Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 5

Personality and Individual Differences 134 (2018) 55–59

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Personality and Individual Differences


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid

Development and validation of the Chinese social media addiction scale T


a b,⁎
Chang Liu , Jianling Ma
a
Yangtze Normal University, Fu'ling District, Chongqing 408100, China
b
Chongqing University of Posts and Telecommunications, Chongwen Road, Nan'an District, Chongqing 400065, China

A R T I C LE I N FO A B S T R A C T

Keywords: Accumulating evidence has shown that social media addiction is a growing problem in China, especially among
Social media emerging adults. Nonetheless, the absence of a psychometrically sound measuring tool obstructs further progress
Addiction in this field. Therefore, we examined the properties of a newly developed social media addiction scale among a
Problematic use sample of 619 college students. An exploratory factor analysis revealed that the scale had six factors: preference
Internet addiction
for online social interaction, mood alteration, negative consequence and continued use, compulsive use and
withdrawal, salience, and relapse, which explained 63.83% of the total variance. Cronbach's alpha and half-split
coefficient of the whole scale were 0.937 and 0.879, respectively. A confirmatory factor analysis indicated that
the six-factor model was a good fit to the data. Moreover, the social media addiction scores were positively
correlated with smartphone addiction, pathological Internet use, and narcissism, but negatively associated with
self-esteem. Our study suggests that the newly developed social media addiction scale could be used as an
effective tool to assess social media addiction among emerging adults in China.

1. Introduction not synonymous with social media addiction (Griffiths, 2012, 2013;
Kuss & Griffiths, 2011); therefore, a broader platform and psychome-
In the past decade, we have witnessed the emergence and explosion trically sound scale is needed to assess this growing problem (Kuss &
of social media, which allows for instant communication, the ability to Griffiths, 2017). Moreover, the dimensions of existing tools vary con-
maintain friendships and interact with friends in real-time, and seeking siderably because these measurements were based on diverse theore-
new acquaintances based on common interests (Ellison, 2007). The use tical backgrounds. In addition, the abovementioned tools were typically
of social media in China is so ubiquitous and popular that people can developed in Western culture, and cross-culture validation of the ex-
access it whenever they want. However, such seemingly incessant isting scales is still necessary.
connection could be problematic due to empirical findings that ex- According to the latest released official report from the China
cessive use could lead to envy, depression, and anxiety (Kross et al., Internet Network Information Center (CINIC), there are almost 0.751
2013; Marino, Gini, Vieno, & Spada, 2018). Consequently, pathological billion netizens in China, accounting for 1/5 of the netizens globally
social media use or addiction to it is a public health concern. (CNNIC, 2017). Of these, 30.3% are aged 20–29 years. The top three
Several screening tools have been explored to assess this evolving most popular used social media cites were Wechat (utilization ratio:
issue. Some instruments cater to specific social media platforms, such as 85.5%), QQ (utilization ratio: 67.8%), and Sina Weibo (utilization ratio:
the wide spread use of the Bergen Facebook Addiction Scale 37.1%) (CNNIC, 2017). Wechat is a Chinese multi-purpose messaging,
(Andreassen, Torsheim, Brunborg, & Pallesen, 2012), which has already social media, and mobile payment app. It was first released in 2011; by
been extensively adapted to diverse culture backgrounds (Phanasathit, 2018, it was one of the world's largest standalone mobile apps by
Manwong, Hanprathet, Khumsri, & Yingyeun, 2015; Pontes, monthly active users (over 1 billion). Like Wechat, QQ is another well-
Andreassen, & Griffiths, 2016; Salem, Almenaye, & Andreassen, 2016). known social media platform consisting of online social games, music,
Others have been constructed based on the Internet gaming disorder shopping, micro-blogging, movies, and group and voice chat software,
criterion, such as the social media disorder scale (van den Eijnden, which has a longer history than Wechat. Sina Weibo implements many
Lemmens, & Valkenburg, 2016). Moreover, some have been developed features from Twitter (i.e., people follow celebrities). Despite the ex-
by adapting existing Internet addiction scales to social media domains tensive reports of the negative effects of social media use by the media,
(Assunção & Matos, 2017; Caci, Cardaci, Scrima, & Tabacchi, 2017; insufficient research has been conducted in China owing to the lack of
Marino, Vieno, Altoè, & Spada, 2016). However, Facebook addiction is reliable tools to gauge this issue. Consequently, we developed and


