Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 20

Original Research Article

Journal of Career Assessment


2022, Vol. 0(0) 1–20
Career Calling and Task © The Author(s) 2022
Article reuse guidelines:
Performance: The Moderating sagepub.com/journals-permissions
DOI: 10.1177/10690727211039454
journals.sagepub.com/home/jca
Role of Job Demand

Michelangelo Vianello1 , Anna Dalla Rosa1 , and Sophie Gerdel1 

Abstract
The Work as a Calling Theory (WCT) predicts that career calling fosters job performance. A
quantitative summary of previous work supports this prediction and shows that the relation
between calling and job performance is moderate in size (ρ = .29, K = 11, N = 2286). Yet, the
environmental conditions that modulate this relation are completely unknown. According to an
interactionist perspective, we argue that calling may predict performance only when job demand
is low. Results of a multisource study on salesmen and managers dyads (N= 965) partially
supported this prediction. We observed that highly demanding work environments, characterized
by pressure to perform, high workload, and unachievable deadlines, suppress the positive relation
between calling and self-reported performance. Job demand directly impairs performance and
suppresses the positive effect of career calling. Theoretical and practical implications of these
results are discussed.

Keywords
career calling, task performance, job demand, moderation, multi-level

In industrialized contemporary societies, working for a promotion or mere compensation is not


sufficient: Employees see their job as an opportunity to fulfill an increasingly larger set of social
and psychological needs (Casey, 1995), such as building a positive work identity (Miscenko&
Day, 2016), deriving a sense of meaning (Rosso et al., 2010), or pursuing a calling (Thompson &
Bunderson, 2019). Matching the neoclassical and modern traditions, we conceptualize calling as a
way to approach work in which individuals feel a strong passion for their job, which becomes
central to their identity, pervasive and worth sacrificing other areas of life, and which transcend
their selves and make them feel that they have a purpose in life and that they are useful to the
society or the greater good (Dik & Duffy, 2009; Dobrow&Tosti-Kharas, 2011; Vianello et al.,
2020). Employees with higher callings derive increased meaningfulness and motivation from their

1
Department of Philosophy, Sociology, Education and Applied Psychology, University of Padova, Padova, Italy

Corresponding Author:
Michelangelo Vianello, Department of Philosophy, Sociology, Education and Applied Psychology, University of Padova, via
Venezia 14, Padova 35131, Italy.
Email: michelangelo.vianello@unipd.it
2 Journal of Career Assessment 0(0)

work (Dik et al., 2008; Duffy et al., 2017). Calling has many beneficial outcomes, such as in-
creased well-being (Conway et al., 2015; Dalla Rosa & Vianello, 2020; Goštautaitė et al., 2020),
job satisfaction (Duffy et al., 2011), and organizational citizenship behavior (Park et al., 2016).

Calling and Job Performance in the Work as a Calling Theory


The positive relation between calling and performance is predicted by the Work as a Calling Theory
(WCT; Duffy et al., 2018), which proposes a complex set of predictors and outcomes of perceiving
and living a calling at work. The WCT starts its chain of predictions from perceiving a calling, which
is the first step toward experiencing the positive effects of living out a calling at work: job satisfaction
and job performance. Once people perceive they have a calling toward a job or a career, the extent to
which they will live it out depends on the degree to which they feel fit with their work environment,
which in turn increase work meaning and career commitment. According to the WCT, the mediation
of person–environment fit in the relation between perceiving a calling and work meaning and career
commitment is moderated by the level of motivation that people have toward pursuing their calling
(Calling Motivation), the extent to which they change the relational, behavioral, and cognitive
engagements involved in one’s work (Job Crafting), and the extent to which they feel supported,
assisted, and encouraged by their work environment (Organizational Support). The theory also states
that, if people have access to opportunities, the extent to which they feel their work has meaning and
feel committed to their career are related to the extent to which they live out their calling at work, in a
virtuous cycle in which all states influence one another.
For the purposes of this study, the most salient part of the theory is the relation that calling is
expected to have with job performance. The WCT predicts that the extent to which people feel that
they are living out their calling at work predicts how they perform in their jobs and how much they
will be satisfied by their job. In the WCT, the positive relation between calling and performance is
explained by the motivating role of calling: Calling can motivate one’s job-directed actions, and the
task itself becomes important for employees to practice their sense of calling (Duffy et al., 2017; Lee
et al., 2018; Lobene& Meade, 2013).
The WCT also admits that for some people in some situations calling may be positively related
with negative outcomes, such as workaholism, burnout, and exploitation. The relation between
calling and possible negative outcomes is predicted to be moderated by both internal and external
factors, such as personality and psychological climate at work. The WCT does not predict any
moderator of the calling–performance relationship. With the aim of enriching our understanding
of the situations in which calling predicts performance, in this paper we investigate whether job
demand moderates the relation between calling and performance. Specifically, we will argue that
when job demand is high, approaching jobs as a calling will not predict job performance. On the
contrary, we expect a positive relation when job demand is low to moderate. Given the focus of
this study and the extant literature that supports the relation between perceiving and living a
calling (see Duffy et al., 2019), we will not investigate this relation any further. Instead, we will
center our study around the extent to which people feel that they are called toward their current job,
which include both perceiving and living out a calling at work.

Empirical Evidence Supporting the Relation Between Calling


and Performance
The calling-performance relation predicted by the WCT has recently been supported empirically
by a few studies. Lee et al. (2018) demonstrated that employees working in service and high-tech
industries who perceived a higher calling reported higher task (r = .20) and contextual performance
(r = .35). The authors argued that perceiving a calling motivates employees to perform well. A
Vianello et al. 3

supportive climate was found to moderate the relation between calling and contextual performance,
but not the relation with task performance. A relation of r = .24 between calling and performance has
been observed by Wu et al. (2019) in a sample of construction project managers. The highest relation
between calling and performance can be found in Liu et al. (2019, r = .58). The authors investigated
the relation between calling and safety performance in a sample of train drivers and found that it is
stronger when perceived organizational support is high. Furthermore, the authors found that the
calling-safety performance relation is partially mediated by work engagement. The positive relation
between perceived calling and task performance, measured as employees’ recall of their manager’s
last rating, has been also observed in teachers (Lobene & Meade, 2013; r = .24): Those who perceive
a calling are more dedicated to their work and show superior performance despite the perception of
overqualification.
Kim et al. (2018) analyzed the relation between calling and self-rated performance (Study 1: r =
.35; Study 2: r = .11) and manager-rated performance (Study 3: r = .33). In Study 1 (involving a
sample of staff members of a Presbyterian megachurch) and in Study 3 (involving teams in
companies operating banking, telecommunications, and electricity services), the authors found that
work commitment partially mediates the relation between calling and performance and that ideo-
logical contract fulfillment moderates the mediation effect. In Study 2, the relation between calling
and self-rated performance, assessed 1 month apart, was found to be nonsignificant. Similarly, Park
et al. (2019) adopted a temporal separation between variables and included managers’ ratings of
performance. They investigated the relation between the calling of newcomers, assessed during the
first week of orientation at work, and managers’ evaluation of their employees’ performance, assessed
2 years after organizational entry. The correlation was found to be positive and significant (r = .24). In
a hospital setting, Romney (2020; Study 3) showed that the positive relation between employees’
calling and manager’s ratings of their performance (r = .24) was suppressed by the managers’
perception of how effective employees deliver their voices regarding suggestions or comments on
how work might be conducted more effectively (constructive voice delivery).
Finally, only one study investigated the relation between calling and productivity. Park et al.
(2016) observed a positive but nonsignificant relation between sales performed in 1 year (total
commission and policies sold) and perceiving a calling (r = .14), and a positive and significant
relation between sales and living out a calling at work (r = .24). The relation between calling and
sales was found to be completely mediated by occupational self-efficacy.
Adopting a different point of view, Park et al. (2018) investigated the relation between leader’s
calling and employees’ calling among matched pairs of combat flight managers and employees.
They observed a positive relation (r = .40) between manager’s calling and manager’s evaluation of
employees’ performance.
Overall, the average meta-analytic correlation that has been observed in the literature across 11
studies on calling and performance is ρ = .29, 95% CI [.18, .39], τ = 0.00, I2 = 0.00, Q(10) = 7.04,
and p = .29. Removing the effect in Park et al. (2018), which measured managers’ calling instead
of employees,’ the meta-analytic average correlation between calling and different measures of
performance is ρ = .28, 95% CI [.16, .39]. Although heterogeneity is null after accounting for
sampling error in the distribution of effect sizes, the number of studies allows investigating the
moderating role of the type of performance measure employed. The relation between calling and
performance is small-to-moderate (Cohen, 1992; Gignac&Szodorai, 2016) in studies that em-
ployed self-reported measures of performance (weighted mean ρ = .21, 95% CI [.14, .28]) and
moderate-to-large when supervisor’s ratings of employees’ performance has been employed as an
outcome (weighted mean ρ = .38; 95% CI [.22, .51]). This difference suggests either that the
managers are influenced in their evaluations by both actual performance and employees’ attitude
toward their job, or that employees with a calling underestimate their actual performance. Figure 1
provides the forest plot of all effects included in the meta-analytic estimation.
4 Journal of Career Assessment 0(0)

