and competent (4 in- beliefs, they reported their trust in scientists on three items
competent to þ4) they viewed the scientific process to be taken from the Pew poll (Funk 2020): “To what extent
(MCompetence ¼ 2.48, MWarmth ¼ 0.63). Finally, partici- would you say you have confidence in scientists to act in
pants provided demographic information and completed a the best interest of the public?,” “How often would you say
data validity indicator. that scientists provide fair and accurate information?,”
No participants failed our data validity indicator, leaving “How often would you say that scientists are transparent
a sample of 503 responses. To test hypothesis 4, we about conflicts of interest?,” which were all measured on
734 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

FIGURE 6

INVOKING SCIENCE WHEN MARKETING A HEDONIC BODY WASH BACKFIRES UNLESS CONSUMERS SEE SCIENCE AS
RELATIVELY WARM (A) OR HAVE HIGH LEVELS OF TRUST IN SCIENTISTS (B). (A) MODERATION BY ENDORSEMENT OF SCIENCE IS
COLD LAY BELIEF. (B) MODERATION BY TRUST IN SCIENTISTS.

(a) (b)
6

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/49/5/721/6581083 by Jnls Cust Serv on 19 January 2023


5

5
Purchase Intentions

Purchase Intentions
4

4
3

Control Control

Science Science
2

−4 −2 0 2 4 2 4 6 8
Endorsement of Lay Intuition Trust in Scientists

NOTE.— Gray areas in figures indicate regions of significance

nine-point scales and combined into an index of trust in Study 5c


scientists (a ¼ 0.91; M ¼ 6.10).
In study 5c, we conceptually replicate the findings of
One participant failed our data validity indicator, leaving
studies 5a and 5b, this time using the industry in which
a final sample of 608 responses. To test hypothesis 4, we
regressed purchase intentions on appeal type (same coding consumers work as the moderating variable of interest.
as previous study), the mean-centered index measuring Specifically, we ran the same study with two different pop-
trust in scientists, and the two-way interaction. There was a ulations: those who work in STEM sectors and those who
main effect of both appeal type (bunstandardized ¼ 0.20, do not, in order to test whether exposure to science and sci-
t(604) ¼ 3.31, p ¼ .001, gp2 ¼ 0.13) and trust in scien- entists attenuates the science backfire effect we observe in
tists (bunstandardized ¼ 0.24, t(604) ¼ 6.90, p < .001, gp2 ¼ earlier studies. We predict that the backfire effect will
0.27, Mtrust ¼ 6.10). Importantly, these two main effects emerge for non-STEM professionals but will be eliminated
were also qualified by our predicted interaction (bunstandar- for individuals working in STEM fields.
2
dized ¼ 0.07, t(604) ¼ 2.78, p ¼ .043, gp ¼ 0.08). Follow
Study 5c (n ¼ 602, Mage: 32.56, 52.30% male) used an
up floodlight analysis revealed a pattern consistent with almost identical design to study 5a and 5b, except, instead
our theorizing and study 5a. We find that the science back- of measuring a moderating variable, we sampled partici-
fire effect occurs for the majority of our sample (69%), but pants from two different industries (STEM vs. non-STEM)
that the effect turns off for those individuals with high lev- using Prolific’s prescreening function, which allows
els of trust in scientists (i.e., those at 7.03 and higher on the researchers to screen on a variety of participant variables
nine-point scale, figure 6B). Study 5b thus provides further including self-identified employment sector. To do this, we
moderation evidence consistent with our theorizing. simultaneously ran two distinct studies on Prolific with the
PHILIPP-MULLER, COSTELLO, AND RECZEK 735

FIGURE 7 science backfire effect for hedonic products. We find that


the effect is eliminated when a consumer does not explic-
SCIENCE APPEALS DO NOT DECREASE PURCHASE itly endorse the science is cold lay belief (study 5a), has
INTEREST FOR A HEDONIC BODY WASH AMONG THOSE WHO high level of trust in scientists (study 5b), and is exposed to
WORK IN STEM
the scientific process and scientists via work experience
(study 5c).10

