Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 15

J Bus Ethics

DOI 10.1007/s10551-014-2347-9

Socio-Cognitive Determinants of Consumers’ Support


for the Fair Trade Movement
Andreas Chatzidakis • Minas Kastanakis •

Anastasia Stathopoulou

Received: 17 February 2014 / Accepted: 29 August 2014


 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2014

Abstract Despite the reasonable explanatory power of challenges of living in contemporary consumption envi-
existing models of consumers’ ethical decision making, a ronments. However, the literature remains limited, and
large part of the process remains unexplained. This article additional work is necessary for a comprehensive and
draws on previous research and proposes an integrated model unified understanding of the role of ethics in consumption.
that includes measures of the theory of planned behavior, In this endeavor, some authors concentrate on developing
personal norms, self-identity, neutralization, past experi- models of consumer ethical decision making, often draw-
ence, and attitudinal ambivalence. We postulate and test a ing on socio-cognitive models originally applied in other
variety of direct and moderating effects in the context of a fields, such as Ajzen’s (1985, 1991) theory of planned
large scale survey study in London, UK. Overall, the behavior (TPB), Schwartz’s (1977) model of norm acti-
resulting model represents an empirically robust and holistic vation, and Hunt and Vitell’s (1986, 1992, 2006) general
attempt to identify the most important determinants of con- theory of marketing ethics. These models build on the
sumers’ support for the fair-trade movement. Implications premise that consumers’ ethical judgments (or related
and avenues for further research are discussed. attitudinal constructs) are consistent with their behavioral
intentions, which in turn are an effective proxy for actual
Keywords Attitude–behavior gap  Consumer ethical behavior in most circumstances (Fukukawa 2002). None-
decision making  Ethical consumerism  Fair trade  theless, studies on ethical consumerism have consistently
Theory of planned behavior challenged this premise owing to the widespread observa-
tion of the gap among attitudes, intentions, and behavior
(e.g. Bray et al. 2011; Carrigan and Attalla 2001; Car-
Introduction rington et al. 2010). For example, consumers often buy
environmentally hazardous products regardless of their
Research on ethical consumerism has grown substantially expressed desire for greener alternatives (Devinney et al.
since the 1990s and has provided valuable insights into the 2010).
ways people respond to the moral and environmental Research in both the broader psychological (Ajzen and
Fishbein 2005) and narrower consumer ethics (e.g. Car-
rington et al. 2010; Yeow et al. 2013) literature has widely
A. Chatzidakis (&) shown that the disparity between attitudes and behavior
Royal Holloway University of London, Egham, UK comprises both a potential gap between attitudes and
e-mail: andreas.chatzidakis@rhul.ac.uk
intentions and a gap between intentions and actual behav-
M. Kastanakis ior. Although various theoretical explanations for both
ESCP Europe, London, UK these discrepancies are available in the literature (e.g. Bray
e-mail: mkastanakis@escpeurope.eu et al. 2011; Carrington et al. 2010; Chatzidakis et al. 2007),
on an empirical level, surprisingly few studies have
A. Stathopoulou
Birkbeck University of London, London, UK attempted to provide a more comprehensive approach to
e-mail: a.stathopoulou@bbk.ac.uk narrowing the attitude–intention–behavior gap. So far, the

123
A. Chatzidakis et al.

dominant approach to increasing the amount of variance Webster 1975), terms that were subsequently replaced with
explained in ethical intentions or behavior has been the ‘‘ethical’’, ‘‘caring’’, and ‘‘responsible’’ to incorporate
addition of variables that may have an effect alongside concerns such as trading relationships with the Third
established attitudinal constructs. For example, in applying World (e.g. Harrison et al. 2005).
the TPB to ethical consumer behavior, Shaw and col- A type of behavior featured predominantly in ethical
leagues (Ozcaglar-Toulouse et al. 2006; Shaw and Clarke consumerism studies is consumers’ support for the fair-
1999; Shaw and Shiu 2002a, b, 2003; Shaw et al. 2000) trade movement. Fair-trade products are ‘‘purchased under
suggest the addition of personal norms and self-identity. equitable trading agreements, involving cooperative rather
However, empirical research is lacking on various other than competitive trading principles, ensuring a fair price
key factors that have since appeared in the literature (An- and fair working conditions for the producers and suppli-
dorfer and Liebe 2012), such as the role of habit, past ers’’ (Strong 1996, p. 5). Consumer support for the fair-
behavior and attitudinal ambivalence. In addition, there is a trade movement can also be expressed by organizing and
need for research to go beyond the postulation of additional participating in fair-trade campaigns, donating to relevant
direct effects to investigate the potential role of constructs organizations, and petitioning (e.g. www.fairtrade.org.uk,
in theoretically moderating rather than directly affecting www.maketradefair.com). Such actions are in line with a
the attitude–intention–behavior relationship, such as the widely adopted (at least by the four main international fair-
role of consumer rationalizations for not behaving ethically trade networks), broader definition of the movement as ‘‘an
(Chatzidakis et al. 2007). alternative approach to conventional international trade.
Drawing on these observations, this study aims to [Fair trade] is a trading partnership which aims for sus-
identify the most important psychological and attitudinal tainable development of excluded and disadvantaged pro-
determinants of consumers’ ethical intentions and, in doing ducers. It seeks to do this by providing better trading
so, to narrow down the attitude–intention gap in particular. conditions, by awareness raising, and by campaigning’’
The contributions of the study are threefold. First, it builds (Krier 2001, p. 5).
on previous research to develop and test a comprehensive The commonplace treatment of fair-trade consumerism
model of consumers’ ethical intentions, incorporating key as paradigmatic of ethically superior lifestyles and choices
additional variables such as attitudinal ambivalence, past is increasingly challenged, however, on various grounds. In
experience, and consumer neutralizations. Second, it the economics literature, for example, the efficacy and
moves beyond the postulation of direct effects to investi- ‘‘fairness’’ of fair trade have been heavily debated on the
gate the potential moderating effects of these variables on notions that it may lead to lower-quality products, that a
the attitude–intention relationship. Third, the study substantial amount of the gains go to the fair-trade
attempts to provide a more empirically robust analysis bureaucracy rather than to the producers, and that in the
through the use of multi-item measures, structural equation long run, it may even destroy some industries (Haight and
modeling analysis, and tests for common method bias. Henderson 2010; Henderson 2008; contrary to e.g. Smith
The remainder of the article proceeds as follows: The 2009). Other scholars have focused on the negative con-
next section reviews the current attempts to understand sequences of mainstreaming fair-trade products, including
consumers’ ethical decision making. Then, we develop our the gradual de-politicization (Low and Davenport 2005)
research model and hypotheses. Next, the study outlines and commoditization of the fair-trade movement (Doherty
the methodology and analysis of data. Finally, we discuss et al. 2013) and the adoption of fair-trade products by big
the findings in light of previous studies and provide retail corporations that are more interested in reputation
implications for further research. than deep engagement with fair-trade principles (Fridell
2009). At a more socio-cultural level, authors such as
Cremin (2012) view fair trade as part of the fetishized guilt
Research Background and Theoretical Framework industry, in which any potential of social conscience on
behalf of affluent consumers is channeled into everyday
‘‘Ethical consumerism’’ incorporates concerns about the purchases of mass-marketed ‘‘ethical signifiers’’, which
environment, business practices, and social justice (e.g. leave the material basis of capitalism (and global
Devinney et al. 2010; Harrison et al. 2005). Much of the inequalities) largely unaffected. Correspondingly, more
research in this field pays attention to the characteristics radically oriented consumers are increasingly against fair-
and motivations of green and ethical niches (Shaw and trade products (e.g. Chatzidakis et al. 2012).
Clarke 1999). Studies have attempted to profile the Notwithstanding, for the purposes of this study, fair-
demographic and socio-psychological characteristics of the trade consumerism represents an appropriate empirical
‘‘socially conscious’’, ‘‘green’’, or ‘‘ecologically con- context because it enables us to build on previous modeling
scious’’ consumer (e.g. Anderson and Cunningham 1972; attempts. In addition, despite recent press coverage (e.g.

