Energy Allocation in Mammalian Reproduction: AMER. ZOOL., 28:863-875 (1988)

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 13

AMER. ZOOL.

, 28:863-875 (1988)

Energy Allocation in Mammalian Reproduction1


JOHN L. GITTLEMAN
Department of Zoology and Graduate Programs in Ecology and Ethology,
University of Tennessee, Knoxville, Tennessee 37916

AND

STEVEN D. THOMPSON
Department of Zoological Research, National Zoological Park,
Smithsonian Institution, Washington D. C. 20008

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/icb/article/28/3/863/99186 by guest on 18 February 2021


SYNOPSIS. On behavioral, hormonal, and physiological grounds, mammalian reproduc-
tion can be compartmentalized into the following continuous sequence of events: mating
(courtship, estrous), gestation, parturition, lactation, post-lactational parental care, and
maternal recovery. We point out that comparing the relative allocation of energy for
these events across mammals is difficult because of life history variability (e.g., litter size,
birth weight), allometry, phylogeny, and individual variation. We review the empirical
and theoretical literature on each of these events with respect to: different methodologies
in measuring energy use; broad patterns of energy consumption across diverse mammalian
taxa; and, identification of particular reproductive characteristics (e.g., birthing, parental
care) which may be costly but have yet to receive energetic measurements. Although most
studies have considered gestation and lactation the critical reproductive events for energy
expenditure, variation in these events is substantial and almost certainly is a function of
relative allocation of time to gestation vs. lactation as well as the presumed energetic costs
of mating, birthing and parental care. In addition, repeated observations show that behav-
ioral compensation is an extremely important strategy for minimizing energy requirements
during reproduction. From this review, we argue that more complete analyses will come
from (1) incorporating energetic measurements in studies of mammalian behavior and (2)
including mechanisms of behavioral compensation into physiological studies.

INTRODUCTION ing from a current reproductive event


Energy use during mammalian repro- (Trivers, 1972). Parental investment has
duction has received considerable atten- typically been assessed via either behav-
tion in recent years (see Harvey, 1986; ioral parameters, such as suckling fre-
Loudon and Racey, 1987), particularly in quency or duration (e.g., Ortiz et al., 1984;
association with studies of sexual selection, Costa and Gentry, 1986), or net produc-
sex allocation, and parental investment. tion (e.g., litter mass at birth or weaning).
Studies usually fall into one of two broadly In contrast, physiologists have studied
denned categories: those concerned pri- energetic costs of reproduction by moni-
marily with evolutionary (=fitness) conse- toring caloric intake, net production, met-
abolic rate, and/or daily energy expendi-
quences (e.g., Clutton-Brock et al., 1982; ture (DEE) during specific reproductive
Ortiz et al., 1984) or those interested in the events (usually gestation or lactation), in
energetic (=physiological) costs associated an attempt to understand the energetic
with varying reproductive patterns (e.g., consequences of variation in parameters
Smith and McManus, 1975; Glazier, 19856; such as body size, litter size, and fecundity
Mattingly and McClure, 1985). Evolu- (e.g., Glazier, 1985a, b; Mattingly and
tionary biologists and behavioral ecologists McClure, 1985). One aim of this paper is
have been primarily concerned with paren- to emphasize the importance of both
tal investment, denned as costs to an indi- behavioral and physiological techniques for
vidual's future reproductive success result- the study of energy use during reproduc-
tion. We hope to show that (1) precise mea-
surements of energetic constraints and
1
From the Symposium on Energetics and Animal consequences of reproductive behaviors
Behavior presented at the Annual Meeting of the must be incorporated into studies of mam-
American Society of Zoologists, 27-30 December malian behavior and (2) behavioral flexi-
1986, at Nashville, Tennessee.
863
864 J . L. GlTTLEMAN AND S. D. THOMPSON

bility (e.g., in duration and/or frequency) nal recovery from reproduction (Elwood
may dramatically alter the relative ener- and Broom, 1978).
getic costs of specific reproductive events Second, most mammalian reproductive
(see also Goldstein, 1988). Although we events and various energetic constituents
primarily address problems related to are correlated with body size (see Eisen-
mammalian reproduction, we believe a berg, 1981; Calder, 1984; Millar, 1984;
general merging of energetics and behav- Schmidt-Nielson, 1984; Gittleman, 1986).
ioral perspectives, encompassing multiple Resting metabolism (Kleiber, 1961), exis-
measures of costs, will foster more com- tence metabolism (Kendeigh, 1969), birth
plete analyses of the costs of reproduction and weaning masses (Millar, 1977), birth
in most organisms (see also Bronson, 1979; weight (Leitch et al., 1959), and gestation

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/icb/article/28/3/863/99186 by guest on 18 February 2021


Calow, 1981, 1984; Altmann, 1983, 1986; length (Kihlstrom, 1972; Eisenberg, 1981),
Knapton, 1984; Bennett, 1986; Costa and among others, demonstrate an exponential
Gentry, 1986; Halliday, 1987). relationship to body size where the expo-
nent of these relationships is < 1.0. In con-
PROBLEMS AND PERSPECTIVES IN THE trast, functions describing capacity terms,
COSTS OF MAMMALIAN REPRODUCTION for example digestive capacity (Kleiber,
On behavioral (Eisenberg, 1981), hor- 1961) and storage capacity (Calder, 1984),
monal (Rosenblatt and Siegel, 1983) and bear a nearly linear relationship to body
physiological (Millar, 1977) grounds, mass. These allometric relationships affect
mammalian reproduction can be compart- the relative ability of mammals differing in
mentalized into the following continuous size to respond to periods of energy depri-
sequence of reproductive events: mating vation or abundance. All else being equal,
(courtship, estrous period), gestation, a larger mammal can store proportionally
parturition, lactation (suckling period), more energy and, likewise, draw upon pro-
post-lactational parental care, and post-lac- portionally more reserves than a small
tational/post-parental care maternal mammal (Lindstedt and Boyce, 1985); as
recovery. Most energetic studies have extreme examples, some large mammals
focused on either caloric consumption or such as lactating gray seals (Davies, 1949),
maternal metabolic rate, in conjunction rutting red deer (Mitchell etal., 1976), and
with changes in maternal (during gesta- many lactating seals (Bartholomew, 1952;
tion) and offspring mass, during only one Bowen et al., 1985) exclusively depend on
reproductive event (usually gestation or an ability to fast through reproductive
lactation). Although an evolutionary per- events. Many large mammals store energy
spective requires that relative values of for carryover from one reproductive event
these measures be used in comparisons of to another, usually from pregnancy to lac-
energetic costs of mammalian reproduc- tation, which may confound the relative
tion, both within and between species, cost of each. This carryover is a particu-
recent detailed studies show that, for sev- larly difficult problem when pregnancy and
eral reasons, comparative statements are lactation events are studied separately.
difficult to make. Third, comparative studies have repeat-
First, even though mammalian repro- edly shown that phylogeny is an important
ductive events appear discrete, intrinsic component to life history evolution (see
variability in parameters such as birth Stearns, 1983; Harvey and Clutton-Brock,
weight, litter size, weaning age, weaning 1985; Gittleman, 1986, 1988a). Life his-
weight, and inter-birth interval, both within tory factors are often coupled with ener-
and across species, often confound com- getic constraints (see McNab, 1980; Lind-
parisons of energy expended during re- stedt and Calder, 1981; Calder, 1984), and
production. For example, larger litters de- it follows that phylogeny may play an
mand greater energy expenditures during important role in setting energetic costs,
lactation (Millar, 1978; Mattingly and even at lower taxonomic (familial, generic)
McClure, 1982; Glazier, 19856) and, in levels. For example, Kenagy and Barthol-
some small mammals, produce slow mater- omew's (1985) study of reproductive pat-
ENERGETICS AND MAMMALIAN REPRODUCTION 865

