Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 22

c o r t e x 4 8 ( 2 0 1 2 ) 1 9 4 e2 1 5

Available online at www.sciencedirect.com

Journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/cortex

Special issue: Research report

A review of fronto-striatal and fronto-cortical brain


abnormalities in children and adults with Attention Deficit
Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and new evidence for
dysfunction in adults with ADHD during motivation and
attention

Ana Cubillo, Rozmin Halari, Anna Smith, Eric Taylor and Katya Rubia*
Department of Child Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London, UK

article info abstract

Article history: Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) has long been associated with abnor-
Received 14 October 2010 malities in frontal brain regions. In this paper we review the current structural and
Revised 12 January 2011 functional imaging evidence for abnormalities in children and adults with ADHD in fronto-
Accepted 11 April 2011 striatal, fronto-parieto-temporal, fronto-cerebellar and fronto-limbic regions and
Published online 27 April 2011 networks. While the imaging studies in children with ADHD are more numerous and
consistent, an increasing number of studies suggests that these structural and functional
Keywords: abnormalities in fronto-cortical and fronto-subcortical networks persist into adulthood,
Adult ADHD despite a relative symptomatic improvement in the adult form of the disorder.
fMRI We furthermore present new data that support the notion of a persistence of neurofunc-
Reward tional deficits in adults with ADHD during attention and motivation functions. We show
Sustained attention that a group of medication-naı̈ve young adults with ADHD behaviours who were followed
Review up 20 years from a childhood ADHD diagnosis show dysfunctions in lateral fronto-striato-
parietal regions relative to controls during sustained attention, as well as in ventromedial
orbitofrontal regions during reward, suggesting dysfunctions in cognitive-attentional as
well as motivational neural networks. The lateral fronto-striatal deficit findings, further-
more, were strikingly similar to those we have previously observed in children with ADHD
during the same task, reinforcing the notion of persistence of fronto-striatal dysfunctions
in adult ADHD. The ventromedial orbitofrontal deficits, however, were associated with
comorbid conduct disorder (CD), highlighting the potential confound of comorbid antiso-
cial conditions on paralimbic brain deficits in ADHD.
Our review supported by the new data therefore suggest that both adult and childhood
ADHD are associated with brain abnormalities in fronto-cortical and fronto-subcortical
systems that mediate the control of cognition and motivation. The brain deficits in
ADHD therefore appear to be multi-systemic and to persist throughout the lifespan.
ª 2011 Elsevier Srl. All rights reserved.

* Corresponding author. Department of Child Psychiatry/SGDP, P046, King’s College London, Institute of Psychiatry, 16 De Crespigny Park,
London SE5 8AF, UK.
E-mail address: katya.rubia@kcl.ac.uk (K. Rubia).
0010-9452/$ e see front matter ª 2011 Elsevier Srl. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2011.04.007
c o r t e x 4 8 ( 2 0 1 2 ) 1 9 4 e2 1 5 195

1. Introduction suggesting multiple developmental pathways for ADHD


(Makris et al., 2009; Nigg and Casey, 2005; Sonuga-Barke et al.,
Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) is charac- 2010; Willcutt et al., 2005), with structural and functional
terised by age-inappropriate symptoms of inattention, abnormalities in shared but dissociable functional networks
impulsiveness and hyperactivity (Diagnostic Standard underlying the observed deficits (Makris et al., 2009).
manual-IV – DSM-IV)(American Psychiatric Association, 1994).
It disrupts academic and social development, and is associated
with significant psychiatric comorbidities and mental health 3. Structural and functional neuroimaging of
problems in adult life (Faraone et al., 2007; Spencer et al., 2007; childhood and adult ADHD
Taylor et al., 1996). ADHD affects 3e8% school-aged children
(American Psychiatric Association, 1994; Froehlich et al., 2007) 3.1. Structural studies
and persists into adulthood in 65% of cases (Barkley et al., 2002;
Biederman et al., 2006), affecting 4% of the adult population Using structural magnetic resonance imaging (sMRI), children
(Faraone and Biederman, 2005; Kessler et al., 2006). with ADHD relative to controls have shown consistent abnor-
malities in late developing fronto-striatal, fronto-temporo-
parietal and fronto-cerebellar networks. These brain regions
2. Neuropsychological deficits in children are known to mediate the above mentioned cognitive control
and adults with ADHD functions that are impaired in the disorder. Thus, reduced
volume and cortical thickness have been observed in several
Executive functions (EF) are defined as functions that are frontal brain regions, in parieto-temporal areas, the basal
necessary for mature adult goal-directed behaviour, such as ganglia, posterior cingulate (PCC), the cerebellum and the
set-shifting and set maintenance, higher level and selective splenium of the corpus callosum (Batty et al., 2010; Carmona
attention, interference control, motor inhibition, integration et al., 2005, 2009; Castellanos et al., 2002; Mackie et al., 2007;
across space and time, planning, decision making, temporal Shaw et al., 2006; for reviews see Krain and Castellanos, 2006;
foresight and working memory (Stuss and Alexander, 2000). It Rubia, 2010). A meta-analysis of structural studies using
should be noted that we use the wider definition of EF that region of interest analyses showed that the largest volume
includes attention functions as well as specific aspects of reductions in ADHD children relative to controls were in
temporal processing such as temporal foresight since they are several frontal brain regions, total and right cerebral volumes,
underlying basic functions for all goal-directed behaviours. the posterior inferior vermis of the cerebellum, the splenium of
“Cool” EF are mediated by ventrolateral and dorsolateral the corpus callosum and right caudate (Valera et al., 2007). A
(DLPFC) fronto-striatal, fronto-cerebellar and fronto-parietal subsequent meta-analysis of whole-brain voxel-based
neural networks. More recently, a differentiation has been morphometry studies in children with ADHD found that the
made between “cool” cognitive EF, typically elicited by rela- most consistent regional gray matter reduction in ADHD
tively abstract and descontextualized problems, and “hot” patients compared to controls was in right putamen and globus
motivation and reward-related EF, which involve the regulation pallidus (Ellison-Wright et al., 2008). Longitudinal imaging
of affect and motivation (Zelazo and Muller, 2002). Thus, “hot” studies have provided some evidence that the structural
EF consist of tasks of reward-related decision making, reversal abnormalities observed in children with ADHD compared to
of rewarded stimuluseresponse associations, temporal dis- healthy peers in frontal, striatal, parietal and cerebellar regions
counting and other EF that are dependent on motivation and may be due to a delay in structural maturation (Castellanos
reward. “Hot” EF are mediated by mesolimbic ventromedial et al., 2002; Shaw et al., 2007). The peak of cortical thickness
(VMPFC) and orbitofrontal (OFC)-striatal and limbic circuits maturation was found to be delayed in ADHD children relative
(Zelazo and Muller, 2002). In neuropsychological studies, as to typical controls by an average of 3 years across all cortical
a group, child and adult patients with ADHD have shown defi- regions, with up to 4e5 years delay in frontal and temporal
cits both in “cool” EF (Marchetta et al., 2008; Martinussen et al., areas, respectively (Shaw et al., 2007). This was further rein-
2005; Rubia et al., 2001, 2007a; Sergeant et al., 2002; Valko et al., forced by findings that the rate of cortical thinning in these
2010; Willcutt et al., 2005) and “hot” EF (Antrop et al., 2006; regions, which is thought to mirror synaptic pruning and
Bitsakou et al., 2009; Dalen et al., 2004; Luman et al., 2005; reflect structural and cognitive maturation, has been shown to
Marco et al., 2009; Sagvolden et al., 1998), for review see be inversely associated with the severity of hyperactivity and
(Rubia, 2010). Deficits have furthermore been observed in impulsiveness in normal development (Shaw et al., 2011).
temporal (for review see Rubia et al., 2009a) and perceptual Structural MRI studies in adult ADHD have observed
processes (Banaschewski et al., 2006; Boonstra et al., 2005). abnormalities in similar cortical brain regions, including
However, while as a group ADHD children show impairments deficits in overall cortical gray matter, volumes and cortical
in these functions, a proportion of ADHD children is not thickness of superior frontal and OFC, anterior cingulate
impaired in any of these functions (Nigg et al., 2005; Sonuga- (ACC), Inferior frontal cortex (IFC), DLPFC, PCC, temporo-
Barke et al., 2010) and there are subgroups of children with parietal, cerebellar and occipital regions (Amico et al., 2010;
ADHD who are impaired in either “cool” EF, “hot” EF or temporal Biederman et al., 2008; Hesslinger et al., 2002; Makris et al.,
processes with only some of them having overlapping deficits 2010; Seidman et al., 2006), as well as in subcortical brain
(Nigg et al., 2005; Sonuga-Barke et al., 2010). Different theoret- areas including the caudate, nucleus accumbens and the
ical approaches have attempted to explain this heterogeneity amygdala (Almeida Montes et al., 2010; Frodl et al., 2009;
196 c o r t e x 4 8 ( 2 0 1 2 ) 1 9 4 e2 1 5

Seidman et al., 2006). However, there have also been negative emotion processing in ADHD. During fearful facial expression
findings with respect to structural differences in frontal lobes, processing, children with ADHD compared to healthy controls
basal ganglia, amygdala and hippocampus (Ahrendts et al., in have shown either no differences in brain activation (Marsh
press; Amico et al., 2010; Depue et al., 2010b; Perlov et al., et al., 2008) or enhanced activation in the amygdala
2008). (Brotman et al., 2010), and during visualisation of negative
Diffusion tensor imaging studies furthermore have arousing pictures reduced activation was observed in insula,
demonstrated abnormalities at the neural network level. Thus, basal ganglia and thalamus (Herpertz et al., 2008).
children and adults with ADHD compared to controls have Fewer fMRI studies have been conducted in adult ADHD,
shown reduced white matter connectivity in fronto-striatal, and findings are more inconsistent. This is likely due to the
cingulate, as well as fronto-parietal, fronto-cerebellar and impact of confounding factors, more pronounced in adult
parieto-occipital white matter tracts (Ashtari et al., 2005; compared to childhood ADHD imaging studies, such as small
Davenport et al., 2010; Konrad et al., 2010; Makris et al., 2008; sample sizes, high rates of comorbidity, long-term medication
Pavuluri et al., 2009; for a review see Konrad and Eickhoff, 2010). history and the need for a retrospective diagnosis of ADHD in
Our recent meta-analysis of 14 whole-brain voxel-based childhood (Cubillo and Rubia, 2010). Adults with ADHD have
morphometry studies in children and adults with ADHD, shown underactivation compared to controls in OFC, IFC,
including in total 378 ADHD and 344 controls, showed that the DLPFC, ACC, striatal, premotor, parietal and cerebellar brain
most consistent regional gray matter reduction in ADHD regions during motor and interference inhibition (Banich et al.,
patients compared to controls was in right lenticular nucleus, 2009; Burgess et al., 2010; Bush et al., 1999; Cubillo et al., 2010, in
including the caudate. A meta-regression analysis, however, press; Epstein et al., 2007; Schneider et al., 2010), inhibition of
showed that the grey matter volume size was associated with memories (Depue et al., 2010a), working memory (Hale et al.,
age, with volumes becoming progressively more normal in 2007; Valera et al., 2005, 2010a; Wolf et al., 2009), cognitive
older patients, resulting in a normalisation in the adult switching (Cubillo et al., 2010; Dibbets et al., 2010) and senso-
subgroup. A meta-regression analysis on the effect of psy- rimotor timing (Valera et al., 2010b). Other studies, however,
chostimulants showed that the morphological deficit was observed increased activation in medial frontal, DLPFC, pre-
associated with medication status, so that studies that included motor, parietal and occipital cortices during inhibitory and
a high proportion of stimulant-medicated patients no longer working memory tasks (Banich et al., 2009; Dibbets et al., 2009,
showed the basal ganglia abnormalities (Nakao et al., in press). 2010; Epstein et al., 2007; Hale et al., 2007; Schneider et al., 2010;
for a review see Cubillo and Rubia, 2010). Functional abnor-
3.2. Functional imaging studies malities during reward-related tasks have been observed in
adults with ADHD in OFC and limbic regions. Thus, patients
Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies have compared to controls showed underactivation in VS during
provided evidence for the fronto-striatal deficit hypothesis of gain anticipation in a monetary incentive delay task, but
ADHD in addition to providing evidence for wider deficits. increased activation in IFC, OFC, DLPFC and striatum during
Thus, children with ADHD have shown underactivation rela- gain outcome (Ströhle et al., 2008). During temporal discount-
tive to controls in the DLPFC/IFC, ACC, caudate, supplemen- ing, reduced activation was found in VS and amygdala during
tary motor area (SMA) as well as in temporo-parietal cortices immediate choices whereas increased striatal and amygdala
during motor response inhibition (Booth et al., 2005; Durston activations were observed during delayed choices (Plichta
et al., 2003, 2006; Epstein et al., 2007; Pliszka et al., 2006; et al., 2009). During emotion processing tasks, medication-
Rubia et al., 1999, 2005, 2008, 2010b; Smith et al., 2006; naı̈ve adults with ADHD compared to healthy controls showed
Suskauer et al., 2008a, 2008b), interference inhibition underactivation in VS in response to unexpected positive
(Konrad et al., 2006; Rubia et al., 2009b, 2011; Vaidya et al., versus neutral pictures, and in subgenual cingulate in
2005) as well as during vigilant, selective and flexible atten- response to unexpected negative versus neutral pictures
tion (Rubia et al., 2009b, 2009c, 2009d, 2010a, 2010b, 2011, in (Schlochtermeier et al., in press). Furthermore, symptom
press; Smith et al., 2006; Stevens et al., 2007; Tamm et al., severity has been found to be negatively correlated with key
2004, 2006; for meta-analysis and review see Dickstein et al., fronto-striato-thalamic, temporo-parietal and cerebellar
2006; and Rubia, 2010, respectively). Furthermore, during regions, thus overlapping with areas observed to be under-
tasks involving temporal processing, children with ADHD activated during motor and interference inhibition, switching
have shown reduced activation compared to controls in dorsal and working memory tasks (Cubillo et al., 2010, in press; Depue
and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, SMA, ACC and cerebellum et al., 2010b; Valera et al., 2010a).
(Durston et al., 2007; Rubia et al., 1999, 2001, 2009a; Smith
et al., 2008; Vloet et al., 2010). Using “hot” EF tasks, abnormal 3.3. Functional connectivity
activation has been observed in children with ADHD relative
to healthy controls in ventral striatum (VS) during reward Recent evidence from functional connectivity fMRI studies
anticipation (Scheres et al., 2007), in ventrolateral demonstrates that the functional abnormalities in ADHD not
fronto-striato-thalamo-parieto-cerebellar network during only affect isolated brain regions but also the functional inter-
a temporal discounting task (Rubia et al., 2009a), in precuneus, regional interconnectivity between these regions. Thus,
PCC (Rubia et al., 2009c) and in OFC, temporal regions and during the resting state, children and adults with ADHD
cerebellum during rewarded trials within a Continuous showed reduced functional connectivity relative to healthy
Performance Task (CPT) (Rubia et al., 2009d). Very few fMRI controls in fronto-striatal, cingulate, fronto-parietal, temporo-
studies have tested for neurofunctional deficits during parietal and fronto-cerebellar networks (Cao et al., 2006, 2009;
c o r t e x 4 8 ( 2 0 1 2 ) 1 9 4 e2 1 5 197

