Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

Playfulness and the Environmental Support

of Play in Children With and Without


Developmental Disabilities
Ellen M. Hamm
Key words: Test of Playfulness, Test of Environmental Support, early intervention

ABSTRACT

The primary purposes of this study were to examine the reliability and validity of the Test of
Playfulness (ToP) and the Test of Environmental Supportiveness (TOES), to compare play-
fulness and environmental support of play in children with and without developmental dis-
abilities, and to examine the correlation between the ToP and the TOES. Participants included
20 children with various developmental disabilities and 20 children without developmental
disabilities between 6 and 38 months old. Results revealed the ToP and the TOES to be reli-
able and valid. Furthermore, children with developmental disabilities were less playful than
children without developmental disabilities. The ToP and the TOES were found to be highly
correlated for both groups; however, the magnitude of this correlation was greater for children
with disabilities.

R
eview of current research regarding play and playfulness and environmental support for play in
playfulness in children reveals a discrepancy young children with and without developmental
between young children with and without disabilities.
developmental disabilities. Although some research-
ers have found a significant difference in the play of Context for Play
children with and without developmental disabili- When children feel safe and comfortable in their
ties (Bromwich, 1985; Brooks-Gunn & Lewis, 1982; environment, they will be able to play (Rubin, 1977).
Gowen, Johnson-Martin, Goldman, & Hussey, 1992; Therefore, the goal of enabling playful interactions
Hanzlik, 1989; Okimoto, Bundy, & Hanzlik, 2000), requires occupational therapists to look beyond skill
others report nonsignificant differences (Buchanan, development and examine the role of the environ-
1995; Harkness & Bundy, 2001). Playfulness goes ment as it supports or inhibits playful interactions.
beyond the performance of play skills because it Kielhofner (1985) stated “. . .the environment both af-
lends description to the quality of a child’s play, fords opportunities for performance and presses for
regardless of physical ability. The more playful child certain types of behavior” (p. 2). Assessment of the
may be more flexible, and flexibility is thought to child’s play environment may be a preliminary step
be an important trait for individuals with disabili- in addressing play. Occupational therapists are often
ties (Harkness & Bundy, 2001). If playfulness is a experienced in how to construct play environments
child’s disposition to play (Lieberman, 1977), and that afford opportunities for play performance.
play is the primary occupation of young children
(Parham & Primeau, 1997; Reilly, 1974), then assess- Play
ment of playfulness in addition to the assessment Children with developmental disabilities often
of play skills may be a consideration in pediatric exhibit deficits in play that parallel deficits in other
occupational therapy. The current study examined areas of development (Field, Roseman, De-Stefano,

Ellen M. Hamm, PhD, is Assistant Professor, Canisius College, Buffalo, New York.
Address correspondence to Ellen M. Hamm at hamme@canisius.edu.

