Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

1016556

research-article20212021
SGOXXX10.1177/21582440211016556SAGE OpenLiu and Wu

Original Research

SAGE Open

Teaching Anxiety and Foreign


April-June 2021: 1­–12
© The Author(s) 2021
DOI: 10.1177/21582440211016556
https://doi.org/10.1177/21582440211016556

Language Anxiety Among Chinese journals.sagepub.com/home/sgo

College English Teachers

Meihua Liu1 and Bin Wu1,2

Abstract
This study explored teaching anxiety and teacher foreign language anxiety (FLA) in 151 Chinese college English teachers
in relation to their individual characteristics. Analyses of data collected from mixed-form questionnaires revealed the
following major findings: (a) Major causes for teaching anxiety were concern about classroom teaching, research, other
work and promotion, and interest and confidence in teaching, and major sources for teacher FLA were apprehension
of speaking English, fear of negative outcomes, and confidence in English competence; (b) the participants of various
backgrounds suffered from varying degrees of teaching anxiety and teacher FLA; (c) gender, age, educational level,
English proficiency, and experience of visiting/studying in English-speaking countries significantly affected the participants’
teaching anxiety and teacher FLA levels; and (d) anxiety seriously affected the participants’ work and life. Evidently,
anxiety is an important issue faced by university language teachers and needs to be further researched and seriously
handled.

Keywords
teaching anxiety, foreign language anxiety, college English teacher, individual characteristics, effect

Introduction Literature Review


As a psychological and affective factor, anxiety often exists Teaching Anxiety
in the teaching profession (Desouky & Allam, 2017; Fimian
& Blanton, 1987). As reviewed below, a multitude of studies Anxiety refers to the subjective feelings of nervousness, fear,
have been done on anxiety in teachers of various back- panic, turmoil, and worry (Spielberger, 1966, 1983). It arises
grounds, especially on pre-service or primary and secondary as a response to the uncertainty of a forthcoming event/eval-
school teachers (Cheung & Hui, 2011; İpek, 2016; Kim & uation and/or worry of the consequence of an event.
Spielberger (1966) proposed and differentiated state anxiety
Kim, 2004; Merç, 2011). Comparatively, fewer studies have
from trait anxiety. State anxiety is situational, involves feel-
been done on anxiety experienced by non-native second/for-
ings of apprehension following activities in the autonomic
eign language (SL/FL) instructors, especially in-service SL/
nervous system, and arises in concrete circumstances deemed
FL instructors (Horwitz, 1996; Kralova & Tirpakova, 2019;
as threatening, while trait anxiety is an “acquired behavioral
Tum, 2014). As discussed in Tum (2014), although foreign
disposition that predisposes an individual to perceive a wide
language anxiety (FLA), a significant issue in SL/FL learn-
range of objectively non-dangerous circumstances as threat-
ing and acquisition, has been widely researched in SL/FL
ening” (Spielberger, 1966, p. 17).
learners, it has been much under-researched on SL/FL teach-
As a type of state anxiety, teaching anxiety refers to
ers. Moreover, as non-native SL/FL teachers, they have to
“anxiety experienced in relation to teaching activities that
confront not only teaching anxiety but also FLA, both of
which are fundamental factors for career performance and 1
Tsinghua University, Beijing, China
development and yet put them under enormous pressure. 2
Chang’an University, Xi’an, China
Thus, research in this area is worthwhile and the present
Corresponding Author:
study sought to examine Chinese college English teachers’ Meihua Liu, Department of Foreign Languages and Literatures, Tsinghua
teaching anxiety and FLA in relation to their individual University, Beijing 100084, China.
characteristics. Emails: liumeihua@mail.tsinghua.edu.cn; ellenlmh@yahoo.com

Creative Commons CC BY: This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 License
(https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) which permits any use, reproduction and distribution of
the work without further permission provided the original work is attributed as specified on the SAGE and Open Access pages
(https://us.sagepub.com/en-us/nam/open-access-at-sage).
2 SAGE Open

involve the preparation and execution of classroom activi- merited researchers’ attention (Horwitz, 2010; Horwitz et al.,
ties” (Gardner & Leak, 1994, p. 28). Alternatively, teaching 1986). Horwitz (2008), a pioneer in research on student FLA,
anxiety is closely related to the task of teaching (Keavney & is also a pioneer in research on teacher FLA who designed
Sinclair, 1978). To gauge teaching anxiety, Parsons (1973) the Teacher Foreign Language Anxiety Scale (TFLAS) to
developed the Teaching Anxiety Scale (TAS), which has gauge the level of anxiety experienced by non-native SL/FL
been widely employed in subsequent studies (Cheung & Hui, teachers. Via the TFLAS, dairies, and/or interviews, increas-
2011; İpek, 2016; Kim & Kim, 2004; Merç, 2011). These ingly more research has been done on teacher FLA, most of
studies, as well as those using other methods (Ahmed & which focuses on FLA in pre-service EFL teachers or ele-
Julius, 2015; Desouky & Allam, 2017; Liu & Yan, 2020; mentary and middle school teachers (Bekleyen, 2009; Kim
Merç, 2011; Parsons, 1973; Peker, 2009; Robinson & Clay, & Kim, 2004; Kralova et al., 2017; Kralova & Tirpakova,
2005; X. Wang & Wang, 2015; Yazici & Altun, 2013), find 2019; Kunt & Tüm, 2010; Machida, 2015; Merç, 2011;
that anxiety exists in many teachers of various backgrounds Yoon, 2012). These studies reveal that EFL (English as a FL)
due to reasons like conflicts of values, workload, students’ teachers are generally anxious about teaching due to various
behavior, relations between faculty members, academic reasons such as English proficiency, pupil–student relation-
demands, and lack of educational resources. These studies ship, lack of educational resources, workload, and classroom
also show that teaching anxiety levels are prone to differ- management. For example, Yoon (2012) studied how much
ences in individual characteristics like teaching experiences, anxiety was experienced by 52 pre-service EFL teachers in
professional title, gender, education, age, and the school Korea during the teaching practicum. Analyses of the ques-
environment. For example, Cheung and Hui’s (2011) study tionnaires showed that sources of their anxiety were mainly
of 195 teachers indicated that teachers in Hong Kong and fear of English speech in the classroom and lack of confi-
Shanghai experienced teaching anxiety, with those in Hong dence about the target language. This finding was further
Kong at a higher level. This finding was supported by supported by Kunt and Tüm (2010). Bekleyen’s (2009) study
research on teachers of specific disciplines, such as university of FL listening anxiety (FLLA) among Turkish student
psychology teachers (Gardner & Leak, 1994), pre-service teachers of English found that the students generally experi-
mathematics teachers (Peker & Ertekin, 2011), and univer- enced high levels of FLLA, and that their FLLA levels
sity teachers of arts (Liu & Yan, 2020). decreased with the increase in their listening grades. The
study also found that the sources of the FLLA were a lack of
emphasis on listening in previous educational stages and
Teacher FLA deficit listening skills. To further explore the topic, Tum
FLA, a situation-specific type of anxiety, is specified as “a (2014) interviewed a cohort of 12 EFL student teachers and
distinct complex of self-perceptions, beliefs, feelings, and discovered that their anxiety stemmed from concern over
behaviors related to classroom language learning arising error and fear of negative evaluation. The study further
from the uniqueness of the language learning experience” revealed that FLA hindered the participants from using
(Horwitz et al., 1986, p. 128). As complicated mental opera- English and language-intensive teaching practices. Kim
tions are “required in order to communicate, any perfor- and Kim’s (2004) study of Korean elementary and middle
mance in the L2 is likely to challenge an individual’s school EFL teachers showed that the most predominant
self-concept as a competent communicator and lead to reti- source of teaching anxiety was inadequate proficiency in
cence, self-consciousness, fear, or even panic” (Horwitz et English, followed by limited confidence, limited knowledge
al., 1986, p. 128). Consequently, SL/FL learners may doubt of linguistics and education, inadequate class preparation
their abilities, worry about their performance, and even have and comparison with native teachers, partially supported in
a mental block (Horwitz et al., 1986; Tobias, 1979). Studies subsequent research (Kralova & Tirpakova, 2019). Other
using various methods like surveys, interviews, and diaries main sources of EFL student teachers’ anxiety were student
generally reveal a predominantly debilitating effect of FLA profiles, teaching procedures, under-preparation, and class-
on SL/FL learning (Dewaele & Alfawzan, 2018; Horwitz, room management (Machida, 2015; Merç, 2011). Kralova
2010; Horwitz et al., 1986; Liu & Xiangming, 2019). For and Tirpakova (2019) also found that non-native teachers’
example, Liu and Xiangming’s (2019) longitudinal study on English-speaking anxiety increased with age.
324 Chinese postgraduate students found that FLA was Studies on FLA in in-service SL/FL teachers show that
highly related to the students’ overall test performance as these teachers also suffer from varying degrees of anxiety
well as performance in English speaking, reading, listening, due to various reasons, such as workload, imperfect perfor-
and writing, and that FLA was a powerful predictor of the mance or competence of the target language, inadequate
latter, especially speaking test performance. preparation, and pressure from research and promotion (İpek,
FLA is not only a crucial issue for SL/FL learners but a 2016; X. Wang & Wang, 2015; Zhang, 2010). For example,
major concern for SL/FL language teachers in teaching İpek’s (2016) qualitative study of Turkish EFL teachers iden-
the SL/FL (Horwitz, 1996; Tum, 2014). Although not much tified three predominant sources of teaching anxiety: fear of
researched as student FLA, teacher FLA has also long failure, making mistakes, and using the mother tongue. This
Liu and Wu 3