Castellanos et al., 2008; Konrad et al., 2010; Uddin et al., 2008; IFC/DLPFC, caudate (Cubillo et al., in press) and ACC (Burgess
Zang et al., 2007; for review see Konrad and Eickhoff, 2010). et al., 2010; Bush et al., 1999) during selective attention/
Some studies, however, also reported increased inter-regional conflict inhibition in interference inhibition tasks, and
connectivity between ACC, striatum and temporo-cerebellar decreased inferior fronto-striatal and parietal activation
regions (Tian et al., 2006; Wang et al., 2009; Zang et al., 2007; during flexible attention in cognitive switching tasks (Cubillo
Zhu et al., 2005). Reduced functional connectivity has been et al., 2010; Dibbets et al., 2010). However, hardly any studies
observed in the context of cognitive tasks in children with have tested for neurofunctional deficits during tasks that are
ADHD relative to controls between IFC and the basal ganglia, purposely designed to measure selective and sustained
parietal lobes and cerebellum, and between cerebellum, attention such as continuous performance or target detection
parietal and striatal brain regions during sustained attention tasks. Given that deficits in sustained attention as measured
(Rubia et al., 2009d), interference inhibition and time estima- in the CPT are one of the most consistent findings of the child
tion (Vloet et al., 2010). and adult ADHD literature (Epstein et al., 2001; Hervey et al.,
In adults with ADHD, deficits in functional inter-regional 2004; Marchetta et al., 2008; Willcutt et al., 2005), it is
connectivity relative to healthy controls were observed surprising that no fMRI study in adult ADHD has tested the
between right and left IFC, and between the right IFC and neurofunctional correlates of this function. Furthermore, the
other areas including basal ganglia, cingulate, parieto- findings between fMRI studies of the reward system in adults
temporal and cerebellar regions during motor response inhi- with ADHD are inconsistent. While functional abnormalities
bition and working memory (Cubillo et al., 2010; Wolf et al., in ADHD adults relative to controls were observed in similar
2009). In adults, however, there is also additional evidence regions across studies, in particular in VS, amygdala and
for compensatory increased connectivity between ACC, VMPFC/OFC cortex, the direction of some of these dysfunc-
superior frontal lobe and cerebellum (Wolf et al., 2009). tions have been different. For example, some studies observed
OFC underactivation (Dibbets et al., 2009) while others
3.4. Conclusions observed OFC overactivation (Ströhle et al., 2008). The incon-
sistent results are likely due to typical confounds in adult
Neuropsychological evidence shows that children and adults ADHD imaging studies, such as small sample sizes, previous
with ADHD are impaired in “cool” as well as “hot” EF. Struc- medication history, presence of comorbidities and retrospec-
tural and functional imaging studies show that these abnor- tive diagnosis of ADHD in childhood (Cubillo and Rubia, 2010).
malities in cognitive control are mediated by abnormalities in To avoid these confounds, we recruited medication-naı̈ve
lateral inferior and dorsolateral prefrontal as well as some adults from an ongoing longitudinal study, with a confirmed
medial frontal regions such as rostral ACC and SMA and their diagnosis of childhood ADHD, who were followed up 20 years
regional interconnections with striatal, cerebellar, and into adulthood, where they showed persistent symptoms of
parieto-temporal areas. Weaknesses in motivation control as inattention/hyperactivity. We aimed to investigate the neural
measured in “hot” EF appear to be related to lateral orbito- correlates of the interaction between “hot”, motivational and
frontal and ventromedial prefrontal regions and their associ- “cool” cognitive processes within a rewarded CPT task that
ated ventral striatal and limbic areas. While studies in measured the effect of reward upon sustained attention.
children with ADHD are more numerous and consistent, the Furthermore, we aimed to investigate whether these adult
emerging evidence from imaging studies in adult ADHD patients present similar functional abnormalities as those
suggests that the structural and functional brain abnormali- observed previously in children with ADHD during the same
ties observed in children with ADHD persist into adult ADHD task (Rubia et al., 2009c, 2009d). Motivation is particularly
in those who do not grow out of the disorder. relevant for attention processes and motivation and attention
are hence closely interrelated. Reward and enhanced arousal
states have shown to potentiate selective (Engelmann and
4. New data: lateral inferior fronto-striatal Pessoa, 2007; Krawczyk et al., 2007; Lang et al., 1990;
and orbitofrontal-ventromedial brain Rothermund et al., 2001) and sustained attention functions
dysfunction in adults with childhood ADHD and (Tomporowski and Tinsley, 1996). Functional MRI studies have
persistent hyperactive/inattentive behaviours demonstrated that motivation in the form of reward can
during sustained attention and reward enhance activation within brain regions that mediate arousal
and selective attention such as ventrolateral prefrontal, pari-
Despite the reported persistence of inattention problems in etal and PCC cortices (Lang et al., 1990; Mohanty et al., 2008;
adult ADHD (Biederman et al., 2000), and consistent evidence Pochon et al., 2002; Rothermund et al., 2001; Small et al.,
for deficits in adult ADHD in tasks of sustained and selective 2005). Furthermore, we have shown that in healthy adoles-
attention such as the CPT (Hervey et al., 2004; Marchetta et al., cents and adults, reward within the CPT task further upre-
2008), few studies in adults with ADHD have focused on the gulates the same inferior fronto-striatal and temporo-parietal
neuroimaging correlates of sustained and selective attention regions that mediate sustained attention under the non-
functions. A few fMRI studies in adult ADHD have rewarded condition (Smith et al., 2011). In healthy children
co-measured higher executive selective attention functions and adults, sustained attention in the same and similar tasks
embedded within cognitive control tasks such as conflict activates inferior frontal, striatal, temporal and parietal
detection tasks or flexible attention during tasks of cognitive regions and cerebellum (Lawrence et al., 2003; Smith et al.,
flexibility. These studies show that adults with ADHD 2011; Tana et al., 2010; Voisin et al., 2006) and elicits under-
compared to healthy controls have underactivation in activation in these regions in children with ADHD (Rubia et al.,
198 c o r t e x 4 8 ( 2 0 1 2 ) 1 9 4 e2 1 5

2009c, 2009d). During the rewarded condition, healthy adults et al., in press). A diagnostic conference was held for each
activate VMPFC/OFC, ACC, striatal and temporo-parietal case, chaired by an experienced child psychiatrist (ET). All
regions (Smith et al., 2011), while children with ADHD show relevant DSM-IV diagnoses were reviewed, including ADHD
abnormal activation relative to controls in PCC and precuneus (Table 1), which required four of the DSM-IV criteria for inat-
(Rubia et al., 2009c), cerebellum, OFC and temporal regions tentiveness, hyperactivity-impulsiveness or both, significant
(Rubia et al., 2009d). functional impairment, and a score >10 on the AHI. As the
We hypothesised that medication-naı̈ve adults with diagnostic process was blind to childhood status, the age of
a confirmed childhood diagnosis of ADHD and persistent onset (before age 7) criteria was not included. All subjects met
hyperactive/inattentive behaviours in adulthood would show DSM-IV diagnostic criteria for ADHD, except for three who had
reduced activation relative to healthy controls in lateral IFC, subthreshold symptoms, coded in DSM-IV as “ADHD in partial
striatal, temporo-parietal and cerebellar brain regions during remission”. Like previous studies in adults with ADHD (i.e.,
sustained attention, and abnormal function in OFC, VMPFC Valera et al., 2010b), we decided to include these subjects,
and VS during reward, similar to the dysfunctions previously taking into account the age-related decrease in hyperactivity/
observed in children with ADHD during the same task and in impulsivity symptoms (Biederman et al., 2000), and the fact
adult ADHD during similar tasks. that remission defined in DSM-IV refers to fewer symptoms
than required and not to functional improvement, which has
4.1. Methods been criticised not to reflect adult characteristics of ADHD
(Faraone et al., 2000). There was no evidence for a selection
4.1.1. Subjects bias, since the scanned adults did not differ from the rest of
The patient group and the diagnostic procedures have been the group in their childhood measures of IQ, classroom or
described previously (Cubillo et al., 2010, in press). In brief, home hyperactivity symptoms, conduct disturbance or
patients were 11 male right-handed adults (mean age emotional problems [F(7,82)<1, p ¼ n.s.].
(years) ¼ 29, standard deviation (SD) ¼ 1, age range ¼ 26e30), Several patients presented a current Axis I comorbid
recruited from a 20-year prospective longitudinal epidemio- diagnosis: Anxiety (n ¼ 1), Mood (n ¼ 3), Conduct (n ¼ 1) and
logical study (Taylor et al., 1991). Six and 7 years old school non-stimulant Substance Related Disorders (n ¼ 2; Cannabis,
children were initially assessed (Taylor et al., 1991). Those Alcohol), although only one subject had suffered enough
who met criteria for hyperactive/inattentive behaviour impairment to attend specialists services.
according to both teacher and parent rating scales were fol- Controls were 15 adult right-handed age-matched males of
lowed up, and re-assessed at age 16e18 years (Danckaerts average intellectual ability, recruited through advertisement
et al., 2000; Taylor et al., 1996) and 26e30 years (Stringaris in the community (mean age ¼ 28, SD ¼ 3). Exclusion criteria
et al., in press). The subjects for this paper had also met for controls were present or past history of any mental
criteria for ADDH, which the DSM-III diagnosis was at the time disorder, substance abuse or psychotropic medication.
corresponding to the contemporary ADHD. Neurological abnormalities were exclusion criteria for all
The assessment in this 20 years follow-up included (i) subjects. All participants scored above cut-off on the Raven’s
Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia (SADS) Standard Progressive Matrices Intelligence Questionnaire
(Endicott and Spitzer, 1978), (ii) Adult Personality Functioning (Raven, 1960) (i.e., over 75; fifth percentile) (Converted IQ
Assessment (APFA) (Hill et al., 1989), (iii) Self-reported check- estimate: Controls: mean IQ ¼ 108, SD ¼ 12, Patients: mean
list of the DSM-IV items comprising the criteria for ADHD, (iv) IQ ¼ 92, SD ¼ 10). One-way analyses of variance (ANOVAs)
Adult Hyperactivity Interview (AHI) (Stringaris et al., in press), showed that the groups did not differ significantly in age
developed for this project which defines symptoms in terms [F (1,23) ¼ 1.67, p ¼ n.s.], but in IQ estimate [F(1,23) ¼ 13.18,
appropriate to adulthood. The scale ranges from 0 to 24. A p ¼ .001]. Since low IQ is associated with ADHD both in
score >10 is associated with poor social functioning (Stringaris children and adults (Bridgett and Walker, 2006; Crosbie and

Table 1 e Description of symptoms for adults with childhood ADHD and persistent hyperactivity/inattention symptoms.

Childhood diagnosis AHI scores Hyperactivity symptomsa Inattention symptomsa ADHD DSM-IV criteria

ADHD hyp þ CD 19 3 3 YES-combined


ADHD combined 24 3 3 YES-combined
ADHD hyp þ CD 24 3 3 YES-combined
ADHD combined 10 0 1 NO-inattentive symptoms
ADHD hyp þ CD 13 0 1 NO-inattentive symptoms
ADHD hyp 16 0 1 NO-inattentive symptoms
ADHD combined þ CD 10 2 1 YES-predominantly Hyperactive
CD þ ADHD hyp 18 2 3 YES-combined
ADHD combined 14 3 3 YES-combined
ADHD combined 25 3 1 YES-predominantly Hyperactive
ADHD hyp þ CD 14 2 2 YES-combined

Note: ADHD hyp ¼ ADHD hyperactive subtype.


a 3 ¼ level of problem impairing function and deserving diagnosis; 2 ¼ definite problem; 1 ¼ moderate problem; 0 ¼ no problem.
c o r t e x 4 8 ( 2 0 1 2 ) 1 9 4 e2 1 5 199

Schachar, 2001), covarying for IQ would not be appropriate end of the scanning session. Single letters were chosen as
(Miller and Chapman, 2001), while matching groups for IQ targets rather than complex letter combinations (CPT-AX) to
would create unrepresentative groups (Dennis et al., 2009). reduce the load on working memory (Rubia et al., 2009c;
Therefore, as suggested by some authors (Bridgett and Schmitz et al., 2008) (Fig. 1).
Walker, 2006), fMRI data were analysed with and without IQ For the fMRI analysis the contrast between non-rewarded
as a covariate to assess the impact of IQ. target trials and non-target trials measures the brain
The study was approved by the local ethics committee and response to sustained attention and will be labelled “sus-
written informed consent was obtained from all participants. tained attention contrast”. The contrast between rewarded
and non-rewarded target trials measures the effect of reward
4.1.2. fMRI paradigm: rewarded CPT upon sustained attention functions and will be labelled
A rapid, mixed trial, event related fMRI design was used with “reward contrast”.
jittered inter-trial-intervals (ITI) and randomised presentation
to optimise statistical efficiency. Subjects practised the task 4.1.3. Analysis of performance data
once prior to scanning. Repeated measures multiple two-way ANOVAs were per-
In the CPT task, subjects have to detect and respond with formed with group (adult ADHD; healthy controls) as
a button press to infrequent targets that are embedded in a between subjects factor and trial type (non-rewarded;
highly frequent non-targets that require no action and can rewarded) as within subjects factor for the dependent vari-
therefore be simply being ignored. The difficulty of the task ables of omission errors and reaction time (RT). Commission
therefore consists in detecting the rare targets. The task errors (responses to non-targets) were compared using
measures selective and sustained attention and target detec- a t-test.
tion (Conners, 1993). The computerised fMRI adaptation of the
rewarded CPT (Rubia et al., 2009c, 2009d; Schmitz et al., 2008; 4.1.4. fMRI image acquisition and analyses
Smith et al., 2011) consists of a stream of 416 stimuli (letters) of Gradient-echo echoplanar MR imaging (EPI) data were
300 milliseconds (msec) presentation time each (mean ITI: acquired on a GE Signa 1.5T Horizon LX System (General Elec-
900 msec), including 48 target stimuli, the letters “X” (24) and tric, Milwaukee, WI, USA) at the Maudsley Hospital, London.
“O” (24) and 368 non-target letters (A, B, C, D, E, F, G, H, K, L, M, Consistent image quality was ensured by a semi-automated
N) (30 or 31 each). There are hence 11.5% target letters that are quality control procedure. A quadrature birdcage head coil
randomly interspersed with 88.5% non-target letters. Each was used for RF transmission and reception. In each of 16 non-
letter had a presentation time of 300 msec and each of the 24 contiguous planes parallel to the anterioreposterior commis-
target letters ‘X’ and ‘O’ were separated in the stream by at sural, 208 T2*-weighted MR images depicting Blood Oxygen
least 5400 msec and at most 9000 msec to allow for separa-
bility of haemodynamic response. Subjects have to respond
with the right hand button box to target letters only (X and O)
and ignore all other letters. One of the target letters was
rewarded (£1 for every three correct responses) and the
amount of money earned during the task (£8 for 100% correct
responses) was displayed throughout the task on the right
screen side by one of two differently coloured rising score-
bars (red/blue). Which target letter was rewarded and which
was not rewarded was counterbalanced across subjects.
Next to the letter frame two feedback bars appeared on
screen at all times with ascending panels numbered from 1 to
8, one of which indicated the accumulation of correct
responses to X (coloured red) and the other, the number of
correct responses to O (coloured blue). These feedback bars
would flash for each correct response to their associated letter
and on every third successful hit would move upwards to fill
a panel with colour. In the case of the rewarded feedback bar
each filled panel signalled to the participant that they had
made three successful responses to a rewarded letter and had
won a £1; in the case of the non-rewarded feedback bar this
signalled to the participant that they had made three Fig. 1 e Schematic illustration of the Rewarded Continuous
successful responses to the non-rewarded letter with no Performance Test. Response required to “X” or “O”, not to
associated winnings. It was emphasised to the subject which any other letters. Reward is given for each response to one
feedback panel informed them of the accumulation of their of the two target letters (which letter was rewarded was
monetary reward (as well as providing feedback) and which randomised across subjects). Red/blue bars indicate correct
bar gave them feedback about the number of successful non- responses to targets (X/O). Three correct responses make
rewarded targets. Since there were 24 rewarded target trials one score on the bar for the rewarded and non-rewarded
there was a maximum of £8 to win. Regardless of their ex- targets, but only the rewarded target scores are
pected winnings, all participants were given this sum at the remunerated with £1. Up to £8 can be won on the task.
200 c o r t e x 4 8 ( 2 0 1 2 ) 1 9 4 e2 1 5

