Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 11

Received: 28 November 2017 | Accepted: 10 January 2018

DOI: 10.1111/1365-2656.12824

RESEARCH ARTICLE

Synergistic selection regimens drive the evolution of display


complexity in birds of paradise

Meredith C. Miles | Matthew J. Fuxjager

Department of Biology, Wake Forest


University, Winston-Salem, NC, USA Abstract
1. Integrated visual displays that combine gesture with colour are nearly ubiquitous
Correspondence
Meredith C. Miles in the animal world, where they are shaped by sexual selection for their role in
Email: milemc16@wfu.edu courtship and competition. However, few studies assess how multiple selection
Funding information regimens operate on different components of these complex phenotypes on a
National Science Foundation, Grant/Award macroevolutionary scale.
Number: IOS-1655730
2. Here, we study this issue by assessing how both sexual and ecological selection
Handling Editor: Elizabeth Derryberry work together to influence visual display complexity in the birds of paradise.
3. We first find that sexual dichromatism is highest in lekking species, which undergo
more intense sexual selection by female choice, than non-lekking species. At the
same time, species in which males directly compete with one another at commu-
nal display courts have more carotenoid-based ornaments and fewer melanin
ornaments.
4. Meanwhile, display habitat influences gestural complexity. Species that dance in
the cluttered understorey have more complex dances than canopy-displaying
species.
5. Taken together, our results illustrate how distinct selection regimens each operate
on individual elements comprising a complex display. This supports a modular
model of display evolution, wherein the ultimate integrated display is the product
of synergy between multiple factors that select for different types of phenotypic
complexity.

KEYWORDS
animal communication, behavioural ecology, display habitat, evolutionary ecology, lek, sexual
selection, signal design

1 | I NTRO D U C TI O N to complex displays, which consist of multiple signalling elements.


Complex displays should evolve in a modular fashion, where each
Animal displays are vital to mediating mate choice and competi- novel signal offers a new opportunity for one or more selection pres-
tion. As a result, these behaviours are thought to contribute to the sures to act (Hebets et al., 2016; Miles, Schuppe, Ligon, & Fuxjager,
process of speciation and thus play a fundamental role in shaping 2018). However, most work that examines multicomponent dis-
global patterns of biodiversity (Grant & Grant, 2010; Price, 1998). plays does so in single-­species systems (e.g., Candolin, 1999; Pryke,
Understanding how displays diverge in response to dynamic selec- Andersson, & Lawes, 2001; Smith et al., 2016; Stange, Page, Ryan,
tion regimens is therefore a major goal in biology. Work to date indi- & Taylor, 2016), an approach that makes it difficult to understand
cates that signal design is the product of multiple selection regimens the macroevolutionary consequences of modularity. There are rela-
operating in tandem (Endler, 1987; Seddon, 2005). This also applies tively few studies that take such a broad approach to this topic (but

J Anim Ecol. 2018;87:1149–1159. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/jane


© 2018 The Authors. Journal of Animal Ecology | 1149
© 2018 British Ecological Society
13652656, 2018, 4, Downloaded from https://besjournals.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/1365-2656.12824 by Welsh Assembly Government, Wiley Online Library on [23/06/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
1150 | Journal of Animal Ecology MILES and FUXJAGER

see Seddon, 2005; Tobias et al., 2010), which leaves a gap in our Lindsay et al., 2015). At the same time, many species undergo dif-
understanding of how multiple drivers act together to shape signal fering degrees of sexual selection by direct male–male competition
divergence. (intrasexual selection), wherein interactions among males determine
Some of the most widespread modular signals are integrated access to territories and mating rights (Andersson, Pryke, Ornborg,
visual displays, which combine physical gestures with colour or- Lawes, & Andersson, 2002; Ord, Blumstein, & Evans, 2001). Even
naments to communicate with signal receivers (Stuart-­Fox & Ord, among lekking species, the intensity of intrasexual selection is vari-
2004). Such displays are nearly ubiquitous in the animal world, used able, occupying a continuum that spans all-­out combat between
by a wide range of invertebrates (Green & Patek, 2015; Hebets & males (Gosling, Petrie, & Rainy, 1987) to species where males at
Uetz, 2000), fishes (Basolo & Alcaraz, 2003; Milinski & Bakker, 1990) dispersed leks only occasionally interact (Ryder, Parker, Blake, &
and tetrapods alike (Klomp, Stuart-­
Fox, Cassidy, Ahmad, & Ord, Loiselle, 2009). This means that some species evolve complex vi-
2017; Miles, Cheng, & Fuxjager, 2017). Each component of an inte- sual displays that are used both in courtship and in competition with
grated display can convey distinct and isolated information to the other males. How do these complementary mechanisms of sexual
receiver (Klomp et al., 2017; Kodric-­Brown, 1989; Price, Earnshaw, & selection, by female choice and male–male competition, interact to
Webster, 2006), or otherwise be combined to supersede the effect shape complex displays? One possibility is that inter-­and intrasex-
of each component alone (Girard, Elias, & Kasumovic, 2015; Hebets ual selection favour the exaggeration of different phenotypes and
& Uetz, 1999; Taylor & Ryan, 2013). As such, displays can become therefore act on different signal components. Alternatively, the
more elaborate either by introducing new signals to increase com- same signals may evolve to serve both functions, which means that
plexity, or by exaggerating existing elements (Hebets, Vink, Sullivan-­ display complexity would reflect the strength of sexual selection (by
Beckers, & Rosenthal, 2013; Lindsay, Houck, Giuliano, & Day, 2015). both mechanisms) that a species undergoes.
For example, sexual selection for colourful males encompasses a Sexual selection, however, does not operate in unchecked isola-
diverse range of evolutionary processes that generate dichroma- tion to drive display elaboration, as many constraints can impede this
tism in different ways. Among the many possibilities are exagger- process (Wilkins, Seddon, & Safran, 2013). One common constraint
ating the degree to which different melanins are deposited across is the signalling environment: to be evolutionarily stable, a signal
the body (Bókony, Liker, Székely, & Kis, 2003; Galván, 2008), and/or must be reliably transmitted through the environment to convey in-
increasing colour complexity by evolving novel metabolic pathways formation to the receiver (Endler, 1992). In this way, signal design
to process diet-­derived carotenoids into new hues (Badyaev, Hill, & is also subject to change based on how effectively it can be trans-
Weckworth, 2002; Goodwin, 1980; Ligon, Simpson, Mason, Hill, & mitted and perceived by the receiver, a phenomenon that has been
McGraw, 2016). Because these different pigment origins for colour explored extensively in studies of acoustic signalling (Seddon, 2005;
represent developmentally distinct pathways, they have the poten- Tobias et al., 2010). Signalling environment and habitat structure af-
tial to act as individual components of the modular visual display fect short-­range visual displays as well, because increasing environ-
(Hebets et al., 2016). Similarly, gestural signals might also undergo mental “noise” such as light availability or visual range disruption,
elaboration independent of colour ornaments because they are on- prompts the evolution of more detectable displays (Ord, Peters,
togenically distinct. If integrated visual displays are indeed modular, Clucas, & Stamps, 2007) and/or more sensitive receivers (Bloch,
then each component should be independently shaped by a distinct Morrow, Chang, & Price, 2015). Habitats associated with increased
selection regimen. Alternatively, signal components may evolve in visual clutter include the forest understorey, where dense herba-
a correlated manner, either by reinforcing or by constraining one ceous vegetation decreases visibility and light availability (Endler,
another independent of selection for display complexity (Mason, 1987; Endler & Thery, 1996). Indeed, species with a preference for
Shultz, & Burns, 2014; Rowe, 1999). this habitat have evolved remarkable colour ornaments (Marchetti,
Visual displays are primarily shaped by sexual selection, both via 1993) and even strategically orient iridescent patches to maximize
female choice and male–male competition. The former mechanism light transmittance (Schultz & Fincke, 2009). In a modular display
(intersexual selection) is the result of indirect competition among framework, it should be possible for signal components to be dif-
males for the opportunity to mate with choosy females, which re- ferentially influenced by the signalling environment in combination
sults in the evolution of progressively elaborate displays (Andersson, with sexual selection.
1994). Although most species undergo intersexual selection to some In this study, we explore how multiple evolutionary pressures
degree, the effects of female choice are at their most extreme in interact to shape complex visual display phenotypes in the birds
lekking species, which have an asymmetrical social mating system of paradise (Passeriformes: Paradisaeidae; Figure 1). These “most
where a minority of males ever get to mate (Borgia, 1979; Payne, beautiful and most wonderful of living things” (Wallace, 1869) rep-
1984; Widemo & Owensi, 1995). The underlying mechanisms of fe- resent the pinnacle of visual display evolution, exhibiting some of
male choice have been a subject of debate for decades—be it for the gaudiest colour and most complex dances in the animal world.
handicaps (Zahavi, 1975), motor skill and vigour (Byers, Hebets, & However, this family is not monolithic in its visual display complexity
Podos, 2010), or material benefits (Alatalo, Lundberg, & Glynn, or social structures; although birds of paradise are known for their
1986; Gray, 1997)—but mechanism aside, a common consequence polygyny and elaborate displays, many species are monogamous
of female choice is increased display complexity (Hebets et al., 2013; and employ more modest visual signals or do not display at all (Frith
13652656, 2018, 4, Downloaded from https://besjournals.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/1365-2656.12824 by Welsh Assembly Government, Wiley Online Library on [23/06/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
MILES and FUXJAGER Journal of Animal Ecology | 1151