finding was supported by X. Wang and Wang (2015), who Participants


found that Chinese EFL teachers generally reported feeling
anxious due to such reasons as heavy workload, failure to Altogether, 151 (21 men and 130 women) college English
engage in family routines, course preparation, teaching abil- teachers working at various universities in China answered
ity, and unfair evaluation by deans and students. the survey in the present research. Of these participants, 71
(47%) aged 31 to 40, 55 (36.4%) aged 41 to 50, 18 (11.9%)
aged 51 to 60, and seven (4.6%) aged 26 to 30; 104 (68.9%)
Research Questions were lectures, 49 (25.8%) associate professors, two (1.3%)
As reviewed, a multitude of studies have been done on anxi- professors, and six (4%) assistant lecturers; 114 (75.5%) held
ety in teachers of various backgrounds (Cheung & Hui, 2011; master’s degree, 25 (16.6%) and 12 (7.9%) held bachelor’s
İpek, 2016; Kim & Kim, 2004; Merç, 2011), most of which degree and doctor’s degree, respectively. With a range of 0 to
target elementary–middle school students and/or pre-service 37 years’ experiences of teaching English in universities, the
student teachers. Comparatively, fewer studies have been participants generally had taught English for 15.17 (SD =
investigated on anxiety experienced by non-native SL/FL 7.255) years. With an average of 11.44 (SD = 3.396) and a
teachers (Horwitz, 1996, 2008; Kralova & Tirpakova, 2019; range of 0 to 24 required hours of teaching per week, most
Tum, 2014). Thus, SL/FL teachers’ FLA is still a much participants taught 12 hr (64/42.4%), followed by 10 hr
under-explored area (Tum, 2014). Research on this is espe- (22/14.6%), 8 hr (16/10.6%), and 9 hr (10/6.6%) per week,
cially worthy given that teaching anxiety and FLA are two respectively. With a mean of 0.44 (SD = 0.821) years of vis-
fundamental factors for non-native EFL teachers’ career per- iting an English-speaking country, over half of the partici-
formance and development. Aiming to explore Chinese col- pants (88/58.3%) had never visited any English-speaking
lege English teachers’ teaching anxiety and FLA in relation countries. With a mean of 0.58 (SD = 3.321) years of study-
to their individual characteristics, the present study intended ing in an English-speaking country, more than 60%
to answer the following three research questions: (104/68.9%) participants had never studied in any English-
speaking countries. On a scale of 1 to 10, the participants
Research Question 1: What are the sources of teaching generally rated their English proficiency as 7.05 (SD =
anxiety and FLA experienced by Chinese college English 1.267), 126 (83.44%) of whom rated themselves as interme-
teachers? diate–advanced English learners.
Research Question 2: How are teaching anxiety and
teacher FLA related to the participants’ individual Instruments
factors?
Research Question 3: What are the effects of teaching The mixed-form survey used in the present research included
anxiety and teacher FLA? the nine-item background questionnaire, the 33-item TAS,
the 18-item TFLAS, and four open-ended questions. The
questions were as follows: “Do you often feel anxious?”;
Research Design “As a university teacher, my anxiety primarily stems from
________”; “As a college English teacher, my anxiety pri-
Context marily stems from ________”; and “As the result of anxiety,
To improve the quality of education and enhance the reputa- I _________.”
tion both at home and abroad, most institutions of higher
education in China set demanding requirements in teaching The Demographic Questionnaire. To collect individual infor-
and research for promotion. Namely, a university teacher has mation about the respondents, the nine-item Demographic
to demonstrate excellent research abilities (e.g., projects, Questionnaire intended to gather such information as profes-
funds, and numbers of publications in high-ranking journals) sional title, age, gender, educational level, years of teaching
and teaching performance (e.g., minimum teaching hours per in universities, teaching hours per week, experience of visit-
week and good student evaluation results) to win chances for ing/studying in English-speaking countries, and self-rated
promotion. Meanwhile, they often have to take administra- English proficiency.
tive work simultaneously to satisfy various needs of the
department, faculty, university, or even higher level adminis- TAS. The 33-item TAS used in the present research (Cron-
tration. All these put university teachers in greater pressure bach’s α = .851) was modified from that used in Parsons
than before. As college English courses are compulsory to all (1973) with reference to the findings in Liu and Yan (2020).
students of a university, college English teachers often have For better clarity, the present study removed statements
to teach more hours per week than those in other disciplines about parent observation/conference and added those about
and thus may face even greater pressure. Housework may promotion and research. Moreover, expressions like pre-ser-
add more pressure and anxiety to them because the majority vice and other interns/teachers were replaced by colleagues/
of FL teachers at all educational levels in China are female. other teachers. The resulted TAS items were placed on a
4 SAGE Open