123
Consumer Ethics

Cloake 2014), fair trade is still recognized as a worthwhile Briefly, the TPB (Ajzen 1985, 1991) is an extension of
cause by many consumers (e.g. Keynote 2013). the theory of reasoned action (TRA; Ajzen and Fishbein
As mentioned, to transcend treatments of fair trade and 1980; Fishbein and Ajzen 1975), which suggests that
ethical consumerism, research has attempted to understand behavior in a specified situation is a direct function of
consumers’ decision-making processes, based on popular behavioral intention, which in turn is a function of attitudes
socio-cognitive models such as Ajzen’s (1985, 1991) TPB, and subjective norms. TPB differs from TRA by adding a
Schwartz’s (1977) norm-activation model, and Hunt and new construct—that is, perceived behavioral control—to
Vitell’s (1986, 1992, 2006) general theory of marketing address behaviors over which individuals have incomplete
ethics. These studies attempt to understand how and why volitional control. Perceived behavioral control influences
consumers behave (un)ethically in a more holistic manner, behavior indirectly through its effect on intention (but also
as opposed to studies that either implicitly or explicitly directly as a proxy for actual behavioral control). The
focus on one or a few components of the decision-making following hypotheses summarize the main premises of the
process (e.g. formation of beliefs, importance of demo- TPB:
graphic and psychographic characteristics; Vitell and Ho
Hypothesis 1 Attitudes positively affect intention to
1997). Nonetheless, although attitudinal models have some
support fair trade.
explanatory power, a large part of the ethical consumer
decision-making process remains unexplained. Research Hypothesis 2 Subjective norms positively affect inten-
into other behavioral and decision-making contexts has tion to support fair trade.
generally attempted to account for attitude–intention–
Hypothesis 3 Perceived behavioral control positively
behavior discrepancies through the addition of other con-
affects intention to support fair trade.
structs, measurement refinements, and behavior-specific
considerations (see, e.g. Ogden 2003). Accordingly, the The sufficiency of the TPB in explaining moral behavior
remainder of this section draws from previous research on is criticized on five main grounds. First, because it is
ethical and pro-social behavior to develop an extended essentially a rational-choice model, the TPB ignores the
conceptualization of consumers’ ethical decision making. role of altruistic, non-rational motives in guiding behavior
The TPB provides a useful initial platform for under- (Kaiser et al. 1999; Sparks and Shepherd 2002).1 Personal
standing consumer ethical decision making for several feelings of rightness or wrongness, as reflected in measures
reasons. First, the TPB is arguably the most robust of all of ‘‘personal norms’’ or ‘‘ethical obligation’’, were delib-
the attitude–behavior models, with an impressive record of erately dropped from the original version of TRA, but they
successful applications in many domains (for reviews, see remain at the forefront of moral behavior research (Man-
Armitage and Conner 2001; Notani 1998). In addition, this stead and Parker 1995) and are key constructs in Sch-
model is widely used in consumer research (De Cannière wartz’s (1977) norm-activation model. In contrast, by
et al. 2009), including ethical contexts such as the purchase incorporating ‘‘subjective norms’’, the TPB focuses on
of fair-trade products (Ozcaglar-Toulouse et al. 2006; conventional responsibility in the form of social expecta-
Shaw and Clarke 1999; Shaw and Shiu 2002a, b, 2003; tions, rather than ethical responsibility based on deliber-
Shaw et al. 2000), various instances of consumer miscon- ately made moral judgments (Kaiser and Shimoda 1999;
duct (Fukukawa 2002), software piracy (Chang 1998), Kaiser et al. 1999). Accordingly, an increasing amount of
waste recycling (Chan 1998), and green purchase behavior literature provides support for the utility of this construct
(e.g. Kalafatis et al. 1999). Conceptualizing consumers’ over and above traditional TPB determinants (e.g. Evans
ethical decision making in relation to this theoretical and Norman 2003; Godin et al. 2005; see also Conner and
framework therefore promotes consistency and compara- Armitage 1998, for a review). Thus:
bility in this nascent area of research. Second, TPB appli-
Hypothesis 4 Personal norms positively affect intention
cations, perhaps more than any other decision-making
to support fair trade.
approach, offer thorough and detailed guidelines on how to
construct and validate respective measures (e.g. Ajzen Second, the TPB treats the (moral) actor primarily as a
2002a; Ajzen and Fishbein 1980; Francis et al. 2004a, b). psychological entity rather than a social construct (Terry
Third, the TPB remains, in principle, open to the inclusion et al. 1999). From this point of view, the conceptualization
of other constructs so long as they increase TPB’s of subjective norms is limited because the construct does
explanatory power (Ajzen 1991). Finally, the TPB is in line
with other ethical decision-making models that allow for a 1
The non-rationality of such motives has been disputed elsewhere,
step-by-step (from attitudes to intentions to behavior) view
such as in game theory advancements (e.g. Camerer et al. 2011) and
of the cognitive process (Fukukawa 2002; Nicholls and Lee the work of the philosopher Bernard Williams (e.g. Smart and
2006). Williams 1973; Williams 1972).

123
A. Chatzidakis et al.

not capture the whole spectrum of socially defined influ- neutralization techniques (Sykes and Matza 1957) in the
ences (Hagger and Chatzisarantis 2006). Identity theory TPB as a taxonomy of typical justifications (i.e. denial of
suggests that ‘‘one’s self concept is organized into a hier- responsibility, denial of injury, denial of victim, condemning
archy of role identities that correspond to one’s positions in the condemners, and appealing to higher loyalties) that
the social sstructure’’ (Charng et al. 1988, p. 304). When a consumers may employ when behaving in ways that con-
particular behavior (e.g. driving a hybrid sport-utility tradict their ethical concerns. Drawing on these authors’
vehicle) becomes associated with someone’s role identity work, we postulate that neutralization techniques may neg-
(e.g. pro-environment ‘‘middle class’’), that person is more atively affect intention and can moderate the relationship
likely to behave consistently with that identity (e.g. Hagger between TPB antecedents and intention:
and Chatzisarantis 2006). Therefore, he or she may form
Hypothesis 7a Neutralization negatively affects intention
positive intentions toward a pro-social activity because
to support fair trade.
related issues (e.g. caring for Third-World producers) have
become an important part of his or her self-identity (Shaw Hypothesis 7b The higher the acceptance of neutralizing
et al. 2000). As in the case of ethical obligation, previous beliefs, the weaker is the relationship between TPB ante-
TPB research provides extensive support for the utility of cedents and intention.
the self-identity construct (Charng et al. 1988; Jackson
Fifth, a common criticism of the TPB is its inability to
et al. 2003; Sparks and Shepherd 2002).
account for habitual or automatic processes (e.g. Eagly and
Hypothesis 5 Self-identity positively affects intention to Chaiken 1993). Although past behavior, strictly speaking,
support fair trade. cannot serve as a causal antecedent of future behavior (Ajzen
2011), several authors have used past behavior measures as
Third, although feelings of ‘‘ambivalence’’ and the con-
proxies for habit strength. In this sense, a more causal role
flictive nature of ethical choices in consumption have been
can be attributed to past behavior, insofar as ‘‘the de-con-
widely reported in ethical consumer research (e.g. Devinney
struction of existing and habitual consumption patterns, and
et al. 2010), few empirical attempts have examined the role
the construction of new habitual routines’’ are integral to the
of ambivalent feelings and cognitions alongside traditional
formation and enactment of consumers’ plans to follow more
TPB constructs. Costarelli and Colloca (2004, p. 280) find
ethical lifestyles (Carrington et al. 2014, p. 2764). In addi-
that ambivalence, defined as ‘‘the simultaneous presence of
tion, past behavior can serve as a proxy for personal expe-
positive and negative evaluations of the same attitude
rience, a factor that has been identified as a key impediment
object’’, has a strong independent effect on intention; con-
to ethical consumption (Bray et al. 2011). Accordingly,
versely, Castro et al. (2009) find support for an additional
studies across various behavioral domains—but so far not in
moderating effect of ambivalence on the attitude–intention
ethical consumption—indicate that past behavior has a strong
relationship. Although these studies focus on pro-ecological
direct effect on intention over and above traditional TPB
behaviors, similar effects could be manifest in the context of
antecedents and may attenuate the attitude–intention and
fair-trade support. Accordingly, we postulate the following:
intention–behavior relationships (e.g. Hagger et al. 2001,
Hypothesis 6a Ambivalence negatively affects intention 2002; Norman and Conner 2006; Norman et al. 2000). Thus:
to support fair trade.
Hypothesis 8a Past behavior positively affects intention
Hypothesis 6b Ambivalence moderates (weakens) the to support fair trade.
relationship between TPB constructs and intention.
Hypothesis 8b Past behavior strengthens the relationship
Fourth, the TPB falls short in explaining the internal between TPB antecedents and intention.
tensions that consumers may face when balancing their own
Figure 1 presents the conceptual model that extends the
desires with moral behavior that favors societal well-being.
TPB by adding five new variables: personal norms, self-
For example, Schwartz’s (1977; see also Schwartz and
identity, ambivalence, neutralization, and past experience. In
Howard 1980, 1981) norm-activation model incorporates the
addition, the figure postulates a series of moderating effects.
concept of ‘‘defensive’’ or ‘‘responsibility denial’’, to
account for the idea that when the costs of pro-social
behavior are high, individuals may redefine the situation as Method
beyond their responsibility and norms will not be activated.
This moderator hypothesis has received support in contexts Design and Procedure
such as helping behavior (Schwartz 1977; Schwartz and
Howard 1980, 1981) and energy conservation (Tyler et al. We used a drop-and-collect survey procedure to collect
1982). Chatzidakis et al. (2007) conceptualize the role of data from a probability sample of 517 residents of London,