Figure 1. Forest Plot of the Meta-Analysis Conducted on 11 Published Studies on the Relation Between
Calling and Job Performance.

The Calling-Performance Relation According to an


Interactionist Perspective
It has been observed that when the environment constrains options and provides clear signals
about what is expected (strong situations; Mischel, 1977), individual factors such as personality
and attitudes may lose their predicting power. According to an interactionist perspective, behavior
is indeed the product of both internal (personal) and external (situational) factors (Endler& Parker,
1992; Weiss & Adler, 1984). Strong situations are characterized by high demands. Conversely,
weak situations do not generate clear expectancies concerning behavior, and “do not offer
sufficient incentives for its performance” (Mischel, 1977, p. 347). Weak situations let the in-
dividual free to choose their behavior, whereas strong situations impose a limit on the range of
actions that the individual can perform. It is therefore likely that individuals who approach their
work as a calling would perform better only when demands are low. Although reasonable, this
assertion lacks empirical support because no study investigated whether job demand suppresses
the positive relation between calling and performance. Furthermore, the different sizes of the
effects that have been observed between calling and performance seem to depend on how
performance is measured: Managers’ ratings are likely more sensitive to employees’ calling.
Hence, in this study we fill a gap of knowledge in the literature by investigating whether job
demands moderate the relation between calling and performance, by means of a multisource
design in which job performance is measured across three different sources: the individual, the line
manager, and archival information on actual productivity.
Multi-source designs have been introduced in publishing practices as a way to mitigate
common method bias (Podsakoff et al., 2012), a source of error that is known at least since
60 years ago (Campbell & Fiske, 1959). Common method bias is a distortion in observed re-
lationships across measurements due to shared method variance that is erroneously interpreted as
shared trait variance (i.e., the “true” score variance that is shared by the constructs under in-
vestigation and that is of theoretical interest). Although it is widely supposed that common method
bias invariably inflates observed relations, this is a common misconception (Conway & Lance,
2010). Shared method variance can either inflate or deflate observed relations. The latter bias
occurs when measurement methods are negatively related. In both cases, controlling for biases in
the estimates is critical because interpreting method variance instead of true variance can er-
roneously inform theories. Another common misconception is that other-reports are superior to
Vianello et al. 5

self-reports (Conway and Lance, 2010). Other methods are also subject to method effects, which
can be the same or different from those that bias self-reports. For instance, supervisors’ ratings of
job performance (rxx=.52; Salgado et al., 2016) are typically much less reliable than self-report
measures. An effective way to gather information on potential biases in the estimates is combining
different sources of information within the same study (i.e., multi-source designs), rather than
substituting self-reports with other-reports or objective measures of performance.

Hypotheses
Career calling and performance. In the previous paragraphs, we proposed a theoretical framework
on the calling–performance relation and quantitively reviewed a summary of empirical evidence
supporting this relation. We expect to replicate previous results suggesting that career calling is
positively related to all measures of performance adopted in this study: self-reported, manager-
reported, and productivity.

Hypothesis 1: There is a positive relation between calling and self-reported task performance
(H1a), manager’s rating of task performance (H1b), and productivity (H1c).

Job demand and performance. Job demands are stressors that can be of physical, psycho-
logical, social, or organizational nature and they are associated with physical or psychological
costs (Bakker & Demerouti, 2007), generally leading to decreased job performance (Bakker et al.,
2008; Gilboa et al., 2008). According to the literature, we expect to observe a negative relation
between job demand and task performance. Higher demands make the employee focus on the gap
between their performance and the expectations of the organization. As a consequence of focusing
on expectations, they concentrate on coping with role stressors instead of committing to per-
forming their tasks (Gilboa et al., 2008). Following this reasoning, we expect that performance
will be lower when job demand is high.

Hypothesis 2: There is a negative relation between demand and self-reported task performance
(H2a), manager’s rating of task performance (H2b), and productivity (H2c).

The moderation role of job demand. As predicted by the Person x Situation interactionist
perspective (Cooper &Withey, 2009; Mischel, 1977), we expect that higher calling will relate to
higher performance only when job demand is low, that is, when the situation is weak. The lack of
strong pressure to perform offers individuals the opportunity to behave according to their calling,
which is already known to lead to effective work behaviors that ultimately result in increased
performance. Conversely, when job demands are high, that is, when the situation is strong,
employees will be less free to choose which behavior to perform, thus calling will not drive their
behavior. In addition, job demand is expected to decrease job performance because employees will
be more focused on complying to organizational expectations, rather than pursuing their sense of
calling and performing their tasks. Therefore, the following hypotheses were developed.

Hypothesis 3: Demand moderates the relation between calling and self-reported task per-
formance (H3a), manager’s rating of task performance (H3b), and productivity (H3c) such that
the relation between calling and task performance is larger when demand is low.

Differential effects. We expect that the presence of common method variance (Podsakoff et al.,
2012) will inflate the observed relation between calling and self-reported task performance, which
will turn out to be stronger than the relation between calling and other task performance measures.
6 Journal of Career Assessment 0(0)

Even in the absence of common method bias, higher correlations between calling and self-reported
performance may be due to individuals with high calling perceiving their ability to be higher than
others, an effect that has been observed by Dobrow and Heller (2015). This may lead to an altered
perception of their results and inflated self-reported performance measures.

Hypothesis 4: The relation between calling and self-reported task performance is stronger than
the relation between calling and other task performance measures (rated by the manager and
productivity).

Within the same reasoning, we expect that the relations between job demand and self-reported
task performance will be stronger than the relation between job demand and managers’ reported
task performance due to common-method bias (Podsakoff et al., 2012). Furthermore, demand
affects performance, but perceptions of performance can also inform employees’ perception of
demand. When employees perceive that they are unable to meet assigned standards of perfor-
mance, they might develop a sense of being overloaded by environmental demands, which would
lead them to perceive (and report) an increased level of demand. A stronger relation between
demand and self-reported performance has also been observed in Gilboa et al. (2008).