terns infivecoexisting desert rodents shows ular reproductive characteristics (e.g., male
that, although the diurnal antelope ground ejaculate; parturition; paternal care) which
squirrel (Ammospermophilus leucurus) is sym-may be costly but have yet to receive ener-
patric with four nocturnal heteromyids, its getic study.
timing of reproduction and reproductive
effort are more similar to marmotine squir- ENERGETICS OF MAMMALIAN
rels (ground squirrels, marmots, prairie
dogs, chipmunks) than to the heteromyids. REPRODUCTIVE EVENTS
As the authors conclude, "The comparison Mating
of A. leucurus with D. merriami [kangaroo Mating behavior includes interactions
rat] illustrates the importance of consid- between a male and female in a situation

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/icb/article/28/3/863/99186 by guest on 18 February 2021


ering whole suites of functional, reproduc- that leads to copulation. These interac-
tive, and life history traits when one exam- tions include investigatory behavior (mate
ines adaptation to a particular environment. searching), mounting, lordosis, intromis-
It also shows that the fixity of a series of sion, ejaculation and any observable
characters in the genotype can be a barrier postcopulatory interactions. Although each
("phylogenetic constraint") to the evolu- of these behaviors has been extensively
tion of a major change in life history." analyzed from hormonal, neural, evolu-
Fourth, experimental studies make it tionary, and genetic approaches (Beach,
apparent that within single populations 1965; Diakow, 1974), energetic studies are
individuals vary considerably in the quality still lacking. This is ironic considering that
and quantity of maternal care, energy uti- some of the earliest studies in ethology used
lization, and ability to produce milk, all of energetic terms. For example, a classic par-
which will tilt the balance of energy costs adigm in studies of mating tested for the
(Bronson and Rissman, 1986; Thompson number of ejaculates before an individual
and Nicoll, 1986). In domestic rats, for reached "exhaustion" (e.g., Beach and Jor-
example, we know that such variation dan, 1956; Tiefer, 1969), with the pre-
relates to heredity, neonatal development, sumption that a male's energetic capacity
prior maternal experiences (age), and psy- was practically "unlimited."
chological state (e.g., Denenberg et al., Apparently only one study has directly
1962; Morton et al, 1963). Although indi- measured energy consumption during the
vidual differences provide case studies for mammalian mating period. Kenagy (1987),
examining adaptive allocation of energy in working with golden-mantled ground
fitness terms, i.e., life-time reproductive squirrels (Spermophilus saturatus), found that
success (see Charnov, 1982; Clutton-Brock, energy expenditure (kj/day; measured
1985; Stewart, 1986), small sample sizes using doubly labeled water) in males is
and high variability should demand cau- 2.5 x BMR and in females 2.0 x BMR,
tion, even when extrapolating between the difference mainly due to greater body
closely related species. mass in males. The total energy expendi-
In addition to the above difficulties, few ture during mating is only slightly greater
studies have directly measured energy costs than periods outside of reproduction.
in mammalian reproduction and the meth- Many behavioral studies have suggested
ods vary greatly from study to study that the energetic costs of mating are sig-
(Randolph et al., 1977; Millar, 1978; nificant. Males of larger species tend to
McClure and Randolph, 1980; Glazier, show significant increases in activity levels
1985a, b; Costa and Gentry, 1986; Kunz (frequency and duration), decline in feed-
and Nagy, 1987). Thus, our discussion of ing, and loss of as much as 20% body weight
the relative energetic costs of reproductive during mating (McCullough, 1969). In male
events will focus on: (1) different meth- red deer the proportion of daytime spent
odologies in measuring energy use, (2) grazing fell from 44% outside of the breed-
broad patterns of energy consumption, ing season to less than 5% during the rut
across diverse taxa, for specific reproduc- (Clutton-Brock et al., 1982); similar pat-
tive events, and (3) identification of" partic- terns are observed in other ungulates
866 J . L. GlTTLEMAN AND S. D . THOMPSON

(Struhsaker, 1967; McCullough, 1969). In reduced numbers of sperm in successive


general, females do not appear to incur ejaculates (Dewsbury and Sawrey, 1984),
large energetic demands while mating. often for periods of up to a week, and lim-
Nevertheless, some energetic costs are sug- itations of other hormonal substances crit-
gested because stress induced situations, ical to successful mating; in humans, when
such where food resources decline, cor- weight loss is in the range of 25% normal
relate with decreases in length of breeding body weight, sperm production ceases
season, breeding success, and estrous (Frisch, 1984). Furthermore, repetitive
cycling (see Sadleir, 1969). It should be copulation for pregnancy initiation, female
pointed out that a primary problem in choice and control, and risks of searching
assessing energy usage during mating for a mate all entail presumed energetic costs

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/icb/article/28/3/863/99186 by guest on 18 February 2021


events is the brief time-frame in which they and limit male reproductive capacity (see
occur. Thus, it is especially difficult to sep- Dewsbury, 1982). Specific energetic mea-
arate the costs of mating per se from other surements, yet to be completed, may pro-
daily activities. vide critical constraints on copulatory abil-
With respect to mate access and repro- ities and, interestingly, may suggest causal
ductive success (RS), some indirect mea- explanations for alternative breeding strat-
sures indicate the importance of metabolic egies in males (see Gibson and Guinness,
rate. In European rabbits there is a positive 1980).
correlation between resting metabolic rate Finally, perhaps the most extreme exam-
(RMR) and social status among groups of ple of the evolutionary and energetic costs
males organized in a stable hierarchy; the of mating are in the marsupials, where post-
costs of body maintenance appear to be mating mortality occurs in males of several
higher for high-ranking males with species of Antechinus. Such mortality is
increased values of RS (Bell, 1983, 1986). partly caused by gastrointestinal ulceration
However, subordinant tree-shrews (Tupaia and suppression of the immune system
glis) and deer mice (Peromyscus maniculatus) brought on by stress. But there also appears
have lower rates of metabolism than dom- to be an energetic reason. Using doubly
inants (Farr and Andrews, 1978a, b; Fuchs labeled water, Nagy el al. (1978) showed
and Kleinknecht, 1986). Certainly, there that there is no substantial increase in daily
are several other interrelated variables energy expenditure during the mating sea-
which influence higher RS in dominant son. However, since the resting metabolic
males (e.g., access to better breeding areas; rate may increase in males by 17% at the
earlier start to breeding: see Rutberg, time of mating (Cheal et al., 1976), there
1986); nevertheless, these high metabolic may be a reduction in energy spent on other
rates in dominants and subordinates of dif- activities. There is indirect evidence that
ferent species may reflect alternative ener- energy used in mating may substitute for
getic constraints rather than contradictory a reduction in feeding (Lee and Cockburn,
data. Thus, in Tupaia and Peromyscus sub- 1985).
ordinates may have high RMRs due to stress
from continual testing of the hierarchy
whereas in rabbits subordinates may be Gestation
more passive. Total energy investment during preg-
It is often assumed that most of the ener- nancy involves many components includ-
getic costs of mating in males is associated ing net production of fetal, uterine, pla-
with searching for prospective mates and cental, and mammary tissue, production
male-male conflicts. Although sperm are costs ("the work of growth" [Brody, 1945]),
vastly smaller and the cost of testicular and increased maintenance costs associ-
growth is negligible, numerous studies now ated with these new tissues (i.e., the metab-
indicate that ejaculate cost in terms of some olism of added mass). Direct measurement
behavioral and physiological measures is of each of these components is difficult and
significant (Dewsbury, 1982). Sperm pro- generally has been restricted to laboratory
duction is generally limited in terms of: or domesticated species (e.g., McC. Gra-
ENERGETICS AND MAMMALIAN REPRODUCTION 867

