Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 6

Current Psychology (2020) 39:1215–1220

https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-018-9824-8

The development of a single item FoMO (Fear of Missing Out) scale


Benjamin C. Riordan 1 & Louise Cody 1 & Jayde A. M. Flett 1 & Tamlin S. Conner 1 & John Hunter 1 & Damian Scarf 1

Published online: 12 March 2018


# Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2018

Abstract
The Fear of Missing Out (FoMO) is the sense that others are having a rewarding experience which one is absent from. Given that
it is associated with the drive to remain socially connected, research has predominantly focused on the link between FoMO and
social networking use. While a 10-item measure of FoMO is widely used (FoMOs), a shorter scale may be preferable in some
circumstances and would allow FoMO to be measured in more diverse contexts. Therefore, we aimed to validate a FoMO short-
form (consisting of a single item: BDo you experience FoMO?^). In Studies 1 to 3, we measured the concurrent validity of the
FoMOsf with the 10-item FoMOs (Pearson’s R correlation between the FoMOs and FoMOsf: Study 1 r = .735, r = .654; Study 2
r = .638; Study 3 r = .807). In Study 2, we measured the test-retest reliability of the FoMOsf (r = .717). In Study 2 and 3, we
measured the construct validity of the FoMOsf by linking the FoMOsf to social networking use. The FoMOsf showed good
concurrent validity, construct validity, and test-retest reliability and is adequate for use in research.

Keywords Fear of missing out . FoMO . Single-item . Ecological momentary assessment . Measurement

The Fear of Missing Out (FoMO) is defined as the B…pervasive (2013) suggest that FoMO derives from a deficit in psycholog-
apprehension that others might be having rewarding experiences ical need satisfaction such as the need for social connection.
from which one is absent^ and is characterized by the need to While one could argue that social networking sites provide an
B…stay continually connected with what others are doing^ avenue for meeting some of these needs (e.g., connection), so-
(Przybylski et al. 2013, p. 1841). Although the feeling of miss- cial networking sites may also serve to exacerbate FoMO by
ing out is not a new concept, with the advent of social network- reminding individuals what experiences they are missing out
ing sites (e.g., Facebook, Snapchat, etc.), people can now be on in real time. Accordingly, research has typically focused on
chronically reminded of events they are missing out on. With correlates between FoMO and unhealthy relationships with me-
respect to its psychological underpinnings, Przybylski et al. dia and technology. For example, FoMO has been associated
with social networking site addiction (Blackwell et al. 2017;
Louise Cody and Jayde A. M. Flett contributed equally to this work. Kuss and Griffiths 2017), the amount of stress experienced
when using social networking sites (Beyens et al. 2016), use
* Benjamin C. Riordan of mobile phones while driving/learning (Przybylski et al.
ben.riordan@postgrad.otago.ac.nz 2013), decreased self-esteem (Buglass et al. 2017), college mal-
* Damian Scarf adjustment (Alt 2016), poor sleep (Adams et al. 2016), and a
damian@psy.otago.ac.nz range of other negative outcomes (Baker et al. 2016; Elhai et al.
Louise Cody
2016; Oberst et al. 2017; Riordan et al. 2015). Adolescents and
louise.cody@postgrad.otago.ac.nz young adults may be particularly sensitive to FoMO as they are
more sensitive to social information than adults (Lamblin et al.
Jayde A. M. Flett
jflett@psy.otago.ac.nz 2017).
To date, FoMO has been measured predominantly using
Tamlin S. Conner
Przybylski et al.’s (2013) 10-item FoMO scale (FoMOs; cf.
tconner@psy.otago.ac.nz
Abel et al. 2016). The FoMOs measures the extent to which
John Hunter individuals fear missing out on events, experiences, or group
jhunter@psy.otago.ac.nz
activities (e.g., BWhen I miss out on a planned get together it
1
Department of Psychology, University of Otago, P.O. Box 56, bothers me^; BI fear others have more rewarding experiences
Dunedin 9054, New Zealand than me^). Participants rate each item on a five-point Likert
1216 Curr Psychol (2020) 39:1215–1220