Corresponding author at: Education Development Department, Chongqing University of Posts and Telecommunications, Chongwen Road, Nan'an District, Chongqing 400065, China.
E-mail address: jianling_ma@163.com (J. Ma).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2018.05.046
Received 20 November 2017; Received in revised form 24 May 2018; Accepted 30 May 2018
Available online 05 June 2018
0191-8869/ © 2018 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
C. Liu, J. Ma Personality and Individual Differences 134 (2018) 55–59

validated a scale to measure social media addiction among Chinese examined the translation of the items when it was not written in
college students to bridge this gap. Chinese. All tests were written and administered in Chinese. Another
After an extensive review of extant measurement tools addressing expert was responsible for translating final social media scale items into
behavior, the Internet, and social media addiction (Griffiths, 2000; English for publication purpose. The wording and arrangement of the
Hormes, Kearns, & Timko, 2014; Weinstein & Lejoyeux, 2010; Young, items was checked prior to administration to avoid any grammar am-
1998), seven classic dimensions of behavioral addiction were included: biguity, errors, repetition, etc. Items were evaluated on a 5-point Likert
compulsive use, withdrawal, negative consequence, mood alteration, scale ranging from (1) strongly disagree to (5) strongly agree. The draft
salience, tolerance, and relapse. Moreover, another two dimensions scale could be finished in approximately 20 min. Sample 2 also com-
were considered. First, recent findings support that “preference for pleted a paper version of the scale. In addition, other measures were
online social interaction” (Caplan, 2003; Casale, Tella, & Fioravanti, included: the Smartphone Addiction Scale, the Adolescent Pathological
2013; Chung, 2013; Fioravanti, Dèttore, & Casale, 2012; Yu et al., Internet Use Scale (APIU), a Self-esteem Scale, and a Narcissistic
2017) could also contribute to Internet addiction tendencies (e.g., Personality Questionnaire for convergent and criterion-related validity
“online social interactions are more comfortable than face-to-face in- analyses.
teractions”). Moreover, a recent study also provided initial evidence Average daily social network use time was collected since this is a
that preference for online interactions was positively associated with valid index of social media addiction (Huang, 2017). Respondents were
excessive social media use (Assunção & Matos, 2017; Chittaro & asked to indicate how much total time they used social media daily: (1)
Vianello, 2013). Therefore, we added this dimension to our scale. < 15 min, (2) 15–30 min, (3) 30 min–1 h, (4) 1–2 h, (5) 2–3 h, or (6)
Second, we included “continued use.” It is noteworthy that many > 3 h.
individuals continue to use social media even though they are aware of The Smartphone Addiction Scale was used to measure cellphone
its negative consequences, which are more likely to be psychosocial addiction (Kwon et al., 2013). Items were evaluated on a 6-point Likert
than physical in nature. This construct has been incorporated in many Scale, ranging from (1) strongly disagree to (6) strongly agree. Cronbach's
Internet addiction surveys. Considering recent evidence that social alpha was 0.93 in this study.
media addiction shares some similarities with Internet addiction and The APIU was used to assess excessive Internet use (Lei & Yang,
Internet gaming disorder (Przepiorka & Blachnio, 2016; Sigerson, Li, 2007). Items were evaluated on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from (1)
Cheung, & Cheng, 2017), this dimension was included in this study. The strongly disagree to (5) strongly agree. Cronbach's alpha was 0.95 in this
continued use dimension differs from both compulsive use and relapse. study.
Continued use specifically refers to a situation in which an individual is Self-esteem was measured using a Chinese adaption of
aware of the negative outcome of excessive social media use, but still Rosenberger's Self-esteem Scale (Wu, 2008), which comprised ten