88 OTJR: Occupation, Participation and Health


Downloaded from otj.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 12, 2016
& Knoewler, 1982; Malone, 1999). Past research has These concepts are not mutually exclusive, because
demonstrated that children with disabilities often do certain play behaviors may suggest that one or more
not have the same play skills or opportunities to play of these elements is present. The degree to which
as their typically developing peers (Barrera & Vella, each element is present determines whether a child’s
1987; Brodin, 1999; Field et al., 1982; Hanzlik, 1989; transaction is considered playful (Bundy, 1991).
Hanzlik & Stevenson, 1986; Hestenes & Carroll, 2000; In a study designed to investigate playfulness in
Hill & McCune-Nicolich, 1981; Mack, Lindquist, & young children, differences in playfulness among 19
Parham, 1982). Although deficits in motor, speech, children without disabilities and 19 children with
cognitive, or social–emotional domains are readily cerebral palsy and developmental disabilities were
addressed in early intervention programs, play defi- examined (Okimoto et al., 2000). The children’s
cits are often neglected because emphasis is directed chronological ages ranged from 3 to 32 months.
toward educational goals (Lane & Mistrett, 1996). Mother–child dyads, matched for gender and mental
The role of play in occupational therapy has age, were videotaped during 15 minutes of free play
evolved from that of a diversionary activity (Slagle in their home with a standard box of toys provided
& Robeson, 1941) to a lifelong occupation (Kielhof- by the researcher. Playfulness was measured using
ner, 1985). Consideration of play in early interven- the Test of Playfulness (ToP) (Bundy, 1997b). Results
tion is important because research has linked play indicated that children with cerebral palsy and de-
to social and emotional development, growth of lan- velopmental disabilities scored significantly lower
guage and motor skills, and creativity and problem- on the playfulness measure than their peers without
solving (Bruner & Sherwood, 1976; Garvey, 1990). disabilities. In addition to comparing playfulness,
Occupational therapists often use play as an activity this study also established reliability and validity for
for assessing and promoting development; however, the ToP to be used with children with cognitive defi-
assessment of children’s play skills often focuses cits and children as young as 3 months.
on the manipulation of play objects and not on the Harkness and Bundy (2001) also examined the
child’s inherent propensity for play known as play- playfulness of children with and without physical
fulness (Bundy, 1993). disabilities. Their sample included 25 children with
a variety of physical disabilities but no cognitive
Playfulness limitation, and 25 children without developmental
Playfulness, defined as a disposition to play, is disability. Children’s ages ranged from 2 to 12 years
thought to be a trait of the individual (Lieberman, and participants were matched for gender, age, and
1977). Based on the work of others (Neumann, 1971; ethnicity. Despite references in the literature to de-
Rubin, Fein, & Vandenberg, 1983), Bundy (1997a) de- creased play ability and playfulness of children with
fined playfulness as contingent on a combination of physical limitations (Mogford, 1977; Russell, 1985;
four elements: intrinsic motivation, internal control, Sheridan, 1975), results of the study by Harkness and
the freedom to suspend reality, and framing. Bundy found no significant difference in mean scores
According to Bundy (1997b), intrinsic motivation of playfulness between the two groups (t = .63; p =
refers to some unnamed aspect of the activity, rath- .53). Study limitations included unexpectedly high
er than to an external reward. This element may be scores on the ToP item that measures a child’s exu-
measured by observing traits of play, including play berance, which may have resulted in children with
that is all-absorbing, play involving more attention disabilities receiving a higher score on this item than
to process than to product, and play that is surpris- was appropriate. Another limitation included the
ing and unpredictable. Internal control suggests that play environments chosen by participating families.
the child is largely in control of his or her actions The authors found that families of children with dis-
and some aspect of the activity’s outcome. This el- abilities often chose to be observed in familiar envi-
ement may be observed through traits consisting ronments with few obstacles, whereas the families of
of the player’s feeling safe and reaching beyond children without disabilities chose novel situations
himself or herself to meet a challenge. Freedom to in which their child was less familiar.
suspend reality means that the individual chooses Further research suggests a positive significant
how close the transaction will be to objective reality. relationship between playfulness, as measured by
This element is manifested in play when the usual the ToP, and environmental support of play, as mea-
meaning of objects no longer applies. Framing is the sured by the Test of Environmental Supportiveness
ability to give and to interpret play cues (Bateson, (TOES), with the magnitude of that relationship be-
1972). This element is measured by the child’s ability ing greater for children without disabilities than for
to give and read social cues and be engaged in play. children with disabilities (Bronson & Bundy, 2001).

