Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 14

This article was downloaded by: [Arizona State University]

On: 09 January 2015, At: 18:49


Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office:
Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Cognition and Emotion


Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription
information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/pcem20

Explicit and implicit emotion regulation: A


dual-process framework
a b c a b
Anett Gyurak , James J. Gross & Amit Etkin
a
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences , Stanford University ,
Stanford, CA, USA
b
MIRECC , Veterans Affairs Alto Health Care System , Palo Alto, CA, USA
c
Department of Psychology , Stanford University , Stanford, CA, USA
Published online: 22 Feb 2011.

To cite this article: Anett Gyurak , James J. Gross & Amit Etkin (2011) Explicit and implicit
emotion regulation: A dual-process framework, Cognition and Emotion, 25:3, 400-412, DOI:
10.1080/02699931.2010.544160

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2010.544160

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”)
contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our
licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or
suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication
are the opinions and views of the authors, and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor &
Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and should be independently
verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any
losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities
whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or
arising out of the use of the Content.

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial
or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or
distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use
can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions
COGNITION AND EMOTION
2011, 25 (3), 400412

Explicit and implicit emotion regulation:


A dual-process framework

Anett Gyurak1,2, James J. Gross3, and Amit Etkin1,2


1
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA
2
MIRECC, Veterans Affairs Alto Health Care System, Palo Alto, CA, USA
3
Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA

It is widely acknowledged that emotions can be regulated in an astonishing variety of ways. Most
Downloaded by [Arizona State University] at 18:49 09 January 2015

research to date has focused on explicit (effortful) forms of emotion regulation. However, there is
growing research interest in implicit (automatic) forms of emotion regulation. To organise emerging
findings, we present a dual-process framework that integrates explicit and implicit forms of emotion
regulation, and argue that both forms of regulation are necessary for well-being. In the first section of
this review, we provide a broad overview of the construct of emotion regulation, with an emphasis on
explicit and implicit processes. In the second section, we focus on explicit emotion regulation,
considering both neural mechanisms that are associated with these processes and their experiential
and physiological consequences. In the third section, we turn to several forms of implicit emotion
regulation, and integrate the burgeoning literature in this area. We conclude by outlining open
questions and areas for future research.

Keywords: Dual-process; Explicit; Implicit; Emotion regulation.

There are virtually endless ways of managing implicit emotion regulation, and outline future
unwanted and distracting emotions. Research on work aimed at linking these two approaches. Our
emotion regulation has historically focused on aim is not to provide a comprehensive review
effortful (explicit) attempts to alter the course and of emotion regulation as a field (see Gross, 1998a;
intensity of emotional responses. More recently, Gross & Thompson, 2007; Koole, 2010,
researchers have started to describe less effortful for reviews). Rather, our aim is to present a
and more automatic (implicit) forms of emotion framework that is useful for integrating research
regulation. Using a dual-process model as a on explicit and implicit emotion, thus choice
framework, our goal in this paper is to review of findings is therefore strategic rather than
and integrate existing research on explicit and exhaustive.

Correspondence should be addressed to: Anett Gyurak, Department of Psychiatry, 401 Quarry Road, MC: 5797, Stanford, CA
94305-5797, USA. E-mail: agyurak@stanford.edu
The authors would like to thank Sander Koole, Klaus Rothermund, two anonymous reviewers, the members of the Stanford
Psychophysiology Lab and the Etkin Lab for their helpful comments and suggestions with this paper.

# 2011 Psychology Press, an imprint of the Taylor & Francis Group, an Informa business 400
http://www.psypress.com/cogemotion DOI:10.1080/02699931.2010.544160
EXPLICIT AND IMPLICIT EMOTION REGULATION

DEFINING EMOTION REGULATION emphasised motivationalfunctional dimensions


in organising the literature.
The importance of regulating emotional impulses In the present paper, we aim to illustrate the
in general, and anxiety in particular, was high- ways in which explicit (often called effortful) and
lighted by early psychological theorising, dating implicit (often called automatic) processes cut
back to Sigmund Freud, who made anxiety- across and organise the existing literature. The
regulation the centrepiece of the psychodynamic difficulty in proposing an integrative model of
theory of mental life (S. Freud, 1926; see also emotion regulation is highlighted more generally
A. Freud, 1936). This early work was extended by by the controversies surrounding other dual-
researchers in the stress and coping tradition, who process conceptualisations (Bargh, 1994; Moors
explored cognitive and behavioural strategies & De Houwer, 2006). Similar challenges exist
aimed at managing specific external and/or inter- when applying an effortful versus automatic dis-
nal demands that are appraised as stressful, and sociation to the field of emotion regulation (Bargh
Downloaded by [Arizona State University] at 18:49 09 January 2015

taxing or exceeding the resources of the person & Williams, 2007). Encouraged by Bargh and
(Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). Williams (2007), we choose to benefit from the
Developmental researchers have also long re- ‘‘hard-earned knowledge gained from the study of
cognised the relationship between emotion reg- automaticity in social cognition’’ (p. 433) and
ulatory abilities and adjustment, and emphasised define two theoretical and empirical spheres that
self- and emotion regulation as a correlate of social organise different areas of emotion regulation and
and emotional functioning in children (Campos, label these as ‘‘explicit’’ and ‘‘implicit’’. Further-
Campos, & Barrett, 1989; Cole, Martin, & more, we define explicit emotion regulation as
Dennis, 2004; Posner & Rothbart, 2003). Studies those processes that require conscious effort for
of emotion regulation in this tradition span from initiation and demand some level of monitoring
infancy (Campos et al., 1989; Posner & Rothbart, during implementation, and are associated with
2003), through childhood (Eisenberg, Fabes, some level of insight and awareness. Implicit pro-
Guthrie, & Reiser, 2000), into adolescence (Lewis, cesses are believed to be evoked automatically by the
Lamm, Segalowitz, Stieben, & Zelazo, 2006) and stimulus itself and run to completion without
old age (Gross et al., 1997). monitoring and can happen without insight and
In the past decade or so, the field of emotion awareness.
regulation has come into its own (Gross, 1998a). Figure 1 captures our conceptualisation and
Researchers interested in emotion regulation have depicts how, in our view, explicit and implicit
generally viewed emotions as whole-body re- forms of emotion regulation form distinct, yet
sponses that signal personally relevant, motivation- somewhat inter-related islands of research. Ex-
ally significant events (Frijda, 1987; Levenson, plicit emotion regulation research is positioned in
1999). From this perspective, emotion regulation the middle because researchers have repeatedly
may be defined as goal directed processes functioning tested and confirmed that it fits our definition of
to influence the intensity, duration and type of explicitness (i.e., requires monitoring during im-
emotion experienced (Gross & Thompson, 2007). plementation and demands some level of insight
Emotion regulation permits flexibility in emotional and awareness). Often when people are faced with
responding in accord with one’s momentary as well an emotion regulatory challenge, the conscious,
as one’s longer term goals in any given situation. effortful, rule-based operations of explicit emotion
A variety of classification systems exist for regulatory processes are necessary to adjust initial
emotion regulatory processes. For example, reactions. Viewed this way, explicit regulation is
Gross’s (e.g., Gross, 1998a) process model of clearly not something one can effectively engage
emotion regulation highlights the time course of in all of the time. Rather, given the high demand
emotion regulation. By contrast, Koole (2010) has for moment-to-moment emotion regulation in