5-point Likert-type scale, with values of 1 to 5 assigned to teaching, and effectiveness of teaching. The second factor,
the five descriptors ranging from “never” to “always,” Confidence in Teaching (TAS2), covered seven items reflec-
respectively. Hence, a score of more than 3, 2 to 3, and below tive of confidence in teaching as a profession of college
2 on a TAS scale signified high, medium, and low teaching English teacher, which accounted for 10.836% of the total
anxiety, respectively (Parsons, 1973; Liu & Yan, 2020). variance. The third factor, Concern About Research (TAS3),
consisted of four items indicating worry about applying for
TFLAS. The 18-item TFLAS utilized in the present research research projects and funds, writing up research papers, and
(α = .902) was modified from that designed by Horwitz getting them published, which explained 10.619% of the
(2008). To better suit the present situation, the item “I would total variance. Factor 4 (TAS4), Concern About Other Work,
not worry about taking a training course conducted entirely which explained 7.537% of the total variance, had four items
in English” was changed to be “I would not worry about suggesting worry about work unrelated to teaching and
teaching a course conducted entirely in English.” The research such as expressing opinions in the public, adminis-
TFLAS was also a 5-point Likert-type scale with values of 1 trative work, planning time for different work, and other
to 5 assigned to the five descriptors ranging from “strongly errands. Three items constituted TAS5 (Interest in Teaching)
disagree” to “strongly agree.” Thus, a respondent’s anxiety which were expressive of interest in and happiness about
level is determined by the sum of his or her responses to all teaching and explained 6.261% of the total variance. TAS6
the questions divided by 18: an average of around 3 means (Concern About Promotion) had two items suggestive of
slight anxiety about language proficiency; a score of around concern about promotion and accounted for 5.777% of the
4 indicates a certain amount of anxiety (Horwitz, 2008). total variance. The loadings of the TAS items are reported in
Table 1.
The analysis of the TFLAS yielded three factors (KMO
Data Collection and Analyses
= .919, Bartlett’s test = 1.191, p = .000), with eigenvalues
The survey was translated into Chinese and then back- being 5.329, 2.836, and 1.607, accounting for 29.606%,
checked by two researchers proficient in both Chinese and 15.746%, and 8.9% of the total variance, respectively.
English, which, together with a consent form, were then Factor 1 (TFLAS1), Apprehension of Speaking English, had
issued online for a month to prospective respondents in nine items expressing worry and fear of speaking English in
China. The participation was voluntary. The collected data diverse situations; Factor 2 (TFLAS2), Fear of Negative
were then analyzed via SPSS 20. The TFLAS and TAS were Outcomes, included five items suggesting fear of making
subjected to rotated (varimax) principal components analy- mistakes and failing to understand others; and Factor 3
ses to explore causes for FLA and teaching anxiety. Then, (TFLAS3), Confidence in English Competence, had four
one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA; Duncan) and inde- items indicative of confidence in one’s own English profi-
pendent-samples t-tests were run to examine patterns and ciency and performance. The loadings of TFLAS items are
differences in TFLAS and TAS scales in relation to indi- reported in Table 2.
vidual factors. The answers to the open-ended questions
were analyzed according to the themes, which could be
words, phrases, ideas, and topics (Richards, 2009). The Correlations Within and Between TFLAS and TAS
themes used in the present study were ideas, such as pres- Scales
sure from research, teaching hours, and health. When cod-
ing the responses, a number was given to each open-question Results of correlation analyses are presented in Table 3,
respondent, which was used when their responses were cited which shows that the TFLAS scales were significantly highly
in the paper. correlated with each other, with coefficients ranging from
.571 to .952 (p ≤ .05). So were TAS scales except for TAS3
and TAS4, with a coefficient range of .182 to .852 (p ≤ .05).
Results TAS4 was significantly (r = .511, p ≤ .05) related to TAS3
but lowly with other TAS scales, and TAS3 was significantly
Causes for Teaching Anxiety and Teacher FLA correlated with TAS1, TAS4, TAS6, and TAS (r = .169–
With reference to Liu and Yan (2020), a six-factor solution .584, p ≤ .05) but lowly with TAS2 and TAS5. These results,
was adopted on TAS (Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin [KMO] = .867, coupled with the loadings presented in Tables 1 and 2, indi-
Bartlett’s test = 2.417, p = .000), with eigenvalues being cated that TFLAS1, TFLAS2, and TFLAS3 were important
5.895, 3.576, 3.504, 2.487, 2.066, and 1.906, respectively. components of TFLAS, and that TAS1, TAS2, TAS3, TAS4,
Accounting for 17.865% of the total variance, Factor 1, TAS5, and TAS6 were important components of TAS.
Concern About Classroom Teaching (TAS1), included 13 These results largely conformed with the respondents’
items indicating concerns about classroom teaching such as answers to the open-ended questions on causes for anxiety.
classroom management, responding to students’ questions, Of 151 survey participants, 87 explained causes for their
teacher–student relationship, being observed, presenting anxiety in working as a college English teacher (Table 4).
Liu and Wu 5

Table 1. Loadings of Rotated Principal Factor Analyses of TAS Items.

TAS items TAS1 TAS2 TAS3 TAS4 TAS5 TAS6


28. I feel uncertain about my ability to improvise in the classroom. .359 .456 .344 .250 −.006 .376
29. Even if I have trouble answering a student’s questions, I find it easy to .059 −.189 .117 −.607 .023 .163
concentrate on questions that follow.
30. I feel anxious when I am preparing lessons. .167 .209 .199 .622 −.028 .345
31. I’m afraid students won’t follow my instructions. .419 .470 .232 .330 .054 .263
32. I would feel calm if the chairperson informed me he or she was coming to −.444 −.401 −.322 −.259 .196 −.149
class to observe.
33. I’m afraid other teachers think I’m incompetent. .240 .392 .088 .512 .285 .268
34. I feel anxious about my ability to keep a class under control. .396 .584 .136 .333 .097 .193
35. I’m happier teaching than I thought I’d be. .712 .166 .014 −.044 −.061 −.024
36. I am confident in teaching. .762 .270 −.067 .168 .063 −.050
37. I am afraid to speak up in staff meetings. .166 .674 .090 −.157 .167 .036
38. I feel certain I really want to be a teacher. .785 .035 .112 .038 .114 .035
39. I feel certain about my ability to keep the students interested in what I .696 .027 .017 .318 .224 .069
teach them.
40. I find it difficult to admit to the class that I don’t know the answer to a .067 .761 −.057 .268 −.016 −.058
question a student asks.
41. I’m worried whether I will find teaching a satisfying profession. .539 .383 .171 .092 .022 .073
42. I feel that I am as good as other teachers in my program. .658 .263 −.074 −.064 .264 .029
43. I feel at ease when I am being observed by my colleague. .630 .257 .266 −.183 −.087 .086
44. I’m afraid I will forget everything I know when I get in front of a class. .308 .618 −.008 .414 −.018 −.054
45. I feel calm and collected when a student asks me a question I can’t answer. .266 .231 −.035 .079 .359 −.241
46. I feel well prepared for teaching. .599 .078 .009 .364 .129 −.005
47. I am able to decide how to present information in the classroom without .517 −.076 .033 .506 .159 .025
a feeling of uncertainty.
48. I feel sure I can be a good teacher. .618 .066 .102 .270 .307 .266
49. Good rapport with my students is one of my strong points. .548 .395 −.052 −.012 .356 .185
50. I worry about students’ evaluation of my teaching. .252 .422 .134 .348 .242 .131
51. I worry about promotion. .160 .136 .571 .081 −.066 −.015
52. I feel sure that my students are satisfied with my teaching. .410 .261 −.133 .253 .567 .111
53. I am satisfied with my research. .006 .045 .483 −.032 .667 .153
54. I am sure that I will be promoted duly. .219 −.080 .433 .022 .643 −.118
55. I am not sure whether I can do research well. .063 .159 .736 −.095 .244 .130
56. I have difficulty balancing research and teaching. −.014 .088 .775 .036 .157 −.084
57. I find it difficult to publish my papers. −.021 −.042 .669 .032 .112 −.416
58. I find it competitive to apply for research grants. −.004 −.121 .668 .015 −.039 −.333
59. Administrative duties make me tired. −.020 .034 .185 −.042 .027 −.650
60. Errands unrelated to teaching and research waste too much of my time −.152 −.250 .384 −.027 −.152 −.654
(e.g., attending unnecessary meetings and filling up various forms).