123
Consumer Ethics

et al. 1982). Finally, respondents answered demographic


and control measures.
Of the respondents, 50.7 % were men and 49.3 %
women, ranging in age from 18 to 88 years (M = 28). In
addition, 14.3 % had completed secondary or tertiary
education, 61.9 % had obtained a Bachelor’s degree, and
the rest (23.8 %) held a Master’s or doctoral degree.
Drop-and-collect surveys typically produce response
rates of 70–90 % (Lovelock et al. 1976). In total, 800
questionnaires were distributed, at various days of the
week, to obtain a broad representation. On weekdays,
distribution occurred in the evening, when most people are
at home, to reduce non-response error, and on weekends,
distribution took place during the entire day. In total, 517
Fig. 1 Proposed theoretical model usable surveys were returned (65 % response rate).

Measures
UK. Recent data illustrate that general awareness of and
involvement in fair-trade issues is higher in the UK capital We modeled traditional TPB measures after those of Ajzen
(e.g. Keynote 2012, 2013; Mintel Report 2007), suggesting and Fishbein (1980), Ajzen (2002a), and Francis et al.
that London could be an adequate sampling frame for this (Francis et al. 2004a), and we adapted items measuring
study, although this does not necessarily translate into personal norms, self-identity, and ambivalence from pre-
higher levels of overall ethical and green consumption vious research. The items measuring neutralization were
activity. The sampling procedure employed a multi-stage newly constructed, and the common denominator was
cluster sampling design, with respondents from six post- meant to be ‘‘justifiability’’ of non-supportive behavior
code areas who were moderately to highly knowledgeable toward fair trade, in line with Chatzidakis et al. (2007). A
about fair trade. Specifically, we qualified respondents by full description of these measures and related portions from
means of screening questions that ensured that they, at least the questionnaire are available in the Appendix.
occasionally, bought fair-trade products and/or supported
fair trade in other ways (e.g. signing a petition). For
assistance during the screening, and to achieve a priming Findings
effect (Sudman et al. 1996), the respondents received the
following definition before completing the survey: Common Method Bias
Supporting the fair-trade movement may involve
To determine the extent of common method bias in the
buying fair-trade products, that is, products that have
study, we performed Harman’s one-factor test, following
been certified by a Fair Trade Labeling Organization
the approach that Podsakoff et al. (2003) outline. We
for being purchased under equitable trading agree-
entered all measurement items for intention, attitude, sub-
ments, involving co-operative rather than competitive
jective norm, perceived behavioral control, neutralization,
trading principles, ensuring a fair price and fair
personal norm, and self-identity into principal axis factor-
working conditions for the producers and suppliers.
ing (unrotated). According to this technique, if a single
Support also includes backing the fair-trade move-
factor emerges from the factor analysis or one ‘‘general’’
ment in other ways, for example, by making a
factor accounts for the majority of the covariance in the
donation to a Fair Trade Organization or signing a
variables, common method variance is present. The results
petition about trade justice.
suggest that common method bias is not a problem because
This process stimulates memory and helps respondents the first factor accounted for 30.08 % of the variance, much
complete the questionnaire in a more focused frame of lower than the 50 % threshold (Podsakoff et al. 2003). In
mind (Podsakoff et al. 2003); in addition, respondents were addition to Harman’s test, we employed the market-vari-
informed that there were no right or wrong answers, just able technique to verify that common method variance is
opinions (Podsakoff et al. 2003). Afterward, respondents not a problem; research in statistics considers this a reliable
completed the questionnaire; the various indicators were technique in testing common method variance (Lindell and
mixed (no scale was filled ‘‘as is’’) to better conceal the Whitney 2001; Malhotra et al. 2006; Rindfleisch et al.
purpose of the study and elicit unbiased answers (Hunt 2008). This technique uses a variable/question in the

123
A. Chatzidakis et al.

questionnaire that is theoretically unrelated to the other ‘‘internal ethics’’ in accordance with previous research
variables. In the current study, the question was, ‘‘I have (Shaw and Shiu 2002b, 2003). This is conceptually sensi-
confidence in the UK economy’’. We calculated common ble, given that self-identification implies that a consumer
method bias with the following equation: who is interested in fair-trade issues likely possesses an
ra ¼ ru  ½ rm =ð1  rm Þ; ethical consumer orientation in the first place (Sparks and
  Shepherd 2002).
talpha=2; n3 ¼ ra = ½SQRT 1  ra2 = ðn  3Þ ; Finally, after these adjustments, to ensure good fit of the
measurement model, we validated both models (initial TPB
where rm is the smallest positive correlation,2 ru is the
and extended TPB) through confirmatory factor analysis.
uncorrected correlation, ra is partialled out of rm from ru,
All values indicate that both models had a good fit (TPB
and n is sample size.
model: CMIN/df = 2.827, GFI = .952, CFI = .964,
With a sample size of 517 and rm equal to .004, we
NFI = .946, RMSEA = .060; extended TPB model:
calculated this equation and investigated the impact on the
CMIN/df = 2.727, GFI = .9.09, CFI = .942, NFI = .912,
degree and significance of the correlations. The level of
RMSEA = .058), as all were above the cutoff values rec-
significance in the original correlations and the adjusted
ommended in the literature (Hair et al. 2009; Kline 2005).
partial correlations remained the same, which suggests that
These values also reflect good convergent validity for each
the results cannot be accounted for by common method
of the sub-scales.
variance (Lindell and Whitney 2001).
Table 1 summarizes Cronbach’s alpha values, compos-
ite reliabilities, and average variance extracted values for
Validity and Reliability of Measurements
the employed multi-item constructs. Table 2 summarizes
the respective correlations.
We estimated individual confirmatory factor analysis
measurement models for all constructs that contained more
than three items, to ensure unidimensionality and internal Assessment of Proposed Model and Hypotheses
consistency (e.g. Hair et al. 2009). After we dropped three
items which had low loadings or substantial cross-loadings The hypotheses suggest that the initial model of TPB can
and allowed three correlations between error terms (when be improved in the ethical consumption context by
based on substantive, theoretical considerations; Byrne including a more holistic approach that examines the direct
2001), most variables displayed desirable psychometric and moderating effects of additional variables. Such rela-
properties, apart from perceived behavioral control, per- tionships can be tested with hierarchical moderated
sonal norms, and self-identity. regression analysis (Cohen and Cohen 1983; Darrow and
Perceived behavioral control exhibited low reliability Kahl 1982; Mohr et al. 1996; Schoonhoven 1981). This
(Cronbach’s a = .571), echoing problems in the measure- analysis provides a ‘‘straightforward and the most general
ment of this construct reported in previous studies (e.g. method for testing contingency hypothesis in which an
Kraft et al. 2005). Thus, we decided to break down the interaction is implied’’ (Arnold 1982, p. 170). To avoid any
construct into two dimensions that were conceptually dis- multicollinearity between the main and interaction terms,
tinct—that is, perceived control versus perceived difficulty we mean-centered all the continuous variables (Aiken and
(see Trafimow et al. 2002), comprising one item each. We West 1991). In addition, in line with Cohen and Cohen
chose to use single-item measures because they appeared (1983), in the hierarchical moderated regression model we
more unidimensional (Rossiter 2002) and their content was mean-centered the interaction variables to partial out the
highly correlated with prior definitions of perceived main effects from the interactions terms. In the first step,
behavioral control and perceived difficulty (Alexandrov we added the initial predictors of the dependent variable
2010). based on the TPB. In the second step, we added the addi-
Subsequent inspection of the correlations between tional independent variables from the extended TPB, and in
variables indicated a potential problem in the relationship the third step, we added the interaction terms between the
between personal norms and self-identity (.714, p \ .001). predictors and the moderators. This process includes the
Lack of discriminant validity, in turn, was established following equation form:
through exploratory factory analysis (using principal axis y ¼ a þ bx;
factoring with Varimax rotation), which resulted in a one-
factor solution (eigenvalue = 3.568, 1 factor extracted). y ¼ a þ bx þ cz;
We aggregated both constructs into a single factor named y ¼ a þ bx þ cz þ dxz;
2
This correlation, which based on the work of Lindell and Whitney where y is the dependent variable; a is the intercept term; b,
(2001), provides a stringent test. c, and d are the regression coefficients; x is the independent