Summer 2006, Volume 26, Number 3 89


Downloaded from otj.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 12, 2016
Participants included 160 children, 51 without dis- abilities were part of a larger study on playfulness
abilities and 109 with disabilities, with a mean cog- (Lane, 2001) and were identified through the Early
nitive age of 70 months. The TOES measured a vari- Intervention Program in a large county in New York
ety of environments, including home, daycare, and State. Participants with developmental disabilities
outdoor play settings. Environments were not con- were matched with participants without develop-
sistent among participants because some children mental disabilities for gender, cognitive age, socio-
were in unfamiliar surroundings and others played economic status, ethnicity, and presence of siblings.
in their home. Based on their results, the authors Cognitive age for children with disabilities was
also concluded that the TOES is a reliable and valid determined from standardized testing completed
measure of environmental support. for the child’s most recent Individualized Family
Based on limited and conflicting research, the cur- Service Plan. The mean cognitive age for this group
rent study examined playfulness and environmental was 16.3 months. For children without disabilities,
support of play for children with and without devel- cognitive age was equivalent to chronological age
opmental disabilities. Other independent variables of and mean cognitive age was 16.2 months. Empiri-
interest included gender, to account for differences cal research has found that the play of children with
in the playfulness of boys and girls, and presence of disabilities is commensurate with cognitive age, not
siblings, to account for differences due to the effect chronological age (Bromwich, 1985, Buchanan, 1995;
of having a sibling as a model for playful interac- Gowen et al., 1992; Okimoto et al., 2000); therefore,
tions. Additionally, the reliability and validity of the children were matched on cognitive age within 2
ToP (version 4.0) and the TOES was examined and months, regardless of chronological age.
the relationship between environmental support and Participants included 27 boys (14 with disabili-
playfulness in each group was probed. The following ties, 13 without) and 13 girls (6 with disabilities, 7
research questions guided this investigation: without). Types of disabilities varied, and ranged
from mild to severe limitations. Twenty-four chil-
1. Is the ToP a reliable and valid measure of playful- dren (12 per group) had older siblings, and 12 (6 per
ness in young children with and without devel- group) were only children at the time of videotap-
opmental disabilities? ing. Subjects included two sets of twins; one set of
2. Is the TOES a reliable and valid measure of the male twins in the group of children with disabilities,
environmental support of play in young children and one set of male–female twins in the group with-
with and without developmental disabilities? out disabilities.
3. Do children with and without developmental dis- According to study classification criteria, all fami-
abilities demonstrate differences in playfulness? lies had a socioeconomic status of working or mid-
Should an overall significant difference be detect- dle class and all children, with the exception of one
ed, can that difference be attributed to one demo- boy with a developmental disability, resided with
graphic characteristic such as disability, gender, both parents. All participating parents had com-
or presence of siblings? pleted high school and most were college educated
4. Do children with and without developmental dis- (15 parents of children without disabilities and 14
abilities receive different levels of environmental parents of children with disabilities). All fathers in
support for play? Should an overall significant both groups were employed outside the home. Nine
difference be detected, can differences be attrib- mothers of children with disabilities worked outside
uted to one demographic characteristic such as the home, whereas 16 mothers of children without
disability, gender, or presence of siblings? disabilities worked outside the home. Nineteen of
5. Do children with and without developmental dis- the 20 children in each group were white, and one
abilities demonstrate differences in strength of child in each group was African American. Nineteen
correlation between playfulness, as measured by of the 20 children in each group were videotaped in
the ToP, and the environmental support for play, a play session with their mother, and one girl in each
as measured by the TOES? group was videotaped in a play session with her fa-
ther.
Method
Participants Instruments
The sample consisted of 20 children with devel- Test of Playfulness (ToP). The ToP (version 4.0)
opmental disabilities and 20 children without dis- (Bundy, 1991) is a 32-item observation instrument
abilities whose chronological ages ranged from 6 designed to evaluate free play in children 6 months
to 38 months. Participants with developmental dis- to 14 years of age regardless of developmental level.

90 OTJR: Occupation, Participation and Health