& Beehler, 1998). Thus, the range of social mating systems in this the confounding environmental and biogeographic factors that can
family provides an excellent opportunity to investigate how female complicate our understanding of how displays diverge (Miles et al.,
choice influences visual display evolution. Similarly, even lekking 2017; Tobias et al., 2010), and makes understanding the effect of
species in the family exhibit uncommon social diversity. For example, display environment more straightforward—even though each spe-
many bird of paradise leks are highly dispersed and males seldom cies inhabits a similar habitat, they display in different forest strata.
interact with one another in a courtship context (Frith & Beehler, All this, combined with an evolutionary context shaped by isolated
1998; Irestedt, Jønsson, Fjeldså, Christidis, & Ericson, 2009). At the island radiation, makes the birds of paradise the ideal study system
other extreme, the males of other species congregate daily and dis- for tackling the challenge of evolutionary synergy driving display
play at one another even when a female is not present. Moreover, diversity.
this family is endemic to the humid forests of Wallacea, where it has Here we investigate how multiple drivers each shape the com-
remained since diverging from a crow-­like ancestor approximately plexity of integrated visual displays that comprise both colour and
10 million years ago (Jønsson et al., 2016). This eliminates many of gesture. If bird of paradise displays are indeed modular (i.e., multiple

F I G U R E 1 Phylogeny of the birds of paradise from Jønsson et al. (2016), with gestural display complexity (outer ring) and dichromatism
(inner ring) scores illustrated on a colour ramp. Blanks indicate species for which there was insufficient information to characterize display
complexity. Illustrations depict males performing their complex integrated visual displays, with species selected that best represent the
breadth of colour and display complexity found in the family [Colour figure can be viewed at wileyonlinelibrary.com]
13652656, 2018, 4, Downloaded from https://besjournals.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/1365-2656.12824 by Welsh Assembly Government, Wiley Online Library on [23/06/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
1152 | Journal of Animal Ecology MILES and FUXJAGER

evolutionary forces independently favour complexity of different provide parental care nor maintain an all-­purpose breeding territory
signal components), then indices of different selection pressures (instead they maintain separate arenas for courtship displays, which
should each predict directional variation in separate signals. can consist of either communal or dispersed courts; e.g., Borgia,
Specifically, we hypothesize that sexual selection by female choice 1979; Payne, 1984; Kirkpatrick, 1987; McDonald & Potts, 1994). This
and male–male competition, as well as the signalling environment, allowed us to approximately separate species undergoing moderate
will each predict the complexity of different display elements. As and extreme intersexual selection, as female choice drives lek evolu-
such, we first explore how proxies that reflect relative degrees of tion (Borgia, 1979; Payne, 1984; Widemo & Owensi, 1995).
female choice and male–male competition predict species-­wide vari- We next aimed to examine how degrees of sexual selection by di-
ation in both gesture and colour. Finally, we examine how display rect male–male competition predict differences in display complex-
microhabitat influences signal design. By comparing how fundamen- ity. Males that display in view of one another must compete directly
tal agents of evolution act on different signals, we aim to establish a for display space and time (and thus mating opportunities), and this
novel framework for understanding how synergistic selection regi- competition is mediated by not only male–male display behaviour,
mens facilitate the emergence of complex modular displays. but also even combat (Reynolds et al., 2009; Robel & Ballard, 1974;
Widemo & Owensi, 1995). By contrast, male birds of paradise that
display on a solitary court largely do not interact agonistically with
2 | M ATE R I A L S A N D M E TH O DS
other males on a regular basis (Frith & Beehler, 1998). Accordingly,
we considered species in which males display close to one another
2.1 | Experimental and statistical approach
(i.e. within visual range) to be undergoing more intense intrasexual
We adopted a hypothesis-­driven approach to examine three indi- selection, compared to species in which males only display alone.
ces of visual display complexity: sexual dichromatism, carotenoid Note that no birds of paradise display cooperatively, which is an al-
and melanin ornamentation, and gestural display complexity. In-­ ternate social system in which female mate choice is contingent on
depth descriptions of the methodology for each hypothesis follow coordinated displays between multiple males (McDonald & Potts,
in their respective sections, while here we describe the overall study 1994). We therefore dichotomously categorized species as either
design. To test how multiple proxies for selection pressure predict displaying with other males, or exclusively alone. Notably, our two
variation in our variables, we used phylogenetic comparative meth- indices of sexual selection exhibit little overlap; of the 36 birds of
ods (Felsenstein, 1985) that account for non-­independence of com- paradise, 26 species have a lek mating system. Of those lekking spe-
parative datasets. Therefore, all analyses incorporate a published cies, 14 have dispersed leks where males display alone and 12 have
phylogeny that reflects our most up-­to-­date understanding of the communal display courts where 2–20 males display together. All
avian radiation that includes birds of paradise (Figure 1; Jønsson non-­lekking species except for one (Manucodia comrii) have solitary-­
et al., 2016). For each of our dependent variables, we first fit con- displaying males.
tinuous trait models (Table S1) using the “fitContinuous” function Finally, we investigated how display habitat affects display com-
in the r package geiger (Harmon, Weir, Brock, Glor, & Challenger, plexity. Habitat structure can impose different challenges to signal
2008) to determine appropriate branch length transformations to transmission and perception by the receiver, particularly between
use in each analysis. All of our models use categorical predictors open (low clutter) and closed (high clutter) habitats (Endler, 1992;
(indices of sexual selection intensity and display habitat) and a con- Ord & Stamps, 2008; Tobias et al., 2010). All birds of paradise inhabit
tinuous response variable (complexity scores), so we used phyloge- the closed humid tropical forest. However, species preferences for
netic ANOVAs (Garland, Dickerman, Janis, & Jones, 1993) to test display courts are more variable, ranging from the dense understo-
for associations between the two. Finally, we only report significant rey to canopy, where tree gaps are more common (Endler & Thery,
p-­values that have been corrected to account for multiple testing 1996; Frith & Beehler, 1998). We classified species by the forest
(Holm, 1988). strata in which they display using a discrete index, because accounts
of display habitat are supplied in metres for some species and qual-
itative terms (e.g., “high in the canopy,” “on the ground”) for others.
2.2 | Independent variables
This allowed for easy delineation of species described qualitatively,
In each of our models, we use categorical independent variables and we followed a previously published forest stratum classification
that reflect variation in the degree to which a species undergoes in- for New Guinea birds to determine display habitat for quantitatively
tersexual and intrasexual selection, as well as display habitat. This described species (Bell, 1982). Briefly, this system considers species
information was derived using authoritative species accounts docu- ≤3 m from the forest floor to inhabit the understorey, ≥10 m from
menting bird of paradise natural history (Frith & Beehler, 1998). We the forest floor to be in the canopy, and considers species between
first aimed to explore how variation in intersexual selection pressure 3 and 10 m to be in the midstorey. However, due to the low number
predicted differences in display complexity. To do this, we used a of midstorey-­displaying species (n = 6) and the marked difference in
dichotomous classification of social mating system that distinguishes vegetation structure between the understorey and all higher forest
only between lekking and non-­lekking species. We defined lekking strata (Bell, 1982; Montgomery & Chazdon, 2001; Wright, 2002), we
species as those in which males mate with many females, and neither grouped midstorey and canopy species into a single “upper strata”
13652656, 2018, 4, Downloaded from https://besjournals.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/1365-2656.12824 by Welsh Assembly Government, Wiley Online Library on [23/06/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
MILES and FUXJAGER Journal of Animal Ecology | 1153