Level Dependent (BOLD) contrast covering the whole brain than one false positive activation locus was expected for
were acquired with TE ¼ 40 msec, TR ¼ 1.8 sec, flip angle ¼ 90 , p < .05 at voxel level and p < .01 at cluster level. ANOVA
in-plane resolution ¼ 3.1 mm, slice thickness ¼ 7 mm, slice- analysis for between-group differences was conducted using
skip ¼ .7 mm. This EPI dataset provided complete brain randomisation-based test for voxel or cluster-wise differences
coverage. (Bullmore et al., 1999). Less than 1 false activated cluster was
For fMRI analysis, the software package of XBAM was used expected at a p-value of p < .05 for voxel and p < .01 for cluster
(www.brainmap.co.uk, Brammer et al., 1997) that uses median comparisons. Thus, an expected cluster-level type I error rate
statistics to control outlier effects and permutation rather of <1 per brain was achieved by first applying a voxel-level
than normal theory based inference, recommended for fMRI threshold of p < .05 followed by thresholding the 3D clusters
(Thirion et al., 2007). formed from the voxels that survived this initial step at
fMRI data were realigned to minimise motion-related a cluster-level threshold of p < .01. The cluster-level threshold
artefacts (Bullmore et al., 1999) and smoothed using of p < .01, was therefore not applied to the whole brain (which
a Gaussian filter (full-width half maximum, 7.2 mm). Time- would be lenient) but rather to the data previously thresh-
series analysis of individual subject activation was per- olded at a voxel-wise level of p < .05. The necessary combi-
formed using XBAM, with a wavelet-based re-sampling nation of voxel and cluster-level thresholds is not assumed
method previously described (Bullmore et al., 2001). Briefly, we from theory but rather determined by direct permutation for
first convolved each experimental condition (i.e., rewarded each data set. White matter regions were extracted from
and non-rewarded target trials vs the implicit baseline of non- analyses using the BET tool from the FSL software package
target trials) with two Poisson model functions (delays of 4 (Smith, 2002). This creates a grey matter mask of the Talairach
and 8 sec). Only correct trials were included in the analyses. template used for normalisation. This mask was subsequently
We then calculated the weighted sum of these two convolu- used to restrict the analysis to those voxels lying within grey
tions that gave the best fit (least-squares) to the time series at matter.
each voxel. A goodness-of-fit statistic (the SSQ-ratio) was then In order to test whether the between-group differences in
computed at each voxel consisting of the ratio of the sum of brain activation were related to performance differences,
squares of deviations from the mean intensity value due to statistical measures of BOLD response for each participant
the model (fitted time series) divided by the sum of squares were extracted in each of the clusters of between-group
due to the residuals (original time series minus model time differences for the sustained attention and reward contrasts.
series). The appropriate null distribution for assessing signif- These BOLD measures were then correlated within each group
icance of any given SSQ-ratio was established using the with RT using Pearson correlations, and with commission
wavelet-based data re-sampling method (Bullmore et al., 2001) errors using Spearman’s rho, given the non-parametric nature
and applying the model-fitting process to the re-sampled data. of these.
This process was repeated 20 times at each voxel and the data
combined over all voxels, resulting in 20 null parametric maps
4.2. Results
of SSQ-ratio for each subject, which were combined to give the
overall null distribution of SSQ-ratio. The same permutation
4.2.1. Task performance
strategy was applied at each voxel to preserve spatial corre-
Repeated measures ANOVA showed that there was no effect
lation structure in the data. Activated voxels, at a <1 level of
of reward on omission errors or RT within each group or
Type I error, were identified through the appropriate critical
reward by group interactions (see Table 2). However, patients
value of the SSQ-ratio from the null distribution. The first
showed significantly reduced mean RT to targets (whether
contrast involved subtracting activation associated with non-
rewarded or not) [F(1,23) ¼ 6, p ¼ .01], and a trend for an
target trials (implicit baseline) from the non-rewarded target
increased number of commission errors [t(23) ¼ 2, p ¼ .08],
trials (non-rewarded target trials- non-target trials),
suggesting a different speed-accuracy trade-off, favouring
measuring sustained attention. The second contrast sub-
speed.
tracted activation from non-rewarded target trials from
rewarded target trials (rewarded e non-rewarded target
trials), measuring effects of reward upon sustained attention.
A group activation map was then produced for the exper- Table 2 e Performance data for adults with childhood
imental conditions by calculating the median observed ADHD and persistent hyperactivity/inattention
symptoms and healthy comparison adults.
SSQ-ratio over all subjects at each voxel in standard space and
testing them against the null distribution of median Performance variable Controls ADHD
SSQ-ratios computed from the identically transformed (N ¼ 14) (N ¼ 11)
wavelet re-sampled data (Brammer et al., 1997). The voxel- Mean (SD) Mean (SD)
level threshold was first set to p < .05 to give maximum
a
sensitivity and to avoid type II errors. Next, a cluster-level Rewarded trials MRT (msec) 420 (48) 382 (42)
Non-rewarded trials MRT (msec)a 422 (39) 388 (43)
threshold was computed for the resulting 3D voxel clusters
Rewarded trials omission errors 0 (1) 0 (0)
such that the final expected number of type I error clusters Non-rewarded trials omission errors 0 (1) 0 (0)
was <1 per whole brain. Cluster mass rather than a cluster Commission errors 1(1) 2 (2)
extent threshold was used, to minimise discrimination
MRT ¼ Mean Reaction Time.
against possible small, strongly responding foci of activation
a Significant between-groups differences.
(Bullmore et al., 1999). For the group activation analyses, less
c o r t e x 4 8 ( 2 0 1 2 ) 1 9 4 e2 1 5 201

4.2.2. Brain activation insula, putamen and thalamus, and one including right PCC,
4.2.2.1. MOTION. Multivariate Analyses of Variance (MANOVA) precuneus, and parahippocampal gyrus (Fig. 3a, Table 3a). In
showed no between-group differences in any of the x, y, z all these clusters, controls showed increased activation during
translation and rotation motion parameters [F(6, 18) ¼ 1, non-rewarded target trials relative to non-target trials, while
p ¼ .44]. patients with ADHD showed decreased activation for this
contrast or increased activation during non-target trials rela-
4.2.2.2. SUSTAINED ATTENTION: NON-REWARDED TARGET TRIALS tive to non-rewarded target trials. Effect sizes were large for all
COMPARED TO NON-TARGET TRIALS. Within-group activation maps these clusters, with Cohen’s d values between .77 and 1.09
for each group are shown in Fig. 2a and Supplementary Table (see Table 3a).
1a. Healthy control adults showed activation in IFC, ACC, Patients compared to controls showed increased activation
putamen and globus pallidus, PCC, primary motor cortex and in bilateral posterior brain regions comprising cerebellum,
SMA, thalamus, temporal, parietal, and occipital cortices and PCC, precuneus, inferior and superior parietal cortices, and
cerebellum. Adults with childhood ADHD showed activation occipital regions (Fig. 3a, Table 3a). This increased activation
in IFC, medial and superior frontal cortices, ACC and PCC, was due to patients showing more activation than control
striatum, thalamus, temporal, parietal, and occipital cortices subjects in these regions during non-rewarded targets
and cerebellum. compared non-target trials while controls showed either less
The ANOVA comparison showed increased activation in activation or deactivation for this contrast. Effect sizes were
the control group during non-rewarded target compared to large for all these clusters, with Cohen’s d values between 1.05
non-target trials compared to adults with childhood ADHD in and 1.81 (see Table 3a).
three clusters, the largest in the left hemisphere including Given evidence for inferior fronto-cerebellar neural
IFC/insula reaching into pre-SMA and ACC, as well as deep networks that mediate selective and sustained attention
into caudate, putamen, globus pallidus, and thalamus, one (Arnsten and Rubia, in press; Smith et al., 2011; Tana et al.,
comprising right premotor and postcentral gyri, extending to 2010), we tested whether the increased cerebellar activation

Fig. 2 e Within group activation maps for controls and adults with childhood ADHD and persistent hyperactivity/inattention
symptoms. Axial sections showing within-group brain activation for the healthy comparison group and the adults with
childhood ADHD and persistent hyperactivity/inattention symptoms for the contrasts (a) Sustained attention:
non-rewarded target e non-target trials, (b) Reward: rewarded e non-rewarded target trials. Tailarach z-coordinates are
indicated for slice distance (in mm) from the intercommissural line.
202 c o r t e x 4 8 ( 2 0 1 2 ) 1 9 4 e2 1 5

Fig. 3 e Results of the ANOVA between-group difference analyses. Axial sections showing the ANOVA between-group
difference effects in brain activation between adults with persistent hyperactive/inattentive behaviours and childhood
ADHD for (a) Sustained attention: non-rewarded target e non-target trials contrast, and (b) Reward: rewarded e
non-rewarded target trials contrast. Tailarach z-coordinates are indicated for slice distance (in mm) from the
intercommissural line. Section (b) includes axial sections showing the ANOVA between-group difference effects in brain
activation for the Reward contrast between healthy controls and only those six adults with persistent hyperactive/
inattentive behaviours and childhood ADHD who also had comorbid childhood CD (N [ 6). Adults with persistent symptoms
in adulthood and with childhood ADHD without comorbid childhood CD showed no brain differences when compared to
controls. Tailarach z-coordinates are indicated for slice distance (in mm) from the intercommissural line.

observed in ADHD patients relative to controls was lateral cerebellum (r ¼ .81, p < .01) (see Supplementary Fig. 1).
a compensatory effect related to the reduced inferior fronto- In healthy control subjects, there was a significant correlation
striatal activation. For this purpose, average scalar measures between activation in left inferior frontal cortex and left
of the BOLD response was extracted for each subject in three lateral cerebellum (r ¼ .51, p < .03) (Supplementary Fig. 1),
of the clusters that differed between groups: in the large left whereas the correlation between the activation in left inferior
inferior fronto-striatal activation cluster and in the two cere- frontal cortex and right cerebellar vermis was not significant
bellar clusters in left lateral cerebellum and right cerebellar (r ¼ .04, p ¼ n.s.).
vermis (see Table 3). Then Pearson correlations were calcu- When IQ was used as a covariate, the main findings
lated between the scalar measures of the BOLD response in remained, at the same p-value of p < .05 for voxel and p < .01
the left inferior frontal cluster and each of the two cerebellar for cluster comparisons, but with slightly smaller cluster
clusters, separately within ADHD and control patients. In the sizes.
group of adults with ADHD, significant negative correlations In order to correlate clusters of between-group differences
were observed between the activation in left inferior frontal with performance measures we also extracted average scalar
cortex and in right cerebellar vermis (r ¼ .71, p < .01), as well measures of the BOLD response for each cluster that differed
as between the activation in left inferior frontal cortex and left between groups and then conducted two-tailed Pearson
c o r t e x 4 8 ( 2 0 1 2 ) 1 9 4 e2 1 5 203

Table 3 e Differences in brain activation between adults with childhood ADHD and healthy comparison adults.