GENERAL DISCUSSION
Across 10 studies, we found evidence that invoking sci-

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/49/5/721/6581083 by Jnls Cust Serv on 19 January 2023


ence backfires when marketing CPGs positioned as he-
donic, leading to reduced product valuation. The same
science appeals were advantageous, however, when used to
promote products positioned as utilitarian. These findings
appeared to be driven by the conceptual disfluency of pair-
ing warm hedonic products with the scientific process,
which is perceived as competent, yet cold. We ruled out
the possibility that perceived artificiality or novelty drives
same number of participants in each study (n ¼ 300): one the backfire effect of science appeals for hedonic products
study was run with a sample of participants who stated along with a variety of other relevant alternative mecha-
they worked in a STEM industry (using the Prolific pre- nisms. We also identified several boundary conditions for
screen filter “Employment-Sector” and selecting the cate- the science backfire effect. When it was salient to consum-
gory “Science, Technology, Engineering, & ers that science is necessary to develop a hedonic product,
Mathematics”), and one study was run with a sample of using a science appeal did not reduce purchase intentions.
participants who stated they worked in any industry other Finally, among those who see science as relatively warm,
than STEM. To analyze the data, we combined these sam- highly trust scientists, or work in a STEM industry, the
ples into one dataset and added a variable indicating the backfire effect was attenuated.
sector in which each participant worked.
Three individuals were excluded for failing our data va- Theoretical Contributions
lidity indicator, leaving 599 responses. We subjected the This research contributes to multiple literatures in con-
data to a two-way ANOVA predicting purchase intentions sumer behavior. Our primary contribution lies in demon-
from appeal type (control ¼ 1, science ¼ 1), participant strating how consumers respond to the invocation of
industry (non-STEM ¼ 1, STEM ¼ 1), and the two-way science in marketing appeals. Although prior literature in
interaction. There was no main effect of appeal type (F(1, marketing has explored reference to specific scientific pro-
595) ¼1.17, p ¼ .279, gp2 ¼ 0.002), but there was a main cesses in marketing appeals (Hingston and Noseworthy
effect of participant’s employment sector (F(1, 595)
2018; Zheng et al. 2019) or the addition or subtraction of
¼8.41, p ¼.004, gp2 ¼ 0.014), such that individuals in
specific ingredients (Andre et al. 2019; Naylor et al. 2009),
non-STEM fields reported higher purchase intentions
no prior research has explored how consumers respond to
(M ¼ 4.15) than those in STEM fields (M ¼ 3.80).
brands invoking science more broadly as part of a market-
Most relevant to our hypotheses, there was a significant
ing appeal. Our research therefore addresses a novel ques-
two-way interaction (F(1, 595) ¼ 4.77, p ¼ .029, gp2 ¼
tion to the consumer behavior literature: How do
0.008; figure 7). Consistent with studies 5a and 5b, we ob-
consumers respond to brands that explicitly link their prod-
served a significant backfire effect of science for partici-
pants working outside of the STEM sector (F(1, 595) ¼ ucts to science?
5.33, p ¼ .021, gp2 ¼ 0.009), such that participants were To answer this question, we introduce a new lay belief
less likely to buy the hedonic product promoted with a sci- about the scientific process in the marketplace: it is seen as
ence (M ¼ 3.95) than a control appeal (M ¼ 4.35). competent but cold. We further demonstrate several of the
However, for participants working in STEM industries, we
10 Although our primary focus in study 5c is the two-way interaction,
find that there was no difference in purchase intentions be- it is noteworthy that there was an unpredicted main effect of industry,
tween those in the control appeal condition (M ¼ 3.73) and such that the scientists seemed less interested in purchasing body
those in the science appeal condition (M ¼ 3.85) (F(1, 595) wash overall. Because industry is not randomly assigned, we are hes-
¼ 0.61, p ¼ .436, gp2 ¼ 0.001). itant to interpret this effect, as there are many demographic or other
correlates of industry that could be driving this effect. In web appen-
Studies 5a, 5b, and 5c all provide support for hypothesis dix D, we show that none of the demographic variables measured in
4, using distinct but conceptually related moderators of the this study (e.g., age, gender) can fully account for this main effect.
736 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

downstream effects of this lay belief. In doing so, we not detrimental in promoting various products. Our findings
only contribute to the literature on consumer lay theories suggest that it would behoove marketers to invoke science
(Cheng et al. 2017; Deval et al. 2013; McFerran and when promoting products that have utilitarian attributes.
Mukhopadhyay 2013; Raghunathan et al. 2006; Zane et al. Likewise, for product categories where it is already natu-
2020) but also to the growing body of literature exploring rally salient to consumers that science is involved in pro-
how consumer response to products varies as a function of duction (e.g., highly technical products), invoking science
what is communicated to consumers about the product’s should not have harmful consequences. For CPGs and
origin and production process (Smith, Newman, and Dhar other categories where scientific development is less
2016; Newman and Dhar 2014; Reich, Kupor, and Smith expected, however, our findings suggest that invoking sci-
2018). ence to market these products might backfire. Thus, when

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/49/5/721/6581083 by Jnls Cust Serv on 19 January 2023