COGNITION AND EMOTION, 2011, 25 (3) 401


GYURAK, GROSS, ETKIN

Emotional
conflict
adaptation

Habitual emotion
regulation Explicit emotion Error-related
regulation regulation
Downloaded by [Arizona State University] at 18:49 09 January 2015

Emotion
Emotion
regulation as a
regulatory goals
result of affect
and evaluations
labelling

Figure 1. A dual-process model of emotion regulation. Explicit emotion regulation processes are depicted in the centre. Everything outside
this circle represents processes that are related to implicit emotion regulation.

everyday life, use of efficient implicit emotion EXPLICIT EMOTION REGULATION


regulation processes, which run without aware-
ness, is critical for well-being. A paradigmatic case of explicit regulation may be
Furthermore explicit/implicit regulation are found in work by James Gross and colleagues
not mutually exclusive categories, but rather (Gross & Levenson, 1993; McRae et al., 2010;
have porous boundaries (denoted by the dashed Ochsner, Bunge, Gross, & Gabrieli, 2002). In a
line that surround each box). That is, processes typical test of explicit emotion regulation, parti-
may vary in explicitness or implicitness over time cipants are presented with a task that involves
or across situations and adaptive emotional re- processing stimuli under two different con-
sponses depend on the interplay between explicit ditions*one in which participants are instructed
and implicit processing*a relationship that is to react naturally (reactivity trial), and another in
denoted by the double arrows that link explicit which participants are instructed to regulate their
and implicit forms of regulation. emotional responses (regulation trial) using a
In drawing attention to qualities of explicitness strategy specified by the researcher, and which
and implicitness in the field of emotion regulation, the participant has had ample opportunity to
we hope our conceptual framework will inspire practise beforehand. Explicit emotion regulation
others to directly examine how explicit or implicit performance is indexed by contrasting emotional
their particular emotion regulatory process under responding in the reactivity and regulation trials.
study is, and relate it to our broader framework. As This basic paradigm has been widely adopted,
such, we expect that, with additional research, our and the body of evidence from these studies
model will become increasingly refined and better suggests that explicit emotion regulation strategies
characterised*developments we would greatly can reliably influence emotional responding. Ty-
welcome. pically, negative or aversive images are used to

402 COGNITION AND EMOTION, 2011, 25 (3)


EXPLICIT AND IMPLICIT EMOTION REGULATION

elicit emotional responses including negative Neuroimaging studies (see Kalisch, 2009;
photographs depicting threat-related images Ochsner & Gross, 2005, for reviews) show that
such as accident scenes (e.g., McRae et al., 2010; explicit emotion regulation results in a dynamic
Ochsner et al., 2002; Ray et al., 2005; Urry, 2010), interchange between frontal-lobe areas implicated
or disgust-eliciting film excerpts (Goldin, McRae, in cognitive control and executive function, and
Ramel, & Gross, 2008). Other researchers have emotion-reactivity areas. Specifically, imaging
used positive stimuli in the form of erotica studies indicate that attempts to reappraise nega-
(Beauregard, Lévesque, & Bourgouin, 2001) or tive stimuli result in increased activation in
pictures of cute pets and sporting events (Kim & ventrolateral and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex
Hamann, 2007). Most commonly, researchers (PFC) areas traditionally implicated in non-
have studied the effects of down-regulation emotional forms of cognitive control (Ochsner
instructions, using a wide range of strategies, but
et al., 2002). This frontal lobe activation in turn is
emotion amplification through up-regulation in-
related to reduced activation in limbic emotion-
Downloaded by [Arizona State University] at 18:49 09 January 2015

structions has also been examined (e.g., Demaree


reactivity-related areas (amygdala and insula),
et al., 2006; Eippert et al., 2007; Kunzmann,
suggesting that engagement of control-related
Kupperbusch, & Levenson, 2005).
As outlined in our process model of emotion areas dampens reactivity in these critical emo-
regulation (Gross, 1998b), researchers have in- tion-reactivity regions (Banks, Eddy, Angstadt,
structed participants to engage explicit down- Nathan, & Phan, 2007; Goldin et al., 2008;
regulatory efforts at various points in the emotion Ochsner et al., 2002; Wager, Davidson, Hughes,
generative process. The majority of these studies Lindquist, & Ochsner, 2008). Interestingly, at-
instructed participants to use reappraisal (change tempts to engage suppression, a late-stage emo-
the way they think about the stimuli in order to tion regulatory strategy, also engage frontal-lobe
reduce negative feelings; e.g., Gross, 1998b; areas*suggesting that explicit attempts to sup-
Ochsner et al., 2002). In other studies, partici- press emotional responses rely on the same areas
pants were instructed to distract themselves that reappraisal attempts do, but fail to reduce
(Kalisch, Wiech, Herrmann, & Dolan, 2006; limbic system activation the same way reappraisal
McRae et al., 2010), to employ attentional control does (Goldin et al., 2008). This result is clearly
(Urry, 2010), to realistically evaluate the stimuli consistent with autonomic nervous system activa-
(Herwig et al., 2007), to distance themselves from tion studies that indicate that suppression has an
the negative stimuli (Kalisch et al., 2005) or to use effect on overt displays of emotions but does not
suppression and hide their emotions so that alter emotional reactivity (Gross & Levenson,
someone watching them would not know what 1993).
they are feeling (Gross & Levenson, 1993; The experimental tasks described here fit our
Lévesque et al., 2003). definition of explicit emotion regulation because
According to the process model of emotion the process is instructed, effortful, and is carried
regulation (Gross & Thompson, 2007), interject- out with considerable awareness. Specifically, the
ing regulation relatively early on in the emotion- emotion regulation process described here is an
generative process appears to be most successful in example of explicit regulation, because (i) indivi-
altering the course of the response (e.g., distraction
duals are aware of the cues that elicited emotional
and attentional control). Similarly, cognitive re-
responses (i.e., images, films); (ii) aware of the
appraisal, a strategy typically engaged before
emotions itself (i.e., report reduction in accom-
full-blown emotional responses develop is more
panying feelings); and (iii) are aware of the effect
successful in altering the emotional response than
suppression, a response-focused strategy function- of the regulation on their behaviour (i.e., if
ing to prevent emotional responses from being prompted, can report back having engaged in
overtly expressed (Gross, 1998b). emotion regulation).