category to compare to understorey counterparts. This dichotomous in tetrahedral colour space; Stoddard & Prum, 2008), because our
habitat classification still has biological relevance, as those species index more closely characterizes complexity, or the number of novel
displaying in the mid-­and upper stories seek out exposed perches components added to the display (Hebets et al., 2013; Miles et al.,
at the forest edge or at a tree’s crown, while species displaying in 2017). Although humans and birds do not perceive the same range of
the understorey maintain display courts on or near the ground and colours, previous work shows that human vision is a valid and effec-
among dense herbaceous vegetation (Frith & Beehler, 1998). tive proxy for quantifying avian coloration and making robust evo-
lutionary inferences (Seddon, Tobias, Eaton, Ödeen, & Byers, 2010).
As such, we adapted methodology from Ornelas et al. (2009) to
2.3 | Gesture complexity scores
compute dichromatism across species. Briefly, using highly accurate
To characterize gestural complexity of each species, we followed a handbook plates as a reference (del Hoyo et al., 1999), we divided
literature-­based numerical scoring index that has been used previ- each bird into 15 distinct plumage regions (Figure S1) and had an in-
ously (Fuxjager et al., 2015; Lindsay et al., 2015; Miles et al., 2017). dividual blind to our study aims compare males and females of each
This index uses published display descriptions to distinguish be- species. Then, each male plumage region was scored based on its
tween unique motor components of a display. For example, many similarity or difference from the female—similar regions received a
species perform a wing display, but some pump the whole wing with score of 0; regions in which a colour was qualitatively different but
the pectoralis and supracoracoideus muscles, whereas others extend shared a fundamental pigment or structural basis received a score
only the wrist using carpal extensors and flexors; and yet others still of 1; and regions that introduced a new mode of coloration received
will incorporate both gestures into their display. Therefore, we con- a score of 2. Again following Ornelas et al. (2009), we classified co-
sider these two wing displays to be unique gestures. lour mode using a conservative system where we considered blacks,
We define display complexity as the sum of discrete physical out- browns and tans, as well as dilute yellows (“buff”) and red-­browns
puts incorporated into a species’ gestural display repertoire (as in (“rufous”) to be melanin-­based, which is consistent with our under-
Fuxjager et al., 2015; Lindsay et al., 2015; Miles et al., 2017). In this standing of avian coloration generated from both eumelanins and
way, display complexity scores are computed by awarding a species phaeomelanins (Hill & McGraw, 2006). Saturated yellows, oranges
1 point for every unique physical manoeuvre it performs as part of and reds we considered to be carotenoid-­based (Cardoso & Mota,
the total dance routine. By adhering to a priori delineations of each 2008; Ligon et al., 2016). Structural coloration can exist on plumage
gesture (Table S1), we were able to avoid double-­counting manoeu- with either pigment, generating colour in the violet-­blue-­teal range
vres. We used published species accounts of display repertoires on melanin pigment and green when combined with carotenoids
(Frith & Beehler, 1998) augmented with information from later expe- (Maia, Rubenstein, & Shawkey, 2016), so we considered colours in
ditions (del Hoyo, Elliott, & Sargatal, 1999; Laman & Scholes, 2012) the violet-­green range to be structural (Figure S1). The result of this
to describe and categorize individual gestures for each taxa. Tallying indexing method is a score for each species (Table S2) that can po-
presence (1 point) or absence (0 points) of each gesture in a species, tentially range from 0 (sexually monomorphic) to 30 (every plumage
and then adding the total score, results in the species gestural com- region changes colour mechanism). As above, all colour data were
plexity score (Table S2). As such, a total score of 0 denotes a species gathered by an individual blind to the study objectives. To validate
that performs no gestural displays whatsoever, and higher scores repeatability of the dichromatism index, a second individual (again
denote species with more complex display repertoires. Because this blind to our hypotheses) independently corroborated the scoring
study’s focus concerns display complexity and not elaboration, we process. Both scores were highly correlated (R2 = .983, p < .0001).
do not account for qualitative differences in gesture performance We further probed visual signal diversity by examining pigment
across species (thus, two species that perform the bow gesture each origins of male coloration. To do this, we followed the scheme out-
receive a score of 1, regardless of the depth and speed of the mo- line above to determine whether each male plumage patch either
tion). To eliminate bias, an individual blind to our hypotheses com- (1) incorporated carotenoid coloration, or (2) only expressed mela-
puted all display complexity scores. Because complexity scoring is nin pigment (only one species, Cicinnurus regius, has unpigmented
based on human interpretation of the literature, a second individ- plumage regions). We then calculated the proportion of carotenoid
( )
ual (similarly blind to the study motives) independently recomputed patches relative to the total number of plumage regions total .
ncarotenoid
patches
complexity scores to ensure that our methodology was repeatable. This results in a proportion between 0 and 1, where species that
Indeed, we had a strong correlation between the two sets of scores score near 1 use carotenoid-­based colours on a greater number of
2
(R = .987, p < .0001). plumage patches compared to the majority melanin species that
score closer to 0. Carotenoid proportion therefore is inversely re-
lated to the extent of colour generated by melanin (melanin pro-
2.4 | Quantifying coloration
portion = 1 – carotenoid proportion) in all species except Cicinnurus
We quantified sexual dichromatism using a published score-­based regius. We therefore ran additional analyses using melanin propor-
index based on human observation (Gray, 1996; Ornelas, González, tion as a dependent variable, but these data are not included in
& Espinosa de los Monteros, 2009). We chose to use this index figures due to redundancy. We also did not evaluate structural col-
over measures of plumage brightness (e.g., quantifying position oration, because it exists on a subtle continuum from matte feathers
13652656, 2018, 4, Downloaded from https://besjournals.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/1365-2656.12824 by Welsh Assembly Government, Wiley Online Library on [23/06/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
1154 | Journal of Animal Ecology MILES and FUXJAGER

