Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

The Gerontologist Copyright 2004 by The Gerontological Society of America

Vol. 44, No. 5, 596–604

Measuring Psychological Well-Being: Insights


From Thai Elders

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/gerontologist/article/44/5/596/798683 by UNIVERSITI MALAYSIA SARAWAK user on 19 October 2023


Berit Ingersoll-Dayton, PhD,1 Chanpen Saengtienchai, MA,2
Jiraporn Kespichayawattana, PhD,3 and Yupin Aungsuroch, PhD3

Purpose: Psychological well-being, an important Understanding processes associated with success-


indicator of successful aging, may be conceptualized ful aging has become a key focus for gerontological
quite differently across cultures. Using a mixed- researchers (Baltes & Carstensen, 1996; Rowe &
methods approach, we developed a measure of Kahn, 1998; Strawbridge, Cohen, Shema, & Kaplan,
psychological well-being based on the indigenous 1996). One important indicator of successful aging is
expertise of Thai elders. Design and Methods: Data psychological well-being; that is, the strengths and
were collected from older people in Thailand in four
capacities of older people (Ryff, 1999). Highlighting
these strengths leads to an understanding of how
stages with staggered qualitative and quantitative
individuals remain resilient in the face of aging-
methods: individual and focus group interviews (n = related changes. However, the way in which people
67); a preliminary survey (n = 477); cognitive conceptualize psychological well-being and its com-
interviews (n = 30); and a second survey (n = ponents may differ markedly from one culture to
460). We analyzed the resulting psychological well- another. To illuminate those aspects of well-being
being items to identify their underlying factor structure that are unique versus those that are common across
and psychometric properties. Results: Confirmatory cultures, we focus on older Thai people. This article
factor analysis suggested that psychological well- describes the development of a culturally relevant
being has two components: intrapersonal and in- measure of psychological well-being and examines
terpersonal. The subscales for this measure have its structure for Thai elders. This exploration of
adequate reliability and validity. Implications: This well-being from the perspective of Asians contributes
research provides evidence for cultural variability in toward our understanding of successful aging from
the nature of psychological well-being and highlights a cross-cultural perspective.
the importance of developing measures that are Psychological well-being is inextricably connected
culturally relevant. to cultural values concerning individuals and indi-
viduals in relation to each other. Qualities of
Key Words: Successful aging, Measurement, independence and interdependence are important in
Culture, Asian elders all cultures; however, most societies value one more
than the other (Greenfield, 1994). For example,
although there is variation within each culture, the
relative importance of independence and interdepen-
dence appears to vary across Western and Eastern
This research was supported by the National Institutes of Health cultures. In particular, Americans generally rate
under Grant 2 D43 TW00657.06 from the Fogarty International Center
to the Population Studies Center of the University of Michigan. We higher in individualistic and lower in collectivistic
express our warm thanks to Jintana Yunibhand for her continued values than people from other countries (Oyserman,
support of this effort; to Neal Krause for his advice on the project design;
to Suvinee Vivatvanich and the graduate nursing students at the Coon, & Kemmelmeier, 2002). The relative empha-
Chulalongkorn University Faculty of Nursing for their help in data sis placed on the individual as compared with the
collection; to Malee Samphuwan for her data management; to Laura
Klem for her assistance with the statistical analysis; to Terri Torkko for collective is intertwined with the way in which well-
editorial support; and to Jasper and Fern Ingersoll, John Knodel, David being is conceptualized in different cultures.
Morgan, and Lydia Li for their helpful comments on earlier versions of
this manuscript. In Western countries, autonomy and indepen-
Address correspondence to Dr. Berit Ingersoll-Dayton, School of dence are emphasized such that the self is seen as
Social Work, The University of Michigan, 1080 South University, Ann
Arbor, MI 48109. E-mail: bid@umich.edu distinct from others (Markus & Kitayama, 1991).
1
2
School of Social Work, The University of Michigan, Ann Arbor. The importance of these qualities is evident in
College of Population Studies, Chulalongkorn University, Bangkok,
Thailand. childhood educational practices that encourage
3
Faculty of Nursing, Chulalongkorn University, Bangkok, Thailand. American elementary-school children to ‘‘show and

596 The Gerontologist


tell.’’ During such activities, children are taught to a single higher order factor’’ (p. 724). The research
develop a sense of who they are and what is uniquely by Ryff and her colleagues suggests that, for
important in their lives (Fiske, Kitayama, Markus, American adults, psychological well-being can be
& Nisbett, 1998). As an extension of these early conceptualized as a hierarchical structure in which
socialization experiences, individuals are expected to well-being has its influence through six distinct
develop personal goals and evaluate themselves in domains, most of which are focused on the in-
relation to their achievements. Relationships with dependent self.
others are sometimes viewed as less important Although Ryff’s measure of psychological well-