COGNITION AND EMOTION, 2011, 25 (3) 403


GYURAK, GROSS, ETKIN

BEYOND EXPLICIT EMOTION slowdown in response time due to emotional


REGULATION conflict has been termed the congruency effect.
Implicit emotion regulation is indexed by trial-to-
In this section, we broaden our focus by consider- trial changes in the congruency effect as a function
ing the emerging literature on five important non- of the congruency effect on the previous trial (trial
explicit forms of emotion regulation. We categorise n  1). Specifically, conflict on trial n  1 triggers
these research areas in shades of implicitness as an increase in emotional control, thereby reducing
there are automatic, routinised components to all. susceptibility to emotional conflict on trial n.
We note that more research is needed to system- Consistent with this, response times to incon-
atically investigate explicit and implicit qualities in gruent trials are usually faster after incongruent
these processes. trials (iI) than after congruent trials (cI). Implicit
emotion regulation is therefore indexed by con-
Emotional conflict adaptation trasting response times on incongruent trials that
Downloaded by [Arizona State University] at 18:49 09 January 2015

Our first case of implicit emotion regulation is are preceded by another incongruent trial (iI) with
based on the emotional conflict task described by incongruent trails that are preceded by a con-
Etkin and colleagues (Egner, Etkin, Gale, & gruent trial (cI), a behavioural effect shown to
Hirsch, 2008; Etkin, Egner, Peraza, Kandel, & occur outside of awareness.1
Hirsch, 2006). The task is the emotional version Parallel studies of adaptation to non-emotional
of the classic Stroop paradigm (Stroop, 1935), and conflict (Egner et al., 2008) suggest that key
takes advantage of a variant of the congruency elements of the neural circuitry involved in
sequence effects originally reported by Gratton implicit regulation of emotional conflict is specific
et al. in non-emotional conflict tasks (Botvinick, to emotional conflict. Specifically, neuroimaging
Nystrom, Fissell, Carter, & Cohen, 1999; studies of implicit emotion regulation, using the
Gratton, Coles, & Donchin, 1992). In this task, emotional conflict task, suggest a regulatory inter-
participants are presented with photographs of play between anterior cingulate cortex (ACC)/
emotional faces (fearful or happy) with a word medial PFC, and emotion reactivity regions in the
(‘‘fear’’ or ‘‘happy’’) written over them. The task is limbic system: increased activation in the ventral
to indicate whether the facial expression is happy portion of the anterior cingulate (vACC), and
or fearful by pressing a button. The word written dampened reactivity in the amygdala (Etkin
on the photo either matches the facial expression et al., 2006; Etkin, Prater, Hoeft, Menon, &
(no-conflict trials: happy face with the word Schatzberg, 2010). In the non-emotional analo-
happy written over it, or fearful face with the gue of the task, wherein subjects judged the
word fear written over it), or is incongruent with gender of emotional faces while trying to ignore
the facial expression (conflict trials: happy face incongruent gender labels, regulation was
with the word fear written over it, or fearful face achieved through a dissociable neural pathway,
with the word happy written over it). involving increased activation in the dorsolateral
Because reading is automatised (Stroop, 1935), PFC, and positive coupling to target-specific visual
during conflict trials participants need to enact cortical areas in the fusiform face area (Egner
control over reading the word in favour of et al., 2008). Importantly, there was no evidence
labelling the emotional expression. Consistent in the non-emotional conflict task of activation of
with this, conflict trials take longer to respond the vACC or dampening of amygdalar reactivity
to as compared to no-conflict trials; this (Egner et al., 2008).

1
The trial-to-trial adaptation (iIcI) index of emotion regulation is a variant of the full Gratton effect [(iI  cI)  (iC  cC)].
This adaptation effect is widely used in the cognitive regulation literature and constitutes a well-accepted measure of cognitive
control. Furthermore, our studies indicate a selective deficit of iIcI adaptation among patients with GAD (Etkin et al., 2010), a
population characterised by severe emotion regulatory deficits.

404 COGNITION AND EMOTION, 2011, 25 (3)


EXPLICIT AND IMPLICIT EMOTION REGULATION

This task fits our definition of implicit emotion looking at emotionally challenging negative faces.
regulation because the process is uninstructed, Similarly, Mauss and colleagues (Mauss, Cook,
effortless and proceeds without awareness; despite Cheng, & Gross, 2007) reported that individuals
careful probing, participants do not report any high on reappraisal tendencies were able to
awareness of the key processes of the task (Etkin regulate their emotions in the throes of powerful
et al., 2010). The emotion regulation process negative emotions (anger) and showed a more
described here is implicit: individuals are (i) beneficial profile of physiological responding than
unaware of the modulation of emotional control their low-reappraisal counterparts in response to
elicited by the stimuli on their behaviour (i.e., an anger provocation.
response on iI vs. cI trials) and (ii) the regulatory Habitual use of emotion regulation is also
process is carried out largely outside of awareness. related to individuals’ implicit theories about
emotions (Tamir, John, Srivastava, & Gross,
Habitual emotion regulation 2007), that is, whether they view emotions as
Downloaded by [Arizona State University] at 18:49 09 January 2015