Note. Coefficient of determination—small: r ≤ .1, medium: r = .3, and large: r ≥ .5 (Cohen, 1988). TAS = Teaching Anxiety Scale; TAS1 = Concern
About Classroom Teaching; TAS2 = Confidence in Teaching; TAS3 = Concern About Research; TAS4 = Concern About Other Work; TAS5 = Interest
in Teaching; TAS6 = Concern About Promotion.

As summarized in Table 4, examination of the 87 teaching, students not liking the class, being unable to moti-
responses identified 20 sources of teaching anxiety, the vate students; 6/6.9%), little/low self-confidence (e.g., being
most important of which were pressure from research (e.g., dissatisfied with self, being not good enough, worrying
applying for projects and funds, writing up papers, getting about own competence; 5/5.7%), too much and endless
papers published, unpredictable policies, poor research work (5/5.7%), difficulty in balancing work and family
ability; 22/25.3%), pressure from promotion (e.g., (3/3.4%), students not working hard (e.g., having inadequate
demanding promotion requirements, unpredictable promo- knowledge of the subject, making inadequate efforts, being
tion policies, unfairness; 14/16.1%), heavy teaching load not interested; 3/3.4%), being not interested in teaching
(e.g., heavy workload, many teaching hours per week; (3/3.4%), and imbalance between efforts and returns
6/6.9%), worry about teaching effectiveness (e.g., not good (3/3.4%). As confided by the respondents, it was difficult
6 SAGE Open

Table 2. Loadings of Rotated Principal Factor Analyses of TFLAS Items.

TFLAS items TFLAS1 TFLAS2 TFLAS3


10. It frightens me when I don’t understand what someone is saying in English. .151 .875 −.129
11. I would not worry about teaching a course entirely in English. .656 .268 −.016
12. I am afraid that native speakers will notice every mistake I make. .144 .744 .308
13. I am pleased with the level of my English language proficiency I have achieved. .641 .345 −.209
14. I feel self-conscious speaking English in front of teachers of English. .487 .496 .347
15. When speaking English, I can get so nervous I forget things I know. .461 .418 .441
16. I feel overwhelmed by the number of rules I have to learn in order to speak English. .282 .289 .530
17. I feel comfortable around native speakers of English. .599 .243 .186
18. I never feel quite sure of myself when I am speaking English in front of native speakers. .593 .276 .466
19. I am not nervous speaking English with students. .588 .153 −.005
20. I don’t worry about making mistakes in English. .773 .214 −.047
21. I speak English well enough to be a good English teacher. .787 .171 .156
22. I get nervous when I don’t understand every word a native speaker says. .336 .593 .458
23. I feel confident when I speak English. .754 .256 .102
24. I always feel that other teachers speak English better than I do. .477 .364 .284
25. I don’t understand why some people think learning English is so hard. .169 .036 −.490
26. I try to speak English with native speakers whenever I can. .528 −.106 .148
27. I feel that my English preparation was adequate for becoming an English teacher. .652 .161 .122

Note. Coefficient of determination—small: r ≤ .1, medium: r = .3, and large: r ≥ .5 (Cohen, 1988). TFLAS = Teacher Foreign Language Anxiety Scale;
TFLAS1 = Apprehension of Speaking English; TFLAS2 = Fear of Negative Outcomes; TFLAS3 = Confidence in English Competence.

Table 3. Correlations Within and Between TAS and TFLAS Scales (N = 151).

Scales TFLAS2 TFLAS3 TFLAS TAS1 TAS2 TAS3 TAS4 TAS5 TAS6 TAS
TFLAS1 .757** .856** .952** .710** .680** .144 −.006 .531** .240** .647**
TFLAS2 1 .571** .893** .638** .609** .257** .031 .431** .265** .617**
TFLAS3 1 .771** .573** .564** .134 −.042 .406** .182* .517**
TFLAS 1 .735** .706** .200* .000 .528** .264* .682**
TAS1 1 .789** .169* .051 .645** .325** .852**
TAS2 .139 −.008 .628** .293** .792**
TAS3 1 .511** .143 .584** .548**
TAS4 1 .083 .361** .410**
TAS5 1 .286** .706**
TAS6 1 .618**
TAS 1

Note. Coefficient of determination—small: r ≤ .1, medium: r = .3, and large: r ≥ .5 (Cohen, 1988). TAS = Teaching Anxiety Scale; TFLAS = Teacher
Foreign Language Anxiety Scale.
*p ≤ .05. **p ≤ .01.

for them to “apply for projects and funds, to write up papers confidence in their own competence put a number of respon-
and get them published,” which drove them “crazy” (No. dents under pressure. Other minor causes for teaching anx-
34). Meanwhile, unpredictable policies of requirements on iety included poor health, worry about self-development,
research and poor research ability were great challenges to low salary, approaching retirement, being old, overexpecta-
some respondents. Concurrently, demanding promotion tion, ongoing reforms, pressure from colleagues, and pursu-
requirements, unpredictable promotion policies, and unfair- ing perfection.
ness in promotion exerted great pressure on many respon- As shown in Table 4, the main source for teacher FLA was
dents. In addition, some respondents were worried about the worry about English proficiency: spoken English being not
effectiveness of their teaching: They were afraid that they good enough (5/5.7%) and low-confidence in English com-
could not teach effectively, that students might not like their petence (3/3.4%). Other causes included inadequate prepara-
teaching, and/or that they would not be able to motivate tion (2/2.3%), pursuing perfection (1/1.15%), and fear of
their students. Many teaching hours per week and low/little teaching outcomes (1/1.15%).
Liu and Wu 7

Table 4. Causes for Anxiety (N = 87).

Causes for teaching anxiety 14. Being unable to teach freely (1/1.15%)
1. Pressure from research (22/25.3%) 15. Low salary (1/1.15%)
2. Pressure from promotion (14/16.1%) 16. Low status (1/1.15%)
3. Heavy teaching load (6/6.9%) 17. Overexpectation (1/1.15%)
4. Worry about teaching effectiveness (6/6.9%) 18. Ongoing reforms on teaching (1/1.15%)
5. Little/low self-confidence (5/5.7%) 19. Pressure from colleagues (1/1.15%)
6. Too much and endless work (5/5.7%) 20. Difficulty in balancing teaching and research (1/1.15%)
7. Difficulty in balancing work and family (3/3.4%) Causes for teacher foreign language anxiety
8. Students not working hard (3/3.4%) 1. Spoken English being not good enough (5/5.7%)
9. Not interested in teaching (3/3.4%) 2. Low-confidence in English competence (3/3.4%)
10. Imbalance between efforts and returns (3/3.4%) 3. Not having enough reading and adequate preparation (2/2.3%)
11. Poor health (2/2.3%) 4. Pursuing perfection (1/1.15%)
12. Worry about self-development (2/2.3%) 5. Fear of teaching outcomes (1/1.15%)
13. Not having enough time (2/2.3%)

Note. The numbers in the brackets refer to frequency and percentage respectively.