cannot stop using it. Compulsive use is a subjective feeling that one is items. Responses ranged from 1 (totally disagree) to 4 (totally agree).
eager to use social media without awareness of the negative outcomes. Items are added to obtain a total score. Cronbach's alpha was 0.78 in
Relapse relates to a situation in which one aims to reduce social media this study.
use; however, they are not successful. The Narcissistic Personality Questionnaire consisting of 28 items
In sum, we developed and validated a Chines social media addiction including overt and covert narcissism subscales (20 and 15 items, re-
scale to inspire innovative research in this area in China. We expected spectively; 7 shared items) was used to measure narcissism (Zheng & Li,
that the developed scale would have good reliability and validity. 2005). Items were evaluated on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from (1)
strongly disagree to (5) strongly agree. The wide use of this questionnaire
2. Method in China and its reliability and validity have been demonstrated (Wang,
Jackson, Zhang, & Su, 2012; You, Leung, Lai, & Fu, 2013). Cronbach's
2.1. Participants alphas for the whole scale and the overt and covert subscales sub-
scale = 0.88, 0.87, and 0.81, respectively.
A convenience sample of 318 (sample 1, men = 59, women = 259,
aged 18–25 years, Mage = 20.43, SD = 1.538 years) and 301 (sample 2, 2.3. Data analyses
boys/men = 219, girls/women = 82, aged 16–28 years, Mage = 26.92,
SD = 1.334 years) was employed. Chinese college students participated Data analyses were performed in two steps. First, an exploratory
in exchange for course credit. Since the sex distribution was unequal, factor analysis (EFA) was performed with sample 1 data to determine
independent t-tests were performed to examine possible sex differences: the factor structure of the scale using SPSS 20.0. Then, the structure of
in sample 1, Mmale = 80.98, SD = 19.92; Mfemale = 77.97, SD = 18.35; the scale was validated with a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) using
t(316) = 1.19, p = .264; and, in sample 2, Mmale = 78.26, SD = 18.30; AMOS 20.0 software in sample 2.
Mfemale = 82.08, SD = 14.34; t(299) = 1.71, p = .09. Therefore, there
were no sex differences. Everyone had at least half a year of social 3. Results
media use experience. In this study, “social media” specifically refers to
these three popular platforms in China. This study was approved by a 3.1. Scale structure
school research committee. Prior to data collection, formal consent was
collected from participants, and a notice explicating the study aims and All items were significantly correlated with total score and retained
the assurance that personal data would remain confidential and anon- after the item-total correlation analysis. The Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin mea-
ymous was sent to participants. sure (0.933) of sample adequacy and Bartlett's Test of Sphericity
(χ2 = 478.16, df = 378, p < .001) indicated that the sample was sui-
2.2. Measures table for an EFA. Given that factors were correlated, the principal axis
factoring method was used to extract factors. A direct oblimin rotation
Students in sample 1 completed a paper-and-pencil version of the was adopted. Retention of the factors was based on following criterion:
58-item draft of the scale, which comprised the 8 dimensions men- eigenvalues > 1, a visual inspection of the scree plot, explaining at
tioned above. Items per each dimension varied from 5 to 8. Items were least 3% variance before rotation, factor loading value > 0.5, and in-
borrowed from the Internet Addiction Test, the Facebook Addiction cluding at least 3 items. Items with factor loadings < 0.5 and > 0.4 on
Scale, and the Internet Gaming Disorder Scale (continued use dimen- two or more factors were deleted. Cronbach's alpha was calculated to
sion); however, the terms “Internet,” “Facebook,” and “Internet estimate internal consistency.
gaming” were replaced with “social media.” Two bilingual experts The extracted six factors were clear, identifiable, and easy-to-name.