fixed (entity theorists), or as more malleable and


Absent of explicit instructions, people report thus ‘‘regulate-able’’ (incremental theorists). In a
using emotion regulation fairly frequently on a longitudinal study, Tamir and colleagues (Tamir
daily basis (Gross, Richards, & John, 2006). We et al., 2007) found that entity beliefs were related
suggest that the habitual pattern of engaged to lower emotion regulation self-efficacy percep-
emotion regulatory strategies*whether assessed tions and less use of cognitive reappraisal.
with retrospective self-report accounts or ques- Habitual use of emotion regulation can also be
tionnaires*reflects a form of regulation that, seen in the unbidden emotion regulatory attempts
depending on situational and personal factors, that participants, when prompted after conclusion
can fluctuate rapidly between the explicit and
of a laboratory-based emotion provocation, retro-
implicit domains. For example, one can explicitly
spectively report having used. For example, Egloff
remind oneself that an angry co-worker had a bad
and colleagues (Egloff, Schmukle, Burns, &
day to reduce frustration, and another time, one
Schwerdtfeger, 2006) showed that spontaneous
might engage in this reappraisal without any
recruitment of reappraisal and suppression during
awareness. Furthermore, frequent use of a given
a stressful speech task results in similar experiential
explicit strategy can quickly render the initiation
of the strategy more implicit during regulation, physiological and self-report effects as laboratory-
thus making it more implicit over time. induced use of reappraisal or suppression.
Gross and John (2003) examined habitual Another related line of work suggests that
emotion regulation with a self-report question- individuals’ action orientation, defined as one’s
naire that measures individual differences in habitual tendency to respond to stress with decisive
customary use of reappraisal and suppression. In actions rather than to dwell on negative affect,
a series of five studies, they showed that the might predispose individuals to quickly and effec-
habitual use of reappraisal was related to greater tively regulate their emotions (Koole & Kuhl,
positive affect, better interpersonal functioning, 2008). Koole and colleagues (Jostmann, Koole,
and higher well-being. By contrast, greater use of van der Wulp, & Fockenberg, 2005; Koole &
suppression was related to a less beneficial profile Coenen, 2007; Koole & Jostmann, 2004) argued
of emotional functioning (Gross & John, 2003). that action orientation predisposes people to ‘‘in-
A recent neuroimaging study (Drabant, McRae, tuitively’’ regulate emotion and quickly decrease
Manuck, Hariri, & Gross, 2009) found that negative affect in demanding situations to facilitate
individuals who report using reappraisal as their higher order goal pursuit. The effects of action
preferred emotion regulatory strategy tend to orientation were documented as reduced suscept-
engage prefrontal areas implicated in cognitive ibility to affective priming by negative stimuli
control, including reappraisal, more when they are (Koole & Fockenberg, 2011 this issue), and more

COGNITION AND EMOTION, 2011, 25 (3) 405


GYURAK, GROSS, ETKIN

efficient action control even if emotions were tendencies to control one’s emotions aid regulation
triggered subliminally (Jostmann et al., 2005). in ‘‘hot’’ anger-provoking situations.

Emotion regulatory goals and values Emotion regulation as a result of affect


labelling
Emotion regulatory goals, values, and beliefs
constitute another form of implicit regulation A large body of work has examined emotional
that routinely runs outside of awareness, but can processing in the context of certain intentional
be made explicit at times when these goals and activities that have incidental/unintended emotion
values are articulated. Schweiger-Gallo and col- regulation consequences. In one such paradigm,
leagues (Schweiger-Gallo, Keil, McCulloch, participants were instructed to either label an
Rockstroh, & Gollwitzer, 2009) documented a emotional face (e.g., indicate whether the face is
possible mechanism for the development of emo- angry or afraid) or to match the gender (Hariri,
Downloaded by [Arizona State University] at 18:49 09 January 2015

tion regulatory goals. Specifically, they showed Bookheimer, & Mazziotta, 2000; Lieberman et al.,
that automatising goal pursuit by creating emo- 2007; Lieberman, Hariri, Jarcho, Eisenberger, &
tion regulation implementation intentions can Bookheimer, 2005). Matching emotional expres-
serve as an effective way to regulate negative sions on faces with affect label words results in
emotions. They furnished participants with im- lower limbic system activation, including reduced
plementation intentions (ifthen plans that detail activation in the amygdala, as compared to match-
the process of goal attainment) and showed that ing expressions to other expressions, matching the
these instructions through repeated practice re- gender to gender labels, or to simply viewing the
duced reactivity to accident-related threatening face. Similar to explicit forms of emotion regula-
images (Schweiger-Gallo et al., 2009), and among tion, labelling emotional faces or focusing on
spider-fearful individuals resulted in reduced physical features of an emotionally evocative scene
reactivity. Eder (2011 this issue) showed that results in increased activation in the lateral and
implementation intentions created in one task medial PFC, despite the fact that emotion regula-
context can carry over to another unrelated task tion in this paradigm is unintentional or
and powerfully influence behaviour. Taken to- implicit (Hariri et al., 2000; Lieberman et al.,
gether, these studies demonstrate the general 2005). Importantly, Lieberman and colleagues
tendency for implementation intentions to very (Lieberman et al., 2007) reported that the ventral
quickly become routinised and automatised, and portion of medial PFC activation mediates the
deploy with little subsequent awareness (Wil- relationship between lateral PFC activation and
liams, Bargh, Nocera, & Gray, 2009). decreased amygdala activation, suggesting that,
Once routinised, individual differences in atti- similar to implicit emotion regulation, it may be
tudes toward emotion regulation can be assessed the medial PFCamygdala pathway that plays a
with reaction-time tasks. Specifically, faster reac- critical role in dampening emotional-reactivity in
tion time in categorising emotion-regulation- incidental forms of emotion regulation.
related words as positive has been shown to relate
to better regulation, and lower emotional reactivity
Error-related regulation
in response to anger provocation (Mauss, Evers,
Wilhelm, & Gross, 2006), and greater well-being, The emotional conflict adaptation task (Etkin
fewer depressive symptoms, and better social et al., 2006) captures the ability to dynamically
adjustment (Hopp, Troy, & Mauss, 2011 this adapt control parameters in the context of emo-
issue). Finally, activating control tendencies im- tionally conflicting stimuli and successfully carry
plicitly through priming seemed to confer similar over regulation from one trial to another. This
benefits during anger provocation (Mauss, Cook, adaptation process can be viewed as the ability of
& Gross, 2007), suggesting that automatic knowing when to ‘‘speed up’’ in emotionally