Downloaded from otj.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 12, 2016
The ToP provides a measure of playfulness without because three items related to play with younger
penalties for motor skill deficits and defines playful- playmates were not observed.
ness as the combination of internal control, intrinsic Items on the TOES are rated on a 4-point contin-
motivation, freedom to suspend reality, and fram- uum (-2, -1, 1, 2). Negative scores indicate that an
ing. The extent, intensity, and skillfulness of out- environmental element is interfering with play and
lined play behaviors are rated on a 4-point (0 to 3) positive scores indicate support from the environ-
scale. Children are scored on items such as the ex- ment. Data on inter-rater reliability and model error
tent, intensity, and skill with which they are actively values provide support for reliability. Model error
engaged in play; the extent that they decide what to values are calculated on each item, subject, and rater
do during play; the intensity with which they try to and provide further evidence of scale reliability. Us-
overcome barriers to persist in play; the skill they ing Rasch analysis, Rogers (1999) examined several
use to modify an activity to maintain challenge and aspects related to construct validity, including fit of
fun; and the extent, intensity, and skill displayed in items and data to the Rasch model, percentage of un-
social play. Scores are derived from a 15- to 20-min- expected ratings, logic of item order, and separation
ute observation of free play with playmates or a fa- of items into levels. Analysis indicated that 94% of
miliar adult caregiver. the items, 95% of environments, and 96% of raters
Preliminary evidence suggests that the ToP is a conformed to the expectations of the Rasch model.
valid and reliable measure of playfulness in chil-
dren with or without disabilities. Support for re- Setting
liability was established using Rasch modeling Based on past research (Buchanan, 1995), the set-
(Wright & Masters, 1982; Wright & Stone, 1979) and ting for the play session was the child’s natural play
with investigations (Brooks, 1995; Bundy, 1997a) environment. This environment was determined by
indicating that 93% to 95% of items, 95% to 98% of the parents and in all cases was a room in the family
children tested, and 100% of raters fell within the home where the child typically engaged in play. For
established measurement model. Reliability and the purposes of this study, a 20-minute videotape of
construct and concurrent validity of the ToP has an indoor free play session in a setting familiar to the
been further established when used with children child was taped for scoring.
with and without developmental disabilities be- After receiving informed consent and consent
tween 15 months and 10 years old (Bundy, Nelson, for video release, videotaping was conducted in the
Metzger, & Bingaman, 2001) and with children with family home. Videotaping was conducted with lim-
cognitive deficits as young as 3 months (Okimoto et ited involvement from the researcher in the play ses-
al., 2000). sion, as suggested in the ToP manual (Bundy, 1997b).
Test of Environmental Supportiveness (TOES). Caregivers were instructed to engage in typical play
Bundy (1999) designed the TOES to be administered with their child, using toys or materials with which
simultaneously with the ToP. The 17-item observa- they were comfortable. No toys or materials were
tional measure assesses both the human and the supplied by the researcher. Data were collected with
non-human elements in the environment that posi- an awareness of the effect of videotaping on sub-
tively or negatively influence playfulness. Human jects; however, due to the age of the children, most
environment consists of caregivers, peers, and play- appeared unaware of the camera.
mates (either children or adults) and non-human en- Videotaped play sessions were scored by two in-
vironment consists of both play items and the play dependent raters trained and calibrated in scoring
setting. Caregivers are scored based on behaviors the ToP and the TOES. The raters were naïve to the
that ensure safety and provide play opportunities. research hypothesis and tapes from each group were
Objects are scored on the existing amount, ability to randomly divided to ensure each rater scored an
support play, and degree of modification they allow. equal number of children representing each group.
Space is rated on amount, configuration, safety, and Inter-rater reliability was performed by randomly
accessibility. The sensory environment consists of selecting 10% of the tapes from the total sample to
colors, sounds, temperature, and the extent to which be scored by both raters. Raters were in 100% agree-
these invite play in the environment. Administration ment on the scoring of this sample of tapes.
of the TOES involves a 15- to 20-minute observation
of free play. Participants in this study were video- Results
taped for 20 minutes of free play in an indoor setting
selected by the parent. As was deemed acceptable by To determine the reliability and validity of the
the test author, partial scoring of the TOES was used ToP to assess playfulness and the TOES to assess the