(no structural colour), to glossy colours, to new colours introduced fit with a lambda transformation (AICc = 223.3, λ = 0.856), and kappa
altogether (Maia, D’Alba, & Shawkey, 2011). As such, it is nearly im- processes underlie both carotenoid proportion (AICc = −25.28,
possible to assess structural coloration through the lens of complex- κ = 0.44) and melanin proportion (AICc = −21.11, κ = 0.15). For each
ity using handbook plates, as our index could not account for subtle of these dependent variables, we therefore supplied trees with the
innovations in structural modifications. ­appropriate branch length transformation to statistical models.
We first found that sexual dichromatism changed among species
with different social mating systems (Figure 2a), where lekking spe-
3 | R E S U LT S cies had significantly higher dichromatism scores than non-­lekking
species (F1,34 = 24.628, p = .033, λ = 0.857). However, there was
We found there to be high diversity in both gesture complexity and no difference among lekking and non-­
lekking species for carot-
dichromatism scores across the birds of paradise (Figure 1), and there enoid proportion (F1,34 = 15.53, p = .100, κ = 0.44), melanin propor-
was no correlation between gesture complexity and sexual dichro- tion (F1,34 = 18.69, p = .100, κ = 0.15) or gestural complexity score
matism (F1,34 = 0.1287, R2 = .0263, p = .722). When fitting a series of (F1,34 = 14.671, p = .114).
continuous trait models to each variable (Table S3), we found that Next, species in which multiple males display at a single court
each one was best fit by Brownian motion (BM) or a modification had significantly more male carotenoid ornaments than those that
thereof. Specifically, gestural complexity scores appear to evolve display alone (Figure 2b; F1,34 = 17.610, p = .006, κ = 0.44). The re-
under full BM (AICc = 197.35), sexual dichromatism scores are best verse was true for melanin patch proportion, as solitary-­displaying

F I G U R E 2 Elements of visual display complexity as they vary among lekking (low intersexual selection pressure) and non-­lekking (high
intersexual selection) species (a); by the number of males displaying together (b), which indicates intrasexual selection pressure; and by
display habitat (c). Statistically significant differences are indicated with an asterisk (*), whereas non-­significant comparisons read n.s. Note
that because values were compared using phylogenetic ANOVA, what appear to be qualitative differences (as in carotenoid ornamentation
among lekking and non-­lekking species) are not always significant, because these trait differences are indistinguishable from discrepancies
produced by evolutionary history alone [Colour figure can be viewed at wileyonlinelibrary.com]
13652656, 2018, 4, Downloaded from https://besjournals.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/1365-2656.12824 by Welsh Assembly Government, Wiley Online Library on [23/06/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
MILES and FUXJAGER Journal of Animal Ecology | 1155

males had higher melanin proportion scores (F1,34 = 17.84, p = .008,


κ = 0.15). However, there was no such difference for dichromatism
(F1,34 = 2.024, p = .333, λ = 0.857) or gestural display complexity
scores (F1,34 = 1.822, p = .180).
Finally, species that display in the forest understorey had greater
gestural complexity scores than those that dance in upper strata
(Figure 2c; F1,34 = 15.471, p = .005). Meanwhile, display stratum
does not explain variation in male coloration, either in total colour
complexity score (F = 1.591,34, p = .526, λ = 0.857) or in the relative
amount of carotenoid patches (F1,34 = 0.79, p = .526, κ = 0.44) or
melanin patches (F1,34 = 0.79, p = .526, κ = 0.15).

4 | D I S CU S S I O N

By breaking down complex visual displays into their constituent


parts, we dissect the diverse evolutionary pressures that shape the F I G U R E 3 Signal space schematic depicting how female choice
(red arrow), direct male–male competition (blue arrow) and display
complexity of the display’s individual components. We first show
habitat (orange arrow) each influence visual displays in birds of
that lekking species (which undergo extreme levels of intersexual
paradise. The four plotted points correspond to four representative
selection) are more sexually dichromatic than non-­lekking species, species that exemplify these effects: (1) Manucodia chalybatus, a
without respect to the pigment origin of male coloration. In other non-­lekking species that displays alone in the canopy, is sexually
words, species undergoing a stronger level of sexual selection by monochromatic and performs a relatively simple display, (2) male
female choice are more dichromatic, compared to species subject Astrapia rothschildi, which display in the canopy at a solitary lek,
are highly sexually dichromatic, (3) Paradisaea rubra gather in large
to a more moderate degree of intersexual selection. Meanwhile,
numbers to display in canopy leks and employ numerous carotenoid
species in which multiple males display together have more red and ornaments, and (4) Parotia carolae have the most complex dance
yellow ornaments (and less apparent melanin coloration) than those routines of all, performing in groups of up to 6 on their forest floor
that display alone. Because males that display together engage in leks [Colour figure can be viewed at wileyonlinelibrary.com]
direct competition for display space and dispersed males do not, this
means that aggregate displayers should be experiencing a stronger answer may lie in dichromatic “innovations”, as males with a rare
degree of intrasexual selection. Therefore, our data suggest that colour pattern can experience elevated mating success (Hughes,
shifting intrasexual selection pressure is associated with changes in Houde, Price, & Rodd, 2013). A similar mechanism could be respon-
carotenoid ornamentation. Finally, we find an association between sible here, as lekking species are more sexually dichromatic (i.e., the
display habitat and gestural display complexity, where species that males have more putative novel colours) than non-­lekking species.
signal in the forest understorey have more complex dances than In other words, the species for which female choice is at its most ex-
those that signal in the canopy and midstorey. This implies that habi- treme are also those species where males have evolved more plum-
tat does indeed influence gestural display complexity. Altogether, age ornaments relative to females. Alternatively, female choice may
these findings point to three distinct evolutionary drivers operating still operate on specific colours in birds of paradise, but this might
in tandem to guide divergence in three different components of an vary in direction among species. This idea is supported by the fact
integrated visual display phenotype (Figure 3). Bird of paradise dis- that plumage can be highly labile (Omland & Lanyon, 2000), and sex-
plays therefore exhibit modularity (Hebets et al., 2016), because in- ual traits persist due to parallel changes in both the male signalling
dividual signal components are modified independently despite their phenotype and female preference for that signal (Servedio, 2016).
function to the integrated visual display. This implies that the total Indeed, effective colour signals used in mate choice are found in
complexity of integrated displays is the result of multiple evolution- melanin (Siefferman & Hill, 2003), carotenoid (Friedman, McGraw, &
ary forces acting synergistically, or in tandem, rather than any one Omland, 2014) and structural colours alike (Keyser, 2000).
operating in isolation. Meanwhile, birds of paradise undergoing intense direct male–
One of the most interesting insights from our data is that the male competition tend to have more carotenoid-­based ornaments
bright plumage of male birds of paradise evolved in response to and fewer melanin patches. This suggests that red and yellow or-
sexual selection by both female choice and male–male competition naments may function in direct male–male competition, possibly
operating on different colours. Indeed, we find that overall dichro- because carotenoid-­based ornaments act as an evolutionarily stable
matism is associated with increasing intersexual selection pressure, mediator of male–male interactions due to their tendency to serve
while aggregate display dynamics predict a shift from melanin-­to as an honest signal (Ligon et al., 2016; Pryke & Andersson, 2003). In
carotenoid-­
based coloration. Why would two indices of sexual passerines like birds of paradise, these colours are exclusively gen-
selection predict different types of colour ornament shifts? One erated by diet-­derived carotenoid pigments (Goodwin, 1980). This
13652656, 2018, 4, Downloaded from https://besjournals.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/1365-2656.12824 by Welsh Assembly Government, Wiley Online Library on [23/06/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
1156 | Journal of Animal Ecology MILES and FUXJAGER