scale from 1 (not at all true of me) to 5 (extremely true of me). interest in taking part in the research via the Department of
Although the FoMOs has proven a valuable tool for standard Psychology’s website were sent an online survey via email
laboratory-based contexts and longer online surveys, a single- where they provided informed consent. Of the 202 partici-
item measure would provide greater flexibility of use. pants who signed up for the survey, 198 provided sufficient
Although sometimes viewed as overly simplistic (e.g., information to be included in the analyses (i.e., provided com-
Konrath et al. 2014), single-item measures are immensely plete data for both FoMO measures).
important in situations where there is limited time (e.g., during
in-situ/intercept interviews), when there are multiple measure- Online Recruitment Participants (n = 139) were recruited
ment points (e.g., a sample of students taking part in an online through social media (58.3% women, 37.4% men, 4.3% oth-
adventure course), or when using text message or smartphone er), 16–51 years old (M = 24.9, SD = 5.7), and 46.0% were
measurement (e.g., when texting students about alcohol con- university students. The participants were largely Caucasian
sumption). In sum, FoMO research is still in its infancy, and a (51.7%) and from New Zealand (33.8%), the US (30.9%), or
single item measure would greatly expand the contexts in Canada (14.4%, 20.9% other). Participants who expressed
which FoMO can be reliably measured. interest were redirected to an online survey where they pro-
The aim of the current paper was to validate a single-item vided informed consent. Of the 240 participants who signed
measure of FoMO – the Fear of Missing Out short form up for the survey, 139 were included in the analyses (101 did
(FoMOsf). Study 1 focused on the concurrent validity of the not provide data for both the FoMOs and FoMOsf).
FoMOsf by testing the relationship between the FoMOsf and
FoMOs. Study 2 focused on the construct validity of the Procedure
FoMOsf by testing the relationship between the FoMOsf
and social networking use. Study 2 also included test-retest Participants completed the 10-item FoMOs (Chronbach’s
reliability of the FoMOsf. Study 3 focused on the construct α = .872 and α = .864 for the Psychology and online sample,
validity of the FoMOsf by testing the link between the respectively) and the FoMOsf. FoMO questions were embed-
FoMOsf and emotions experienced while using social net- ded within a larger survey including several health measures
working sites. not relevant to the present report. Informed consent was ob-
tained from all individual participants included in the study.

Scale Creation
Results
In order to develop the FoMOsf, we used other single item
measures and methodologies as a guideline (e.g., Konrath There was a strong relationship between the FoMOs and
et al. 2014; Nichols and Webster 2013). The FoMOsf wording FoMOsf for both the sample recruited through the
was: BDo you experience FoMO (the fear of missing out)?^ Department of Psychology (r = .735, p < .001) and the sample
and participants were asked to rate the item on a scale from 1 recruited online (r = .654, p < .001).
(no, not true of me) to 5 (yes, extremely true of me). In Study
1, we assessed the concurrent validity of the FoMOsf in a Study 2
sample of university students recruited via psychology
courses and a more diverse sample recruited online. In Study 2, we assessed the construct validity of the FoMOsf
by examining the link between the FoMOsf and Facebook
engagement, distracted learning, and distracted driving
Study 1 (Przybylski et al. 2013). We used Fisher’s r-to-z transforma-
tions to determine whether the relationship between each of
Participants the FoMO scales and the outcome measures were significantly
different.
Psychology Recruitment Participants were a sample of 198
university students (80.8% women, 18.7% men, 0.5% other), Participants
17–50 years old (M = 19.7, SD = 3.5), who were predominant-
ly in their first three years at a major New Zealand university Participants were a university sample of 330 university stu-
(first year = 24.7%; second year = 48.5%; third year = 23.7%; dents (73.9% women, 25.5% men, 0.6% other), 17–40 years
fourth or above = 3.0%), where undergraduate degrees typi- old (M = 19.6, SD = 2.2), who were predominantly in their
cally take three years. The participants were largely of New first three years at university (first year = 34.5%; second
Zealand European descent (67.2%; 15.6% Asian; 5.0% Māori year = 50.0%; third year = 12.7%; fourth or above = 2.4%).
or Pacific Islander; 12.2% other). Participants who expressed As in Study 1, participants were largely of New Zealand
Curr Psychol (2020) 39:1215–1220 1217