123
Consumer Ethics

Table 1 Reliability scores Factor Number of Cronbach’s Construct Average variance


loadings items alpha reliability extracted

Internal ethics 0.812 4 0.839 0.842 0.574


0.854
0.662
0.686
Subjective 0.765 3 0.811 0.812 0.590
norms 0.743
0.795
Attitudes 0.799 6 0.854 0.859 0.554
0.591
0.849
0.820
0.622
Past behavior 0.888 2 0.893 0.893 0.806
0.908
Intentions 0.431 4 0.812 0.833 0.570
0.841
0.924
0.731
Ambivalence 0.877 2 0.872 0.872 0.773
0.881
Neutralization 0.687 2 0.701 0.710 0.551
0.794

Table 2 Correlations
Correlations (1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8)

(1) Intention 1 – – – – – – –
(2) Subjective norms 0.591** 1 – – – – – –
(3) Attitude 0.561** 0.420** 1 – – – – –
(4) Perceived control 0.224** 0.183** 0.202** 1 – – – –
(5) Perceived difficulty -0.406** -0.163** -0.259** -0.167** 1 – – –
(6) Internal ethics 0.776** 0.625** 0.577** 0.165** -0.334** 1 – –
(7) Past behavior 0.564** 0.560** 0.479** 0.272** -0.316** 0.550** 1 –
(8) Neutralization -0.346** -0.116** -0.354** -0.053 0.252** -0.338** -0.232** 1
(9) Ambivalence -0.392** -0.249** -0.448** -0.038 0.168** -0.408** -0.275** 0.242**

variable; z is the moderator variable; and xz depicts the be at the .10 level for the moderating effects because the
independent variable–moderator variable interaction. hierarchical moderated regression analyses are conservative.
Hierarchical moderated regression analysis aims to iden- When only traditional TPB determinants were included in
tify any changes in R-square while testing the three regres- the equation, the adjusted R-square was .47 (F(3, 303.772) =
sion equations. Significant changes in R-square from the first 153.297, p \ .000). Subjective norms contributed most to
to the second equation indicate a significant improvement of predicting intention (standardized b = .423, p \ .000), fol-
the model, and further significant changes of R-square to the lowed by attitudes and perceived control (b = .368, p \ .000;
third equation indicate that the moderating effects signifi- b = .073, p \ .05, respectively). In the second step, internal
cantly improve the overall model. Aiken and West (1991) ethics, neutralization, ambivalence, past behavior, and per-
suggest that the significance criteria for such analysis should ceived difficulty were also included in the equation, resulting

123
A. Chatzidakis et al.

Table 3 Hierarchical Models 1 2 3


moderated regression results y Intention Intention Intention

Main effects of traditional TPB


Subjective norms 0.423*** 0.170*** 0.177***
Attitude 0.368*** 0.111** 0.129***
Perceived control 0.073* 0.052 0.069**
Main effects of extended TPB
Perceived difficulty -0.167*** -0.145***
Internal ethics 0.409*** 0.396***
Past behavior 0.090** 0.087*
Ambivalence -0.060 -0.042
Neutralization -0.101*** -0.092**
Interaction effects
Neutralization 9 subjective norms -0.068(?)
Neutralization 9 attitude 0.052
Neutralization 9 perceived control 0.031
Neutralization 9 perceived difficulty -0.022
Neutralization 9 internal ethics 0.015
Ambivalence 9 subjective norms 0.060
Ambivalence 9 attitude -0.035
Ambivalence 9 perceived control -0.012
Ambivalence 9 perceived difficulty 0.096***
Ambivalence 9 internal ethics 0.014
Past Behavior 9 subjective norms -0.037
Past Behavior 9 attitude 0.008
Past Behavior 9 perceived control 0.095**
Past Behavior 9 perceived difficulty 0.077*
Past Behavior 9 internal ethics -0.028
R2 0.473 0.649 0.671
Unstandardized beta Adj. R2 0.47 0.643 0.656
coefficients are presented F 153.297 117.255 43.747
* p \ .05, ** p \ .01, Sig. 0.000 0.000 0.000
*** p \ .001, (?) p \ .10

in a significant R-square change of .17 (p \ .000; adjusted The addition of the product terms showed a significant R-
R2 = 649, F(8, 416.868) = 117.255, p \ .000). Internal square change of .013 (p \ .000; adjusted R2 = .656, F(23,
ethics was now the most important predictor of intention 431.299) = 43.747, p \ .000). These results provide partial
(b = .409, p \ .000), followed by subjective norms support for Hypotheses 6b, 7b, and 8b, taking into account
(b = .170, p \ .000), perceived difficulty (b = -.167, significance criteria of p \ .10 (Aiken and West 1991).
p \ .000), attitude (b = .111, p \ .01), neutralization Table 3 summarizes these results, and Fig. 2 depicts the final
(b = -.101, p \ .000), and past behavior (b = .090, model. Finally, Fig. 3 plots all significant interaction terms,
p \ .01); ambivalence and perceived control did not have a which we discuss in the following section.
significant direct influence on intention. These results provide
support for Hypotheses 6a, 7a, and 8a. Hypotheses 4 and 5 are
also supported, albeit through the construction of a composite Discussion
measure labeled internal ethics. In addition, there is a strong
negative effect for perceived difficulty, a construct that can be Sufficiency of TPB
considered either additional or a sub-component of perceived
behavioral control (Trafimow et al. 2002). The findings from the linear regression reveal that the original
Finally, we examined moderating effects in an additional TPB antecedents—that is, attitude, subjective norms, and
step, in which we entered all interaction terms simultaneously. perceived behavioral control—explain a substantial amount

