#instabod

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 13

Psychology of Popular Media

#Instabod Versus #BoPo: An Experimental Study of the Effects of Viewing


Idealized Versus Body-Positive Content on Collegiate Males’ and Females’
Body Satisfaction
Mary Pritchard and Anna Button
Online First Publication, April 27, 2023. https://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ppm0000454

CITATION
Pritchard, M., & Button, A. (2023, April 27). #Instabod Versus #BoPo: An Experimental Study of the Effects of Viewing
Idealized Versus Body-Positive Content on Collegiate Males’ and Females’ Body Satisfaction. Psychology of Popular Media.
Advance online publication. https://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ppm0000454
Psychology of Popular Media
© 2023 American Psychological Association
ISSN: 2689-6567 https://doi.org/10.1037/ppm0000454

#Instabod Versus #BoPo: An Experimental Study of the Effects of Viewing


Idealized Versus Body-Positive Content on Collegiate Males’ and Females’
Body Satisfaction
Mary Pritchard and Anna Button
Department of Psychological Science, Boise State University

Multiple studies have reported the harmful effects of appearance-related social media content on women’s
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

body image; however, few studies have examined gender differences in the impact of Instagram images on
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

body satisfaction and body appreciation. In addition, no studies have examined the influence of body-
positive images on males’ body image and body appreciation. Thus, the purpose of the present study was
to examine the influence of three different types of Instagram posts (idealized, body-positive, and nature
images) on males’ and females’ body satisfaction and appreciation. Three hundred seventy-one emerging
adults (18–29 years old) viewed one of three sets of Instagram posts based on gender and completed
body image measures before and after viewing those images. While the experimental condition only
impacted overall posttest body satisfaction in females, when the salience of the images was highlighted
by asking about how viewing these Instagram posts made them feel about specific body parts, both
males’ and females’ feelings about their bodies seemed to be negatively affected by idealized Instagram
images and positively affected by viewing body-positive posts. However, while females’ body image
may benefit more from viewing body-positive images than idealized or nature images, males seem to benefit
from viewing nonidealized images (either body-positive or nature images).

Public Policy Relevance Statement


An experiment examined the influence of Instagram posts (i.e., idealized, body-positive, nature images)
on males’ and females’ body image. Our results suggest that males and females are affected by idealized
(thin ideal, muscular ideal) posts on Instagram, especially when the images are made more salient by
asking participants about their satisfaction with their specific body parts. However, while females’
body image may benefit more from viewing body-positive images than idealized or nature images,
males seem to benefit from viewing nonidealized images (either body-positive or nature images).

Keywords: body dissatisfaction, gender, Instagram, idealized images, body positivity

Today, many individuals spend more time viewing appearance- 2016; Bair et al., 2012; Cramblitt & Pritchard, 2013; Stein et al.,
focused media online than appearance-focused images in traditional 2019).
media, such as magazines (Bair et al., 2012). Stein et al. (2019) sug- With over one billion monthly active users, over 70% of those
gested that social media has replaced traditional media as the primary under the age of 34, Instagram is one of the most popular picture-
source in which beauty standards are introduced or maintained. and video-sharing social media networks for college-aged students
However, a growing body of literature has indicated that collegiate (Dixon, 2022). Research on Instagram has become particularly prom-
males and females who internalize unrealistic body, and appearance inent over the past few years as research has shown that the use of
ideals may be more vulnerable to feelings of body dissatisfaction social media that is primarily picture-based is often linked with
after viewing appearance-focused online media (Andrew et al., body dissatisfaction (Cohen et al., 2018; Holland & Tiggemann,
2016; Meier & Gray, 2014), particularly among female college stu-
dents (S. H. Thompson & Lougheed, 2012). Instagram’s portrayal
of the idealized body has been dubbed “Instabod” and has become
Mary Pritchard https://orcid.org/0000-0002-4704-5869 an international body image trend (Chatzopoulou et al., 2020).
Anna Button https://orcid.org/0000-0001-6994-5537 Although body dissatisfaction is a multidimensional construct
We have no known conflicts of interest or funding sources to disclose. that goes beyond weight (Cash & Smolak, 2011), research focuses
Data are available upon request. on two types of body dissatisfaction experienced by collegiate
Mary Pritchard contributed toward conceptualization, formal analysis,
males and females: drive for thinness and drive for muscularity.
writing and supervision. Anna Button contributed toward conceptualization,
methodology, investigation and writing.
Research suggests that females across cultures feel pressured to be
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to slender, commonly referred to as the “drive for thinness” (Girard
Mary E. Pritchard, Department of Psychological Science, Boise State et al., 2018; Johnson et al., 2015). On the other hand, males tend
University, 1910 University Drive, Boise, ID 83725-1715, United States. to have more muscle dissatisfaction and are more likely than females
Email: marypritchard@boisestate.edu to want to lose body fat and increase muscle mass to better define

1
2 PRITCHARD AND BUTTON

their musculature (Leit et al., 2002; McCabe & Ricciardelli, 2004; exposed to thin, athletic, or hyper-muscular images, indicating that
McCreary & Sasse, 2000). Research suggests that up to 90% of body appreciation may only serve as a protective factor for females.
undergraduate males of normal weight think they are underweight This may be because males had higher levels of body appreciation
and want to obtain more muscle mass (Frederick et al., 2007). than females overall. Interestingly, Tiggemann and Anderberg
But, regardless of the type of dissatisfaction, not everyone (2020a) found that body appreciation decreases in females exposed
who views appearance-focused media on social networking sites to idealized images. This may explain why in some cases, even those
experiences body dissatisfaction. The Tripartite Influence Model who report initially having higher body esteem are susceptible to
suggests that one’s perception of the ideal body comes from three decreased body satisfaction after viewing thin appearance-related
primary sources: peers, family, and the media (J. K. Thompson online media. This suggests that having greater body satisfaction
et al., 1999). Although media shapes and influences body ideals and appreciation before viewing appearance-related imagery does
by displaying “ideal” bodies (Grabe et al., 2008), body dissatisfac- not always buffer against negative perceptions about one’s body in
tion develops when an individual internalizes those idealized body females (Mulgrew et al., 2021).
ideals and then compares their appearance to those ideals and
finds themselves lacking (Rodgers et al., 2020; Stein et al., 2019;
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Body Positivity and Instagram


