Guralnick Neville-Designing Early Intervention TheEffectiveness-1997

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 32

In M. J. Gurlanick (Ed.), The Effectiveness of Early Intervention. Baltimore, MD: Brookes (1997).

Designing Early Intervention


Programs to Promote Children's
23 Social Competence
Michael J. Guralnick and Brian Neville

SOCIAL COMPETENCE has emerged as a cen- one's peers (Guralnick, 1986; Howes, 1988).
tral organizing construct in the study of In view of the now well-recognized signifi-
human development (Guralnick, 1986, cance of this construct to human develop-
1990a; Hartup, 1983; Sroufe, 1983). Consen- ment, it comes as little surprise that chil-
sus as co the complete meaning of the term dren's social competence during the early
social competence remains elusive, yet there is years has become a prominent issue for both
general agreement chat this construct cap- researchers and clinicians.
tures how individuals define and solve the Appeals co the field of early intervention
most fundamental problems in human rela- co consider social competence as a valued
tionships. Essential challenges include the outcome during the first generation of re-
ability co initiate and sustain interactions with search went largely unheeded (Guralnick,
others. co resolve conflicts, co build friend- 1988, 1989, l990b; Taft, 1983; Zigler & Trick-
ships, and to achieve related interpersonal ett, 19i8). In many ways. chis reluctance was
goals. Agreement can also be found in the understandable. During that period. the con··
recognition chat social competence is a dy- strucc of social competence was still mired in
namic and higher order construct in which definitional .problems, and its measurement
skills and abilities categorized within the tra- was correspondingly unclear (Bailey & Wol-
ditional domains of cognitive, communica- ery. 1989; Guralnick & Weinhouse, 1983).
tive, affective, and motor development are This was particularly the case in comparison
integrated in the service of specific interper- co the generally agreed on and standardized
sonal goals. !\iforeover, social competence is a assessments offered for the primary domains
developmental construct, with distinctive be- of cognition, communication, motor, and af-
havioral patterns emerging co meet adaptive fective development. With rare exceptions, it
demands associated with each developmen- was these well-developed, domain-specific
tal period. In particular, enthusiasm, respon- assessments that formed the basis for evalu-
sivicy, persistence, sensitivity, and flexibility ating the effectiveness of early intervention
constitute primary organizing characteristics for children at risk and those with estal>
chat guide adaptations of existing develop- lished disabilities (Casto & Mastropieri, 1986;
mental skills and abilities to establish socially Guralnick & Bennett, 1987). Moreover, no
competent exchanges with adults during the coherent developmental framework related
first 2-3 years of life (Spiker, Ferguson, & co social competence had yet been formu-
Brooks-Gunn, 1993; Sroufe, 1983). These lated. What were the relevant psychological
characteristics then form the foundation for processes involve& How did families influ-
the development of complex processes and ence a child's developing social competence?
be havioraJ patterns that address the social In what ways did a child's unique biological
tasks associated with building relationships characcerislics contribute co this process?
during the preschool years, especially with How did competence with farn·ity members

579
580 GURA LN IC K A N D NEVILLE

or other adults relate to competence with pressly to e\'aluate social competence in the
peers? In the absence of a reasonable frame- context of early intervention programs. For
work to address these and related questions, example, Beckman and Lieber (1994) have
first-generation research in early interven- described the Social Strategy Rating Scale
tion found little incentive to develop, imple- intended for young children with disabili-
ment, and evaluate programs related to chil- ties. This scale requires professionals to rate
dren's social competence. the frequency and appropriateness of 25 so-
cial activities judged to reflect a child's over-
all social competence. For children at risk.
EMERGING INTEREST
Hogan, Scott, and Bauer (1992) have devel-
IN SOCIAL COMPETENCE
oped an Adaptive Social Behavior Inventory
Fortunately, within the 1990s many of these consisting of three scales relevant to social
concerns have started co be addressed, and competence. In contrast, Spiker et al. (1993)
arguments to include social competence as have taken a more focused approach to as-
part of early intervention programs have in- sessing social competence for populations of
tensified (Guralnick, 1990b). Specifically, children at risk, t~pping the important af·
considerable progress has been achieved in fective dimensions of enthusiasm and persis-
arriving at useful definitions of the construct tence when children are interacting with their
of social competence, recognizing the im- mothers. For more general populations of
portance of both information-processing preschool-age children, Guralnick (1992a,
(Dodge. Peuit, McClaskey, & Brown, 1986; 1992b) developed·an Assessment of Peer Re-
Rubin & Krasnor, 1986) and emotional lations. This instrument is organized in terms
( Dodge, 1991; Cottman, 1986) components of the social tasks of entry into peer groups.
of social competence, as well as the interac- resolving conflicts, and maintaining play. So-
tions occurring between the two (Asher, cial strategies children use to accomplish
1983; Dodge, 1991; Guralnick, 1992a; their interpersonal goals (social tasks) are as-
Howes, 1988; Odom, McConnell, & McEvoy, sessed, as are the contributions of social-
1992; Rubin & Coplan, 1992; Strain , Gural- cognitive, shared understanding, emotional
nick, & Walker, 1986). Correspondingly, a regulation, and higher order processes. Al-
developmental framework has emerged chat though only a clinical instrument. at this time,
has created a new and exciting understand- the development of a more formal research
ing of the factors governing the development ·version is underway. However, the Preschool
of children's social competence in general Socioaffective Profile (Lafreniere, Dumas,
(Sroufe, 1983) and peer-related social com- Capuano, & Dubeau, 1992) is a rating scale
petence in particular (Guralnick, 1986; Rubin that-in 1996-does have research applica·
& Coplan, 1992). The relationship between tion. It is composed of eight scales, including
family and peer systems, including the con- a broad-band measure of social competence.
tributions o f child characteristics, .has been Factors assessing emotional aspects of anger-
especially instructive (see Parke & Ladd, aggression and anxiety-withdrawal are also
1992, for a review), as has the role of moder- part of this profile. A more abbreviated but
ating factors such as social supports (e.g.. Pi- nevertheless useful scale has been developed
a nta & Ball, 1993). Taken together, this new . by Dodge, McClaskey, and Feldman (1985).
framework has pro\'ided clear directions for This scale is a checklist that yields composite
the design of early intervention programs. scores for aggressiveness and social skills with
Of equal importance, this approach fits com· peers and has been applied effectively to chil-
fon ably within second-generation research dren at risk as a result of stressful home en-
in the f ield of early intervention (Guralnick, vironments (Pettit, Dodge, & Brown, 1988).
1988. 1993b). Increased interest in social competence on
Supporting these developments has been the part of early interventionists also has re-
the e mergence of assessments designed ex- sulted from a growing awareness that inter-
P Ro;vioTt:'-iG CHILDRE:-.;'s Socl..\L Co~1PETE:\:CE 381
- -- - - - -- - - - -
\·emions intended to achieve positive effects ( Bredekamp. 1987). Similarly. promoting an
for o ne or more of the traditional develop- indi\'idual's soc ial competence, particular!\·
mental domains are likely to have o nly lim- when conceptualized as the ability to earn·
ited impact on the domain of social compe- out one's interpersonal goals. is now seen as
te nce. For example, although cognitive essential fo r maximizing independence for
competence is a n important correlate of those children requiring early intenention
young children 's social competence ( Wright, services (G uralnick, 1993a).
1980), within typically developing, at-risk. or Compleme nting the emphasis on social
established disability groups. only modest re- competence in relation to independence is
lationships exist between social and cogni- the press to include or integrate children
tive competence (Guralnick & Croom. 1985; with and without disabilities in school and
Putallaz, 1983; Quay &Jarrett, 1984; Rubin & community settings. This has res ul ted in at-
Krasnor. 1986). Even levels of language de- te ntion being focused on the social aspects
velopment yield surprisingly small associa- of development (Guralnick, 1990a, 1990b), a
tions with social compe tence for young chil- view suppo rted by the majority of parents of
dren despite their apparent importance in children in general (Guralnick, Connor, &
advanced forms of social pretend play (Gu- Hammond. 1995: Quirk, Sexton, Ciottone,
ralnick, Connor, Hammond, Cottman, & Kin- Minarni. & Wapner. 1984). As a consequence,
nish. in press: Guralnick & Croom, 1985). an emphasis on social competence stands as
These findings reflect the dynamic and inte· a major agenda item for young children and
grative nature of social competence as well their families and serves. in many respects. as
as the ability of many young children to com- a developmental theme unifying the fields of
pensate for difficulties associated with o ne general early childhood development and
or more of the fundamental developmental early intervention (Guralnick, 1993b, 1994).
domains. Interestingly. it appears that im- Finally, a sense of urgency in the field of
provements in peer-related social compe- early intervention has now developed as an
tence may actually encourage advances in awareness of the scope and magn itude of dif-
children's cognitive and communicative de- ficulties in soc ial competence has become
velopment as well as other aspects of proser apparent for both children considered to be
cial behavior (Bates, 1975; Carvey. 1986; at risk and those with established disabilities.
Hartup. 1983; Howes, 1988: Rubin & Lollis, For example. problems in the domain of scr
1988). Clearly. then, if improved social com- cial competence have been well documented
petence is to be a goal of early intervention, for low-birch-weight/ premature children as-
programs must be designed to address this sessed during coddler (Landry. Chapieski,
higher order and integrative domain specif- Richardson , Palmer, & Hall, 1990) and school
ically. age (Ross. Lipper, & Auld, 1990) periods.
From a broader framework , the disability Similar proble ms in peer-related social com-
field has, since the 1980s, emphasized the petence have been found to affect a disprcr
concepts of independence and inclusion as portionately large number of children at risk
primary goals of intervention programs and because of stressful environmental circum-
the design of support systems in general (Tay- stances (e.g., Booth, Rose-Krasnor. & Rubin,
lor. 1988). The emphasis on independence in 1991 ). Moreover, numerous studies have now
particular extends to the early years, with scr established the unusual and pervasive diffi-
cial competence seen as a central mecha- culties exhibited by young children wi th doc-
nism fostering this goal (Guralnick, 1990a ). umented disabilities in their efforts to estab-
In fact, in the field of general early child- lish relationships with their peers and to
hood education, social competence ser ves as develop friendships (see Guralnick. 1990a).
an important means for promming indepen- Considerable attention has been given to the
dence, a lo ng-established priority associated peer interaction difficulties of young chil-
with developmentally appropriate practice dren with general (cognitive) delays, because
582 GL:RAL '.'J ICK A N D NEVILLE