123
Consumer Ethics

Most notably, the measure of ‘‘internal ethics’’ was the


most important predictor of intention, over and above tra-
ditional determinants such as attitude and subjective norms.
In this study, this measure combines feelings of personal
norms and self-identity, given the lack of discriminant
validity between the two constructs. In line with this
finding, Sparks and Guthrie (1998, p. 1397; see also Sparks
and Shepherd 2002) note: ‘‘Not only may some identities
(e.g. Socialist, Christian, vegetarian) be associated with
values that may be moral values of one sort or other, cer-
tain identity ascriptions (e.g. benevolent, loyal, compas-
sionate) may refer to aspects of character that are seen as
being of intrinsic moral value’’. Similarly, in their review
Fig. 2 Final model
of relevant TPB literature, Conner and Armitage (1998)
suggest that given the often mixed findings, the relationship
(47 %) of the variance in intention to support fair trade. This is between personal norms and self-identity may vary
in line with the typical 30–50 % range of explained variance in depending on the behavior in question. Regardless, the
TPB research (Fife-Schaw et al. 2007) but is well over the central role of internal ethics undermines the TPB as a
24 % of variance explained in a previous application of the rational choice model of self-interest, insofar as altruistic
TPB in fair-trade consumption (Shaw and Shiu 2002a, b, motives and concerns about other people’s welfare are not
2003), perhaps due to the use of multi-item measures (e.g. sufficiently taken into account (Kaiser et al. 1999; Sparks
Armitage and Conner 2001; Eagly and Chaiken 1993). and Shepherd 2002). As Eagly and Chaiken (1993, p. 178)
Nonetheless, in line with criticisms of the sufficiency of TPB argue, measures of personal norms and self-identity are
in explaining moral behavior, inclusion of additional mea- likely to carry both a cognitive and an emotional compo-
sures contributed to an additional 17 % of the variance nent, which is not ‘‘especially salient when respondents
explained. This also resulted in a final model that departs rate behaviors on the evaluative scales used to assess atti-
considerably from the original TPB conceptualization. tude toward the act’’.

Fig. 3 Plots of significant


interaction terms

123
A. Chatzidakis et al.

The current findings also confirm the importance of Altogether, the presence of moderating effects suggests
additional variables proposed in previous research, such as an alternative route to understanding the so-called attitude–
perceived difficulty, neutralization, and past behavior. We intention gap in ethical consumption research (e.g. Bray
introduce perceived difficulty as an additional dimension et al. 2011; Carrington et al. 2010). Beyond the addition of
that is not adequately captured by conventional measures further variables, which has so far been the predominant
of perceived behavioral control and which in turn was the approach to increasing attitude–intention correspondence,
third most significant predictor of intention. Indeed, the the current study highlights the need to explore and
conceptualization and measurement of perceived behav- effectively operationalize potential impediments to indi-
ioral control has been one of the most controversial issues viduals’ otherwise positive inclination toward ethical pro-
in TPB research, and several authors have suggested that it ducts. In other words, the gap could be effectively
should be operationalized as a multi-dimensional variable narrowed by identifying variables that directly affect the
(e.g. perceived behavioral control versus self-efficacy, attitude–intention relationship itself, rather than or along-
perceived behavior control versus perceived difficulty; see side their additive effect as independent antecedents.
Ajzen 2002b). Other important predictors included neu-
tralizations or justifications used for (un)ethical behavior
(Chatzidakis et al. 2007) and past behavior, a measure that Implications and Future Research Avenues
serves as a proxy for both personal experience (Bray et al.
2011) and habitual strength (Verplanken and Aarts 1999). The findings suggest that the psychological processes
underlying fair-trade consumerism are inherently more
Role of Moderating Effects complex than assumed in previous research. For example,
subjective feelings of internal ethics seem to be more
In addition to various additive effects, the current findings important than rational considerations encapsulated in
lend support to the role of moderating effects in models of measures of attitudes and subjective norms. Furthermore,
ethical decision making. The addition of interaction terms given the significance of several additive and moderating
improved the prediction of intention by 1.3 %. Although effects in the traditional TPB framework, this study aligns
this may seem a relatively small amount of improvement, with Hassan et al.’s (2014) recent call to engage in research
note that detection of moderating effects in field studies is ‘‘that would allow a more comprehensive assessment of the
particularly difficult, and non-detection remains the rule motivational pathway between words and deeds’’. The route
rather than the exception (Frazier et al. 2004; McClelland to a more comprehensive understanding of consumers’
and Judd 1993). In addition, the general difficulty in ethical decision making requires that researchers remain
detecting moderating effects could be due to the notion that both critical and creative in their adoption of such models.
linear models provide good accounts of psychological data Despite the contribution of this study to the under-
even when, conceptually, interaction effects should be standing of consumers’ ethical decision making, various
present (Ajzen 1991, p. 188). Regardless, the current potential research avenues exist. First, the difficulties noted
findings provide support for four significant interaction in the measurement of perceived behavioral control are
effects. Most notably, attitudinal ambivalence, a variable common in TPB studies (Conner and Armitage 1998) and
that has no significant direct effect on intention, moderates underscore the need to operationalize control-related feel-
the perceived difficulty–intention relationship, in that the ings as a multi-dimensional construct in further research
higher the ambivalence, the weaker is the negative effect of (Ajzen 2002b). Second, the present model of ethical deci-
perceived difficulty on intention (Fig. 2). A conceptual sion making uses measures of intention rather than actual
explanation for this finding is that ambivalence distorts behavior and, as such, contributes to narrowing the atti-
consumers’ perceptions of the difficulty they may experi- tude–intention rather than the intention–behavior gap. A
ence in performing ethically superior behaviors. Past recent review of TPB studies in the context of ethical
behavior also significantly weakens the perceived diffi- consumption suggests that there can also be significant
culty–intention relationship, perhaps through processes of variation in the intention–behavior relationship (Hassan
learning and consolidating past experiences of difficulty et al. 2014). In addition, measures of actual behavior would
into habitual routines. Arguably on the same grounds, past have facilitated the exploration of additional paths and
behavior accentuates the positive effect of perceived con- moderating effects in the proposed model. Third, moder-
trol on intention. Finally, neutralization also has a moder- ating effects could be explored with greater precision in
ating effect on the subjective norms–intention relationship. experimental or scenario-based approaches rather than
The justifications or excuses for not engaging in socially survey-based designs. For example, participants could be
desirable behaviors seem to weaken the positive effect of introduced to a high cost, pro-social behavior both before
subjective norms on intention. and after completing a questionnaire, by being told that

123
Consumer Ethics

they will be asked to donate some money, or part of their • ‘‘Most people who are important to me think that I
reimbursement, to a relevant cause (e.g. Basil et al. 2006). should support fair trade’’. (‘‘strongly disagree/strongly
Finally, although the sample selected is adequate for test- agree’’)
ing the model, further research is needed in order to be able • ‘‘The people in my life whose opinions I value would
to generalise the findings; such research could attempt to not approve of my supporting for fair trade’’. (‘‘strongly
replicate the model through cross-cultural and longitudinal disagree/strongly agree’’)
studies that link, for example, panel data of fair-trade • ‘‘The people in my life whose opinions I value support
product purchases with survey data. fair trade’’. (‘‘strongly disagree/strongly agree’’)
• ‘‘It is expected of me that I support fair trade in the near
future’’. (‘‘strongly disagree/strongly agree’’).
Perceived behavioral control We measured perceived
Appendix: Excerpts from the Survey Instrument
behavioral control with four statements (Ajzen 2002a):
and Explanation of the Items
• ‘‘For me to support the fair trade movement in the near
Intention Following Francis et al.’s (2004) suggested for- future would be difficult’’. (‘‘strongly disagree/strongly
mat, we assessed general intention to support fair trade agree’’)
using three items: • ‘‘If I wanted to I could support the fair trade movement
in the near future’’. (‘‘strongly disagree/strongly
• ‘‘I expect to support the fair trade movement in the near
agree’’)
future’’. (‘‘strongly disagree/strongly agree’’)
• ‘‘It is mostly up to me whether or not I support fair
• ‘‘I want to support the fair trade movement in the near
trade in the near future’’. (‘‘strongly disagree/strongly
future’’. (‘‘strongly disagree/strongly agree’’)
agree’’)
• ‘‘I intend to support the fair trade movement in the near
• ‘‘How much control do you believe you have over
future’’. (‘‘strongly disagree/strongly agree’’).
supporting fair trade in the near future’’? (‘‘no control/
We used three additional items to measure intentions for complete control’’).
specific behaviors: Personal norms We measured personal norms with three
• ‘‘I would support the fair trade movement in the near questions:
future, by buying fair trade products’’. (‘‘strongly • ‘‘I feel that I have an ethical/moral obligation to support
disagree/strongly agree’’) fair trade’’. (‘‘strongly disagree/strongly agree’’)
• ‘‘I would support the fair trade movement in the near • ‘‘I personally feel I should support fair trade’’.
future, by signing a petition for fair trade’’. (‘‘strongly (‘‘strongly disagree/strongly agree’’)
disagree/strongly agree’’) • ‘‘Supporting the fair trade movement would be the right
• ‘‘I would support the fair trade movement in the near thing for me to do’’. (‘‘strongly disagree/strongly
future, by donating to the fair trade organization’’. agree’’).
(‘‘strongly disagree/strongly agree’’).
The first question retained the suggested format of
Attitudes We assessed attitudes by employing a semantic
Sparks et al. (1995), while the second and third were of
differential scale, as Ajzen (2002a) suggests. Respondents
similar format to measures employed by Sparks and
were presented with the statement, ‘‘Supporting the fair
Guthrie (1998) and Davies et al. (2002).
trade movement is …’’, followed by seven pairs of adjec-
tives: harmful/beneficial, good/bad, pleasant/unpleasant,
Self-identity We constructed three questions to assess self-
worthless/valuable, enjoyable/unenjoyable, rewarding/not
identification with fair-trade issues:
rewarding, and the right thing to do/the wrong thing to do. An
additional question, ‘‘In general, my attitude towards fair • ‘‘To support fair trade is an important part of who I
trade is …’’ was followed by two pairs of adjectives intended am’’. (‘‘strongly disagree/strongly agree’’)
to capture overall evaluation (Sparks and Shepherd 2002): • ‘‘I think of myself as someone who is concerned about
unfavorable/favorable (Ajzen and Fishbein 1980) and neg- ethical issues in consumption’’. (‘‘strongly disagree/
ative/positive (e.g. Sparks and Shepherd 2002). strongly agree’’)
• ‘‘I am not the type of person oriented to support fair
Subjective norms We measured subjective norms with five trade’’. (‘‘strongly disagree/strongly agree’’).
sentences following the recommendations of Ajzen (2002a):
The first two questions retained the suggested format of
• ‘‘Most people who are important to me support fair Terry et al. (1999), and the third was based on the wording
trade’’. (‘‘strongly disagree/strongly agree’’) used by Sparks and Shepherd (2002).