ham, 1964; Myrcha et al., 1969; Studier et white rats, for example, there was little or
al., 1973; Havera, 1979; Oftedal, 1985). no difference in food consumption between
Neonatal mass (birth weight) is often pregnant and non-reproductive females
employed as an indirect measure of uterine (Slonaker, 1925; Wang, 1925); however,
and placental tissue masses and conse- activity (=wheel running) was 57-96%
quently may estimate the energetic cost of lower in the pregnant individuals. Thus, at
pregnancy (Millar, 1977, 1981; Oftedal, least some species may shift allocation of
1985; Kunz, 1987; Kurta and Kunz, 1987). energy from activity to gestation by reduc-
Caloric consumption studies reveal consid- ing the frequency and/or duration of cer-
erable variation in net production of fetal tain behaviors (Racey, 1981, 1982); with-
and placental tissues. Production efficiency out integration of behavioral and energetic

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/icb/article/28/3/863/99186 by guest on 18 February 2021


ranges from about 10—15%, with domestic data, this tactic can result in an underes-
mammals clustering at the high end and timation of the total energy allocated to
wild mammals tending towards the low end gestation. A combination of caloric con-
of the range (Myrcha et al., 1969; Studier sumption, respirometry, and time budgets
et al, 1973; Oftedal, 1985); placental and of behavior, before and during gestation
uterine proliferations account for about (reproduction) is a requisite for assessment
20% of the net caloric investment during of the allocation of energy to maintenance,
gestation (Oftedal, 1985). Howover, al- "the work of growth" (Brody, 1945), and
though the relative size of neonate(s) may net production during gestation. Respi-
serve as a useful first approximation of rometry may be particularly important
energy consumption during gestation, sig- because some species, especially those with
nificant changes in maintenance require- low basal metabolic rates (Nicoll and
ments, due to increased mass a n d / o r Thompson, 1987; but see Nagy and Mont-
increased specific metabolic rates, cannot gomery, 1980), show increased resting
be detected solely through examination of metabolic rates during gestation, presum-
net production; these may involve signifi- ably as a result of costs associated with
cant energetic expenditures during gesta- growth of the fetus. However, this
tion (Partridge et al., 1986; Thompson and increased metabolism is not due to a dif-
Nicoll, 1986; Nicoll and Thompson, 1987). ferentially high rate of metabolism in fetal
tissue (Kleiber, 1961).
Studies of the energetic costs of gesta-
tion have focused on caloric intake, some- For pregnant rodents, increase in mean
times augmented with respirometry {e.g., daily caloric intake over non-reproductive
Randolph et al., 1977). Measurement of rates ranges from 18-25% {e.g., Kaczmar-
caloric intake by itself can produce mis- ski, 1966; Migula, 1969; Myrcha et al.,
leading estimates of reproductive costs. 1969; Mattingly and McClure, 1982) and
Digestive efficiencies do change slightly at least one of these species may store
during gestation and lactation, although energy as fat for use during lactation {i.e.,
the low magnitude of these changes usually Sigmodon hispidus: Randolph et al., 1977;
leads to dismissal of their importance {e.g., see also Mattingly and McClure, 1985).
Oftedal, 1985; but see Mattingly and For most rodents, stored fat is not impor-
McClure, 1982). Of greater significance, tant for gestation. White-footed mice
however, is the potential for compensa- {Peromyscus) with food availability 80-90%
tion, via reallocation of energy from some of ad lib had no effect on body weight or
nonreproductive level of activity or ther- fat content, but nevertheless fecundity sig-
moregulatory (maintenance) expenditure. nificantly decreased (Merson and Kirkpat-
It is difficult to compare caloric intake rick, 1981); similar effects of reduced
data between species (or individuals) with- fecundity associated with lowered food
out knowledge of (1) ambient tempera- reserves are found in other small mammals
tures during measurement, (2) limits of (see Kenagy and Bartholomew, 1985). Fat
thermoneutrality (or natural nest temper- storage may be of importance to bats, how-
atures), and (3) variation in activity levels ever, because of their unavoidably high
before and after reproduction. In some activity levels and obvious flight costs. For
868 J . L. GlTTLEMAN AND S. D. THOMPSON

example, in some species (e.g., big brown ponent of the daily energy budget of a
bat, Eptesicus fuscus) the fraction of body female mammal and, in the least, may influ-
fat increases by as much as 68% in one week ence the ability of a female to expend
during mid-pregnancy (Stack, 1985) and if energy during early lactation.
storage is unavailable foetal growth rates
and pregnancy rates often decline (Racey, Lactation
1973; Kurta, 1986). Although the causal Milk production represents the single
mechanisms of these results are not known, most influential and unique feature of
it appears from more extensive data on mammalian reproduction (Maynard Smith,
other species that smaller mammals may 1977; Pond, 1977; Daly, 1979). The com-
be more "hard-wired" to gestation length plexity of energy transfer and energy use

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/icb/article/28/3/863/99186 by guest on 18 February 2021