problems extend well be~·ond those expected ti o n programs has the potential co substan-
based on the child's overall developmental tially ad\'ance the process of second-genera-
level (Field, 1980; Guralnick & Groom, 1985, ti o n research (Guralnick, 1988, 1993a).
1987, 1988; Guralnick & Weinhouse, 1984; Accordingly, a major portion of this chap-
Lieber, Beckman, & Strong, 1993). Yee, simi- ter considers the role families play in foster-
lar difficulties have been reported for young ing che development of children's peer rela-
children with ocher disabilities (Odom et·al., tions. The now extensive research linking
1992), including children with communica- family and peer systems in the general popu-
tion disorders (Guralnick et al., in press; lation has successfully identified interaction
Rice, Sell, & Hadley, 1991), hearing loss patterns chat appear co have substantial in-
(Higgenbotham & Baker, 1981; McKirdy & fluences on the development of children's
Blank, 1982; Vandell & George, 1981), and vi- peer-related social competence and is con-
sual impairments (Erwin, 1994; Markovits & sidered in detail. The pathways through
Strayer, 1982). It should also be noted chat ev- which family influences are transmitted, the
idence continues to suggest that peer rela- influence of specific child characteristics on
tionship difficulties observed during early this process, and the moderating role of so-
childhood are predictive of later adjustment c ial support are of special interest. These di~
problems for children who are otherwise de- cussions are followed by analyses of family in-
veloping typically (Parker & Asher, 1987). Al- fluences in relation to children in high-risk
though no long-term studies of a similar na- groups as well as those with established di~
ture have been carried out for children at abilities. Considered at this point is the ex-
risk or for chose with established disabilities, tent to which family patterns and circum-
there is every reason to believe that similar stances associated with children at high risk
outcomes would result. Consequently, prior- or with established. disabilities are likely to
ity given to issues related to social compe- create conditions in which children become
tence in early intervention programs may more vulnerable to difficulties in social com-
well yield significant long-term benefits. petence. An additional section provides sug-
gestions for general intervention approaches
and specific strategies in the context of early
PURPOSE AND
intervention programs in relation co family
ORGANIZATION OF THIS CHAPTER
influences. A brief section focusing on inter-
The overall purpose of this chapter is to in- ventions occurring in the context of pre-
tegrate theoretical and empirical work in. the school programs also is included, followed
domain of social competence in order to by concluding comments that address the
establish a framework and direction for com- design of comprehensive early intervention
prehensive early intervention programs de- programs within a social competence frame-
signed to promote children's social compe- work.
tence. An emphasis is placed on family
influences, especially the relationship be-
FAMILY INFLUENCES
tween family factors and social competence
with peers. Because interest in this area is a Research and theoretical advances have doc-
primarily 1990s phenomenon, this chapter is umented the intricate linkages that exist be-
not able to provide a review of the effective- tween family and peer relationships in che
ness of early intervention programs focusing general population (Ladd, 1991; Parke &
on children's social competence, although Ladd, 1992). Four aspects offamily influence
relevant research that has been conducted is that can be readily incorporated into an early
noted. This focus on social competence intervention framework and appear co have
as an outcome of early intervention and its strong associations with children's peer-
implications for the design of early incerven- related social competence are as follows:
PR0MoT1:--;c CHILDRE N'S Soc l.-".L C o :-.1rETE'.'C E 583

l. Earlv caregi\·er-child relationships separation-reunion episodes are obsen·ed


2. Parent-child interactions and coded. The infant's reactions to che re-
3. The child's peer social network union are placed into four broad categories
4. Parental attitudes and beli.efs regard- (which can be further subdi\·ided): insecure-
ing peer relatio~ships. avoidant, secure, insecure-resistant, and
Separate linkages between peer-related so- insecure-disorganized. Secure babies show lit-
cial competence and each of these influences tle anxiety prior to separation. are distressed
can be identified and are discussed in this by the separation, and seek comfort from
section. It is important to emphasize, how- mocher on reunion. Insecure-avoidant babies
ever, as expected from family systems theory rarely cry on separation and avoid their moth-
(see Krauss & Jacobs, 1990) and ecological ers on reunion. Insecure-resistant babies tend
models of development ( Bronfenbrenner, to show preseparation anxiety, are highly dis-
1979), that these influences are all interre- tressed by separa~ion, and seek close contact
lated. Where appropriate, interrelationships on reunion while also resisting the contact
and developmental continuities are dis- (Ainsworth, 1979). The insecure-disorga-
cussed. In addition, although the direction nized babies display a combination of both
of influence is thought to flow from families avoidance and resistance (~fain & Solomon,
to children when accounting for differences 1986). In studies linking attachment and so-
in peer-related social competence, a plausi- cial competence, only the broad categories of
ble argument also can be made that endoge- secure versus insecure are typically employed.
nous factors governing children's behavior Following this approach, quality of auach-
or levels of competence influence the nature ment has indeed been shown to be associ-
of family patterns. There is no doubt that ated with measures. of peer social compe-
. such reciprocal relationships exist, but fam- tence. For example, Pastor (1981) examined
ily adaptations to the characteristics of their 18-month-olds' responsiveness to other tod-
children and other prevailing conditions dlers and found the securely attached tod-
remain important sources of influence on dlers to be more sociable and positively ori-
children's social competence (laFrenier:-e & ented toward other toddlers. The work of
Dumas, 1992; Rubin & Lollis, 1988). Sroufe and colleagues has extended the gen-
eral attadiment-social competence linkage
Early Caregiver-Child Relationships to preschool settings. In an early study. in-
Bowlby (1980) proposed that the adaptation fant a.t tachment status was significantly asso-
of the individual is a function of both cur- ciated with global social competence ratings
rent circumstances and early caregiver-child in a 3'/?-year-old middle-class, preschool
relationships. A number of studies have sample (Waters, Wippman, & Sroufe, 1979).
tested the importance of early atta"chment These findings were replicated in a high-risk
on peer social competence at a variety of sample of 4-year-olds in the laboratory
age points. Because attachment is a molar preschool at the University of Minnesota
construct in which the measurement para- (Lafreniere & Sroufe, 1985; Sroufe, 1983).
digm was designed to be an indicator of the Similarly, teachers have been asked to rate
global affective bond between child and the behavior of children who had been pre-
caregiver, it can provide a good general test viously rated for attachment quality. Com-
of the linkage between the family and peer pared with insecurely attached children, se-
social systems. curely attached children are rated by teachers
Attachment ratings are typically made as having fewer behavior problems (Cohn,
using the Strange Situation paradigm (Ains- 1990; Erickson, Sroufe, & Egeland, 1985)
worth, Blehar, Waters, & Wail, 1978). In this and as being more sociaJly competent and
system, 1-year-old children are separated emotionally healthy (Arend, Gove, & Sroufe,
from their mothers, and their responses to 1979; Cohn, 1990; Sroufe, 1983).
58-! GL"RAL:-.:ICK A:--:D \IEVILLE