123
A. Chatzidakis et al.

Neutralisation We measured neutralization with three • Regarding supporting the Fair Trade movement I feel
questions meant to capture ‘‘justifiability’’ of not support- that my attitude is…. (‘‘not at all contradictory/very
ing fair trade, following Chatzidakis et al. (2007): contradictory’’)
• Considering only the unfavorable qualities of Fair
• ‘‘For me, not supporting fair trade is justifiable’’.
Trade and ignoring the favorable characteristics, how
(‘‘strongly disagree/strongly agree’’)
unfavorable is your evaluation of supporting the Fair
• ‘‘I have many arguments against supporting fair trade’’.
Trade movement? (‘‘not at all unfavorable/extremely
(‘‘strongly disagree/strongly agree’’)
unfavorable’’).
• ‘‘I’ve got reasons for not supporting fair trade’’.
• Considering only the negative qualities of Fair Trade
(‘‘strongly disagree/strongly agree’’).
and ignoring the positive characteristics, how negative
Past behavior We assessed past behavior with a variety of is your evaluation of supporting the Fair Trade
differently worded questions, as Ajzen (2002a) movement? (‘‘not at all negative/extremely negative’’)
recommends: • Considering only the favorable qualities of Fair Trade
• In the course of the past 3 months, how many times and ignoring the unfavorable characteristics, how favor-
have you decided to support the Fair Trade movement? able is your evaluation of supporting the Fair Trade
(please tick one statement) movement? (‘‘not at all favorable/extremely favorable’’)
• Considering only the positive qualities of Fair Trade
Every time that I had the opportunity. ____ and ignoring the negative characteristics, how positive
Almost every time that I had the opportunity. ____ is your evaluation of supporting the Fair Trade
Most of the time that I had the opportunity. ____ movement? (‘‘not at all positive/extremely positive’’).
About half of the times that I had the opportunity.
____ The first question retained the format used in Castro
Sometimes, but less than half of the times I had the et al. (2009); we adapted the rest of the questions from
opportunity. ____ Conner et al. (2002).
Few times that I had the opportunity. ____
Not at all when I had the opportunity. ____
References
I have not had the opportunity. ____
• How often do you support the Fair Trade Movement? Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and
interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
(‘‘never/always’’) Ajzen, I. (1985). From intentions to actions: A theory of planned
• How often do you purchase Fair Trade products? behavior. In J. Kuhl & J. Beckman (Eds.), Action-control: From
(‘‘never/always’’) cognition to behavior (pp. 11–39). Heidelberg: Springer.
• Have you ever bought Fair Trade products? (please tick Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational
Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50, 179–211.
one) Ajzen, I. (2002a). Constructing a TPB questionnaire: Conceptual and
methodological considerations. Retrieved May 29, 2014, from
Yes __ No, but I have had the opportunity __ No, I have
www.people.umass.edu/aisen/pdf/tpb.measurement.pdf.
not had the opportunity __ Ajzen, I. (2002b). Residual effects of past on later behavior:
Habituation and reasoned action perspectives. Personality and
• Have you ever signed a petition for Fair Trade? (please Social Psychology Review, 6, 107–122.
tick one) Ajzen, I. (2011). The theory of planned behavior: Reactions and
reflections. Psychology & Health, 26(9), 1113–1127.
Yes __ No, but I have had the opportunity __ No, I have Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1980). Understanding attitudes and
not had the opportunity__ predicting social behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Ajzen, I. & Fishbein, M. (2005). The influence of attitudes on
• Have you ever donated to the Fair Trade Organization? behavior. In D. Albarracı́n, B. T. Johnson & M. P. Zanna (Eds.),
(please tick one) The Handbook of Attitudes (pp. 173–221). Mahwah, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Yes __ No, but I have had the opportunity __ No, I have Alexandrov, A. (2010). Characteristics of single-item measures in
not had the opportunity__ Likert scale format. Electronic Journal of Business Research
Methods, 8(1), 1–12.
• Have you ever supported Fair Trade through other Anderson, W. T., & Cunningham, W. H. (1972). The socially
ways? (please tick one) conscious consumer. Journal of Marketing, 36(3), 23–31.
Andorfer, V. A., & Liebe, U. (2012). Research on fair trade
Yes __ No __ If yes, please specify:__ consumption—A review. Journal of Business Ethics, 106(4),
415–435.
Armitage, C., & Conner, M. (2001). Efficacy of the theory of planned
Ambivalence We measured ambivalence with five behaviour: A meta-analytic review. British Journal of Social
questions Psychology, 40, 471–497.