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/gerontologist/article/44/5/596/798683 by UNIVERSITI MALAYSIA SARAWAK user on 19 October 2023


because they interfere with personal goals (Fiske being (1989a, 1989b) appears to be well suited for
et al., 1998). Instead, Westerners strive for ‘‘self- Europe and America, its conceptualization of self-
actualization,’’ ‘‘realizing oneself,’’ and ‘‘developing hood makes it less applicable to Asia. In a critique of
one’s distinct potential’’ (Markus & Kitayama, 1991, this measure, Christopher (1999) questions the
p. 226). Given this view of the self, independence is universality of the six domains used as criteria for
highly valued and associated with well-being for psychological well-being. He observes that, though
older people. certain components of well-being may be shared
Asians have distinctly different views of the across cultures, their meaning and relative impor-
person. From their perspective, people are funda- tance may vary. Christopher concludes that, ‘‘as
mentally interconnected. They value belongingness, a result, different components of our understanding
reciprocity, empathy, and respect. Within Eastern of psychological well-being (like autonomy or
cultures, the self is viewed as interdependent, people happiness) cannot simply be transported to another
are mutually responsible for one another, and culture without risk of serious misrepresentation and
individuals evaluate themselves based on their misunderstanding’’ (p. 149).
contributions to the collective good (Fiske et al., Our work represents an effort to understand the
1998). Early socialization experiences emphasize components of psychological well-being that are
group collaborations and concern for others. For indigenous to older Thai people. Research has
example, in Japan the goals of ‘‘helping one already been conducted on the psychological views
another’’ and ‘‘pooling strength’’ are interwoven and values of other developed Asian countries, such
into school subject areas. As part of a social studies as Japan (Kitayama, Markus, Matsumoto, &
assignment in elementary school, Japanese children Norasakkunkit, 1997; Kumagai, 1981). However,
are told to observe their mothers’ work at home. The unlike these other Asian countries, Thailand has
children are then taught to set goals for themselves never been occupied by a Western power; thus the
that relate to easing their mothers’ responsibilities views and values of Thai people may be less
(Lewis, 1995). In Asia, harmonious relationships influenced by Western thought (Weisz, 1989). By
with others are a vital source of well-being, and the focusing on Thailand, we enlarge our understand-
ability to adapt one’s own interests to those of ing about the cultural context of psychological
the dyad, group, or institution is highly valued (Fiske well-being.
et al., 1998; Oerter, Oerter, Agostiani, Kim, & As a first step in developing measures that are
Wibowo, 1996; Triandis, 1989). It follows that, authentic to indigenous cultures, cross-cultural
within Asian models of the ideal self, interdepen- psychologists suggest a so-called ‘‘bottom-up ap-
dence is associated with well-being among the proach’’ that involves consulting with the individuals
elderly population. for whom the measure is developed (Kim, Park, &
Thus far, most standardized measures of psycho- Park, 2000, p. 66). Qualitative methods based on the
logical well-being have been based on the Western use of open-ended questions and in-depth explora-
conceptualization of the ideal self as autonomous tion are especially effective in the initial stages of
and independent. In particular, Ryff (1989a, 1989b) measurement development for at least two reasons
has developed a widely used measure based on (Carter-Edwards, Bynoe, & Svetkey, 1998; Krause,
a content analysis of writings by prominent Western 2002). First, by encouraging people to talk freely
clinicians and theoreticians (e.g., Erikson, 1959; about a substantive domain, researchers can identify
Jung, 1933; Maslow, 1968; Neugarten, 1968; Rogers, the dimensions most relevant to the people for whom
1961). The measure includes six distinct facets of they are developing the measure. Second, the words
positive psychological functioning (i.e., self-accep- and phrases evoked during this free-flowing conver-
tance, positive relations with others, autonomy, sation can be used in the construction of closed-
environmental mastery, purpose in life, and personal ended items.
growth). The psychometric properties of this multi- The development of our Thai measure of
dimensional measure indicate good reliability and psychological well-being relied heavily on a bot-
validity (Ryff & Keyes, 1995). Further, on the basis tom-up approach. We were guided by the sugges-
of confirmatory factor analyses, Ryff and Keyes tions of Krause (2002), whose multistage
determined that the structure of well-being is best measurement development process begins with
represented by a second-order global factor in which qualitative methods and moves toward quantitative
the six well-being factors are ‘‘joined together by methods. This process includes the following steps:

Vol. 44, No. 5, 2004 597


(a) focus groups, (b) in-depth interviews, (c) input from local health care centers to identify elders who
from quantitative studies, (d) development of pre- had different levels of education and income. A total
liminary quantitative measures, (e) review by expert of 67 people (29 men and 38 women) participated
panel, (f) cognitive interviews, (g) pilot study, (h) in this phase of the study. We collected data from
nationwide survey, and (i) psychometric testing. Our 23 in-depth interviewees and 44 participants in five
approach was further informed by Morgan’s (1998) focus groups.
framework of complementary qualitative and quan- To identify dimensions of well-being, we asked
titative methods. According to this framework, participants the questions developed by Ryff (1989b)

