Abrahansen e Col 1995

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

Fable Comprehension by Children

with Learning Disabilities

Eileen P. Abrahamsen and Pamela T. Sprouse

Seven fables were read to 14 average learners and 14 children with learning disabilities, all 1990; Lutzer, 1988; Seidenberg & Bern-
of whom ranged in age from 10 to 13 years. They were presented with four possible moral stein, 1986).
choices: one correct, one related, and two incorrect. They were instructed to select the correctDifficulty comprehending metaphor-
moral choice and were asked to explain their choice. The average learners' ability to select the
ical language has been attributed to a
correct moral was significantly better than that of the children with learning disabilities. In
addition, even when they chose correct morals, the children with learning disabilities were
metacognitive deficit (Lutzer, 1988;
less capable of explaining their choice. Seidenberg & Bernstein, 1986) and
general language deficits (Lee & Kahmi,
1990; Seidenberg & Bernstein, 1988).

C
hildren with learning disabil- Reynolds, & Wilson, 1984), there is Although cognitive deficits at one time
ities (LD) frequently demon- a marked improvement in the ability were hypothesized to be a contribut-
strate a variety of language of children ages 9 to 14 years (who are ing factor in metaphoric comprehen-
deficits. It has been known for some typically attending Grades 4 to 8) to sion (Billow, 1975; Cometa & Eson,
time that language delays in the pre- understand and explain figurative lan- 1978; Douglas & Peel, 1979), subse-
school years signal possible academic guage, including idioms (Ackerman, quent research (Nippold & Fey, 1983;
difficulties in later years (de Hirsch, 1982; Cacciari & Levorato, 1989; Gibbs, Vosniadou et al., 1984) has failed to
Jansky, & Langsford, 1965). Observed 1987; Lodge & Leach, 1975; Prinz, confirm that cognitive deficits are con-
language impairments in the preschool 1983); similes (Gardner, Winner, Bech- tributing factors. There is more consen-
years frequently seem to disappear, hofer, & Wolf, 1978; Malgady, 1977); sus regarding the pattern of meta-
only to resurface several years later, metaphors (Demorest, Silberstein, phoric comprehension demonstrated
often in reading and writing deficits Gardner, & Winner, 1983; Nippold, by children with LD. These children
(Aram, Ekelman, & Nation, 1984; Aram Leonard, & Kail, 1984; Winner, Rosen- demonstrate a similar but delayed pat-
& Nation, 1980; Bashir & Scavuzzo, stiel, & Gardner, 1976); and proverbs tern for metaphors (Nippold & Fey,
1992; Hall & Tomblin, 1978; King, (Douglas & Peel, 1979; Resnick, 1982; 1983; Seidenberg & Bernstein, 1986)
Jones, & Lasky, 1982). Richardson & Church, 1959). and proverbs (Lutzer, 1988) when
As children progress through the Children with language learning dis- compared to nondisabled peers.
elementary grades, they experience a abilities frequently have difficulty Children are more successful with
major shift in educational and auric- understanding figurative language multiple choice formats in compari-
ular requirements at about the fourth (Donahue & Bryan, 1984; Lee & Kah- son to explanation formats in demon-
grade. One characteristic of this shift mi, 1990; Lutzer, 1988; Nippold & strating comprehension of figurative
is the increased frequency of nonliteral Fey, 1983; Seidenberg & Bernstein, language (Gardner, 1974; Nippold,
language. This is developmentally ap- 1986, 1988). Significantly impaired Martin, & Erskine, 1988; Winner et al.,
propriate because understanding of metaphoric comprehension has been 1976). Children are also more likely to
figurative language increases during found in children with histories of successfully differentiate between lit-
the ages consistent with these grades. auditory comprehension disorders and eral and figurative meanings when lin-
Although some rudimentary under- expressive syntax disorders (Nippold guistic context is provided in the form
standing of metaphor may occur even & Fey, 1983) and in children with of short stories or paragraphs. This has
in the preschool years (Vosniadou & learning disabilities with and with- been demonstrated for idioms (Acker-
Ortony, 1983; Vosniadou, Ortony, out language deficits (Lee & Kahmi, man, 1982; Cacciari & Levorato, 1989;