Table 5. Means and Standard Deviations of TAS and TFLAS confided that they did not feel anxious about teaching and/or
Scales for the Whole Sample (N = 151). teaching English.
Scales M SD Skewness Kurtosis
Anxiety levels in relation to individual factors. Results of one-
TAS1 2.48 0.43 0.732 0.842 way ANOVA (Duncan) conducted on TAS and TFLAS scales
TAS2 2.30 0.50 1.337 4.24 in relation to age, professional title, educational level, and
TAS3 3.90 0.70 −0.499 0.271 independent-samples t-tests in relation to experience of visit-
TAS4 3.51 0.68 −0.601 0.557 ing/studying in English-speaking countries are presented in
TAS5 2.42 0.69 1.103 1.671 Tables 6 to 9.
TAS6 3.32 0.93 −0.011 −0.769
As seen from Table 6, the participants’ TAS and TFLAS
TAS 2.79 0.39 0.499 1.780
scores of different age groups embodied a similar pattern to
TFLAS1 2.48 0.61 0.447 −0.195
those of the whole sample. Meanwhile, the respondents
TFLAS2 2.697 0.76 0.374 −0.198
aged 26 to 30 scored the highest on TAS1, TAS3, TAS4,
TFLAS3 2.79 0.54 0.832 1.487
TFLAS 2.61 0.57 0.571 0.317
TAS6, and TAS; those aged 31 to 40 scored the lowest on
TAS2 and TAS5; those aged 41 to 50 scored the lowest on
Note. TAS = Teaching Anxiety Scale; TFLAS = Teacher Foreign Language TAS1 and the highest on TAS2; those aged 51 to 60 scored
Anxiety Scale. the lowest on TAS4 and TAS6 and the highest on TAS5; and
the participants in higher age groups scored higher on
TFLAS scales than those in lower age groups. Statistically
Anxiety Levels in Relation to Individual Factors significant differences were observed in TAS1 between age
General anxiety levels for the whole sample. The means and group 26 to 30 and age groups 31 to 40 and 41 to 50, in
standard deviations of TFLAS and TAS scales for the whole TAS2 between age group 31 to 40 and age groups 26 to 30
sample were reported in Table 5, which shows that the and 41 to 50, in TAS3 between age group 51 to 60 and the
participants scored above 3 on TAS3 (M = 3.90), TAS4 other three groups, in TAS4 between age group 26 to 30 and
(M = 3.51), and TAS6 (3.32), but below 3 on the other TAS age group 51 to 60, in TAS6 between age group 51 to 60 and
scales. This indicated that the respondents generally did not age groups 26 to 30 and 41 to 50, and in TAS between age
feel worried about classroom teaching (TAS1). Meanwhile, group 26 to 30 and the other three groups. Alternatively, the
they reported to be confident (TAS2) and interested in teach- participants aged 31 to 40 were significantly more worried
ing (TAS5), (fairly) worried about research (TAS3), other about classroom teaching than their peers aged 41 to 50,
work (TAS4), and promotion (TAS6). Likewise, the partici- those aged 31 to 40 were significantly more confident in
pants scored below 3 on all TFLAS scores (M = 2.48–2.79), teaching than their peers aged 26 to 30 and 41 to 50, those
meaning that they were generally not anxious about their aged 51 to 60 were significantly less concerned about
English proficiency. On the contrary, they seemed to be con- research than their peers in other age groups, those aged 26
fident in speaking English, not fearful of failure, and confi- to 30 were significantly more worried about other work
dent in their English proficiency. than their peers aged 51 to 60, those aged 51 to 60 were
By contrast, when responding to the open-ended question, significantly less worried about promotion than their peers
56 (64.37%) respondents reported that they were often or aged 26 to 30 and 41 to 50, and those aged 26 to 30 were
sometimes anxious about teaching, and 35 (40.23%) reported significantly more anxious about teaching in general than
feeling anxious about teaching English; only 17 (11.3%) their peers in other age groups.
8 SAGE Open

Table 6. ANOVA Results (age group).

G1 (N = 7) G2 (N = 71) G3 (N = 55) G4 (N = 18)


Sum of Places of significance
Scales M SD M SD M SD M SD squares F p (p = .05)
TAS1 2.79 0.43 2.48 0.38 2.40 0.38 2.60 0.67 1.289 2.398 .070 G1&G2-3
TAS2 2.63 0.38 2.26 0.43 2.68 0.47 2.45 0.78 1.391 1.878 .136 G2&G1,G3
TAS3 4.14 0.64 3.94 0.67 3.98 0.61 3.40 0.94 5.366 3.803 .012 G4&G1-3
TAS4 3.82 0.49 3.45 0.597 3.63 0.695 3.24 0.87 3.034 2.271 .083 G1&G4
TAS5 2.52 0.66 2.36 0.66 2.38 0.55 2.78 1.09 2.77 1.956 .123 —
TAS6 3.71 0.86 3.30 0.92 3.48 0.84 2.72 1.05 8.981 3.659 .014 G4&G1,G3
TAS 3.08 0.25 2.77 0.34 2.78 0.34 2.77 0.64 0.631 1.410 .242 G1&G2-4
TFLAS1 2.62 0.39 2.45 0.56 2.46 0.63 2.63 0.80 0.611 0.538 .657 —
TFLAS2 2.97 0.55 2.72 0.66 2.62 0.79 2.73 1.08 0.934 0.534 .660 —
TFLAS3 2.89 0.56 2.76 0.47 2.76 0.57 2.88 0.73 0.313 0.353 .787 —
TFLAS 2.78 0.397 2.59 0.497 2.57 0.59 2.71 0.83 0.489 0.492 .688 —

Note. G1 = age 26–30; G2 = age 31–40; G3 = age 41–50; G4 = age 51–60. ANOVA = analysis of variance; TAS = Teaching Anxiety Scale; TFLAS =
Teacher Foreign Language Anxiety Scale.

Table 7. ANOVA Results (Professional Title).

AL (N = 6) L (N = 104) AP (N = 39) P (N = 2)
Sum of Places of significance
Scales M SD M SD M SD M SD squares F p (p = .05)
TAS1 2.65 0.49 2.46 0.38 2.49 0.53 2.77 0.33 0.376 0.676 .568 —
TAS2 2.43 0.46 2.27 0.45 2.34 0.64 2.43 0.61 0.377 0.496 .686 —
TAS3 4.00 0.57 3.93 0.72 3.82 0.70 3.63 0.88 0.568 0.376 .770 —
TAS4 3.67 0.70 3.46 0.66 3.62 0.71 3.13 0.18 1.174 0.854 .466 —
TAS5 2.22 0.54 2.43 0.65 2.44 0.84 2.33 0.47 0.269 0.183 .908 —
TAS6 3.25 1.04 3.45 0.87 3.00 1.01 3.00 0.71 5.908 2.347 .075 —
TAS 2.90 0.30 2.78 0.35 2.78 0.49 2.82 0.47 0.084 0.184 .907 —
TFLAS1 2.48 0.42 2.47 0.58 2.51 0.72 2.56 0.63 0.049 0.042 .988 —
TFLAS2 2.70 0.62 2.69 0.71 2.72 0.93 2.70 0.42 0.025 0.014 .998 —
TFLAS3 2.75 0.63 2.73 0.48 2.88 0.67 3.13 0.53 0.898 1.028 .382 —
TFLAS 2.60 0.48 2.59 0.52 2.65 0.72 2.72 0.55 0.129 0.129 .943 —

Note. ANOVA = analysis of variance; AL = assistant lecturer; L = lecturer; AP = associate professor; P = professor; TAS = Teaching Anxiety Scale;
TFLAS = Teacher Foreign Language Anxiety Scale.