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/gerontologist/article/44/5/596/798683 by UNIVERSITI MALAYSIA SARAWAK user on 19 October 2023


qualitative approaches can precede or follow quan- concerning the characteristics of an elderly person
titative approaches. When qualitative methods are who was well adjusted, mature, personally fulfilled,
used first, they can illuminate the domains to be and fit the notion of the ideal person. In addition, we
quantified. When qualitative methods follow quan- asked questions based on research by Ingersoll
titative methods, they can help explain the quanti- (1985), whose work in rural Thailand highlighted
tative findings. enjoyment and hopes for the future as key elements
The discussion that follows is divided into three of life quality. Respondents’ answers were tape-
main sections. First, we describe in detail the recorded, transcribed into Thai, and translated into
methods by which a measure of psychological well- English.
being relevant to older Thai people was developed. Each of us participated in the analysis of the
Second, we present findings from an analysis of the qualitative data. We developed our conceptual
structure of well-being for our older Thai sample framework through a series of discrete steps, with
and describe the psychometric properties of the two of us reading the Thai transcripts and two
measure. Third, we explain the highlights of our reading the English transcripts. First, using a subset
research findings, discuss the limitations, and offer of the transcriptions, each of us independently
directions for future research. developed a preliminary coding schema. Second,
we collectively reviewed each others’ schemas to
identify a comprehensive set of categories, which we
Methods then augmented with topics suggested by previous
When we applied Krause’s (2002) multistep pro- research on well-being (Ingersoll, 1985; Ryff, 1989a,
cess to the development of a Thai measure of 1989b). Third, using the resulting coding schema,
psychological well-being, we made some adapta- two of us independently coded the Thai transcripts
tions. We juxtaposed qualitative and quantitative and two coded the English transcripts. We entered
methods in a circular, iterative process that involved our coding decisions by using Ethnograph 4.0
consultation with our older participants at multiple (Seidel, Friese, & Leonard, 1995), a software pack-
points throughout the measurement development age that facilitates the organization and retrieval of
process. That is, we adapted Krause’s method by specific segments of qualitative data for analysis.
incorporating qualitative methods at later stages as Finally, we met as a group to review the data that
well as earlier stages of the process. Here, we emerged within each coding category. Comparing
describe the procedures involved in each stage in both the Thai version and the English translations,
conjunction with the substantive findings that guided we collectively consolidated the coding categories by
subsequent stages of measurement development. identifying the conceptual overlap within the data.
This analysis resulted in five dimensions of
psychological well-being: harmony (experiencing
Qualitative Methods: Focus-Group and peaceful and happy interactions with others), in-
In-Depth Interviews terdependence (providing assistance to and receiving
assistance from family members and others), accep-
Qualitative methods are ideally suited for identi- tance (relinquishing upsetting thoughts and accept-
fying the key domains of a construct that are ing life’s circumstances), respect (feeling one’s advice
‘‘grounded in local meanings’’ (Miles & Huberman, is heeded and one’s wisdom is appreciated), and
1994, p. 36). Consequently, in the first phase of this enjoyment (appreciating simple pleasures that in-
project, we used qualitative methods to illuminate volve others as well as solitary pursuits).
the various aspects of psychological well-being that
are relevant for older Thai people. (For more details,
see Ingersoll-Dayton, Saengtienchai, Kespichayawat- Quantitative Methods: Preliminary Survey
tana, & Aungsuroch, 2001.) Consistent with Krause,
we used two qualitative approaches (i.e., in-depth Once the domains of a construct have been
interviews and focus-group interviews) to uncover identified, quantitative approaches enable us to
the dimensions of psychological well-being. Our measure the frequency of phenomena (Morgan,
sample consisted of people aged 60 and older who 1998). Thus, in the next phase of our project, we
lived in urban and rural areas in and around developed closed-ended survey items that would lend
Bangkok; we drew it purposively by asking staff themselves to measuring psychological well-being

598 The Gerontologist


quantitatively. The construction of these items help us understand the difficulties associated with
involved several distinct steps similar to those our close-ended items and to suggest alternative
described by Krause (2002). First, for each of the approaches. Specifically, we used a cognitive inter-
five dimensions, we constructed closed-ended state- viewing technique, which was another step in
ments (e.g., ‘‘In your family, people get along well measurement development suggested by Krause
together’’) based on relevant phrases from the (2002); we had not used it prior to developing our
transcripts of the in-depth and focus-group inter- survey items because of time constraints.
views. Second, to assess content validity, we had the Cognitive interviewing techniques are routinely

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/gerontologist/article/44/5/596/798683 by UNIVERSITI MALAYSIA SARAWAK user on 19 October 2023