JOURNAL OF LEARNING DISABILITIES


Downloaded from ldx.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on March 4, 2016
VOLUME 28, NUMBER 5, MAY 1995
PAGES 302-308
VOLUME 28, NUMBER 5, MAY 1995 303

Gibbs, 1987; Nippold & Martin, 1989) LD to understand the problems these teria. They ranged in age from 10 years
and proverbs (Resnick, 1982). children may encounter interpreting 5 months to 13 years 2 months (X =
Ortony, Schallert, Reynolds, and this form of literature. The purpose of 12-1). IQs as measured by the Wechs-
Antos (1978) concluded that the com- the present study was to examine the ler Intelligence Scale for Children-
prehension of literal and figurative lan- comprehension of fables by 10- to 13- Revised (WISC-R; Wechsler, 1974)
guage requires similar processes. A year-old average learners and children were in the normal range. They were
schema is generated by the child as a with learning disabilities, as demon- being educated in a self-contained
passage is read, and the context gener- strated by their selection of the appro- classroom for children with learning
ates expectations that the information priate moral in a multiple choice format disabilities. Age and IQ scores for these
that follows can be explained in terms and the explanation of their choice, fol- students are reported in Table 1.
of these schemata. The relatedness of lowing selection. The children without learning dis-
the idiom or metaphor to the context abilities who participated in this study
determines the difficulty. were enrolled in a regular fifth- or
When a multiple choice format is sixth-grade classroom. Teachers were
used in conjunction with context, chil-
Method asked to identify children who demon-
dren may be drawn to foils that con- strated average academic achievement.
tain words highly associated with
Subjects They ranged in age from 10 years 5
words or ideas presented in the story Fourteen children identified as LD months to 13 years 2 months (X =
if they are unable to correctly iden- and 14 nondisabled (NLD) peers at- 11-8) and were not receiving remedial
tify the target response. Reynolds and tending school in Virginia Beach, Vir- services or participating in gifted and
Ortony (1980) examined children's ginia, served as subjects. To be classi- talented programs in school.
ability to complete short stories, some fied as learning disabled in this district,
of which required literal responses and children must meet criteria outlined in
Stimulus Materials
some of which required metaphorical the Education for All Handicapped
answers. None of the foils in the meta- Children Act of 1975 (P.L. 94-142) and Ten fables were chosen from Fables
phorical set made literal sense, but demonstrate a discrepancy (1 standard (Lobel, 1980) to serve as stimuli in this
some contained words that were highly deviation and/or 2 years) between a investigation. It was anticipated that
associated with words or concepts in measure of aptitude (either verbal or these fables would be unfamiliar to the
the story presented. When children nonverbal IQ) and achievement. children participating in the study.
did not perceive the metaphorical re- The 14 children identified as learn- Four moral choices were selected to
sponse to be appropriate, they tended ing disabled in this study met these cri- accompany each fable. Only one moral
to select foils with distractors contain-
ing high associates of the theme in
the story. Context, therefore, always
seems to influence responses but does TABLE 1
not ensure that figurative language will Age and IQ of Subjects with Learning Disabilities
be interpreted correctly.
One form of figurative language that WISC-R
has not been examined is the compre- Subject Age VIQ PIQ Full-Scale IQ
hension of fables and their correspond-
ing morals. Fables are brief tales in 1 11-7 72 90 79
2 12-6 77 95 84
which animals talk and act as humans,
3 10-5 75 ± 4 80 ± 4 76 ± 3
and these tales contain a moral lesson 4 12-8 86 93 89
or satirize human conduct. Although 5 11-6 100 ± 5 88 ± 7 93 ± 5
children may encounter some classic 6 12-8 91 ± 5 95 ± 8 92 ± 5
fables in the early primary grades, 7 12-6 NA NA 90-109
8 12-7 91 95 93
fables usually become part of the edu-
9 12-8 109 75 92
cational language arts curriculum in 10 12-3 86 80 84
the fifth grade. With the trend toward 11 13-2 74 85 78
inclusive classes and the widespread 12 11-8 80-89 90-109 NA-Average
use of the whole language approach, 13 11-1 86 93 89
14 12-0 92 104 97
more and more children with learning
disabilities will be encountering this
Note. The psychologist chose to report some scores by range label. WISC-R = Wechsler Intelligence
form of literature in school. It is impor- Scale for Children-Revised; VIQ = Verbal IQ; PIQ = Performance IQ; NA = Measured by the Wechsler
tant for those who teach children with Intelligence Scale for Children.