means that generating red and yellow ornaments relies not only on is only one potential solution to signalling in a challenging envi-
an individual’s ability to acquire food, but also on converting dietary ronment; alternative routes include enhancing highly directional
forms into usable pigments along numerous metabolic pathways iridescent ornaments (White et al., 2015), or even modifying the
(Cardoso & Mota, 2008; Ligon et al., 2016). As a result, carotenoid signalling environment itself to maximize light transmission to the
colours sometimes function as condition-­dependent badges used to display court (Uy & Endler, 2004). Indeed, both of these traits are
negotiate male–male competition (Peek, 1972; Pryke & Andersson, common in birds of paradise, although they are shared by under-
2003). This may also not be the case, however, as carotenoid color- storey and canopy displayers alike (Frith & Beehler, 1998).
ation is not the only route to a condition-­dependent ornament. For Importantly, bird of paradise displays are multimodal as well as
example, both melanin and structural coloration can also be honest complex, incorporating many non-­visual cues that could also func-
signals due to condition dependence (Roulin, 2016; Siefferman & tion as redundant messages (Rowe, 1999). For example, most species
Hill, 2003), so we cannot rule out the possibility that these alternate call extensively before dancing, often using a different perch specif-
modes may also play some role. ically for vocalizing. Some birds of paradise incorporate tactile and
Birds of paradise are perhaps best known for their bizarre court- even olfactory signalling into their displays as well, which further ex-
ship dances, but our indices of sexual selection do not predict any pands the opportunities for sexual selection to elaborate the signal
variation in gestural display complexity scores. This does not mean (Frith & Beehler, 1998). Therefore, the integrated visual display itself
that gesture is unimportant to mate choice in birds of paradise, of is yet another component embedded in a larger multimodal court-
course; for these species, gesture appears to be a foundational ship display, which may evolve in surprising ways. For example, co-
element of mate choice, with females closely observing and even lour and gesture complexity appear to evolve independently (rather
probing displaying males before deciding to copulate or visit an- than by transfer from one signal to another Mason et al., 2014).
other display court (Frith & Beehler, 1998; Laman & Scholes, 2012; However, visual display complexity may still interact differently with
Scholes, 2008). One explanation for our data is that female birds of other multimodal signals.
paradise likely evaluate gesture on some other qualitative basis, per-
haps of motor skill or vigour (Byers et al., 2010), which may influence
phenotypic patterning of individual gesture exaggeration and/or co- 5 | CO N C LU S I O N S
ordination instead of complexity. Studies in other avian taxa support
this point of view, showing that fraction-­of-­a-­second differences in Altogether, these results offer novel insight into the macroevolu-
gestural performance indeed influence female mate choice (Barske, tionary pattern of visual display design: different signal components
Schlinger, Wikelski, & Fusani, 2011; Manica, Macedo, Graves, & can be simultaneously and independently influenced by multiple
Podos, 2016; Schuppe & Fuxjager, 2018). core evolutionary processes, which interact to produce the ultimate
We also find that gestural complexity is best explained by integrated visual display. This highlights the remarkable potential of
display habitat, where males displaying in the forest understorey diverse selection regimens, often viewed as discrete operators, to
perform more complex dance routines than those that display in instead function independently on a single signal component, while
the upper strata. This phenomenon is likely rooted in the com- still interacting with other processes to shape overall complexity.
plex interactions between signal production, transmission and The result is an integrated display that conveys more information
perception by the receiver, as sexual selection can only act on than the sum of its constituent signals—and, in the case of extraor-
signals that can be reliably propagated through the environment dinary bird of paradise visual displays, exemplifies the diversifying
(Endler, 1992). Although few studies investigate the effect of sig- potential of animal behaviour.
nalling environment on gesture, previous work shows that anoles
similarly overcome visual “noise” in a wind-­disturbed habitat by
AC K N OW L E D G E M E N T S
introducing new gestures to their display (Ord et al., 2007). In this
case, a broad range of environmental factors may inhibit signal The work was funded by NSF grant IOS-­1655730 to M.J.F. The au-
perception. Among dense (understorey) or open (upper strata) thors have no conflicts of interest to declare.
display microhabitats, potential sources of ecological pressure for
increased complexity may include physical breakup of the scene
AU T H O R S ’ C O N T R I B U T I O N S
by foliage (Endler, 1993; Théry, 2001), restricted light availability
(White, Zeil, & Kemp, 2015) and/or visual distance from the sig- M.C.M. and M.J.F. conceived of and designed the study, analysed
naller (Fleishman, 1992). Alternatively, the difference in gestural the data and wrote the manuscript. Both authors gave final approval
complexity across display habitats may be a product of physiolog- of the study for publication.
ical and/or morphological constraints associated with dancing on
different perching substrates; while most understorey species dis-
DATA AC C E S S I B I L I T Y
play on the ground or on vertical perches, canopy-­displaying spe-
cies exclusively use larger perches. How does evolution proceed in Data from this study are available online at the Dryad Digital R
­ epository:
the face of these environmental challenges? Using more gestures https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.9m47pj0 (Miles & Fuxjager, 2018).
13652656, 2018, 4, Downloaded from https://besjournals.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/1365-2656.12824 by Welsh Assembly Government, Wiley Online Library on [23/06/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
MILES and FUXJAGER Journal of Animal Ecology | 1157