The introduction of consumer lay beliefs about the role promoting hedonically positioned products in the CPG do-
of science in developing products also contributes to the main, marketers should avoid the use of science appeals.
literature on perceptions of science and scientists. Though This work also provides insight into how the negative
science is generally respected in the United States, there impact of invoking science in the marketing of consumer
has been a recent erosion of trust in the scientific process products can be circumvented. There are, of course, times
and scientists as individuals (American Academy of Arts where there are potential benefits of highlighting the scien-
and Sciences 2018; Gauchat 2012). Perhaps lay beliefs tific process used in creating a hedonic good or service
about the scientific process as lacking warmth contribute to (e.g., if a proprietary scientific process is what gives a
this growing distrust. Thus, our work builds upon past brand their competitive advantage). In these cases, study 4
work that has identified stereotypes about scientists (as a suggests that explaining to consumers that science is neces-
group), which include beliefs that they are cold and compe- sary for creating a product mitigates the negative impact of
tent (Brown et al. 2018; McPherson et al. 2018). science appeals on purchase behavior. Thus, a campaign
Our work also builds on the personalized matching liter- highlighting the scientific engineering of even the most he-
ature (Goenka and Van Osselaer 2019; Stuppy, Mead, and donically oriented product might be well received if it is
van Osselaer 2020; Teeny et al. 2021), which finds that accompanied by an explanation for how and why science is
consumers are more persuaded by appeals that are framed necessary for developing the product, which could even in-
to match features of the consumer (e.g., their attitude ba- clude detailing the exact ways in which the science was
sis). We introduce the idea of lay belief matching: When used.
an appeal matches consumers’ lay beliefs about a product’s Finally, this work identifies several variables that mod-
development, they will be more persuaded and more likely erate when invoking science in marketing consumer prod-
to act consistently with the appeal. One key distinction be- ucts backfires. These insights can be used in segmentation
tween lay belief matching and the existing work on direct strategies by managers. For example, those who have a
matching effects is that in this work we never directly com- high degree of trust in scientists or who work in STEM in-
pare warm versus cold appeals when promoting hedonic dustries do not exhibit the science backfire effect. We
versus utilitarian products. Instead, we rely on the lay intui- assessed trust in scientists using items derived from a na-
tion that science is cold to indirectly create a mismatch be- tional Pew poll, which has identified other demographic
tween science appeals and hedonic products. Additionally, correlates of trust in scientists (Funk et al. 2019). For ex-
this work goes beyond global affective/cognitive matching ample, Black Americans and Republicans report lower
(Fabrigar and Petty 1999; Klein and Melnyk 2016) to re- trust in scientists (Funk, Kennedy, and Tyson 2020;
fute the notion that any cognitive appeal would mismatch Kennedy, Tyson, and Funk 2022). Thus, brand managers
with any affective product. Instead, cold cognitive appeals wishing to use science appeals to promote a hedonic prod-
mismatch with products where consumers anticipate uct could target segments of consumers who are likely to
warmth. trust scientists or take care to explain why science is neces-
Finally, although we found that a mismatch between sci- sary for creating the product when targeting a segment
ence appeals and hedonic products or attributes reduced likely to exhibit the science backfire effect.
product valuation via disfluency, the results of study 3b
suggest that perceived brand competence might also play a Limitations and Future Research
role in driving matching effects for utilitarian products. We
In this research, we explore the consequences of lay the-
look forward to future research that further explores this
ories around science but do not investigate the origins of
likely multiply determined effect.
these beliefs. It is difficult to pinpoint the exact source of a
lay belief, as they are typically rooted in a combination of
Practical Implications sociocultural messages (Haws et al. 2017; Morris, Menon,
The present findings offer marketers insight into when and Ames 2001) and personal experience (Ross and
the use of science appeals might be effective versus Nisbett 1991; Tonietto et al. 2021). We suspect that both
PHILIPP-MULLER, COSTELLO, AND RECZEK 737

factors may be at play here and that experiences/messaging DATA COLLECTION INFORMATION
related to the scientific process can influence beliefs about
scientists, and vice versa. For example, personal experi- Studies 1b, 1c, 2, 3a, 3b, 4, 5a, 5b; the three pilots dis-
ence with science (e.g., in school) as a cold, careful process cussed in the introduction; the study 2 pretest, the study 3b
may influence perceptions of scientists. Further, research pretest, the two study 4 pretests, and the study 5a–5c pre-
has found that those who have a particular job, role, or en- test were collected by all three authors using Amazon
gage in particular behaviors tend to be perceived as pos- Mechanical Turk. Study 5c was collected by all three
sessing traits associated with that behavior or role (i.e., authors using Prolific. The study 1a pretest was collected
Spontaneous Trait Inference; Carlston and Skowronski at the Fisher Behavioral Lab at The Ohio State University
2005; Newman and Uleman 1990). Conversely, portrayals with the assistance of a lab manager and research assistants