56
C. Liu, J. Ma Personality and Individual Differences 134 (2018) 55–59

Table 1
Exploratory factor analysis of the social media addiction scale.
Items Factor

1 2 3 4 5 6

42. I get more respect through social media. 0.681


50. Friends on social media sites are friendlier to me. 0.675
36 I feel more confident through social media communication. 0.632
37. I feel more comfortable through social media communication. 0.632
45. I feel safer through social media communication. 0.620
8. Friends on social media sites give me more support. 0.596
33. When I am not happy, the use of social media can make me happy. 0.849
28. When I am in a bad mood, using social media can make me feel better. 0.839
4. When I get bored, using social media can make me feel better. 0.613
58. Social media can keep me from unpleasant emotions. 0.567
15. When I feel lonely, using social media can ease my sense of loneliness. 0.500
46. Although social media has had some negative impact on me, I will continue to use it. 0.825
54. Although social media has a negative impact on me, I still check/send posts, reply to others, etc. 0.795
52 Although social media has a negative impact on me, I continue browsing, commenting, etc. 0.781
44. I have been distracted due to the use of social media. 0.455
47. I have reduced sleep time more than once due to the use of social media. 0.455
39. If there is no time to use social media, I feel like I missed something. 0.758
43. If I cannot use social media, I feel anxious. 0.752
35. If I cannot use social media, I will feel lost. 0.685
48. If there is a period I am without social media, I want to know what is happening on it. 0.669
22. If I cannot use social media, I would miss it. 0.548
14. When I am not allowed to use social media, I feel a little irritable. 0.511
32. I almost forget all the other things when using social media. 0.914
38. I will not think about anything else when using social media. 0.639
56. When using social media, I am almost entirely devoted to it. 0.455
20. I have tried to reduce the use of social media, but not successfully. 0.860
26. I endeavored to control the time I use social media; however, I failed. 0.725
17. I attempted to reduce the times I logged on to social media; however, I failed. 0.662
Eigenvalues 10.58 2.02 1.66 1.35 1.17 1.1
Explained variance (%) 37.79 7.22 5.92 4.82 4.17 3.91
Cronbach's alpha 0.83 0.84 0.83 0.86 0.79 0.82

Note: Factor extraction method: principal axis factoring; rotation method: direct oblimin.

Table 2 reactions when not using social media. Salience (Factor 5, 3 items)
Correlations among social media addiction scale dimensions and total score. constituted items that described the fascination with using social media.
1 2 3 4 5 6
Lastly, relapse (Factor 6, 3 items) entailed items characterizing the
repeated occurrence of social media use. Cronbach's alpha and half-split
1. Preference for online 1 coefficient of the whole scale were 0.937 and 0.879, respectively. In
social interactions addition, the correlations between each dimension and the total score
2. Mood alteration 0.546⁎⁎ 1
3. Negative outcomes 0.404⁎⁎ 0.447⁎⁎ 1
are listed in Table 2.
and continued use
⁎⁎ ⁎⁎
4. Compulsive use/ 0.560 0.555 0.547⁎⁎ 1
withdrawal 3.2. Scale validation
5. Salience 0.539⁎⁎ 0.368⁎⁎ 0.433⁎⁎ 0.619⁎⁎ 1
6. Relapse 0.451⁎⁎ 0.387⁎⁎ 0.416⁎⁎ 0.570⁎⁎ 0.437⁎⁎ 1
7. Total score 0.787⁎⁎ 0.751⁎⁎ 0.726⁎⁎ 0.857⁎⁎ 0.714⁎⁎ 0.685⁎⁎ To validate the scale structure, the following indices were calculated
for the six-factor model: χ2, comparative fit index (CFI), adjusted
⁎⁎
p < .01. N = 301. goodness of fit index (AGFI), and root of means square error of ap-
proximation (RMSEA). Our analysis revealed the following results:
Table 1 shows the six-factor model with an explained total variance to χ2 = 676.979, df = 335, χ2/df = 2.02 < 3, p < .001, CFI = 0.929,
63.83%. Preference for online social interaction (Factor 1, 6 items) AGFI = 0.935, and RMSEA = 0.042, thus satisfying the model's fit-to-
referred to preferring social media communication over face-to-face data.
interactions. Mood alteration (Factor 2,5 items) denoted that social The correlational analysis showed that total scores were significant
media could be used for emotion modification. The negative con- negatively related to self-esteem (r = −0.161, p < .01), and positively
sequence and continued use (Factor 3, 5 items) factor described the associated with narcissism, smartphone addiction (r = 0.486, p < .01)
detrimental effect of social media on daily functioning and consecutive and adolescents' pathological Internet use (r = 0.176, p < .01)
using despite adverse outcomes. The compulsive and withdrawal factor (Table 3).
indicated preoccupation (Factor 4, 6 items) with using and emotional

Table 3
The association between total score and daily use time, self-esteem, narcissism, APIU, and smartphone addiction.
Daily use time Self-esteem Overt narcissism Covert narcissism Narcissism APIU Smartphone addiction

Total score 0.275⁎⁎ −0.161⁎⁎ 0.380⁎⁎ 0.480⁎⁎ 0.441⁎⁎ 0.176⁎⁎ 0.486⁎⁎

⁎⁎
p < .01, N = 301.