406 COGNITION AND EMOTION, 2011, 25 (3)


EXPLICIT AND IMPLICIT EMOTION REGULATION

evocative contexts. The flip side of this phenom- FUTURE DIRECTIONS


enon is the ability to ‘‘slow down’’, or to adjust
cognitive control following a failure in that same As our targeted survey of the emotion-regulation
process. This post-error adjustment effect can be literature suggests, there is a large number of
characterised at the level of reaction time (Rabbitt, compelling directions for future research. In the
1966), or at the level of post-error accuracy following sections we detail some of the directions
(Laming, 1979). While error-related slowing in that we believe may be most fruitful in clarifying
cognitive tasks is an active area of research using explicit and implicit qualities of each research
non-emotional stimuli, to our knowledge, no domain.
study has examined error-related slowing and
error-related accuracy in the context of emotion- Measurement
ally challenging stimuli. Nonetheless, errors
themselves are negative emotional events, as Research on implicit emotion regulation has
Downloaded by [Arizona State University] at 18:49 09 January 2015

evidenced by increased activation of the defensive grown by leaps and bounds in recent years, raising
motivational system following errors (Hajcak & important questions about the measurement and
Foti, 2008). operationalisation of these processes. We know
Scalp EEG studies and intracranial recordings from existing research that conscious deployment
show that error-related processing is marked by of emotion regulation can reliably and positively
the presence of a characteristic negative deflec- shape the course of emotional responses, but it is
tion, called the error related negativity (ERN) less clear whether deploying and executing reg-
and is dependent on the interplay between ulation without awareness can have similar ben-
efits. We also expect future research to assess
cognitive areas (ACC) and emotional (amygdala)
whether understanding the temporal unfolding of
brain regions (Olvet & Hajcak, 2008; Pourtois
an emotional response will help conceptually
et al., 2010). Furthermore, the size of the ERN
organise implicit emotion regulation in the way
has been studied as a marker of emotion regula-
that it has for explicit regulation (Gross &
tion abilities in children (Dennis & Hajcak,
Thompson, 2007). Finally, another important
2009) and shown to be related to psychopathol-
issue is whether the effects of implicit emotion
ogy characterised by emotion dysregulation
regulation can be measured on subjective experi-
(Holmes & Pizzagalli, 2008; Olvet & Hajcak, ence without perturbing the process.
2008; Pizzagalli, Peccoralo, Davidson, & Cohen, As suggested by the studies cited above, there
2006). Furthermore, and in line with the func- is growing evidence that explicit regulation in-
tional properties of post-error adjustment, volves lateral PFC regulatory sites, while at least
Robinson and colleagues found that greater some forms of implicit regulation involves vACC
error-related slowing is associated with greater or medial PFC sites (Phillips, Ladouceur &
well-being (Robinson, 2007) and better stress- Drevets, 2008). At the same time, the affect-
regulation abilities (Compton et al., 2008). labelling studies (e.g., Lieberman et al., 2007)
Collectively, these studies argue that efficient and a study by Meyer and colleagues (Meyer,
post-error adjustment depends on an interplay Berkman, Karremans, & Lieberman, 2011 this
between cognitive and emotional areas. Illustrat- issue) examining implicit regulation in response to
ing the complexity of implicitness in this work, a face of an attractive and available dating partner
research shows that the behavioural marker of indicate that implicit emotion regulation can
error-related slowing is more strongly associated involve lateral PFC systems. Both the medial
with consciously perceived errors, but that ERN is and lateral PFC have been broadly associated with
detectable after both conscious and non-conscious goal-directed behaviours, deliberate actions, and
errors (Nieuwenhuis, Ridderinkhof, Blom, Band, executive functioning (e.g., Miller & Cohen,
& Kok, 2001). 2001), but it is not clear whether activation in

COGNITION AND EMOTION, 2011, 25 (3) 407


GYURAK, GROSS, ETKIN

this area is uniquely associated with either explicit regulation appropriately, but also by inflexible use
or implicit forms of emotion regulation, or rather, of emotion regulation, as exemplified by the role of
as part of a regulatory network, the neural source worry in GAD (Borkovec, Alcaine, & Behar,
of regulation that is dependent on task context. 2004). More research is needed to clarify the role
of implicit processes in psychopathology.
Functional consequences
Training and modifying emotion regulation
A growing body of evidence suggests that con-
Another important area for future research is to
textually appropriate and flexible use of emotion
explore ways in which explicit forms of regulation
regulation may be a marker of mental health. For
can be made more implicit and vice versa. For
example, explicit emotion regulation, particularly
example, one way in which explicit regulatory
cognitive reappraisal, has been shown to result in
processes may be made more implicit (and hence
lower physiological reactivity and less intense
potentially more resource efficient) is through
Downloaded by [Arizona State University] at 18:49 09 January 2015

emotional experiences than suppression (Gross,


practice. For example, explicit learning of an
1998b, 2002) and has also been related to lower
emotion-regulation-related skill might render
self-reported emotions in a lab stress and better
that skill implicit after sufficient repetition and
functional life outcomes in an at-risk sample of
mastery. There is evidence that intervening early in
distressed community women (Troy, Wilhelm,
the emotion generative process through repeated
Shallcross & Mauss, 2010). Better performance
training of attention allocation away from negative
(less emotional behaviour) on a task of emotional
stimuli and toward neutral stimuli has proven
suppression in two different studies has been
effective in reducing symptoms and improving
related to higher well-being and socioeconomic
functioning in patients with psychiatric disorders
status, suggesting that the ability to implement
(e.g., Hakamata et al., 2010). Similarly Tran and
emotion regulation following explicit instructions
colleagues (Tran, Siemer, & Joormann, 2011 this
might be related to broader measures of positive
issue) show that inducing automatic negative
life outcomes (Côté, Gyurak, & Levenson, 2010).
interpretative biases in a single training session
Research suggests that implicit emotion regula-
resulted in heightened reactivity and lower self-
tion represents a similarly adaptive process. There
esteem following negative feedback. These results
is evidence of marked implicit emotion regulatory
indicate that implicit biases can be altered via
deficits in generalised anxiety disorder (GAD;
repetitive training, as well as one-time manipula-
Etkin et al., 2010). Importantly, failure to engage
tions, though additional research is necessary to
implicit emotion regulatory processes is related to
determine the extent to which these interventions
symptom severity in this group, suggesting that
target emotion regulation per se.
implicit emotion regulatory performance is related
to adaptive functional outcomes.
In future work, it will be important to study CONCLUDING COMMENT
explicit and implicit emotion regulation in differ-
ent forms of psychopathology. Specifically, pre- It is now firmly established that wilfully and
vious studies suggest that the root of emotion consciously employed emotion regulatory strate-
regulatory difficulties in psychopathology, particu- gies can reliably alter the course of emotional
larly in the anxiety or mood domain, might be in responding, but it is also apparent that people do
more spontaneous, implicit forms of emotion not and will not pursue conscious regulatory goals
regulation (e.g., Ehring, Tuschen-Caffier, Schnülle, unless they have the motivation and the ability to
Fischer, & Gross, 2010; Etkin et al., 2010), but this do so. Additionally, because emotions are quick
hypothesis needs to be more extensively tested. and fast-changing phenomena, emotion regulation
Additionally, psychopathology may be charac- in its implicit forms has the advantage of
terised not only by a failure to use emotion being more efficient and effortless than explicit