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/49/5/721/6581083 by Jnls Cust Serv on 19 January 2023


of scientists as lacking warmth (e.g., the unemotional sci- working under the supervision of all three authors. Study
entific genius trope often seen in popular culture) may in- 1a was simultaneously collected at the Fisher Behavioral
fluence perceptions of the scientific process. Future Lab at The Ohio State University and the Mendoza
research might explore how the lay beliefs we identify in Behavioral Lab at the University of Notre Dame with the
this research are formed. assistance of lab managers and research assistants, at each
Another limitation of the present work is that the effect respective institution, working under the supervision of all
sizes we observe for the science backfire effect would be three authors. Studies 1b, 2; the second and third pilot stud-
classified as small (Cohen 1988). However, we also find ies in the introduction; and the first study 4 pretest were
that the effect is very robust, as it appears across a variety collected in fall 2020. Study 3a was collected in summer
of different product categories, paradigms, and ways of in- 2020. Study 1c was collected in summer 2021. Study 1a,
voking science. Additionally, the effect size is relatively 3b, 5a, 5b, 5c; the first pilot study in the introduction; the
stable across studies, suggesting that the studies reported study 1a pretest, study 2 pretest, study 3b pretest, the sec-
here reliably captured the science backfire effect. In accor- ond study 4 pretest, and the study 5a–5c pretest were all
dance with recommended best practices, we conducted collected in fall 2021. All studies were analyzed by the first
studies with large samples (greater than 100 participants author with the exception of studies 5a–c which were ana-
per cell in every study) to ensure that we would have suffi- lyzed by the first and second authors. All studies were
cient power to detect any effects that were present. designed by all three authors. The data are currently stored
However, even if the true effect size is indeed small by on the Open Science Framework.
traditional standards, we still suggest that this effect has
substantive importance (Kelley and Preacher 2012 for a REFERENCES
discussion of the different facets of effect size). For exam-
ple, we find that the use of science appeals reduces likeli- Allport, Gordon Willard (1954), The Nature of Prejudice, Boston,
hood of choosing a cookie by over 30% and WTP by over MA: Addison-Wesley.
9% (in terms of percentage change with a control message Aaker, Jennifer L., Kathleen D. Garbinsky, and Emily N. Vohs
as the baseline), decreases that would be viewed as impor- (2012), “Cultivating Admiration in Brands: Warmth,
Competence, and Landing in the ‘Golden Quadrant,” Journal
tant from a managerial perspective, particularly since
of Consumer Psychology, 22 (2), 191–94.
changing language in appeals is essentially costless for Aaker, Jennifer, Kathleen D. Vohs, and Cassie Mogilner (2010),
firms. Critically, although we found that the overall effect “Nonprofits Are Seen as Warm and for-Profits as Competent:
size is small, it is not homogenous across the entire popula- Firm Stereotypes Matter,” Journal of Consumer Research, 37
tion, and thus the backfire effect varies across segments. (2), 224–37.
Studies 5a, 5b, and 5c show that although the effect is rela- American Academy of Arts and Sciences (2018), “Perceptions of
tively small at a population level, it varies depending on in- Science in America,” https://www.amacad.org/publication/
perceptions-science-america.
dividual differences and thus may vary from a large effect
Andre, Quentin, Pierre Chandon, and Kelly Haws (2019),
to a null effect depending on the segment of interest (as is “Healthy through Presence or Absence, Nature or Science?:
the case with any moderated effect). A Framework for Understanding Front-of-Package Food
Finally, another limitation of the current work is that al- Claims,” Journal of Public Policy & Marketing, 38 (2),
though our studies reference science explicitly, we also 172–91.
suspect that references to science need not be explicit to Becker, G. M., M. H. DeGroot, and J. Marschak (1964),
have the effects we theorize. For example, including “Measuring Utility by a Single-Response Sequential
images of scientists developing the product or of a labora- Method,” Behavioral Science, 9 (3), 226–32.
Brown, Elizabeth R., Mia Steinberg, Yun Lu, and Amanda B.
tory setting may produce the same disfluency for hedonic Diekman (2018), “Is the Lone Scientist an American Dream?
products. Thus, future research could examine whether Perceived Communal Opportunities in STEM Offer a
more subtle instantiations of our paradigm are sufficient to Pathway to Closing US–Asia Gaps in Interest and Positivity,”
produce the backfire effect we document. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 9 (1), 11–23.
738 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