statements examined by four experts in gerontology used by government data-collection agencies and
or quality of life in Thailand. There was consensus major survey-research centers to assess the thought
among the experts that the five dimensions repre- processes of survey respondents (Schwartz, 1999).
sented psychological well-being for Thai elders. In These techniques help survey researchers understand
addition, they provided suggestions to enhance the how respondents interpret questionnaire items in
clarity of specific items and suggested wording for order to develop more comprehensible questions
additional items. On the basis of their feedback, we and response alternatives (Oksenberg, Cannell, &
revised the list of items. Third, we pretested the Kalton, 1991; Sudman, Bradburn, & Schwartz,
optimal number of response choices for these items 1996). Specific cognitive interviewing techniques
by using a format consistent with Ryff’s (1989a) that include concurrent think-alouds (i.e., respondents
ranged from strongly disagree to strongly agree. describe their thoughts as they answer questionnaire
Our respondents indicated that they were able to items; see Genest & Turk, 1981), which are followed
distinguish among four responses; thus we developed by detailed probes that ask respondents further
a consistent set of response choices (i.e., 1 = strongly questions about specific items (Biemer, Groves,
disagree, 2 = disagree, 3 = agree, and 4 = strongly Lyberg, Mathiowetz, & Sudman, 1991). From these
agree) for each item. responses, the survey questionnaires are revised and
Finally, we included these items in a preliminary then reassessed based on additional cognitive inter-
survey conducted in Bangkok and three nearby views (Sudman et al., 1996).
provinces representing both rural and urban areas. Cognitive interviews were conducted with 30
Our purposive sampling strategy involved asking people (10 men and 20 women) aged 60 and older
staff in rural and urban health care centers in these who were selected for their ability to be reflective
provinces to identify elders (those aged 60 years and about their thought processes. Interviews were
older) who represented variability with respect to conducted in Bangkok and two nearby provinces.
socioeconomic status, education, and health. The Respondents were identified through various means,
resulting sample of 477 participants included 182 generally by staff at urban and rural health care
men and 295 women. centers or elderly clubs in these provinces. The
The feedback from the interviews in conjunction resulting sample was fairly well educated (i.e., most
with preliminary data analyses indicated several had more than a fourth-grade education, which is
problems with the psychological well-being items. the highest grade completed by the majority of older
Specifically, interviewers reported that the respon- Thai people).
dents did not understand many of the items and that Our approach to cognitive interviewing relied on
they had difficulty with the strongly disagree to open-ended questions in which interviewers asked
strongly agree response set. In addition, research respondents to think about the interpretability of
team members observed that the presence of family items both from their own perspectives as well as
members during the interview resulted in two that of older people with less education. Respon-
problems. First, respondents provided answers that dents frequently suggested wording changes that we
appeared to be more positive and socially desirable. applied to subsequent cognitive interviews in which
Second, family members sometimes tried to assist the other respondents evaluated the revised items for
older respondents by answering survey questions for clarity and understanding. For example, items from
them. These problems were reflected in our quanti- the preliminary survey that had a negative term (e.g.,
tative analyses of the well-being items, which ‘‘not happy’’) appeared to be particularly confusing
showed little variability and were highly skewed to respondents because they were paired with
toward positive well-being. a response set that included negative options (e.g.,
strongly disagree). During the cognitive interviews,
respondents helped to identify clearer terminology.
Qualitative Methods: Cognitive Interviews Although changes in terminology usually resulted
from open-ended questions about the meaning of
When quantitative findings are confusing, quali- words, they also emerged from our observations. To
tative methods can often provide clarification and illustrate, the term for extended family emerged
suggest new areas of exploration (Miles & Huber- spontaneously when a respondent showed a team
man, 1994). To address the problems identified in the member a picture of her family, which included her
preliminary survey, we used qualitative methods to brothers, sisters, nieces, and nephews. A team

Vol. 44, No. 5, 2004 599


member made note of this term, which was then people (aged 60 and older) in Central Thailand used
incorporated into survey items that were again a multistage selection process to enhance represen-
reviewed during subsequent cognitive interviews. In tativeness of individuals across sociodemographic
these interviews, respondents had a common un- variables. First, Bangkok and three other provinces
derstanding of its meaning, so we used this term in were selected randomly, as were districts within
the second survey. these provinces. Second, officers at District Health
The cognitive interviews also resulted in important Offices selected subdistricts and health centers
insights related to the response set for the psycholog- within these subdistricts. Third, villages were

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/gerontologist/article/44/5/596/798683 by UNIVERSITI MALAYSIA SARAWAK user on 19 October 2023