408 COGNITION AND EMOTION, 2011, 25 (3)


EXPLICIT AND IMPLICIT EMOTION REGULATION

regulation. Implicit processes in general are much development research. Child Development, 75(2),
more consistent and reliable (and are at times also 317333.
less flexible), as they are triggered automatically Compton, R. J., Robinson, M. D., Ode, S., Quandt,
L. C., Fineman, S. L., & Carp, J. (2008). Error-
and run to completion without conscious effort or
monitoring ability predicts daily stress regulation.
monitoring. A complete understanding of emotion
Psychological Science, 19(7), 702708.
regulation and its role in mental health thus Côté, S., Gyurak, A., & Levenson, R. W. (2010). The
requires a detailed investigation of implicit ability to implement emotion regulation is asso-
forms of regulation. It is our hope that the dual- ciated with greater well-being, income, and socio-
process theoretical framework presented here will economic status. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 10(6),
serve as a systematising and organising basis for 923933. doi:10.1037/a0021156.
future work. Demaree, H. A., Schmeichel, B. J., Robinson, J. L., Pu,
J., Everhart, D. E., & Berntson, G. G. (2006). Up-
and down-regulating facial disgust: Affective, vagal,
Downloaded by [Arizona State University] at 18:49 09 January 2015

REFERENCES sympathetic, and respiratory consequences. Biological


psychology, 71(1), 9099.
Banks, S. J., Eddy, K. T., Angstadt, M., Nathan, P. J., Dennis, T. A., & Hajcak, G. (2009). The late positive
& Phan, K. L. (2007). Amygdala-frontal connectiv- potential: A neurophysiological marker for emotion
ity during emotion regulation. Social Cognitive and regulation in children. Journal of Child Psychology and
Affective Neuroscience, 2(4), 303312. Psychiatry, 50(11), 13731383.
Bargh, J. A. (1994). The four horsemen of automaticity: Drabant, E. M., McRae, K., Manuck, S. B., Hariri,
Awareness, intention, efficiency, and control in A. R., & Gross, J. J. (2009). Individual differences in
social cognition. In R. S. Wyer Jr. & T. K. Srull typical reappraisal use predict amygdala and
(Eds.) Handbook of social cognition. Vol. 1: Basic prefrontal responses. Biological Psychiatry, 65(5),
processes (pp. 140). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence 367373.
Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Eder, A. B. (2011). Control of impulsive emotional
Bargh, J. A., & Williams, L. E. (2007). The noncon- behaviour through implementation intentions.
scious regulation of emotion. In J. J. Gross (Ed.), Cognition and Emotion, 25, 478489.
Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 429445). New Egloff, B., Schmukle, S. C., Burns, L. R., & Schwerdt-
York, NY: Guilford Press.
feger, A. (2006). Spontaneous emotion regulation
Beauregard, M., Lévesque, J., & Bourgouin, P. (2001).
during evaluated speaking tasks: Associations with
Neural correlates of conscious self-regulation of
negative affect, anxiety expression, memory, and
emotion. The Journal of Neuroscience, 21(18), 6993
physiological responding. Emotion, 6(3), 356366.
7000.
Egner, T., Etkin, A., Gale, S., & Hirsch, J. (2008).
Borkovec, T. D., Alcaine, O. M., & Behar, E. (2004).
Dissociable neural systems resolve conflict from
Avoidance theory of worry and generalized anxiety
disorder. In R. G. Heimberg, C. L. Turk, & D. S. emotional versus nonemotional distracters. Cerebral
Mennin (Eds.), Generalized anxiety disorder: Ad- Cortex, 18(6), 14751484.
vances in research and practice (pp. 77108). New Ehring, T., Tuschen-Caffier, B., Schnülle, J., Fischer,
York, NY: Guilford Press. S., & Gross, J. J. (2010). Emotion regulation and
Botvinick, M., Nystrom, L. E., Fissell, K., Carter, vulnerability to depression: Spontaneous versus
C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (1999). Conflict monitoring instructed use of emotion suppression and reapprai-
versus selection-for-action in anterior cingulate sal. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 10(4), 563-572.
cortex. Nature, 402(6758), 179181. doi:10.1037/a0019010.
Campos, J. J., Campos, R. G., & Barrett, K. C. (1989). Eippert, F., Veit, R., Weiskopf, N., Erb, M.,
Emergent themes in the study of emotional devel- Birbaumer, N., & Anders, S. (2007). Regulation
opment and emotion regulation. Developmental of emotional responses elicited by threat-related
Psychology, 25(3), 394402. stimuli. Human Brain Mapping, 28(5), 409423.
Cole, P. M., Martin, S. E., & Dennis, T. A. (2004). Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Guthrie, I. K., & Reiser,
Emotion regulation as a scientific construct: M. (2000). Dispositional emotionality and regula-
Methodological challenges and directions for child tion: Their role in predicting quality of social

COGNITION AND EMOTION, 2011, 25 (3) 409


GYURAK, GROSS, ETKIN

functioning. Journal of personality and social psycho- behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
logy, 78(1), 136157. 64(6), 970986.
Etkin, A., Egner, T., Peraza, D. M., Kandel, E. R., & Gross, J. J., Richards, J. M., & John, O. P. (2006).
Hirsch, J. (2006). Resolving emotional conflict: A Emotion regulation in everyday life. In D. K.
role for the rostral anterior cingulate cortex in Snyder, J. Simpson, & J. N. Hughes (Eds.), Emotion
modulating activity in the amygdala. Neuron, regulation in couples and families: Pathways to
51(6), 871882. dysfunction and health (pp. 1335). Washington,
Etkin, A., Prater, K. E., Hoeft, F., Menon, V., & DC: American Psychological Association.
Schatzberg, A. F. (2010). Failure of anterior cingu- Gross, J. J., & Thompson, R. A. (2007). Emotion
late activation and connectivity with the amygdala regulation: Conceptual foundations. In J. J. Gross
during implicit regulation of emotional processing in (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 324).
generalized anxiety disorder. The American Journal of New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Psychiatry, 167(5), 545554. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp. Hajcak, G., & Foti, D. (2008). Errors are aversive:
2009.09070931. Defensive motivation and the error-related negativ-
Downloaded by [Arizona State University] at 18:49 09 January 2015