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Van den Berg et al., 2002). Research further suggests that this path-
way of exposure leading to internalization and social comparison Given that some research has suggested that body appreciation may
may be more common when one feels that the “target’ image should help offset the harmful effects of social media, it is of no surprise that
be attainable for them (Knobloch-Westerwick & Romero, 2011; the body positivity movement developed as a way to use social media
Veldhuis et al., 2014), as might be the case with friends in one’s in a beneficial capacity by promoting body appreciation as well as the
peer group (Charoensukmongkol, 2018; Fardouly et al., 2018; normalization of bodies that do not fit the “ideal” (Brathwaite &
Webb et al., 2014). DeAndrea, 2021). Notably, current literature indicates that body-
In addition to perceived attainability (Knobloch-Westerwick & positive content on social media may help buffer or moderate feelings
Romero, 2011), researchers further suggest that there may be gender of negative body image in college-aged females (Cohen, Fardouly,
differences in these pathways of internalization and social compari- et al., 2019; Cohen, Irwin, et al., 2019; Ogden et al., 2020). The
son. For example, the research suggests that viewing an abundance term body positivity is malleable, shaped by social identities, and con-
of photos containing the muscular ideal may contribute to body dis- nected to one’s own perceived body acceptance by others (Tylka &
satisfaction in men (Gültzow et al., 2020). Franzoi et al. (2012) sug- Wood-Barcalow, 2015b). The characteristics of body positivity or
gest that males are less negatively affected by appearance-focused the body-positive image are also multifaceted, including different
posts than females (see also Watson et al., 2019). Franzoi et al. aspects or levels such as body appreciation, body love, inner positiv-
found that female college students were more likely than male col- ity, and body protectiveness (Tylka & Wood-Barcalow, 2015b).
lege students to negatively compare their faces and bodies to same- Specifically, body positivity or body-positive content on social
sex targets (see also Webb et al., 2014). However, males were more media refers to the growing trend of content featuring users or posts
likely than females to believe that “perfection” for their desired face that aim to challenge current thin-ideal beauty standards by encourag-
or body was personally attainable. That is, males are more hopeful ing the acceptance of all body types and features (Cohen, Fardouly,
about achieving their ideal (muscular) looks than females (see also et al., 2019; Cohen, Irwin, et al., 2019).
Tamplin et al., 2018). In contrast, women’s body esteem is more Instagram is one of the most popular picture- and video-sharing
likely to be negatively affected after viewing appearance-related social media networks for body-positive content (Instagram, July
media as women tend to be more self-critical during social compar- 2021). Searching hashtags relating to body positivity, such as #body-
ison (Franzoi et al., 2012). positive, on Instagram generates over 16.3 million body-positive posts
or videos (Instagram, July 2021). Cohen, Irwin, et al. (2019) con-
Body Appreciation ducted a content analysis on body-positive accounts on Instagram
and found that about 95% of body-positive content consisted of a
If comparing oneself to and internalizing the images presented on visual representation while the rest contained text only. These visual
social media posts is linked to body image dissatisfaction in some, representations mainly consisted of images of women with body
are there any factors that mitigate this? Research has suggested sizes ranging from underweight to overweight. In addition, about
two important factors: body appreciation and body positivity. forty percent of posts included visual representations of human figures
Body appreciation is having an intuitive, respectful relationship with a bodily feature not typically deemed ideal, such as cellulite or
with one’s body, focusing on the body’s functionality and ability skin blemishes.
to engage in activities that contribute to one’s overall wellbeing, as Research suggests that the usage of appearance-focused online
opposed to focusing on contentedness with one’s shape and appear- media that pertains to thin-ideal standards has been shown to be cor-
ance, as is the case with body satisfaction. (Sundgot-Borgen et al., related to eating pathology and body dissatisfaction in collegiate
2021). Research has suggested that body appreciation may be a pro- women, especially with those who frequently engage in social com-
tective factor against body dissatisfaction when females are exposed parison and internalize the thin-ideal (Bair et al., 2012; Fardouly
to ideal images (Andrew et al., 2015; Halliwell, 2013). When et al., 2018; Hendrickse et al., 2017). Therefore, it is not surprising
Watson et al. (2019) exposed females to thin ideal images, those that Ogden et al. (2020) suggest that increasing the number of posts
who displayed higher levels of body appreciation displayed lower with more diverse bodies may effectively lower body dissatisfaction
levels of negative social comparison with said thin ideal images. levels and affect how participants perceive current notions of beauty
However, males in their study did not show significant relationships ideals. On the other hand, some researchers criticize the popularized
between body appreciation and negative social comparison when body positivity trend, stating that the expectations for women to
#INSTABOD VERSUS #BOPO 3

love their bodies still promote an unhealthy appearance-focused male’s and female’s responses to (a) gender-based “ideal” images
behavior that ultimately fuels the idea that appearance and looks are (thin for women and muscular for men), (b) body-positive images,
valued above all else (Cohen et al., 2020). In addition, some studies or (c) neutral (i.e., nature) images. We chose to showcase different
promote the idea that viewing images that are nonappearance-related, “ideal” images for males (muscular) and females (thin) as research
such as images of travel, nature, or objects, may even increase body suggests that females across cultures feel pressured to be slender
satisfaction (Cohen et al., 2020; Rounds & Stutts, 2021). Many (Girard et al., 2018; Johnson et al., 2015), whereas males tend to
researchers recommend that more research is needed to better under- want to lose body fat and increase muscle mass to better define
stand the impact of body-positive content on viewers’ body image sat- their musculature (Leit et al., 2002; McCabe & Ricciardelli, 2004;
isfaction, especially in men (Bair et al., 2012; Cohen et al., 2020; McCreary & Sasse, 2000). We decided to examine the impact of
Hendrickse et al., 2017). In addition, no studies have examined how neutral/nature images as research has suggested that viewing images
body-positive content affects men’s body image. The lack of male that are nonappearance-related, such as images of travel, nature, or
representation in body positivity research may create the expectation objects, may increase body satisfaction in female college students
that women are more likely to be positively affected by body-positive (Rounds & Stutts, 2021). Thus, we wanted to compare the influence
content than men, diminishing possible solutions to help men over- of neutral (i.e., nature) images to those specifically designed to
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

come low body satisfaction related to social media use (Frederick improve body image (i.e., body positivity images).
et al., 2017). In sum, we hypothesized that:

1. Collegiate females exposed to idealized images (thin-ideal)


The Present Study would display lower levels of body satisfaction (Franzoi
Research suggests that collegiate men and women may be vulner- et al., 2012) and body appreciation (Tiggemann &
able to feelings of body dissatisfaction after viewing appearance- Anderberg, 2020a) as compared to those viewing body-
focused online media showcasing the “ideal” body (Andrew et al., positive or neutral/nature images. We also expected that
2016; Bair et al., 2012; Cramblitt & Pritchard, 2013; Stein et al., females exposed to body-positive images would display
2019). In particular, the social media site Instagram has increasingly greater levels of body satisfaction than those exposed to
been the focus of attention as the use of picture-based social media is nature images (Rounds & Stutts, 2021).
often linked with body dissatisfaction (Cohen et al., 2018; Holland 2. Males exposed to idealized images (muscular-ideal) would
& Tiggemann, 2016; Meier & Gray, 2014). However, research has also display lower levels of body satisfaction (Gültzow et al.,
suggested that males may be less vulnerable than females to social 2020; Tiggemann & Anderberg, 2020b) compared to those
media posts displaying the “ideal” body type (Franzoi et al., 2012; viewing body-positive or nature images.
Tamplin et al., 2018; Watson et al., 2019). Thus, the first purpose 3. Women would display lower levels of body satisfaction
of this study was to extend previous research by examining (Franzoi et al., 2012) and body appreciation (Watson et al.,
gender differences in exposure to different types of images on 2019) after viewing idealized images compared to males.
Instagram.
Second, research suggests that body appreciation may be a protec- Method
tive factor against the harmful effects of exposure to images on
Participants
Instagram in collegiate females but not collegiate males (Watson
et al., 2019). Thus, the second purpose of this study was to add to Three hundred ninety-three college students completed an omni-
the literature exploring potential gender differences in body apprecia- bus survey about social media’s influence on body image. To keep
tion and its relationship with body image satisfaction after exposure to the results relevant to emerging adults (18–29), 22 participants
the ideal body image on Instagram. Finally, similar to body apprecia- were deleted, leaving us with 371 participants (230 identified as
tion, research suggests that body-positive content on social media may female and 141 as male). Ages of participants ranged from 18 to
help buffer or moderate feelings of negative body image in college- 29 (M = 20.30, SD = 2.66). Most participants identified themselves
aged females (Cohen, Fardouly, et al., 2019; Cohen, Irwin, et al., as White (81.6%), with 7.9% as mixed race, 4.3% as Latinx, 4.1% as
2019; Ogden et al., 2020). However, to our knowledge, no research Asian, 1.1% as American Indian or Alaskan Native, 0.5% as African
has studied how body-positive content affects men’s body image American, and 0.5% as other.
due to the expectation that women are more likely to be positively
affected by body-positive content than men (Frederick et al., 2017). Procedure
Thus, another purpose of this study is to assess how body-positive
content impacts male college students. Once approval was received from the Institutional Review Board,
The present study sought to expand on existing research in two participants were recruited from multiple places. Most participants
ways. First, we wanted to examine the influence of body-positive were recruited via a Psychology 101 subject pool using Sona
images on males’ body image and body appreciation as, to our Systems. Other participants were recruited through a psychology
knowledge, that has not been assessed before. Second, we sought department newsletter sent to students and social media that adver-
to add to the literature examining gender differences in the impact tised a self-report survey on the topic of Instagram and Memory.
of Instagram images on body satisfaction and body appreciation in Participants 18 years and older were eligible to participate. At the
male and female college students. Whereas Watson et al. (2019) beginning of the survey, an explanation of the purpose of the study
exposed participants to one of three different types of “ideal” images and informed consent was presented. However, following the direc-
(thin, athletic, or hyper-muscular), similar to Cohen, Fardouly tion of Cohen, Fardouly, et al. (2019), the study’s true purpose was
et al.’s (2019) study of collegiate females, we sought to compare masked. The study was advertised to participants as being about
4 PRITCHARD AND BUTTON