Like auachment. soc ial competence is a day camp a nd spent a g reater po rtion of
molar construct composed of numerous com- their t ime wi th peers than did those with in-
ponent heha,·iors. Such social beha,·iors secure histories. When asked about the ir
have also been examined. pro,·iding further friendships. 10-year-olds in a German ~ample
rnpport for the importance of a secure at- we re also more likely to repo rt one or more
tachment in peer-related social development. good friends and less likely to report prolr
In the a ffect i,·e domain. ~e curely attached !ems w.ith peers if they had secure histories
children have been distinguished as display- (Grossman & Grossman. 1991 ).
ing more positive and less negative affect Beyond the ability to establish friendships.
( Lafreniere & Sroufe, 1985; Sroufe, 1983; studies have also examined the quality of
Waters et al.. 1979). They have also been olr those relationships. Park and Waters (1989)
served to be more positively responsive to obtained contemporaneous attachment rat-
other children both emotionally (Kesten- ings using the Attachment Q-set (Waters &
baum . Farber, & Sroufe. 1989) and in the Deane, 1985) on ·a sample of 33 children 42
typical give-and-take of play ( Lieberman, to 48 months old and their best friends. Chil-
1977). Cn an important early study. Lieber- d_ren were observed playing with their
man ( 1977 ) examined concurrent attach- friends for l hour, and the quality of the in-
ment ratings (both in the home and in the teractions was rated. Friend pairs that con-
laboratory situation) as well as 3-year-old tained both secure or one secure and one in-
children·s play in a familiar laboratory play- secure child were compared. Secure-secure
room with an unfamiliar peer. Csing partial pairs were rated as more harmonious, less
correlations. she found a negative associa- controlling, · more responsive , and happier
tion (after partialling out the effects of than insecure-secure pairs.
amount of peer experience) between se cu- In a longitudinal analysis, Youngblade and
rity of attachment and negative child-peer Belsky ( 1992) examined children playing with
exchanges (e.g.. threats, aggression, crying). close friends at age 5. The attachment status
Similarly, Booth et al. (1991) found that of the target children with each parent had
-l-vear-old children who had been rated as been assessed at age 13 months. Attachment
insecurely attached at 20 months displayed in this study was weakly and inconsistently as--
more negative affect ; they also noted a trend sociated with observations of children play-
indicating the use of more aggressive strate- ing with a friend . Although a weak, negative
gies when asked to share a single attractive association was found between mother-child
toy with a peer. Cn related social domains, se- attachment and negative aspects of child-
curely attached children have been found to friend play. father-child attachment was
display more positive self-es.t eem (Sroufe, found to be negatively associated with posi-
1983) and a more open and flexible, as op- tive aspects of play. Although the findings
posed to deprecating and defensive, self- across studies consistently support the im-
e,·aluative style (Cassidy, 1988). Although portance of parent-child attachment in the
these studies indicate that poor attachment development of peer relations. this study chal-
is associated with lower social competence, lenges the robustness of the effects of early
they do not necessarily speak to the overall attachment on later peer functioning in close
quality of the friendships these children relationships.
form. Elicker, Englund, and Sroufe's (1992) The question remains as to the process by
summer camp study examined just this abil- which parent-child attachment influences
ity bv analyzing sociometric friendship nom- relationships with peers. The attachment
inatio ns, backed by observations by camp model posits an "internal working model" for
staff. ln this study, 11-year-old children with relationships that is learned in the attach-
secure attachments in infancy were more ment relationship and generalizes to other
likely to form friendships during the 4-week relationships ( Bowlby, 1973; Bretherton,
PRo\.'tO T! '.\."G CHrLD RE:-.;'s Soc rAL C o'.'v1rETE'.\.'CE 585

1985; Srou fe & Fleeson. l986). It is composed ingl\" ca pable of bo th ,·en· .;cnsiti \"e .rnd in-
of conscio us and subconscio us cogniti,·e/ af- sensiti,·e respo ndi ng ( Isabe lla. 1993 l ,1 n d
fecti,·e mental representations that g uide at- less in\"Olved with their babies ( Beish.
titudes and expectations abou t relationships. Ro,·ine. & Taylo r, 1984 ). ·
Putallaz and Heflin ( 1990), following their re- Co nsistent with this approach "Of t~·ing earl~·
,·iew of the literature, cited two hypothesized caregiver interactions and au achment co
explanations for the associations between in- competence \\'ith peers, linkages between
fant-parent auachment and children's social similar parental behavio rs and the peer re la-
behavior: 1) the attachment relationship es- tions of children beyo nd in fanc~· ha"e also
tablishes a social orientation that generalizes been dire ctly examined. De tailed molecular
to o thers. a nd 2) the establishment of a se- observations of the interacti o n processes be-
cure home base facilitates more confident, tween parents and their todd)er and
less anxious exploration of the social world. preschool-age children hold the promise of
To these mechanisms, Elicker et al. ( 1992) yielding knowledge of the specific interac-
added that these explo rations result in a tional styles that may influence relationships
learned sense of self-worth and efficacy, and outside the parent-child dyad. These studies
that the rudiments of reciprocity are learned are rev.iewed next.
in the attachment relationship. Studies di-
rectly testing these mechanisms have begun. Parent-Child Interactions
but methodological and assessment issues are A variety of parent-child interactional style
still being worked out. Although a promising variables occurring beyond the early period
a\'enue of study, no firm conclusions can be of a child's life has been linked to children's
made about these mechanisms at this point. social compete nce. However, the most con-
Some of the best general support for the in- sistent and replicated finding in this area is
ternal working model, however, comes from the importance of the dimensions of parental
the social cognition literature, which is dis- control and warmth (see Baumrind, l9il).
cussed below. Similar to the findings in the attachment lit-
Given the links between parent-child at- erature, higher levels of child social compe-
tachment and peer social acceptance, what, tence (measured with a varie ty of techniques )
then , do the parent- child relationships of se- have been tied to positive displays of parental
cure ly and insecurely attached dyads look affect and, reciprocally, lower levels of com-
like? Although peer studies in the attach- petence have been tied co negative parental
ment literature have focused on links affect (Cottman & Katz, 1989; ~lacDonald &
between overall attachment and peer rela- Parke, 1984; Putallaz, 1987). Lower levels of
tio ns, ocher studies have examined the be- peer competence have also co nsistently been
havioral correlates of parental interactions associated with both an ove rly controlling
with their children across attachment cate- parental style ( Howes & Stewart, 1987;
gories. Briefly, mothers of securely attached Kochanska, 1992; MacDonald & Parke, 1984:
babies have been found to be more sensitive Putallaz, 1987) and a lack of control o r limit
to the cues of their babies, more consistent setting (Cottman & Katz, 1989). M:oreover,
and appropriate in their responses, as well as harsh maternal disciplinary styles have been
more positive in their emotio nal expressions tied co lower sociometric racings by peers
during interactions (e .g .. Ainsworth et al., and more disruptive behaviors among
1978; Isabella, 1993). In contrast, mothers of preschool- and school-age children ( Hart.
avoidant babies have been found co be more DeWolf, Wozniak, & Burts, 1992; Hart, Ladd,
angry, controlling, and intrusive with their & Burleson, 1990). As asserted by Baumrind
babies (Ainsworth et al., 1978); mothers of (1971) approximately 25 years ago, the opti-
resistant babies have been found to be less mal parental strategy to enhance children's
consistently responsive across time, seem- social development combines moderate lev-
586 GCRA L:-.JIC K A:-.:D NEVILLE
- - - - -- -- -
e ls of control with a warm e motional c li- (Parke, Cassidy, Burks. Carson, & Boyum.
mate, at least among middle-class families. 1992). Studies from this group have found
Beyond these s t~·listic approaches with social competence to be positively associated
\,·hich parents engage their children. par- with parental affect in play, particularly the
ents' responsivity to the child's behavior has level of emotional arousal during parent-
also been found to be predictive of children's child interactions and the quality of the af-
social competence. Lafreniere and Dumas fect produced (Boyum, 1991: Carson . 1991;
( 1992) found that preschoolers' ratings of ~lacDonald & Parke, 1984). ~toreover, the
children's social competence were associated ability of the parent and child to sustain play
with the conditional probabili~y of maternal interactions was positively associated with
responses to positive and negative affect and children's ability to decode the expressions
behavior, as well as compliance and noncom- of others and produce (encode) expressions
pliance. Specifically, mothers of socially that are more easily decoded by others
competent children appropriately recipro- (Parke et al., 1992). Such emotion regula-
cated positive and negative affect and behav- tion skills have elsewhere been established as
ior and responded positively to compliance important elements of social competence
and negatively to noncompliance; mothers (Buck, 1975; Field & Walden, 1982: Gural-
of children rated average in terms of social ni.ck, 1992a).
competence reciprocated positive and nega- The other proposed mechanism that may
tive behavior, and negative but not positive mediate the family-peer linkage is the child's
affect, and responded aversively to noncom- social information processing. Social
pliance but inconsistently to compliance; and problem-solving skills learned in the home
mochers of anxious-withdrawn children reci- have been proposed to generalize to peer in-
procated negative affect and behavior, were teractions, influencing social competence
nonresponsive to positive affect and behav- (Pettit et al., 1988). Furthermore. drawing
ior, and responded aversively to both non- on Dodge's (1986) more general information-
compliance and compliance. Similarly, processing model, Parke et al. (1994) have
Dumas, Lafreniere. Beaudin, and Verlaan added children's goals, expectations, antici-
( 1992) found that mothers of aggressive pated consequences, and efficacy beliefs to
preschoolers displayed a generally noncon- the list of potential cognitive mediators. Al-
tingent pattern of communication charac- though this is likely co be a fruitful avenue of
terized by reinforcement and punishment of research, few studies are available to support
both aversive and compliant child behavior. the model at this time. Pettit et al. (1988)
Consistent with the ~ctachment literature, demonstrated that, although maladaptive
these studies nicelv demonstrate a iink be- parental atti tudes had no direct effects on
tween the contingency of parental respond- peer and teacher ratings of social compe-
ing and child social competence. tence, they were fo und to be associated
How, then, do these parent-child interac- with poorer child social problem-solving
tion patterns influence the child's peer rela- skills, which were, in turn, directly associated
tionships? Two broad sets of mediators have with lower levels of social competence. Thus,
been proposed: social-cog nitive and e mo- the effect of maladaptive maternal attitudes
tional processing factors (Parke, Burks, Car- on child social competence was mediated by
son, ~eville , & Boyum, 1994: Pettit et at:, the social problem-solving skills of the child.
1988). With regard to emotional factors, No other study could be found that mea-
Parke and his colleagues have proposed that sured family factors, child social information-
emotional encoding, decoding, and regula- processing factors. and peer competence.
tor~· ski lls are (at least partially) learned Burks and Parke (1991) have found a corre-
within the parent-child relationship and spondence among the attributions, goals,
are generalized to interactions with others and anticipated consequences of fourth and
PROMOTI'.\:G CHrLDRE'.\: 's Soc1AL C o :-..1rETE.' - C E 38i