Freud, A. (1936). The ego and the mechanisms of defense: ity. Psychological Science: A Journal of the American
The writings of Anna Freud (Vol. 2 Revised). Psychological Society, 19(2), 103108.
Madison, CT: International Universities Press. Hakamata, Y., Lissek, S., Bar-Haim, Y., Britton, J. C.,
Freud, S. (1926). Inhibitions, symptoms and anxiety (The Fox, N. A., Leibenluft, E., et al.. (2010). Attention
Standard Edition). New York, NY: W. W. Norton. bias modification treatment: A meta-analysis
Frijda, N. H. (1987). The emotions [reprint]. toward the establishment of novel treatment for
anxiety. Biological Psychiatry, 68(11), 982990.
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Goldin, P. R., McRae, K., Ramel, W., & Gross, J. J. (doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.07.021)
Hariri, A. R., Bookheimer, S. Y., & Mazziotta, J. C.
(2008). The neural bases of emotion regulation:
(2000). Modulating emotional responses: Effects
Reappraisal and suppression of negative emotion.
of a neocortical network on the limbic system.
Biological Psychiatry, 63(6), 577586.
NeuroReport, 11(1), 4348.
Gratton, G., Coles, M. G., & Donchin, E. (1992).
Herwig, U., Baumgartner, T., Kaffenberger, T., Brühl,
Optimizing the use of information: Strategic control
A., Kottlow, M., Schreiter-Gasser, U., et al. (2007).
of activation of responses. Journal of Experimental
Modulation of anticipatory emotion and perception
Psychology. General, 121(4), 480506.
processing by cognitive control. NeuroImage, 37(2),
Gross, J. J. (1998a). The emerging field of emotion
652662.
regulation: An integrative review. Review of Gen- Holmes, A. J., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2008). Spatiotem-
eral Psychology. Special Issue: New Directions in poral dynamics of error processing dysfunctions
Research on Emotion, 2(3), 271299. in major depressive disorder. Archives of General
Gross, J. J. (1998b). Antecedent- and response-focused Psychiatry, 65(2), 179188.
emotion regulation: Divergent consequences for Hopp, H., Troy, A. S., & Mauss, I. B. (2011). The
experience, expression, and physiology. Journal of unconscious pursuit of emotion regulation:
Personality and Social Psychology, 74(1), 224237. Implications for psychological health. Cognition
Gross, J. J. (2002). Emotion regulation: Affective, and Emotion, 25, 532545.
cognitive, and social consequences. Psychophysiology, Jostmann, N. B., Koole, S. L., van der Wulp, N. Y., &
39(3), 281291. Fockenberg, D. A. (2005). Subliminal affect regula-
Gross, J. J., Carstensen, L. L., Pasupathi, M., Tsai, J., tion: The moderating role of action vs. state
Skorpen, C. G., & Hsu, A. Y. (1997). Emotion and orientation. European Psychologist, 10(3), 209217.
aging: Experience, expression, and control. Psychol- Kalisch, R. (2009). The functional neuroanatomy of
ogy and Aging, 12(4), 590599. reappraisal: Time matters. Neuroscience and Biobeha-
Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences vioral Reviews, 33(8), 12151226.
in two emotion regulation processes: Implications Kalisch, R., Wiech, K., Critchley, H. D., Seymour, B.,
for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of O’Doherty, J. P., Oakley, D. A., et al. (2005).
Personality and Social Psychology, 85(2), 348362. Anxiety reduction through detachment: Subjective,
Gross, J. J., & Levenson, R. W. (1993). Emotional physiological, and neural effects. Journal of Cognitive
suppression: Physiology, self-report, and expressive Neuroscience, 17(6), 874883.

410 COGNITION AND EMOTION, 2011, 25 (3)


EXPLICIT AND IMPLICIT EMOTION REGULATION

Kalisch, R., Wiech, K., Herrmann, K., & Dolan, R. J. Lieberman, M. D., Eisenberger, N. I., Crockett, M. J.,
(2006). Neural correlates of self-distraction from Tom, S. M., Pfeifer, J. H., & Way, B. M. (2007).
anxiety and a process model of cognitive emotion Putting feelings into words: Affect labeling disrupts
regulation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(8), amygdala activity in response to affective stimuli.
12661276. Psychological Science: A Journal of the American
Kim, S. H., & Hamann, S. (2007). Neural correlates of Psychological Society, 18(5), 421428.
positive and negative emotion regulation. Journal of Lieberman, M. D., Hariri, A. R., Jarcho, J. M.,
Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(5), 776798. Eisenberger, N. I., & Bookheimer, S. Y. (2005).
Koole, S. L. (2010). The psychology of emotion An fMRI investigation of race-related amygdala
regulation: An integrative review. In J. De Houwer activity in African-American and Caucasian-
& D. Hermans (Eds.), Cognition and emotion: American individuals. Nature Neuroscience, 8(6),
Reviews of current research and theories (pp. 128 720722.
167). New York, NY: Psychology Press. Mauss, I. B., Cook, C. L., Cheng, J. Y. J., & Gross, J. J.
Koole, S. L., & Coenen, L. H. M. (2007). Implicit self (2007). Individual differences in cognitive reapprai-
Downloaded by [Arizona State University] at 18:49 09 January 2015