Instagram and Memory instead of Instagram and Body Positivity male body positivity condition, posts consisted of men of different
to limit biased data. Participants were then asked to report age, gender, sizes and posts of quotes related to male body positivity. The neutral
race, weight, height, and how often they spent time on the Instagram Instagram posts were the same for males and females and consisted
platform. After taking a premeasure of body satisfaction, participants of nature photography, such as landscapes, sunsets, and plants. Each
were randomly assigned to different exposure conditions (ideal, body post’s number of likes was shown to replicate a natural virtual environ-
positivity, and nature images) based on a question asking participants ment. However, comments were not shown in the posts. The posts
to choose the gender they identify with most. Participants viewed 20 could be viewed on a computer or smartphone and were presented
photos of their exposure condition for 7 s each, along with distractor to the viewer for at least 7 s before they were allowed to move on.
memory questions between every five posts (i.e., “In the last five Between every five posts were distractor memory questions to hide
posts, how many images contained the color red?”). The following the real purpose of the study (i.e., “In the last five posts, how many
section included postmeasures of body satisfaction, perceived effects people were wearing hats?”).
of media exposure, body appreciation, and attitudes toward body-
positive accounts. The survey took approximately 30–45 min to com-
plete, and participants were eligible to receive university credit or par-
Measures
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

ticipate in a raffle for one of four $25 gift cards. Participants were then Body Satisfaction
debriefed at the end of the study.
Similar to Cohen, Fardouly, et al.’s (2019) study of women, body
satisfaction was measured both before (pretest) and after (posttest)
Materials the experimental manipulation to understand if the level of satisfac-
tion of appearance and weight would change before and after expo-
Pilot Study sure conditions. Participants were asked three questions about their
Three sets of visual exposure conditions for each gender were body satisfaction: “I am satisfied with my weight,” “I am satisfied
included in the study (male body-positive, male muscular-ideal, with my overall appearance,” and “I am satisfied with my body
nature images, female body-positive, female thin-ideal, nature shape.” These statements were distributed among distractor items
images), with each of the conditions containing 20 posts each. that asked about the satisfaction of personal relationships, financial
Initially, 50 posts for each condition were collected via public status, housing situation, and occupation to help disguise the purpose
Instagram accounts by searching hashtags such as #bodypositive, of the study. Participants were asked to indicate their level of satis-
#malebodypositive, #bodypositivity, and #beauty. A pilot study faction according to each statement by dragging a slider from 0 to
was conducted with 22 participants for the male set of conditions 100, with 0 representing not at all and 100 representing very
(bopo and muscular-ideal posts) and 31 participants to rate the much. Scores were averaged for each item related to body satisfac-
female set of conditions (bopo and thin-ideal posts). Raters were tion. Cohen, Fardouly, et al. (2019) reported the body satisfaction
given a definition of body-positive (“body-positive” content is scale had good to excellent internal consistency at pre- (α = .84)
defined as images or posts that encourage self-acceptance and sup- and postexposure (α = .92) in their sample of collegiate females.
port of various body types, as well as body characteristics not typi- Similarly, in our study, Cronbach’s alphas were calculated separately
cally deemed ideal, such as large stomachs or stretch marks.) and for men and women for both the pretest and posttest version of the
thin-ideal (“idealized images” on Instagram refer to posts or content body satisfaction scale: α = .91 pretest, female; α = .89, pretest,
that contain images of people who fit the thin-ideal, specifically male; α = .94 posttest, female; α = .93, posttest, male, indicating
those with thin and toned bodies, who typically pose in revealing high reliability.
or form-fitting fashion.) before being asked to rate each post to the
extent of how accurately it portrayed the definitions. Perceived Effects of Media Exposure Scale
Ratings were conducted using a visual analog scale (0 = not at all,
100 = to a great extent). These definitions were also adjusted for the The Perceived Effects of Media Exposure Scale (PEMES) was
male set of conditions, with the body-positive definition reading created by Frederick et al. (2017) and was used in this study to mea-
(“body-positive” content is defined as images or posts that encourage sure how participants felt about certain bodily (e.g., weight, thighs,
self-acceptance and support of various body types, as well as body stomach) and facial (e.g., eyes, nose, the shape of the face) features
characteristics not typically deemed ideal, such as large stomachs or after viewing the Instagram posts in their assigned condition. For
absence of muscle mass.). The muscular-ideal definition was: “‘ideal- example, “How did viewing these posts make you feel about your
ized images’ on Instagram refer to posts or content that contain images weight?” Responses were rated on a seven-point Likert-type scale
of people who fit the lean and muscular-ideal, specifically those with to indicate how viewing the posts made them feel about their own
bodies that appear muscular and thin.” The 20 posts with the highest weight (PEMES-weight) or face (PEMES-Face). Responses ranged
ratings in each condition were selected to be included in the study from feeling “much worse” to “much better” about a certain physical
(see Figure 1 for a sample of these images). The posts representing characteristic, and scores were averaged for each subscale. Scores
the thin ideal for the female condition consisted of women who appear above a 4.0 indicated that participants felt better about a certain
to be in tight-fitting clothing, bikinis, or revealing clothing. Posts rep- characteristic, and scores below 4.0 indicated they felt worse. The
resenting female body positivity comprised those who appeared over- Cronbach’s α from the original study of female participants were
weight or with a body not typically deemed ideal and those with stretch .97 (PEMES_Weight) and .93 (PEMES_Face). The αs for the pre-
marks, disabilities, or acne. For the ideal male condition, posts repre- sent study also indicated high reliability (α = .97 PEMES_Weight,
senting the male lean and muscular ideal consisted of men wearing female; α = .95, PEMES_Weight, male; α = .89 PEMES_Face,
clothing that revealed muscular arms, legs, or abdomen. For the female; α = .92, PEMES_Face, male).
#INSTABOD VERSUS #BOPO 5