European descent (67.0%; 13.4% Asian; 4.8% Māori or Results and Discussion
Pacific Islander; 14.8% other). Participants who expressed
interest in taking part in the research via the Department of As in Study 1, there was a significant relationship between the
Psychology’s website were sent an online survey via email FoMOsf and FoMOs (r = .638, p < .001). With respect to
where they provided informed consent. Data were combined Facebook engagement, both the FoMOs (r = .283, p < .001)
from two surveys that used the same recruitment and reim- and the FoMOsf (r = .206, p < .001) were related to overall
bursement methods, with the surveys only differing in the Facebook engagement. These correlations were not significant-
time of year they were completed. Forty-three participants ly different from each other (z = 1.05, p = 0.294). Moreover,
completed both surveys. Therefore, we included their first both scales were related to each of the individual items on the
survey response in the main analyses. For secondary analy- Facebook engagement scale and r-to-z transformations showed
ses, we assessed the test-retest validity of the two FoMO that there was no significant difference between the relation-
scale using the 43 participants who took part in both sur- ships: Bused Facebook within 15 minutes of waking up^
veys. For completeness, we controlled for length of time (FoMOs: r = .169, p = .002; FoMOsf: r = .159, p = .004; z =
between surveys. 0.13, p = 0.897), Bwhile eating breakfast^ (FoMOs: r = .211,
p < .001; FoMOsf: r = .179, p = .001; z = 0.43, p = 0.667),
Bwhile eating lunch^ (FoMOs: r = .208, p < .001; FoMOsf:
Procedure r = .116, p = .036; z = 1.21, p = 0.226), Bwhile eating dinner^
(FoMOs: r = .235, p < .001; FoMOsf: r = .143, p = .010; z =
Participants completed a number of questions that included 1.22, p = 0.223), Bwithin 15 minutes of going to sleep^
the FoMOs (Cronbach’s α = .848) and the FoMOsf. (FoMOs: r = .242, p < .001; FoMOsf: r = .174, p = .002; z =
Participants also answered questions on typical social net- 0.91, p = 0.363).
working use, Facebook engagement, distracted learning, and Only 261 participants answered the distracted learning
distracted driving. FoMO questions were embedded within a question. Both the FoMOs (r = .268, p < .001) and the
larger survey including several health measures not relevant to FoMOsf (r = .244, p < .001) were significantly related to dis-
the present report. Informed consent was obtained from all tracted learning. These correlations were not significantly dif-
individual participants included in the study. ferent from each other (z = 0.29, p = 0.772). In contrast, nei-
ther the FoMOs (r = −.011, p = .841) nor the FoMOsf
(r = .056, p = .308) were related to distracted driving (z =
Measures −0.86, p = 0.390). When removing those who reported no
driving (i.e., Not applicable; n = 114), there was still no rela-
Facebook Engagement Facebook engagement was assessed tionship between the FoMOs (r = .096, p = .162) or the
using the five-item Social Media Engagement questionnaire FoMOsf (r = .108, p = .113) and distracted driving (z = 0.13,
(α = .803; Przybylski et al. 2013). Participants were asked to p = 0.897). There was, however, a relationship between the
indicate how often they had used Facebook in a number of FoMOs and FoMOsf on the item asking about Bglancing at
situations in the past week (e.g., BWithin 15 minutes of wak- phone while driving^ (FoMOs: r = .141, p = .038; FoMOsf:
ing up^). Responses were scored on 5-point Likert scales (1 = r = .137, p = .044; z = 0.04, p = 0.968). Thus, both FoMO
not one day last week to 5 = every day last week). scales showed similar patterns with respect to distracted driv-
ing. The lack of link between both FoMO scales and distracted
Distracted Learning Distracted learning was assessed using driving likely reflected the fact that the participants were at-
the single item employed by Przybylski et al. (2013). tending a university located in a small city (i.e., driving is not
Participants were asked to report the number of lectures they the dominant mode of transport).
had used Facebook in during the last week. Responses were
scored on a 5-point scale (1 = zero lectures, 2 = 1–2 lectures,
3 = 3–4 lectures, 4 = 5–6 lectures, and 5 = 7 or more lectures). Study 2b Test-Retest Reliability