Hypothesis 5: The relation between demand and self-reported task performance is stronger than
the relation between demand and other task performance measures (rated by the manager and
productivity).

All hypothesized relations are summarized in Figure 2.

Method
Sample and Procedure
The data were collected online in 51 branches of a frozen food delivery company in Italy that
serves clients all over the national territory. Italy is an industrialized society and the third-largest
economy in the euro zone. Its economy is dominated by private companies and driven in large part
by the manufacture of high-quality consumer goods, many of which are family-owned. Italy’s
culture is individualist and avoids uncertainty (Hofstede, 2021), which means that Italians value
individual’s (vs. group’s) ideas and goals and that bureaucracy and detailed planning are generally
favored over a flexible planning process. Although the company in which data were collected is
multinational with a headquarter in Germany, the organizational culture in which our participants
live their workday mostly reflect the Italian culture. Each branch is led by one Italian manager,
who coordinates from 14 to 46 salesmen. The data were collected within a larger project aimed at
assessing work-related stress risks, a yearly process that is enforced by the Italian law. The CEO
invited employees to take part to the study by email. Two sets of online surveys were administered:
one for subordinates and another one for their line managers. All participants were provided with
measures of Career Calling, Job Demand, and Task Performance. The survey also included
measures of Organizational Citizenship Behaviors (OCB: altruism, courtesy, and compliance),
Job Control, Social Support, Job Satisfaction, and turnover intentions. Scores at these scales are
not used in the present study. The complete list of questions is provided here: https://osf.io/6nkue/.
Managers were also asked to rate their employees’ task performance. The company delivered
1.530 questionnaires to the employees (51 managers and 1479 subordinates) and we collected 965
responses (46 managers and 919 subordinates), yielding a response rate of 63%. In this sample,
783 participants were male, 16 were female, and 24 preferred not to answer. The average age of the
Vianello et al. 7

Figure 2. Moderation of Job Demand Between Calling and Measures of Performance: Self-rated (panel a),
Manager-rated (panel b), and Productivity (panel c). Direction of Relations and Hypotheses are Reported.
H = Hypothesis.

subordinates was 40.31 years (SD = 9.1) and 43.25 years for managers (SD = 7.8). Most sub-
ordinates have a high school degree (64.6%), 27.1% completed a middle school, 3.5% got a
Bachelor’s degree, 1.5% completed a Master’s degree, 0.1% finished a post-graduate study, and
3.2% did not report a specific education level. Most managers have a high school degree (65.9%),
29.5% stopped their education at the middle school, 2.3% completed a Master’s degree, and 2.3%
finished a post-graduate study. On average, subordinates worked for 9 years (SD = 9.0) in their
profession and for 8 years (SD = 8.8) in their current organization. The average working time is
47.44 hours per week. Managers worked on average for 13 years (SD = 9.5) in their profession and
for 12 years (SD = 9.2) in their current organization. The average working time for managers is
51.13 hours per week.
8 Journal of Career Assessment 0(0)

Measures
The questionnaire was administered in Italian. All scales were developed or validated in Italian
except for Task Performance, which has been translated and back-translated following Brislin
(1970).

Career Calling
Calling was measured through an improved version of the UMCS (Vianello et al., 2018). The
UMCS measures the extent to which individuals perceive a calling for their current job by means
of seven factors: passion, pervasiveness, purpose, prosocial orientation, transcendent summons,
sacrifice, and identity. The UMCS was validated in a sample of Italian college students and adult
workers and demonstrated good internal and external validity (Vianello et al., 2018). Scores on
the calling scale showed good internal consistency (α = 0.93) and measurement invariance
across time and study domains. The UMCS showed convergent validity with both the CVQ-
presence scale (Dik et al., 2008) that measures perceiving a calling (r=.72, Dalla Rosa et al.,
2019b), a single-item measure of living a calling (r=.58, Vianello et al., 2020), and the living a
calling scale by Duffy et al. (2012; r(348)=.78, Dalla Rosa & Vianello, 2021; r(152)=.73, Vianello
& Dalla Rosa, 2020). Example items are “I am passionate about my work,”“I believe that I have
been called to pursue my current line of work,” and “My work helps me live out my life’s
purpose.” The original scale is composed of 22 items, in this study we used a longer version of
the UMCS composed of 28 items. To test whether the UMCS-28 is an improved version of the
original 22-item scale, we compared nested second-order confirmatory factor analysis models.
The analysis showed slightly better fit indexes for the 28-item version and overall higher
loadings on the items: UMCS-28: χ 2(343) = 1021.084, CFI = .952, RMSEA = .049, 95% CI
[.045–.052], SRMR = .039; UMCS-22: χ 2(202) = 730.501, CFI = .947, RMSEA = .056, 95% CI
[.052–.060], SRMR = .044; Δχ 2(141) = 290.58, p< .001, ΔCFI = .01; ΔRMSEA = .01; ΔSRMR
= .01. The best fitting model (UMCS-28) showed a good fit to the data according to the most
stringent criteria reported in Marsh et al. (2004). In the current study, answers were given on a
Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree) and Cronbach’s α was .96.
Supplemental information and detailed results supporting the use of the 28-item scale are
reported at the following link: https://osf.io/7ftx6/.

Job Demand
Demand was measured with eight items taken from the Italian version of the Health and Safety
Executive Indicator Tool (HSE-IT; INAIL, 2017). The HSE-IT is used as a first step in risk
assessment for work-related stress. The demand scale is a subscale of the HSE-IT that assesses
potential sources of stress in the workplace, such as issues regarding workload, work patterns,
and the work environment. The scale was validated in a sample of 6,378 Italian workers and
30,903 employees in the UK, the internal reliability of the demand scale scores was .75 and
.87, respectively (Edwards et al., 2008; Rondinone et al., 2012). To account for construct
validity, we conducted a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA). The results showed that a one-
factor model had a good fit to the data: χ 2(19) = 87.301, CFI = .958, RMSEA = .063, 95% CI
[.050, .077], SRMR = .033, and is in line with previous literature (Edwards et al., 2008).
Example items are: “I have unrealistic time pressures” and “I have unachievable deadlines.”
Answers were given on a Likert scale ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (always). In the current
study, Cronbach’s α was .79.
Vianello et al. 9