57
C. Liu, J. Ma Personality and Individual Differences 134 (2018) 55–59

Table 4
The results of the multiple regression analysis related to social media addiction.
Model Predictive variable B Standard error B β t Sig. t R R square R square change F

1 Constant 40.680 4.198 9.691 < 0.001 0.487 0.237 0.237 88.39⁎⁎
Smartphone addiction 0.352 0.037 0.487 9.402 < 0.001
2 Constant 14.472 5.071 2.854 0.005 0.611 0.373 0.136 84.31⁎⁎
Smartphone addiction 0.284 0.035 0.394 8.107 < 0.001
Covert narcissism 0.788 0.101 0.381 7.838 < 0.001
3 Constant 8.232 5.279 1.559 0.120 0.632 0.400 0.026 62.59⁎⁎
Smartphone addiction 0.263 0.035 0.364 7.533 < 0.001
Covert narcissism 0.767 0.099 0.370 7.767 < 0.001
Daily use time 2.156 0.613 0.166 3.516 < 0.001

⁎⁎
p < .01, N = 301. Dependent variable: social media addiction.

3.3. Self-esteem, narcissism, smartphone addiction, and social media higher narcissism (as opposed to lower) was correlated with extensive
addiction use of online communities and presenting more self-promoting content
(Carpenter, 2012; Ong et al., 2011). Recently, the relationship between
To investigate the contribution of self-esteem, covert and overt narcissism and excessive use of social media sites was supported
narcissism, daily time use, Internet addiction, and smartphone addic- through several empirical studies (Andreassen, Pallesen, & Griffiths,
tion on social media addiction, these six variables were used as pre- 2017). Although several studies have already explored the relationship
dictors. A multiple stepwise linear regression analysis was conducted between self-esteem, narcissism, and addictive use of social media
after the appropriateness of the data was examined. (Andreassen, Pallesen, & Griffiths, 2017; Eşkisu, Hoşoğlu, & Rasmussen,
Then, the data were analyzed through a multiple regression ana- 2017), we elucidated how self-esteem and specific types of narcissism
lysis. As can be seen in Table 4, smartphone addiction, covert narcis- (i.e., covert and overt) contribute to general social media addiction in
sism, and daily use time were all key predictors of social media ad- China. Interestingly, covert rather than overt narcissism contributed
diction—explaining 40% of the total variance related to social media more to social media addiction. Covert narcissism, or vulnerable nar-
addiction. Smartphone addiction explained 23.7% of the total variance cissism, is characterized by a defensive and insecure sense of grandi-
and appeared to be the most important predictor. Covert narcissism, the osity that is associated with low self-esteem, shame proneness, shyness,
second important variable that predicted addiction behavior, explained and hypersensitivity to the evaluation of others. Our results were con-
13.6% of the total variance. The third variable, daily use time, ex- sistent with a recent study (Casale, Fioravanti, & Rugai, 2016) that
plained only 2.6% of the total variance. found that vulnerable narcissism may contribute more to problematic
use of social media than grandiose narcissism. However, the mechanism
underlying the relationship between narcissism that social media ad-
4. Discussion diction is still unclear and requires further investigation.
Further, the relationship between social media addiction, smart-
The present scale included six core dimensions: preference for on- phone addiction, and Internet addiction is consisted with recent study
line social interaction, mood alteration, negative consequence and findings (Sigerson, Li, Cheung, & Cheng, 2017), suggesting the inter-