Downloaded from ldx.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on March 4, 2016


304 JOURNAL OF LEARNING DISABILITIES

was the correct one; the other three


TABLE 2
were selected from other fables in the
Correct Moral Selections for Fables
book. One of the three incorrect morals
was related to the correct moral; the Learning disability group3 Average learner groupb
other two were not. Subject No. correct Subject No . correct
Interexaminer reliability measures
were then calculated to ensure agree- 1 15
ment on correct, incorrect, and related 2 16
moral choices for each fable. Copies of 3 17
4 18
the fables and moral choices were 5 19
given to six speech-language pathol- 6 20
ogists holding Certificates of Clin- 7 21
ical Competency from the American 8 22
Speech-Language-Hearing Associa- 9 23
10 24
tion. They marked each moral choice 11 25
as correct, related, or incorrect. Agree- 12 26
ment on the correct moral could not be 13 27
attained on three of the fables. Two 14 28
speech pathologists chose the related
a b
moral as the correct moral and the X = 3.5. X = 5.28.
correct moral as the related moral.
These three fables were then excluded
from statistical analysis, leaving only TABLE 3
seven fables for analysis. There was Analysis of Variance: Correct Moral Choices for Both Groups
100% agreement among the six speech
pathologists on these seven fables SS df MS F P
for correct, related, and incorrect re-
Total 86.43 27
sponses. Between groups 20.57 1 20.57 8.13 .01
Within groups 65.86 26 2.53
Procedure
Each child was read the seven fables,
and the four moral choices were pre- cant. Results indicate that children analysis of variance with repeated
sented aloud. The child was requested without learning disabilities were sig- measures was performed to determine
to select the correct moral choice and nificantly more capable of selecting cor- if significant differences existed for par-
to explain his or her reasons for mak- rect morals for fables than were their ticular fables and group assignment.
ing that choice. Selections were writ- peers exhibiting learning disabilities, Nondisabled subjects had significantly
ten down and explanations were tape- F(l, 26) = 8.13, p < .01. These results higher means than those with learning
recorded for future analysis. No time are reported in Table 3. disabilities, F(l, 26) = 9.38, p < .01.
limit was imposed on the child's re- The number of subjects selecting cor- Significant differences were also noted
sponse, and repetitions were given if rect (LD = 49, NLD = 73), related among the actual fables, F(6, 352) =
necessary. (LD = 25, NLD = 12), and incorrect 11.13, p < .001. The interaction effect
(LD = 24, NLD = 13) moral choices for between fable type and group assign-
fables are also calculated. Although it ment was not significant, F(6, 352) =
Results appears that related and incorrect 1.18, p > .05. Results of this ANOVA
choices were made with equal fre- can be found in Table 5.
The numbers of correct moral selec- quency, there was only one related Following the selection of each
tions for each subject are reported in moral and two incorrect morals for moral, subjects were asked to explain
Table 2. The mean number of correct each fable. This pattern reflects a clear why they chose their answers. Re-
moral choices made by children with- preference by both groups of subjects sponses were tape-recorded and later
out learning disabilities was 5.28, for selecting related morals when the transcribed. The explanations were
whereas the mean number of correct correct one was not chosen. grouped into three categories: meta-
moral choices made by children with The rank order of difficulty of fable phorical, appropriate/literal, and incor-
LD was 3.5. An analysis of variance and the mean number of correct re- rect. For the explanation to be con-
was performed to determine if differ- sponses for each fable in each group sidered metaphorical, the subject must
ences between the means were signifi- are reported in Table 4. A two-way have generalized the moral to a real-
Downloaded from ldx.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on March 4, 2016
VOLUME 28, NUMBER 5, MAY 1995 305