Distracted Driving Distracted driving was assessed using The 43 participants who completed both surveys were pre-
Przybylski et al.'s (2013) distracted driving question (α = .960). dominantly women (79.1% women, 20.9% men), 18–22 years
Participants were asked to indicate how often they had experi- old (M = 19.5, SD = 1.0), in their first three years at university
enced a number of situations as a driver in the past 3 months (first year = 11.5%; second year = 67.4%; third year = 20.9%),
(e.g., text/emailed while driving). Responses were scored on a 5- and were largely of New Zealand European descent (76.7%;
point scale (1 = Not applicable, 2 = No, 3 = Yes, once or 16.3% Asian; 4.7% Indian; 2.3% other). On average, they
twice, 4 = Yes, occasionally, 5 = Yes, often). The five items completed the two surveys 53.7 days apart (SD = 10.7;
were averaged to create a Distracted Driving score. range = 29–75).
1218 Curr Psychol (2020) 39:1215–1220

There was a strong correlation between the FoMOs at Time distracted driving (α = .962). Finally, we also assessed ambiv-
1 and the FoMOs at Time 2 (r = .716, p < .001) and FoMOsf at alent emotional experiences when using Facebook with the 20-
Time 1 and the FoMOsf at Time 2 (r = .717, p < .001). These item Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS;
correlations were not significantly different from each other α = .862; Watson et al. 1988) The scale is composed of 10
(z = 0.01, p = 0.992). When running a partial correlation, con- items measuring activated positive affect (e.g., excited, in-
trolling for days since the last survey, there was still a strong spired) and 10 items measuring activated negative affect (e.g.,
link between the FoMOsf scores (r = .727, p < .001) and distressed, irritable). Responses were scored on a 5-point scale
FoMOs scores (r = .720, p < .001; z = 0.07, p = 0.944). (1 = Very slightly or not at all to 5 = Extremely). Informed con-
Suggesting both measures have good test-retest reliability. sent was obtained from all individual participants included in
Overall, the results of Study 2a and 2b indicate the the study.
FoMOsf has good construct validity, displaying similar
(if slightly weaker) relations with measures of Facebook
engagement, distracted learning, and distracted driving, Results and Discussion
as the FoMOs. Further, much like the FoMOs, the
FoMOsf displays good test-retest reliability. As in Study 1, there was a strong relationship between
the FoMOsf and FoMOs (r = .807, p < .001) and, as in Study
Study 3 2, the FoMOs (r = .307, p < .001) and the FoMOsf (r = .290,
p < .001) were related to overall Facebook engagement. These
In Study 3, we aimed to replicate and extend the con- correlations were not significantly different from each other
struct validity of the FoMOsf by examining the relation- (z = 0.12, p = 0.905). Surprisingly, for the 87 participants that
ship between the FOMOs, the FoMOsf, and the same completed the distracted learning question, neither the FoMOs
measures tested in Study 2 (Facebook engagement, dis- (r = .181, p = .088) nor the FoMOsf (r = .166, p = .119) were
tracted learning, distracted driving), and further, to ex- related to distracted learning (z = 0.10, p = 0.920). A potential
amine their relationships with emotions experienced explanation for this finding is that Study 3 was conducted
when using Facebook as found in Przybylski et al.’s toward the end of the semester, when exams were imminent,
(2013) study. As in Study 2, we used Fisher’s r-to-z leading students to be more focused during lectures. Similar to
transformations to determine whether the relationship Study 2, neither the FoMOs (r = −.187, p = .078) nor the
between each FoMO scales and outcome measure were FoMOsf (r = −.116, p = −.116) were related to distracted driv-
significantly different. ing (z = 0.48, p = 0.631). As in Study 2, this likely reflects the
campus location in relation to student housing. Finally, both
Participants the FoMOs (r = .534, p < .001) and the FoMOsf (r = .470,
p < .001) were related to negative emotions when using
Participants were 90 third year undergraduate students taking Facebook (z = 0.56, p = 0.576). Similarly, both the FoMOs
part in a psychology course (84.4% women, 14.4% men, 1.1% (r = .356, p < .001) and the FoMOsf (r = .274, p < .001) were
other). Participants were predominantly NZ European (74.7%, related to positive emotions when using Facebook (z = 0.6,
3.3% Māori, 13.2% Asian, 8.8% Other) and were 18–26 years p = 0.549). These patterns suggest that those higher in
old (M = 20.9, SD = 1.4). Slightly fewer individuals identified as FoMO were more likely to experience greater emotional highs
Māori and Asian than the wider university population and there and lows when using Facebook. Finally, in Study 3 the
were no participants who identifed as Pacific Islander. FoMOsf was presented before the FoMOs and the findings
were comparable to those of Study 1 and 2 in which the
Procedure, Materials, and Measures FoMOs was presented first.