Summer 2006, Volume 26, Number 3 91


Downloaded from otj.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 12, 2016
Table 1
A Summary for 2 3 2 3 2 Multivariate Analysis of Variance
Univariate
Multivariate ToP TOES
Source of Variation df F p df F p df F p
Main Effects
Group 2, 31 5.5 .008* 1, 32 10.90 .002* 1, 32 3.3 .08
Gender 2, 31 0.6 .320 1, 32 2.00 .16 1, 32 0.3 .58
Siblings 2, 31 0.6 .550 1, 32 0.20 .65 1, 32 1.0 .30
Interaction Effects
Group 3 Gender 2, 31 0.25 .77 1, 32 0.49 .48 1, 32 .13 .72
Group 3 Siblings 2, 31 1.30 .26 1, 32 1.70 .21 1, 32 .02 .87
Siblings 3 Gender 2, 31 0.19 .82 1, 32 0.11 .73 1, 32 .02 .88
Group 3 Gender 3 Siblings 2, 31 1.08 .34 1, 32 1.70 .19 1, 32 .17 .68
Note. ToP = Test of Playfulness; TOES = Test of Environmental Supportiveness.
*p < .01.

environmental support of play in young children and the TOES was examined through analysis of
with and without developmental disabilities, good- the percentage of individual item ratings that fit
ness-of-fit was analyzed using fit statistics generated the measurement model. For this sample, 100% of
by Rasch analysis. Goodness-of-fit is determined the ratings assigned conformed to the Rasch model
through mean square (MnSq) and t statistics. MnSq supporting reliability of the scale when scored by
is the ratio of the observed score and the score ex- trained raters.
pected by the measurement model. The t statistic is Standard scores for the ToP and the TOES were
the standardized difference between the observed used for data analysis. A three-way multivariate
and expected scores. The ideal value is 1.0 for MnSq analysis of variance (2 3 2 3 2 MANOVA) was
and 0 for t statistic; however, MnSq values 1 ± 0.4 conducted to determine the effect of the three in-
and t values 0 ± 2.0 are acceptable. MnSq values less dependent variables (group, gender, and presence
than 0.6 or greater than 1.4, accompanied by t sta- of siblings) on the two dependent variables (ToP
tistics less than -2 or greater than +2 for either infit and TOES scores). To examine the role of the en-
or outfit, failed to conform adequately to the Rasch vironment in relation to playfulness, correlation of
model and indicated data that were too consistent the ToP and the TOES was generated for the entire
or too erratic and required further investigation sample of 40 children and subsequent correlation of
(Wright & Linacre, 1994). the ToP and the TOES was generated for each of the
Item response validity of the TOES was exam- two groups of 20 children for comparison among
ined by determining how well data from items fit groups.
the Rasch model. Fit statistics of all 17 items (100%) MANOVA was computed to examine playful-
fell within acceptable limits. Person-response valid- ness and environmental support of play in the two
ity of the ToP was examined by determining the fit groups. Because differences between the two groups
of both items and participants to the Rasch measure- were significant, follow-up univariate tests were con-
ment model. Data from 100% of the participants and ducted. Independent variables of interest included
raters conformed to the Rasch model. Additionally, group, gender, and presence of siblings. MANOVA
construct validity was examined for the ToP. All results are reported in Table 1.
items (100%) fit the model supporting that the ToP Correlation of the ToP and the TOES for all sub-
reflects a unidimensional construct of playfulness. jects was followed by correlation of the tests for
To determine inter-rater reliability, rater calibrations each group (n = 20). Correlations were subjected
were examined for fit to the measurement model. to Fisher’s z to determine differences in magnitude
Both raters fit the measurement model for the ToP of correlation of the ToP and the TOES for children
and the TOES, indicating that the scales were used with and without disabilities (Minium, King, & Bear,
in a consistent manner. Scale reliability for the ToP 1993). Correlation results are reported in Table 2.

92 OTJR: Occupation, Participation and Health


Downloaded from otj.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 12, 2016
Multivariate and Univariate Effects
Significant multivariate effects were found be- Table 2
tween the two groups. Using Wilk’s Lambda, results Correlation Between ToP and TOES Scores
of the MANOVA indicated a significant effect for Correlation 2-tailed
group. Univariate effects were analyzed to deter- Coefficient Significance
mine which of the three independent variables con- Subjects (Pearson r) (Fisher’s z)
tributed to the multivariate effect. Inspection of the Both groups .723 .000
means revealed that children without developmen- (N = 40)
tal disabilities scored significantly higher on the ToP With disabilities .757 .000
compared with peers with developmental disabili- (n = 20)
ties; however, TOES scores were not significantly Without disabilities .636 .000
different between the two groups. Mean scores are (n = 20)
reported in Table 3. Note. ToP = Test of Playfulness; TOES = Test of Environmental
Supportiveness.
Effect sizes are reported in terms of Partial Eta
Squared. Partial Eta Squared represents the variance
accounted for by the dependent variables. Partial
Eta Squared is a correlation squared and effect sizes ple of 40 children and then separately for the two
reported are correlations that may be interpreted as groups. The coefficient for ToP and TOES scores in
follows: 0 = trivial, .1 = small, .3 = moderate, .5 = the entire sample of children was found to be sig-
large, and .7 = very large. With small numbers of nificant (df = 38; p < .01), with the amount of shared
participants, as in this study, inferential statistics can variance at 49%. The coefficient for the sample of
produce misleading results. Moderate to large effect children without developmental disabilities was
sizes indicative of substantial differences in means .636 (df = 18; p < .01), whereas the coefficient for the
are likely to be insignificant with a small number sample of children with developmental disabilities
of participants because of restricted power. Signifi- was .757 (df = 18; p < .01). The percentage of shared
cant multivariate effects were found among the two variance between ToP and TOES scores for these
groups with effect size, (ES) r = .477. Univariate ef- two groups was 42% and 56%, respectively. For both
fects were significant for group on the ToP only, (ES) groups, a significant and positive relationship was
r = .356. found between playfulness and environmental sup-
port of play. Fisher’s z transformation was used to
Analysis of Correlations compare the coefficients of the two samples. Results
Regarding magnitude of correlation for the ToP of the analysis indicated a significantly higher cor-
and the TOES for the two groups, data were ana- relation between the ToP and the TOES for children
lyzed using the Pearson product moment correla- with developmental disabilities compared with their
tion. Coefficients were calculated for the entire sam- peers without developmental disabilities.

Table 3
Mean Scores for Playfulness and Environmental Support
Group ToP Mean SD TOES Mean SD
With disability -.2995 .8882 2.1995 1.0186
Male with siblings -.4318 .7935 2.0473 .93871
Male no siblings -.4000 .45211 2.1733 .09815
Female with siblings .3763 1.4607 2.6767 1.4950
Female no siblings -.3633 1.0979 2.2800 1.6513
Without disability .6510 .9114 2.8995 1.2153
Male with siblings 1.0556 1.0880 2.9110 1.4745
Male no siblings .0100 .58583 2.4675 .99225
Female with siblings .5220 .62815 3.2000 1.2758
Female no siblings .4350 .6434 2.9600 .0000
Note. ToP = Test of Playfulness; SD = standard deviation; TOES = Test of Environmental Supportiveness.