life situation, rather than specifying counted for 9.6% of all explanations.
ions. In many ways, fables are metaphors
particular events in the fable. For Appropriate/literal explanations ac- because animals depict human condi-
example, in the fable ' T h e Camel counted for 65.75% (n = 48) of expla-
<pla- tions and relationships. Metaphors con-
Dances/' a camel who wanted to be a nations made by the children withouthout sist of a topic, a vehicle, and a ground.
ballerina performed for a critical audi- learning disabilities and 40.8% (n == 20) In the metaphor "The billboard was a
ence and, after being laughed at, de- of those given by the children with wart on the highway," the billboard is
cided to continue dancing to please learning disabilities. Incorrect explana-
iana- the topic, the wart is the vehicle, and
herself. The correct moral choice was tions or "no response" accountedd for the shared characteristics (unattractive,
"Satisfaction will come to those who 24.6% (n = 18) of explanations from raised) are considered to be the ground.
please themselves." A metaphorical nondisabled children and 59.1% (n = To understand the fable and the moral
explanation would be "To thine own 29) of those provided by the children
dren attached to it, it is necessary to perceive
self be true," or, in the words of one with LD. the relationship among the characteris-
subject, "Because if you do something tics of the participants in the story, the
that you really like, it will make you events that occur, and events that
happy and you'll like doing it and Discussion could happen in everyday life. The
you'll really be happy and you won't morals of fables are similar to proverbs
care if anybody else likes it or not." An The children with LD had signifi- ;nifi- in that they are wise sayings that teach
appropriate/literal explanation cen- cantly more difficulty selecting and a lesson. Morals are presented in ab-
tered on specifics in the fable and fre- explaining moral choices for fables es in stract language that can relate to both
quently involved a summary of the comparison to nondisabled peers, sup- the actual story in the fable and to real-
events that occurred in the fable. An porting previously reported findingsgs of life experiences in general.
example of this was, "Because the difficulties with figurative languagee for The children with learning disabili-
camel had tried her hardest to work this population (Donahue & Bryan, yan, ties were less able to select correct
out something and critics said, and all 1984; Lutzer, 1988; Nippold & Fey, morals for fables, but they did not
her friends said that she was all lumpy 1983; Seidenberg & Bernstein, 1986).)86). demonstrate qualitative differences
and bumpy and stuff and then she
decided to dance just for herself and
that way she got satisfaction from
pleasing herself."
TABLE 4
Explanations were categorized as in- Rank Order of Fable Difficulty and Corresponding Means of Correct Moral Choices
correct if they did not correctly or fully
explain the moral of the story. Exam- Learning disability group Average learner group
ples included, "Because it sounded Fable M Fable M
like it was the right one and has some
of the words in it," or, "Cause she "The Hippopotamus at Dinner" .78 "The Hippopotamus at Dinner" 1.0
was training out in the desert when "The Hen and the Apple Tree" .64 "The Camel Dances" 1.0
"The Camel Dances" .57 "The Bear and the Crow" .78
she was training, dancing—her feet
"The Bear and the Crow" .50 "The Hen and the Apple Tree" .71
were getting all blistered and when "The Mouse at the Seashore" .38 "The Mouse at the Seashore" .71
she finally thought she was ready, "The Pig at the Candy Store" .38 "The Pig at the Candy Store" .57
she went out to her place, and when "The Elephant and His Son" .28 "The Elephant and His Son" .5
she was finished her dance, nobody
gave her an applause—when she was
done." "No responses" rarely oc-
curred and were grouped with the in-
correct responses. TABLE 5
The two authors independently clas- Analysis of Variance: Group Assignment x Fable
sified the explanations. Initial inter-
SS df MS F p
examiner reliability was 90%. Differ-
ences between examiners concerning Total 45.82 391
classification disagreements were re- Between subjects 12.10 27
solved through discussion. None of LD x Controls 3.19 1 3.19 9.38 .01
the children with learning disabilities Error b 8.91 26 .34
Within subjects 33.72 364
gave metaphorical explanations for a
Fable 5.28 6 .88 11.13 .001
moral choice. Seven metaphorical ex- Fable x Group .564 6 .094 1.18 ns
planations occurred among six dif- Error w 27.87 352 .079
ferent nondisabled children and ac-
Downloaded from ldx.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on March 4, 2016
JOURNAL OF LEARNING DISABILITIES