itself rather than storage. After controlling during lactation has motivated a wide array
for body size, birth weight, litter size, and of physiological and behavioral studies. In
body temperature (Racey, 1981), increases addition to measuring caloric intake,
in ingestion rates correlate most closely with labeled water, analysis of milk composition
gestation length (e.g., Millar, 1977, 1978; and production/consumption have been
McClure, 1987). widely employed to estimate the energy
In contrast to rodents, larger mammals requirements for lactation (Oftedal, 1985;
(ungulates) buffer the costs of gestation by Gittleman and Oftedal, 1987; Kunz and
laying down fat reserves prior to concep- Nagy, 1987); respirometry has been used
tion (see Frisch, 1984); if storage is not to a lesser extent (e.g., Thompson and
adequate, pregnancy rates decline (peary Nicoll, 1986). Measurements of the caloric
caribou: Thomas, 1982; barren ground content of milk have been combined with
caribou: Dauphine, 1976; wild and domes- either behavioral indices of suckling rates
tic reindeer: Klein and White, 1978). Well- (frequency and duration) or changes in mass
fed pregnant ewes with heavy fat reserves of suckling young and mother (Ortiz et al.,
had only about half the foraging intake of 1984; Gittleman and Oftedal, 1987). How-
lean ewes (Reid, 1961). Ingestion rates dur- ever, analyses of milk production do not
ing gestation are generally not as high as assess metabolic costs of milk production
during lactation (see Fleming et al., 1981), or maternal maintenance (e.g., increased
thus the inference being that milk produc- rates of maternal metabolism) and will tend
tion is more costly; indeed, because ges- to underestimate overall costs of lactation
tation costs are less (particularly per day), (also see discussion in Oftedal, 1985).
this may explain why, rather than have Lactation is generally considered the
flexible lactation periods, in some taxa ges- most expensive aspect of reproduction for
tation is extended during harsh ecological a female mammal (e.g., Hanwell and Peaker,
conditions (e.g., hystricomorphs: Short, 1977; Millar, 1977, 1978; Randolph et al.,
1985) or gestation varies in relation to 1977; Oftedal, 1985). Mean caloric intake
dominance status (e.g., anthropoid pri- during lactation ranges from 66-188%
mates: Altmann, 1986). greater than for non-reproductives (e.g.,
Although the birthing period is brief rel- McC. Graham, 1964; Kaczmarski, 1966;
ative to other reproductive events, behav- Migula, 1969; Stebbins, 1977; Randolph
ioral, hormonal, and energetic changes et al. 1977; Millar, 1978; Mattingly and
indicate some costs. Peak rates of energy McClure, 1982; Sadleir, 1982; Glazier,
use by pregnant mammals typically occur 1985a, b). These levels may vary directly
within the few days preceding parturition. with litter size (e.g., Smith and McManus,
In domestic cattle, heat loss increases sig- 1975; Millar, 1978; Sadleir, 1982)andmay
nificantly (200-300 kcal/day) during labor be over 200% in digestively inefficient
and parturition due to some combination species (e.g., the folivorous "carnivore," the
of increased fetal activity and the onset of red panda, Ailurus fulgens: Gittleman,
fetal thermoregulation (Brockway et al., 19886; see also Studier, 1979). Peak levels
1963; Oftedal, 1985). Thus, energetic costs of energy use may approach 2.5-5 times
of parturition may be an important com- those of non-reproductive females (see ref-
ENERGETICS AND MAMMALIAN REPRODUCTION 869

erences above). However, despite high nancy and lactation (Racey and Speakman,
energy expenditures during lactation, net 1987).
production ranges from about 15-45% (see The daily rates of energy transfer are
Smith and McManus, 1975; Mattingly and most spectacular in some marine mammals
McClure, 1982; Glazier, 19856; Oftedal, where females lactate intensively for rela-
1985) which is similar to, or even slightly tively brief periods (four weeks or less), fast
higher than, gestation. from food and water during lactation, pro-
Behavioral changes parallel caloric intake duce energy-rich milk with high fat con-
results. Numerous studies, particularly of tent, and wean their pups abruptly (see
larger species, show that feeding bout Fedak and Anderson, 1982;Bonner, 1984;
lengths may increase by as much as 30%; Ortiz et al, 1984; Costa et al, 1986; Costa

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/icb/article/28/3/863/99186 by guest on 18 February 2021


frequency of feeding, and increased food and Gentry, 1986). Prior to weaning, the
selection are associated with the onset of sole source of nutrients for the developing
lactation (see Clutton-Brock et al, 1982; pup is mother's milk. Thus, the total
Dunbar, 1984; Duncans al., 1984;Berger, amount of milk and metabolic energy
1986). However, as previously mentioned, expended by a female during the entire
fat deposition prior to breeding or during lactation period can be calculated by
gestation may supplement energy needs extrapolating the daily milk (via pup water
during lactation (see also Pond, 1984; Ver- flux and milk composition) and metabolic
non and Flint, 1984; McClure, 1987) and expenditure rates. Costa et al. (1986) show
may affect litter size, sex ratio, offspring that, in elephant seals (Mirounga angusti-
size and brain size at weaning (e.g., McC. rostris), the high energy cost of lactation is
Graham, 1964; Widdowson, 1981; reflected by females losing approximately
McClure, 1987). 42% of their body mass and expending
Lactation also imposes significant 4,330 mj even though lactation only lasts
demands to water balance, particularly for 26.5 days. Further metabolic savings are
mammals from xeric habitats (e.g., Soholt, achieved by minimizing activity levels:
1977). The water in milk may be partly females spend considerable amounts of
recycled via the young (Friedman and time asleep and remain within a few meters
Bruno, 1976): in rats, if micturation is pre- of the parturition site for the entire lac-
vented by urethral ligation, the transfer of tation period. Such energy saving mecha-
water from mother to pups is greatly nisms, especially the lowered dependence
reduced, and the mother's water intake on food energy intake to offset lactation
increases. As would be predicted, when a costs, may be a critical strategy allowing
lactating female is deprived of water intake for the elephant seal's remarkable return
(e.g., during hibernation), the degree of in population numbers (see Costa et al,
water recycling is higher (e.g., black bears, 1986). Unfortunately, there are no mea-
Ursus americanus: Oftedal, in preparation). surements of the energetic costs of gesta-
tion in marine mammals.
As in gestation, there is evidence that
some species divert energy from activity or Three caveats need to be made with the
maintenance to reproduction by reducing general presumption that lactation is
time allocations. In bats, even though the expensive. First, milk composition, milk
total energy expenditure during lactation quantity, and consequently maternal
may increase by 20-40% (see Anthony and energy output vary during the lactation
Kunz, 1977; Kunz, 1987), females may shift period (Oftedal, 1980, 1984) and this may
energy expenditure from maintenance drastically affect perceptions of efficiency
metabolism, via relaxation of homeo- and investment (Glazier, 19856). Thus, data
thermy, to lactation, thus avoiding or min- from studies not controlling for lactation
imizing increased food consumption. In stage are difficult to evaluate. Second,
brown long-eared bats (Plecotus auritus) the because significant increases occur during
rate of increase in both absolute energy lactation in various behaviors (e.g., activity
expenditure and expenditure relative to cycle, nest building, huddling, aggression,
basal metabolism were similar during preg- licking: see Ewer, 1973; Leuthold, 1977;
870 J . L. GlTTLEMAN AND S. D. THOMPSON

Galef, 1981; Clutton-Brock et al., 1982; Transporting an infant weighing 1.5 kg


Ostermeyer, 1983; Gittleman, 19886) and when six months old will increase the
in anatomical weight {e.g., mammary tissue, energy cost of locomotion to a 15 kg male
gut, heart: see Williamson, 1980; Sampson by 10% for the interval of carrying while
and Jansen, 1984), it is difficult to tease eliminating the added cost of locomotion
apart the relative energetic costs of lacta- to the infant. Energy cost to such a male
tion (milk production) versus these other carrier will involve an increased metabolic
factors. Future studies should include mea- cost of 5.6% of his energy expenditure dur-
surements of energy use in various aspects ing a 12 hr day if an infant is carried over
of lactation, not only those associated with an all day route and 2.8% for a half day
milk production. Third, considerable vari- route. Alternatively, many female mam-