Cadbury Dairy Milk (2010), “Cadbury Advert—Cadbury Fiske, Susan T., Amy J. C. Cuddy, and Peter Glick (2007),
Chocolate Charmer Ad,” Last Accessed May 17, 2022, “Universal Dimensions of Social Cognition: Warmth and
https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=Bv7mETANB7A. Competence,” Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11 (2), 77–83.
Carlston, Donal E. and John J. Skowronski (2005), “Linking ver- Fuchs, Christoph, Martin Schreier, and Stijn M. J. van Osselaer
sus Thinking: Evidence for the Different Associative and (2015), “The Handmade Effect: What’s Love Got to Do with
Attributional Bases of Spontaneous Trait Transference and It?,” Journal of Marketing, 79 (2), 98–110.
Spontaneous Trait Inference,” Journal of Personality and Funk, Cary (2020), “Key Findings about Americans’ Confidence
Social Psychology, 89 (6), 884–98. in Science and their Views on Scientists’ Role in Society,”
Chattalas, Michael and Hirokazu Takada (2013), “Warm versus Pew Research Center, Last Accessed May 17, 2022, https://
Competent Countries: National Stereotyping Effects on www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2020/02/12/key-findings-
Expectations of Hedonic versus Utilitarian Product about-americans-confidence-in-science-and-their-views-on-
Properties,” Place Branding and Public Diplomacy, 9 (2), scientists-role-in-society/.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/49/5/721/6581083 by Jnls Cust Serv on 19 January 2023


88–97. Funk, Cary, Meg Hefferon, Brian Kennedy, and Courtney Johnson
Cheng, Yimin, Anirban Mukhopadhyay, and Rom Y. Schrift (2019), “Trust and Mistrust in Americans’ Views of
(2017), “Do Costly Options Lead to Better Outcomes? How Scientific Experts,” Pew Research Center, Last Accessed
the Protestant Work Ethic Influences the Cost-Benefit May 17, 2022, https://www.pewresearch.org/science/2019/
Heuristic in Goal Pursuit,” Journal of Marketing Research, 08/02/trust-and-mistrust-in-americans-views-of-scientific-
54 (4), 636–49. experts/.
Chitturi, Ravindra, Vijay Raghunathan, and Rajagopal Mahajan Funk, Cary, Brian Kennedy, and Alec Tyson (2020), “Black
(2008), “Delight by Design: The Role of Hedonic versus Americans Have Less Confidence to Act in the Public
Utilitarian Benefits,” Journal of Marketing, 72 (3), 48–63. Interest,” Pew Research Center, Last Accessed May 17,
Chmielewski, Michael and Sarah C. Kucker (2020), “An MTurk 2022, https://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2020/08/28/
Crisis? Shifts in Data Quality and the Impact on Study black-americans-have-less-confidence-in-scientists-to-act-in-
Results,” Social Psychological and Personality Science, 11 the-public-interest/.
(4), 464–73. Gauchat, Gordon (2012), “Politicization of Science in the Public
Cohen, Jacob (1988), Statistical Power Analysis for the Sphere: A Study of Public Trust in the United States, 1974 to
Behavioral Sciences, Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
2010,” American Sociological Review, 77 (2), 167–87.
Costello, John P. and Rebecca Walker Reczek (2020), “Providers
Goddard, E. Desmond and James V. Gruber, ed. (1999),
versus Platforms: Marketing Communications in the Sharing
Principles of Polymer Science and Technology in Cosmetics
Economy,” Journal of Marketing, 84 (6), 22–38.
and Personal Care, Edison, NJ: CRC Press.
Cuddy, Amy, Peter Fiske, and Susan Glick (2008), “Warmth and
Goenka, Shreyans and Stijn M. J. Van Osselaer (2019), “Charities
Competence as Universal Dimensions of Social Perception:
Can Increase the Effectiveness of Donation Appeals by
The Stereotype Content Model and the BIAS Map,”
Using a Morally Congruent Positive Emotion,” Journal of
Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 40 (March),
61–149. Consumer Research, 46 (4), 774–90.
Dennis, Sean A., Christopher A. Goodson, and Brian M. Pearson Haws, Kelly L., Kevin L. Walker Reczek, and Rebecca Sample
(2020), “Online Worker Fraud and Evolving Threats to the (2017), “Healthy Diets Make Empty Wallets: The Healthy ¼
Integrity of MTurk Data: A Discussion of Virtual Private Expensive Intuition,” Journal of Consumer Research, 43
Servers and the Limitations of IP-Based Screening (April), ucw078.
Procedures,” Behavioral Research in Accounting, 32 (1), Hayes, Andrew F. (2017), Introduction to Mediation, Moderation,
119–34. and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based
Deval, Helène, Susan P. Mantel, Frank R. Kardes, and Steven S. Approach, New York, NY: Guilford.
Posavac (2013), “How Naive Theories Drive Opposing Hingston, Sean T. and Theodore J. Noseworthy (2018), “Why
Inferences from the Same Information,” Journal of Consumers Don’t See the Benefits of Genetically Modified
Consumer Research, 39 (6), 1185–201. Foods, and What Marketers Can Do about It,” Journal of
Dhar, Ravi and Klaus Wertenbroch (2000), “Consumer Choice be- Marketing, 82 (5), 125–40.
tween Hedonic and Utilitarian Goods,” Journal of Marketing Hirschman, Elizabeth C. and Morris B. Holbrook (1982),
Research, 37 (1), 60–71. “Hedonic Consumption: Emerging Concepts, Methods and
Diekman, Amanda B., Emily K. Clark, Amanda M. Johnston, Propositions,” Journal of Marketing, 46 (3), 92–101.
Elizabeth R. Brown, and Mia Steinberg (2011), “Malleability Judd, Charles M., Laurie James-Hawkins, Vincent Yzerbyt, and
in Communal Goals and Beliefs Influences Attraction to Yoshihisa Kashima (2005), “Fundamental Dimensions of
STEM Careers: Evidence for a Goal Congruity Perspective,” Social Judgment: Understanding the Relations between
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101 (5), Judgments of Competence and Warmth,” Journal of
902–18. Personality and Social Psychology, 89 (6), 899–913.
Duhachek, Adam, Nidhi Agrawal, and Dahee Han (2012), “Guilt Kelley, Ken and Kristopher J. Preacher (2012), “On Effect Size,”
versus Shame: Coping, Fluency, and Framing in the Psychological Methods, 17 (2), 137–52.
Effectiveness of Responsible Drinking Messages,” Journal of Kennedy, Brian, Alec Tyson, and Cary Funk (2022), “Americans’
Marketing Research, 49 (6), 928–41. Trust in Scientists, Other Groups Declines,” Pew Research
Fabrigar, Leandre R. and Richard E. Petty (1999), “The Role of Center, Last Accessed May 17, 2022, https://www.pewre-
the Affective and Cognitive Bases of Attitudes in search.org/science/2022/02/15/americans-trust-in-scientists-
Susceptibility to Affectively and Cognitively Based other-groups-declines/.
Persuasion,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25 Kidwell, Blair, Adam Farmer, and David M. Hardesty (2013),
(3), 363–81. “Getting Liberals and Conservatives to Go Green: Political
PHILIPP-MULLER, COSTELLO, AND RECZEK 739