when fable difficulty levels were com- learning disabilities and 7 of the 14 The language concepts expressed in
pared. Both groups of children found nondisabled children chose the correct the fable and moral are also quite ab-
some fables to be easier than others moral. This story involves an elephant stract. For example, in the fable just
(see Table 4). Factors that may have and his father spending a quiet eve- discussed, the vocabulary words obser-
contributed to variability in difficulty ning together. The young elephant vation and knowledge could have pre-
include vocabulary, the degree of the begins to sing, and the father, who is sented far more difficulty than the
fable's adherence to story grammar, reading, tells him to stop, as he can words love and reward, which appeared
inclusion of one primary actor versus pay attention to only one thing at a in the popular incorrect choice. In
a relationship between actors, and the time. The father lights a cigar, and the addition, the related moral selected by
child's familiarity with the relation- ash sets his slipper on fire as he con- many nondisabled learners was itself
ships portrayed in the fable. tinues to read. The boy elephant asks metaphorical in nature, as "the high
Winner et al. (1976) suggested that his father if he can still only attend to and mighty" is not meant in the literal
both the inability to understand words one thing at a time. When the father sense but in the figurative one.
and difficulty perceiving similarities answers in the affirmative, the boy ele- Children with and without learning
between disparate objects could ac- phant directs his father's attention to disabilities found it easier to select the
count for variation in the ability to the slipper and the father puts the fire correct moral for a fable than to explain
understand metaphors. Vocabulary dif- out by sticking his foot in a bucket of their choices. Metaphorical explana-
ficulty varies, and the events depicted water. The boy elephant begins to sing tions occurred occasionally in the non-
in one fable may correspond more once again. The correct moral of this disabled population, but appropriate/
closely to children's stored experiential fable was "Knowledge will not always literal explanations were the most
schemas than to events in other fables. take the place of simple observation." common. None of the children with
The more distance between the items Certainly most children would have a LD gave metaphorical explanations,
being compared—the more distance more difficult time identifying with the and only 40.8% of their explanations
between the topic and the vehicle—the parent-child relationship and outcome were classified as appropriate/literal.
more difficult the metaphor is to com- presented in this story. Four of the Appropriate/literal explanations had to
prehend (Winner, Engel, & Gardner, nondisabled children selected the re- be specific to the story. Because many
1980). lated moral, "It is the high and mighty of the children with learning disabili-
Fables that closely follow story gram- who have the longest distance to fall," ties had difficulty producing cohesive
mars may generate appropriate sche- possibly reflecting some satisfaction in texts and omitted details, many of their
mas. It is beyond the scope of this the child being right while the father explanations were classified as incor-
study to analyze each fable relative to was wrong. Only one child with LD rect. Other children with LD used non-
its adherence to story grammar, but a selected this related moral. The most communicative comments—possibly a
cursory examination indicates that common moral selected by children compensatory strategy (e.g., "Because
fables vary greatly in this regard. "The with LD was "Love can be its own re- it's nice and pleasant and I think it's
Hippopotamus at Dinner," the easiest ward." These children may have ab- the right one. Just the way it fit in").
fable for both groups of children, in- stracted the love that the parent and All children showed a preference for
volves a hippopotamus who goes to child had for one another but did not the related moral when they did not
dinner, eats too much, gets caught be- detect the relationship between knowl- select the correct one. However, be-
tween the table and the chair, and can- edge and observation. This can be sub- cause the children with learning dis-
not get out. The elephant expresses the stantiated by several of their explana- abilities were less successful in select-
idea that maybe he ate too much. The tions of their moral choice. One said, ing the correct moral, they were more
correct moral of the story is "Too much " 'Cause his son love him so he told likely than the nondisabled learners to
of anything often leaves one with a him his foot was on fire," and another select a related moral.
feeling of regret." This probably is a situ- said, "Because the kid loved him and Additional research in the area of
ation that children have encountered— so he told him." These explanations in- figurative language abilities of children
eating too much at Thanksgiving, too dicate that the children understood the with LD is warranted. Lee and Kahmi
much candy, or too many green apples. literal details in the story. In fact, sev- (1990) found that although children
They may even have been exposed to eral others retold the story to explain who had learning disabilities but no
the saying, "Your eyes are bigger than their moral selection but failed to select oral language deficits experienced
your stomach." The story line is also the appropriate moral. This is consistent difficulty with metaphoric language,
fairly straightforward. with the idea that although context those with language deficits were even
The most difficult fable for all chil- may aid in the interpretation of meta- more impaired. The present study
dren was "The Elephant and His phorical and nonliteral language, it does grouped all children with learning dis-
Father." Four of the 14 children with not ensure a figurative interpretation. abilities together, preventing exami-