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/icb/article/28/3/863/99186 by guest on 18 February 2021


ation in lactation costs may be associated mals show significant weight loss during
with intra-litter variation. Recent empiri- lactation and often display maternal weight
cal data and theoretical models show that at weaning that is lower than that before
in some species mothers differentially invest mating. The cost of reattaining that initial
in males and females (see, e.g., Maynard body mass may be significant, and yet it is
Smith, 1980; Clutton-Brock and Albon, altogether unstudied.
1982; Lee and Moss, 1986), with increased
investment in the sex which has the more
variable reproductive success (in most
mammals, males) and is influenced by SYNTHESIS
parental investment. As Clutton-Brock et Several implications derive from this dis-
al. (1984) illustrate in red deer (Cervus ela-cussion for future studies of mammalian
phus), indicators of a female's energetic reproduction. First, most studies have con-
capacity (body weight, dominance) appear sidered gestation and lactation as the most
to be good predictors of differential invest- important reproductive events with respect
ment. Physiologists should, therefore, to energy expenditures. In this perspec-
incorporate this evolutionary approach in tive, about 20% of the energy is allocated
estimating costs of lactation. to gestation, 80% to lactation (e.g., Oftedal,
1985; see Table 1). However, the variation
in this allocation should be substantial and
Post-lactational parental care almost certainly is a function of the relative
Despite an explosion of studies exam- allocation of time to gestation vs. lactation;
ining the ecological and evolutionary fea- thus, as reflected by the predominance of
tures of parental care in mammals (see, e.g., question marks in Table 1, the use of one
Eisenberg, 1981; Gubernick and Klopfer, variable (e.g., gestation length) as an index
1981; Elwood, 1983; Taub, 1984; Gittle- of energetic cost (or parental investment)
man, 1985), no study has directly measured is likely to lead to erroneous explanations
the metabolic costs of parental care, either of reproductive strategies, sexual selec-
in a male or female. This is a glaring omis- tion, and sex allocation. Second, behav-
sion given repeated observations showing ioral compensation is potentially the most
the presumed costly increase in food pro- important tactic for minimizing additional
visioning, den site defense, infant carrying energy requirements during reproduction.
behavior, and teaching duties. Take, for The complexity of this strategy is such that
example, the energetic costs of carrying an attention need be given to changes in
infant by a male primate, a common fea- maternal time/energy budgets from mat-
ture in monogamous species (see Hamil- ing through weaning; without this infor-
ton, 1984; Altmann, 1986). Increases in mation, the costs of reproduction may be
energy costs of transporting a mass are seriously underestimated. Related to this
directly proportional to the relative mass are potential problems of presumed ener-
of the transported object and the lean body getic costs of behavior; every attempt
mass of the carrier (Taylor et al., 1980). should be made to determine that, for
ENERGETICS AND MAMMALIAN REPRODUCTION 871

TABLE 1. Relative energy allocation for mammalian reproduction.

Reproductive event vt total costs Efficiency


Mate access
Mating and courtship p
Gestation 20-30 20 10-15%
Lactation 35-149 80 15-45%
Parental care/recovery 5 3 5

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/icb/article/28/3/863/99186 by guest on 18 February 2021


example, suckling frequency and duration Evolution of behavior: Paleontological and field
actually reflect energy transfer. approaches, pp. 161-178. Oxford University Press,
Oxford.
Finally, our knowledge of reproductive Anthony, E. L. P. and T. H. Kunz. 1977. Feeding
energetics is limited to domesticated mam- strategies of the little brown bat, Myotis lucifugus,
mals, cricetine rodents, murine rodents, in southern New Hampshire. Ecology 58:775-
bats, pinnipeds, and a few wild ungulates. 786.
Bartholomew, G. A. 1952. Reproductive and social
Given the variability in energy use and allo- behavior of the northern elephant seal. Univ.
cation within these few species (see Nicoll Cal. Publ. Zool. 47:369-472.
and Thompson, 1987), phylogenetic con- Beach, F. A. 1965. Retrospect and prospect. In F.
straints on life history patterns (Gittleman, A. Beach (ed.), Sex and behavior, pp. 535-568.
1986) make it imperative that we increase Wiley, New York.
Beach, F. A. and L.Jordan. 1956. Sexual exhaustion
the breadth of our knowledge by exami- and recovery in the male rat. Q. Rev. Biol. 8:
nation of reproductive energetics in more 121-133.
distantly related taxa. Bell, D.J. 1983. Mate choice in the European rabbit.
In P. Bateson (ed.), Mate choice, pp. 211-233.
Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
Bell, D. J. 1986. Social effects on physiology in the
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS European rabbit. Mammal Rev. 16:131-137.
We thank the following for sending Bennett, A. F. 1986. Measuring behavioral energet-
ics. In M. E. Feder and G. V. Lauder (eds.), Pred-
reprints and information included in this ator-prey relationships, pp. 69—81. University of
review: D. Bell, D. A. Dewsbury, G. J. Chicago Press, Chicago.
Kenagy, P. A. McClure, J. S. Millar, O. T. Berger, J. 1986. Wild horses. University of Chicago
Oftedal, P. A. Racey. T. H. Kunz and G. Press, Chicago.
F. McCracken made useful comments on Bonner, W. N. 1984. Lactation strategies in pin-
nipeds: Problems for a marine mammal. Symp.
the manuscript. During preparation of this Zool. Soc. London 51:253-272.
paper, J.L.G. was supported by the Grad- Bowen, W. D., O. T. Oftedal, and D. J. Bonness.
uate Programs in Ecology and Ethology 1985. Birth to weaning in four days: Remarkable
(NIH Training Grant, T32-HD-07303), growth in the hooded seal, Systophora cristata. Can.
Department of Zoology, and the Science J. Zool. 63:2841-2846.
Brockway, J. M., J. D. Macdonald, and J. D. Pullar.
Alliance Program, all of the University of 1963. The energy cost of reproduction. J. Phys-
Tennessee; S.D.T. was supported by an iol. 167:318-327.
NSF Postdoctoral Fellowship in Environ- Brody, S. 1945. Bioenergetics and growth, with special
mental Biology. reference to the efficiency complex in domestic animals.
Reinhold, New York.
Bronson, F. H. 1979. The reproductive ecology of
the house mouse. Q. Rev. Biol. 54:265-299.
REFERENCES Bronson, F. H. and E. F. Rissman. 1986. The biology
of puberty. Biol. Rev. 61:157-195.
Altmann.J. 1983. Costs of reproduction in baboons. Calder, W. H., III. 1984. Size, function, and life history.
In W. P. Aspey and S. I. Lustick (eds.), Behavioral Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Massa-
energetics: The costs of survival, pp. 67-88. Ohio chusetts.
State University Press, Columbus, Ohio. Calow, P. 1981. Resource utilization and reproduc-
Altmann, J. 1986. Parent-offspring interactions in tion. In C. R. Townsend and P. Calow (eds.),
the Anthropoid primates: An evolutionary per- Physiological ecology, pp. 245-270. Sinauer, Sun-
spective. In M. H. Nitecki and J. A. Kitchell (eds.), derland, Massachusetts.
872 J . L. GlTTLEMAN AND S. D . THOMPSON

Calow, P. 1984. Economics of ontogeny—adapta- Eisenberg,J. F. 1981. The mammalian radiations. Uni-
tional aspects. In B. Shorrocks (ed.), Evolutionary versity of Chicago Press, Chicago.
ecology, pp. 81-104. Blackwell, Oxford. Elwood, R. W. (ed.) 1983. Parental behaviour in rodents.
Charnov, E. L. 1982. The theory of sex allocation. John Wiley and Sons, New York.
Princeton University Press, Princeton, New Jer- Elwood, R. W. and D. M. Broom. 1978. The influ-
sey. ence of litter size and parental behaviour on the
Cheal, P. D., A. K. Lee, and J. L. Barnett. 1976. development of the Mongolian gerbil pups. Anim.
Changes in the haemotology of Antechinus stuartii Behav. 26:438-454.
(Marsupialia), and their association with male Ewer, R. F. 1973. The carnivores. Cornell University
mortality. Aust.J. Zool. 24:299-311. Press, Ithaca, New York.
Clutton-Brock, T. H. 1985. Birth sex ratios and the Farr, L. and R. V. Andrews. 1978a. Rank-associated
reproductive success of sons and daughters. In P. differences in metabolic rates and locomotor
J. Greenwood, P. H. Harvey, and M. Slatkin (eds.), activity of dominant and subordinate Peromyscus