ical well-being items. Though this response set selected randomly from the health centers. Fourth,
(ranging from strongly disagree to strongly agree) potential research participants were selected ran-
is commonly used in Western scales, our respondents domly from each of the villages.
were confused by the options. Team members This sampling strategy resulted in a total of 460
observed that interviewees spontaneously responded respondents (177 men and 283 women) with a mean
with ‘‘that’s true’’ or ‘‘that’s not true’’ while age of 70.89 years. On average, the respondents had
answering the items about psychological well-being. completed 6.78 years of school and rated their health
Therefore, in subsequent cognitive interviews, we as fair to good. In addition, to allow for test–retest
used a response set that incorporated different degrees reliability of our psychological well-being instrument,
of truth (ranging from not at all true to very true), we had the residents who lived in Bangkok reinter-
which was more easily understood by the respon- viewed 3 weeks later. This subsample of 70 respon-
dents. When asked to explain the meaning of the dents (27 men and 43 women) had an average age of
alternative response sets, cognitive interviewees ex- 70.53 years, had completed an average of 6.30 years
plained that response choices involving agreement of school, and rated their health as fair to good.
versus disagreement made them think about how Several indicators of well-being were included in
their life should be. In contrast, responses concerning this second survey. One included items measuring
truth versus nontruth evoked reactions to their own each of the five domains of well-being identified
current situation rather than normative concerns during the first phase of our research: harmony (7
about what should or should not occur. This items), interdependence (7 items), acceptance (6
explanation helped account for the lack of variability items), respect (7 items), and enjoyment (8 items).
in the psychological well-being items used in the Another indicator was a 1-item measure of life
preliminary survey. That is, because our original satisfaction (i.e., ‘‘Overall, how satisfied are you
response set involved agreeing versus disagreeing with with your life now?’’), with a high score indicating
items, survey participants may have responded in greater satisfaction. A third indicator was the
relation to how they felt their lives should be, resulting Geriatric Depression Scale (Sheikh & Yesavage,
in inflated estimates of psychological well-being. 1986), which resulted in a sum of 15 items such that
The cognitive interviews also helped us revise our a high score represented greater depression.
interviewing methods. Several of our interviewees Our analyses of the data included a confirmatory
provided valuable suggestions for how to enhance factor analysis, in which we used AMOS 4.01
privacy when family members and friends were (Arbuckle, 1994) to determine the optimal indicators
present during the interview. These suggestions of our Thai measure of psychological well-being and
included direct strategies (e.g., asking the family to identify the measurement model that best fit the
members for permission to interview the elderly data. We then tested the items that emerged from
person alone) as well as indirect strategies (e.g., these analyses for reliability and validity.
moving to portions of the interview that were not
related to interpersonal relationships when others
were present and shifting back to these topics when Results
they left).
We present the findings from the survey in two
parts. The first section summarizes findings from the
confirmatory factor analysis; the second describes
Quantitative Methods: Second Survey the psychometric properties of the resulting well-
being factors.
Insights from qualitative methods can provide
direction for further quantitative research (Morgan,
1998). In our case, the cognitive interviews led to Confirmatory Factor Analysis
significant changes in both the content of the
psychological well-being measures (i.e., the wording We used a confirmatory factor analysis to trim the
of items and the response set) as well as the process number of items for the five dimensions of well-
of interviewing (i.e., obtaining privacy). being. We used two criteria: (a) the items adequately
We incorporated these revisions of the psycho- measured the content domain for each dimension, as
logical well-being measure and the new interviewing described by participants in the first phase of
techniques into a second survey. This survey of older research; and (b) the items had acceptable loadings

600 The Gerontologist


Table 1. Item Factor Loadings From the Confirmatory
Factor Analysis

Item Factor Loading


Harmony
1. In your extended family, people get .76
along well together.
2. Members of your extended family .75

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/gerontologist/article/44/5/596/798683 by UNIVERSITI MALAYSIA SARAWAK user on 19 October 2023


care about each other.
3. In your neighborhood, people are .45
friendly to each other.
Interdependence
1. In your extended family, people can .74
depend on each other for help.
2. People in your extended family .68
take care of you.
3. Neighbors depend on each other. .47
Respect
1. Younger members of your extended .67
family or other young people obey you.
2. Younger members of your extended .58
family or other young people talk
and behave politely toward you.
3. Younger members of your extended .64
family or other young people treat
you with respect.
Acceptance
1. You are a relaxed person and are not .44
easily worried.
2. When you have small problems, you .89
can let go of your worries. Figure 1. Alternative factor structure models of psychological
3. When something bad happens to you, .58 well-being.
you can accept it.
Enjoyment
1. You have a good sense of humor. .66 by the significant chi-square statistic for all three
2. You laugh easily. .77
3. You have good times with other people. .65
models, a comparison of the fit indices allows us to
determine which of these models represents the best
fit. According to Hu and Bentler (1999), a good model
fit is indicated by comparative fit index values of
(i.e., greater than .40) on their respective dimensions greater than .95 and root mean square error of
of well-being. The confirmatory factor analyses approximation values of less than .06.
resulted in three items with acceptable loadings The fit indices for our initial model (conceptual-
(ranging from .44 to .89) for each of the five well- izing psychological well-being as a five-factor model
being dimensions. (See Table 1 for these items and in which each of the dimensions was distinct) are not
their respective factor loadings.) adequate. The fit indices for the second model
To address the underlying factor structure of the (conceptualizing psychological well-being as a sec-
Thai psychological well-being measure, we compared ond-order global factor with five dimensions of well-
our initial five-factor model with two alternative being) indicate a good fit. Similarly, the fit indices for
models (see Figure 1): a second-order global factor the third model (conceptualizing psychological well-
model to parallel the one specified by Ryff and Keyes being as two second-order factors consisting of
(1995), and a second-order two-factor model that interpersonal and intrapersonal well-being) also
distinguished between interpersonal and intraper- indicate a good fit. A comparison of the global
sonal well-being. The statistics we used to compare model (Model 2) and the two-factor model (Model
these three models included the following: chi-square 3) results in a chi-square difference of 10.43, df = 1,
statistics, a goodness-of-fit index, an adjusted good- p  .01, indicating that the two-factor model is
ness-of-fit index, a comparative fit index, and a root superior. Further support for the superiority of this
mean square error of approximation. Table 2 model is shown in Figure 1. Here we see that the
presents a comparison of these models based on all standardized loadings for the two-factor model are
respondents with nonmissing data. We did not adequate for all the scales as compared with those
correlate the item errors in these models. Although for the global factor model, which are particularly
none of the models represents an ideal fit, as indicated low for two of the scales (i.e., acceptance and