ORCID forest-­dwelling birds. The American Naturalist, 148, 421–452. https://


doi.org/10.1086/285934
Meredith C. Miles http://orcid.org/0000-0002-7307-0195 Felsenstein, J. (1985). Phylogenies and the comparative method. The
American Naturalist, 125, 1–15. https://doi.org/10.1086/284325
Matthew J. Fuxjager https://orcid.org/0000-0003-0591-6854
Fleishman, L. J. (1992). The influence of the sensory system and the en-
vironment on motion patterns in the visual displays of anoline lizards
and other vertebrates. The American Naturalist, 139, S36. https://doi.
REFERENCES org/10.1086/285304
Friedman, N. R., McGraw, K. J., & Omland, K. E. (2014). Evolution of
Alatalo, R. V., Lundberg, A., & Glynn, C. (1986). Female pied flycatchers carotenoid pigmentation in caciques and meadowlarks (Icteridae):
choose territory quality and not male characteristics. Nature, 323, Repeated gains of red plumage coloration by carotenoid C4-­
152–153. https://doi.org/10.1038/323152a0 oxygenation. Evolution, 68, 791–801. https://doi.org/10.1111/
Andersson, M. B. (1994). Sexual selection. Princeton, NJ: Princeton evo.12304
University Press. Frith, C. B., & Beehler, B. M. (1998). The birds of paradise. Oxford, UK:
Andersson, S., Pryke, S. R., Ornborg, J., Lawes, M. J., & Andersson, M. Oxford University Press.
(2002). Multiple receivers, multiple ornaments, and a trade-­off be- Fuxjager, M. J., Eaton, J., Lindsay, W. R., Salwiczek, L. H., Rensel, M. A.,
tween agonistic and epigamic signaling in a widowbird. The American Barske, J., … Schlinger, B. A. (2015). Evolutionary patterns of adaptive
Naturalist, 160, 683–691. acrobatics and physical performance predict expression profiles of
Badyaev, A. V., Hill, G. E., & Weckworth, B. V. (2002). Species divergence androgen receptor – but not oestrogen receptor – in the forelimb
in sexually selected traits: Increase in song elaboration is related to musculature (ed W Hopkins). Functional Ecology, 29, 1197–1208.
decrease in plumage ornamentation in finches. Evolution, 56, 412– https://doi.org/10.1111/1365-2435.12438
419. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0014-3820.2002.tb01350.x Galván, I. (2008). The importance of white on black: Unmelanized
Barske, J., Schlinger, B. A., Wikelski, M., & Fusani, L. (2011). Female plumage proportion predicts display complexity in birds. Behavioral
choice for male motor skills. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Ecology and Sociobiology, 63, 303–311. https://doi.org/10.1007/
Biological Sciences, 278, 3523–3528. https://doi.org/10.1098/ s00265-008-0662-9
rspb.2011.0382 Garland, T., Dickerman, A. W., Janis, C. M., & Jones, J. A. (1993).
Basolo, A. L., & Alcaraz, G. (2003). The turn of the sword: Length in- Phylogenetic analysis of covariance by computer simula-
creases male swimming costs in swordtails. Proceedings of the tion. Systematic Biology, 42, 265–292. https://doi.org/10.1093/
Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 270, 1631–1636. https://doi. sysbio/42.3.265
org/10.1098/rspb.2003.2388 Girard, M. B., Elias, D. O., & Kasumovic, M. M. (2015). Female preference
Bell, H. (1982). A bird community of lowland rainforest in New Guinea. for multi-­modal courtship: Multiple signals are important for male
I. Composition and density of the avifauna. Emu, 82, 24. https://doi. mating success in peacock spiders. Proceedings of the Royal Society
org/10.1071/MU9820024 B: Biological Sciences, 282, 20152222. https://doi.org/10.1098/
Bloch, N. I., Morrow, J. M., Chang, B. S. W., & Price, T. D. (2015). SWS2 vi- rspb.2015.2222
sual pigment evolution as a test of historically contingent patterns of Goodwin, T. W. (1980). The biochemistry of the carotenoids. Dordrecht, the
plumage color evolution in warblers. Evolution, 69, 341–356. https:// Netherlands: Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-009-5860-9
doi.org/10.1111/evo.12572 Gosling, L. M., Petrie, M., & Rainy, M. E. (1987). Lekking in topi: A high
Bókony, V., Liker, A., Székely, T., & Kis, J. (2003). Melanin-­based plumage cost, specialist strategy. Animal Behaviour, 35, 616–618. https://doi.
coloration and flight displays in plovers and allies. Proceedings of the org/10.1016/S0003-3472(87)80296-8
Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 270, 2491–2497. https:// Grant, B. R., & Grant, P. R. (2010). Songs of Darwin’s finches diverge
doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2003.2506 when a new species enters the community. Proceedings of the
Borgia, G. (1979). Sexual selection and the evolution of mating systems. National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107,
In M. S. Blum, & N. A. Blum (Eds.), Sexual selection and reproductive 20156–20163. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1015115107
competition in insects (pp. 19–80). New York, NY: Academic Press. Gray, D. A. (1996). Carotenoids and sexual dichromatism in North
Byers, J., Hebets, E., & Podos, J. (2010). Female mate choice based upon American passerine birds. The American Naturalist, 148, 453–480.
male motor performance. Animal Behaviour, 79, 771–778. https://doi. https://doi.org/10.1086/285935
org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.01.009 Gray, E. M. (1997). Female red-­winged blackbirds accrue material bene-
Candolin, U. (1999). Male-­male competition facilitates female choice in fits from copulating with extra-­pair males. Animal Behavior, 53, 625–
sticklebacks. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 639. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1996.0336
266, 785–789. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.1999.0706 Green, P. A., & Patek, S. N. (2015). Contests with deadly weapons:
Cardoso, G. C., & Mota, P. G. (2008). Speciational evolution of color- Telson sparring in mantis shrimp (Stomatopoda). Biology Letters, 11,
ation in the genus Carduelis. Evolution, 62, 753–762. https://doi. 20150558. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2015.0558
org/10.1111/j.1558-5646.2008.00337.x Harmon, L. J., Weir, J. T., Brock, C. D., Glor, R. E., & Challenger, W. (2008).
del Hoyo, J., Elliott, A., & Sargatal, J. (1999). Handbook of the birds of the GEIGER: Investigating evolutionary radiations. Bioinformatics, 24,
world, vol. 5. Barcelona, Spain: Lynx Edicions. 129–131. https://doi.org/10.1093/bioinformatics/btm538
Endler, J. A. (1987). Predation, light intensity and courtship behaviour in Hebets, E. A., Barron, A. B., Balakrishnan, C. N., Hauber, M. E., Mason,
Poecilia reticulata (Pisces: Poeciliidae). Animal Behaviour, 35, 1376– P. H., & Hoke, K. L. (2016). A systems approach to animal commu-
1385. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(87)80010-6 nication. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 283,
Endler, J. A. (1992). Signals, signal conditions, and the direction of 20152889. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2015.2889
evolution. The American Naturalist, 139, S125–S153. https://doi. Hebets, E. A., & Uetz, G. W. (1999). Female responses to isolated signals
org/10.1086/285308 from multimodal male courtship displays in the wolf spider genus
Endler, J. A. (1993). The color of light in forests and its implications. Schizocosa (Araneae: Lycosidae). Animal Behaviour, 57, 865–872.
Ecological Monographs, 63, 1–27. https://doi.org/10.2307/2937121 https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1998.1048
Endler, J. A., & Thery, M. (1996). Interacting effects of lek placement, dis- Hebets, E. A., & Uetz, G. W. (2000). Leg ornamentation and the effi-
play behavior, ambient light, and color patterns in three neotropical cacy of courtship display in four species of wolf spider (Araneae:
13652656, 2018, 4, Downloaded from https://besjournals.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/1365-2656.12824 by Welsh Assembly Government, Wiley Online Library on [23/06/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
1158 | Journal of Animal Ecology MILES and FUXJAGER