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/icb/article/28/3/863/99186 by guest on 18 February 2021


Evolution—essays in honour of John Maynard Smith, maniculatus. Comp. Biochem. Phys. 61A:401-406.
pp. 221-236. Cambridge University Press, Cam- Farr, L. and R. V. Andrews. 1978£. Rank-associated
bridge. desynchronization of metabolic and activity
Clutton-Brock, T. H. and S. D. Albon. 1982. Paren- rhythm of Peromyscus maniculatus in response to
tal investment in male and female offspring in social stress. Comp. Biochem. Phys. 61A:539-
mammals. In King's College Sociobiology Group 542.
(eds.), Current problems in Sociobiology, pp. 2 2 3 - Fedak, M. A. and S. S. Anderson. 1982. The ener-
247. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. getics of lactation: Accurate measurements from
Clutton-Brock, T. H., S. D. Albon, and F. E. Guinness. a large mammal, the Grey seal (Halichoerus gry-
1984. Maternal dominance, breeding success and pus). J. Zool. 198:473-479.
birth sex ratios in red deer. Nature 308:358-360. Fleming, M. W., J. D. Harder, andj. J. Wukie. 1981.
Clutton-Brock, T. H., F. E. Guinness, and S. D. Albon. Reproductive energetics of the Virginia opposum
1982. Red deer: Behavior and ecology of two sexes. compared with some Eutherians. Comp. Bio-
University of Chicago Press, Chicago. chem. Physio. 70B:645-648.
Costa, D. P. and R. L. Gentry. 1986. Free-ranging Friedman, M. I. andj. P. Bruno. 1976. Exchange of
energetics of northern fur seals. In R. L. Gentry water during lactation. Science 197:409-410.
and G. L. Kooyman (eds.), Fur seals, pp. 79—101. Frisch, R. E. 1984. Body fat, puberty and fertility.
Princeton University Press, Princeton, New Jer- Biol. Rev. 59:161-188.
sey. Fuchs, E. and S. Kleinknecht. 1986. The influence
Costa, D. P., B. J. Le Boeuf, A. C. Huntley, and C. of chronic social confrontation on oxygen con-
L. Ortiz. 1986. The energetics of lactation in sumption of Iupia belangeri under resting con-
the northern elephant seal, Mirounga angustiros- ditions. Z. Sauget. 51:57-59.
tris.J. Zool. 209:21-33. Galef, B. G., Jr. 1981. The ecology of weaning. In
Daly, M. 1979. Why don't male mammals lactatePJ. D. J. Gubernick and P. H. Klopfer (eds.), Parental
Theor. Biol. 78:325-345. care in mammals, pp. 211-241. Plenum, New York.
Dauphine, T. C. 1976. Biology of Kamanuriak pop- Gibson, R. M. and F.E. Guinness. 1980. Behavioural
ulation of barren-ground caribou. Part 4: Growth, factors affecting male reproductive success in red
reproduction and energy reserves. Can. Wild. deer (Cervus elaphus L.). Anim. Behav. 28:1163—
Serv. Rep. Ser. No. 38. 1174.
Davies, J. L. 1949. Observations on the grey seal
(Halichoerus grypus) at Ramsey Island, Pembroke- Gittleman, J. L. 1985. Functions of communal care
shire. Proc. Zool. Soc. London 119:673-692. in mammals. In P. J. Greenwood, P. H. Harvey,
Denenberg, V. H., D. R. Ottinger, and M. W. Ste- and M. Slatkin (eds.), Evolution: Essays in honour
phens. 1962. Effects of maternal factors upon ofJohn Maynard Smith, pp. 187—205. Cambridge
growth and behavior of the rat. Child Develop. University Press, Cambridge.
33:65-71. Gittleman, J. L. 1986. Carnivore life history pat-
Dewsbury, D. A. 1982. Ejaculate cost and mate choice. terns: Allometric, phylogenetic and ecological
Am. Nat. 119:601-610. associations. Am. Nat. 127:744-771.
Dewsbury, D. A. and D. K. Sawrey. 1984. Male Gittleman, J. L. 1988a. The comparative approach
capacity as related to sperm production, preg- in ethology: Aims and limitations. In P. P. G.
nancy initiation, and sperm competition in deer Bateson and P. H. Klopfer (eds.), Perspectives in
mice (Peromyscus maniculatus). Behav. Ecol. Socio- ethology, Vol. 8, Plenum Press, New York.
biol. 16:37-47. Gittleman, J. L. 19886. Behavioral energetics of lac-
Diakow, C. 1974. Male-female interactions and the tation in a herbivorous carnivore, the red panda
organization of mammalian mating patterns. Adv. (Ailurus fulgens). Ethology. (In press)
Study Behav. 5:227-268. Gittleman, J. L. and O. T. Oftedal. 1987. Compar-
Dunbar, R. I. M. 1984. Reproductive decisions. Prince- ative growth and lactation energetics in carni-
ton University Press, Princeton, New Jersey. vores. In A. Loudon and P. A. Racey (eds.), Repro-
Duncan, P., P. H. Harvey, and S. M. Wells. 1984. ductive energetics in mammah, pp. 41—77. Oxford
On lactation and associated behaviour in a nat- University Press, Oxford.
ural herd of horses. Anim. Behav. 32:255-263. Glazier, D. S. 1985«. Energetics of litter size in five
ENERGETICS AND MAMMALIAN REPRODUCTION 873

species of Peromyscus with generalizations for other and behavioral methods for the study of bats. Smith-
mammals. J. Mamm. 66:629-642. sonian Institution Press, Washington, D.C.
Glazier, D. S. 19856. Relationship between metabolic Kurta, A. 1986. Factors affecting the resting and
rate and energy expenditure for lactation in Pero- postflight body temperature of little brown bats,
myscus. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. 80A:587-590. Myotis lucifugus. Phys. Zool. 59:429-438.
Goldstein, D. L. 1988. Estimates of daily energy Kurta, A. and T. H. Kunz. 1987. Size of bats at birth
expenditure in birds: The time-energy budget as and maternal investment during pregnancy. In
an integrator of laboratory and field studies. A. Loudon and P. A. Racey (eds.), Reproductive
Amer. Zool. 28:829-844. energetics in mammals, pp. 79-106. Oxford Uni-
Gubernick, D. J. and P. H. Klopfer. (eds.) 1981. versity Press, Oxford.
Parental care in mammals. Plenum, New York. Lee, A. K. and A. Cockburn. 1985. The evolutionary
Halliday, T. R. 1987. Physiological constraints on ecology of marsupials. Cambridge University Press,