Ideology and Congruent Appeals,” Journal of Consumer Naylor, Rebecca Walker, Courtney M. Droms, and Kelly L. Haws
Research, 40 (2), 350–67. (2009), “Eating with a Purpose: Consumer Response to
Klein, Kristina and Valentyna Melnyk (2016), “Speaking to the Functional Food Health Claims in Conflicting versus
Mind or the Heart: Effects of Matching Hedonic versus Complementary Information Environments,” Journal of
Utilitarian Arguments and Products,” Marketing Letters, 27 Public Policy & Marketing, 28 (2), 221–33.
(1), 131–42. Newman, George E. and Ravi Dhar (2014), “Authenticity Is
Kostyk, Alena, James M. Leonhardt, and Mihai Niculescu (2017), Contagious: Brand Essence and the Original Source of
“Simpler Online Ratings Formats Increase Consumer Trust,” Production,” Journal of Marketing Research, 51 (3), 371–86.
Journal of Research in Interactive Marketing, 11 (2), Newman, Leonard S. and James S. Uleman (1990), “Assimilation
131–41. and Contrast Effects in Spontaneous Trait Inference,”
Kramer, Thomas and Lauren Block (2011), “Nonconscious Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16 (2), 224–40.
Effects of Peculiar Beliefs on Consumer Psychology and Parker, Jeffrey R., Iman Paul, Ryan Hamilton, Omar Rodriguez-

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/49/5/721/6581083 by Jnls Cust Serv on 19 January 2023