Brain regions of activation Brodman Peak Talairach N of p Effect Sizes


area (BA) coordinates (x; y; z) voxels values (Cohen’s d )

(a) Sustained attention: non-rewarded target e non-target trials


C > ADHD
L inferior frontal/insula/premotor/putamen/globus pallidus/ 6/44/9/24/32 29; 4; 26 136 >.001 1.09
caudate/thalamus/insula/ACC/pre-SMA
R premotor/postcentral gyrus/insula/putamen 4/6/43 58; 4; 15 43 .004 .93
R PCC/precuneus/parahippocampal gyrus 18/19/31/7 11; 33; 20 64 .01 .77
ADHD > C
L inferior/superior parietal gyrus 40/7 43; 37; 42 28 .001 1.05
R inferior/superior parietal gyrus 40/7 47; 41; 42 109 .002 1.08
R þ L cuneus/precuneus/PCC 18/19/7/29/30 7; 74; 26 408 >.001 1.43
R vermis cerebellum/occipital/PCC 18/23/30/31 4; 67; 13 244 .003 1.43
L cerebellum/occipital/inferior temporal 19/18/37 25; 63; 13 309 >.001 1.81
(b) Reward: rewarded target e non-rewarded target trials
C > ADHD
R medial/superior frontal gyrus 10/46/9/8 36; 52; 15 140 .014 1.35
R ventromedial orbitofrontal 11/9/10 25; 48; 13 105 .011 1.24

N voxels ¼ number of voxels. L ¼ left; R ¼ right. The maps are thresholded to give less than 1 Type I error cluster per map.

correlations between those measures and performance vari- controls. The group differences were due to an increased
ables within each group. There was a significant negative activation in these clusters for controls during the rewar-
correlation within the ADHD group between commission dedenon-rewarded target contrast, while ADHD patients
errors (that were trend-wise increased in ADHD compared to showed increased activation in these regions during non-
controls) and activation in the right PCC/precuneus activation rewarded target compared to rewarded target trials. No
difference cluster (r ¼ .63, p < .03) (that was reduced in ADHD brain regions were increased in adults with childhood ADHD
patients relative to control). No significant correlation was compared to controls. The effect sizes were large (Cohen’s
observed between activation in this cluster and commission d ¼ 1.35 and 1.24) (see Table 3b).
errors in the healthy adults group (r ¼ .06, p ¼ n.s.). However, When covarying for IQ at a p-value of p < .05 for voxel and
within the control subject group there was a positive corre- at a more lenient p < .03 for cluster comparisons, the findings
lation between activation in this PCC/precuneus cluster and remained essentially unchanged. No significant correlations
RT (that was increased relative to patients) (r ¼ .65, p < .01), were observed between brain activation in these clusters and
whereas this correlation was not significant within the ADHD performance measures.
group (r ¼ .03, p ¼ n.s.). RT also correlated negatively within Given that patients with CD have been shown to have
the healthy adults group with activation in right inferior consistent functional and structural abnormalities in ventro-
parietal lobe (that was decreased in controls relative to ADHD medial and orbitofrontal cortices (Rubia, 2010); and, further-
patients) (r ¼ .65, p < .01). No significant correlation was more, that ventromedial orbitofrontal activation (in a similar
observed between RT and parietal activation within the ADHD location to this one) has been shown to be disorder-
group (r ¼ .21, p ¼ n.s.). Differences between the correlations specifically underactivated in children with CD compared to
observed within the two groups were only significant for the children with ADHD during the same reward contrast of this
correlation between RT and activation in parietal cortex CPT task (Rubia et al., 2009c), we tested for the potential
(z ¼ 2.16, p < .03) (Supplementary Fig. 1). impact of the presence of comorbid CD in childhood on the
functional brain activation abnormalities. For this purpose,
4.2.2.3. EFFECTS OF REWARD: REWARDED TARGETS COMPARED TO NON- two exploratory between-group ANOVAs were conducted,
REWARDED TARGET TRIALS. Within-group activation maps for comparing brain activation during the reward condition in
each group are shown in Fig. 2b and Supplementary Table 1b. controls (N ¼ 14) with activation in the subsample of patients
Healthy adults showed activation in OFC, IFC, medial frontal with additional comorbid CD in childhood (N ¼ 6), and with
lobe, striatum, ACC, SMA, insula, thalamus, amygdala, the activation observed in patients without comorbid CD in
temporal, parietal and occipital cortices and cerebellum. childhood (N ¼ 5). Less than 1 false activated cluster was ex-
Adults with childhood ADHD showed activation in IFC, medial pected at a p-value of p < .05 for voxel and p < .01 for cluster
frontal regions, ACC and PCC, SMA, striatum, thalamus, comparisons.
insula, temporal, parietal, occipital cortices and cerebellum. The subgroup of patients with comorbid CD in childhood
The ANOVA comparison showed increased activation in when compared to controls showed underactivation in one
the control group compared to adults with childhood ADHD in large cluster, comprising the same regions as previously
two clusters, one comprising right OFC and VMPFC and observed in the whole group, in bilateral OFC/VMPFC and
a second one including right medial and superior frontal superior frontal cortices, including ACC and reaching deep
cortices (Fig. 3b, Table 3b). No areas of increased activation into left caudate (Fig. 3b). Despite the small sample size, the
were detected for the patient group when compared to effect size was large (Cohen’s d ¼ 2.8). No areas of increased
204 c o r t e x 4 8 ( 2 0 1 2 ) 1 9 4 e2 1 5

activation in ADHD patients with comorbid CD compared to ADHD studied here, we observed underactivation in the SMA
controls were observed. The subgroup of ADHD patients during motor response inhibition (Cubillo et al., 2010).
without comorbid CD, however, showed no differences in However, increased activation in the SMA has also been
brain activation when compared to controls. observed in children with ADHD after failed motor response
inhibition (Spinelli et al., in press) and in adults with ADHD
4.3. Discussion during an attentional switching task (Dibbets et al., 2010). The
pre-SMA has been shown to be involved in sustained attention
Medication-naı̈ve adults with a confirmed diagnosis of child- in healthy adults (Lawrence et al., 2003; Tana et al., 2010) and to
hood hyperactivity who displayed persistent ADHD symp- have a role in motor response preparation and selection as well
toms in adulthood showed reduced activation compared to as motor response inhibition (Mostofsky and Simmonds, 2008;
healthy controls in lateral fronto-striatal and superior parietal Sharp et al., 2010; Simmonds et al., 2008; Tabu et al., in press).
regions during “cool” sustained attention processes and in The pre-SMA in particular has been associated with free
paralimbic VMPFC/OFC areas during “hot” reward-associated response selection as well as attention processes such as
functions. Subsequent exploratory analyses furthermore attention to intention and attention to action (Lau et al., 2004a,
showed that the VMPFC/OFC underactivation was observed 2004b). The abnormal activation in ADHD adults during this
only in those adults who had comorbid CD in childhood. target detection task may hence reflect abnormalities in exec-
During sustained attention, adults with ADHD seemed to utive attention and response selection networks.
compensate with cerebellar overactivation, which correlated ADHD patients, on the other hand, showed increased
negatively with the underactivation observed in IFC. The activation in several posterior brain regions, comprising
reduced activation in PCC, furthermore, appeared to be asso- lateral and medial cerebellum, PCC, parietal and occipital
ciated with a more impulsive performance style, as it was areas. The enhanced activation in cerebellar regions is likely
negatively associated with commission errors in patients but to be compensatory, as it correlated negatively with the
positively with slower RTs in controls. (reduced) activation in inferior frontal regions in both patients
For the sustained attention contrast, ADHD patients and controls. The cerebellum as part of fronto-cerebellar
showed reduced activation in key areas of sustained atten- neural networks (Arnsten and Rubia, in press) is involved in
tion, in IFC, striatum, thalamus, anterior insula and PCC higher cognitive processes (Steinlin, 2007), including sus-
(Lawrence et al., 2003; Tana et al., 2010; Voisin et al., 2006). IFC- tained attention (Lawrence et al., 2003; Voisin et al., 2006). The
striatal underactivation has previously been observed in adult underactivation in the frontal parts of this network in patients
ADHD during inhibitory and attention processes (Banich et al., may hence have triggered a compensatory activation increase
2009; Cubillo et al., 2010, in press; Depue et al., 2010a; Dibbets in the cerebellum. Functional abnormalities in the cerebellum
et al., 2010; Epstein et al., 2007; for review see Cubillo and have previously been observed in adults with ADHD during
Rubia, 2010). IFC dysfunction during cognitive tasks is one of other functions that involve attention such as working
the most consistent fMRI findings in children with ADHD memory and timing (Valera et al., 2005, 2010a, 2010b; Wolf
(Booth et al., 2005; Durston et al., 2006; Konrad et al., 2006; et al., 2009). The PCC together with the ACC forms part of
Pliszka et al., 2006; Rubia et al., 1999, 2001, 2005, 2008; Smith the midline attention network, and mediates visualespatial
et al., 2006; Vaidya et al., 2005; for review see Rubia, 2010), attention to saliency (Mesulam et al., 2001; Mohanty et al.,
and has been shown to be disorder-specific compared to 2008; Small et al., 2003), whereas precuneus has been
children with CD (Rubia et al., 2008, 2009b, 2009c, 2010a) and predominantly associated with voluntary attention shifting
OCD (Rubia et al., 2010b, 2011; for review see Rubia, 2010). The and directed spatial attention (Cavanna and Trimble, 2006).
findings thus show that the key abnormality of IFC dysfunc- The PCC and precuneus are typically reduced in activation in
tion in childhood ADHD, which may potentially be a disorder- children with ADHD during salient stimuli such as stop errors
specific neurofunctional biomarker, persists into adulthood. (Rubia et al., 2005, 2008), oddball or incongruent targets (Rubia
The underactivation findings in ACC is in line with et al., 2007b, 2009b, 2011; Tamm et al., 2006), rare targets in
previous fMRI findings in adults with ADHD during tasks of sustained attention tasks (Rubia et al., 2009c) as well as during
interference inhibition (Banich et al., 2009; Burgess et al., 2010; a motor delay task (Rubia et al., 1999).
Bush et al., 1999; Cubillo et al., in press), motor response The pattern of underactivation in IFC and striatum
inhibition (Cubillo et al., 2010) and working memory (Valera together with enhanced activation in cerebellar and occipital
et al., 2010a) and may be associated with the generic role of brain regions are strikingly similar to that we have previously
this area in output related attention functions (MacDonald observed in children with ADHD relative to the healthy
et al., 2000; Ridderinkhof et al., 2003). controls during the same rewarded sustained attention task
The observed underactivation in the group of adults with (Rubia et al., 2009c, 2009d) (see Fig. 4). These findings of similar
ADHD extended to pre-SMA. Reduced activation in SMA and brain under- and overactivation during the same task in
pre-SMA has previously observed in children with ADHD children and adults with ADHD relative to their respective
during tasks of cognitive control such as sustained (Rubia et al., age-matched controls support the notion of a continuity of
2009d) and selective attention in conflict tasks (Rubia et al., dysfunctions that are typically observed in children with
2011, in press), attentional switching (Rubia et al., 2010b), as ADHD into adult ADHD.
well as motor response inhibition (Suskauer et al., 2008a, 2008b; During the rewarded relative to the non-rewarded sus-
Tamm et al., 2004). It has also been found to be underactivated tained attention condition, adults with ADHD showed reduced
during motor and perceptual temporal processes (Rubia et al., activation in paralimbic VMPFC and OFC regions, key areas of
1999; Smith et al., 2008). In the same group of adults with reward processing, which are typically activated in this task
c o r t e x 4 8 ( 2 0 1 2 ) 1 9 4 e2 1 5 205

Fig. 4 e Similarities in reduced and increased brain activation in medication-naı̈ve adults with childhood ADHD and
persistent hyperactivity/inattention symptoms relative to their age-matched controls and in medication-naı̈ve children
with ADHD during the Sustained Attention condition compared to their respective age-matched healthy comparison
subjects.

(Smith et al., 2011). The lateral OFC mediates stimulus- finding by Dibbets et al. (2009) in adults with ADHD in bilateral
reinforcement learning (Baxter and Murray, 2002; IFC/OFC during positive feedback during a motor inhibition
Schoenbaum et al., 2006). The VMPFC is associated specifi- task. However, it is not in line with finding of increased acti-
cally with reward as opposed to punishmentedriven vation in OFC in adults with ADHD during reward outcome
processes (Christakou et al., 2009; Knutson et al., 2003; (Ströhle et al., 2008). We argue that underactivation in OFC may
O’Doherty, 2004; Windmann et al., 2006). Furthermore, the potentially be associated with the presence of CD comorbidity,
lateral and VMPFC/OFC modulate interconnected paralimbic given that children with CD relative to healthy controls typi-
brain regions and mediate topedown affect regulation that is cally show underactivation in VMPFC/OFC (Rubia, 2010).
typically weak in disorders of impulsiveness and aggression Furthermore, this region has been shown to be disorder-
(Davidson et al., 2000a, 2000b; Haber, 2008; Haber et al., 2006; specifically underactivated in children with pure CD relative
Catani et al., 2012; Thiebaut de Schotten et al., 2012). These to pure ADHD and control children during the reward compo-
networks of affect regulation and motivation are nent of the same task (Rubia et al., 2009c). In this sample, six
typically involved in “hot” EF (Zelazo and Muller, 2002). subjects had comorbid CD in childhood, although it only per-
The underactivation in OFC cortex during the reward sisted into adulthood in one case. Our exploratory analysis
contrast in adult ADHD is in line with the underactivation supported this hypothesis of an association between CD and
206 c o r t e x 4 8 ( 2 0 1 2 ) 1 9 4 e2 1 5

OFC dysfunction. Only those patients with ADHD with comor- prefrontal cortex and the basal ganglia (Fig. 5). However, the
bid CD in childhood showed underactivation in VMPFC/OFC exact location of the inferior prefrontal location differed
regions compared to healthy controls during the reward aspect between tasks and task conditions. During motor inhibition,
of the task, while patients without comorbid CD in childhood switching and sustained attention, the location was in
showed no abnormalities in this condition (Fig. 3). The findings predominantly right hemispheric or bilateral deep inferior
hence suggest that comorbid CD in childhood may have prefrontal junction reaching into insula, in line with the
accounted for the OFC/VMPFC abnormalities, despite the known role of the right IFC in inhibitory control and sustained
symptomatic improvement of CD symptoms in adulthood. attention processes (Derrfuss et al., 2005; Rubia et al., 2003,
Previous studies of ADHD in adulthood did not assess or report 2007c; Voisin et al., 2006). During an interference inhibition
the presence of comorbid CD in childhood. Presence or absence and oddball tasks, the location was exclusively left hemi-
of comorbid CD thus could potentially explain the differences spheric and in a more superior, dorsolateral prefrontal loca-
in OFC findings across studies (Sundram et al., 2012). Our tion and included also ACC, in line with evidence implicating
findings of a significant impact of childhood CD on reward- left DLPFC and ACC in conflict monitoring (MacDonald et al.,
associated functional brain activation in OFC/VMPFC stresses 2000). In line with the role of ventromedial orbitofrontal
the importance of assessing childhood CD in addition to cortex with emotional-driven action selection and reward
childhood ADHD in studies of adult ADHD. processing (Bechara et al., 2000; Kringelbach, 2005), this loca-
The data thus provide additional evidence for dysfunctions tion was underactivated in adult ADHD patients during the
in overlapping “cool” attention and “hot” motivation brain only “hot” EF, the reward contrast in the sustained attention
networks in a group of adult patients with ADHD who were task. Furthermore, this activation may have been due to
followed up from childhood. However, potential caveats of comorbid childhood CD rather than ADHD. With respect to the
a relatively small sample size, of IQ differences and of the basal ganglia deficits, the inhibition tasks (motor and inter-
potential impact of CD comorbidity on the OFC deficit findings ference inhibition) elicited reduced activation in the caudate
need to be taken into consideration. The findings of lateral in the group of ADHD adults, while during sustained and
fronto-striatal deficits during “cool” EF, however, are in line flexible attention the putamen was also underactivated, in
with previous fronto-striatal deficit findings in the same group line with fronto-caudate implications in cognitive control
of patients during other “cool” EF, including motor and inter- (Aron et al., 2007; Rubia et al., 2007c) and putamen involve-
ference inhibition, cognitive switching and oddball detection ment in attention functions (Adler et al., 2001).
(Cubillo et al., 2010, in press). Across all tasks, these adults The underactivation observed in caudate and putamen
with childhood ADHD showed underactivation in inferior across tasks (Cubillo et al., 2010, in press) suggests persistent