continued use, compulsive use and withdrawal, salience, and relapse, connections among social media addiction, smartphone addiction, In-
which were successfully confirmed by a CFA. ternet gaming disorder, and Internet addiction, which may comprise a
Although some criteria were identical to previously tools, continued global construct: information technology addiction. Although all four
use and preference for online social interaction are two dimensions that kinds of behavioral addiction share similar qualities, each has unique
are not part of the Facebook Addiction Scale. Consistent with past re- dimensions. Therefore, it is necessary to address each of them in-
search that found that users' preference for online social interaction dividually with sound measures. Moreover, to clarify the contributors
accounted for Internet and social media addiction (Caplan, 2003; to social media addiction, a regression analysis was performed, which
Casale, Tella, & Fioravanti, 2013; Chung, 2013; Fioravanti, Dèttore, & provided evidence that smartphone addiction, covert narcissism, and
Casale, 2012; Yu et al., 2017), we found that fascination with online daily use time were key predictors of social media addiction. This
communication partially accounted for social media addiction. Sur- finding is plausible because ubiquitous mobile smartphone use provides
prisingly, the tolerance dimension disappeared in the final scale. One access to social media anytime and anywhere (Salehan & Negahban,
possible explanation is that, contrary to the Facebook addiction scale 2013; Wu, Cheung, Ku, & Hung, 2013) without physical constraint.
and social media disorder scale, which are intended for one platform,
we examined participants' general addiction tendencies for several
popular platforms. These platforms vary with features and functions; 4.1. Limitations and recommendations
therefore, tolerance could exhibit on one platform while fade on an-
other. However, such tentative assumptions should be tested in future This study had some limitations. The cross-sectional, self-report
studies. design limits the ability to infer causality between variables. Moreover,
Regarding reliability and validity, all the related-indices were sa- the scale does not provide cut-off points to distinguish addictive from
tisfactory. Self-esteem and narcissism were correlated with social media non-addictive, and only the structure, internal consistency, and validity
addiction. The negative association between self-esteem and social of the scale were examined. Consequently, further studies are needed to
media addiction was in line with previous studies (Andreassen, report its test-retest reliability. Moreover, participants were college
Pallesen, & Griffiths, 2017; Eşkisu, Hoşoğlu, & Rasmussen, 2017; students. Further studies should examine whether the scale is applic-
Fioravanti, Dèttore, & Casale, 2012; Valkenburg, Peter, & Schouten, able for use with young adolescents. The scale could also be used to
2006). Self-esteem, the core evaluation of individuals' own worth, has assess behavioral addiction of other social media platform such as
long been regarded as a predictor of Internet use. It is plausible that Facebook, Twitter, and others.
people with low self-esteem think of social media sites as safer places to
express themselves than do people with high self-esteem; therefore,
they devote more time to using such media. Previous studies found that