Choice,” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 21 (1), 101–11. Vila, and Sundar G. Bharadwaj (2021), “How Product Type
Krause, Nicole M., Dietram A. Brossard, Michael A. Scheufele, and Organic Label Structure Combine to Influence
Keith Xenos, and Dominique Franke (2019), “Trends— Consumers’ Evaluations of Organic Foods,” Journal of
Americans’ Trust in Science and Scientists,” Public Opinion Public Policy & Marketing, 40 (3), 419–28.
Quarterly, 83 (Winter), 817–36. Peter, Christina and Milan Ponzi (2018), “The Risk of Omitting
Krems, Jaimie Arona, Ahra Ko, Jordan W. Moon, and Michael E. Warmth or Competence Information in Ads: Advertising
W. Varnum (2021), “Lay Beliefs about Gender and Sexual Strategies for Hedonic and Utilitarian Brand Types,” Journal
Behavior: First Evidence for a Pervasive, Robust (but of Advertising Research, 58 (4), 423–32.
Seemingly Unfounded) Stereotype,” Psychological Science, Pham, Nguyen and Naomi Mandel (2019), “What Influences
32 (6), 871–89. Consumer Evaluation of Genetically Modified Foods?”
Lee, Saerom, Lisa E. Bolton, and Karen P. Winterich (2017), “To Journal of Public Policy & Marketing, 38 (2), 263–79.
Profit or Not to Profit? The Role of Greed Perceptions in Raghunathan, Rajagopal, Rebecca Walker Naylor, and Wayne D.
Consumer Support for Social Ventures,” Journal of Hoyer (2006), “The Unhealthy ¼ Tasty Intuition and Its
Consumer Research, 44 (4), 853–76. Effects on Taste Inferences, Enjoyment, and Choice of Food
Lee, Angela Y. and Jennifer L. Aaker (2004), “Bringing the Frame Products,” Journal of Marketing, 70 (4), 170–84.
into Focus: The Influence of Regulatory Fit on Processing Reich, Taly, Rosanna K. Kupor, and Daniella M. Smith (2018),
“Made by Mistake: When Mistakes Increase Product
Fluency and Persuasion,” Journal of Personality and Social
Preference,” Journal of Consumer Research, 44 (5),
Psychology, 86 (2), 205–18.
1085–103.
Lee, Angela Y. and Aparna A. Labroo (2004), “The Effect of
Riis, Jason, Joseph P. Simmons, and Geoffrey P. Goodwin (2008),
Conceptual and Perceptual Fluency on Brand Evaluation,”
“Preferences for Enhancement Pharmaceuticals: The
Journal of Marketing Research, 41 (2), 151–65.
Reluctance to Enhance Fundamental Traits,” Journal of
Lellis, Julie C. (2016), “Waving the Red Flag: FTC Rgulation of
Consumer Research, 35 (3), 495–508.
Deceptive Weight-Loss Advertising 1951–2009,” Health
Rocklage, Matthew D. and Russell H. Fazio (2020), “The
Communication, 31 (1), 47–59. Enhancing versus Backfiring Effects of Positive Emotion in
Luchs, Michael G., Rebecca Walker Naylor, Julie R. Irwin, and Consumer Reviews,” Journal of Marketing Research, 57 (2),
Rajagopal Raghunathan (2010), “The Sustainability 332–52.
Liability: Potential Negative Effects of Ethicality on Product Rosenthal, Robert, Ralph L. Rosnow, and Donald B. Rubin
Preference,” Journal of Marketing, 74 (5), 18–31. (2000), Contrasts and Effect Sizes in Behavioral Research: A
Marinova, Detelina, Irina V. Kozlenkova, Leona Cuttler, and J. B. Correlational Approach, Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
Silvers (2017), “To Prescribe or Not to Prescribe? Consumer University Press.
Access to Life-Enhancing Products,” The Journal of Ross, Lee and Richard E. Nisbett (1991), The Person and the
Consumer Research, 43 (5), 806–23. Situation: Perspectives of Social Psychology, New York,
McFerran, Brent and Anirban Mukhopadhyay (2013), “Lay NY: McGraw-Hill.
Theories of Obesity Predict Actual Body Mass,” Rozin, Paul (2005), “The Meaning of ‘Natural’ Process More
Psychological Science, 24 (8), 1428–36. Important than Content,” Psychological Science, 16 (8),
McPherson, Erin, Bernadette Park, and Tiffany A. Ito (2018), 652–58.
“The Role of Prototype Matching in Science Pursuits: Scott, Sydney E., Christopher D. Inbar, Dominique Wirz, Paul
Perceptions of Scientists That Are Inaccurate and Diverge Brossard, and Yoel Rozin (2018), “An Overview of Attitudes
from Self-Perceptions Predict Reduced Interest in a Science toward Genetically Engineered Food,” Annual Review of
Career,” Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 44 (6), Nutrition, 38 (August), 459–79.
881–98. Scott, Sydney E. and Paul Rozin (2017), “Are Additives
Morris, Michael W., Tanya Menon, and Daniel R. Ames (2001), Unnatural? Generality and Mechanisms of Additivity
“Culturally Conferred Conceptions of Agency: A Key to Dominance,” Judgment and Decision Making, 12
Social Perception of Persons, Groups, and Other Actors,” (November), 572–83.
Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5 (2), 169–82. Scott, Sydney E., Paul Rozin, and Deborah A. Small (2020),
Mukhopadhyay, Anirban and Gita Venkataramani Johar (2005), “Consumers Prefer “Natural” More for Preventatives than for
“Where There Is a Will, Is There a Way? Effects of Lay Curatives,” Journal of Consumer Research, 47 (3), 454–71.
Theories of Self-Control on Setting and Keeping Singh, Rakesh K. and Nepal Singh (2005), “Quality of Packaged
Resolutions,” Journal of Consumer Research, 31 (4), Foods,” in Innovations in Food Packaging, ed. Jung H. Han,
779–86. Cambridge, MA: Academic Press, 24–44.
740 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