Lycosidae). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 47, 280–286. https:// McDonald, D. B., & Potts, W. K. (1994). Cooperative display and relat-
doi.org/10.1007/s002650050667 edness among males in a lek-­mating bird. Science, 266, 1030–1032.
Hebets, E. A., Vink, C. J., Sullivan-Beckers, L., & Rosenthal, M. F. (2013). https://doi.org/10.1126/science.7973654
The dominance of seismic signaling and selection for signal com- Miles, M. C., Cheng, S., & Fuxjager, M. J. (2017). Biogeography predicts
plexity in Schizocosa multimodal courtship displays. Behavioral macro-­evolutionary patterning of gestural display complexity in a
Ecology and Sociobiology, 67, 1483–1498. https://doi.org/10.1007/ passerine family. Evolution, 71, 1406–1416. https://doi.org/10.1111/
s00265-013-1519-4 evo.13213
Hill, G. E., & McGraw, K. J. (2006). Bird coloration: Mechanisms and mea- Miles, M. C., & Fuxjager, F. J. (2018). Data from: Synergistic selection reg-
surements. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. imens drive the evolution of display complexity in birds of paradise.
Holm, S. (1988). A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure. Dryad Digital Repository, https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.9m47pj0
Scandinavian Journal of Statistics, 6, 65–70. Miles, M. C., Schuppe, E. R., Ligon, R. M., & Fuxjager, M. J. (2018).
Hughes, K. A., Houde, A. E., Price, A. C., & Rodd, F. H. (2013). Mating Macroevolutionary patterning of woodpecker drums reveals how
advantage for rare males in wild guppy populations. Nature, 503, sexual selection elaborates signals under constraint. Proceedings
108–110. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature12717 of the Royal Society B, 285, 20172628. https://doi.org/10.1098/
Irestedt, M., Jønsson, K. A., Fjeldså, J., Christidis, L., & Ericson, P. rspb.2017.2628
G. (2009). An unexpectedly long history of sexual selection in Milinski, M., & Bakker, T. C. M. (1990). Female sticklebacks use male col-
birds-­of-­paradise. BMC Evolutionary Biology, 9, 235. https://doi. oration in mate choice and hence avoid parasitized males. Nature,
org/10.1186/1471-2148-9-235 344, 330–333. https://doi.org/10.1038/344330a0
Jønsson, K. A., Fabre, P.-H., Kennedy, J. D., Holt, B. G., Borregaard, Montgomery, R. A., & Chazdon, R. L. (2001). Forest structure, canopy
M. K., Rahbek, C., & Fjeldså, J. (2016). A supermatrix phy- architecture, and light transmittance in tropical wet forests. Ecology,
logeny of corvoid passerine birds (Aves: Corvides). Molecular 82, 2707–2718. https://doi.org/10.1890/0012-9658(2001)082[270
Phylogenetics and Evolution, 94, 87–94. https://doi.org/10.1016/ 7:FSCAAL]2.0.CO;2
j.ympev.2015.08.020 Omland, K. E., & Lanyon, S. M. (2000). Reconstructing plum-
Keyser, A. J. (2000). Structurally based plumage coloration is an hon- age evolution in orioles (Icterus): Repeated convergence and
est signal of quality in male blue grosbeaks. Behavioral Ecology, 11, reversal in patterns. Evolution, 54, 2119–2133. https://doi.
202–209. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/11.2.202 org/10.1111/j.0014-3820.2000.tb01254.x
Kirkpatrick, M. (1987). Sexual selection by female choice in polygynous Ord, T. J., Blumstein, D. T., & Evans, C. S. (2001). Intrasexual selection
animals. Annual Review of Evology Systematics, 18, 43–70. https://doi. predicts the evolution of signal complexity in lizards. Proceedings
org/10.1146/annurev.es.18.110187.000355 of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 268, 737–744. https://doi.
Klomp, D. A., Stuart-Fox, D., Cassidy, E. J., Ahmad, N., & Ord, T. J. (2017). org/10.1098/rspb.2000.1417
Color pattern facilitates species recognition but not signal detection: Ord, T. J., Peters, R. A., Clucas, B., & Stamps, J. A. (2007). Lizards
A field test using robots. Behavioral Ecology, 218, 1556–1563. speed up visual displays in noisy motion habitats. Proceedings of
Kodric-Brown, A. (1989). Dietary carotenoids and male mating success in the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 274, 1057–1062. https://doi.
the guppy: An environmental component to female choice. Behavioral org/10.1098/rspb.2006.0263
Ecology and Sociobiology, 25, 393–401. https://doi.org/10.1007/ Ord, T. J., & Stamps, J. A. (2008). Alert signals enhance animal communi-
BF00300185 cation in “noisy” environments. Proceedings of the National Academy
Laman, T., & Scholes, E. (2012). Birds of paradise: Revealing the world’s most of Sciences of the United States of America, 105, 18830–18835. https://
extraordinary birds. Washington, DC: National Geographic Books. doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0807657105
Ligon, R. A., Simpson, R. K., Mason, N. A., Hill, G. E., & McGraw, K. J. Ornelas, J. F., González, C., & Espinosa de los Monteros, A. (2009).
(2016). Evolutionary innovation and diversification of carotenoid-­ Uncorrelated evolution between vocal and plumage coloration traits
based pigmentation in finches. Evolution, 70, 2839–2852. https://doi. in the trogons: A comparative study. Journal of Evolutionary Biology,
org/10.1111/evo.13093 22, 471–484. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1420-9101.2008.01679.x
Lindsay, W. R., Houck, J. T., Giuliano, C. E., & Day, L. B. (2015). Payne, R. B. (1984). Sexual selection, lek and arena behavior, and sex-
Acrobatic courtship display coevolves with brain size in manak- ual size dimorphism in birds. Ornithological Monographs, 33, iii–52.
ins (Pipridae). Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 85, 29–36. https://doi. https://doi.org/10.2307/40166729
org/10.1159/000369244 Peek, F. W. (1972). An experimental study of the territorial function of
Maia, R., D’Alba, L., & Shawkey, M. D. (2011). What makes a feather vocal and visual display in the male red-­winged blackbird (Agelaius
shine? A nanostructural basis for glossy black colours in feathers. phoeniceus). Animal Behaviour, 20, 112–118. https://doi.org/10.1016/
Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 278, 1973– S0003-3472(72)80180-5
1980. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2010.1637 Price, T. (1998). Sexual selection and natural selection in bird speciation.
Maia, R., Rubenstein, D. R., & Shawkey, M. D. (2016). Selection, con- Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences,
straint, and the evolution of coloration in African starlings. Evolution, 353, 251–260. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.1998.0207
70, 1064–1079. https://doi.org/10.1111/evo.12912 Price, J. J., Earnshaw, S. M., & Webster, M. S. (2006). Montezuma oro-
Manica, L. T., Macedo, R. H., Graves, J. A., & Podos, J. (2016). Vigor pendolas modify a component of song constrained by body size
and skill in the acrobatic mating displays of a Neotropical songbird. during vocal contests. Animal Behaviour, 71, 799–807. https://doi.
Behavioral Ecology, 28, 164–173. org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.05.025
Marchetti, K. (1993). Dark habitats and bright birds illustrate the role Pryke, S. R., & Andersson, S. (2003). Carotenoid-­based epaulettes reveal
of the environment in species divergence. Nature, 362, 149–152. male competitive ability: Experiments with resident and floater red-­
https://doi.org/10.1038/362149a0 shouldered widowbirds. Animal Behaviour, 66, 217–224. https://doi.
Mason, N. A., Shultz, A. J., & Burns, K. J. (2014). Elaborate visual and org/10.1006/anbe.2003.2193
acoustic signals evolve independently in a large, phenotypically Pryke, S. R., Andersson, S., & Lawes, M. J. (2001). Sexual selection of
diverse radiation of songbirds. Proceedings of the Royal Society multiple handicaps in the red-­collared widowbird: Female choice
B: Biological Sciences, 281, 20140967. https://doi.org/10.1098/ of tail length but not carotenoid display. Evolution, 55, 1452–1463.
rspb.2014.0967 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0014-3820.2001.tb00665.x
13652656, 2018, 4, Downloaded from https://besjournals.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/1365-2656.12824 by Welsh Assembly Government, Wiley Online Library on [23/06/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
MILES and FUXJAGER Journal of Animal Ecology | 1159