Task Performance
Task Performance was measured with the 7-item In-Role Behavior scale by Williams and
Anderson (1991). Example items are: “adequately completes assigned duties” or “meets
formal performance requirements of the job.” Items were adjusted for use with employees or
managers (e.g., “I fulfill all the requirements of the job”; “This employee fulfills all the
requirements of the job”). The scale was validated in a sample of 127 employees (α = .91,
Williams & Anderson, 1991). Answers were given on a Likert scale from 1 (strongly disagree)
to 5 (strongly agree). In the current study, Cronbach’s α was .77 for self-reported task
performance. The results showed that a one-factor model for self-reported task performance
had a good fit to the data, χ 2(13) = 62.542, CFI = .977, RMSEA = .066, 95% CI [.050, .083],
SRMR = .024.
The reliability of managers’ rating of employee’s task performance was estimated using a three-
level model (scores are nested within employees, employees are nested within managers). The
multilevel model was estimated using formula 8 in Jeon et al. (2009). The reliability of the
manager-level scores was estimated as the proportion of between-managers variance (σ2 = .10) on
the total variance (variance of employees within managers, σ2 = .27; variance of scores among
employees, σ2 = .34), corrected for the average number of employees for managers (n = 29) and
the number of items (seven items for each employee; formula 9 in Jeon et al., 2009). Considering
an average of 29 employees for each manager and seven scores for each employee, the reliability
of manager-rated performance scores was .88.
A multi-level confirmatory factor analysis (MCFA; Dyer et al., 2005) was performed for
task performance evaluations provided by managers. Running a single-level CFA on man-
agers’ ratings would have introduced two potential sources of biases in the estimates. A CFA
conducted on the variance–covariance matrix estimated on the entire dataset would have
ignored the dependency that exists among ratings provided by the same manager. A CFA
conducted on the sample between-group variance–covariance matrix would have under-
estimated the fit of the group level factor structure. None of them would have informed us on
whether the factor structure holds both in the within- and between-group variance–covariance
matrix. Hence, we estimated a two-level CFA, which specifies two different factor structures
for the within- and between-group levels. The within-group level parallels a traditional CFA
on disaggregated data with seven observed indicators, seven random errors, and one latent
factor. The between-manager structure specifies seven latent indicators estimated from group
means of all observed indicators, seven random errors, and one latent factor. The two
structures are linked by regressing group means on the observed indicators. The results of the
MCFA suggests that the same factor structure holds at both the organizational and individual
levels: χ 2(26) = 73.014, CFI = .982, RMSEA = .038, SRMR within = .019, and SRMR between
= .0311.

Productivity
The productivity levels of employees were provided by the company using a 5-level discrete
measure. Employees were ranked from 1 (lowest productivity) to 5 (highest productivity) ac-
cording to their yearly amount of sales. Salesmen in rank 1 are those who realized the lowest
amount of sales (less than € 200.000,00 in a year) and salesmen in rank 5 are those who realized
the highest amount of sales (more than € 288.500,00). The intervals determining the levels of
performance (from 1 to 5) were set by the organization based on their historical record of sales.
Further information on this measure is protected by industrial secrecy.
10 Journal of Career Assessment 0(0)

Preliminary Analysis
Preliminary analyses were performed to test for assumptions behind multilevel linear models. The
skewness and kurtosis of the calling, demand, self-reported task performance, manager’s ratings
of employees’ performance and productivity distributions were all within plus or minus one,
indicating univariate normality. To test whether calling and job demand are linearly related to self-
reported task performance, managers’ rating of task performance and productivity, plots of OLS
estimates and raw residuals against each predictor were examined. Results support linearity of
relations among the dependent and independent variables considered in this study.
Missing values for variables ranged between 0% (productivity and managers’ rating of task
performance) and 13% (calling). Little’s MCAR test including calling, job demand, and self-
reported task performance suggests that data are missing completely at random: χ 2(3) = 2.720, p =
.437. Missing data were handled with the maximum likelihood (ML) estimation procedure
available in IBM SPSS 27.

Results
Correlations among the study variables are reported in Table 1. Calling was positively correlated
with self-reported task performance and task performance rated by the manager. Demand was
negatively correlated with self-reported task performance and task performance rated by the
manager. Neither demand nor calling were significantly correlated with productivity.
In this dataset, employees are nested within branches. It is reasonable that employees within the
same branch are more similar in work-related attitudes and behaviors compared to employees
working in different branches, as a result of selection practices and subcultures within the or-
ganization. Hence, we accounted for possible clusters in the dataset, decomposing variance within
branches from variance across branches. Failure to do so may provide biased estimates of model
parameters and their standard errors. We estimated for each hypothesis a series of nested gen-
eralized linear multilevel models (West et al., 2007). As a baseline model (M0), we estimated a
null model with no predictors and a random intercept to partition the variance of the dependent
variable into between- and within-branches variation. Then, we added fixed effects of demand and
calling (M1) and kept them in subsequent models even if they were non-significant. In model 2
(M2), we added the random effects of demand and calling, and removed them from subsequent
models if the variances of the slopes were non-significant to avoid estimation problems. Model 3
tested the fixed interaction effect by adding the product term between demand and calling. The last
model (M4) tests the variance across branches of the interaction slope, which was removed if non-
significant. The Intraclass Correlation Coefficient (ICC) was used as an indicator of the proportion

Table 1. Descriptive Statistics, Cronbach’s Alphas, and Correlations for All Study Variables.

Variables n M SD 1 2 3 4 5

1. Demand 900 2.66 .61 .79 837 869 723 549


2. Calling 837 3.54 .64 .30** .96 837 669 502
3. Self-reported task performance 869 4.29 .48 .23** .23** .77 697 527
4. Managers’ratings of task performance 1220 3.99 .69 .13** .12** .18** .88a 806
5. Productivity 954 4.44 1.19 .02 .05 .08 .43** —
这句话的意思Note. Numbers above the main diagonal report sample size of Pearson’s zero-order correlations, which are provided
是对角线以上below the main diagonal that reports scale scores’ reliabilities (Cronbach’s alpha unless otherwise noted).
的数字是每一a
对相关系数的 Manager-level reliability was estimated using formula 9 in Jeon et al. (2009).
样本量,对角**p< .01, two-tailed.
线以下才是相
关系数。
Vianello et al. 11

of between-branch variance on the total variance and can be interpreted as the degree of group
heterogeneity for a measure (Hox, 2002). To correctly interpret interaction effects, measures of
performance were standardized; calling, demand and the interaction term were grand-mean
centered in all models. The codes for reproducing all analysis presented in this article are available
here: https://osf.io/aqpjr/.

Self-Reported Task Performance


The null model (M0) showed that the between-branches variance of self-reported task perfor-
mance is non-significant (μbetween = .02, p = .19, μwithin = .98, p< .001). The proportion of between-
branches variance is tiny (ICC = .02). The fixed effects of calling (H1a, β = .25, p< .001) and
demand (H2a, β = .28, p< .001) as level 1 predictors of self-reported task performance are
significant (M1). M2 tested the between-branches variance of slopes, which is small and non-
significant (μdemand = .06, p =.17; μcalling = .01, p =.81), hence random slopes were dropped from
subsequent models testing fixed (M3; H3a, β = .28, p< .001) and random interaction effects (M4;
μ= .01, p = .72). Table 2 provides parameter estimates and Figure 3 shows the moderation effect of
demand on the relation between calling and self-reported task performance. To illustrate further
the shape of the interaction, we estimated the effects at different levels of the moderator (Aiken
et al., 1991). When job demand is low (1 SD below the mean), the calling–performance relation is
β = .57, 95% CI [.31, .78]. When job demand is high (1 SD above the mean), the calling–
performance relationship is β = .003, 95% CI [ .21, .21].
Consistently with our predictions, we observed that calling is positively related to self-reported
task performance (H1a), that job demand is negatively related to self-reported task performance
(H2a), and that job demand suppresses the positive effects of calling on performance (H3a).

Table 2. Parameter Estimates for Fixed Main and Interaction Effects on Self-reported Task Performance,
Manager’s Ratings of Task Performance and Productivity (M3).