Fig. 5 e Reduced brain activation in adults with childhood ADHD relative to healthy controls across a series of cognitive
tasks in fMRI. For details on images a, b, eeg see Cubillo et al., 2010, in press.
c o r t e x 4 8 ( 2 0 1 2 ) 1 9 4 e2 1 5 207

striatal dysfunctions in adults with symptoms of ADHD. This is with and without IQ as covariate, however, suggests that the
neither in line with longitudinal findings of structural caudate observed dysfunctions are associated with the disorder and
normalisation in young ADHD adulthood (Castellanos et al., not IQ.
2002) nor with our meta-regression analysis showing that the A common problem with fMRI adaptations of CPT tasks is
caudate normalises with age (Nakao et al., in press). However, it that motor responses to targets are not controlled for since
is in line with caudate and putamen abnormality findings from a motor response to non-targets would place unwanted atten-
recent cross-sectional structural (Almeida Montes et al., 2010; tion demands. While this does not affect the reward contrast
Seidman et al., 2011) and functional (Epstein et al., 2007; that was well controlled, this could have affected the sustained
Schneider et al., 2010) imaging studies in adults with ADHD. attention contrast. Thus, some activation differences between
As discussed above, the SMA was underactivated during both groups for this contrast could potentially be motor- rather than
sustained attention and successful inhibition (Cubillo et al., purely attention-related, such as the differences in premotor
2010), in line with the role of this region in both motor and SMA regions. The majority of ANOVA findings, however,
response inhibition and attention to response (Lau et al., 2004a, were not in motor regions. Inferior prefrontal, cingulate and
2004b; Mostofsky and Simmonds, 2008; Sharp et al., 2010; striato-thalamic activation for example that was reduced in
Simmonds et al., 2008; Tabu et al., in press). Overall, the find- activation in ADHD adults, is known to mediate sustaining
ings show that across tasks, including “cool” and “hot” EF as well attention in motor-controlled vigilance and parametric sus-
as simple (oddball task) and higher level executive attention tained attention tasks (Lawrence et al., 2003; Voisin et al., 2006).
functions (interference inhibition and sustained attention A key strength of this study is the medication-naivety of
tasks), adults with childhood ADHD have deficits in their acti- patients, which allowed us to avoid the common confound of
vation of fronto-striatal neural networks. However, the exact the long-term effects of stimulant medication history on brain
location of frontal and striatal dysfunctions is task-dependent, function. This, together with the clearly established presence
with dorsolateral fronto-striatal deficits during cognitive of hyperactivity during childhood, which avoids the potential
control and selective attention, inferior fronto-striatal recall bias present in retrospective diagnosis (Mannuzza et al.,
dysfunctions during inhibitory control and sustained atten- 2002), make this a valuable sample. Additionally, the
tion and ventromedial orbitofrontal deficits during reward enhanced homogeneity of the sample by the inclusion of
processing. The findings suggest that overlapping fronto- males only, and a restricted age range, helped us to avoid the
striatal neural networks, mediating both “cool” attentional confounding effects of gender and age.
and cognitive control as well “hot” motivation processes may be In conclusion, we observed that medication-naı̈ve adults
dysfunctional in ADHD, in line with models of multisystem who were known to have ADHD symptoms in childhood and
dysfunctions in ADHD (Makris et al., 2009; Nigg and Casey, 2005). persistent inattention/hyperactivity symptoms in adult life
The findings have to be interpreted in light of some limi- showed “cool” attention-related inferior fronto-striatal brain
tations. One is the small sample size. Large numbers were dysfunctions as well as presumably compensatory hyper-
difficult to obtain due to factors inherent to long-term follow- activation in cerebellum, strikingly similar to previous find-
up studies: many patients grew out of the disorder, changed ings in adolescents with ADHD. We also observed abnormal
their geographic area, making contact impossible, or refused brain function in “hot” reward-related EF, with reduced par-
to participate in the scanning study. Despite the small sample alimbic VMPFC/OFC activation during rewarded sustained
size, however, we observed significant differences in brain attention trials. However, these lateral OFC and VMPFC acti-
activation between cases and controls, which are consistent vation dysfunctions were only present in those subjects with
with previous findings. Furthermore, the effect sizes for the comorbid CD during childhood, suggesting that childhood CD
between-group findings were relatively large. problems may have accounted for these. The findings stress
The presence of comorbid conditions constitutes another the importance for assessing CD in childhood and for testing
limitation. However, comorbidity is extremely common with their impact on brain deficits. Overall, the findings therefore
up to 87% both in children (Blackman et al., 2005; Kadesjo and suggest that attention and motivation-related lateral and
Gillberg, 2001; Spencer, 2006) and in adults with ADHD ventromedial fronto-striatal brain abnormalities observed in
(Biederman et al., 2006; Kessler et al., 2006; Sobanski et al., children with ADHD may persist into adulthood, despite
2007), most commonly mood (40e61%), substance related a relative symptomatic improvement.
(15e70%), and anxiety disorders (30e47%) (Kessler et al., 2006;
Miller et al., 2007; Sobanski et al., 2007, 2008; Wilens et al.,
2009). Pure cases are therefore the rare exception, and the 5. Overall conclusions
selection of an ADHD group without any psychiatric comor-
bidities would not be representative for ADHD in adulthood We show in this review that structural and functional imaging
(Biederman et al., 2006), since it would only include subjects studies provide evidence for abnormalities in children and
with milder forms of the disorder, or of higher functioning. adults with ADHD both in lateral inferior/dorsolateral and
That is also the reason why most previous fMRI studies of dorsomedial fronto-striatal, fronto-parietal and fronto-
adult ADHD have typically included samples with comorbid cerebellar neural networks that mediate “cool” abstract-
psychiatric disorders (Dibbets et al., 2009; Epstein et al., 2007; cognitive EF, including higher level motor, attention, and
Hale et al., 2007; Valera et al., 2010a). temporal processes, as well as in lateral orbitofrontal and
The groups differed significantly in IQ. However, given the ventromedial networks that mediate “hot” motivation control
association between low IQ and ADHD, matching for IQ would functions, all of which are behaviourally and cognitively
create unrepresentative groups. The similarity in the findings compromised in the disorder. We furthermore provide new
208 c o r t e x 4 8 ( 2 0 1 2 ) 1 9 4 e2 1 5

functional imaging evidence in support of the notion of the et al., 2006). The findings are therefore in line with previously
adult persistence of dysfunctions previously observed in chil- suggested concepts of multiple neural system impairment in
dren with ADHD. Medication-naı̈ve adults with hyperactive/ ADHD associated with the different motor, attention, cognitive
inattentive behaviours, followed up from a confirmed presence control and motivational processes that are impaired in the
of ADHD symptoms in childhood, showed underactivation in disorder (Makris et al., 2009; Nigg and Casey, 2005).
both “cool” and “hot” lateral inferior fronto-striatal and There are, however, several potential caveats in the imaging
ventromedial fronto-striatal networks involved in sustained literature of ADHD that need to be considered. The majority of
attention and motivation, respectively, in association with structural and functional imaging studies in children and adults
a more impulsive performance style. The findings were similar with ADHD have not excluded comorbidity with conduct
to those previously observed in the same group of adults during disorder (CD)/antisocial personality disorder. We have shown
other cognitive functions. They were furthermore strikingly in a previous review that antisocial behaviours are associated
similar to those findings previously observed in childhood with abnormalities in neural networks of the paralimbic system
ADHD during the same tasks and hence are in support of our that mediate affect and motivation (i.e., “hot” EF), comprising
reviewed evidence that adults with ADHD continue to show the ventromedial frontal cortex, superior temporal lobe and
same abnormalities in lateral fronto-striato-parietal and underlying limbic structures (Rubia, 2010). Furthermore,
fronto-cerebellar cognitive control networks as well as in abnormalities in these paralimbic regions appear to be
ventromedial fronto-striatal and fronto-temporo-limbic disorder-specific when compared with non-comorbid ADHD
networks during motivation control as children with ADHD. patients, while “cool” EF lateral inferior fronto-cortical and
The review together with the new data therefore suggest that fronto-subcortical networks appear to be disorder-specific in
childhood and adult ADHD, as a group, are characterised by the non-comorbid ADHD patients relative to pure CD cases (Rubia,
impairment of several overlapping neural networks, affecting 2010). While some ADHD patients, in particular those with
fronto-subcortical, fronto-cortical and fronto-cerebellar neural emotion dysregulation or irritability, or comorbid emotional
circuitries that mediate “cool” attention and cognitive control and antisocial problems, are likely to suffer from abnormalities
functions as well as fronto-temporo-limbic neural networks of in both “cool” and “hot” EF neurocircuitries, it is possible that
affect and motivation control (see schematic Fig. 6). The “cool” pure, non-comorbid ADHD groups may only be impaired in
neural circuitries furthermore are known to interact closely “cool” EF brain systems. In adult ADHD, the majority of imaging
with the “hot” neural circuitries (Goel and Dolan, 2003; Krain studies have included high rates of comorbid cases with

Fig. 6 e Schematic representation of the MRI evidence for structural and functional brain abnormalities in children and
adults with ADHD in overlapping neural networks that mediate “cool” cognitive-abstract and “hot” reward-associated
executive and cognitive functions. “Cool” cognitive networks of dysfunction in ADHD include inferior, dorsolateral and
medial fronto-striatal, fronto-parieto-temporal and fronto-cerebellar regions and networks that mediate functions such
as motor response and interference inhibition, cognitive flexibility, temporal foresight, selective and sustained attention,
working memory, motor and timing processes. “Hot” EF network dysfunctions in ADHD patients have been observed in
the context of temporal discounting, reward processing and reward anticipation. IFG [ inferior frontal gyrus;
OFC [ orbitofrontal cortex; DLPFC [ dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; vmOFC [ ventromedial orbitofrontal cortex;
d/vACC [ dorsal/ventral ACC cortex; SMA [ Supplementary Motor Area.
c o r t e x 4 8 ( 2 0 1 2 ) 1 9 4 e2 1 5 209

emotional and affective problems. Our findings of no orbito- demonstrated a delay in cortical thickness maturation in
frontal deficits in adults with ADHD without antisocial prob- children with ADHD (Shaw et al., 2007). Unfortunately, these
lems in the reward condition would be in line with the notion studies have not scanned ADHD patients beyond the age of 20.
that some ADHD patients, who have no association with anti- If a delay is defined as a maturational lag that normalises
social or affective behaviours, may not share affective circuit eventually with age (i.e., catches up), then this seems not to
impairment. This would also be in line with meta-analysis apply to ADHD, given that similar brain structure, function and
studies of whole brain structural imaging studies that do not structural and functional interconnectivity deficits are still
find fronto-limbic but predominantly basal ganglia impair- observed in adults with ADHD in cross-sectional imaging
ments both in children with ADHD (Ellison-Wright et al., 2008) studies. One possible exception may be basal ganglia struc-
and across the lifespan (Nakao et al., in press). On the other tures, where there is some evidence for normalisation from
hand, however, there has been a bias in structural MRI studies longitudinal (Castellanos et al., 2002) and meta-regression
of ADHD towards the selection of regions of interest that studies (Nakao et al., in press). The findings of persisting
mediate “cool” cognitive functions or fMRI paradigms of brain abnormalities in adult ADHD seem to suggest that the
abstractecognitive control. Only relatively recent structural maturational “delay”, if it is a “delay” persists throughout adult
and functional imaging studies have focussed their interest on life. Also, to our knowledge, no imaging studies have been
limbic regions and on fMRI tasks of motivation and affect conducted in elderly patients with ADHD, but there may well be
control. More imaging studies of ADHD are needed that focus on differences in normal ageing processes too. Future longitudinal
limbic regions and limbic white matter networks as well as structural and functional imaging studies will be needed to
emotional fMRI paradigms to assess more thoroughly to what elucidate whether there is a structural and functional matu-
degree these systems are affected in ADHD and their subgroups. rational delay in ADHD patients that persists throughout the
A limitation of the field is furthermore the fact that most lifespan or whether this normalises at any given age. Such
imaging studies are based on group statistics on relatively studies should also test for different factors that may influence
small numbers. This applies particularly to the adult imaging a potential normalisation process, including symptom
literature. There are as yet few studies that tested for neural severity, environmental factors, cognitive faculties, genetic
networks in subgroups of ADHD patients. There is likely to be predisposition or pharmacological and behavioural treatment.
heterogeneity in neural network impairments between ADHD
patients and subgroups, similar to the observed heterogeneity
in neuropsychological impairments (Nigg et al., 2005; Sonuga-
Barke et al., 2010). Thus, some children may have no brain Acknowledgements
abnormalities, while others may have abnormalities in
specific fronto-striatal circuitries of specific “cool” EF, timing, The research was supported by grants from the Medical
motor and/or attention functions, and yet others may suffer Research Council (G9900839) and The Wellcome Trust (053272/
from dysfunctions in fronto-limbic emotion/motivation Z/98/Z/JRS/JP/JAT). AC and ABS were supported by PHD
circuits or in both, overlapping cognitive and affective neu- studentship/post-doctoral fellowships by the National Insti-
rocircuitries. This would be in line with the notion of ADHD as tute for Health Research (NIHR) Biomedical Research Centre
a multisystem developmental disorder, where the clinical (BRC) for Mental Health at the South London and Maudsley
expression is based on the degree and heterogeneity of the NHS Foundation Trust (SLaM) and the Institute of Psychiatry
neural system dysfunction (Makris et al., 2009). at King’s College, London.
An important confound in the majority of adult ADHD
imaging studies is long-term medication history. Our recent
meta-regression analysis showed that basal ganglia abnor- Supplementary data
malities were correlated with the percentage of medication-
naı̈ve patients included in the studies (Nakao et al., in press). Supplementary data related to this article can be found online
This would suggest that the current literature, mostly con- at doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2011.04.007.
ducted in adult ADHD patients with a medication history,
shows a more lenient deficit picture from what would be
observed in untreated patients. The findings of striking simi-
references
larities in functional deficits between our previously scanned
medication-naı̈ve ADHD children and the here presented
medication-naı̈ve adults with ADHD are in line with this and
Adler CM, Sax KW, Holland SK, Schmithorst V, Rosenberg L, and
suggest that deficit findings may be more similar when Strakowski SM. Changes in neuronal activation with
medication-naı̈ve patients are being compared. increasing attention demand in healthy volunteers: An fMRI
It also remains to be investigated to what degree ADHD is study. Synapse, 42(4): 266e272, 2001.
a delay of normal brain structure or function maturation. Ahrendts J, Rüsch N, Wilke M, Philipsen A, Eickhoff SB, Glauche V,
Overall, the cross-sectional findings from the different imaging Perlov E, Ebert D, Hennig J, Tebartz van Elst L. Visual cortex
abnormalities in adults with ADHD: A structural MRI study.
modalities suggest that ADHD children have deficits in the very
World Journal of Biological Psychiatry, in press [epub ahead of
brain regions, functions and functional and structural neural
printing, doi:10.3109/15622975.2010.518624, 2010].
networks that develop late between childhood and adulthood Almeida Montes LG, Ricardo-Garcell J, Barajas De La Torre LB,
(Geier and Luna, 2009; Giedd et al., 2001; Konrad and Eickhoff, Prado AH, Martinez Garcia RB, Fernandez-Bouzas A, et al.
2010; Makris et al., 2009). Longitudinal imaging studies have Clinical correlations of grey matter reductions in the caudate
210 c o r t e x 4 8 ( 2 0 1 2 ) 1 9 4 e2 1 5