58
C. Liu, J. Ma Personality and Individual Differences 134 (2018) 55–59

5. Conclusions Huang, C. (2017). Time spent on social network sites and psychological well-being: A
meta-analysis. Cyberpsychology, Behavior and Social Networking, 20, 346–354. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2016.0758.
This study contributes to the literature by providing a scale that Kross, E., Verduyn, P., Demiralp, E., Park, J., Lee, D. S., Lin, N., ... Ybarra, O. (2013).
measures broad social media addiction in China and has good psy- Facebook use predicts declines in subjective well-being in young adults. PLoS One, 8,
chometric properties. The Chinese social media addiction scale could be e69841. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0069841.
Kuss, D. J., & Griffiths, M. D. (2011). Online social networking and addiction—A review
used to assess social media addiction in both clinical and nonclinical of the psychological literature. International Journal of Environmental Research and
settings since it addresses variables not tacked in the well-known Public Health, 8, 3528–3552. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/ijerph8093528.
Facebook Addiction Scale. Kuss, D. J., & Griffiths, M. D. (2017). Social networking sites and addiction: Ten lessons
learned. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 14, 311.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/ijerph14030311.
Acknowledgement Kwon, M., Lee, J.-Y., Won, W.-Y., Park, J.-W., Min, J.-A., Hahn, C., ... Kim, D.-J. (2013).
Development and validation of a smartphone addiction scale (SAS). PLoS One, 8,
e56936. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0056936.
The present study were supported by the humanities and social
Lei, L., & Yang, Y. (2007). The development and validation of adolescent pathological
science research project of the Chongqing Education Committee internet use scale. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 39, 688–696.
(17SKG192, 16SKGH054) and the key social science program of Marino, C., Gini, G., Vieno, A., & Spada, M. M. (2018). A comprehensive meta-analysis on
Chongqing University of Posts and Telecommunications (2017KZD08). problematic Facebook use. Computers in Human Behavior, 83, 262–277. http://dx.doi.
org/10.1016/j.chb.2018.02.009.
Marino, C., Vieno, A., Altoè, G., & Spada, M. M. (2016). Factorial validity of the pro-
References blematic Facebook use scale for adolescents and young adults. Journal of Behavioral
Addictions, 6, 5–10. http://dx.doi.org/10.1556/2006.6.2017.004.
Ong, E. Y. L., Ang, R. P., Ho, J. C. M., Lim, J. C. Y., Goh, D. H., Lee, C. S., & Chua, A. Y. K.
Andreassen, C. S., Pallesen, S., & Griffiths, M. D. (2017). The relationship between ad- (2011). Narcissism, extraversion and adolescents' self-presentation on Facebook.
dictive use of social media, narcissism, and self-esteem: Findings from a large na- Personality and Individual Differences, 50, 180–185. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.
tional survey. Addictive Behaviors, 64, 287–293. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh. 2010.09.022.
2016.03.006. Phanasathit, M., Manwong, M., Hanprathet, N., Khumsri, J., & Yingyeun, R. (2015).
Andreassen, C. S., Torsheim, T., Brunborg, G. S., & Pallesen, S. (2012). Development of a Validation of the Thai version of Bergen Facebook addiction scale (Thai-BFAS).
Facebook addiction scale. Psychological Reports, 110, 501–517. http://dx.doi.org/10. Journal of the Medical Association of Thailand, 98, S108–S117.
2466/02.09.18.pr0.110.2.501-517. Pontes, H. M., Andreassen, C. S., & Griffiths, M. D. (2016). Portuguese validation of the
Assunção, R. S., & Matos, P. M. (2017). The generalized problematic internet use scale 2: Bergen Facebook addiction scale: An empirical study. International Journal of Mental
Validation and test of the model to Facebook use. Journal of Adolescence, 54, 51–59. Health and Addiction, 14, 1–12. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11469-016-9694-y.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2016.11.007. Przepiorka, A., & Blachnio, A. (2016). Time perspective in internet and Facebook ad-
Caci, B., Cardaci, M., Scrima, F., & Tabacchi, M. E. (2017). The dimensions of Facebook diction. Computers in Human Behavior, 60, 13–18. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.
addiction as measured by Facebook addiction Italian questionnaire and their re- 2016.02.045.
lationships with individual differences. Cyberpsychology, Behavior and Social Salehan, M., & Negahban, A. (2013). Social networking on smartphones: When mobile
Networking, 20, 251–258. http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2016.0073. phones become addictive. Computers in Human Behavior, 29, 2632–2639. http://dx.
Caplan, S. E. (2003). Preference for online social interaction. Communication Research, 30, doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2013.07.003.
625–648. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0093650203257842. Salem, A. A. M., Almenaye, N. S., & Andreassen, C. S. (2016). A psychometric evaluation
Carpenter, C. J. (2012). Narcissism on Facebook: Self-promotional and anti-social beha- of Bergen Facebook addiction scale (BFAS) of university students. International
vior. Personality and Individual Differences, 52, 482–486. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j. Journal of Psychology and Behavioral Sciences, 6, 199–205.
paid.2011.11.011. Sigerson, L., Li, A. Y. L., Cheung, M. W. L., & Cheng, C. (2017). Examining common