A classroom performance system programme (CPS; Banxia


Software Ltd. UK, 2012) was used for data collection. General Discussion
Specifically, participants were provided with classroom
clickers and answered questionnaire items by pressing the letter With the advent of social networking sites, people are more
or number on the clicker that corresponded to their preferred chronically aware of what they are missing out on than ever
response. In addition to the questionnaires, basic demographic before and there is a growing body of work demonstrating that
information such as gender, age group, and ethnicity was also this feeling (i.e., FoMO) is related to a number of negative
collected. Participants completed a number of questions and behaviours (Przybylski et al. 2013) and health outcomes
completed the FoMOsf before the FoMOs (Chronbach’s (Baker et al. 2016; Beyens et al. 2016; Riordan et al. 2015).
α = .841). As per Study 2, participants also answered questions The aim of the current series of studies was to develop and test
on Facebook engagement (α = .824), distracted learning, and a single-item FoMO scale (FoMOsf) in order to extend the
Curr Psychol (2020) 39:1215–1220 1219

contexts in which FoMO can be measured. Supporting the Adams, S. K., Williford, D. N., Vaccaro, A., Kisler, T. S., Francis, A., &
Newman, B. (2016). The young and the restless: Socializing trumps
validity of the FoMOsf, Study 1 demonstrated it has good
sleep, fear of missing out, and technological distractions in first-year
concurrent validity, correlating well with the FoMOs. college students. International Journal of Adolescence and Youth,
Studies 2a and 3 demonstrated it has high construct validity, 22(3), 337–348. https://doi.org/10.1080/02673843.2016.1181557.
displaying a similar relationship as the FoMOs with measures Alt, D. (2016). Students’ wellbeing, fear of missing out, and social media
engagement for leisure in higher education learning environments.
of Facebook engagement, distracted learning, distracted driv-
Current Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-016-9496-1.
ing, and measures of positive and negative activated affect Baker, Z. G., Krieger, H., & LeRoy, A. S. (2016). Fear of missing out:
experienced while using Facebook. Further, Study 2b demon- Relationships with depression, mindfulness, and physical symp-
strated that the FoMOsf displays acceptable test-retest reliabil- toms. Translational Issues in Psychological Science, 2(3), 275.
https://doi.org/10.1037/tps0000075.
ity. Although there are no firm guidelines regarding the level
Beyens, I., Frison, E., & Eggermont, S. (2016). BI don’t want to miss a
of reliability short-form measures should meet, according to thing^: Adolescents’ fear of missing out and its relationship to ado-
Widaman et al. (2011) the FoMOsf could be classified as lescents’ social needs, Facebook use, and Facebook related stress.
Badequate for research purposes^ (pp. 46). Computers in Human Behavior, 64, 1–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
chb.2016.05.083.
Blackwell, D., Leaman, C., Tramposch, R., Osborne, C., & Liss, M.
(2017). Extraversion, neuroticism, attachment style and fear of miss-
Conclusions ing out as predictors of social media use and addiction. Personality
and Individual Differences, 116, 69–72. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
paid.2017.04.039.
Three studies demonstrate the validity and reliability of the Buglass, S. L., Binder, J. F., Betts, L. R., & Underwood, J. D. (2017).
FoMOsf as a measure of the Fear of Missing Out. The Motivators of online vulnerability: The impact of social network site
FoMOsf now joins a number of other short-form measures use and FOMO. Computers in Human Behavior, 66, 248–255.
that have been developed to overcome some of the limitations https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.09.055.
Elhai, J. D., Levine, J. C., Dvorak, R. D., & Hall, B. J. (2016). Fear of
associated with longer measures (Nichols and Webster 2013; missing out, need for touch, anxiety and depression are related to
Nichols and Webster 2014; Robins et al. 2001; Woods and problematic smartphone use. Computers in Human Behavior, 63,
Hampson 2005). It is important to note, however, that the 509–516. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.05.079.