fifth graders and their mothers. Such fac tors child 's beha,·ior. they are not controlling to
ha,·e e lsewhere been found to influence chil- the point of being intrusi,·e , allo,,·ing che
dre n's social competence (Crick & Dodge. child to influence his or her exchanges when
1994: Dodge, 1986), but support for all three appropriate. \iloreo,·er. they are able to re-
components of the model within the same spond in this way consistently. Children
sample has ~·et to be reported. learn from such experiences to tn,tst in their
ln add ition to these basic processes, more safety to explore the social world. to expect
complex interpersonal processes also may positive reactions from others. to be able to
be learned in the parent-child context that regulate their own emotional reactions and
are generalized to the peer context. For ex- decode those of others, to solve social prob-
ample, children's attempts to influence lems in a more effecth·e manner. and to
their mother's behavior during interaction use some of the specific strategies for nego-
have been found to be positively associated tiating with others when goals and desires
with soc iometric ratings of peer status conflict.
( Putallaz, 1987). Children are more likely to
influence the behavior of mothers than they Peer Social Network
are the behavior of peers ( Kochanska, Another avenue through which families in-
1992), thus providing a better opportunity fluence the peer relations of their children is
for children to learn negotiation strategies. by providing access to a peer social network
~foreover, mothers' greater responsivity is (Parke et al. , 1992). Experience with peers
more likely to promote children's sense of self- (productive experience, presumably) is an
efficacv. important pathway to peer social competence
Similarly. conversational skills necessary for ( Mueller & Brenner, 1977 ). For example,
maintaining verbal exchanges may also be Lieberman (1977) found that the amount of
learned within the parent-child context. experience with peers that the 3-~·ea r- olds in
\'lartinez (1987) studied the use of skills for her sample had over the past year was posi-
maintaining and controlling conversational tively associated with peer competence,
exchanges in mother-child and child-child specifically their verbal responsivity and abil-
interactions with 2- and 4-year-old children. ity to sustain play. Reciprocally. Pettit et al.
Children used.simpler versions of strategies (1988) showed ·that rejected preschool ch il-
to control exchanges with other children dren had fewer opportunities with peers
than those used by mothers to control ex- based on mother's report.
c hanges with their children. Although no di- There are a ,·ariety of ways that parents in-
rect association was found between mothers' fluence children's opportunities to play with
and children's use of these strategies, it may other children. Parents often have the op-
be that only when the children are old portunity to choose the neighborhoods in
enough to master the strategies can the true which to live. which vary in terms of child
relationship be established. This issue awaits population density, distance and barriers be-
further study. tween houses, number of playgrounds. and
The characteristic parental behavior asso- level of safety, all of which influence the
ciated with higher levels of peer social com- number of friends children have and their
petence centers around issues of affect, con- opportunities for informal unstructured play
trol, responsivity, and consistency. Such ( Berg & ~ledrich. 1980; \'ledrich. Roizen,
parents are able to create a generally warm, Rubin, & Buckley, 1982). Parents also facili-
positive emotional tone to their exchanges tate children's enrollment in organized ac-
and are sensitive to the behavioral and emo- tivities, which have been linked to greater
tio nal cues of the child. They use this infor- social perspective-taking skills in 10-year-
mation to respond contingently to the child. o lds ( Bryant, 1985 ), an important compo-
Whereas they are able to set limits on the nent of peer interaction. Other factors. such
588 GL:RAL:--;ICK A:--;D NEVILLE