Casale, S., Fioravanti, G., & Rugai, L. (2016). Grandiose and vulnerable narcissists: Who is information technology addictions and their relationships with non-technology-re-
at higher risk for social networking addiction? Cyberpsychology, Behavior and Social lated addictions. Computers in Human Behavior, 75, 520–526. http://dx.doi.org/10.
Networking, 19, 510–515. http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2016.0189. 1016/j.chb.2017.05.041.
Casale, S., Tella, L., & Fioravanti, G. (2013). Preference for online social interactions Valkenburg, P. M., Peter, J., & Schouten, A. P. (2006). Friend networking sites and their
among young people: Direct and indirect effects of emotional intelligence. Personality relationship to adolescents' well-being and social self-esteem. Cyberpsychology &
and Individual Differences, 54, 524–529. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2012.10. Behavior, 9, 584–590. http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cpb.2006.9.584.
023. van den Eijnden, R. J., Lemmens, J. S., & Valkenburg, P. M. (2016). The social media
Chittaro, L., & Vianello, A. (2013). Time perspective as a predictor of problematic internet disorder scale. Computers in Human Behavior, 61, 478–487. http://dx.doi.org/10.
use: A study of Facebook users. Personality and Individual Differences, 55, 989–993. 1016/j.chb.2016.03.038.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2013.08.007. Wang, J.-L., Jackson, L. A., Zhang, D.-J., & Su, Z.-Q. (2012). The relationships among the
Chung, J. E. (2013). Social interaction in online support groups: Preference for online big five personality factors, self-esteem, narcissism, and sensation-seeking to Chinese
social interaction over offline social interaction. Computers in Human Behavior, 29, university students' uses of social networking sites (SNSs). Computers in Human
1408–1414. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2013.01.019. Behavior, 28, 2313–2319. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2012.07.001.
CNNIC (2017). China Internet Network Information Center (CNNIC). The 40 internet Weinstein, A., & Lejoyeux, M. (2010). Internet addiction or excessive internet use. The
network development statistic report of China (Retrieved from) http://www.cnnic. American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse, 36, 277–283. http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/
net.cn/hlwfzyj/hlwxzbg/hlwtjbg/201708/P020170807351923262153.pdf. 00952990.2010.491880.
Ellison, N. B. (2007). Social network sites: Definition, history, and scholarship. Journal of Wu, A. M. S., Cheung, V. I., Ku, L., & Hung, E. P. W. (2013). Psychological risk factors of
Computer-Mediated Communication, 13, 210–230. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1083- addiction to social networking sites among Chinese smartphone users. Journal of
6101.2007.00393.x. Behavioral Addictions, 2, 160. http://dx.doi.org/10.1556/jba.2.2013.006.
Eşkisu, M., Hoşoğlu, R., & Rasmussen, K. (2017). An investigation of the relationship Wu, C.-H. (2008). An examination of the wording effect in the Rosenberg self-esteem
between Facebook usage, big five, self-esteem and narcissism. Computers in Human scale among culturally Chinese people. Journal of Social Psychology, 148, 535–551.
Behavior, 69, 294–301. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.12.036. http://dx.doi.org/10.3200/socp.148.5.535-552.
Fioravanti, G., Dèttore, D., & Casale, S. (2012). Adolescent internet addiction: Testing the You, J., Leung, F., Lai, K. K. Y., & Fu, K. (2013). Factor structure and psychometric
association between self-esteem, the perception of internet attributes, and preference properties of the pathological narcissism inventory among Chinese university stu-
for online social interactions. Cyberpsychology, Behavior and Social Networking, 15, dents. Journal of Personality Assessment, 95, 309–318. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
318–323. http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2011.0358. 00223891.2012.718303.
Griffiths, M. D. (2000). Does internet and computer “addiction” exist? Some case study Young, K. S. (1998). Internet addiction: The emergence of a new clinical disorder.
evidence. CyberPsychology and Behavior, 3, 211–218. http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/ Cyberpsychology & Behavior, 1, 237–244. http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cpb.1998.1.237.
109493100316067. Yu, Q., Zhang, L., Wu, S., Guo, Y., Jin, S., & Sun, Y. (2017). The influence of juvenile
Griffiths, M. D. (2012). Facebook addiction: Concerns, criticism, and preference for online social interaction on problematic internet use: The moderating
recommendations—A response to Andreassen and colleagues. Psychological Reports, effect of sibling condition and the moderated moderating effect of age cohort.
110, 518–520. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/01.07.18.pr0.110.2.518-520. Computers in Human Behavior, 68, 345–351. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.
Griffiths, M. D. (2013). Social networking addiction: Emerging themes and issues. Journal 11.026.
of Addiction Research & Therapy, 4. http://dx.doi.org/10.4172/2155-6105.1000e118. Zheng, Y., & Li, H. (2005). Overt and convert narcissism: A psychological exploration of
Hormes, J. M., Kearns, B., & Timko, C. A. (2014). Craving Facebook? Behavioral addiction narcissistic personality. Psychological Science, 28, 1259–1262.
to online social networking and its association with emotion regulation deficits.
Addiction, 109, 2079–2088. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/add.12713.

59

You might also like