Figure 1
The Sample Images for Four Different Experimental Conditions Including Female Ideal, Female Body Positivity, Male Ideal, and Male Body
Positivity
Female Ideal Female Body-Positivity Male Ideal Male Body-Positivity
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Note. Each sample image was taken from Instagram and used in the pilot study or survey. These images were reproduced with permission. Female Ideal
image photo provided by Tonia Atieno. Female Body-Positivity photo provided by Dora Lopez. Source: https://www.instagram.com/p/CC67eaMHcxK/?
igshid=YmMyMTA2M2Y=. Male Ideal photo provided by Chris Haase. Source: https://www.instagram.com/p/CDLpHk7hwgt/?igshid=
YmMyMTA2M2Y%3D. Male Body-Positivity photo provided by Panagiotis Papadopoulos. Source: https://www.instagram.com/p/CE85mSjAXR7/?
igshid=YmMyMTA2M2Y%3D. Original study materials are available by request from the corresponding author. See the online article for the color version
of this figure.

Body Appreciation Statistical Analysis Plan


The body appreciation scale (BAS-2) was created by Tylka and Data were entered into the Statistical Package for Social Sciences
Wood-Barcalow (2015a) and used to measure levels of acceptance, (SPSS Version 27). Means and standard deviations were calculated
respect, and positive regard towards one’s own body after viewing for all variables. Analyses focused on scores on the Body
exposure conditions. The scale consisted of 10 items and included Satisfaction survey (posttest) using a 2 (Gender) × 3 (Experimental
statements such as “I take a positive attitude towards my body,” Condition) multivariate analysis of variance (ANOVA), holding pre-
“I am attentive to my body’s needs,” and “I feel love for my test body satisfaction scores constant. Differences in participants’
body.” Participants were asked to indicate whether the statements PEMES (Face, Weight) posttest scores were examined using a
are true about them never, seldom, sometimes, often, or always. 2 (Gender) × 3 (Experimental Condition) multivariate ANOVA.
Scores were rated on a 5-point scale and were averaged, with higher
scores indicating higher levels of body appreciation and lower
Results
scores indicating lower body appreciation. In the original study,
Tylka and Wood-Barcalow (2015a) reported Cronbach’s alphas Preliminary Analyses
of .97 for women and .96 for men. Similarly, in the present
study, α = .96 for female and α = .96 for male, indicating high Participants who did not complete the posttest section of the survey
reliability. were considered incomplete responses and deleted. All remaining par-
ticipants passed survey attention checks and were retained. Our final
Attitudes Toward Bopo Accounts data set included 230 women (75 in the ideal body image condition,
80 in the body positivity condition, and 75 in the nature image condi-
Towards the end of the study, participants were provided a def- tion) and 141 men (50 in the ideal body image condition, 45 in the
inition of body positivity (Body-positive content is defined as body positivity condition, and 46 in the nature image condition).
encouraging the acceptance and support of body types of various Descriptive statistics and correlations between variables are
shapes and sizes, as well as body characteristics not typically displayed in Table 1. To ensure there were no significant differ-
deemed ideal, such as stretch marks or body rolls.). They were ences in demographic characteristics between participants across
asked how often they encountered body positivity content on social the three experimental conditions and gender, we ran a series of
media in their daily lives (responses ranged from always to never on 2 (Gender) × 3 (Experimental Condition) ANOVAs. Men (M =
a 5-point scale) and how likely they would be to follow body- 21.21, SD = 3.02) were slightly older than women (M = 19.73,
positive accounts in the future (responses ranged from very likely SD = 2.24), F(1, 365) = 29.20, p , .001, η2 = .07. There were
to very unlikely on a 5-point scale). These statements were created no age differences between participants assigned to the experi-
by Cohen, Fardouly, et al. (2019) in their Attitudes toward mental conditions, F(2, 365) = 2.71, p = .068; however, there
Body-Positive accounts scale. was an interaction effect of gender and experimental condition,
6 PRITCHARD AND BUTTON

Table 1
Descriptive Statistics and Correlations for Study Variables
Variable M (SD) Female M (SD) Male 1 2 3 4 5
1. BAS 3.24 (0.91) 3.50 (0.93) — .72*** .75*** .32*** .23***
2. BodySatPre 53.65 (25.96) 59.71 (25.38) .65*** — .94*** .31*** .18**
3. BodySatPost 53.93 (27.55) 60.04 (26.61) .72*** .92*** — .36*** .24***
4. PEMES_Weight 3.96 (1.27) 3.99 (0.92) .39*** .36*** .38*** — .71***
5. PEMES_Face 4.04 (0.91) 4.10 (0.85) .35*** .28*** .30*** .75*** —
Note. Values above the diagonal represent correlations in females; values below the diagonal are correlations in males; BAS = Body Appreciation Scale;
BodySatPre = Body Satisfaction Pretest score; BodySatPost = Body Satisfaction Posttest score; PEMES_Weight = score on the Perceived Effects of Media
Exposure Scale weight subscale; PEMES_Face = score on the Perceived Effects of Media Exposure Scale face subscale.
**p , .01. ***p , .001.

F(2, 365) = 3.08, p = .047, η2 = .02. While there were no age premanipulation body satisfaction than males, there was no differ-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