95% CI
Variable Estimate SE t p
LL UL

Self-reported task performance


Intercept .02 .04 .09 .05 .59 .59
Demand .28 .06 .40 .17 .4.94 <.01
Calling .29 .06 .18 .40 5.17 <.01
Demand*Calling .28 .08 .43 .13 3.60 <.01
Manager’s rating of task performance
Intercept .13 .08 .03 .28 1.66 .10
Demand .14 .06 .26 .03 2.47 <.05
Calling .12 .06 .004 .23 2.04 <.05
Demand*Calling .004 .08 .15 .16 .05 .96
Productivity
Intercept .06 .08 .09 .21 .78 .44
Demand .07 .07 .08 .21 .91 .36
Calling .19 .07 .05 .32 2.68 <.01
Demand*Calling .04 .09 .22 .15 .38 .71
Note. CI = confidence interval; LL = lower limit; UL = upper limit.
12 Journal of Career Assessment 0(0)

Figure 3. Job Demand Suppresses the Relation Between Calling and Self-Reported Task Performance.
Note: “Low” categories represent participants who are 1 SD below the sample mean. “High” categories
represent participants who are +1 SD above the sample mean (Aiken et al., 1991).

Managers’ Ratings of Task Performance


M0 partitioned the variance of manager’s ratings of task performance into between- (μ = .21, p<
.001) and within-subjects variance (μ = .78, p < .001), indicating that 21% of the overall variation
in managers-rated performance lies between managers or branches (ICC=.21). M1 tested the fixed
effects of calling (H1b, β = .12, p =.04) and demand (H2b, β = .14, p = .01), which turned out to
be significant predictors of managers’ rating of employee’s task performance. M2 allowed to
observe that these effects do not vary across branches (μdemand = .01, p =.72; μcalling = .002, p =
.96). The fixed interaction effect between calling and demand was not significant (M3; H3b, β =
.004, p = .96) and did not vary across branches (M4; μ = .06, p = .23).
Consistently with our predictions, we observed that calling is positively related to managers’
ratings of task performance (H1b), and demand is negatively related to managers’ ratings of task
performance (H2b). Contrary to our expectation (H3b), job demand does not moderate the effects
of calling on managers’ ratings of task performance. When job demand is low (1 SD below the
mean), the calling–performance relationship is β = .12, 95% CI [ .10, .37]. When job demand is
high (1 SD above the mean), the calling-performance relationship is very similar, β = .15, 95% CI
[ .07, .42].

Productivity
Hypotheses regarding productivity were tested in the third set of nested models. We observed that
the between-branches variance is μ = .16 (p< .001), whereas the within-subjects variance in
productivity is μ = .82 (p< .001, ICC = .16), indicating that 16% of the variance in productivity
occurs between branches. According to the same strategy adopted for previous dependent variables,
M1 specified the random intercepts, and added the fixed effects of calling (H1c, β = .18, p = .001)
and demand (H2c, β = .07, p = .36). Only calling turned out to predict productivity. M2 showed that
between-branches variance of the main effects of demand and calling are null (μdemand = .003,
Vianello et al. 13

p =.92; μcalling = .002, p = .97). The fixed interaction effect is nonsignificant (M3; H3c, β =-.04,
p = .71), and does not vary across branches (M4; μ = .004, p = .94).
Consistently with our prediction, we observed that calling positively relates to productivity
(H1c) and, contrary to our expectations, job demand does not affect productivity and does not
moderate the effect of calling on productivity (H3c). When job demand is 1 SD below the mean,
the calling–performance relationship is β = .04, 95% CI [ .23, .30]. When job demand is high (1
SD above the mean), the calling-performance relationship is β = .08, 95% CI [ .14, .31].

Differential Effects of Calling and Demand on Measures of Performance


Hypotheses 4 and 5 state that the effects of calling and demand on self-reported task performance
will be stronger than the effects on other task performance measures (rated by the manager and
productivity). To examine these hypotheses, two multivariate multilevel models (one for calling
and one for demand) were estimated and post analysis contrasts were computed to compare the
fixed effects across measures of performance (Baldwin et al., 2014). Self-reported task perfor-
mance, manager’s ratings of task performance, and productivity were allowed to correlate.
Measures of task performance, calling, and demand were standardized to interpret the differences
between fixed effects.
The fixed effect of calling on self-reported task performance (β = .21, p< .001), manager’s
rating of task performance (β = .09, p = .001), and productivity (β = .11, p = .001) were all
statistically significant. The effect of calling on self-reported task performance was stronger than
the effect of calling on manager’s ratings of task performance (Δβ = .13, SE = .002, p = .002) and
stronger than the effect of calling on productivity (Δβ = .11, SE = .05, p = .03).
The fixed effect of demand on self-reported task performance (β = .24, p< .001) and on
manager’s rating of task performance (β = .12, p< .001) were statistically significant. The effect
of demand on productivity (β < .001, p = .996) was not statistically significant. The effect of
demand on self-reported task performance was stronger than the effect on managers’ ratings of
task performance (Δβ = .12, SE = .04, p = .021) and stronger than the effect of demand on
productivity (Δβ = .24, SE = .06, p< .001). As predicted in hypotheses 4 and 5, the relation
between calling, demand, and self-reported task performance was stronger than the relation
between calling, demand, and managers’ ratings of task performance and productivity.

Discussion
In this study, we investigated the role of job demand as a moderator of the relation between calling
and three measures of task performance: self-rated, manager-rated, and productivity. We observed
a positive relation between calling and all measures of task performance. The size of the relation
between calling and self-rated task performance is within the confidence interval around the meta-
analytic estimate of the correlation observed in the literature. Hence, our results add consistency to
this estimate. Interestingly, we observed that the relation between calling and performance is
higher when performance is self-reported by the employee compared to when it is measured using
managers’ ratings or productivity. This result could be due to method variance that both calling
and self-rated performance have in common, but it could also be explained by an altered per-
ception of ability that has previously been observed to accompany high levels of calling (Dobrow
& Heller, 2015). Called employees might prioritize their own perceptions over objective external
information about their performance. Importantly, the results of this study show that common
method variance or employees’ overestimation of their performance do not account for all the
variance shared by calling and performance, which is smaller for managers’ ratings of perfor-
mance and productivity but not null. In this regard, our results are inconsistent with the literature.
14 Journal of Career Assessment 0(0)

The meta-analytic average effect of studies that adopted managers’ ratings of employees’ per-
formance is higher than those that employed a self-report by employees. Yet, this is the first study
that employs both measures. It may be that previously published studies that measured per-
formance using managers’ ratings were run on a sample in which the relations between calling and
self-reported performance were even higher. More research is needed that directly compares self-
reports versus managers’ rating of performance to understand how calling predicts performance in
more detail.
Importantly, in this study we observed that when employees perceive high demands from the
environment, the impact of calling on performance is null. In 2009, Cooper and Withey lamented
that the Person x Situation hypothesis lacks empirical evidence. This study partially fills this gap,
providing empirical evidence that the perception of a strong situation, such as a high level of
demand, suppresses the positive effect of an individual characteristic such as calling on job
performance. Strong situations limit individuals’ behavior, forcing employees to focus on or-
ganizational expectations rather than let them free to perform their tasks in line with their sense of
calling. Interestingly, we also observed that demand has a negative, rather than positive, effect on
performance, in line with previous meta-analyses (e.g., Gilboa et al., 2008). The higher the
environmental pressure to work hard, the smaller the employees’ performance. This result could
apparently be interpreted as if low performance leads to higher pressure, but we observed no
relation between demand and actual productivity. Demands only affect subjective measures of
performance (self-reports and managers’ ratings). Hence, it seems more likely that the extent to
which employees feel pressured to perform impacts their behavior. In summary, we observed that
a demanding work environment harms performance and totally suppresses the positive effect that
callings have when demand is low.
Theoretically, our results both support and extend the theory on career calling in clarifying the
characteristics of the work environment that can either foster or inhibit the positive effect of calling
on work performance. Specifically, these results support and extend the WCT (Duffy et al., 2018),
adding to the literature substantive evidence that calling is related to work performance across
multiple independent measures. This result is not surprising if we consider that people ap-
proaching work as a calling feel passion for their job and derive from it a stronger identity and a
purpose in life. Most importantly, this study extends the WCT clarifying that self-reported
performance is higher in people with higher levels of calling if and only if job demand is low.
Generalizing the concept of demand to situations within an interactionist perspective, it seems
reasonable to state that calling matters most in weak situations and least in strong situations. In
strong situations, interindividual variation in behavior is reduced, because strong environmental
cues for appropriate behavior are provided: People are not free to behave according to their calling.
In weak situations, greater discretion and fewer cues regarding appropriate behaviors are pro-
vided. As such, performance can be determined by individual differences in calling. Previous
evidence supporting an interactionist perspective of the effect of calling on behavior comes from
Chang et al. (2020): The authors observed that calling significantly contributes to employees’
crafting behaviors through career commitment, but only when job autonomy is high. Higher
autonomy is a defining feature of weak situations: When job autonomy is high, individuals feel
free to actively shape and redefine job tasks and settings to reflect their calling and the subjective
meaning and purpose they attach to work. On the contrary, the constraints of strong situations
make it less likely for individuals to incorporate and emphasize aspects of their calling at work,
because adaptive moves represent a deviant behavior that may be difficult or impossible to
undertake.
Understanding that calling may impact behavior only in weak situations is crucial for the
development of the field. Thompson and Bunderson (2019) were concerned that the existing
literature suffered from a lack of evidence regarding the relation between calling and behavior. Our
Vianello et al. 15