Table 7 shows that TAS and TFLAS scores of respondents difference occurred only in TAS3 between PhD respondents
with differing professional titles demonstrated a similar pat- and the other two groups, indicating that PhD respondents
tern to that of the whole sample. In addition, respondents were significantly less worried about research than their
with higher professional titles tended to score higher on peers with BA and MA.
TFLAS scales, but no significant difference was observed. Meanwhile, Table 8 shows that BA participants scored the
Assistant lecturers scored the highest on TAS2, TAS3, TAS4, highest on all TFLAS scales, MA participants scored the
and TAS but the lowest on TAS5; lecturers scored the lowest lowest on TFLAS2, and PhD participants scored the lowest
on TAS1, TAS2, and TAS but the highest on TAS6; associate on TFLAS1, TFLAS2, and TFLAS. Statistically significant
professors scored the highest on TAS5 but the lowest on differences occurred in TFLAS1 and TFLAS between BA
TAS; and the professors scored the highest on TAS1 but the participants and the other two groups, and in TFLAS2
lowest on TAS4 and TAS6. Nevertheless, no statistically sig- between BA and PhD participants. This finding meant that
nificant difference was identified. BA respondents were significantly more apprehensive of
Table 8 demonstrates a similar score pattern to that of the speaking English and anxious about their English than their
whole sample for respondents with different educational MA and PhD, and were significantly more fearful of nega-
levels. BA respondents scored the highest on TAS1, TAS2, tive outcomes than their PhD peers.
TAS4, TAS5, and TAS; MA respondents scored the highest Table 9 demonstrates a similar score pattern to that of
on TAS3 and TAS6; and PhD respondents scored the lowest the whole sample for respondents with differing experi-
on all TAS scales. Nevertheless, statistically significant ences of visiting or studying in English-speaking countries.
Liu and Wu 9

Table 8. ANOVA Results (Educational Level).

BA (N = 25) MA (N = 114) PhD (N = 12)


Sum of Places of significance
Scales M SD M SD M SD squares F p (p = .05)
TAS1 2.55 0.61 2.48 0.39 2.40 0.36 0.219 0.592 .554 —
TAS2 2.38 0.69 2.29 0.46 2.19 0.46 0.319 0.632 .533 —
TAS3 3.90 0.71 3.95 0.69 3.46 0.73 2.62 2.697 .071 PhD & BA, MA
TAS4 3.60 0.63 3.50 0.69 3.35 0.64 0.499 0.543 .582 —
TAS5 2.60 0.995 2.39 0.63 2.33 0.49 0.990 1.03 .360 —
TAS6 3.02 1.08 3.42 0.89 3.00 0.798 4.543 2.696 .071 —
TAS 2.84 0.58 2.79 0.34 2.63 0.33 0.371 1.237 .293 —
TFLAS1 2.80 0.66 2.43 0.595 2.34 0.48 3.071 4.273 .016 BA & MA, PhD
TFLAS2 2.99 0.99 2.67 0.70 2.30 0.49 4.129 3.704 .027 BA & PhD
TFLAS3 2.98 0.74 2.73 0.49 2.79 0.47 1.259 2.195 .115 —
TFLAS 2.89 0.73 2.56 0.53 2.43 0.44 2.626 4.176 .017 BA & MA, BA & PhD

Note. ANOVA = analysis of variance; BA = respondents with bachelor’s degree; MA= respondents with master’s degree; PhD = respondents with PhD
degree; TAS = Teaching Anxiety Scale; TFLAS = Teacher Foreign Language Anxiety Scale.

Table 9. Independent-Samples t-test Results (Experience of Visiting/Studying in English-Speaking Countries).

NV (N = 88) V (N = 63) NS (N = 104) S (N = 47)


Cohen’s
Scales M SD M SD t p d M SD M SD t p
TFLAS1 2.59 0.60 2.33 0.598 2.68** .008 0.43 2.49 0.64 2.47 0.55 0.19 .954
TFLAS2 2.79 0.76 2.56 0.74 1.86 .065 — 2.73 0.796 2.62 0.67 0.87 .388
TFLAS3 2.84 0.58 2.69 0.47 1.70 .091 — 2.80 0.57 2.73 0.47 0.76 .451
TFLAS 2.70 0.58 2.47 0.54 2.48* .014 0.41 2.63 0.61 2.57 0.49 0.58 .567
TAS1 2.58 0.43 2.35 0.397 3.30** .001 0.56 2.51 0.45 2.42 0.39 1.26 .211
TAS2 2.37 0.54 2.21 0.42 1.98* .05 0.33 2.30 0.52 2.29 0.47 .099 .921
TAS3 3.92 0.79 3.87 0.57 0.43 .667 — 3.95 0.75 3.79 0.58 1.35 .178
TAS4 3.48 0.68 3.54 0.68 −0.51 .613 — 3.49 0.70 3.55 0.62 −0.50 .616
TAS5 2.48 0.73 2.34 0.63 1.17 .245 — 2.45 0.75 2.35 0.56 0.797 .427
TAS6 3.32 0.94 3.32 0.93 0.01 .996 — 3.30 0.93 3.35 0.94 −0.29 .769
TAS 2.84 0.42 2.71 0.33 2.13* .03 0.34 2.80 0.41 2.74 0.33 0.85 .397

Note. Effect size of Cohen’s d—small: d ≤ 0.2, medium: d = 0.5, and large: d ≥ 0.8 (Cohen, 1988). NV = never visited; V = visited; NS = never studied;
S = studied; TFLAS = Teacher Foreign Language Anxiety Scale; TAS = Teaching Anxiety Scale.
*p ≤ .05. **p ≤ .01.

Respondents who had never visited English-speaking coun- Effects of Anxiety


tries scored higher on all TFLAS scales and TAS scales
except for TAS6 than those who had done so. Statistically Of 82 survey respondents who explained how anxiety
significant differences were identified in TFLAS1, TFLAS, affected them, four said that anxiety had no effect on them,
TAS1, TAS2, and TAS (d = 0.34–0.56 with a medium effect two reported that anxiety was two-sided, and the others
size). This suggested that respondents who had never visited mainly described the negative effects of anxiety. The results
English-speaking countries were significantly more appre- are reported in Table 10, which reveals 24 negative effects
hensive of speaking English, more anxious about their and three positive effects. The greatest negative effects were
English proficiency, more worried about classroom teach- bad sleep (e.g., insomnia, inadequate sleep, poor sleeping
ing, less confident in teaching, and more anxious about quality; 21/26.9%), depression (e.g., being upset/dismayed,
teaching in general. in a low mood; 11/14.1%), lowered devotion to teaching
Table 9 also shows that participants who had never stud- (e.g., being tired, being unable to be devoted to or concen-
ied in English-speaking countries scored higher on all trate on teaching; 8/10.3%), sub-health (e.g., lowered body
TFLAS scales as well as TAS scales except for TAS4 and immunity/resistance to viruses; 5/6.4%), lowered motivation
TAS6. Nevertheless, no statistically significant differences to work (e.g., not being motivated, feeling burnt out; 5/6.4%),
occurred in any scale. becoming nervous (3/3.8%), being bad-tempered (e.g., being
10 SAGE Open

Table 10. Effects of Anxiety (N = 78).