differences for participants in the body positivity condition; men ences in premanipulation body satisfaction between participants in
in the ideal and nature image conditions were slightly older than the three experimental conditions, F(2, 343) = .68, nor was the
the women in those conditions. Thus, we controlled for age in all interaction between gender and experimental condition significant,
of our analyses. There were no significant gender, χ2(6) = 9.30, F(2, 343) = .58.
p = .157, Cramer’s V = .16, p = .157, or experimental condition To examine the influence of gender and our experimental manip-
differences in racial background, χ2(12) = 20.23, p = .063, ulation on postexperimental manipulation body satisfaction scores,
Cramer’s V = .17, p = .063. we ran a 2 (Gender) × 3 (Experimental Condition) ANCOVA, con-
trolling for age and pretest score on the body satisfaction measure.
Body Appreciation There was a significant effect of the experimental condition on
body satisfaction, F(2, 336) = 8.95, p , .001, η2 = .05. Although
We hypothesized that collegiate females exposed to idealized there was no effect of gender by itself, F(1, 336) = .598, p = .440,
images (thin-ideal) would display lower levels of body appreciation η2 = .00, the interaction between gender and experimental condition
than those viewing body-positive or neutral/nature images. We also approached statistical significance and we felt it bore further exam-
hypothesized that women would display lower body appreciation ination, F(2, 336) = 2.77, p = .064, η2 = .02 (see Figure 2). LSD
levels than men. To test these hypotheses, we ran a 2 (Gender) × posthoc tests revealed that females in the thin-ideal condition dis-
3 (Experimental Condition) ANOVA, controlling for age. There played lower levels of posttest body satisfaction than females in
were gender differences in body appreciation, with men (M = the body positivity condition, p = .023, but there were no significant
3.50, SD = .93) scoring higher than women (M = 3.25, SD = .91), differences between the thin-ideal and nature image conditions,
F(1, 360) = 8.78, p = .003, η2 = .02; however, there were no effects p = .631. Although there was a trend for females in the body positiv-
of experimental condition, F(2, 360) = 1.72, p = .180, η2 = .01, or ity condition to score higher on the body satisfaction posttest than
an interaction effect, F(2, 360) = .53, p = .590, η2 = .00, indicating females in the nature condition, this difference did not quite reach
that body appreciation was not affected by our experimental statistical significance, p = .073. None of the conditions differed sig-
manipulation. nificantly in males (see Figure 2).
The second way we assessed body satisfaction was to give partici-
Body Satisfaction pants the PEMES on the posttest. A 2 (Gender) × 3 (Experimental
Condition) multivariate ANCOVA, with age as a covariate,
Body satisfaction was assessed in two ways in our study. First, revealed that there was no effect of age; Wilks’s λ = .996,
participants completed a body satisfaction scale before and after F(2, 359) = .51, η2 = .00, or gender on the overall PEMES; Wilks’s
exposure to one of the three experimental conditions. We had λ = .998, F(2, 359) = .73, η2 = .00. However, there was a significant
hypothesized that collegiate females exposed to idealized images multivariate effect of experimental condition; Wilks’s λ = .706,
(thin-ideal) would display lower levels of body satisfaction than F(4, 722) = 31.36, p , .001, η2 = .16, as well as a two-way interac-
those viewing body-positive or neutral/nature images, with females tion between gender and experimental condition; Wilks’s λ = .928,
exposed to body-positive images displaying greater levels of body F(4, 722) = 6.58, p , .001, η2 = .04. Follow-up univariate tests
satisfaction than those exposed to nature images. We expected that revealed a significant effect of experimental condition on both
males exposed to idealized (muscular-ideal) images would also dis- PEMES_Weight, F(2, 360) = 74.07, p , .001, η2 = .29, as well as
play lower levels of body satisfaction compared to those viewing PEMES_Face, F(2, 360) = 28.50, p , .001, η2 = .14. There was
body-positive or nature images. Finally, we hypothesized that also a significant two-way interaction between gender and experimen-
women would display lower levels of body satisfaction after viewing tal condition on PEMES_Weight, F(2, 360) = 12.70, p , .001,
idealized images compared to males. η2 = .07, as well as PEMES_Face, F(2, 360) = 3.63, p , .05,
To ensure there were no differences in body satisfaction prior η2 = .02. LSD Posthoc tests revealed that, for the PEMES-Weight,
to our experimental manipulation, we first ran a 2 (Gender) × females in the ideal condition scored significantly lower than
3 (Experimental Condition) analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) on those in the body positivity (p , .001) and nature image conditions
pre-experimental manipulation body satisfaction scores, controlling (p , .001). In addition, females in the body positivity condition
for age. Although there was a significant gender effect, F(1, 343) = scored significantly higher than those in the nature image condition
5.25, p , .05, η2 = .02, with females displaying lower levels of (p , .001). However, while males in the ideal condition scored
#INSTABOD VERSUS #BOPO 7

Figure 2
Body Satisfaction by Experimental Condition
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Note. Females in the thin-ideal condition displayed lower levels of posttest body satisfaction than females
in the body positivity condition. There was a trend for females in the body positivity condition to score higher
on the body satisfaction posttest than females in the nature condition. None of the conditions differed sig-
nificantly in males or females. See the online article for the color version of this figure.

significantly lower than those in the body positivity (p , .001) and positivity condition being significantly less likely than those in the
nature image conditions (p , .001), there were no significant differ- nature image condition to view body positivity posts in their
ences in scores on the PEMES-Weight between males in the body everyday lives, F(2, 361) = 4.92, p = .008, η2 = .03. However,
positivity and nature image conditions (p = .298; see Figures 3 there was no significant interaction effect, F(2, 361) = .02,
and 4). For the PEMES-Face, women in the ideal condition scored p = .983, η2 = .00.
significantly lower than those in the body positivity (p , .001) and
nature image conditions (p , .001). However, while there was Discussion
a trend for significant differences in scores on the PEMES-Face
between females in the body positivity and nature image conditions, Research on the impact of Instagram on body dissatisfaction has
it did not quite reach significance (p = .053). Males in the ideal con- increased in recent years as numerous studies have suggested that
dition scored significantly lower on the PEMES-Face than those in the picture-based social media is often linked with body dissatisfaction
body positivity condition (p = .006). Males in the ideal condition also (Cohen et al., 2018; Holland & Tiggemann, 2016; Meier & Gray,
scored lower than those in the nature image condition (p = .028). 2014). However, research has further suggested that males and
However, there were no significant differences between PEMES- females may not be equally affected by images portraying the
Face scores between the body positivity and nature image conditions “ideal” body (Franzoi et al., 2012; Tamplin et al., 2018; Watson
(p = .564; see Figures 3 and 4). et al., 2019) or by potential protective factors like body appreciation
(Watson et al., 2019). Furthermore, while research suggests that
Frequency of Viewing Body Positivity Posts body-positive content on social media may help buffer feelings of
body dissatisfaction in college-aged females (Cohen, Fardouly,
As we were assessing the impact of body positivity posts et al., 2019; Cohen, Irwin, et al., 2019; Ogden et al., 2020), no
on body image, we felt it was important to understand how often research to date has studied how body-positive content affects
our participants saw these types of images in “real life” and men’s body image (Frederick et al., 2017). Thus, the primary aim
whether this differed by gender or experimental condition. Thus, of the present study was to examine the impact of idealized, body-
we ran a 2 (Gender) × 3 (Experimental Condition) ANCOVA, con- positive, and nature images on collegiate male and female students’
trolling for age. There were gender differences in the frequency body image and levels of body appreciation.
of viewing body-positive posts on their own social media Consistent with previous research, we found that collegiate
accounts, with females (M = 3.15, SD = .95) being more likely to females exposed to thin-ideal images displayed lower levels
view body positivity posts than males (M = 2.14, SD = .95), of body satisfaction (Franzoi et al., 2012) as compared to those
F(1, 361) = 100.41, p , .001, η2 = .22. There were also effects viewing body-positive images across all three of our measures of
of experimental condition, with those assigned to the body body satisfaction (body satisfaction posttest, PEMES-Face, and
8 PRITCHARD AND BUTTON

Figure 3
Gender by Experimental Condition Differences in Scores on the PEMES_Weight
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Note. Females in the ideal condition scored significantly lower than those in the body positivity and nature
image conditions. In addition, females in the body positivity condition scored significantly higher than those
in the nature image condition. Males in the ideal condition scored significantly lower than those in the body
positivity and nature image conditions. PEMES_Weight = score on the Perceived Effects of Media Exposure
Scale weight subscale. See the online article for the color version of this figure.