nucleus of adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Boonstra A, Oosterlaan J, Sergeant J, and Buitelaar J. Executive
Journal of Psychiatry Neuroscience, 35(4): 238e246, 2010. functioning in adult ADHD: A meta-analytic review.
American Psychiatric Association Diagnostic and Statistical Manual Psychological Medicine, 35(8): 1097e1108, 2005.
of Mental Disorders. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Booth JR, Burman DD, Meyer JR, Lei Z, Trommer BL,
Association, 1994. Davenport ND, et al. Larger deficits in brain networks for
Amico F, Stauber J, Koutsouleris N, and Frodl T. Anterior cingulate response inhibition than for visual selective attention in
cortex gray matter abnormalities in adults with attention attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Journal of Child
deficit hyperactivity disorder: A voxel-based morphometry Psychology and Psychiatry, 46(1): 94e111, 2005.
study. Psychiatry Research, 191(1): 31e35, 2010. Brammer M, Bullmore ET, Simmons A, Williams SC, Grasby PM,
Antrop I, Stock P, Verte S, Wiersema JR, Baeyens D, and Howard RJ, et al. Generic brain activation mapping in
Roeyers H. ADHD and delay aversion: The influence of non- functional magnetic resonance imaging: A nonparametric
temporal stimulation on choice for delayed rewards. Journal of approach. Magnetic Resonance Imaging, 15(7): 763e770, 1997.
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47(11): 1152e1158, 2006. Bridgett DJ and Walker ME. Intellectual functioning in adults with
Arnsten A and Rubia K. Neurobiological circuits regulating ADHD: A meta-analytic examination of full scale IQ
attention, movement and emotion and their disruptions in differences between adults with and without ADHD.
pediatric neuropsychiatric disorders. Journal of the American Psychological Assessment, 18(1): 1e14, 2006.
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, in press. Brotman MA, Rich BA, Guyer AE, Lunsford JR, Horsey SE,
Aron AR, Durston S, Eagle DM, Logan GD, Stinear CM, and Reising MM, et al. Amygdala activation during emotion
Stuphorn V. Converging evidence for a fronto-basal-ganglia processing of neutral faces in children with severe mood
network for inhibitory control of action and cognition. Journal dysregulation versus ADHD or bipolar disorder. American
of Neuroscience, 27(44): 11860e11864, 2007. Journal of Psychiatry, 167(1): 61e69, 2010.
Ashtari M, Kumra S, Bhaskar SL, Clarke T, Thaden E, Bullmore E, Long C, Suckling J, Fadili J, Calvert G, Zelaya F, et al.
Cervellione KL, et al. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Colored noise and computational inference in
A preliminary diffusion tensor imaging study. Biological neurophysiological (fMRI) time series analysis: Resampling
Psychiatry, 57(5): 448e455, 2005. methods in time and wavelet domains. Human Brain Mapping,
Banaschewski T, Ruppert S, Tannock R, Albrecht B, Becker A, 12(2): 61e78, 2001.
Uebel H, et al. Colour perception in ADHD. Journal of Child Bullmore ET, Suckling J, Overmeyer S, Rabe-Hesketh S, Taylor E,
Psychology and Psychiatry, 47(6): 568e572, 2006. and Brammer M. Global, voxel, and cluster tests, by theory and
Banich MT, Burgess GC, Depue BE, Ruzic L, Bidwell LC, permutation, for a difference between two groups of
Hitt-Laustsen S, et al. The neural basis of sustained and structural MR images of the brain. IEEE Transactions on Medical
transient attentional control in young adults with ADHD. Imaging, 18(1): 32e42, 1999.
Neuropsychologia, 47(14): 3095e3104, 2009. Burgess GC, Depue BE, Ruzic L, Willcutt EG, Du YP, and
Barkley RA, Fischer M, Smallish L, and Fletcher K. The persistence Banich MT. Attentional control activation relates to working
of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder into young memory in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological
adulthood as a function of reporting source and definition of Psychiatry, 67(7): 632e640, 2010.
disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111(2): 279e289, 2002. Bush G, Frazier JA, Rauch SL, Seidman LJ, Whalen PJ, Jenike MA,
Batty MJ, Liddle EB, Pitiot A, Toro R, Groom MJ, Scerif G, et al. et al. Anterior cingulate cortex dysfunction in attention-
Cortical gray matter in attention-deficit/hyperactivity deficit/hyperactivity disorder revealed by fMRI and the
disorder: A structural magnetic resonance imaging study. Counting Stroop. Biological Psychiatry, 45(12): 1542e1552, 1999.
Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Cao QJ, Zang YF, Sun L, Sui MQ, Long XY, Zou QH, et al. Abnormal
Psychiatry, 49(3): 229e238, 2010. neural activity in children with attention deficit hyperactivity
Baxter MG and Murray EA. The amygdala and reward. Nature disorder: A resting-state functional magnetic resonance
Reviews. Neuroscience, 3(7): 563e573, 2002. imaging study. NeuroReport, 17(10): 1033e1036, 2006.
Bechara A, Tranel D, and Damasio H. Characterization of the Cao X, Cao Q, Long X, Sun L, Sui M, Zhu C, et al. Abnormal resting-
decision-making deficit of patients with ventromedial state functional connectivity patterns of the putamen in
prefrontal cortex lesions. Brain, 123(11): 2189e2202, 2000. medication-naive children with attention deficit hyperactivity
Biederman J, Mick E, and Faraone SV. Age-dependent decline of disorder. Brain Research, 1303: 195e206, 2009.
symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Impact Carmona S, Vilarroya O, Bielsa A, Tremols V, Soliva JC, Rovira M,
of remission definition and symptom type. American Journal of et al. Global and regional gray matter reductions in ADHD:
Psychiatry, 157(5): 816e818, 2000. A voxel-based morphometric study. Neuroscience Letters,
Biederman J, Monuteaux MC, Mick E, Spencer T, Wilens TE, 389(2): 88e93, 2005.
Silva JM, et al. Young adult outcome of attention deficit Carmona S, Proal E, Hoekzema EA, Gispert JD, Picado M, Moreno I,
hyperactivity disorder: A controlled 10-year follow-up study. et al. Ventro-striatal reductions underpin symptoms of
Psychological Medicine, 36(2): 167e179, 2006. hyperactivity and impulsivity in attention-deficit/hyperactivity
Biederman J, Makris N, Valera EM, Monuteaux MC, Goldstein JM, disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 66(10): 972e977, 2009.
Buka S, et al. Towards further understanding of the co- Castellanos FX, Lee PP, Sharp W, Jeffries NO, Greenstein DK,
morbidity between attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and Clasen LS, et al. Developmental trajectories of brain volume
bipolar disorder: A MRI study of brain volumes. Psychological abnormalities in children and adolescents with attention-
Medicine, 38(7): 1045e1056, 2008. deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American Medical
Bitsakou P, Psychogiou L, Thompson M, and Sonuga-Barke EJ. Association, 288(14): 1740e1748, 2002.
Delay aversion in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: An Castellanos FX, Margulies DS, Kelly C, Uddin LQ, Ghaffari M,
empirical investigation of the broader phenotype. Kirsch A, et al. Cingulate-precuneus interactions: A new locus
Neuropsychologia, 47(2): 446e456, 2009. of dysfunction in adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity
Blackman GL, Ostrander R, and Herman KC. Children with ADHD disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 63(3): 332e337, 2008.
and depression: A multisource, multimethod assessment of Catani M, Dell’Acqua F, Vergani F, Malik F, Hodge H, Roy P, et al.
clinical, social, and academic functioning. Journal of Attention Short frontal lobe connections of the human brain. Cortex,
Disorders, 8(4): 195e207, 2005. 48(2): 273e291, 2012.
c o r t e x 4 8 ( 2 0 1 2 ) 1 9 4 e2 1 5 211

Cavanna AE and Trimble MR. The precuneus: A review of its Dickstein SG, Bannon K, Castellanos FX, and Milham MP. The
functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. Brain, neural correlates of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder:
129(Pt 3): 564e583, 2006. An ALE meta-analysis. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
Christakou A, Brammer M, Giampietro V, and Rubia K. Right 47(10): 1051e1062, 2006.
ventromedial and dorsolateral prefrontal cortices mediate Durston S, Tottenham NT, Thomas KM, Davidson MC, Eigsti IM,
adaptive decisions under ambiguity by integrating choice Yang YH, et al. Differential patterns of striatal activation in
utility and outcome evaluation. Journal of Neuroscience, young children with and without ADHD. Biological Psychiatry,
29(35): 11020e11028, 2009. 53(10): 871e878, 2003.
Conners CK. The Conners Continuous Performance Test. North Durston S, Mulder M, Casey BJ, Ziermans T, and van Engeland H.
Tonawanda, NY: Multi-Health Systems, 1993. Activation in ventral prefrontal cortex is sensitive to genetic
Crosbie J and Schachar R. Deficient inhibition as a marker for vulnerability for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder.
familial ADHD. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158(11): Biological Psychiatry, 60(10): 1062e1070, 2006.
1884e1890, 2001. Durston S, Davidson MC, Mulder MJ, Spicer JA, Galvan A,
Cubillo A, Halari R, Ecker C, Giampietro V, Taylor E, and Rubia K. Tottenham N, et al. Neural and behavioral correlates of
Reduced activation and inter-regional functional connectivity expectancy violations in attention-deficit hyperactivity
of fronto-striatal networks in adults with childhood Attention disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48(9):
Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and persisting 881e889, 2007.
symptoms during tasks of motor inhibition and cognitive Ellison-Wright I, Ellison-Wright Z, and Bullmore E. Structural
switching. Journal of Psychiatry Research, 44(10): 629e639, 2010. brain change in Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder
Cubillo A, Halari R, Giampietro V, Taylor E and Rubia K. Fronto- identified by meta-analysis. BMC Psychiatry, 8: 51e58, 2008.
striatal hypo-activation during interference inhibition and Endicott J and Spitzer RL. A diagnostic interview: The schedule for
attention allocation in a group of grown up children with affective disorders and schizophrenia. Archives of General
ADHD with persistent hyperactive/inattentive behaviours in Psychiatry, 35(7): 837e844, 1978.
adulthood. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, in press. Engelmann JB and Pessoa L. Motivation sharpens exogenous
Cubillo A and Rubia K. Structural and functional brain imaging spatial attention. Emotion, 7(3): 668e674, 2007.
in adult Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): Epstein JN, Johnson DE, Varia IM, and Conners CK.
A review. Expert Reviews of Neurotherapeutics, 10(4): Neuropsychological assessment of response inhibition in
603e620, 2010. adults with ADHD. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Dalen L, Sonuga-Barke EJ, Hall M, and Remington B. Inhibitory Neuropsychology, 23(3): 362e371, 2001.
deficits, delay aversion and preschool AD/HD: Implications for Epstein JN, Casey BJ, Tonev ST, Davidson MC, Reiss AL, Garrett A,
the dual pathway model. Neural Plasticity, 11(1e2): 1e11, 2004. et al. ADHD- and medication-related brain activation effects in
Danckaerts M, Heptinstall E, Chadwick O, and Taylor E. A natural concordantly affected parent-child dyads with ADHD. Journal
history of hyperactivity and conduct problems: Self-reported of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48(9): 899e913, 2007.
outcome. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 9(1): 26e38, 2000. Faraone SV, Biederman J, Spencer T, Wilens T, Seidman LJ, Mick E,
Davenport ND, Karatekin C, White T, and Lim KO. Differential et al. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adults: An
fractional anisotropy abnormalities in adolescents with ADHD overview. Biological Psychiatry, 48(1): 9e20, 2000.
or schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 181(3): 193e198, 2010. Faraone SV and Biederman J. What is the prevalence of adult
Davidson RJ, Putnam KM, and Larson CL. Dysfunction in the ADHD? Results of a population screen of 966 adults. Journal of
neural circuitry of emotion regulation e A possible prelude to Attention Disorders, 9(2): 384e391, 2005.
violence. Science, 289(5479): 591e594, 2000a. Faraone SV, Wilens TE, Petty C, Antshel K, Spencer T, and
Davidson RJ, Jackson DC, and Kalin NH. Emotion, plasticity, Biederman J. Substance use among ADHD adults: Implications
context, and regulation: Perspectives from affective of late onset and subthreshold diagnoses. American Journal of
neuroscience. Psychological Bulletin, 126(6): 890e909, 2000b. Addictions, 16(Suppl 1): 24e32, 2007.
Dennis M, Francis DJ, Cirino PT, Schachar R, Barnes MA, and Frodl T, Stauber J, Schaaff N, Koutsouleris N, Scheuerecker J,
Fletcher JM. Why IQ is not a covariate in cognitive studies of Ewers M, et al. Amygdala reduction in patients with ADHD
neurodevelopmental disorders. Journal of the International compared with major depression and healthy volunteers. Acta
Neuropsychological Society: JINS, 15(3): 331e343, 2009. Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 121(2): 111e118, 2009.
Depue BE, Burgess GC, Willcutt EG, Ruzic L, and Banich MT. Froehlich TE, Lanphear BP, Epstein JN, Barbaresi WJ, Katusic SK,
Inhibitory control of memory retrieval and motor processing and Kahn RS. Prevalence, recognition, and treatment of
associated with the right lateral prefrontal cortex: Evidence attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in a national sample
from deficits in individuals with ADHD. Neuropsychologia, of US children. Archives of paediatrics and adolescent medicine,
48(13): 3909e3917, 2010a. 161(9): 857e864, 2007.
Depue BE, Burgess GC, Willcutt EG, Bidwell LC, Ruzic L, and Geier C and Luna B. The maturation of incentive processing
Banich MT. Symptom-correlated brain regions in young adults and cognitive control. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior,
with combined-type ADHD: Their organization, variability, 93(3): 212e221, 2009.
and relation to behavioral performance. Psychiatry Research, Giedd JN, Blumenthal J, Molloy E, and Castellanos FX. Brain
182(2): 96e102, 2010b. imaging of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Annals of
Derrfuss J, Brass M, Neumann J, and von Cramon DY. the New York Academy of Sciences, 931: 33e49, 2001.
Involvement of the inferior frontal junction in cognitive Goel V and Dolan RJ. Reciprocal neural response within lateral
control: Meta-analyses of switching and Stroop studies. and ventral medial prefrontal cortex during hot and cold
Human Brain Mapping, 25(1): 22e34, 2005. reasoning. NeuroImage, 20(4): 2314e2321, 2003.
Dibbets P, Evers L, Hurks P, Marchetta N, and Jolles J. Differences Haber S. Functional anatomy and physiology of the basal ganglia:
in feedback- and inhibition-related neural activity in adult Non-motor functions. In Tarsy D (Ed), Deep Brain Stimulation in
ADHD. Brain and Cognition, 70(1): 73e83, 2009. Neurological and Psychiatric Disorders. Humana Press, 2008:
Dibbets P, Evers EA, Hurks PP, Bakker K, and Jolles J. Differential 33e62.
brain activation patterns in adult attention-deficit Haber SN, Kim KS, Mailly P, and Calzavara R. Reward-related
hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) associated with task switching. cortical inputs define a large striatal region in primates that
Neuropsychology, 24(4): 413e423, 2010. interface with associative cortical connections, providing
212 c o r t e x 4 8 ( 2 0 1 2 ) 1 9 4 e2 1 5