Negative effects 17. Worry about losing the teaching job (1/1.3%)
1. Bad sleep (21/26.9%) 18. Worry about own teaching effectiveness (1/1.3%)
2. Depression (11/14.1%) 19. Wish to retire (1/1.3%)
3. Lowered devotion to teaching (8/10.3%) 20. Difficulty in balancing teaching and research (1/1.3%)
4. Sub-health (5/6.4%) 21. Disrupting the current rhythm (1/1.3%)
5. Lowered motivation to work (5/6.4%) 22. Taking too much time (1/1.3%)
6. Becoming nervous (3/3.8%) 23. Imbalance between efforts and returns (1/1.3%)
7. Bad-tempered (3/3.8%) 24. A strong sense of frustration (1/1.3%)
8. Being unable to focus on something (3/3.8%) 25. Doubting own teaching ability (1/1.3%)
9. Losing pride in teaching profession (2/2.6%) Positive effects
10. Becoming unhappy (2/2.6%) 1. Working harder to better oneself (2/2.6%)
11. Stuck in a vicious circle of anxiety (2/2.6%) 2. Being a motivator (1/1.3%)
12. Having gray hair young (1/1.3%) 3. Urging one to plan time more reasonably and
13. Losing enthusiasm in teaching (1/1.3%) prepare adequately in advance (1/1.3%)
14. Losing confidence in the profession (1/1.3%)
15. Becoming sick (1/1.3%)
16. Procrastination (1/1.3%)

Note. The numbers in the brackets refer to frequency and percentage respectively.

easy to lose temper, feeling upset and irritated; 3/3.8%), and research (e.g., projects and number of publications) and
being unable to focus on something (3/3.8%). As confided by other administrative requirements to win chances for promo-
the respondents, anxiety led to a series of bad consequences, tion. This added to their pressure and anxiety. Furthermore,
such as insomnia; poor sleeping quality; feelings of depres- lack of training and support, low salary, and low self-confi-
sion, frustration, and unhappiness; lowered devotion; moti- dence in teaching drove some participants anxious, as hap-
vation and enthusiasm; and sub-health. For example, due to pened in Parsons (1973) and Liu and Yan (2020).
anxiety, “I often can’t sleep well” (No. 42), “I feel tired and Meanwhile, the present study showed that major sources
can’t concentrate on teaching” (No. 90), and “I am always in for teacher FLA were apprehension of speaking English, fear
a bad mood, and become easy to lose my temper” (No. 100). of negative outcomes, and confidence in English compe-
Moreover, anxiety could also result in gray hair, procrastina- tence, as discussed in Horwitz (2008). Similar to their coun-
tion, sickness, lowered confidence, and so on, which defi- terparts in Bekleyen (2009), Machida (2015), Yoon (2012),
nitely affected the respondents’ work and life. and Merç (2011), the participants feared making mistakes
Meanwhile, Table 10 shows that anxiety could be a moti- and failing to understand others, were anxious about speak-
vator to a few respondents, pushing them to work harder to ing English, and worried about their English competence and
better themselves. teaching effectiveness. Although EFL learners had much
more access and exposure to English than before nowadays
Discussion and Conclusion in China, they still did not have much chance to use English
daily yet, which might even lower their ability to use English,
Sources of Teaching Anxiety and Teacher FLA especially speaking abilities. Hence, they understandably
worried about their English competence and English teach-
The present study revealed that major causes for teaching
ing effectiveness.
anxiety were concern about classroom teaching, research,
other work and promotion, and interest and confidence in
teaching. Similar to their counterparts in other contexts
Anxiety Levels in Relation to Individual Factors
(Ahmed & Julius, 2015; Desouky & Allam, 2017; Liu &
Yan, 2020; Parsons, 1973; Robinson & Clay, 2005; Yazici & Similar to the current literature (Ahmed & Julius, 2015;
Altun, 2013), the participants in the present study experi- Desouky & Allam, 2017; Liu & Yan, 2020; Parsons, 1973;
enced (great) anxiety due to applying for projects and funds, Shao, 2009; X. Wang & Wang, 2015; Yazici & Altun, 2013),
writing up papers, getting papers published, and getting pro- the participants of various backgrounds (e.g., gender, age,
moted. Meanwhile, they had to face heavy teaching load and educational level, professional title) in the present study also
worry about the effectiveness of their teaching and self- suffered from varying degrees of teaching anxiety. Although
development. This might be largely attributed to the fact that the survey data showed that the respondents generally did
as college English teachers, they had to teach generally more not feel worried about teaching and were confident and inter-
than 12 hr per week to meet the needs of almost all students ested in teaching, more respondents confided to be anxious
of a university, because English courses were compulsory about teaching, indicating that actually more teachers experi-
in all universities in China. Meanwhile, they had to meet enced anxiety about teaching. This was probably related to
Liu and Wu 11

the pressure from research and promotion, as previously on student FLA (Dewaele & Alfawzan, 2018; Horwitz
discussed. et al., 1986; Liu & Xiangming, 2019). Administrators and
Statistical analyses also revealed that gender, age, educa- policy makers need to implement policies for assessment and
tional level, English proficiency, and experience of visiting/ promotion appropriate to the local context so that English
studying in English-speaking countries significantly affected teachers may feel (more) secure and less anxious. Equally
the participants’ teaching anxiety and teacher FLA levels, as importantly, it is useful to establish a conducive rapport
found in the current literature (Cheung & Hui, 2011; İpek, among colleagues, which may influence their perceptions
2016; Kim & Kim, 2004; Kralova & Tirpakova, 2019; Liu & and conduction of teaching in the classroom.
Yan, 2020; Machida, 2015; Merç, 2011; L. Wang & Zhang, Although the present study revealed several enlightening
2011; X. Wang & Wang, 2015; Yazici & Altun, 2013). These findings, some limitations existed in the study. The biggest
findings might be because generally the older a teacher is, limitation was that the participants were all FL teachers and
the more experienced he or she is; the better educated a predominantly female. The results would have been more
teacher is, the more competent in research and English he or generalizable if teachers of other subjects with different gen-
she is; the higher professional title a teacher has, the less der ratios had been recruited, which will be the focus of
worried he or she is about research and promotion. Moreover, future research. In addition, a better picture would have been
male teachers generally tend to focus more on work but less revealed if the quantitative data had been complemented by
on housework and family life, which releases them from more qualitative data like interviews. For a more in-depth
much pressure, as found in Liu and Yan (2020). And the understanding of teaching anxiety and FLA in China at a cer-
experience of visiting/studying in English-speaking coun- tain educational level, triangulated data need to be collected.
tries undoubtedly helped improve proficiency in English and All these justify continuous research on the issues.
reduce English-speaking anxiety, as discussed in DeKeyser
(2007). Acknowledgments
We are grateful to all the participants, without whom the present
Effects of Anxiety paper would have been impossible.

Although some respondents reported that anxiety could be a Declaration of Conflicting Interests
motivator and urged them to do better, more respondents
reported it to be a debilitator, as found in studies on SL/FL The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect
to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
learners’ FLA (Horwitz et al., 1986; Liu & Xiangming,
2019; Tobias, 1979; Tum, 2014). As confided by the respon-
dents, anxiety led to a series of bad consequences, the worst Funding
of which were bad sleep, depression, lowered devotion to The author(s) received no financial support for the research and/or
teaching, lowered motivation to work, becoming nervous, authorship of this article.
bad-temper, inability to focus on something, gray hair, pro-
crastination, sickness, and lowered confidence, as found in Ethical Statement
similar studies (Cheung & Hui, 2011; Liu & Yan, 2020; We state that this research is approved by the Research Ethics
Robinson & Clay, 2005; Yazici & Altun, 2013). All these Committee of Tsinghua University.
definitely affected the respondents’ work and life.
As discussed above, college English teachers in China ORCID iD
currently face teaching anxiety and FLA due to differing rea-
Meihua Liu https://orcid.org/0000-0002-5951-4167
sons such as workload, and worry about research and promo-
tion. To alleviate the anxiety, training, raising self-awareness,
preparation, and counseling are all helpful, as discussed in References
Liu and Yan (2020), and Peker (2009). It is important for col- Ahmed, Z., & Julius, S. H. (2015). The relationship between depres-
lege English teachers to be clearly aware of what is required sion, anxiety and stress among women college students. Indian
and expected of them in teaching and research and take Journal of Health and Wellbeing, 6(12), 1232–1234.
actions correspondingly. For example, they can improve Bekleyen, N. (2009). Helping teachers become better English
their research ability by taking programs/courses and attend- students: Causes, effects, and coping strategies for foreign
language listening anxiety. System, 37, 664–675. https://doi.
ing lectures and conferences. They can also regularly reflect
org/10.1016/j.system.2009.09.010
on their work to get (more) prepared and plan their time care- Cheung, H. Y., & Hui, S. K. F. (2011). Teaching anxiety amongst
fully and to achieve balance between family and work, and Hong Kong and Shanghai in-service teachers: The impact
between teaching and research. Meanwhile, universities and of trait anxiety and self-esteem. The Asia-Pacific Education
colleges should provide appropriate resources and support Researcher, 20(2), 395–409. https://scholars.cityu.edu.hk/en
for English teachers to increase their confidence in the lan- /publications/publication(c606d5d5-6392-453b-a672-
guage and in teaching the language, as evidenced in studies 3cc2273f4dba).html
12 SAGE Open