Downloaded from ldx.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on March 4, 2016


VOLUME 28, NUMBER 5, MAY 1995 307

nation of the effect of oral language ABOUT THE AUTHORS tation: A Piagetian model. Child Devel-
delays in relation to fable comprehen- opment, 49, 649-659.
Eileen P. Abrahamsen received her EdD in
sion. Furthermore, reading achieve- speech-language pathology from Teachers Col- Hirsch, K., Jansky, J., & Langsford, W.
de
ment scores were not available. Read- lege at Columbia University in 1980. She is an (1965). Predicting reading failure. New
ing achievement and/or experience associate professor of speech-language pathol- York: Harper & Row.
could have affected results. In addi- ogy at Old Dominion University and maintains Demorest, A., Silberstein, L., Gardner, H.,
tion, IQ scores were not available for a small private practice. She is currently inves- & Winner, E. (1983). Telling it as it isn't:
the nondisabled control g r o u p . It is tigating the relationship between phonological Children's understanding of figurative
possible that these children h a d higher encoding abilities and reading in children with language. British Journal of Developmental
learning disabilities. Pamela T. Sprouse re- Psychology, 1, 121-134.
nonverbal and verbal abilities than
ceived her master's degree in speech-language Donahue, M., & Bryan, T. (1984). Commu-
those classified as learning disabled.
pathology from Old Dominion University in nicative skills and peer relations of learn-
Finally, vocabulary and adherence to
1991 and was awarded a certificate of clinical ing disabled adolescents. Topics in Lan-
story grammar were not controlled. competence in speech-language pathology in guage Disorders, 4, 10-21.
Blue (1981) r e c o m m e n d e d that par- 1993. She is currently providing services to Douglas, J. D., & Peel, B. (1979). The devel-
ents, teachers, and clinicians avoid the elementary-age children in public school and has opment of metaphor and proverb trans-
use of some forms of figurative lan- developed a special interest in children with LD lation in children grades one through
guage with children having severe lan- as a result of her work with this population. seven. Journal of Educational Research, 73,
guage delays. Although children with Address: Eileen P. Abrahamsen, Child Study 116-119.
LD manifest deficits in comprehending Center, Old Dominion University, Norfolk, Gardner, H. (1974). Metaphors and modal-
and producing figurative language, it VA 23529-0136. ities: How children project polar adjec-
is not necessary to avoid figurative lan- tives onto diverse domains. Child Devel-
guage with those of school age. In fact, opment, 45, 84-91.
REFERENCES
in light of the Regular Education Initi- Gardner, H., Winner, E., Bechhofer, R., &
ative, the increased incidence of inclu- Ackerman, B. P. (1982). On comprehend- Wolf, D. (1978). The development of fig-
sive classes, a n d the popularity of the ing idioms: Do children get the picture? urative language. In K. Nelson (Ed.),
whole language approach, such rec- Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, Children's language (Vol. 1, pp. 1-38).
33, 439-454. New York: Gardner.
ommendations would come into con-
Aram, D., Ekelman, B., & Nation, J. (1984). Gibbs, R. W. (1987). Linguistic factors in
flict with the curricular d e m a n d s of
Preschoolers with language disorders: 10 children's understanding of idioms. Jour-
general education.
years later. Journal of Speech and Hearing nal of Child Language, 14, 1034-1039.
Some promising suggestions for Research, 27, 232-244. Hall, P., & Tomblin, J. (1978). A follow-up
remediating figurative language defi- Aram, D., & Nation, J. (1980). Preschool study of children with articulation and
cits in children with LD are beginning language disorders and subsequent lan- language disorders. Journal of Speech and
to emerge. These include (a) teaching guage and academic difficulties. Journal Hearing Disorders, 43, 227-241.
decision-making strategies that would of Communication Disorders, 13, 159-170. King, R., Jones, C , & Lasky, E. (1982). In
help children with LD to identify w h e n Baechle, C. L., & Ming-Gon, J. L. (1990). retrospect: A fifteen year follow-up report
a literal or figurative interpretation is The effects of direct feedback and prac- of speech-language disordered children.
tice on metaphor performance in children Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in
necessary (Lutzer, 1988; Seidenberg &
with learning disabilities. Journal of Learn- the Schools, 13, 24-32.
Bernstein, 1986) and (b) using feedback
ing Disabilities, 23, 451-455.
and practice in small g r o u p settings to Lee, R. F., & Kahmi, A. G. (1990). Meta-
Bashir, A. S., & Scavuzzo, A. (1992). Chil-
enhance the comprehension of figura- phoric competence in children with learn-
dren with language disorders: Natural
tive language (Baechle & Ming-Gon, ing disabilities. Journal of Learning Dis-
history and academic success. Journal of
1990). abilities, 23, 476-482.
Learning Disabilities, 25, 53-65.
Although there is still m u c h to learn Billow, R. M. (1975). A cognitive develop- Lobel, A. (1980). Fables. New York: Harper
mental study of metaphor comprehen- & Row.
about the nature of figurative language
sion. Developmental Psychology, 11, Lodge, D., & Leach, E. A. (1975). Chil-
deficits in children with learning dis-
415-423. dren's acquisition of idioms in the Eng-
abilities, there is growing evidence that
Blue, C M . (1981). Types of utterances to lish language. Journal of Speech and Hearing
these children demonstrate develop- Research, 18, 521-529.
avoid when speaking to language im-
mental lags and/or deficits in this area.
paired children. Language, Speech, and Lutzer, V. D. (1988). Comprehension of
Future research should continue to
Hearing Services in the Schools, 12, 120-124. proverbs by average children and chil-
investigate m e t h o d s of improving the dren with learning disorders. Journal of
Cacciari, C , & Levorato, M. C. (1989). How
ability of children with LD to cor- children understand idioms in discourse. Learning Disabilities, 21, 104-108.
rectly interpret figurative language in Journal of Child Language, 16, 387-405. Malgady, R. G. (1977). Children's interpre-
order to be more successful in general Cometa, M. S., & Eson, M. E. (1978). Log- tation and appreciation of similes. Child
classrooms. ical operations and metaphor interpre- Development, 48, 1734-1738.