a substrate for incentive-based learning. Journal of Luman M, Oosterlaan J, and Sergeant JA. The impact of
Neuroscience, 26(32): 8368e8376, 2006. reinforcement contingencies on AD/HD: A review and
Hale ST, Bookheimer S, McGough JJ, Phillips JM, and theoretical appraisal. Clinical Psychology Review, 25(2):
McCracken JT. Atypical brain activation during simple & 183e213, 2005.
complex levels of processing in adult ADHD: An fMRI study. MacDonald 3rd AW, Cohen JD, Stenger VA, and Carter CS.
Journal of Attention Disorders, 11(2): 125e139, 2007. Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and
Herpertz SC, Huebner T, Marx I, Vloet TD, Fink GR, Stoecker T, anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. Science,
et al. Emotional processing in male adolescents with 288(5472): 1835e1838, 2000.
childhood-onset conduct disorder. Journal of Child Psychology Mackie S, Shaw P, Lenroot R, Pierson R, Greenstein DK, Nugent TF,
and Psychiatry, 49(7): 781e791, 2008. et al. Cerebellar development and clinical outcome in
Hervey AS, Epstein JN, and Curry JF. Neuropsychology of adults attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. American Journal of
with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analytic Psychiatry, 164(4): 647e655, 2007.
review. Neuropsychology, 18(3): 485e503, 2004. Makris N, Buka SL, Biederman J, Papadimitriou GM, Hodge SM,
Hesslinger B, Tebartz van Elst L, Thiel T, Haegele K, Hennig J, and Valera EM, et al. Attention and executive systems
Ebert D. Frontoorbital volume reductions in adult patients abnormalities in adults with childhood ADHD: A DTeMRI
with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neuroscience study of connections. Cerebral Cortex, 18(5): 1210e1220, 2008.
Letters, 328(3): 319e321, 2002. Makris N, Biederman J, Monuteaux MC, and Seidman LJ. Towards
Hill J, Harrington R, Fudge H, Rutter M, and Pickles A. Adult conceptualizing a neural systems-based anatomy of
personality functioning assessment (APFA). An investigator- attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Developmental
based standardised interview. British Journal of Psychiatry, Neuroscience, 31(1e2): 36e49, 2009.
155(1): 24e35, 1989. Makris N, Seidman LJ, Valera EM, Biederman J, Monuteaux MC,
Kadesjo B and Gillberg C. The comorbidity of ADHD in the general Kennedy DN, et al. Anterior cingulate volumetric alterations in
population of Swedish school-age children. Journal of Child treatment-naive adults with ADHD: A pilot study. Journal of
Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(4): 487e492, 2001. Attention Disorders, 13(4): 407e413, 2010.
Kessler RC, Adler L, Barkley R, Biederman J, Conners CK, Demler O, Mannuzza S, Klein RG, Klein DF, Bessler A, and Shrout P. Accuracy
et al. The prevalence and correlates of adult ADHD in the United of adult recall of childhood attention deficit hyperactivity
States: Results from the National Comorbidity Survey disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159(11):
Replication. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163(4): 716e723, 2006. 1882e1888, 2002.
Knutson B, Fong GW, Bennett SM, Adams CM, and Hommer D. A Marchetta ND, Hurks PP, De Sonneville LM, Krabbendam L, and
region of mesial prefrontal cortex tracks monetarily Jolles J. Sustained and focused attention deficits in adult
rewarding outcomes: Characterization with rapid event- ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 11(6): 664e676, 2008.
related fMRI. NeuroImage, 18(2): 263e272, 2003. Marco R, Miranda A, Schlotz W, Melia A, Mulligan A, Muller U,
Konrad A, Dielentheis TF, El Masri D, Bayerl M, Fehr C, Gesierich T, et al. Delay and reward choice in ADHD: An experimental
et al. Disturbed structural connectivity is related to inattention test of the role of delay aversion. Neuropsychology, 23(3):
and impulsivity in adult attention deficit hyperactivity 367e380, 2009.
disorder. European Journal of Neuroscience, 31(5): 912e919, 2010. Marsh AA, Finger EC, Michell DGV, Sims C, Kosson DS, Towbin KE,
Konrad K, Neufang S, Hanisch C, Fink GR, and Herpertz- et al. Reduced amygdala response to fearful expressions in
Dahlmann B. Dysfunctional attentional networks in children children and adolescents with callous-unemotional traits and
with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Evidence from disruptive behaviour disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry,
an event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging 165(6): 712e720, 2008.
study. Biological Psychiatry, 59(7): 643e651, 2006. Martinussen R, Hayden J, Hogg-Johnson S, and Tannock R. A
Konrad K and Eickhoff SB. Is the ADHD brain wired differently? A meta-analysis of working memory impairments in children
review on structural and functional connectivity in attention with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the
deficit hyperactivity disorder. Human Brain Mapping, 31(6): American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 44(4):
904e916, 2010. 377e384, 2005.
Krain AL and Castellanos FX. Brain development and ADHD. Mesulam MM, Nobre AC, Kim YH, Parrish TB, and Gitelman DR.
Clinical Psychology Review, 26(4): 433e444, 2006. Heterogeneity of cingulate contributions to spatial attention.
Krain AL, Wilson AM, Arbuckle R, Castellanos FX, and Milham MP. NeuroImage, 13(6): 1065e1072, 2001.
Distinct neural mechanisms of risk and ambiguity: A meta- Miller G and Chapman J. Misunderstanding analysis of
analysis of decision-making. NeuroImage, 32(1): 477e484, 2006. covariance. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110(1): 40e48, 2001.
Krawczyk DC, Gazzaley A, and D’Esposito M. Reward modulation Miller TW, Nigg JT, and Faraone SV. Axis I and II comorbidity in
of prefrontal and visual association cortex during an incentive adults with ADHD. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116(3):
working memory task. Brain Research, 1141: 168e177, 2007. 519e528, 2007.
Kringelbach ML. The human orbitofrontal cortex: Linking reward Mohanty A, Gitelman DR, Small DM, and Mesulam MM. The
to hedonic experience. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 6(9): spatial attention network interacts with limbic and
691e702, 2005. monoaminergic systems to modulate motivation-induced
Lang W, Obrig H, Lindinger G, Cheyne D, and Deecke L. attention shifts. Cerebral Cortex, 18(11): 2604e2613, 2008.
Supplementary motor area activation while tapping Mostofsky SH and Simmonds DJ. Response inhibition and
bimanually different rhythms in musicians. Experimental Brain response selection: Two sides of the same coin. Journal of
Research, 79(3): 504e514, 1990. Cognitive Neurosciences, 20(5): 751e761, 2008.
Lau HC, Rogers RD, Ramnani N, and Passingham RE. Willed action Nakao T, Radua J, Rubia K and Mataix-Cols D. Gray matter volume
and attention to the selection of action. NeuroImage, 21(4): abnormalities in ADHD and the effects of stimulant
1407e1415, 2004a. medication: Voxel-based meta-analysis. American Journal of
Lau HC, Rogers RD, Haggard P, and Passingham RE. Attention to Psychiatry, in press.
intention. Science, 303(5661): 1208e1210, 2004b. Nigg JT, Willcutt EG, Doyle AE, and Sonuga-Barke EJ. Causal
Lawrence NS, Ross TJ, Hoffmann R, Garavan H, and Stein EA. heterogeneity in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Do
Multiple neuronal networks mediate sustained attention. we need neuropsychologically impaired subtypes? Biological
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15(7): 1028e1038, 2003. Psychiatry, 57(11): 1224e1230, 2005.
c o r t e x 4 8 ( 2 0 1 2 ) 1 9 4 e2 1 5 213

Nigg JT and Casey BJ. An integrative theory of attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder during attention allocation and its
hyperactivity disorder based on the cognitive and affective relation to response variability. Biological Psychiatry, 62(9):
neurosciences. Development and Psychopathology, 17(3): 999e1006, 2007b.
785e806, 2005. Rubia K, Smith A, Taylor E, and Brammer M. Linear age-correlated
O’Doherty JP. Reward representations and reward-related functional development of right inferior fronto-striato-
learning in the human brain: Insights from neuroimaging. cerebellar networks during response inhibition and anterior
Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 14(6): 769e776, 2004. cingulate during error-related processes. Human Brain
Pavuluri MN, Yang S, Kamineni K, Passarotti AM, Srinivasan G, Mapping, 28(11): 1163e1177, 2007c.
Harral EM, et al. Diffusion tensor imaging study of white Rubia K, Halari R, Smith A, Mohammed M, Scott S, Giampietro V,
matter fiber tracts in pediatric bipolar disorder and attention- et al. Dissociated functional brain abnormalities of inhibition
deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 65(7): in boys with pure conduct disorder and in boys with pure
586e593, 2009. attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. American Journal of
Perlov E, Philipsen A, Tebartz van Elst L, Ebert D, Henning J, Psychiatry, 165(7): 889e897, 2008.
Maier S, et al. Hippocampus and amygdala morphology in Rubia K, Halari R, Christakou A, and Taylor E. Impulsiveness as
adults with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of a timing disturbance: Neurocognitive abnormalities in
Psychiatry and Neuroscience, 33(6): 509e515, 2008. attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder during temporal
Plichta MM, Vasic N, Wolf RC, Lesch KP, Brummer D, Jacob C, et al. processes and normalization with methylphenidate.
Neural hyporesponsiveness and hyperresponsiveness during Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London,
immediate and delayed reward processing in adult attention- 364(1525): 1919e1931, 2009a.
deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 65(1): Rubia K, Halari R, Smith A, Mohammad M, Scott S, and
7e14, 2009. Brammer M. Shared and disorder-specific prefrontal
Pliszka SR, Glahn DC, Semrud-Clikeman M, Franklin C, Perez R, abnormalities in boys with pure attention-deficit/
and Xiong JJ. Neuroimaging of inhibitory control areas in hyperactivity disorder compared to boys with pure CD during
children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder who interference inhibition and attention allocation. Journal of Child
were treatment naive or in long-term treatment. American Psychology and Psychiatry, 50(6): 669e678, 2009b.
Journal of Psychiatry, 163(6): 1052e1060, 2006. Rubia K, Smith A, Halari R, Matukura F, Mohammad M, Taylor E,
Pochon JB, Levy R, Fossati P, Lehericy S, Poline JB, Pillon B, et al. et al. Disorder-specific dissociation of orbitofrontal
The neural system that bridges reward and cognition in dysfunction in boys with pure conduct disorder during reward
humans: An fMRI study. Proceedings of the National Academy of and ventrolateral prefrontal dysfunction in boys with pure
Sciences of the United States of America, 99(8): 5669e5674, 2002. attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder during sustained
Raven J. Guide to the Standard Progressive Matrices. London: HK attention. American Journal of Psychiatry, 166(1): 83e94, 2009c.
Lewis, 1960. Rubia K, Halari R, Cubillo A, Mohammad M, and Taylor E.
Ridderinkhof KR, Nieuwenhuis S, and Bashore TR. Errors are Methylphenidate normalises activation and functional
foreshadowed in brain potentials associated with action connectivity deficits in attention and motivation networks in
monitoring in cingulate cortex in humans. Neuroscience Letters, medication-naı̈ve children with ADHD during a Rewarded
348(1): 1e4, 2003. Continuous Performance Task. Neuropharmacology, 57(7e8):
Rothermund K, Wentura D, and Bak PM. Automatic attention to 640e652, 2009d.
stimuli signalling chances and dangers: Moderating effects of Rubia K, Halari R, Cubillo A, Mohammad M, Scott S, and
positive and negative goal and action contexts. Cognition and Brammer M. Disorder-specific inferior prefrontal
Emotion, 15(2): 231e248, 2001. hypofunction in boys with pure attention-deficit/hyperactivity
Rubia K. “Cool” inferior fronto-striatal dysfunction in attention- disorder compared to boys with pure conduct disorder during
deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) versus “hot” cognitive flexibility. Human Brain Mapping, 31(12):
ventromedial orbitofronto-limbic dysfunction in conduct 1823e1833, 2010a.
disorder: A review. Biological Psychiatry,. epub ahead of Rubia K, Cubillo A, Smith A, Woolley J, Heyman I, and
printing, doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.09.023 2010. Brammer M. Disorder-specific dysfunction in right inferior
Rubia K, Overmeyer S, Taylor E, Brammer M, Williams SC, prefrontal cortex during two inhibition tasks in boys with
Simmons A, et al. Hypofrontality in attention deficit attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder compared to boys with
hyperactivity disorder during higher-order motor control: A obsessive-compulsive disorder. Human Brain Mapping, 31(2):
study with functional MRI. American Journal of Psychiatry, 287e299, 2010b.
156(6): 891e896, 1999. Rubia K, Cubillo A, Woolley J, Halari R, Smith A, and Brammer M.
Rubia K, Taylor E, Smith A, Oksanen H, Overmeyer S, and Disorder-specific dysfunctions in boys with attention-deficit/
Newman S. Neuropsychological analyses of impulsiveness in hyperactivity disorder compared to boys with obsessive-
childhood hyperactivity. British Journal of Psychiatry, 179(2): compulsive disorder during interference inhibition and
138e143, 2001. attention allocation. Human Brain Mapping, 32(4):
Rubia K, Smith A, Brammer M, and Taylor E. Right inferior 601e611, 2011.
prefrontal cortex mediates response inhibition while mesial Rubia K, Halari R, Cubillo A, Mohammad AM, M B and E T.
prefrontal cortex is responsible for error detection. Methylphenidate normalises fronto-striatal underactivation
NeuroImage, 20(1): 351e358, 2003. during interference inhibition in medication-naive children
Rubia K, Smith A, Brammer M, Toone B, and Taylor E. Abnormal with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder.
brain activation during inhibition and error detection in Neuropsychopharmacology, in press [epub ahead of printing, doi:
medication-naive adolescents with ADHD. American Journal of 10.1038/npp.2011.30,2011].
Psychiatry, 162(6): 1067e1075, 2005. Sagvolden T, Aase H, Zeiner P, and Berger D. Altered
Rubia K, Smith A, Brammer M, and Taylor E. Performance of reinforcement mechanisms in attention-deficit/hyperactivity
children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) disorder. Behavioural Brain Research, 94(1): 61e71, 1998.
on a test battery for impulsiveness. Child Neuropsychology, Scheres A, Milham MP, Knutson B, and Castellanos FX. Ventral
30(2): 659e695, 2007a. striatal hyporesponsiveness during reward anticipation in
Rubia K, Smith A, Brammer M, and Taylor E. Temporal lobe attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry,
dysfunction in medication-naive boys with attention-deficit/ 61(5): 720e724, 2007.
214 c o r t e x 4 8 ( 2 0 1 2 ) 1 9 4 e2 1 5