study highlights the need to analyze this relation across different levels of situational strength, and
suggest integrating situational strength as a moderator of the calling–performance relation in the
WCT.
An interactionist perspective also helps to consider that individuals can alter social situations
according to their personal characteristics, and tend to choose settings that are congruent with their
traits (Stewart & Barrick, 2004). This is relevant for people working in teams, where the behavior
of one individual can alter the situation for others, ultimately impacting their colleagues’ behavior.
Empirical evidence that calling can alter a situation and guide people’s preference for a setting has
already been found in the literature. For instance, there is evidence that leaders’ calling impact
employees’ behavior and performance (Park et al., 2018). In addition, career calling was found to
impact career choices (Dobrow & Tosti-Kharas, 2011; Duffy & Sedlacek, 2010) and job search
behaviors (Dalla Rosa et al., 2020). Future studies are recommended to consider that, according to
the Person x Situation approach, the strength of a situation can impact the likelihood of expressing
a calling and that approaching work as a calling can alter relations and the social context.
Practical implications of the results of this study call managers that want to foster performance
to constantly monitor workloads and plan their employees’ activities according to their perception
of demand, especially for individuals who feel called to their job. While planning their employees’
activities, managers should take into consideration individual factors that favor or impair per-
formance. There is extant empirical evidence suggesting that employees’ calling is one of those
factors. At this point, it would be worth wondering whether calling should be taken into account
not only during performance management activities, but also in selection practices. The literature
on personnel selection clearly suggests that General Mental Ability (GMA) is the best predictor of
performance (Schmidt & Hunter, 1998), and that only a few selection tools contribute to predict
performance beyond GMA: structured interviews, moral integrity, conscientiousness, emotional
intelligence and goal orientations (O’Boyle et al., 2011; Payne et al., 2007; Van Iddekinge et al.,
2012). We suggest future research to study whether applicants’ calling for the job predicts
performance above GMA and other common selection tools, that is, whether calling has in-
cremental predictive validity. Should this be the case, measures of career calling could be
successfully employed to select individuals for a job.

Limitations and Future Directions


The results of the current study need to be considered in light of some limitations. Longitudinal
precedence between calling, performance, and demand cannot be established due to the cross-
sectional nature of these data. In this study, we assumed calling and demand to be antecedents of
performance. However, the opposite might also be true: self- and other-evaluations of individual’s
performance might influence how employees perceive their calling and their job’s demands. For
instance, perceiving high levels of performance might increase the sense of being called to the job
and might favor the employees to downsize the demands of their job. The idea of calling as an
antecedent of performance is in line with the WCT (Duffy et al., 2018), although empirical
evidence suggests that positive experiences such as engagement and a clear professional identity
actually promote the development of a calling rather than being a consequence of discerning a
calling (Dalla Rosa et al., 2019a). A positive feedback from a manager regarding performance can
be seen as a positive experience that promotes the development of a sense of calling for that job. In
addition, low levels of performance, both perceived and manager-rated, can be justified by an
unbalance between demand and resources, which might increase the perception of being
overloaded. Future studies should adopt experimental or longitudinal study designs to test whether
calling precedes or follows performance over time and whether performance and the experience of
perceiving a calling influence the perception of job demand.
16 Journal of Career Assessment 0(0)

Differently from previous studies, calling was found to have a stronger effect on self-rated
performance rather than on other measures of performance. This difference might be accounted for
by measurement artifacts such as different reliabilities of measures across studies, which are known
to set the upper limit of correlations. Yet, it is possible that the stronger calling/performance relation
that we observed using self-reports (vs. managers’ ratings or an objective measure of productivity)
may be due to the specific sample involved in this study. Specifically, the employees involved in this
research are required to spend the majority of their working time outside of the organization. This
limits their interaction with the managers, leading to less information that managers can use to rate
their employees. In the absence of information regarding employee’s behavior outside of the or-
ganization, managers might have been influenced by productivity in rating employees’ task per-
formance. Indeed, we observed a moderate-to-large positive correlation between managers’ ratings
of performance and productivity. Hence, the results of this study may be generalizable to salesmen,
but it seems premature to generalize them to very different jobs. To gain a more complete un-
derstanding of the relation between calling and managers’ rating of performance across different
jobs, future research investigating the role of the relation between managers and employees on the
effect of calling on performance needs to be conducted on different samples.
Finally, a limitation of our study is that we conceptualized calling towards one’s own current
job, without separately measuring the extent to which one perceives a calling and lives it out. Our
conceptualization includes both perceiving and living a calling. It is possible that explicitly
measuring the extent to which people are living out their calling would have led us to observe
higher relations between calling and performance across sources and measures.

Conclusion
This study suggests that career calling is positively related to self-rated performance, managers-
rated performance, and productivity, and that job demand suppresses the positive relation between
calling and self-reported performance. According to the Person x Situation interaction hypothesis,
individuals who approach their work as a calling perform better only when the working conditions
allow them to behave according to their inner states and traits, that is, when job demand is low and
the situation is weak (Mischel, 1977). Conversely, when job demand is high and the strong
situation limits the effect of individual differences on behavior, calling does not lead to increased
performance. Future research is needed to identify the situational boundaries of the positive effect
of calling on performance.
Identifying the settings in which calling is relevant in work organizations has important
theoretical and practical implications. Progress will be made as situational boundaries and
theoretical explanations for the relation between calling and performance are developed. As
differences in the effect of calling across settings are observed, a clearer understanding of the
proper use of calling in selection and human resource management practices may be developed.

Endnotes
1For multilevel CFA models, the covariance matrices for the within-group model and the
between-group model are computed separately; therefore, the SRMR is computed separately
for each level.

Declaration of Conflicting Interests


The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or
publication of this article.
Vianello et al. 17

Funding
The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of
this article: This work has been supported by a PhD fellowship funded by the CARIPARO foundation and awarded
to the last author. The funding source was not involved in study design, in the collection, analysis, and inter-
pretation of the data, in the writing of the report, or in the decision to submit the article for publication.