as parents' choice of child care or preschool children during peer interaction (e .g .. ,·er-
experiences and connections to extended bal coaching, specific suggestions for group
family members. also create o pportunities activity. positive discipline) than mothers of
for children to interact with peers (Rubin & ·low-social-skill children, who tended to use
Slomin, 1984 ). such tactics as avoidance and power-assertive
Parents also can initiate indi\'idual contacts discipline (Finnie & Russell. 1988). When
for their child with likely play partners. Ladd mothers' advice given prior to group entry
and his colleagues ( Ladd & Goiter, 1988; was examined, similar results were obtained:
Ladd & Han, 1992) found that preschool chil- Mothers of preschool children of high-soc ial-
dren whose parents regularly initiated con- status (rated sociometrically) encouraged
tact with peers had larger peer networks with group entry and suggested specific ideas for
whom they played more frequently than chil- accomplishing this goal, whereas mothers of
dren whose parents were not active arrangers. children of low-social-status tended to focus
These children were also more likely to initi- their child's attention on the toys available in
ate contacts with peers themselves and dis- the setting (Russell & Finnie, 1990).
played less anxiety in school. The boys in In sum, the literature suggests a number
these studies were also better.liked by peers in of general familial factors that are important
the preschool setting and less rejected if their in the development of good peer relations: a
parents actively initiated contacts. These stud- secure early attachment and parents' ability
ies nicely demonstrate the effectiveness of this to create a generally warm emotional tone.
simple parental activity. to set appropriate limits while not being
Parents also directly supervised and inter- overly controlling or intrusive, to respond
\'e ned in the play of children. with few peer consistently and contingently to child behav-
contacts completely unmonitored among iors, and to create opportunities for their
preschool-age children ( Ladd & Goiter, child to regularly interact with peers. These
1988). Parents' degree of involvement and experiences for children in the family con-
quality of support can obviously vary and text result in cognitive. affective, and behav-
cha nge with child development. Generally, ior skills that they take into their relation-
children require less direct he lp as they shi ps with peers. Such skills are the building
grow older. Whereas the level of peer com- blocks of social competence.
petence increases with direct parental inter-
,·ention at age 2-3, it has little appreciable Parental Attitudes and Beliefs
benefit in preschool-age children ( Bhavna- The extent to which parents work to extend
gri & Parke, 1991 ). Furthermore, children and enhance their child's social network and
whose parents report using direct super- the ways in which they carry this out may well
,·ision rather than indirect methods (e.g., be mediated by attitudinal and belief systems
o\·ersee from a distance) receive lower so- regarding their child's developing social com-
ciometric ratings and have been viewed as petence (see Mize. Pettit, & Brown. 1995). At
more hostile coward peers by teac hers any given point in time, the importance par-
( Ladd & Goiter, 1988). ents attach to the domain of social compe-
When parents do attempt to provide in- tence, how they conceptualize reactions to
structions and advice to their children re- their child's problematic social behavior, and
garding social tasks such' as gaining entry their beliefs as to whether specific social skills
into a peer group, the quality of those in- are determined more by environmental cir-
structions and advice is clearly associated cumstance than by intrinsic characteristics
with children's acceptance by their peers. . of their child are among the logical candi-
\fothers of preschoolers rated by teachers as dates guiding the peer social network pat-
having high social skills were observed to use terns discussed in the previous section
more skillful assistance strategies with their (Rubin & Mills, 1990).
PRO~OT!"iG CHILDRE:\: ' S SOCl..\L C O \! PETE :\: CE

Research hv Rubin and his colleagues \[ills. 1990). Spec ificalh. mnt her'\ o t \,·ith-
( \[ills & Rubin. 1990. 1992; Rubin & \[ills, drawn children reported thac the,· ,,·o uld use
1990) pro,·ided the first svstematic accempc to more hight~· coerci\·e strategies inrnh-ing
describe typical paremal attitudes and beliefs force. threats. commands. and the like than
in relacion to a child's social competence. mothers of typical children for boch ,-i-
When mothers of 4-vear-old children en- gnettes, whereas mothers whose children
rolled in preschool and child care centers were judged to be more negati,·e and ag-
were asked to identify factors contributing to gressive in their own social plav were more
preschool children's abilities to get ac- likely to report they would use either indirect
quainted with someone new, to resolve con- or no strategies at all. ln general, mothers of
flicts, to emer an ongoing group containing children without difficulties suggested fewer
unfamiliar peers, or to persuade others, a coercive strategies chan mothers of children
consistent pattern emerged. '.\fothers believed wich problematic social skills. Of equal inter-
that a child learned these social skills primar- est was the finding· char mothers of with-
ily through personal experiences in direct so- drawn children amibuted cheir child's be-
cial exchanges. Other factors, in descending haviors in the vignettes more to an enduring
order of importance, were observational trait rather than to transitor y factors such as
learning, adult explanations, and directive che child's mood or an age-related phenom-
teaching, such as rewarding or punishing spe- enon. In the general population. parents at-
cific behaviors or being told what to do. cribuce possible problematic behayiors of
:\!though there was some difference in the their child primarily to cransicory scares or
order found for resolving conflicts, the pat- situational influences, ~ut infrequently to
tern was nevertheless highly consistent across internal and stable factors such as traits or
che various social skills (for similar results, see dispositions (Mills & Rubin. 1990).
\i[ize et al., 1995 ). Accordingly, based on ex- These assessments of attitudes and beliefs
pressed beliefs, it can be expected that the of parents of children having difficult peer
majority of mothers who value their child's so- relationships appear to be consistent wich ac-
cial development will seek to provide as many tual parental behaviors (see Lafreniere &
experiences with peers as possible for their Dumas, 1992), further indicating char a con-
child (assuming all is going well), and are not trolling, directive style by parents is not con-
likely to take a directive role. ducive to promoting children's social com-
Even when mothers were asked to describe petence. Although it is not possible to
what they would do to help their children if determine the extent to which chese paremal
they demonstrated problematic social behav- patterns contributed to their child's peer in-
iors (by providing specific vignettes reflect- teraction difficulties initially, the existing at-
ing aggressive behavior and social with- titudes and beliefs do not present an opti-
drawal), primarily nondirective strategies mistic picture for che future. \iloreover, that
such as modeling, reasoning, information parents of withdrawn children tended to at-
seeking, or redirecting their children pre- cribuce cheir child's peer interaction prob-
dominated (Mills & Rubin, 1990). This ap- lems. to stable child characteristics funher
pears to be the normative reaction char- suggests thac there may be less of an incen-
acterizing families whose children are tive to promote the child's social competence.
interacting effectively with their peers. How- Consequently, unless these attitudes and be-
ever, when children exhibiting actual prob- liefs are examined in more depth, a pattern
lematic social behavior were identified of coercive interactions or lack of interest al-
based on observations during free play and together regarding their child's peer rela-
teacher racings (either withdrawn or aggres- tions and friendships is most likely co de-
sive ), a different pattern of responses by par- velop and to 'remain stable over time ( :Vlills
ents to the vignettes was obtained (Rubin & & Rubin, 1992).
590 Gt;RAL'.\i!CK .-\;\;D ~EV[LLE

SOCIAL SUPPORT AND stances such as those associated with a child's


CHILD CHARACTERISTICS characteristics. [n fact, social support fig-
ures prominently in earl~· intervention pro-
Individual child characteristics that produce grams, is potentially amen able to change,
stressful circumsrnnces for fam ilies, such as and has both direct and indirect linkages to
irrirnbility. wariness. O\·eracti\·ity. and being children's social competence. For example,
difficult to soothe during infancy, can, as Crockenberg (1981) found that, in infants,
Rubin ancl his colleagues ( Rubin , Le\.lare, & social support was associated with greater se-
Lollis, 1990; Rubin & Lollis, 1988) have de- curity of attachment. However, this effect
scribed, create higher risks for insecure at- was most powerful for those infants who were
tachment and correspondingly difficult and difficult to manage because of their tendency
nonfacilitating parent-child interactions that to be irritable. Apparently, the stress created
carry over to the preschool periods. It is this by children with difficult temperaments can
pattern that can lead directly to problematic b~ buffered by the availability of sufficient
peer relationships. Belsky. Robins, and Cam- support. Similarly, Pianta and Ball's (1993)
ble ( 1984) noted that it is "parenting that is research suggests that a~verse effects on child
sensitively attuned to children's capabilities competency related to stressors created by a
and to the developmental tasks they face that child's low cognitive levels or low family so-
promotes the kinds of developmental out- ciodemographic factors (e.g., being a young
comes thought important: emotional secu- mother, having a poor education, holding a
rity, behavioral .independence, social com- job considered to be of low status) could be
petence, and intellectual achievement" buffered to some extent by social support.
(p. 254). Difficult child characteristics pose The pathways through which social SUJr
a further threat to this already difficult task. port influences peer-related social compe-
Fortunately, most parents are able to tence are not well established, but presum-
establish "competence-inducing parenting ably indirect effects (through facilitating
practices" ( Belsky et al.. 1984. p. 254), even secure attachments, helping to establish pos-
for challenging children. Parents' personal itive maternal perceptions or cognitions, or
characteristics and resourcefulness are of reducing intrusive parenting styles) are pri-
course vital, allowing them to attend to the mary mechanisms. For preschool-age chil-
parenting process despite many stressors. dren, research on typically developing sam-
~oreover, those parents who have adequate ples has confirmed the positive association
supports are likely to be successful irrespec- between social support and parent-child in-
tive of their child's characteristics. Contex- teractions (Jennings. Stagg, & Connors,
tual sources of support related to finances, 1991) and between maternal support net-
work, and health, for example, are all associ- works and peer-related social competence
ated with the quality of parenting (e.g., Bel- ( Melson, Ladd. & Hsu, 1993). The latter as-
sky, 1984 ). Similarly. as discussed elsewhere sociation appears to be mediated indirectly
in this rnlume (see Chapters 10 and 20), so- through maternal perceptions and attribu-
cial support appears to be an especially im- tions. Even parents' attitudes and beliefs re-
portant factor in fostering development, garding the use of coercive strategies in re-
consisting of both informal sources of SUJr sponse to vignettes in which their child was
port pro,·ided by family members (especially having difficulties in peer relationships are
the spouse) and friends and formal sources related to perceived availability of social SUJr
of support provided by agencies and profes- port (Mills & Rubin, 1990). Support from a
sionals (e.g .. informational support). spouse is especially consistent with a mecha-
Socia l support, in the broadest sense of the nism that operates through indirect paths.
term, appears to be particularly valuable in For example, Cottman and Katz (1989) have
moderating the effects of difficult circum- offered a model suggesting that marital con-
PRoMOTI:"G CH1LDRE:-.:' s Soc1AL Co:vtrETE'.':CE 391