Downloaded from ldx.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on March 4, 2016


308 JOURNAL OF LEARNING DISABILITIES

Nippold, M. A., & Fey, S. H. (1983). Meta- Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 17, performance on metaphor-related lan-
phoric understanding in preadolescents 465-477. guage tasks: A comparison of good and
having history of language acquisition Prinz, P. M. (1983). The development of poor readers. Remedial and Special Educa-
difficulties. Language, Speech, and Hearing idiomatic meaning in children. Language tion, 9(2), 39-45.
Services in the Schools, 14, 171-180. and Speech, 26, 263-272. Vosniadou, S., & Ortony, A. (1983). The
Nippold, M. A., Leonard, L. B., & Kail, R. Resnick, D. A. (1982). A developmental emergence of the literal-metaphorical-
(1984). Syntactic and conceptual factors study of proverb comprehension. Journal anomalous distinction in young children.
in children's understanding of meta- of Psycholinguistic Research, 11, 521-538. Child Development, 54, 154-161.
phors. Journal of Speech and Hearing Re- Reynolds, R. E., & Ortony, A. (1980). Some Vosniadou, S., Ortony, A., Reynolds, R.,
search, 27, 197-205. issues in the measurement of children's & Wilson, T. (1984). Sources of difficulty
Nippold, M. A., & Martin, S. T. (1989). comprehension of metaphorical lan- in the young child's understanding of
Idiom interpretation in isolation versus guage. Child Development, 51, 1110-1119. metaphorical language. Child Develop-
context: A developmental study with Richardson, C , & Church, J. (1959). A ment, 55, 1588-1606.
adolescents. Journal of Speech and Hearing developmental analysis of proverb inter- Wechsler, D. (1974). Wechsler intelligence
Research, 32, 59-66. pretations. Journal of Genetic Psychology, scale for children-Revised. San Antonio, TX:
Nippold, M. A., Martin, S. A., & Erskine, 94, 169-179. Psychological Corp.
B. J. (1988). Proverb comprehension in Seidenberg, P. L., & Bernstein, D. K. (1986). Winner, E., Engel, M., & Gardner, H.
context: A developmental study with The comprehension of similes and meta- (1980). Misunderstanding metaphor:
adolescents. Journal of Speech and Hearing phors by learning-disabled and non- What's the problem? Journal of Experimen-
Research, 31, 19-28. learning-disabled children. Language, tal Child Psychology, 30, 22-32.
Ortony, A., Schallert, D. L., Reynolds, Speech, and Hearing Services in the Schools, Winner, E., Rosenstiel, A., & Gardner, H.
R. E., & Antos, S. J. (1978). Interpreting 17, 219-229. (1976). The development of metaphoric
metaphors and idioms: Some effects of Seidenberg, P. L., & Bernstein, D. K. understanding. Developmental Psychology,
context on comprehension. Journal of (1988). Metaphor comprehension and 12, 289-297.