Summer 2006, Volume 26, Number 3 93


Downloaded from otj.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 12, 2016
Discussion significant difference in the magnitude of correla-
tion for the two groups. Children with disabilities
In response to the first research question regarding demonstrated a stronger correlation of the ToP and
the reliability and validity of the ToP for measuring the TOES compared with their peers without dis-
playfulness in young children, results suggest that abilities. This finding does not support the work of
the ToP is both reliable and valid and may provide Bronson and Bundy (2001), who found the magni-
clinicians and researchers with a practical means of tude of the relationship of the ToP and the TOES
measuring the construct of playfulness. Knowledge was greater for children without disabilities than
and understanding of the four elements measuring for children with disabilities. The significant dif-
the construct of playfulness (intrinsic motivation, in- ference in the correlation of the ToP and the TOES
ternal control, freedom to suspend reality, and fram- suggests that the environment may play a more
ing) may be useful in assessment and intervention important role in supporting playfulness for chil-
for the pediatric therapist. dren with developmental disabilities compared
Reliability and validity of the TOES as a measure- with those without. The environment under study
ment of the environmental support of play was also involved the physical environment and the human
supported. Caution should be taken in interpreting environment, which in this case was represented by
this finding because the environments assessed were parent as play partner.
limited to play with a parent in the family home. The This finding lends support to the findings of Malo-
results provide evidence that the TOES is reliable ne (1994) and Malone and Langone (1999), who sug-
with respect to raters, suggesting that the TOES may gested that exposure to toys alone is not adequate to
be a viable option for therapists to use in assessing increase play, and recommended adult intervention
the human and non-human environment as it sup- to assist children in the selection of appropriate toys
ports play. and modeling toy use. Lane and Mistrett (1996) also
Examination of mean scores between the two suggested that parents of children with disabilities
groups of children suggests that children with de- may need intervention to learn to be playful with
velopmental disabilities were less playful than their their children. Implications of this finding place
peers without developmental disabilities, regardless emphasis on the therapist’s role in structuring the
of the presence of siblings or gender. This finding is child’s environment to promote play. Constructing
consistent with previous research conducted by Oki- an overall environment that encourages play is pre-
moto et al. (2000), who measured playfulness in chil- requisite to a child’s exploration of the environment
dren with and without cerebral palsy and concluded (Morrison, Metzger, & Pratt, 1996).
that the ToP mean score for children without a dis- Results of this study should be considered in light
ability was significantly higher than the mean score of several limitations, including the use of a small
for children with cerebral palsy. However, the re- sample of convenience. Additionally, only partial
sults contradict findings from Harkness and Bundy scoring of the TOES was analyzed because the hu-
(2001), who measured playfulness of children with man environment focused on play with a parent as
and without physical disabilities and found no sig- playmate and did not address play with peer play-
nificant differences between the two groups. mates.
Regarding the environmental support of play, no
significant differences were found for environmental Conclusions
supportiveness as measured by the TOES. This find-
ing suggests that the environments where the par- Based on the current data analysis, the ToP ap-
ticipating children were videotaped for evaluation pears to be a valid and reliable assessment of play-
were all equally supportive of play. For this study, all fulness and the TOES a valid and reliable assessment
children were videotaped in their home in a famil- of the environmental support of play for young chil-
iar environment. One interpretation of this result is dren with and without developmental disabilities.
that the parents had anticipated the specific needs of As measured by the ToP, children with and without
their child with or without a disability and adapted developmental disabilities demonstrated differenc-
the environment to fit the requirements. Additional es in playfulness, as those with developmental dis-
research is suggested to determine the supportive- abilities scored lower on the ToP compared to their
ness of play in a greater variety of environments, peers without developmental disabilities. No differ-
such as community playgrounds. ences in the environmental supportiveness of play
The final research question examined the corre- were detected between the two groups; however, the
lation of the ToP and the TOES. Results identified a correlation between playfulness and environmental