flict affects peer relatio ns indirectly thro ugh social competence ..-\s noted pre,·ioush-. chi l-
ics influence on parenting style, thereby in- dren at biological risk. especially prema-
duc ing chronic stress that interferes with the cure / low-birth-weighc children , and chose
child's ability co regulate e motions during with established disabilities manifest unusual
peer play. However, a more direcc path difficulties in peer-related soc ial competence.
inrnh·ing modeling of the inappropriate The question addressed here is whether these
conflicc resolutio n scracegies of parents has peer re la tio ns problems can be understood,
a lso been suggested, especially in connec- at lease in part. as a consequence of nonopci-
tion with behavior problems likely to influ- mal family interaction patterns that arise
. e nce children's social competence (Katz & from difficulties accommodating co a child's
Gattman , 1993). risk or disability status. Similarly. increasingly
Of course, many factors, such as the inter- larger numbers of families today lack ade-
personal characteristics of parents (e.g., de- quate social a nd related contextual supports,
pression, emc;>tional adjust~!:!nt), parenting which, as suggested in the previous section,
styles (e.g., approaches to discipline), a nd may well be associated with parenting prac-
even parents' own childhood peer relation- tices inconsistent with promoting the child's
ships ( Pucallaz. Costanzo, & Smith, 1991; social competence. Should one or more
Putallaz & Heflin, 1990), contribute signifi- of the areas of family influence discussed
cantly to the emergence of children's peer- previously in this chapter (i.e .. early care-
relaced social competence. Nevertheless, dif- giver-child relationships, parent-child in-
ficult child characteristics and the absence of teractio ns. peer social network, or parental
adequate social supports can be said to con- attitudes a nd beliefs) in fact be affected by
stitute risk factors that, under certain condi- these stressful conditions, appropriate early
tions, will adversely affect those family intervention programs can be established co
re lationships that influence a child's devel- assist families in developing strategies that
oping social competence. Moreover, this dis- will maximize their children's social compe-
cussion has provided the background for tence. Suggested intervention approaches
examining the potential effects of risk factors are discussed in the following sections.
chat are of a different order of magnitude
than variations in social support or child Risk Factors
temperament. Specifically, when parents (in le is important to note chat families consid-
particular, mothers) are nearly completely so- ered to be at high risk based on sociodemo-
cially isolated with few contextual supports graphic factors achieve secure attachment
or when children exhibit difficult-to-under- relationships with their children to about the
stand behavioral patterns associated with bi- same extent as families at low risk (Spieker &
ological risk factors or a developmental dis- Booth, 1988), at least when cases of child
ability, the increased risk for creating maltreatment or other extreme instances of
no noptimal family relationships linked to a inadequate care are excluded from the sam-
child's social competence can be readily afr ples. Yet, when high-: and low-risk samples
predated. In fact, these additional stressors are compared in terms of mother-child in-
are likely co influence all the areas of fam- teractions in the context of .peer-related so-
ily-child interaction discussed.previously. cial competence, a number of interesting
patterns emerge (Booth et al., 1991 ). Specif-
ically, when mothers were asked to manage a
IMPACT OF RISK
task in which their child was required to co-
AND DISABILITY STATUS
operate with a peer, interactional differ-
This section examines how additional stres- ences became apparent between the mothers
sors on families can produce circumstances at high and low risk that are relevant co issu.es
chat adversely affect their child's developing of peer-related social compe tence. In partic-
592 . Gt..;RAL.'-JICK A N D NEV!LLE

ular. mothers at high risk (defined as those 1983). Yet, despite this early pattern. few dif-
with an absence of social support during the ferences in attachment relationships ha,·e
prenatal period combined with low educa- been found that distinguish groups at bio-
tional level, young age, or low income} were logical risk from nonrisk groups (e.g .. Frodi
more coercive and adult centered and dis- & Thompson, 1985), a finding similar to that
played a beha\'ioral pattern less likely to fos- obtained from comparisons between fami-
ter social interaction's between the children lies at high and low risk based on sociode-
in the setting. These maternal interaction mographic factors. Although severity of
patterns corresponded to the varying levels neonatal illness may increase the risk of inse-
of socially competent behavior evident in the cure attachments, preterm children do not
children from the families at high and low appear to create unusual attachment prob-
risk as observed in a separate situation. The lems (Beckwith, 1990).
unique contributions of the absence of so- Accordingly, many if not most families of
cial support to these patterns cannot be de- preterm children are able to make successfol
termined because social support is inevitably adaptations . between the first and second
part of a constellation of variables associated year, at least for relatively healthy preterm
with families at high risk as defined in terms children (Greenberg & Crnic, 1988). How-
of sociodemographic factors. Nevertheless, ever, residual problems may remain for some
research on attitudes and beliefs suggests subgroups. because preventive intervention
that social support can in fact mitigate par- studies for preterm children have been suc-
ents' negative emotional arousal projected cessful in facilitating parenting competence,
to occur in response to their child's prob- including parent-child interactions. It is im-
lematic social behavior (vignettes of aggres- portant to note, these improved parent-child
sive and withdrawn behavior) and reduces interactions have a developmental impact.
reactions to those vignettes that would lead Specifically, in the absence of these preven-
to more coercive strategies, at least for fami- tive interventions, a gradual decline in intel-
lies at lower occupational status (Mills & lectual development occurs over the first 3
Rubin, 1990). years of life, a pattern that can be avoided
Biologically vulnerable children introduce through family-centered interventions (e.g ..
an entirely new set of issues for families, often Rauh, Achenbach, Nurcombe, Howell, &
posing an array of difficult challenges to the Teti, 1988; Resnick, Armstrong. & Carter,
parent-child relationship (see Chapters 3, 4, 1988). Perhaps for some subgroups of fami-
and 12). ~ost research has focused on pre-. lies and children, adaptations by parents to
mature/ low-birth-weight children and has their child's biological risk have not been
identified a number of important character- completely successful, thereby affecting the
istics that distinguish preterm from full-term course of the child's cognitive development.
infants. In general, preterm children exhibit In fact, as suggested by the study by Spiker et
more overall distress, lower responsivity, al. (1993), it would seem that these preven-
gaze aversion , less smiling, and poorer read- tive interventions are of the form that would
ability of social cues (Barnard & Kelly, 1990; likely have a positive impact on social as well
Beckwith, 1990: Crnic, Greenberg, Ragozin, as cognitive competence (Belsky, 1984). It is
Robinson, & Basham, 1983; Field, 1983). possible that subgroups at high risk, based
Compounded by the stress and uncertainty on medical factors alone (Landry et al., 1990)
associated with preterm birth (Bennett & or combined with sociodemographic factors
Guralnick, 1992), it is not surprising that (Resnick et al., 1988), would benefit most
early parent-child interactions have often from early intervention programs focusing
been described as less contingent and less on social competence.
positive, with mothers being more active in The difficulties that exist in identifying
stimulating their child (see Crnic et al., the characteristics of children and families
PRo M O Tl:-JG CHILDRE:-.i's So c1AL Co:virET E'.\:CE 393