Vol. 44, No. 5, 2004 601


Table 2. Fit Statistics for Three Models

Model v2 df p GFI AGFI TLI CFI RMSEA


1. Five factors 603.62 90 ,.001 .82 .76 .62 .68 .12
2. Second-order, global factor 187.18 85 ,.001 .95 .92 .92 .94 .05
3. Second-order, two factor 176.75 84 ,.001 .95 .93 .93 .94 .05
Notes: For the table, n = 425. GFI = goodness-of-fit index; AFGI = adjusted goodness-of-fit index; TLI = Tucker–Lewis
index; CFI = comparative fit index; RMSEA = root mean square error of approximation.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/gerontologist/article/44/5/596/798683 by UNIVERSITI MALAYSIA SARAWAK user on 19 October 2023


enjoyment). Taken together, these findings suggest developing culturally relevant measures rather than
that, among our elderly Thai respondents, psycho- importing standardized instruments from the West.
logical well-being may have two distinct compo- As suggested by others (Christopher, 1999; Ho,
nents: interpersonal and intrapersonal. 1998), reliance on Western conceptual frameworks
and measurements when one is conducting research
in Asia may result in incomplete and distorted
Psychometrics understandings.
A major contribution of the present study is our
On the basis of the results of the confirmatory
use of a bottom-up approach in developing a cultur-
factor analysis, we created an interpersonal well-
ally sensitive measure of psychological well-being.
being index (a mean of the harmony, interdepen-
By asking elderly Thai people about the ingredients
dence, and respect subscales) and an intrapersonal
of maturity, fulfillment, and adjustment, we could
well-being index (a mean of the acceptance and
identify the components of well-being that were
enjoyment subscales). The average score is 3.31 on the
meaningful to them. The multistage process de-
interpersonal index (possible range, 1–4; SD = .52)
scribed by Krause (2002) provided a useful frame-
and 3.24 on the intrapersonal index (possible range,
work for moving from qualitative to quantitative
1–4; SD = .54), indicating that responses are skewed
data-gathering methods. We needed to adapt his
toward more positive well-being on both scales.
framework when, as a result of time limitations, we
Cronbach’s alpha revealed that there is adequate
were unable to conduct cognitive interviews during
internal consistency for the interpersonal well-being
the first phase of qualitative data gathering. Seren-
index (a = .82) as well as the intrapersonal well-being
dipitously, eliminating this step from the earlier
index (a = .69). Further, the 3-week test–retest
phases of measurement development gave us the
reliability was acceptable for the intrapersonal well-
opportunity to determine its importance. Without
being scale (r = .68) but somewhat lower for the
the crucial insights provided by cognitive interview-
interpersonal well-being scale (r = .59).
ing, our initial measure of psychological well-being
To determine the validity of our measures, we
was inadequate. Fortunately, we were able to rectify
correlated the Thai psychological well-being scales
the situation by applying a strategy suggested by
with the other measures of well-being. As an
Morgan (1998) in which qualitative approaches are
indication of convergent validity, the interpersonal
used to follow up on perplexing quantitative
well-being scale and the intrapersonal well-being
findings. Ultimately, the cognitive interviews resulted
scale are both positively correlated with the life
in some of our richest information. Working
satisfaction item (r = .31, p  .001 and r = .27, p 
alongside the interviewers, our older respondents
.001, respectively) and negatively correlated with the
provided critiques of our initial items and suggested
Geriatric Depression Scale (r = .38, p  .001 and
improvements. Furthermore, we learned that the
r = .39, p  .001, respectively).
response choices that we had imported from
standardized Western measures (i.e., strongly dis-
Discussion agree to strongly agree) were not authentic and
natural for our interviewees.
In this research project, we used the indigenous This project also highlights the influence of
expertise of Thai elders to develop a measure of interview context on obtaining valid data in other
psychological well-being. We discovered that their cultural settings. In Thailand, as in many Asian
conceptualization of well-being (e.g., acceptance, countries, older people generally live with their
interdependence, and harmonious relationships) family members, usually their children (Chayovan &
differed distinctly from components of well-being Knodel, 1997). In addition, their homes are often
identified by Western researchers. Our findings very close to one another, especially in rural areas.
bolster the work of cross-cultural psychologists Thus family, friends, and neighbors are frequently
who question ‘‘the sovereignty of the American view present during an interview. Because the psycholog-
of the mature person as autonomous, self-deter- ical well-being items included questions about
mined, and unencumbered’’ (Markus & Kitayama, relationships with family and neighbors, interviewers
1991, p. 228). Our efforts highlight the importance of were directed to interview older people in private, if

602 The Gerontologist


at all possible. To do so required considerable community, most older Thais experience high well-
ingenuity and creativity on the part of the inter- being. This alternative explanation is bolstered by
viewers, who often used multiple strategies to obtain Ryff and Keyes (1995), who observed that, for
privacy. One strategy was for the interviewers to a community-based sample of Americans, their
move closer physically and lower their voice. A measure of well-being was similarly skewed. Future
second was to show the older interviewee the research should use our measure of psychological
response choices and encourage him or her to point well-being with more vulnerable subgroups of Thai
to an answer rather than say the answer aloud. elders (such as those who are very frail or living in

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/gerontologist/article/44/5/596/798683 by UNIVERSITI MALAYSIA SARAWAK user on 19 October 2023