94 OTJR: Occupation, Participation and Health


Downloaded from otj.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 12, 2016
support was greater for children with developmen- Bruner, J., & Sherwood, V. (Eds.). (1976). Play: Its role in develop-
ment and evolution. Harmondsworth: Penguin.
tal disabilities.
Because of the importance of adaptability in the Buchanan, M. (1995). The home play of toddlers with disabili-
ties and typically developing toddlers. (Doctoral disserta-
life of a person with a disability (Bundy, 1993) and tion, University of Washington, 1995). Dissertation Abstracts
the potential relationship between adaptability and International, 56, 4349.
playfulness (Harkness & Bundy, 2001), occupational Bundy, A. C. (1991). Play theory and sensory integration. In A. C.
therapists should consider playfulness as an area of Bundy, E. A. Murray, & A. G. Fisher (Eds.), Sensory integration:
assessment and intervention. In designing effective Theory and practice (pp. 46-68). Philadelphia: F. A. Davis.
interventions to increase playfulness in young chil- Bundy, A. C. (1993). Assessment of play and leisure: Delineation
dren, each element of Bundy’s model of playfulness of the problem. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 47,
(intrinsic motivation, internal control, freedom to 217-222.
suspend reality, and framing) should be examined Bundy, A. C. (1997a). Play and playfulness: What to look for. In
L. D. Parham & L. S. Fazio (Eds.), Play in occupational therapy
for its contribution to the overall construct of play-
for children (pp. 52-66). St. Louis, MO: Mosby.
fulness.
Bundy, A. C. (1997b). The Test of Playfulness. Fort Collins, CO:
Based on interpretation of the results, interven- Colorado State University.
tion to increase playfulness should include direct
Bundy, A. C. (1999). Test of Environmental Supportiveness (TOES).
involvement with the child and with the child’s hu- Fort Collins, CO: Colorado State University.
man and non-human environment. This support Bundy, A. C., Nelson, L., Metzger, M., & Bingaman, K. (2001).
may be provided by training parents or caregivers Validity and reliability of a test of playfulness. The Occupational
and service providers in effective ways to promote Therapy Journal of Research, 21, 276-292.
play in children with developmental disabilities, or Field, T. M., Roseman, S., De-Stefano, L. J., & Knoewler, J.
by making changes in the physical environment to (1982). The play of handicapped preschool children with
accommodate children with disabilities and encour- handicapped and non-handicapped peers in integrated and
non-integrated situations. Topics in Early Childhood Special
age playfulness. Furthermore, in light of findings
Education, 2, 28-38.
that indicate a high correlation between playfulness
Garvey, C. (1990). Play. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
and the environmental support of play, continued ex- Press.
amination of the role of the human and non-human
Gowen, J. W., Johnson-Martin, N., Goldman, B. D., & Hussey, B.
environment in play is warranted. Further studies (1992). Object play and exploration in children with and with-
are also recommended to compare the effectiveness out developmental disabilities: A longitudinal study. American
of interventions to increase play and playfulness in Journal on Mental Retardation, 97(1), 21-38.
children with developmental disabilities. Hanzlik, J. R. (1989). The effect of intervention on the free-play
experience for mothers and their infants with developmental
References delay and cerebral palsy. Physical and Occupational Therapy in
Barrera, M. E., & Vella, D. M. (1987). Disabled and nondisabled Pediatrics, 9(2), 33-51.
infants’ interactions with their mothers. American Journal of Hanzlik, J. R., & Stevenson, M. B. (1986). Interactions of moth-
Occupational Therapy, 41, 168-172. ers with their infants who are mentally retarded, mentally
Bateson, G. (1972). Toward a theory of play and phantasy. In G. retarded with cerebral palsy, or nontreated. American Journal
Bateson (Ed.), Steps to an ecology of mind (pp. 14-20). New York: of Mental Deficiency, 90, 513-520.
Bantam. Harkness, L., & Bundy, A. C. (2001). The Test of Playfulness and
Brodin, J. (1999). Play in children with severe multiple disabili- children with physical disabilities. The Occupational Therapy
ties: Play with toys: A review. International Journal of Disability, Journal of Research, 21, 73-89.
Development and Education, 46(1), 25-34. Hestenes, L. L., & Carroll, D. E. (2000). The play of young children
Bromwich, R. M. (1985). Play behaviors of handicapped and non- with and without disabilities: Individual and environmental
handicapped infants between 9 and 24 months. In S. Harel & influences. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 15, 229-246.
N. J. Anastasiow (Eds.), The at-risk infant: Psycho-socio-medical Hill, P. M., & McCune-Nicolich, L. (1981). Pretend play and
aspects (pp. 379-387). Baltimore: Paul H. Brooks Publishing patterns of cognition in Down’s syndrome children. Child
Co. Development, 52, 611-617.
Bronson, M. R., & Bundy, A. C. (2001). A correlational study of Kielhofner, G. (1985). A model of human occupation: Theory and
a test of playfulness and a test of environmental supportive- application. Baltimore: Williams and Wilkins.
ness for play. The Occupational Therapy Journal of Research, 21, Kielhofner, G. (1995). Human occupation (2nd ed.). Baltimore:
241-259. Williams and Wilkins.
Brooks, L. (1995). Reliability and validity of the revised Test of Lane, S. J. (2001). Assistive technology to promote playfulness
Playfulness. Unpublished master’s thesis, Colorado State in children with disabilities. (CFDA # 84.327A). Richmond,
University, Ft. Collins, Colorado. VA: Department of Occupational Therapy, Virginia
Brooks-Gunn, J., & Lewis, M. (1982). Development of play Commonwealth University.
behavior in handicapped and normal infants. Topics in Early Lane, S. J., & Mistrett, S. G. (1996). Play and assistive technology
Childhood Special Education, 2(3), 14-27. issues for infants and young children with disabilities: A pre-