Schlochtermeier L, Stoy M, Schlagenhauf F, Wrase J, Park SQ, Smith A, Taylor E, Brammer M, Halari R, and Rubia K. Reduced
Friedel E, Huss M, Lehmkuhl U, Heinz A and Strohle A. activation in right lateral prefrontal cortex and anterior
Childhood methylphenidate treatment of ADHD and response cingulate gyrus in medication-naive adolescents with
to affective stimuli. European Neuropsychopharmacology, in press attention deficit hyperactivity disorder during time
[epub ahead of printing, doi:10.1016/j.euroneuro.2010.05.001]. discrimination. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
Schmitz N, Rubia K, van Amelsvoort T, Daly E, Smith A, and 49(9): 977e985, 2008.
Murphy DG. Neural correlates of reward in autism. British Smith A, Halari R, Giampietro V, Brammer M, and Rubia K.
Journal of Psychiatry, 192(1): 19e24, 2008. Developmental effects of reward on sustained attention
Schneider MF, Krick CM, Retz W, Hengesch G, Retz-Junginger P, networks. NeuroImage, 56(3): 1693e1704, 2011.
Reith W, et al. Impairment of fronto-striatal and parietal Smith SM. Fast robust automated brain extraction. Human Brain
cerebral networks correlates with attention deficit Mapping, 17(3): 143e155, 2002.
hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) psychopathology in adults e Sobanski E, Bruggemann D, Alm B, Kern S, Deschner M, Schubert T,
a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study. et al. Psychiatric comorbidity and functional impairment in
Psychiatry Research, 183(1): 75e84, 2010. a clinically referred sample of adults with attention-deficit/
Schoenbaum G, Roesch MR, and Stalnaker TA. Orbitofrontal hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). European Archives of Psychiatry
cortex, decision-making and drug addiction. Trends in and Clinical Neurosciences, 257(7): 371e377, 2007.
Neurosciences, 29(2): 116e124, 2006. Sobanski E, Bruggemann D, Alm B, Kern S, Philipsen A,
Seidman LJ, Valera EM, Makris N, Monuteaux MC, Boriel DL, Schmalzried H, et al. Subtype differences in adults with
Kelkar K, et al. Dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) with regard to
cortex volumetric abnormalities in adults with attention- ADHD-symptoms, psychiatric comorbidity and psychosocial
deficit/hyperactivity disorder identified by magnetic resonance adjustment. European Psychiatry, 23(2): 142e149, 2008.
imaging. Biological Psychiatry, 60(10): 1071e1080, 2006. Sonuga-Barke E, Bitsakou P, and Thompson M. Beyond the dual
Seidman LJ, Biederman J, Liang L, Valera EM, Monuteaux MC, pathway model: Evidence for the dissociation of timing,
Brown A, Kaiser J, Spencer T, Faraone SV, and Makris N. Gray inhibitory, and delay-related impairments in attention-deficit/
matter alterations in adults with attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child
hyperactivitydisorder identified by voxel based morphometry. and Adolescent Psychiatry, 49(4): 345e355, 2010.
Biological Psychiatry, 69(9): 857e866, 2011. Spencer T, Biederman J, and Mick E. Attention-deficit/
Sergeant JA, Geurts H, and Oosterlaan J. How specific is a deficit of hyperactivity disorder: Diagnosis, lifespan, comorbidities, and
executive functioning for attention-deficit/hyperactivity neurobiology. Ambulatory Pediatrics, 7(1): 73e81, 2007.
disorder? Behavioural Brain Research, 130(1e2): 3e28, 2002. Spencer TJ. ADHD and comorbidity in childhood. Journal of Clinical
Sharp DJ, Bonnelle V, De Boissezon X, Beckmann CF, James SG, Psychiatry, 67(Suppl. 8): 27e31, 2006.
Patel MC, et al. Distinct frontal systems for response Spinelli S, Vasa RA, Joel S, Nelson TE, Pekar JJ and Mostofsky SH.
inhibition, attentional capture, and error processing. Variability in post-error behavioral adjustment is associated
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States with functional abnormalities in the temporal cortex in
of America, 107(13): 6106e6111, 2010. children with ADHD. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
Shaw P, Lerch J, Greenstein D, Sharp W, Clasen L, Evans A, et al. in press [epub ahead of printing, doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.
Longitudinal mapping of cortical thickness and clinical 02356.x].
outcome in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/ Steinlin M. The cerebellum in cognitive processes: Supporting
hyperactivity disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63(5): studies in children. Cerebellum, 6(3): 237e241, 2007.
540e549, 2006. Stevens MC, Pearlson GD, and Kiehl KA. An FMRI auditory
Shaw P, Eckstrand K, Sharp W, Blumenthal J, Lerch JP, oddball study of combined-subtype attention deficit
Greenstein D, et al. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder is hyperactivity disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 164(11):
characterized by a delay in cortical maturation. Proceedings of 1737e1749, 2007.
the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, Stringaris A, Hawkins A, McLoughlin L, Moya Querejeta J,
104(49): 19649e19654, 2007. Asherson P, Sandberg S and Taylor E. Childhood hyperactivity
Shaw P, Gilliam M, Liverpool M, Weddle C, Malek M, Sharp W, as a risk factor for adult psychopathology: results from a 20
et al. Cortical development in typically developing children year prospective community-based study in London. Journal of
with symptoms of hyperactivity and impulsivity: Support for Child Psychology and Psychiatry, in press.
a dimensional view of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Ströhle A, Stoy M, Wrase J, Schwarzer S, Schlagenhauf F, Huss M,
American Journal of Psychiatry, 168(2): 143e151, 2011. et al. Reward anticipation and outcomes in adult males with
Simmonds DJ, Pekar JJ, and Mostofsky SH. Meta-analysis of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. NeuroImage, 39(3):
Go/No-go tasks demonstrating that fMRI activation associated 966e972, 2008.
with response inhibition is task-dependent. Neuropsychologia, Stuss DT and Alexander MP. Executive functions and the frontal
46(1): 224e232, 2008. lobes: A conceptual view. Psychological Research, 63(3e4):
Small DM, Gitelman DR, Gregory MD, Nobre AC, Parrish TB, and 289e298, 2000.
Mesulam MM. The posterior cingulate and medial prefrontal Sundram F, Deeley Q, Sarkar S, Daly E, Latham R, Craig M, et al.
cortex mediate the anticipatory allocation of spatial attention. White matter microstructural abnormalities in the frontal
NeuroImage, 18(3): 633e641, 2003. lobe of adults with antisocial personality. Cortex, 48(2):
Small DM, Gitelman D, Simmons K, Bloise SM, Parrish T, and 199e212, 2012.
Mesulam MM. Monetary incentives enhance processing in Suskauer SJ, Simmonds DJ, Caffo BS, Denckla MB, Pekar JJ, and
brain regions mediating topedown control of attention. Mostofsky SH. fMRI of intrasubject variability in ADHD:
Cerebral Cortex, 15(12): 1855e1865, 2005. Anomalous premotor activity with prefrontal compensation.
Smith A, Taylor E, Brammer M, Toone B, and Rubia K. Task- Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
specific hypoactivation in prefrontal and temporoparietal Psychiatry, 47(10): 1141e1150, 2008a.
brain regions during motor inhibition and task switching in Suskauer SJ, Simmonds DJ, Fotedar S, Blankner JG, Pekar JJ,
medication-naive children and adolescents with attention Denckla MB, et al. Functional magnetic resonance imaging
deficit hyperactivity disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, evidence for abnormalities in response selection in attention
163(6): 1044e1051, 2006. deficit hyperactivity disorder: Differences in activation
c o r t e x 4 8 ( 2 0 1 2 ) 1 9 4 e2 1 5 215

associated with response inhibition but not habitual motor Valera EM, Brown A, Biederman J, Faraone SV, Makris N,
response. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(3): 478e493, 2008b. Monuteaux MC, et al. Sex differences in the functional
Tabu H, Mima T, Aso T, Takahashi R and Fukuyama H. Functional neuroanatomy of working memory in adults with ADHD.
relevance of pre-supplementary motor areas for the choice to American Journal of Psychiatry, 167(1): 87e94, 2010a.
stop during Stop signal task. Neuroscience Research, in press Valera EM, Spencer RM, Zeffiro TA, Makris N, Spencer TJ,
[epub ahead of printing, doi:10.1016/j.neures.2011.03.007]. Faraone SV, et al. Neural substrates of impaired sensorimotor
Tamm L, Menon V, Ringel J, and Reiss AL. Event-related FMRI timing in adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
evidence of frontotemporal involvement in aberrant response Biological Psychiatry, 68(4): 359e367, 2010b.
inhibition and task switching in attention-deficit/ Valko L, Schneider G, Doehnert M, Muller U, Brandeis D,
hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child Steinhausen HC, et al. Time processing in children and
and Adolescent Psychiatry, 43(11): 1430e1440, 2004. adults with ADHD. Journal of Neural Transmission, 117(10):
Tamm L, Menon V, and Reiss AL. Parietal attentional system 1213e1228, 2010.
aberrations during target detection in adolescents with Vloet TD, Gilsbach S, Neufang S, Fink GR, Herpertz-Dahlmann B,
attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Event-related fMRI and Konrad K. Neural mechanisms of interference control and
evidence. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163(6): time discrimination in attention-deficit/hyperactivity
1033e1043, 2006. disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
Tana MG, Montin E, Cerutti S, and Bianchi AM. Exploring cortical Psychiatry, 49(4): 356e367, 2010.
attentional system by using fMRI during a Continuous Voisin J, Bidet-Caulet A, Bertrand O, and Fonlupt P. Listening in
Performance Test. Computational Intelligence and Neuroscience, silence activates auditory areas: A functional magnetic
2010(329213): 1e6, 2010. resonance imaging study. Journal of Neuroscience, 26(1):
Taylor E, Sandberg S, Thorley G, and Giles S. The Epidemiology of 273e278, 2006.
Childhood Hyperactivity. Oxford, England: Oxford University Wang J, Wang L, Zang Y, Yang H, Tang H, Gong Q, et al.
Press, 1991. Parcellation-dependent small-world brain functional
Taylor E, Chadwick O, Heptinstall E, and Danckaerts M. networks: A resting-state fMRI study. Human Brain Mapping,
Hyperactivity and conduct problems as risk factors for 30(5): 1511e1523, 2009.
adolescent development. Journal of the American Academy of Wilens TE, Biederman J, Faraone SV, Martelon M, Westerberg D,
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 35(9): 1213e1226, 1996. and Spencer TJ. Presenting ADHD symptoms, subtypes, and
Thiebaut de Schotten M, Dell’Acqua F, Valabregue R, and comorbid disorders in clinically referred adults with ADHD.
Catani M. Monkey to human comparative anatomy of the Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 70(11): 1557e1562, 2009.
frontal lobe association tracts. Cortex, 48(1): 82e96, 2012. Willcutt EG, Doyle AE, Nigg JT, Faraone SV, and Pennington BF.
Thirion B, Pinel P, Meriaux S, Roche A, Dehaene S, and Poline JB. Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/
Analysis of a large fMRI cohort: Statistical and methodological hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analytic review. Biological
issues for group analyses. NeuroImage, 35(1): 105e120, 2007. Psychiatry, 57(11): 1336e1346, 2005.
Tian LX, Jiang TZ, Wang YF, Zang YF, He Y, Liang M, et al. Altered Windmann S, Kirsch P, Mier D, Stark R, Walter B, Gunturkun O,
resting-state functional connectivity patterns of anterior et al. On framing effects in decision making: Linking lateral
cingulate cortex in adolescents with attention deficit versus medial orbitofrontal cortex activation to choice
hyperactivity disorder. Neuroscience Letters, 400(1e2): 39e43, 2006. outcome processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(7):
Tomporowski PD and Tinsley VF. Effects of memory demand and 1198e1211, 2006.
motivation on sustained attention in young and older adults. Wolf RC, Plichta MM, Sambataro F, Fallgatter AJ, Jacob C,
American Journal of Psychology, 109(2): 187e204, 1996. Lesch KP, et al. Regional brain activation changes and
Uddin LQ, Kelly AM, Biswal BB, Margulies DS, Shehzad Z, Shaw D, abnormal functional connectivity of the ventrolateral
et al. Network homogeneity reveals decreased integrity of prefrontal cortex during working memory processing in adults
default-mode network in ADHD. Journal of Neuroscience with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Human Brain
Methods, 169(1): 249e254, 2008. Mapping, 30(7): 2252e2266, 2009.
Vaidya CJ, Bunge SA, Dudukovic NM, and Zalecki CA. Altered Zang YF, He Y, Zhu CZ, Cao QJ, Sui MQ, Liang M, et al. Altered
neural substrates of cognitive control in childhood ADHD: baseline brain activity in children with ADHD revealed by
Evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging. resting-state functional MRI. Brain and Development, 29(2):
American Journal of Psychiatry, 162(9): 1605e1613, 2005. 83e91, 2007.
Valera EM, Faraone SV, Biederman J, Poldrack RA, and Seidman LJ. Zelazo PD and Muller U. Executive function in typical and atypical
Functional neuroanatomy of working memory in adults with development. In Goswami U (Ed), Handbook of Cognitive
attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, Development. Oxford: Blackwell, 2002: 445e469.
57(5): 439e447, 2005. Zhu CZ, Zang YF, Liang M, Tian LX, He Y, Li XB, et al. Discriminative
Valera EM, Faraone SV, Murray KE, and Seidman LJ. Meta-analysis analysis of brain function at resting-state for attention-deficit/
of structural imaging findings in attention-deficit/hyperactivity hyperactivity disorder. Medical Image Computing and Computer-
disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 61(12): 1361e1369, 2007. Assisted Intervention e Miccai 2005, 3750(Pt 2): 468e475, 2005.

You might also like