Smith, Rosanna K., George E. Newman, and Ravi Dhar (2016), Responses to Comparative Advertising,” Journal of
“Closer to the Creator: Temporal Contagion Explains the Consumer Research, 32 (4), 530–40.
Preference for Earlier Serial Numbers,” Journal of Consumer Tonietto, Gabriela N., Selin A. Malkoc, Rebecca Walker Reczek,
Research, 42 (5), 653–68. and Michael I. Norton (2021), “Viewing Leisure as Wasteful
Spencer, Steven J., Geoffrey T. Zanna, and Mark P. Fong (2005), Undermines Enjoyment,” Journal of Experimental Social
“Establishing a Causal Chain: Why Experiments Are Often Psychology, 97, 104198.
More Effective than Mediational Analyses in Examining Wadhwa, Monica and Kuangjie Zhang (2015), “This Number Just
Psychological Processes,” Journal of Personality and Social Feels Right: The Impact of Roundedness of Price Numbers
Psychology, 89 (6), 845–51. on Product Evaluations,” Journal of Consumer Research, 41
Spiller, Stephen A., Gavan J. Fitzsimons, John G. Lynch Jr, (5), 1172–85.
and Gary H. Mcclelland (2013), “Spotlights, Floodlights, Wang, Wenbo, Hean Tat Keh, and Lisa E. Bolton (2010), “Lay
and the Magic Number Zero: Simple Effects Tests in Theories of Medicine and a Healthy Lifestyle,” Journal of

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/49/5/721/6581083 by Jnls Cust Serv on 19 January 2023


Moderated Regression,” Journal of Marketing Research, Consumer Research, 37 (1), 80–97.
50 (2), 277–88. Williams, Lawrence E. and John A. Bargh (2008), “Experiencing
Strahilevitz, Michal John and G. Myers (1998), “Donations to Physical Warmth Promotes Interpersonal Warmth,” Science
Charity as Purchase Incentives: How Well They Work May (New York, N.Y.), 322 (5901), 606–7.
Depend on What You Are Trying to Sell,” Journal of Williams, Elanor F. and Mary Steffel (2014), “Double Standards
Consumer Research, 24 (4), 434–46. in the Use of Enhancing Products by Self and Others,”
Stuppy, Anika, Nicole L. Mead, and Stijn M. J. van Osselaer Journal of Consumer Research, 41 (2), 506–25.
(2020), “I Am, Therefore I Buy: Low Self-Esteem and the Zane, Daniel M., Robert W. Smith, and Rebecca Walker Reczek
Pursuit of Self-Verifying Consumption,” Journal of (2020), “The Meaning of Distraction: How Metacognitive
Consumer Research, 46 (5), 956–73. Inferences from Distraction during Multitasking Affect
Teeny, Jacob, Pablo Siev, Richard E. Bri~nol, and Joseph Petty Brand Evaluations,” Journal of Consumer Research, 46 (5),
(2021), “A Review and Conceptual Framework for 974–94.
Understanding Personalized Matching Effects in Persuasion,” Zheng, Yanmei, Lisa E. Bolton, and Joseph W. Alba (2019),
Journal of Consumer Psychology, 31 (2), 382–414. “Technology Resistance: The Case of Food Production
Thompson, Debora Viana and Rebecca W. Hamilton (2006), “The Processes,” Journal of Public Policy & Marketing, 38 (2),
Effects of Information Processing Mode on Consumers’ 246–62.
© 2023 Journal of Consumer Research Inc. Copyright of Journal of Consumer Research is the
property of Oxford University Press / USA and its content may not be copied or emailed to
multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission.
However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.

You might also like