Summer 2006, Volume 26, Number 3 95


Downloaded from otj.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 12, 2016
liminary examination. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental children with and without disability: Measurement and inter-
Disabilities, 11(2), 96-104. vention. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 54, 73-82.
Lieberman, J. N. (1977). Playfulness: Its relationship to imagination Parham, L. D., & Primeau, L. A. (1997). Play and occupational
and creativity. New York: Academic Press. therapy. In L. D. Parham & L. S. Fazio (Eds.), Play in occupa-
Mack, W., Lindquist, J. E., & Parham, L. D. (1982). A synthesis of tional therapy for children (pp. 2-21). St. Louis, MO: Mosby.
occupational behavior and sensory integration concepts in Reilly, M. (Ed.). (1974). Play as exploratory learning: Studies of curi-
theory and practice, part 1: Theoretical foundations. American osity behavior. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications.
Journal of Occupational Therapy, 36, 365-374. Rogers, M. (1999). A correlational study of the Test of Environmental
Malone, D. M. (1994). Developmental correlates of social Supportiveness and the Test of Playfulness. Unpublished
engagement in preschool children with mental retardation. master’s thesis, Colorado State University, Ft. Collins,
International Play Journal, 2, 189-207. Colorado.
Malone, D. M. (1999). Contextual factors informing play- Rubin, K. H. (1977). Play behaviors of young children. Young
based program planning. International Journal of Disability, Children, 32, 16-24.
Development and Education, 46(3), 307-324. Rubin, K., Fein, G. G., & Vandenberg, B. (1983). Play. In P. H.
Malone, D. M., & Langone, J. (1999). Teaching object-related play Musten (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (4th ed., Vol. 4, pp.
skills to preschool children with developmental concerns. 693-774). New York: John Wiley and Sons.
International Journal of Disability, Development and Education, Russell, P. (1985). The wheelchair child: How handicapped children can
46(3), 325-336. enjoy life to its fullest. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Minium, E. W., King, B. M., & Bear, G. (1993). Statistical reasoning Sheridan, M. (1975). The importance of spontaneous play in the
in psychology and education (3rd ed.). New York: John Wiley fundamental learning of handicapped children. Child: Care,
and Sons. Health and Development, 1, 3-17.
Mogford, K. (1977). The play of handicapped children. In B. Slagle, E. C., & Robeson, H. A. (1941). Syllabus for training nurses in
Tizard & D. Harvey (Eds.), Biology of play, clinics in developmen- occupational therapy. Utica, NY: State Hospitals Press.
tal medicine (pp. 170-184). Philadelphia: J. B. Lippincott.
Wright, B. D., & Linacre, J. M. (1994). Reasonable mean-square fit
Morrison, C. D., Metzger, P., & Pratt, P. N. (1996). Play. In J. Case- values. Rasch Measurement Transactions, 8, 370.
Smith, A. S. Allen, & P. N. Pratt (Eds.), Occupational therapy for
Wright, B. D., & Masters, G. N. (1982). Rating scale analysis.
children (3rd ed., pp. 504-523). New York: Mosby.
Chicago: MESA Press.
Neumann, E. A. (1971). The elements of play. New York: MSS
Wright, B. D., & Stone, M. H. (1979). Best test design. Chicago:
Information.
MESA Press.
Okimoto, A. M., Bundy, A., & Hanzlik, J. (2000). Playfulness in

96 OTJR: Occupation, Participation and Health


Downloaded from otj.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 12, 2016

You might also like