(Continued from p. 301)

REFERENCES Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Dis- Nicolson, R. I., Fawcett, A. J., & Miles,
ciplines, 27, 45-54. T. R. (1993). Feasibility study for the develop-
Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. E. (1978). Difficul- Lovegrove, W. (1994). Visual deficits in dys- ment of a computerized screening test for
ties in auditory organization as a possible lexia: Evidence and implications. In A. J. dyslexia in adults (Report No. OL176).
cause of reading backwardness. Nature, Fawcett & R. I. Nicolson (Eds.), Dys- Sheffield: Employment Department.
271, 746-747. lexia in children: Multidisciplinary perspec- Olson, R. K., Gillis, J. J., Rack, J. P.,
De Gelder, B., & Vroomen, J. (1991). Phono- tives (pp. 113-136). London: Harvester DeFries, J. C , & Fulker, D. W. (1991).
logical deficits: Beneath the surface of Wheatsheaf. Confirmatory factor analysis of word
reading-acquisition problems. Psycholog- Lovegrove, W. J., Garzia, R. P., & Nichol- recognition and process measures in the
ical Research, 53, 88-97. son, S. B. (1990). Experimental evidence Colorado reading project. Reading and
Elliott, C. D. (1992). British ability scales- of a transient system deficit in specific Writing, 3, 235-248.
Spelling scale. Windsor, England: NFER- reading disability. Journal of the American Stanovich, K. E. (1986). Matthew effects in
Nelson. Optometric Association, 61, 137-146. reading: Some consequences of individ-
Fawcett, A. J., & Nicolson, R. I. (1993). McNicol, D. M. (1972). A primer in signal ual differences in the acquisition of liter-
Children with dyslexia show deficits in detection theory. London: George Allen & acy. Reading Research Quarterly, 16, 32-71.
most primitive skills. In W. Kintsch (Ed.), Unwin. Stein, J. F. (1989). Visuospatial perception
Proceedings of the 15th annual conference of Nicolson, R. I., & Fawcett, A. J. (1990). and reading problems. Irish Journal of Psy-
the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 422-427). Automaticity: A new framework for dys- chology, 10, 521-533.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. lexia research. Cognition, 35, 159-182. Stein, J. F. (1994). A visual defect in dys-
Finucci, J. M., Guthrie, J. T., Childs, A. L., Nicolson, R. I., & Fawcett, A. J. (1993). lexics? In A. J. Fawcett & R. I. Nicolson
Abbey, H., & Childs, B. (1976). The Towards the origins of dyslexia. In S. F. (Eds.), Dyslexia in children: Multidiscipli-
genetics of specific reading disability. Wright & R. Groner (Eds.), Facets of dys- nary perspectives (pp. 137-157). London:
Annals of Human Genetics, 50, 1-23. lexia and its remediation (pp. 371-392). Harvester Wheatsheaf.
Jorm, A. F., Share, D. L., McLean, R., & Amsterdam: Elsevier. Thomson, M. E. (1984). Developmental dys-
Matthews, D. (1986). Cognitive factors at Nicolson, R. L, & Fawcett, A. J. (1994). lexia: Its nature, assessment and remediation.
school entry predictive of specific read- Reaction times and dyslexia. Quarterly London: Edward Arnold.
ing retardation and general backward- Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47A, Vellutino, F. R. (1979). Dyslexia: Theory and
ness: A research note. Journal of Child 29-48. research. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Downloaded from ldx.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on March 4, 2016

You might also like