PEMES-Weight). However, the second part of this hypothesis was side-by-side—one photo was idealized and one was real/edited)
only partially supported. While females in the thin-ideal condition instead of body positivity or nature images.
scored significantly lower on the PEMES-Weight and PEMES- Based on previous research, we expected that males exposed to
Face than females in the nature condition, this was not true of the muscular images would display lower levels of body satisfaction
body satisfaction posttest scores. Our hypothesis that females (Gültzow et al., 2020; Tiggemann & Anderberg, 2020b) compared
exposed to body-positive images would display greater levels of to those viewing body-positive or nature images. The impact of
body satisfaction than those exposed to nature images (Rounds & idealized images (muscular-ideal) on body image in male college
Stutts, 2021) was supported by the PEMES-Weight measure but students was inconsistent across our measures of body satisfaction.
not by the PEMES-Face subscale, and there was only a trend for sig- While males in the ideal condition scored significantly lower than
nificance on the body satisfaction posttest score. This finding is those in the body positivity and nature image conditions on the
consistent with Cohen, Fardouly, et al. (2019) as they found no PEMES-Weight and PEMES-Face, there were no differences in
impact of nature images on collegiate females’ body satisfaction males’ body satisfaction posttest scores across experimental con-
in their study. Frederick et al. (2017) also noted that the ditions. Recent research indicates that the impact of body image
PEMES-Weight exhibited stronger effects in their study than the exposure on one’s body satisfaction may depend upon the level of
PEMES-Face. Because the images in our study, and those in self-discrepancy one holds, such that viewing an “ideal” body
the Frederick et al. (2017) study, featured near full bodies, rather depicted would not necessarily lead to body dissatisfaction if some-
than just focusing on the face, it may make sense that scores on one had low levels of self-discrepancy (Kim, 2021). It may be that
the PEMES-Weight would be more affected than those on the the nature of the questions asked on the PEMES, because it asks
PEMES-Face. In addition, it may be that the measure used to assess about specific body parts, may be more likely to highlight levels
body satisfaction (PEMES vs. Body Satisfaction Scale) may impact of self-discrepancy than more general questions about body satisfac-
the results found. Future studies may wish to use other measures of tion as asked on the Body Satisfaction Scale. Again, future research
body satisfaction to determine when nature images relate to body sat- may wish to replicate this study with different measures of body
isfaction and when they do not. Unlike previous research satisfaction to help determine this. Further, Chatzopoulou et al.
(Tiggemann & Anderberg, 2020a), we found no effect of experi- (2020) found that males with low body esteem were much more neg-
mental condition on body appreciation in collegiate females. This atively affected by idealized images on Instagram than were males
may be due to the difference in stimulus materials. While both of with higher body esteem. Thus, future studies may wish to measure
our studies utilized thin-ideal images, Tiggemann and Anderberg body esteem in male participants to ascertain how body esteem
used “real” (e.g., not posed or photoshopped) and “paired” images relates to general versus specific body part dissatisfaction after
(i.e., the same woman was shown in two images paired viewing idealized images on Instagram.
#INSTABOD VERSUS #BOPO 9

Figure 4
Gender by Experimental Condition Differences in Scores on the PEMES_Face
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Note. Women in the ideal condition scored significantly lower than those in the body positivity and nature
image conditions. There was a trend for significant differences in scores on the PEMES-Face between
females in the body positivity and nature image conditions. Males in the ideal condition scored significantly
lower on the PEMES-Face than those in the body positivity condition, as well as the nature image condition.
PEMES_Face = score on the Perceived Effects of Media Exposure Scale face subscale. See the online article
for the color version of this figure.

The main purpose of the present study was to assess gender differ- condition reported lower scores on the PEMES-Weight and on
ences in the impact of idealized, body-positive, and nature Instagram the PEMES-Face than those in the body positivity and nature image
images on body satisfaction and body appreciation in male and conditions. Thus, our hypothesis that females would be more affected
female college students. Based on previous research, we expected by idealized images seems partially supported. Regarding overall
that women would display lower levels of body satisfaction body satisfaction, it appears that females in the idealized condition
(Franzoi et al., 2012) and body appreciation (Watson et al., 2019) displayed lower levels of body satisfaction than females viewing non-
after viewing idealized images compared to males. As expected, idealized (body-positive or nature images). In contrast, image type did
we did find that males scored higher than females on measures of not seem to affect overall body satisfaction in males. However, when
body appreciation, suggesting that men may have an overall more body satisfaction was assessed using measures that asked about how
positive relationship with the functionality of their bodies than viewing these Instagram posts made them feel about specific body
women. However, there was no significant interaction between gender parts (i.e., the PEMES-Weight and PEMES-Face), both males and
and experimental condition on body image. The discussion of our females in the idealized condition reported lower levels of body satis-
next finding might help explain why. Specifically, when the inter- faction compared to those in the other conditions. While it may be eas-
action between gender and experimental condition on posttest ier for males to “brush” off overall body comparisons as males are
body satisfaction approached statistical significance, we conducted more hopeful about their ability to achieve their overall body goals
follow-up tests to further evaluate this trend. Although posttest body (Franzoi et al., 2012; Tamplin et al., 2018), perhaps asking about
satisfaction did seem to be affected by the experimental condition specific body parts makes those negative comparisons more salient,
in women, with females in the thin-ideal condition displaying lower and thus, harder to brush off. Fardouly et al. (2018) suggest that view-
levels of posttest body satisfaction than females in the body positivity ing images on Instagram is associated with higher self-objectification
condition, none of the conditions differed significantly from one because viewing those images increases the salience of one’s physical
another in males. As both the Body Appreciation Scale and Body appearance.
Satisfaction Scale refer to the overall body, it is not surprising that
we found no interaction between gender and experimental condition Limitations and Future Directions
on body appreciation when male overall body satisfaction did not
appear to be influenced by the type of image (i.e., experimental con- This study had several limitations that need to be addressed. First,
dition). Unlike our results with posttest overall body satisfaction, both due to the Covid-19 pandemic, our study was conducted online
males and females seemed to be affected by idealized images when it instead of in a laboratory setting. Although we had attention checks
came to their weight and face. Both males and females in the idealized and screen timers as part of our experimental manipulation, it is
10 PRITCHARD AND BUTTON

unclear how much participants paid attention to the images pre- on the type of body satisfaction assessed. While the experimental
sented. Similarly, as our study was a force-exposure experimental condition only impacted overall posttest body satisfaction in
design, it may not reflect real-life scrolling habits on Instagram. females, when salience of the images are highlighted by asking
Instagram users likely have a variety of content in their feed rather about how viewing these Instagram posts made them feel about spe-
than all idealistic images or all nature images. Future studies should cific body parts (as on the PEMES-Face and PEMES-Weight), both
study the impact of viewing different types of Instagram images in a males’ and females’ feelings about their bodies seemed to be nega-
more naturalistic way (i.e., having Instagram users use their own tively affected by idealized Instagram images and positively affected
accounts and frequently “ping” them to answer questions related by viewing body-positive posts. Interestingly, females in our study
to the types and impact of the images they are viewing). seemed to benefit more from viewing body-positive images than
A few key differences between the types of images partici- nature images as PEMES-Weight and PEMES-Face scores were
pants were shown that may have created confounds in our study. higher for females in the body-positive condition than in the nature
For example, while we tried to choose idealized and body-positive condition. In contrast, there were no significant differences between
images for both males and females wearing the same amount of males in the body positivity and nature image conditions. This may
clothing (i.e., many images were of individuals in bathing suits), be because females in our study were more likely than males to view
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