Reynolds, S. M., Christman, M. C., Uy, J. A. C., Patricelli, G. L., Braun, M. world buntings. The American Naturalist, 171, 755–776. https://doi.
J., & Borgia, G. (2009). Lekking satin bowerbird males aggregate with org/10.1086/587526
relatives to mitigate aggression. Behavioral Ecology, 20, 410–415. Stuart-Fox, D. M., & Ord, T. J. (2004). Sexual selection, natural selec-
https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arn146 tion and the evolution of dimorphic coloration and ornamentation in
Robel, R. J., & Ballard, W. B. (1974). Lek social organization and reproduc- agamid lizards. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences,
tive success in the greater prairie chicken. Integrative and Comparative 271, 2249–2255. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2004.2802
Biology, 14, 121–128. Taylor, R. C., & Ryan, M. J. (2013). Interactions of multisensory compo-
Roulin, A. (2016). Condition-­d ependence, pleiotropy and the hand- nents perceptually rescue Tungara frog mating signals. Science, 341,
icap principle of sexual selection in melanin-­ b ased coloura- 273–274. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1237113
tion. Biological Reviews, 91, 328–348. https://doi.org/10.1111/ Théry, M. (2001). Forest light and its influence on habitat selection. In
brv.12171 K. E. Linsenmair, A. J. Davis, B. Fiala, & M. R. Speight (Eds.), Tropical
Rowe, C. (1999). Receiver psychology and the evolution of multicompo- forest canopies: Ecology and management (pp. 251–261). Dordrecht,
nent signals. Animal Behaviour, 58, 921–931. https://doi.org/10.1006/ the Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers. https://doi.
anbe.1999.1242 org/10.1007/978-94-017-3606-0
Ryder, T. B., Parker, P. G., Blake, J. G., & Loiselle, B. A. (2009). It takes Tobias, J. A., Aben, J., Brumfield, R. T., Derryberry, E. P., Halfwerk, W.,
two to tango: Reproductive skew and social correlates of male mat- Slabbekoorn, H., & Seddon, N. (2010). Song divergence by sensory
ing success in a lek-­breeding bird. Proceedings of the Royal Society drive in amazonian birds. Evolution, 64, 2820–2839.
B: Biological Sciences, 276, 2377–2384. https://doi.org/10.1098/ Uy, J. A. C., & Endler, J. A. (2004). Modification of the visual background
rspb.2009.0208 increases the conspicuousness of golden-­collared manakin displays.
Scholes, E. (2008). Evolution of the courtship phenotype in the bird of Behavioral Ecology, 15, 1003–1010. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/
paradise genus Parotia (Aves: Paradisaeidae): Homology, phylogeny, arh106
and modularity. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 94, 491–504. Wallace, A. R. (1869). The Malay Archipelago: The land of the orang-utan
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1095-8312.2008.01012.x and the bird of paradise; a narrative of travel, with studies of man and
Schultz, T. D., & Fincke, O. M. (2009). Structural colours create a flash- nature. North Chelmsford, MA: Courier Corporation.
ing cue for sexual recognition and male quality in a Neotropical White, T. E., Zeil, J., & Kemp, D. J. (2015). Signal design and courtship pre-
giant damselfly. Functional Ecology, 23, 724–732. https://doi. sentation coincide for highly biased delivery of an iridescent butterfly
org/10.1111/j.1365-2435.2009.01584.x mating signal. Evolution, 69, 14–25. https://doi.org/10.1111/evo.12551
Schuppe, E. R., & Fuxjager, M. J. (2018). High-­speed displays encod- Widemo, F., & Owensi, I. P. F. (1995). Lek size, male mating skew
ing motor skill trigger increased territorial aggression in downy and the evolution of lekking. Nature, 373, 148–151. https://doi.
woodpeckers. Functional Ecology, 32, 450–460. https://doi. org/10.1038/373148a0
org/10.1111/1365-2435.12993 Wilkins, M. R., Seddon, N., & Safran, R. J. (2013). Evolutionary divergence
Seddon, N. (2005). Ecological adaptation and species recognition drives in acoustic signals: Causes and consequences. Trends in Ecology and
vocal evolution in neotropical suboscine birds. Evolution, 59, 200– Evolution, 28, 156–166. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2012.10.002
215. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0014-3820.2005.tb00906.x Wright, J. J. (2002). Plant diversity in tropical forests: A review of mech-
Seddon, N., Tobias, J. A., Eaton, M., Ödeen, A., & Byers, B. E. (2010). anisms of species coexistence. Oecologia, 130, 1–14. https://doi.
Human vision can provide a valid proxy for avian perception of sex- org/10.1007/s004420100809
ual dichromatism. The Auk, 127, 283–292. https://doi.org/10.1525/ Zahavi, A. (1975). Mate selection-­ A selection for a handi-
auk.2009.09070 cap. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 53, 205–214. https://doi.
Servedio, M. R. (2016). Geography, assortative mating, and the ef- org/10.1016/0022-5193(75)90111-3
fects of sexual selection on speciation with gene flow. Evolutionary
Applications, 9, 91–102. https://doi.org/10.1111/eva.12296
Siefferman, L., & Hill, G. E. (2003). Structural and melanin coloration S U P P O R T I N G I N FO R M AT I O N
indicate parental effort and reproductive success in male eastern
bluebirds. Behavioral Ecology, 14, 855–861. https://doi.org/10.1093/ Additional Supporting Information may be found online in the
beheco/arg063 ­supporting information tab for this article.
Smith, K. R., Cadena, V., Endler, J. A., Porter, W. P., Kearney, M. R., &
Stuart-Fox, D. (2016). Colour change on different body regions pro-
vides thermal and signalling advantages in bearded dragon lizards.
How to cite this article: Miles MC, Fuxjager MJ. Synergistic
Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 283, 20160626.
https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2016.0626 selection regimens drive the evolution of display complexity in
Stange, N., Page, R. A., Ryan, M. J., & Taylor, R. C. (2016). Interactions birds of paradise. J Anim Ecol. 2018;87:1149–1159.
between complex multisensory signal components result in unex-
https://doi.org/10.1111/1365-2656.12824
pected mate choice responses. Animal Behaviour, 134, 239–247.
Stoddard, M. C., & Prum, R. O. (2008). Evolution of avian plumage
color in a tetrahedral color space: A phylogenetic analysis of new

You might also like