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sci- Liu, M., & Yan, Y. (2020). Anxiety and stress in in-service Chinese
ences (2nd ed.). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. university teachers of arts. International Journal of Higher
DeKeyser, R. (2007). Study abroad as foreign language prac- Education, 9(1), 237–248. http://ijhe.sciedupress.com
tice. In R. DeKeyser (Ed.), Practicing in a second language: Machida, T. (2015). Japanese elementary school teachers and
Perspectives from applied linguistics and cognitive psychology English language anxiety. TESOL Journal, 7(1), 40–66. https://
(pp. 208–226). Cambridge University Press. doi.org/10.1002/tesj.189
Desouky, D., & Allam, H. (2017). Occupational stress, anxiety and Merç, A. (2011). Sources of foreign language student teacher
depression among Egyptian teachers. Journal of Epidemiology anxiety: A qualitative inquiry. Turkish Online Journal of
and Global Health, 7, 191–198. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Qualitative Inquiry, 2(4), 80–94. https://eric.ed.gov/?id=ED
jegh.2017.06.002 537806
Dewaele, J. M., & Alfawzan, M. (2018). Does the effect of enjoy- Parsons, J. S. (1973, February–March). Assessment of anxiety
ment outweigh that of anxiety in foreign language perfor- about teaching using the Teaching Anxiety Scale: Manual
mance? Studies in Second Language Learning and Teaching, and research report. The Annual Meeting of American
8(1), 21–45. https://doi.org/10.14746/ssllt.2018.8.1.2 Educational Research Association. Louisiana. https://eric.
Fimian, M. J., & Blanton, L. P. (1987). Stress, burnout, and role ed.gov/?id=ED079330
problems among teacher trainees and first-year teachers. Peker, M. (2009). The use of expanded microteaching for reduc-
Journal of Occupational Behavior, 8, 157–165. https://www. ing preservice teachers’ teaching anxiety about mathematics.
jstor.org/stable/3000368 Scientific Research and Essay, 4(9), 872–880. http://www.
Gardner, L. E., & Leak, G. E. (1994). Characteristics and corre- academicjournals.org/SRE
lates of teaching anxiety among college psychology teachers. Peker, M., & Ertekin, E. (2011). The relationship between math-
Teaching of Psychology, 21(1), 28–32. https://doi.org/10.1207/ ematics teaching anxiety and mathematics anxiety. The New
s15328023top2101_5 Educational Review, 23(1), 213–226.
Horwitz, E. K. (1996). Even teachers get the blues: Recognizing Richards, L. (2009). Handling qualitative data. SAGE.
and alleviating language teachers’ feelings of foreign language Robinson, J. H., & Clay, D. L. (2005). Potential school violence:
anxiety. Foreign Language Annals, 29(3), 365–372. https:// Relationship between teacher anxiety and warning-sign iden-
doi.org/10.1111/j.1944-9720.1996.tb01248.x tification. Psychology in the Schools, 42(6), 623–635. https://
Horwitz, E. K. (2008). Becoming a language teacher: A practical doi.org/10.1002/pits.20100
guide to second language learning and teaching (2nd ed.). Shao, H. (2009). A study of anxiety in and coping strategies used
Pearson. by university teachers. China Adult Education, 6, 78–79.
Horwitz, E. K. (2010). Foreign and second language anxiety. Spielberger, C. D. (1966). Theory and research on anxiety. In C. D.
Language Teaching, 43(2), 154–167. https://doi.org/10.1017/ Spielberger (Ed.), Anxiety and behavior (pp. 3–20). Academic
S026144480999036X Press.
Horwitz, E. K., Horwitz, M., & Cope, J. (1986). Foreign language Spielberger, C. D. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety
classroom anxiety. The Modern Language Journal, 70,
Inventory (Form Y). Consulting Psychologists Press.
125–132. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4781.1986.tb05256.x
Tobias, S. (1979). Anxiety research in educational psychology.
İpek, H. (2016). A qualitative study on foreign language teaching
Journal of Educational Psychology, 71(5), 573–582. https://
anxiety. Journal of Qualitative Research in Education, 4(3),
doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.71
92–105. https://doi.org/10.14689/issn.2148-2624.1.4c3s5m
Tum, D. O. (2014). Foreign language anxiety’s forgotten study:
Keavney, G., & Sinclair, K. E. (1978). Teacher concerns and
The case of the anxious preservice teacher. TESOL Quarterly,
teacher anxiety: A neglected topic of classroom research.
49(4), 627–658. https://doi.org/10.1002/tesq.190
Review of Educational Research, 48(2), 273–290. https://doi.
Wang, L., & Zhang, D. (2011). A study of state anxiety in elemen-
org/10.3102/00346543048002273
tary and middle school teachers. China Education Journal, 3,
Kim, S., & Kim, J. (2004). When the learner becomes a teacher:
19–22.
Foreign language anxiety as an occupational hazard. English
Teaching, 59(1), 165–185. http://journal.kate.or.kr/wp-con- Wang, X., & Wang, Y. (2015). Analysis on the state anxiety of
tent/uploads/2015/02/kate_59_1_9.pdf college English teachers during transitional period of col-
Kralova, Z., Skorvagova, E., Tirpakova, A., & Markechova, D. lege English teaching reform and coping strategies. Foreign
(2017). Reducing student teachers’ foreign language pronun- Language Learning Theory and Practice, 2, 31–28. http://
ciation anxiety through psycho-social training. System, 65, www.cnki.com.cn/Article/CJFDTOTAL-GWJX201502006.
49–60. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.system.2017.01.001 htm
Kralova, Z., & Tirpakova, A. (2019). Nonnative EFL teachers’ Yazici, H., & Altun, F. (2013). Conditions of beginning teach-
speaking anxiety: Post-communist country context. SAGE ing and anxiety perceived in teacher candidates. Procedia—
Open, 9(2), 1–13. https://doi.org/10.1177/2158244019846698 Social and Behavioral Sciences, 93, 974–977. https://doi.
Kunt, N., & Tüm, D. Ö. (2010). Non-native student teachers’ org/10.1016/j.sbspro.2013.09.313
feelings of foreign language anxiety. Procedia—Social and Yoon, T. (2012). Teaching English through English: Exploring
Behavioral Sciences, 2(2), 4672–4676. https://doi.org/10.1016 anxiety in non-native pre-service ESL teachers. Theory and
/j.sbspro.2010.03.748 Practice in Language Studies, 2(6), 1099–1107. https://doi.
Liu, M., & Xiangming, L. (2019). Changes in and effects of anxi- org/10.4304/tpls.2.6.1099-1107
ety on English test performance in Chinese postgraduate EFL Zhang, X. (2010). A study of anxiety in college English teachers.
classrooms. Education Research International, vol. 2019, Education and Career, 24, 175–176. https://doi.org/10.13615/j.
Article ID 7213925. https://doi.org/10.1155/2019/7213925 cnki.1004-3985.2010.24.020

You might also like