the idealized images tended to be more sexualized (i.e., provocative body-positive posts in their daily lives. On the other hand, there were
poses) than the body-positive images. Thus, it may be that the sexual- no differences between PEMES scores in males who viewed nature
ized nature of the idealized image rather than the image content itself versus body-positive images.
contributed to the differences between idealized and body-positive Our results align with Cohen, Fardouly, et al. (2019) and Gültzow
images in our study. In addition, while all of the idealized images et al. (2020) in that males and females are affected by idealized (thin
were photos, a few of the body-positive images contained quotes (or ideal, muscular ideal) posts on Instagram, especially when the images
cartoonish figures with quotes), especially for the male body-positive are made more salient for them by asking about specific body parts.
condition. While our image choices represent the type of body-positive However, while females’ body image may benefit more from viewing
images on Instagram, the fact that not all body-positive images were body-positive images than idealized or nature images, males seem to
photos may have contributed to any differences found between ideal- benefit from viewing nonidealized images (either body-positive or
ized and body-positive posts. Finally, participants were shown images nature images). Thus, college mental health counselors assisting col-
in their original condition, including the text that accompanied the orig- lege students with body image issues may wish to inquire about their
inal post and the number of likes the post received. Research suggests social media habits, specifically Instagram, and encourage them to
that likes do influence social media user behavior; specifically, the view fewer idealized images if they are suffering from body image dis-
number of likes on a post can affect appearance comparison and facial satisfaction. In addition, encouraging discussion of social media and
dissatisfaction in female Instagram users (Tiggemann et al., 2018). body image in educational programs while fostering body apprecia-
Thus, future research may wish to control for image type and number tion in young individuals may influence a more positive relationship
of likes when conducting further experiments on the influence of social between individuals and their bodies, theoretically providing a buffer
media on body image. against negative body image and low body appreciation, and ulti-
Furthermore, our data was limited to a convenience sample of self- mately increasing the health and wellness of those facing poor body
reported data from predominantly White participants. Although we image (Andrew et al., 2015; Cohen, Irwin, et al., 2019; Halliwell,
tried to expand our participation by posting our study on social 2013; Ogden et al., 2020).
media, most of our participants came from one university in the
Rocky Mountain region of the United States. Therefore, our findings References
may not fully represent collegiate females and males across the
United States, or students enrolled within different college courses Andrew, R., Tiggemann, M., & Clark, L. (2015). The protective role of body
and majors; a more diverse sample (age, race, ethnicity, gender iden- appreciation against media-induced body dissatisfaction. Body Image, 15,
tity) would help address this limitation. Third, as we wanted to exam- 98–104. https://doi.org/10.10166/j.bodyim.2015.07.005
ine differences between males and females, we forced participants to Andrew, R., Tiggemann, M., & Clark, L. (2016). Predicting body apprecia-
tion in young women: An integrated model of positive body image. Body
self-identify as one gender (male or female). This may not account
Image, 18, 34–42. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2016.04.003
for participants’ lived experiences of how they best identify their Bair, C. E., Kelly, N. R., Serdar, K. L., & Mazzeo, S. E. (2012). Does the
gender orientation. We also did not ask participants’ sexual orienta- Internet function like magazines? An exploration of image-focused
tion. Previous research has suggested that gay men are more likely to media, eating pathology, and body dissatisfaction. Eating Behaviors,
exhibit body image concerns than heterosexual men (Strübel & 13(4), 398–401. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2012.06.003
Petrie, 2019). Thus, future studies may wish to explore whether Brathwaite, K. N., & DeAndrea, D. C. (2021). Bopopriation: How
social media images affect body image in the same way in heteoro- self-promotion and corporate commodification can undermine the body
sexual versus nonheterosexual samples. positivity (bopo) movement on Instagram. Communication Monographs,
89(1), 25–46. https://doi.org/10.1080/03637751.2021.1925939
Cash, T. F., & Smolak, L. (2011). Body image: A handbook of science, prac-
Conclusion tice, and prevention (2nd ed). Guilford Press.
Charoensukmongkol, P. (2018). The impact of social media on social com-
Despite these limitations, our study was the first to experimentally parison and envy in teenagers: The moderating role of the parent compar-
examine the impact of body-positive Instagram images on body ing children and in-group competition among friends. Journal of Child &
satisfaction and body appreciation in collegiate males and females. Family Studies, 27(1), 69–79. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-017-0872-8
Our results suggest that males and females are differentially affected Chatzopoulou, E., Filieri, R., & Dogruyol, S. A. (2020). Instagram And body
by the images they see on Instagram; however, this partially depends image: Motivation to conform to the “instabod” and consequences on
#INSTABOD VERSUS #BOPO 11

young male wellbeing. Journal of Consumer Affairs, 54(4), 1270–1297. Holland, G., & Tiggemann, M. (2016). A systematic review of the impact of
https://doi.org/10.1111/joca.12329 the use of social networking sites on body image and disordered eating out-
Cohen, R., Fardouly, J., Newton-John, T., & Slater, A. (2019). #Bopo on comes. Body Image, 17, 100–110. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2016
Instagram: An experimental investigation of the effects of viewing body pos- .02.008
itive content on young women’s mood and body image. New Media & Johnson, S. M., Edwards, K. M., & Gidycz, C. A. (2015). Interpersonal
Society, 21(7), 1546–1564. https://doi.org/10.1177/1461444819826530 weight-related pressure and disordered eating in college women: A test
Cohen, R., Irwin, L., Newton-John, T., & Slater, A. (2019). #Bodypositivity: of an expanded tripartite influence model. Sex Roles: A Journal of
A content analysis of body positive accounts on Instagram. Body Image, Research, 72(1–2), 15–24. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-014-0442-0
29, 47–57. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2019.02.007 Kim, H. M. (2021). What do others’ reactions to body posting on Instagram
Cohen, R., Newton-John, T., & Slater, A. (2018). “Selfie”-objectification: tell us? The effects of social media comments on viewers’ body image per-
The role of selfies in self-objectification and disordered eating in young ception. New Media & Society, 23(12), 3448–3465. https://doi.org/10
women. Computers in Human Behavior, 79, 68–74. https://doi.org/10 .1177/1461444820956368
.1016/j.chb.2017.10.027 Knobloch-Westerwick, S., & Romero, J. P. (2011). Body ideals in the media:
Cohen, R., Newton-John, T., & Slater, A. (2020). The case for body positivity Perceived attainability and social comparison choices. Media Psychology,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