Another method was for the interviewers to suggest institutions) to determine whether the measure is
that they find a quieter place to conduct the sensitive to such differences.
interview. Despite their efforts, interviewers were Another limitation is that this measure of well-
not always successful at gaining private interviews. being has been applied only to Thai elders. This
Because socially desirable responses increase with measure should be used in cross-cultural research
age and vary across cultures (Ross & Mirowsky, conducted in other Asian and non-Asian countries.
1984), it is particularly important that, as part of the Such efforts will help determine whether the
bottom-up approach to interviewing, cross-cultural components of psychological well-being identified
researchers seek to identify interviewing techniques by older Thais are as prevalent in other Asian
that enhance privacy. countries as compared with non-Asian countries.
The present research represents a step toward Further, it would be interesting to conduct a multi-
understanding cultural variability in the nature of group confirmatory factor analysis on our measure
psychological well-being. Whereas previous re- of psychological well-being to more directly compare
search, based on American adults (Ryff & Keyes, the single versus dual aspects of psychological well-
1995), has conceptualized psychological well-being being for American versus Thai elders.
as a single global factor that is predominantly Despite its limitations, this work enhances our
intrapersonal (e.g., self-acceptance, autonomy, and understanding of cross-cultural variation by focusing
personal growth), our research reveals a different on psychological well-being as one indicator of aging
structure for older Thai people. For them, psycho- successfully. Building on the work of Ryff (1989a,
logical well-being appears to have two facets: 1999), who has identified dimensions of psycholog-
intrapersonal and interpersonal. This dual nature ical well-being among older Americans, we have
of well-being may be linked to Thais’ belief in mapped the components of psychological well-being
Theravada Buddhism (Limanonda, 1995). According among Thai elders. Taken together, such efforts will
to Buddhist beliefs about karma, doing good results allow us to develop a framework for understanding
in merit, which in turn influences quality of life in which ingredients of successful aging are universal or
current and future incarnations (Muecke, 1992). For unique to specific cultures.
many Thais, merit making represents a blending of
autonomous and interpersonal processes. For exam-
ple, Ingersoll (1975) found that, by giving gifts or
References
services to the local Buddhist priests, Thai villagers
Arbuckle, J. L. (1994). AMOS (Version 4.01) [Computer software]. Chicago:
believed that they made merit for themselves as well Small Waters Corporation.
as for their families and communities. Ingersoll Baltes, M. M., & Carstensen, L. L. (1996). The process of successful aging.
observed that, ‘‘although individuals acquire and Aging and Society, 16, 397–422.
Biemer, P. P., Groves, R. M., Lyberg, L. E., Mathiowetz, N. A., & Sudman,
possess merit and receive its consequences, they do S. (1991). Measurement error in surveys. New York: Wiley.
so almost entirely in association with other people’’ Carter-Edwards, L., Bynoe, M., & Svetkey, L. (1998). Knowledge of diet and
(1975, p. 225). Our work suggests that this duality blood pressure among African Americans: Use of focus groups for
questionnaire development. Ethnicity and Disease, 8, 184–197.
extends beyond merit making such that, for older Chayovan, N., & Knodel, J. (1997). A report on the survey of the welfare of
Thai people, the nature of psychological well-being the elderly in Thailand. Bangkok, Thailand: Chulalongkorn University,
is both intrapersonal and interpersonal. Institute of Population Studies.
Christopher, J. C. (1999). Situating psychological well-being: Exploring the
Using a combination of qualitative and quantita- cultural roots of its theory and research. Journal of Counseling and
tive methods, we developed a measure of psycho- Development, 77, 141–152.
Erikson, E. (1959). Identity and the life cycle. Psychological Issues, 1, 18–164.
logical well-being and explored its psychometric Fiske, A. P., Kitayama, S., Markus, H., & Nisbett, R. E. (1998). The cultural
properties. Although the validity and reliability of matrix of social psychology. In. D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey
this measure of Thai psychological well-being is (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (pp. 915–981). Boston: McGraw-
Hill.
adequate, we must acknowledge its limitations. Our Genest, M., & Turk, D. C. (1981). Think-aloud approaches to cognitive
measure is positively skewed; that is, most of the assessment. In T. V. Merluzzi, C. R. Glass, & M. Genest (Eds.),
older respondents had high psychological well-being Cognitive assessment (pp. 233–269). New York: Guilford Press.
Greenfield, P. M. (1994). Independence and interdependence as developmen-
scores. This skewness may indicate a problem with tal scripts: Implications for theory, research, and practice. In P. M.
the measure suggesting that further research is Greenfield & R. R. Cocking (Eds.), Cross-cultural roots of minority
needed to construct items and response sets that child development (pp. 1–37). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Ho, D. Y. (1998). Indigenous psychologies: Asian perspectives. Journal of
result in greater measurement variability. Alterna- Cross-Cultural Psychology, 29, 88–103.
tively, it may be that, among those living in the Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance

Vol. 44, No. 5, 2004 603


structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Oksenberg, L., Cannell, C., & Kalton, G. (1991). New strategies for
Structural Equation Modeling, 6(1), 1–55. pretesting survey questions. Journal of Official Statistics, 7(3), 350–365.
Ingersoll, J. (1975). Merit and identity in village Thailand. In G. W. Skinner Oyserman, D., Coon, H. M., & Kemmelmeier, M. (2002). Rethinking
& A. T. Kirsch (Eds.), Change and persistence in Thai society (pp. 219– individualism and collectivism: Evaluation of theoretical assumptions and
251). Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press. meta-analyses. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 3–72.
Ingersoll, J. (1985, April). Are they better off? Social well-being as Rogers, C. (1961). On becoming a person. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
a framework for judging development results. Paper presented at the Ross, C. E., & Mirowksy, J. (1984). Socially desirable responses and
meeting of the Society for Economic Anthropology, Warrenton, VA. acquiescence in a cross-cultural survey of mental health. Journal of
Ingersoll-Dayton, B., Saengtienchai, C., Kespichayawattana, J., & Aungsur- Health and Social Behavior, 25, 189–197.
och, Y. (2001). Psychological well-being Asian style: The perspective of Rowe, J. W., & Kahn, R. L. (1998). Successful aging. New York: Pantheon.
Thai elders. Journal of Cross-Cultural Gerontology, 16, 283–302. Ryff, C. D. (1989a). Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/gerontologist/article/44/5/596/798683 by UNIVERSITI MALAYSIA SARAWAK user on 19 October 2023


Jung, C. (1933). Modern man in search of a soul (W. W. Dell & C. F. meaning of psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social
Baynes, Trans.). New York: Harcourt, Brace, & World. Psychology, 57, 1069–1081.
Kim, U., Park, Y. S., & Park, D. (2000). The challenge of cross-cultural Ryff, C. D. (1989b). In the eye of the beholder: Views of psychological well-
psychology: The role of the indigenous psychologies. Journal of Cross- being among middle-aged and older adults. Psychology and Aging, 4,
Cultural Psychology, 31, 63–75. 195–210.
Kitayama, S., Markus, H., Matsumoto, H., & Norasakkunkit, V. (1997). Ryff, C. D. (1999). Psychology and aging. In W. R. Hazzard, J. P. Blass, W.
Individual and collective processes in the construction of the self: Self- H. Ettinger, J. B. Halter, & J. G. Ouslander (Eds.), Principles of geriatric
enhancement in the United States and self-criticism in Japan. Journal of medicine and gerontology (4th ed., pp. 159–169). New York: McGraw-
Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 1245–1267. Hill.
Krause, N. (2002). A comprehensive strategy for developing closed-ended Ryff, C. D., & Keyes, C. L. (1995). The structure of psychological well-
survey items for use in studies of older adults. Journal of Gerontology: being revisited. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69,
Social Sciences, 57B, S263–S274. 719–727.
Kumagai, H. A. (1981). A dissection of intimacy: A study of ‘‘bipolar Schwartz, N. (1999). Self-reports: How the questions shape the answers.
posturing’’ in Japanese social interaction—amaeru and amayakasu, in- American Psychologist, 54, 93–105.
dulgence and deference. Culture, Medicine, and Psychiatry, 5, 249–272. Seidel, J., Friese, S., & Leonard, D. C. (1995). The enthnograph v4.0: A
Lewis, C. C. (1995). Educating hearts and minds. New York: Cambridge user’s guide. Amherst, MA: Qualis Research Associates.
University Press. Sheikh, J. I., & Yesavage, J. A. (1986). The geriatric depression scale (GDS):
Limanonda, B. (1995). Families in Thailand: Beliefs and realities. Journal of Recent evidence and development of a shorter version. In T. L. Brink
Comparative Family Studies, 26(1), 67–82. (Ed.), Clinical gerontology: A guide to assessment and intervention (pp.
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and self: Implications for 165–173). New York: Haworth Press.
cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224–253. Strawbridge, W. J., Cohen, R. D., Shema, S. J., & Kaplan, G. K. (1996).
Maslow, A. (1968). Toward a psychology of being (2nd ed.). New York: Van Successful aging: Predictors and associated activities. American Journal
Nostrand. of Epidemiology, 144, 135–141.
Miles, M. B., & Huberman, A. M. (1994). Qualitative data analysis. Sudman, S., Bradburn, N. M., & Schwartz, N. (1996). Thinking about
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. answers: The application of cognitive processes to survey methodology.
Morgan, D. L. (1998). Practical strategies for combining qualitative and San Francisco: Jossey Bass.
quantitative methods: Applications to health research. Qualitative Triandis, H. C. (1989). The self and social behavior in differing cultural
Health Research, 8, 362–376. contexts. Psychological Review, 96, 506–520.
Muecke, M. (1992). Mother sold food, daughter sells her body: The cultural Weisz, J. R. (1989). Culture and the development of child psychopathology:
continuity of prostitution. Social Science and Medicine, 35, 891–901. Lessons from Thailand. In D. Cicchetti (Ed.), The emergence of
Neugarten, B. L. (1968). The awareness of middle age. In B. L. Neugarten a discipline. Rochester symposium on developmental psychopathology
(Ed.), Middle age and aging (pp. 93–98). Chicago: University of Chicago (pp. 89–117). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Press.
Oerter, R., Oerter, R., Agostiani, H., Kim, H. O., & Wibowo, S. (1996). The Received November 26, 2003
concept of human nature in East Asia: Etic and emic characteristics. Accepted May 7, 2004
Culture and psychology 2, 9–51. Decision Editor: Linda S. Noelker, PhD

604 The Gerontologist

You might also like