who mighc best benefit fro m preventi\·e in- that an unusually large number o f ~·oung
ten·ention programs are underscored by the children with Down syndrome do not be-
fact that many pre term infants, especially come distressed during separation / reun ion
those born at extremely low birth weight, are episodes and infrequently seek contact with
likely to manifest a wide range of develop- or proximity to their mothers (Vaughn et al..
me nta l problems over time (Bennett & Gu- 1994 ). Indeed. many children do not display
ralnick. 1992). In general, the developmental those social cues associated with distress that
trajectories of children, particularly those at tend to elicit parental behaviors of holding
biological risk, are highly unpredictable. or comforting the child. These investigators
Even children at biological risk who score in suggested that the situations that are part of
che typical range based on neurodevelop- the attachment protocol (e.g., separation
mental testing during infancy may develop and reunion) may not be stressful for chil-
significant problems during the preschool dren with Down syndrome, perhaps because
period (Collin, Halsey, & Anderson, 1991). of dampened arousal mechanisms (Emde.
This raises the possibility that parent-child Katz, & Thorpe, 1978). From an assessment
interaction patterns for a subgroup of . perspective, these unusual behavior patterns
pre term children may actually reflect, at result in a high proportion of children de-
some point in development, those family in- scribed as unclassifiable within the attach-
teraction patterns that correspond to the ment evaluation system. Given the unusual
emergence of a child's established disability. nature of these c lassificacion patterns, the
In the following section, the relati0nship be- meaning of the attachment paradigm for
tween possible family influences and social children with Down syndrome, and perhaps
competence for children with established di~ ocher groups of childre n with established
abilities is examined more closely. disabilities, can be called into question
( Vaughn et al., 1994 ). However this issue is
Established Disability resolved, the less-expressive behavioral reper-
:\s was the case for children at risk, despite toire characteristic of children with Down
numerous obstacles, secure attachment rela- syndrome as well as other groups of children
tionships are formed in most instances be- with established disabilities (Stahlecker &
tween children with established disabilities Cohen, 1985) may well require substantial
and their primary caregiver (see Blacher & adaptations by parents to create harmonious
~1eyers, 1983; Cicchetti & Beeghly, 1990). Pat- and synchronous relationships with their
terns of attachment similar to those of typi- child (see also Capps, S\gman, & :vtundy.
cally developing children have been obtained 1994).
for many groups of children, including those In fact, as demonstrated for heterogeneous
with hearing impairments (Lederberg groups of children with developmental di~
& \fobley, 1990) and physical/ neurological abilities, social interactions occurring be-
disabilities (Stahlecker & Cohen, 1985; tween parents and children often take on a
Wasserman, Lennon, Allen, & Shilansky, quality that is different from that of
1987 ). :vtoreover, the organization of attach- parent-child interactions when children do
ment relationships appropriate for develop- not have disabilities (see Marfo, 1988). In
mental level has been observed for children many ways, these unique patterns can be un-
with Down syndrome (Cicchetti & Serafica, . derstood as parental adaptations to specific
1981 ). characteristics of their children, such as
Despite these similarities, some notable dif- lower levels of child responsiveness and re-
ferences in the behavior of children with e~ duced social initiations ( Beeghly, Weiss-
tablished disabilities have been observed Perry, & Cicchetti, 1989;Jones, 1980; Landry,
when paruc1pating in the attachment Garner, Pirie, & Swank, 1994). The most con-
protocol. For example, observations suggest sistent finding is the tendency of parents of
594 GCRAL~ICK A;\;D NEVILLE
-- - - - ·- -- -- - ----- - -

children with disabilities to become more di- consequence. requests for the child to do
recti\·e and controlling during soc ial ex- something (action requests) tend to pre-
changes than parents of children without dominate. Correspondingly. positive reci-
disabilities ( Buium, Rynders, & Turnure, procity, as indexed by playfulness and faugh-
1974; Cunningham, Reuter, Blackwell, & ing, has been observed to occur less
Deck, 1981; Jones, 1980; ~fahoney, Fors, & frequently for children with disabilities across
Wood, 1990; Terdal. Jackson, & Garner, a range of ages ( Floyd & Phillippe, 1993 ).
1976). From the perspective of promoting This more performance-oriented pattern is
children's social competence, however, this indeed of concern from the perspecti\·e of
pattern could well turn out to be counter- promoting young children's competence
productive. A lack of balance in social ex- and appears to extend into the preschool
changes can limit opportunities for the give- years ( Eheart, 1982).
and-take needed to practice important .-\ directive tend.ency may also be more ev-
social sk.ills, and fewer social initiations re- ident when parents attempt to encourage
duce a child's ability to derermine the goals their children to play with peers in home or
of parent-child interactions. playgroup settings. Indeed, children's com-
How to interpret the developmental impli- pliance to parents' directives is less likely
cations of this difference in parental direc- to occur in unstructured social settings,
tives is now a matter of intense debate (Ma- thereby encouraging even greater use of di-
honey, Robinson, & Powell, 1992; Marfa, rectives (Landry et al., 1994; see also Floyd &
1990). The primary issues center around Phillippe, 1993 ). If this does occur, parents of
whether parents are providing the appropri- children with disabilities may have consider-
ate structure and stimulation levels or are in- able difficulty facilitating their child's peer
terfering with or inhibiting the development social network (e.g., helping their child to
of children's self-initiated interactions. Re- play more effectively with a peer at home),
lated issues with regard to the possible dif- perhaps discouraging future peer-related ac-
ferential effects of the type of directive used tivities.
(e.g., imperatives, suggestions, restrictions), Available research indicates that parents
and how other aspects of the parent-child should make the most of any opportunities
interaction are affecred (i.e., the responsive- to strengthen their child's peer social net-
ness, warmth, and sensitivity observed in the work because social contacts with peers in
relationship), are yet to be resolved (see the neighborhood and community appear to
Chapter 22) and will certainly provide in- be more limited for children with estab-
sight into this complex problem. lished disabilities. Research by Lewis, Feir-
Yet, evidence is accumulating to suggest ing, and Brooks-Gunn (1987), for example,
that there exists a substantial subgroup of on the social networks of young children
parents of children with established disabili- (social contacts) indicates that children with
ties whose social interactions with their chil- disabiUties have proportionately fewer peer
dren seem to have a different purpose than contacts, relative to adult contacts, ~han do
those of parents of children without disabili- children without disabilities. Moreover, this
ties. Specifically, rather than primarily en- relative proportion does not appear to
couraging pleasurable and extended social change over the preschool period. The rea-
exchanges, engaging in toy play, or calling sons for these differences are likely to be
attention to interesting features of the envi- many and varied, perhaps related to issues of
ronment, as is most common, this subgroup stigmatization (Goffman, 1963), limited OJ>
of parems of children with established dis- tions in the community to imeract with
abilities is far more focused on encouraging peers, or the demands of specialized thera-
their c hildren to perform certain behaviors pies. ~evertheless, the limited peer experi-
( Mahoney et al., 1990; Marfa, 1991 ). As a ence in community settings is likely to slow
PRO~OTl;\iG CHILDRE'.\i S SOCIAL CO'.'.IPETE:-.:CE
1
395

the de\'elopment of children's peer-related \'elopment. Chapters in this \·o lume con-
social competence. cerned with fostering optimal parent-child
Finallv. onlv limited information is avail- interactions and prodding supports (e.g.,
able with regard to the attitudes and beliefs Chapters 10, 12. 20. and 22) reflect that per-
about the de\'elopment of social competence spective. In addition, a brief discussion of
of mothers of rnung children with disabili- programs designed to enhance the quality of
ties. However. a 1994 study carried out by attachment between parents and children is
Booth (1994) provides insight into this issue. presented at the end of this section. How-
Specifically, maternal responses to a stan- ever, for preschool-age children, additional
dard series of assessments requesting infor- efforts that are more directly focused on chil-
mation about the causal attitudes and beliefs dren's social competence (particularly peer-
regarding their child's developing social com- related social competence) can be estab-
petence (s haring. making friends, gaining lished. This section focuses primarily on
acceptance into a group) were carefully eval- intervention suggestions for preschool-age
uated. Interestingly, results revealed that children.
mothers of children with disabilities tended As in the case of the younger child, air
to attribute the development of their child's proaches to working with families of
social com petence more to traits or disposi- preschool-age children to promote the social
tional factors than to factors external to the competence of their children should fall well
child. In addition, mothers also believed that within established early intervention para-
children primarily learn social skills through digms. ·Promoting a child's social competence
their own experiences rather than from should not be seen as a separate enterprise;
adult intervention. It is quite possible that rather, it should be integrated into the child's
this combination of attitudes and beliefs re- overall early intervention program. Fortu-
garding the importance of child disposi- nately, the emerging family-centered pro-
tional characteristics and reliance on the grams (Guralnick, 1989) are highly consis-
children themselves to learn from their own tent with our understanding of the factors
experience without adult intervention may that influence children's social competenee
create little enthusiasm on the part of par- described previously, particularly those pro-
ents for the prospects of a systematic inter- grams that advocate for an emphasis on
~·ention program. The difficulties parents of strengthening relationships among family
children with disabilities have in arranging members (Affleck, McGrade, McQueeney, &
for peer experiences for their child noted Allen, 1982) and on developing a meaning-
previously may dilute parental enthusiasm ful parent-professional partnership ( Dunst,
even further, despite the fact that the devel- Trivette, & Deal, 1988).
opment of their child's social competence is At this point, only some general sugges-
highly valued (Booth, 1994). To the extent tions to organize an intervention in the area
that this is the case, these attitude and belief of social competence can be provided. Al-
systems of mothers of preschool-age children though the approach that follows has been
with disabilities should be addressed directly structured in terms of the areas of family in-
(see Chapter 25). fluence on children's peer-related social com-
petence presented in the previous section,
that framework must be translated into clin-
INTERVENTION SUGGESTIONS ical information (i.e., assessments) need.e d
As shown , early intervention strategies to as- to develop a coherent intervention plan. In
sist families of very young children to su~ addition, a process must be established that
port their child's developing social compe- will effectively involve the clinician well
tence are likely to follow closely approaches within the complex and sensitive relation-
suggested to support general aspects of de- ships existing within the family network.
596 GURALNICK AND NEVILLE