on social media: Perspectives on current advances and future directions. 14(1), 27–48. https://doi.org/10.1080/15213269.2010.547833
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Journal of Health Psychology, 26(13), 2365–2373. https://doi.org/10 Leit, R. A., Gray, J. J., & Pope, H. G. (2002). The media’s representation of
.1177/1359105320912450 the ideal male body: A cause for muscle dysmorphia? International
Cramblitt, B., & Pritchard, M. (2013). Media’s influence on the drive for Journal of Eating Disorders, 31(3), 334–338. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat
muscularity in undergraduates. Eating Behaviors, 14(4), 441–446. .10019
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2013.08.003 McCabe, M. P., & Ricciardelli, L. A. (2004). Weight and shape concerns of
Dixon, S. (2022, May 12). Instagram: distribution of global audiences 2022, boys and men. In J. K. Thompson (Ed.), Handbook of eating disorders and
by age group. Statista.com https://www.statista.com/statistics/325587/ obesity (pp. 606–634). John Wiley & Sons
instagram-global-age-group/#:~:text=Instagram%3A%20distribution% McCreary, D. R., & Sasse, D. K. (2000). An exploration of the drive for mus-
20of%20global%20audiences%202022%2C%20by%20age%20group& cularity in adolescent boys and girls. Journal of American College Health,
text=As%20of%20April%202022%2C%20roughly,platform%20particularly 48(6), 297–304. https://doi.org/10.1080/07448480009596271
%20attractive%20for%20marketers Meier, E. P., & Gray, J. (2014). Facebook Photo activity associated
Fardouly, J., Willburger, B. K., & Vartanian, L. R. (2018). Instagram Use and with body image disturbance in adolescent girls. Cyberpsychology,
young women’s body image concerns and self-objectification: Testing Behavior, and Social Networking, 17(4), 199–206. https://doi.org/10
mediational pathways. New Media & Society, 20(4), 1380–1395. 1177/ .1089/cyber.2013.0305
1461444817694499 Mulgrew, K. E., Findlay, C., Lane, B. R., & Halliwell, E. (2021). Does body
Franzoi, S. L., Vasquez, K., Sparapani, E., Frost, K., Martin, J., & Aebly, M. appreciation or satisfaction buffer against idealised functionality-focused
(2012). Exploring body comparison tendencies: Women are self-critical images of models? Body Image, 36(1351), 45–52. https://doi.org/10
whereas men are self-hopeful. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 36(1), .1016/j.bodyim.2020.09.007
99–109. https://doi.org/10.1177/0361684311427028 Ogden, J., Gosling, C., Hazelwood, M., & Atkins, E. (2020). Exposure to
Frederick, D. A., Buchanan, G. M., Sadehgi-Azar, L., Peplau, L. A., body diversity images as a buffer against the thin-ideal: An experimental
Haselton, M. G., Berezovskaya, A., & Lipinski, R. E. (2007). Desiring study. Psychology, Health & Medicine, 25(10), 1165–1178. https://
the muscular ideal: Men’s body satisfaction in the United States, doi.org/10.1080/13548506.2020.1734219
Ukraine, and Ghana. Psychology of Men & Masculinity, 8(2), 103–117. Rodgers, R. F., Slater, A., Gordon, C. S., McLean, S. A., Jarman, H. K., &
https://doi.org/10.1037/1524-9220.8.2.103 Paxton, S. J. (2020). A biopsychosocial model of social media use and
Frederick, D. A., Daniels, E. A., Bates, M. E., & Tylka, T. L. (2017). Exposure body image concerns, disordered eating, and muscle-building behaviors
to thin-ideal media affect most, but not all, women: Results from the among adolescent girls and boys. Journal of Youth & Adolescence,
Perceived Effects of Media Exposure Scale and open-ended responses. 49(2), 399–409. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-019-01190-0
Body Image, 23, 188–205. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2017.10.006 Rounds, E. G., & Stutts, L. A. (2021). The impact of fitspiration content
Girard, M., Rodgers, R. F., & Chabrol, H. (2018). Prospective predictors of on body satisfaction and negative mood: An experimental study.
body dissatisfaction, drive for thinness, and muscularity concerns among Psychology of Popular Media, 10(2), 267–274. https://doi.org/10
young women in France: A sociocultural model. Body Image, 26, 103– .1037/ppm0000288
110. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2018.07.001 Stein, J.-P., Krause, E., & Ohler, P. (2019). Every (Insta)Gram counts?
Grabe, S., Ward, L. M., & Hyde, J. S. (2008). The role of the media in body Applying cultivation theory to explore the effects of Instagram on young
image concerns among women: A meta-analysis of experimental and cor- users’ body image. Psychology of Popular Media Culture, 10(1), 87–97.
relational studies. Psychological Bulletin, 134(3), 460–476. https:// https://doi.org/10.1037/ppm0000268
doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.134.3.460 Strübel, J., & Petrie, T. A. (2019). Appearance and performance enhancing
Gültzow, T., Guidry, J., Schneider, F., & Hoving, C. (2020). Male body drug usage and psychological well-being in gay and heterosexual men.
image portrayals on Instagram. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Psychology & Sexuality, 10(2), 132–148. https://doi.org/10.1080/
Networking, 23(5), 281–289. https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2019.0368 19419899.2019.1574879
Halliwell, E. (2013). The impact of thin idealized media images on body sat- Sundgot-Borgen, C., Sundgot-Borgen, J., Bratland-Sanda, S., Kolle, E.,
isfaction: Does body appreciation protect women from negative effects? Torstveit, M. K., Svantorp-Tveiten, M. K., & Mathisen, T. F. (2021).
Body Image, 10(4), 509–514. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2013.07 Body appreciation and body appearance pressure in Norwegian university
.004 students comparing exercise science students and other students. BMC
Hendrickse, J., Arpan, L. M., Clayton, R. B., & Ridgway, J. L. (2017). Public Health, 21(1), 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-021-10550-0
Instagram and college women’s body image: Investigating the Tamplin, N. C., McLean, S. A., & Paxton, S. J. (2018). Social media literacy
roles of appearance-related comparisons and intrasexual competition. protects against the negative impact of exposure to appearance ideal social
Computers in Human Behavior, 74(9), 92–100. https://doi.org/10 media images in young adult women but not men. Body Image, 26, 29–37.
.1016/j.chb.2017.04.027 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2018.05.003
12 PRITCHARD AND BUTTON

Thompson, J. K., Heinberg, L. J., Altabe, M., & Tantleff-Dunn, S. (1999). Tylka, T. L., & Wood-Barcalow, N. L. (2015b). What is and what is not
Exacting beauty: Theory, assessment, and treatment of body image dis- positive body image? Conceptual foundations and construct definition.
turbance. American Psychological Association. Body Image, 14, 118–129. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2015.04.001
Thompson, S. H., & Lougheed, E. (2012). Frazzled by Facebook? An explor- Van den Berg, P., Thompson, J. K., Obaremski-Brandon, K., & & Coovert,
atory study of gender differences in social network communication among M. (2002). The tripartite influence model of body image and eating
undergraduate men and women. College Student Journal, 46(1), 88–98. disturbance. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 53(5), 1007–1020.
Tiggemann, M., & Anderberg, I. (2020a). Social media is not real: The effect https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-3999(02)00499-3
of ‘Instagram vs reality’ images on women’s social comparison and body Veldhuis, J., Konijn, E. A., & Seidell, J. C. (2014). Negotiated media effects
image. New Media & Society, 22(12), 2183–2199. https://doi.org/10.1177/ peer feedback modifies effects of media’s thin-body ideal on adolescent
1461444819888720 girls. Appetite, 73, 172–182. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2013.10.023
Tiggemann, M., & Anderberg, I. (2020b). Muscles and bare chests on Watson, A., Murnen, S. K., & College, K. (2019). Gender differences in
Instagram: The effect of Influencers’ fashion and fitspiration images on responses to thin, athletic, and hyper-muscular idealized bodies. Body
men’s body image. Body Image, 35(2), 237–244. https://doi.org/10 Image, 30, 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2019.03.010
.1016/j.bodyim.2020.10.001 Webb, H. J., Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J., & Donovan, C. L. (2014). The appear-
Tiggemann, M., Hayden, S., Brown, Z., & Veldhuis, J. (2018). The effect ance culture between friends and adolescent appearance-based rejection
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

of Instagram “likes” on women’s social comparison and body dissatis- sensitivity. Journal of Adolescence, 37(4), 347–358. https://doi.org/10
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

faction. Body Image, 26, 90–97. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2018 .1016/j.adolescence.2014.02.008


.07.002
Tylka, T. L., & Wood-Barcalow, N. L. (2015a). The Body Appreciation Received October 7, 2021
Scale-2: Item refinement and psychometric evaluation. Body Image, Revision received November 8, 2022
12C(1), 53–67. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2014.09.006 Accepted December 9, 2022 ▪

You might also like