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/icb/article/28/3/863/99186 by guest on 18 February 2021


sexual selection. In J. W. Bradbury and M. B. Cambridge.
Andersson (eds.), Sexual selection: Testing the Lee, P. C. and C. J. Moss. 1986. Early maternal
alternatives, pp. 247-264. John Wiley & Sons, investment in male and female African elephant
New York. calves. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 18:353-361.
Hamilton, W.J., III. 1984. Significance of paternal Leitch, I. F., F. F. Hytten, and W. Z. Billewicz. 1959.
investment by primates to the evolution of adult The maternal and neonatal weights of some
male-female associations. In D. M. Taub (ed.), Mammalia. Proc. Zool. Soc. London 135:11-28.
Primate paternalism, pp. 309-335. Van Nostrand Leuthold, W. 1977. African ungulates. Springer-Ver-
Reinhold, New York. lag, Berlin.
Hanwell, A. and M. Peaker. 1977. Physiological Lindstedt, S. L. and M. S. Boyce. 1985. Seasonality,
effects of lactation on the mother. Symp. Zool. fasting endurance, and body size in mammals.
Soc. London 41:297-311. Am. Nat. 125:873-878.
Harvey, P. H. 1986. Energetic costs of reproduction. Lindstedt, S. L. and W. A. Calder. 1981. Body size,
Nature 321:648-649. physiological time, and longevity of homeo-
Harvey, P. H. and T. H. Clutton-Brock. 1985. Life thermic animals. Q. Rev. Biol. 56:1-16.
history variation in primates. Evolution 39:559- Loudon, A. and P. A. Racey. (eds.) 1987. The repro-
581. ductive energetics of mammals. Oxford University
Havera, S. P. 1979. Energy and nutrient costs of Press, Oxford.
lactation in fox squirrels. J. Wild. Mgt. 43:958- Mattingly, D. K. and P. A. McClure. 1982. Ener-
965. getics of reproduction in large-littered cotton rats
Kaczmarski, F. 1966. Bioenergetics of pregnancy and (Sigmodon hispidis). Ecology 63:183-195.
lactation in the bank vole. Acta Theriol. 11:409- Mattingly, D. K. and P. A. McClure. 1985. Energy
417. allocation during lactation in cotton rats (Sigmo-
Kenagy, G.J. 1987. Energy allocation for reproduc- don hispidus) on a restricted diet. Ecology 66:928—
tion in the golden-manteled ground squirrel. In 937.
A. Loudon and P. A. Racey (eds.), Reproductive Maynard Smith, J. 1977. Parental investment—a
energetics in mammals, pp. 259-273. Oxford Uni- prospective analysis. Anim. Behav. 25:1-9.
versity Press, Oxford. Maynard Smith, J. 1980. A new theory of sexual
Kenagy, G.J. and G. A. Bartholomew. 1985. Sea- investment. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 7:247-251.
sonal reproductive patterns infivecoexisting Cal- McC. Graham, N. 1964. Energy exchanges of preg-
ifornia desert rodent species. Ecol. Mon. 55:371 — nant and lactating ewes. Aust. J. Agric. Res. 15:
397. 127-141.
Kendeigh, S. C. 1969. Tolerance of cold and Berg- McClure, P. A. 1987. The energetics of reproduc-
mann's rule. Auk 86:13-25. tion and life histories of cricetine rodents. In A.
Loudon and P. A. Racey (eds.), The reproductive
Kihlstrom, J. P. 1972. Period of gestation and body energetics of mammals, pp. 241-258. Oxford Uni-
weight in some placental mammals. Comp. Bio- versity Press, Oxford.
chem. Physio. 43A:673-679. McClure, P. A. and J. C. Randolph. 1980. Relative
Kleiber, M. 1961. Thefereof life. An introduction to allocation of energy to growth and development
animal energetics. Wiley, New York. of homeothermy in the eastern wood rat (Neoloma
Klein, D. R. and R. G. White. 1978. Parameters of ftoridana) and hispid cotton rat (Sigmodon hispi-
caribou population ecology in Alaska. Biol. Pap. dus). Ecol. Mon. 50:199-219.
Univ. Alaska Spec. Rep. No. 3. McCullough.D. R. 1969. The tule elk: Its history, behav-
Knapton, R. W. 1984. Parental investment: The ior and ecology. University of California Press,
problem of currency. Can. J. Zool. 62:2673-2674. Berkeley, California.
Kunz, T. H. 1987. Post-natal growth and energetics McNab, B. K. 1980. Food habits, energetics, and the
of suckling bats. In M. B. Fenton, P. A. Racey, population biology of mammals. Am. Nat. 116:
and J. M. V. Rayner (eds.), Recent advances in the 106-124.
study of bats, pp. 395-420. Cambridge University Merson, M. H. and R. C. Kirkpatrick. 1981. Relative
Press, Cambridge. sensitivity of reproduction activity and body-fat
Kunz, T. H. and K. A. Nagy. 1987. Methods of energy level to food restriction in white mice. Am. Midi.
budget analysis. In T. H. Kunz (ed.), Ecological Nat. 106:305-312.
874 J . L. GlTTLEMAN AND S. D . THOMPSON

Migula, P. 1969. Bioenergetics of pregnancy and lac- importance of energy storage in the evolution of
tation in European common vole. Acta Theriol. lactation: Evidence for a common pattern of ana-
14:167-179. tomical organization of adipose tissue in mam-
Millar, J. S. 1977. Adaptive features of mammalian mals. Symp. Zool. Soc. London 51:1—32.
reproduction. Evolution 31:370-386. Racey, P. A. 1973. Environmental factors affecting
Millar.J. S. 1978. Energeticsof reproduction in Pero- the length of gestation in bats. J. Reproduct. Fer-
myscus leucopus: The cost of lactation. Ecology 59: til. Suppl. 19:175-189.
1055-1061. Racey, P. A. 1981. Environmental factors affecting
Millar, J. S. 1981. Pre-partum reproductive charac- the length of gestation in mammals. In D. Gil-
teristics of eutherian mammals. Evolution 35: more and B. Cook (eds.), Environmental factors in
1149-1163. mammal reproduction, pp. 199-213. University
Millar, J. S. 1984. The role of design constraints in Park Press, Baltimore, Maryland.
the evolution of mammalian reproduction. Acta Racey, P. A. 1982. Ecology of bat reproduction. In

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/icb/article/28/3/863/99186 by guest on 18 February 2021