Data Availability Statement


The full data set used in this study is available at https://osf.io/aqpjr/, doi: 10.17605/OSF.IO/AQPJR. Please
contact the corresponding author (michelangelo.vianello@unipd.it) with questions regarding the survey or data.

Open Practices

All study materials, data, and analysis scripts are publicly available on the Open Science Framework:
https://osf.io/yzxnh/

ORCID iDs
Michelangelo Vianello  https://orcid.org/0000-0002-1530-1469
Anna Dalla Rosa  https://orcid.org/0000-0003-4862-4077
Sophie Gerdel  https://orcid.org/0000-0001-9027-490X

References
Aiken, L. S., West, S. G., & Reno, R. R. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions.
Thousand Oaks, US: Sage.
Bakker, A. B., & Demerouti, E. (2007). The job demands-resources model: state of the art. Journal of
Managerial Psychology, 22(3), 309-328. https://doi.org/10.1108/02683940710733115.
Bakker, A. B., Van Emmerik, H., & Van Riet, P. (2008). How job demands, resources, and burnout predict
objective performance: a constructive replication. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 21(3), 309-324. https://
doi.org/10.1080/10615800801958637.
Baldwin, S. A., Imel, Z. E., Braithwaite, S. R., & Atkins, D. C. (2014). Analyzing multiple outcomes in
clinical research using multivariate multilevel models. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
82(5), 920-930.https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035628.
Brislin, R. W. (1970). Back-translation for cross-cultural research. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology,
1(3), 185-216.https://doi.org/10.1177/135910457000100301.
Campbell, D. T., & Fiske, D. W. (1959). Convergent and discriminant validation by the multitrait-
multimethod matrix. Psychological Bulletin, 56(2), 81-105.https://doi.org/10.1037/h0046016.
Casey, C. (1995). Work, self, and society: After industrialism. Abingdon, Oxon: Routledge.
Chang, P. C., Rui, H., & Lee, A. Y. P. (2020). How having a calling leads to job crafting: a moderated
mediation model. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 552828. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.552828.
Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112(1), 155-159.https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-
2909.112.1.155.
Conway, N., Clinton, M., Sturges, J., & Budjanovcanin, A. (2015). Using self-determination theory to
understand the relationship between calling enactment and daily well-being. Journal of Organizational
Behavior, 36(8), 1114-1131. https://doi.org/10.1002/job.2014.
Conway, J. M., & Lance, C. E. (2010). What reviewers should expect from authors regarding common
method bias in organizational research. Journal of Business and Psychology, 25(3), 325-334. https://doi.
org/10.1007/s10869-010-9181-6.
18 Journal of Career Assessment 0(0)

Cooper, W. H., & Withey, M. J. (2009). The strong situation hypothesis. Personality and Social Psychology
Review, 13(1), 62-72. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868308329378.
Dalla Rosa, A., & Vianello, M. (2020). Linking calling with workaholism: examining obsessive and
harmonious passion as mediators and moderators. Journal of Career Assessment, 28(4), 589-607.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1069072720909039.
Dalla Rosa, A., & Vianello, M. (2021). [Unpublished raw data on the dark side of career calling]. University
of Padova.
Dalla Rosa, A., Vianello, M., & Anselmi, P. (2019a). Longitudinal predictors of the development of a calling:
new evidence for the a posteriori hypothesis. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 114, 44-56. https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.jvb.2019.02.007.
Dalla Rosa, A., Vianello, M., Galliani, E. M., Boffo, V., Fedeli, M., Presti, F. L., & Melacarne, C. (2019b).
Data from a three-wave complete longitudinal design survey on career calling and related constructs (N=
6368). Data in Brief, 25, 104310. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dib.2019.104310.
Dalla Rosa, A., Vianello, M., Galliani, E. M., & Duffy, R. D. (2020). Moderators of career calling and job-
search behaviors among unemployed individuals. Career Development Quarterly, 68(4), 318-331.
https://doi.org/10.1002/cdq.12239.
Dik, B. J., & Duffy, R. D. (2009). Calling and vocation at work: definitions and prospects for research and
practice. The Counseling Psychologist, 37(3), 424-450. https://doi.org/10.1177/0011000008316430.
Dik, B. J., Sargent, A. M., & Steger, M. F. (2008). Career development strivings. Journal of Career De-
velopment, 35(1), 23-41. https://doi.org/10.1177/0894845308317934.
Dobrow, S. R., & Heller, D. (2015). Follow your heart or your head? A longitudinal study of the facilitating
role of calling and ability in the pursuit of a challenging career. Journal of Applied Psychology, 100(3),
695-712. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038011.
Dobrow, S. R., & Tosti-Kharas, J. (2011). Calling: the development of ascale measure. Personnel Psychology,
64(4), 1001-1049. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1744-6570.2011.01234.x.
Duffy, R. D., Allan, B. A., & Bott, E. M. (2012). Calling and life satisfaction among undergraduate students:
investigating mediators and moderators. Journal of Happiness Studies, 13(3), 469-479. https://doi.org/
10.1007/s10902-011-9274-6.
Duffy, R. D., Dik, B. J., Douglass, R. P., England, J. W., & Velez, B. L. (2018). Work as a calling: a theoretical
model. Journal of CounselingPsychology, 65(4), 423.https://doi.org/10.1037/cou0000276.
Duffy, R. D., Dik, B. J., & Steger, M. F. (2011). Calling and work-related outcomes: career commitment as a
mediator. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 78(2), 210-218. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvb.2010.09.013.
Duffy, R. D., Douglass, R. P., Gensmer, N. P., England, J. W., & Kim, H. J. (2019). An initial examination of
the work as calling theory. Journal of CounselingPsychology, 66(3), 328.https://doi.org/10.1037/
cou0000318.
Duffy, R. D., England, J. W., Douglass, R. P., Autin, K. L., & Allan, B. A. (2017). Perceiving a calling and
well-being: motivation and access to opportunity as moderators. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 98,
127-137. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvb.2016.11.003.
Duffy, R. D., & Sedlacek, W. E. (2010). The salience of a career calling among college students: exploring
group differences and links to religiousness, life meaning, and life satisfaction. The Career Development
Quarterly, 59(1), 27-41. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2161-0045.2010.tb00128.x.
Dyer, N. G., Hanges, P. J., & Hall, R. J. (2005). Applying multilevel confirmatory factor analysis techniques
to the study of leadership. The Leadership Quarterly, 16(1), 149-167.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.
2004.09.009.
Edwards, J. A., Webster, S., Van Laar, D., & Easton, S. (2008). Psychometric analysis of the UK Health and
Safety Executive’s Management Standards work-related stress Indicator Tool. Work & Stress, 22(2),
96-107. https://doi.org/10.1080/02678370802166599
Endler, N. S., & Parker, J. D. (1992). Interactionism revisited: reflections on the continuing crisis in the per-
sonality area. European Journal of Personality, 6(3), 177-198. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.2410060302.
Vianello et al. 19