FoMOsf is not a replacement for the FoMOs. When the time Konrath, S., Meier, B. P., & Bushman, B. J. (2014). Development and
validation of the single item narcissism scale (SINS). PLoS One,
is available and the context is appropriate, the FoMOs should 9(8), e103469. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0103469.
be employed. However, when time is short and non-traditional Kuss, D. J., & Griffiths, M. D. (2017). Social networking sites and ad-
assessment approaches are employed (e.g., in-situ/intercept diction: Ten lessons learned. International Journal of Environmental
interviews), the FoMOsf provides a valid and reliable method Research and Public Health, 14(3), 311. https://doi.org/10.3390/
ijerph14030311.
of measuring FoMO. Future research should test the psycho- Lamblin, M., Murawski, C., Whittle, S., & Fornito, A. (2017). Social
metric properties of the FoMOsf for wider population-based connectedness, mental health and the adolescent brain.
surveys and smartphone-based experience sampling studies Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 80, 57–68. https://doi.org/
where survey space is limited. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.05.010.
Nichols, A. L., & Webster, G. D. (2013). The single-item need to belong
scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 55(2), 189–192.
Acknowledgements This research was funded by the Health Research https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2013.02.018.
Council of New Zealand (Grant Number: 17/568) and University of Nichols, A. L., & Webster, G. D. (2014). The single-item need for con-
Otago Research Grant, both awarded to Damian Scarf. Benjamin Riordan sistency scale. Individual Differences Research, 12(2), 50–58.
was sponsored by a Fulbright New Zealand General Graduate Award. Oberst, U., Wegmann, E., Stodt, B., Brand, M., & Chamarro, A. (2017).
Negative consequences from heavy social networking in adoles-
Compliance with Ethical Standards cents: The mediating role of fear of missing out. Journal of
Adolescence, 55, 51–60. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.
Conflict of Interest On behalf of all authors, the corresponding author 2016.12.008.
states that there is no conflict of interest. Przybylski, A. K., Murayama, K., DeHaan, C. R., & Gladwell, V. (2013).
Motivational, emotional, and behavioral correlates of fear of missing
out. Computers in Human Behavior, 29(4), 1841–1848. https://doi.
Ethical Approval All procedures performed in studies involving human
org/10.1016/j.chb.2013.02.014.
participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institu-
Riordan, B. C., Flett, J. A. M., Hunter, J. A., Scarf, D., & Conner, T. S.
tional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki
(2015). Fear of missing out (FoMO): the relationship between
declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.
FoMO, alcohol use, and alcohol-related consequences in college
students. Annals of Neuroscience and Psychology, 2, Article 7.
Robins, R. W., Hendin, H. M., & Trzesniewski, K. H. (2001). Measuring
global self-esteem: Construct validation of a single-item measure
References and the Rosenberg self-esteem scale. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 27(2), 151–161. https://doi.org/10.1177/
Abel, J. P., Buff, C. L., & Burr, S. A. (2016). Social media and the fear of 0146167201272002.
missing out: Scale development and assessment. Journal of Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and val-
Business & Economics Research, 14(1), 33. idation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The
1220 Curr Psychol (2020) 39:1215–1220

PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Secondary data analysis: An introduction for psychologists (pp.
54(6), 1063–1070. 39–61). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Widaman, K. F., Little, T. D., Preacher, K. J., & Sawalani, G. M. (2011). Woods, S. A., & Hampson, S. E. (2005). Measuring the big five with
On creating and using short forms of scales in secondary research. In single items using a bipolar response scale. European Journal of
K. H. Trzesniewski, M. B. Donnellan, & R. E. Lucas (Eds.), Personality, 19(5), 373–390. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.542.

You might also like