Zool. Fennica 17:133-136. T. H. Kunz (ed.), Ecology of bats, pp. 57-104. Ple-
Mitchell, B.,D. McCowen,and I. A. Nicholson. 1976. num Press, New York.
Annual cycles of body weight and condition in Racey, P. A. and J. R. Speakman. 1987. The energy
Scottish red deer. J. Zool. 180:107-127. of pregnancy and lactation in heterothermic bats.
Morton, J. R. C , V. H. Denenberg, and M. X. Zar- In A. Loudon and P. A. Racey (eds.), Reproductive
row. 1963. Modification of sexual development energetics in mammals, pp. 107-125. Oxford Uni-
through stimulation in infancy. Endocrinology versity Press, Oxford.
72:439-442. Randolph, P. A., J. C. Randolph, K. Mattingly, and
Myrcha, A., L. Ryszkowski, and W. Walkowa. 1969. M. M. Foster. 1977. Energy costs of reproduc-
Bioenergetics of pregnancy and lactation in the tion in the cotton rat, Sigmodon hispidus. Ecology
white mouse. Acta Theriol. 12:161 — 166. 58:31-45.
Nagy, K. A. and G. G. Montgomery. 1980. Field Reid.J.T. 1961. Nutrition of lactating farm animals.
metabolic rate, water flux, and food consumption In S. K. Kon and A. T. Cowie (eds.), Milk: The
in three-toed sloths (Bradypus varigatus). J. mammary gland and its secretion. II. Academic Press,
Mamm. 61:465-472. New York.
Nagy, K. A., R. S. Seymour, A. K. Lee, and R. W. Rosenblatt.J. S. and H. I. Siegel. 1983. Physiological
Braithwaite. 1978. Energy and water budgets in and behavioral changes during pregnancy and
free-living Antechinus stuartii (Marsupialia: Dasy- parturition underlying the onset of maternal
uridae).J. Mamm. 59:60-68. behavior in rodents. In R. W. Elwood (ed.), Paren-
Nicoll, M. and S. D. Thompson. 1987. Basal meta- tal behavior in rodents, pp. 23—66. John Wiley &
bolic rates and energetics of reproduction in Sons, New York.
eutherian mammals: Marsupials and placentals Rutberg, A. T. 1986. Dominance and its fitness con-
compared. In A. Loudon and P. A. Racey (eds.), sequences in American bison cows. Behaviour 96:
Reproductive energetics of mammals, pp. 7-27. 62-91.
Oxford University Press, Oxford. Sadleir, R.M.F.S. 1969. The ecology of reproduction in
Oftedal.O. T. 1980. Milk and mammalian evolution. wild and domestic mammals. Methuen, London.
In K. Schmidt-Nielson, L. Bollis, and C. R. Taylor Sadleir, R.M.F.S. 1982. Energy consumption and
(eds.), Comparative physiology: Primitive mammals, subsequent partitioning in lactating black-tailed
pp. 31—42. Cambridge University Press, Cam- deer. Can. J. Zool. 60:382-386.
bridge. Sampson, D. A. and G. R.Jansen. 1984. Protein and
Oftedal, O. T. 1984. Milk composition, milk yield energy nutrition during lactation. Ann. Rev. Nutr.
and energy output at peak lactation: A compar- 4:43-67.
ative review. Symp. Zool. Soc. London 51:33- Schmidt-Nielson, K. 1984. Scaling: Why is animal size
85. so important?. Cambridge University Press, Cam-
Oftedal, O. T. 1985. Pregnancy and lactation. In R. bridge.
J. Hudson and R. G. White (eds.), Bioenergetics of Short, R. V. 1985. Species differences in reproduc-
wild herbivores, pp. 215-238. CRC Press, Boca tion mechanisms. In C. R. Austin and R. V. Short
Raton, Florida. (eds.), Reproduction in mammals 4. Reproductive fit-
Ortiz, C. L., B. J. Le Boeuf, and D. P. Costa. 1984. ness, pp. 24—61. Cambridge University Press,
Milk intake of elephant seal pups: An index of Cambridge.
parental investment. Am. Nat. 124:416—422. Slonaker,J. R. 1925. The effect of copulation, preg-
Ostermeyer, M. C. 1983. Maternal aggression. In R. nancy, pseudopregnancy and lactation on the vol-
C. Elwood (ed.), Parental behaviour of rodents, pp. untary activity and food consumption of the albino
151-179. John Wiley and Sons, New York. rat. Am.J. Physiol. 71:362-394.
Partridge, G. G., G. E. Lobley, and R. A. Fordyce. Small, M. F. 1981. Body fat, rank, and nutritional
1986. Energy and nitrogen metabolism of rab- states in a captive group of rhesus macaques. Int.
bits during pregnancy, lactation, and concurrent J. Primatol. 2:91-95.
pregnancy and lactation. Br. J. Nutr. 56:199- Smith, B. W. andj. J. McManus. 1975. The effects
207. of litter size on the bioenergetics and water
Pond, C. M. 1977. The significance of lactation in requirements of lactating Mus musculus. Comp.
the evolution of mammals. Evolution 31:177-199. Biochem. Physiol. 51 A:l 11-115.
Pond, C. M. 1984. Physiological and ecological Soholt, L. F. 1977. Consumption of herbaceous vege-
ENERGETICS AND MAMMALIAN REPRODUCTION 875

tation and water during reproduction and devel- Taylor, C. R-, N. C. Heglund, T. A. McMahon, and
opment of Merriam's kangeroo rat, Dipodomys T. R. Looney. 1980. Energetic cost of gener-
merriami. Am. Midi. Nat. 98:445-457. atingmuscularforceduringrunning.J. Exp. Biol.
Speakman, J. R. and P. A. Racey. 1987. The ener- 86:9-18.
getics of pregnancy and lactation in the brown Thomas, D. C. 1982. The relationship between fer-
long-eared bat, Plecotus aurilus. In M. B. Fenton, tility and fat reserves of Peary caribou. Can. J.
P. A. Racey, and J. M. V. Rayner (eds.), Recent Zool. 60:597-602.
advances in the study of bats, pp. 367-393. Cam- Thompson, S. D. and M. E. Nicoll. 1986. Basal met-
bridge University Press, Cambridge. abolic rate and energetics of reproduction in
Stack, H. 1985. The energetics of reproduction in therian mammals. Nature 321:690-693.
Eptesicus fuscus, the big brown bat. Ph.D. Diss., Tiefer, L. 1969. Copulatory behavior of male Rattus
Boston University. norwegicus in a multiple-female exhaustion test.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/icb/article/28/3/863/99186 by guest on 18 February 2021


Stearns, S. C. 1983. The influence of size and phy- Anim. Behav. 17:718-721.
logeny on patterns of covariation among life-his- Trivers, R. L. 1972. Parental investment and sexual
tory traits in the mammals. Oikos 41:173-187. selection. In B. Campbell (ed.), Sexual selection and
Stebbins, L. L. 1977. Energy requirements during the descent of man, 1871-1971, pp. 136-179.
reproduction in Peromyscus maniculatus. Can J. Aldine, Chicago.
Zool. 55:1701-1704. Vernon, R. E. and D. J. Flint. 1984. Adipose tissue:
Stewart, R. E. A. 1986. Energetics of age-specific Metabolic adaptation during lactation. Symp.
reproductive effort in female harp seals, Phoca Zool. Soc. London 51:119-140.
groenlandica. J. Zool. 208:503-517. Wang, G. H. 1925. The changes in the amount of
Struhsaker, T. T. 1967. Behavior of elk (Cervus can- daily food-intake of the albino rat during preg-
adensis) during the rut. Z. Tierpsychol. 24:80- nancy and lactation. Am. J. Physiol. 71:736—741.
114. Widdowson, E. M. 1981. The role of nutrition in
Studier, E. H. 1979. Bioenergetics of growth, preg- mammalian reproduction. In D. Gilmore and B.
nancy and lactation in the laboratory mouse, Mus Cook (eds.), Environmentalfactors in mammal repro-
musculus. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. 64A:473-481. duction, pp. 145-159. University Park Press, Bal-
Studier, E. H., V. L. Lysengen, and M. J. O'Farrell. timore, Maryland.
1973. Biology of Myotis thysanodes and M. lucifu- Williamson, D. H. 1980. Integration of metabolism
gus (Chiroptera: Vespertilionidae). II. Bioener- in tissues of the lactating rat. FEBS Lett. 117:
getics of pregnancy and lactation. Comp. Bio- k93-klO5.
chem. Physiol. 44A:467-471.
Taub, D. M. (ed.) 1984. Primate paternalism. Van Nos-
trand Reinhold, New York.

You might also like