Gignac, G. E., & Szodorai, E. T. (2016). Effect size guidelines for individual differences researchers.
Personality and Individual Differences, 102, 74-78. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.06.069.
Gilboa, S., Shirom, A., Fried, Y., & Cooper, C. (2008). A meta-analysis of work demand stressors and job
performance: examining main and moderating effects. Personnel Psychology, 61(2), 227-271.https://
doi.org/10.1111/j.1744-6570.2008.00113.x.
Goštautaitė, B., Bučiunienė, I., Dalla Rosa, A., Duffy, R. D., & Kim, H. (2020). Healthcare professionals with
calling are less likely to be burned out: the role of social worth and career stage. Career Development
International, 25(6), 649-670. https://doi.org/10.1108/CDI-10-2018-0255.
Hofstede, G. (2021, July 12). Country comparison. Hofstede Insights. https://www.hofstede-insights.com/
country-comparison/italy/.
Hox, J. J. (2002). Multilevel analysis: Techniques and applications. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
INAIL. (2017). La Metodologia per la Valutazione e Gestione del Rischio Stress Lavoro-Correlato. Milan, Italy:
Tipografia INAIL. https://www.inail.it/cs/internet/docs/alg-pubbl-la-metodologia-per-la-valutazione-e-
gestione_6443112509962.pdf.
Jeon, M. J., Lee, G., Hwang, J. W., & Kang, S. J. (2009). Estimating reliability of school-level scores using
multilevel and generalizability theory models. Asia Pacific Education Review, 10(2), 149-158. https://
doi.org/10.1007/s12564-009-9014-3.
Kim, S. S., Shin, D., Vough, H. C., Hewlin, P. F., & Vandenberghe, C. (2018). How do callings relate to job
performance? The role of organizational commitment and ideological contract fulfillment. Human
Relations, 71(10), 1319-1347. https://doi.org/10.1177/0018726717743310.
Lee, A. Y. P., Chen, I. H., & Chang, P. C. (2018). Sense of calling in the workplace: the moderating effect of
supportive organizational climate in Taiwanese organizations. Journal of Management and Organi-
zation, 24(1), 129-144. https://doi.org/10.1017/jmo.2016.16.
Liu, Y., Ye, L., & Guo, M. (2019). The influence of occupational calling on safety performance among train
drivers: the role of work engagement and perceived organizational support. Safety Science, 120(3),
374-382. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ssci.2019.07.025.
Lobene, E. V., & Meade, A. W. (2013). The effects of career calling and perceived overqualification on work
outcomes for primary and secondary school teachers. Journal of Career Development, 40(6), 508-530.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0894845313495512.
Marsh, H. W., Hau, K. T., & Wen, Z. (2004). In search of golden rules: comment on hypothesis-testing
approaches to setting cutoff values for fit indexes and dangers in overgeneralizing Hu and Bentler’s (1999)
findings. Structural Equation Modeling, 11(3), 320-341.https://doi.org/10.1207/s15328007sem1103_2.
Miscenko, D., & Day, D. V. (2016). Identity and identification at work. Organizational Psychology Review,
6(3), 215-247. https://doi.org/10.1177/2041386615584009.
Mischel, W. (1977). The interaction of person and situation. In D. Magnusson, & N. S. Endler (Eds.),
Personality at the cross-roads: Current issues in interactional psychology (pp. 333-352). Hillsdale,
New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum.
O’Boyle, E. H. Jr., Humphrey, R. H., Pollack, J. M., Hawver, T. H., & Story, P. A. (2011). The relation
between emotional intelligence and job performance: a meta-analysis. Journal of Organizational
Behavior, 32(5), 788-818. https://doi.org/10.1002/job.714.
Park, J., Kim, S., Lim, M., & , and Sohn, Y. W. (2019). Having a calling on board: effects of calling on job
satisfaction and job performance among south Korean newcomers. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 1584.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01584
Park, J., Lee, K., Lim, J. I., & Sohn, Y. W. (2018). Leading with callings: effects of leader’s calling on
followers’ team commitment, voice behavior, and job performance. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1706.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01706.
Park, J., Sohn, Y. W., & Ha, Y. J. (2016). South Korean salespersons’ calling, job performance, and or-
ganizational citizenship behavior: the mediating role of occupational self-efficacy. Journal of Career
Assessment, 24(3), 415-428. https://doi.org/10.1177/1069072715599354.
20 Journal of Career Assessment 0(0)

Payne, S. C., Youngcourt, S. S., & Beaubien, J. M. (2007). A meta-analytic examination of the goal orientation
nomological net. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92(1), 128-150. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.92.1.128.
Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., & Podsakoff, N. P. (2012). Sources of method bias in social science
research and recommendations on how to control it. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 539-569. https://
doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100452.
Romney, A. C. (2020). It’s not just what you say, it’s how you say it: How callings influence constructive
voice delivery. Human Relations, 74(12), 2021-2050.https://doi.org/10.1177/0018726720945775.
Rondinone, B. M., Persechino, B., Castaldi, T., Valenti, A., Ferrante, P., Ronchetti, M., & Iavicoli, S. (2012).
Work-related stress risk assessment in Italy: the validation study of health safety and executive indicator
tool. Giornale italiano di medicina del lavoro ed ergonomia, 34(4), 392-399.
Rosso, B. D., Dekas, K. H., & Wrzesniewski, A. (2010). On the meaning of work: a theoretical integration
and review. Research in Organizational Behavior, 30, 91-127. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.riob.2010.09.
001.
Salgado, J. F., Moscoso, S., & Anderson, N. (2016). Corrections for criterion reliability in validity gen-
eralization: the consistency of Hermes, the utility of Midas. Journal of Work and Organizational
Psychology, 32(1), 17-23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rpto.2015.12.001.
Schmidt, F. L., & Hunter, J. E. (1998). The validity and utility of selection methods in personnel psychology:
practical and theoretical implications of 85 years of research findings. Psychological Bulletin, 124(2),
262.https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.124.2.262.
Stewart, G. L., & Barrick, M. R. (2004). Four lessons learned from the person-situation debate: a review and
research agenda. In B. Schneider, & D.B. Smith (Eds.), Personality and organizations (pp. 61-85).
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Thompson, J. A., & Bunderson, J. S. (2019). Research on work as a calling, and how to make it matter.
Annual Review of Organizational Psychology and Organizational Behavior, 6, 421-443. https://doi.org/
10.1146/annurev-orgpsych-012218-015140.
Van Iddekinge, C. H., Roth, P. L., Raymark, P. H., & Odle-Dusseau, H. N. (2012). The criterion-related
validity of integrity tests: an updated meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 97(3), 499-530.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021196.
Vianello, M., & Dalla Rosa, A. (2020). Career calling and performance.[Unpublished raw data].University
of Padova.
Vianello, M., Dalla Rosa, A., Anselmi, P., & Galliani, E. M. (2018). Validity and measurement invariance of
the unified multidimensional calling scale (UMCS): a three-wave survey study. PlosOne, 13(12),
1-13.https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0209348.
Vianello, M., Galliani, E. M., Dalla Rosa, A., & Anselmi, P. (2020). The developmental trajectories of calling:
predictors and outcomes. Journal of Career Assessment, 28(1), 128-146. https://doi.org/10.1177/
1069072719831276.
Weiss, H. M., & Adler, S. (1984). Personality and organizational behavior. Research in Organizational
Behavior, 6, 1-50.
West, B. T., Welch, K. B., & Galecki, A. T. (2007). Linear mixed models. A practical guide using statistical
software. Boca Raton, FL: Chapman and Hall/CRC.
Williams, L. J., & Anderson, S. E. (1991). Job satisfaction and organizational commitment as predictors of
organizational citizenship and in-role behaviors. Journal of Management, 17(3), 601-617.https://doi.
org/10.1177/014920639101700305.
Wu, G., Hu, Z., & Zheng, J. (2019). Role stress, job burnout, and job performance in construction project
managers: the moderating role of career calling. International Journal of Environmental Research and
Public Health, 16(13), 2394.https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph16132394.

You might also like