123
Consumer Ethics

Arnold, H. J. (1982). Moderator variables: A clarification of Davies, J., Foxall, G. R., & Pallister, J. (2002). Beyond the intention–
conceptual, analytic and psychometric issues. Organizational behaviour mythology an integrated model of recycling. Market-
Behavior and Human Performance, 29, 143–174. ing Theory, 2(1), 29–113.
Basil, D. Z., Ridgway, N. M., & Basil, M. D. (2006). Guilt appeals: De Cannière, M. H., De Pelsmacker, P., & Geuens, M. (2009).
The mediating effect of responsibility. Psychology and Market- Relationship quality and the theory of planned behavior models
ing, 23(12), 1035–1054. of behavioral intentions and purchase behavior. Journal of
Bray, J., Johns, N., & Kilburn, D. (2011). An exploratory study into Business Research, 62, 82–92.
the factors impeding ethical consumption. Journal of Business Devinney, T. M., Auger, P., & Eckhardt, G. (2010). The myth of the
Ethics, 98, 597–608. ethical consumer. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Byrne, B. M. (2001). Structural equation modeling with AMOS, basic Doherty, B., Davies, I. A., & Tranchell, S. (2013). Where now for fair
concepts, applications and programming. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. trade? Business History, 55(2), 161–189.
Camerer, C. F., Loewenstein, G., & Rabin, M. (Eds.). (2011). Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of attitudes. Fort
Advances in behavioral economics. Princeton, NJ: Princeton Worth, TX: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.
University Press. Evans, D., & Norman, P. (2003). Predicting adolescent pedestrians’
Carrigan, M., & Attalla, A. (2001). The myth of the ethical road-crossing intentions: An application and extension of the
consumer—Do ethics matter in purchase behaviour? Journal of theory of planned behaviour. Health Education Research, 18,
Consumer Marketing, 18(7), 560–577. 267–277.
Carrington, M. J., Neville, B. A., & Whitwell, G. J. (2010). Why Fife-Schaw, C., Sheeran, P., & Norman, P. (2007). Simulating
ethical consumers don’t walk their talk: Towards a framework behaviour change interventions based on the theory of planned
for understanding the gap between the ethical purchase inten- behaviour: Impacts on intention and action. British Journal of
tions and actual buying behavior of ethically minded consumers. Social Psychology, 46, 43–68.
Journal of Business Ethics, 97, 139–158. Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, attitude, intention, and
Carrington, M. J., Neville, B. A., & Whitwell, G. J. (2014). Lost in behavior: An introduction to theory and research. Reading, MA:
translation: Exploring the ethical consumer intention–behavior Addison-Wesley.
gap. Journal of Business Research, 67(1), 2759–2767. Francis, J. J., Eccles, M. P., Johnston, M., Walker, A., Grimshaw, J.,
Castro, P., Garrido, M., Reis, E., & Menezes, J. (2009). Ambivalence Foy, R., et al. (2004a). Constructing questionnaires based on the
and conservation behaviour: An exploratory study on the theory of planned behaviour: A manual for health services
recycling of metal cans. Journal of Environmental Psychology, researchers. Newcastle: University of Newcastle.
29(1), 24–33. Francis, J. J., Johnston, M., Eccles, M. P., Grimshaw, J., & Kaner, E.
Chan, K. (1998). Mass communication and pro-environmental F. S. (2004b). Measurement issues in the theory of planned
behaviour: Waste recycling in Hong Kong. Journal of Environ- behaviour: A supplement to the manual for constructing
mental Management, 52, 317–325. questionnaires based on the theory of planned behaviour.
Chang, M. K. (1998). Predicting unethical behavior: A comparison of Newcastle: University of Newcastle.
the theory of reasoned action and the theory of planned behavior. Frazier, P. A., Tix, A. P., & Barron, K. E. (2004). Testing moderator
Journal of Business Ethics, 17(16), 1825–1834. and mediator effects in counselling psychology research. Journal
Charng, H.-W., Piliavin, J. A., & Callero, P. L. (1988). Role identity of Counselling Psychology, 51(1), 115–134.
and reasoned action in the prediction of repeated behavior. Fridell, G. (2009). The co-operative and the corporation: Competing
Social Psychology Quarterly, 51, 303–317. visions of the future of fair trade. Journal of Business Ethics,
Chatzidakis, A., Hibbert, S., & Smith, A. P. (2007). Why people don’t 86(1), 81–95.
take their concerns about fair trade to the supermarket: The role Fukukawa, K. (2002). Developing a framework for ethically
of neutralization. Journal of Business Ethics, 74, 89–100. questionable behavior in consumption. Journal of Business
Chatzidakis, A., Maclaran, P., & Bradshaw, A. (2012). Heterotopian Ethics, 14, 99–119.
space and the utopics of ethical and green consumption. Journal Godin, G., Conner, M., & Sheeran, P. (2005). Bridging the intention–
of Marketing Management, 28(3–4), 494–515. behaviour ‘‘gap’’: The role of moral norm. British Journal of
Cloake, F. (2014, May). Where are we to turn if we want to be ethical Social Psychology, 44, 497–512.
food shoppers these days? The Guardian, Retrieved May 29, Hagger, M. S., & Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. (2006). Self-identity and the
2014 from http://www.theguardian.com/commentisfree/2014/ theory of planned behaviour: Between- and within-participants
may/24/ethical-food-shopping-fairtrade-report. analyses. British Journal of Social Psychology, 45, 731–757.
Cohen, J., & Cohen, P. (1983). Applied multiple regression/correla- Hagger, M. S., Chatzisarantis, N., & Biddle, S. J. (2001). The
tion analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. influence of self-efficacy and past behaviour on the physical
Conner, M., & Armitage, C. J. (1998). Extending the theory of activity intentions of young people. Journal of Sports Sciences,
planned behavior: A review and avenues for further research. 19(9), 711–725.
Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 28, 1429–1464. Hagger, M. S., Chatzisarantis, N. L., & Biddle, S. J. (2002). A meta-
Conner, M., Sparks, P., Povey, R., James, R., Shepherd, R., & analytic review of the theories of reasoned action and planned
Armitage, C. J. (2002). Moderator effects of attitudinal ambiv- behavior in physical activity: Predictive validity and the
alence on attitude–behaviour relationships. European Journal of contribution of additional variables. Journal of Sport & Exercise
Social Psychology, 32(5), 705–718. Psychology, 24(1), 3–32.
Costarelli, S., & Colloca, P. (2004). The effects of attitudinal Haight, C., & Henderson, D. R. (2010). Fair trade is counterproduc-
ambivalence on pro-environmental behavioural intentions. Jour- tive and unfair: Rejoinder. Economic Affairs, 30(1), 88–91.
nal of Environmental Psychology, 24(3), 279–288. Hair, J. F., Anderson, R. E., Tatham, R. L., & Black, W. C. (2009).
Cremin, C. (2012). The social logic of late capitalism: Guilt fetishism Multivariate data analysis (7th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ:
and the culture of crisis industry. Cultural Sociology, 6(1), Prentice Hall.
45–60. Harrison, R., Newholm, T., & Shaw, D. (Eds.). (2005). The ethical
Darrow, A. L., & Kahl, D. R. (1982). A comparison of moderated consumer. London: Sage.
regression techniques considering strength of effect. Journal of Hassan, L., Shaw, D., & Shiu, D. (2014). Who says there is an
Management, 8, 35–47. intention-behaviour gap? Assessing the empirical evidence of an