and affect regulation: Effects of action orientation sal: Experiential and physiological responses to
and subliminal self priming in an affective priming an anger provocation. International Journal of
task. Self and Identity, 6(23), 118136. Psychophysiology, 66(2), 116124.
Koole, S. L., & Fockenberg, D. A. (2011). Implicit Mauss, I. B., Cook, C. L., & Gross, J. J. (2007).
emotion regulation under demanding conditions: Automatic emotion regulation during anger provo-
The moderating role of action versus state orienta- cation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,
tion. Cognition and Emotion, 25, 440452. 43(5), 698711.
Koole, S. L., & Jostmann, N. B. (2004). Getting a grip Mauss, I. B., Evers, C., Wilhelm, F. H., & Gross, J. J.
on your feelings: Effects of action orientation and (2006). How to bite your tongue without blowing
external demands on intuitive affect regulation. your top: Implicit evaluation of emotion regulation
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87(6), predicts affective responding to anger provocation.
974990. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32(5),
Koole, S. L., & Kuhl, J. (2008). Dealing with unwanted 589602.
feelings: The role of affect regulation in volitional McRae, K., Hughes, B., Chopra, S., Gabrieli, J. D. E.,
action control. In J. Y. Shah & W. L. Gardner Gross, J. J., & Ochsner, K. N. (2010). The neural
(Eds.), Handbook of motivation science (pp. 295 bases of distraction and reappraisal. Journal of
307). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(2), 248262.
Kunzmann, U., Kupperbusch, C. S., & Levenson, Meyer, M. L., Berkman, E. T., Karremans, J. C., &
R. W. (2005). Behavioral inhibition and amplifica- Lieberman, M. D. (2011). Incidental regulation of
tion during emotional arousal: A comparison of two attraction: The neural basis of the derogation of
age groups. Psychology and Aging, 20(1), 144158. attractive alternatives in romantic relationships.
Laming, D. (1979). Autocorrelation of choice-reaction Cognition and Emotion, 25, 490505.
times. Acta Psychologica, 43(5), 381412. Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative
Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review
and coping. New York, NY: Springer. of Neuroscience, 24, 167202.
Levenson, R. W. (1999). The intrapersonal functions of Moors, A., & De Houwer, J. (2006). Automaticity:
emotion. Cognition and Emotion. Special Functional A theoretical and conceptual analysis. Psychological
Accounts of Emotion, 13(5), 481504. Bulletin, 132(2), 297326.
Lévesque, J., Eugène, F., Joanette, Y., Paquette, Nieuwenhuis, S., Ridderinkhof, K. R., Blom, J., Band,
V., Mensour, B., Beaudoin, G., et al. (2003). Neural G. P., & Kok, A. (2001). Error-related brain
circuitry underlying voluntary suppression of sad- potentials are differentially related to awareness of
ness. Biological Psychiatry, 53(6), 502510. response errors: Evidence from an antisaccade task.
Lewis, M. D., Lamm, C., Segalowitz, S. J., Stieben, J., Psychophysiology, 38(5), 752760.
& Zelazo, P. D. (2006). Neurophysiological corre- Ochsner, K. N., Bunge, S. A., Gross, J. J., & Gabrieli,
lates of emotion regulation in children and J. D. E. (2002). Rethinking feelings: An fMRI study
adolescents. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, of the cognitive regulation of emotion. Journal of
18(3), 430443. Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(8), 12151229.

COGNITION AND EMOTION, 2011, 25 (3) 411


GYURAK, GROSS, ETKIN

Ochsner, K. N., & Gross, J. J. (2005). The cognitive Robinson, M. D. (2007). Gassing, braking, and self-
control of emotion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, regulating: Error self-regulation, well-being, and
9(5), 242249. goal-related processes. Journal of Experimental Social
Olvet, D., & Hajcak, G. (2008). The error-related Psychology, 43(1), 116.
negativity (ERN) and psychopathology: Toward an Schweiger-Gallo, I., Keil, A., McCulloch, K. C.,
endophenotype. Clinical Psychology Review, 28(8), Rockstroh, B., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2009). Strate-
13431354. gic automation of emotion regulation. Journal of
Phillips, M. L., Ladouceur, C. D., & Drevets, W. C. Personality and Social Psychology, 96(1), 1131.
(2008). A neural model of voluntary and automatic Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial
emotion regulation: Implications for understanding verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology,
the pathophysiology and neurodevelopment of bi- 18(6), 643662.
polar disorder. Molecular Psychiatry, 13(9), 833857. Tamir, M., John, O. P., Srivastava, S., & Gross, J. J.
Pizzagalli, D. A., Peccoralo, L. A., Davidson, R. J., & (2007). Implicit theories of emotion: Affective and
Cohen, J. D. (2006). Resting anterior cingulate social outcomes across a major life transition. Journal
activity and abnormal responses to errors in subjects of Personality and Social Psychology, 92(4), 731744.
Downloaded by [Arizona State University] at 18:49 09 January 2015

with elevated depressive symptoms: A 128-channel Tran, T. B., Siemer, M., & Joormann, J. (2011).
EEG study. Human Brain Mapping, 27(3), 185 Implicit interpretation biases affect emotional vul-
201. nerability: A training study. Cognition and Emotion,
Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2003). Developing 25, 546558.
mechanisms of self-regulation. In Annual progress in Troy, A., Wilhelm, F. H., Shallcross, A., & Mauss,
child psychiatry and child development: 20002001 I. B. (2010) Seeing the silver lining: Cognitive
(pp. 2142). New York, NY: Brunner-Routledge. reappraisal ability moderates the relationship be-
Pourtois, G., Vocat, R., N’Diaye, K., Spinelli, L., tween stress and depressive symptoms. Emotion
Seeck, M., & Vuilleumier, P. (2010). Errors recruit (Washington, D.C.), 10(6), 783795. Doi:10.1037/
both cognitive and emotional monitoring systems: a0020262.
Simultaneous intracranial recordings in the dorsal Urry, H. L. (2010). Seeing, thinking, and feeling:
anterior cingulate gyrus and amygdala combined Emotion regulating effects of gaze-directed cogni-
with fMRI. Neuropsychologia, 48(4), 11441159. tive reappraisal. Emotion, 10(1), 125135.
Rabbitt, P. M. (1966). Errors and error correction Wager, T. D., Davidson, M. L., Hughes, B. L.,
in choiceresponse tasks. Journal of Experimental Lindquist, M. A., & Ochsner, K. N. (2008).
Psychology, 71(2), 264272. Prefrontal-subcortical pathways mediating success-
Ray, R. D., Ochsner, K. N., Cooper, J. C., Robertson, ful emotion regulation. Neuron, 59(6), 10371050.
E. R., Gabrieli, J. D. E., & Gross, J. J. (2005). doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2008.09.006.
Individual differences in trait rumination and the Williams, L. E., Bargh, J. A., Nocera, C. C., & Gray, J.
neural systems supporting cognitive reappraisal. R. (2009). The unconscious regulation of emotion:
Cognitive. Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, Nonconscious reappraisal goals modulate emotional
5(2), 156168. reactivity. Emotion, 9(6), 847854.

412 COGNITION AND EMOTION, 2011, 25 (3)

You might also like