123
A. Chatzidakis et al.

intention-behaviour gap in ethical consumption. Journal of Mohr, J. J., Fisher, R. J., & Nevin, J. R. (1996). Collaborative
Business Ethics, forthcoming. communication in interfirm relationships: moderating effects of
Henderson, D. R. (2008). Fair trade is counterproductive—and unfair. integration and control. Journal of Marketing, 60(3), 103–115.
Economic Affairs, 28(3), 62–64. Nicholls, A., & Lee, N. (2006). Purchase decision making in fair trade
Hunt, S. D., Sparkman, R. D, Jr, & Wilcox, J. B. (1982). The pretest and the ethical purchase ‘‘gap’’: ‘‘Is there a fair-trade Twix?’’.
in survey research: Issues and preliminary Findings. Journal of Journal of Strategic Marketing, 14, 369–386.
Marketing Research, 19(2), 269–273. Norman, P., & Conner, M. (2006). The theory of planned behaviour
Hunt, S. D., & Vitell, S. J. (1986). A general theory of marketing and binge drinking: Assessing the moderating role of past
ethics. Journal of Macromarketing, 6, 5–15. behaviour within the theory of planned behaviour. British
Hunt, S. D., & Vitell, S. J. (1992). The general theory of marketing Journal of Health Psychology, 11(1), 55–70.
ethics: A retrospective and revision. In C. Smith & J. Quelch Norman, P., Conner, M., & Bell, R. (2000). The theory of planned
(Eds.), Ethics in marketing (pp. 775–784). Homewood, IL: behaviour and exercise: Evidence for the moderating role of past
Irwin. behaviour. British Journal of Health Psychology, 5(3), 249–261.
Hunt, S. D., & Vitell, S. J. (2006). The general theory of marketing Notani, A. S. (1998). Moderators of perceived behavioral control’s
ethics: A revision and three Questions. Journal of Macromar- predictiveness in the theory of planned behavior: A meta-
keting, 26(2), 143–153. analysis. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 7, 247–271.
Jackson, C., Smith, A., & Conner, M. (2003). Applying an extended Ogden, J. (2003). Some problems with social cognition models: A
version of the theory of planned behaviour to physical activity. pragmatic and conceptual analysis. Health Psychology, 22,
Journal of Sports Sciences, 21(2), 119–133. 424–428.
Kaiser, F. G., Ranney, M., Hartig, T., & Bowler, P. A. (1999). Ecological Ozcaglar-Toulouse, N., Shiu, E., & Shaw, D. (2006). In search of fair
behaviour, environmental attitude, and feelings of responsibility for trade: Ethical consumer decision making in France. International
the environment. European Psychologist, 4(2), 59–74. Journal of Consumer Studies, 30(5), 502–514.
Kaiser, F. G., & Shimoda, T. A. (1999). Responsibility as a predictor Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Lee, J.-Y., & Podsakoff, N. P.
of ecological behaviour. Journal of Environmental Psychology, (2003). Common method biases in behavioral research: A
19, 243–253. critical review of the literature and recommended biases. Journal
Kalafatis, S. P., Pollard, M., East, R., & Tsogas, M. H. (1999). Green of Applied Psychology, 88, 879–903.
marketing and Ajzen’s theory of planned behaviour: A cross- Rindfleisch, A., Burroughs, J. A., & Wong, N. (2008). The safety of
market examination. Journal of Consumer Marketing, 16(5), objects: Materialism, existential insecurity and brand connec-
441–460. tion. Journal of Consumer Research, 36(1), 1–16.
Keynote Report. (2012). Green and ethical consumer market assess- Rossiter, J. R. (2002). The C-OAR-SE procedure for scale develop-
ment. Retrieved May 29, 2014 from http://www.keynote.co.uk/ ment in marketing. International Journal of Research in
market-intelligence/view/product/10537/green-and-ethical-con Marketing, 19(4), 305–335.
sumer?medium=download. Schoonhoven, C. B. (1981). Problems with contingency theory:
Keynote Report. (2013). Green and ethical consumer market update. Testing assumptions hidden within the language of contingency
Retrieved May 29, 2014 from http://www.keynote.co.uk/market- ‘‘theory’’. Administrative Science Quarterly, 26, 349–377.
intelligence/view/product/10854/green-%26-ethical-consumer. Schwartz, S. H. (1977). Normative influences on altruism. In L.
Kline, R. B. (2005). Principles and practice of structural equation Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology
modeling (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press. (Vol. 10, pp. 221–279). New York: Academic Press.
Kraft, P., Rise, J., Sutton, S., & Røysamb, E. (2005). Perceived Schwartz, S. H., & Howard, J. A. (1980). Explanations of the
difficulty in the theory of planned behaviour: Perceived behav- moderating effect of responsibility denial on the personal norm-
ioural control or affective attitude? British Journal of Social behavior relationship. Social Psychology Quarterly, 43(4),
Psychology, 44(3), 479–496. 441–446.
Krier, J.-M. (2001). Fair trade in Europe 2001: Facts and figures on Schwartz, S. H., & Howard, J. A. (1981). A normative decision-
the fair trade sector in 18 European countries. Maastricht: making model of altruism. In J. P. Rushton & R. M. Sorrentino
EFTA Research Report. (Eds.), Altruism and helping behavior: Social, personality, and
Lindell, M. K., & Whitney, D. J. (2001). Accounting for common developmental perspectives (pp. 189–211). Hillsdale, NJ:
method variance in cross-sectional research designs. Journal of Erlbaum.
Applied Psychology, 86(1), 114–121. Shaw, D., & Clarke, I. (1999). Belief formation in ethical consumer
Lovelock, C. H., Stiff, R., Cullwick, D., & Kaufman, I. M. (1976). An groups: An exploratory study. Marketing Intelligence and
evaluation of the effectiveness of the drop-off questionnaire Planning, 17(2), 109–119.
delivery. Journal of Marketing Research, 13(4), 358–364. Shaw, D., & Shiu, E. (2002a). An assessment of ethical obligation and
Low, W., & Davenport, E. (2005). Has the medium (roast) become self-identity in ethical consumer decision-making: A structural
the message? The ethics of marketing fair trade in the equation modeling approach. International Journal of Consumer
mainstream. International Marketing Research, 22(5), 494–511. Studies, 26(4), 286–293.
Malhotra, N. K., Kim, S. S., & Patil, A. (2006). Common method Shaw, D., & Shiu, E. (2002b). The role of ethical obligation and self-
variance in IS research: A comparison of alternative approaches identity in ethical consumer choice. International Journal of
and a reanalysis of past research. Management Science, 52(12), Consumer Studies, 26(2), 109–116.
1865–1883. Shaw, D., & Shiu, E. (2003). Ethics in consumer choice: A
Manstead, A. S. R., & Parker, D. (1995). Evaluating and extending multivariate modelling approach. European Journal of Market-
the theory of planned behaviour. European Review of Social ing, 37(10), 1485–1498.
Psychology, 6(1), 69–95. Shaw, D., Shiu, E., & Clarke, I. (2000). The contribution of ethical
McClelland, G. H., & Judd, C. M. (1993). Statistical difficulties of obligation and self-identity to the theory of planned behaviour:
detecting interaction and moderation effects. Psychological An exploration of ethical consumers. Journal of Marketing
Bulletin, 114(2), 376–390. Management, 16, 879–894.
Mintel Report. (2007). Green and ethical consumers. Retrieved May Smart, J. J. C., & Williams, B. (1973). Utilitarianism for & against.
29, 2014 from http://oxygen.mintel.com/display/221206/. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

123
Consumer Ethics

Smith, A. (2009). Evaluating the criticisms of fair trade. Economic Trafimow, D., Sheeran, P., Conner, M., & Finlay, K. A. (2002).
Affairs, 29(4), 29–36. Evidence that perceived behavioural control is a multidimen-
Sparks, P., & Guthrie, C. A. (1998). Self-identity and the theory of sional construct: Perceived control and perceived difficulty.
planned behavior: A useful addition or an unhelpful artifice? British Journal of Social Psychology, 41(1), 101–121.
Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 28, 1393–1410. Tyler, T. R., Orwin, R., & Schurer, L. (1982). Defensive denial and
Sparks, P., & Shepherd, R. (2002). The role of moral judgments high cost prosocial behaviour. Basic and Applied Social
within expectancy-value-based attitude-behavior models. Ethics Psychology, 3(4), 267–281.
and Behavior, 12(4), 299–321. Verplanken, B., & Aarts, H. (1999). Habit, attitude, and planned
Sparks, P., Shepherd, R., & Frewer, L. J. (1995). Assessing and behaviour: Is habit an empty construct or an interesting case of
structuring attitudes toward the use of gene technology in food goal-directed automaticity? European Review of Social Psychol-
production: The role of perceived ethical obligation. Basic and ogy, 10(1), 101–134.
Applied Social Psychology, 16(3), 267–285. Vitell, S. J., & Ho, F. N. (1997). Ethical decision making in
Strong, C. (1996). Features contributing to the growth of ethical marketing: A synthesis and evaluation of scales measuring the
consumerism: A preliminary investigation. Marketing Intelli- various components of decision making in ethical situations.
gence & Planning, 14(5), 5–13. Journal of Business Ethics, 16, 699–717.
Sudman, S., Bradburn, N. M., & Schwarz, N. (1996). Thinking about Webster, F. E. (1975). Developing the characteristics of the socially
answers. The application of cognitive processes to survey conscious consumer. Journal of Consumer Research, 2,
methodology. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. 185–196.
Sykes, G. M., & Matza, D. (1957). Techniques of neutralization: A Williams, B. A. (1972). Knowledge and reasons. Problems in the
theory of delinquency. American Sociological Review, 22(6), theory of knowledge/problèmes de la théorie de la connaissance
664–670. (pp. 1–11). Hague, Netherlands: Springer.
Terry, D. J., Hogg, M. A., & White, K. M. (1999). The theory of Yeow, P., Dean, A., & Tucker, D. (2013). Bags for life: The
planned behaviour: Self-identity, social identity and group embedding of ethical consumerism. Journal of Business Ethics,.
norms. British Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 225–244. doi:10.1007/s10551-013-1900-2.

123

You might also like