General principles and practices as well as is- critical role in the choices they make and a~
sues of concern for both professionals and proaches they take in fostering their child's
parents are available for guidance (Bailey, peer-related social development. A series of
1987; Comfort & Farran, 1994). It is impor- interview questions related to beliefs and at-
tant to point out that, to our knowledge, titudes, emphasizing parental perceptions of
no comprehensive approach such as the their child's developing peer-related social
one presented in chis section has been competence-particularly its importance, its
attempted. malleability, and the way in which children
learn to interact with their peers-is now
Clinical Assessment available (Booth, 1994). It is during this in-
A first step is to gain an overall perspective terview process that a dialogue can be estab-
on child and family functioning in areas lished between parents and professionals re-
relevant to a child's social competence. garding these most critical of issues.
Assessments of the child's developmental The next element of the assessment is an
profile, usually obtained from existing clin- evaluation of the child's social network. The
ical or educational sources, provide such opportunity to interact with peers is, · of
a perspective of the child's cognitive, com- course, essential for the· development of
municative, behavioral, and emotional peer-related social skills. Therefore, it is im-
strengths and concerns. In addition, spe- portant to know how many children the
cific clinical assessments directed toward child of interest plays· with and the fre-
understanding the child's peer-related scr quency with which they contact one another.
cial competence are now available (e.g., Gu- Also of interest are the circumstances under.
ralnick, 1992b) and are discussed briefly in which the child has access to playmates, how
the next section of the chapter. Once this often the parents arrange social activities,
·child-focused information is obtained, the and how the parents monitor and supervise
family situation is then considered, empha- their child's play. Numerous checklists and in-
sizing available supports and the stressors terview formats are now available (e.g., Ladd
impinging on the family. Information re- & Colter, 1988) and can be easily adapted
lated to the economic and occupational sta- for clinical use. However, assessments of the
tus of family members, how the family work strategies parents use to foster their child's
is divided, their level and sources of stress, social competence during play with peers
and what supports they have to help cope (i.e., strengthen the peer social network) re-
with child-rearing and related issues prcr main to be developed. Observations of
vides an essential context for a comprehen- child-child social play situations in the
sive intervention program. Interviews and home in which the parent is asked to en-
questionnaires are available to organize this courage productive play when difficulties
information. For example, the Pare~ting arise will certainly form a component of any
Stress Index (Abidin, 1990) is just one of a useful clinical protocol. Moreover, impor-
number of standardized measures that can tant insights can be obtained through dis-
be of value in evaluating the stress levels of cussions of the child's compliance with
families. Similarly, a variety of methods are parental requests when the child plays with
also available to assess social support (see other children.
Chapters 10 a nd 20) that can be easily in~ Finally, the parent-child relationship
corporated into a clinical assessment. must be evaluated and will likely reveal pat-
Parents' beliefs and attitudes about the terns similar to those obtained from obser-
importance of social development and how vations of parent involvement in child-child
social skills are acquired and fostered should play just noted. This is an especially difficult
also be assessed. As the studies reviewed ear- task, requiring extensive observations in a
lier revealed, parents' attitudes can play a. variety of situations as well as interviews with
PROMOTING CHILDREN'S SOCIAL COMPETENCE 597

parents and others familiar with the family. build a sense of self-efficacy in relation to
Of particular interest for the development of their child 's social development provides a
peer relations is the emotional tone of foundation for more active involvement to
parent-child interactions, the responsive- promote their child's skills that may follow.
ness of parents to child behavior, the contin- However, before more demanding and
gent relationship between child and parent time-consuming activities related to strength-
behavior, the degree to which parem~ main- ening the child's peer social network or fos-
tain control over the exchanges (i.e., sup- tering more optimal parent-child interac-
port vs. intrusiveness), and the opportunities tions emerge from the process, issues of
parents and children take to play together. A family stress and social support must be con-
number of observational instruments for as- sidered. If the parents are experiencing a
sessing parent-child interactions are avail- high level of stress and feeling that they have
able that can be of value (e.g., Comfort & little extra energy co expen~ on a new activ-
Farran, 1994). ity, suggestions that place additional de-
mands on them will likely result in failure. As
Intervention a result, every effort should be made to de-
The outcome of this interactive assessment sign an intervention that brings additional
process, which typically would include ex- resources to the problems at hand.
tensive sharing of views and information by Because the goal is to establish an inter-
participants, can then be used to create a list vention with long-term generalizability, the
of mutually agreed on issues in order to focus ideal would be to encourage the use of sup-
intervention efforts. Of primary importance ports that will continue beyond the life of the
is the recognition that any intervention must intervention. Naturally existing supports
correspond to the beliefs and attitudes of such as other family members, friends, and
parents if they are to participate in a mean- neighbors would therefore be the first place
ingful manner. There are a myriad of attitu- to look for additional resources. If these re-
dinal factors that are potentially important. sources are not currently being tapped, the
However, the attributions parents make re- reasons will need to be explored. It may also
garding the source and malleability of possi- be that families have not thought broadly
ble problems in social skills are likely to de- enough about what resources are available.
termine their willingness to put effort into Al ternatively, attitudinal factors, such as the
creating change'. For example, attributions parents' comfort with asking for and accept-
ofat least some of their child's peer social in- ing help, may be interfering. Creativity in
teraction difficulties to more transitory states brainstorming about untapped resources in
or to situational factors can create a more the family, neighborhood, or larger commu-
hopeful scenario for change than attribu- nity can result in unexpected solutions to
tions associated with aspects of the child's problems. Organized community resources,
disability or enduring psychological traits. such as support groups, child care facilities,
Interventions consisting of discussions of re- preschool services, and health service agen-
alistic developmental expectations, case his- cies, may also be available to the family: Sup-
tories of experiences of how other families port, reassurance, and building on the
have approached similar problems, conver- strengths of parents are necessary in order
sations focusing on the child's strengths in to foster change in a family system that will
peer interactions (best obtained from direct establish the conditions for maximizing the
and joim observations by the parent and pro- child's social competence.
fessional during an arranged play-situation), A more demanding phase is to begin to
and simply providing as much information address problems related to more specific is-
as possible on these issues constitute impor- sues-ones that require extensive involve-
tant beginning points. Assisting parents to ment of the parents themselves. Expanding a
598 GL:RAL'.\/ICK A ,'.\.'D N EV ILL E

child's peer contacts in the community is a the parents during the inten·iew process.
likely a\'enue of intervention for children with pare nt-child play can be readilv discussed in
disabilities. This approach may also fit well a nonjudgmental way.
with many parents' beliefs regarding effec- . Should assessments suggest that nonopti-
tive means of improving their child's social mal styles predominate, the parent-child
skills (i.e., through experience with peers). play context may be an ideal way to address
Typical opportunities include community or these issues. For example, after discussing
informally organized playgroups and parent- the importance of initiation skills in peer re-
arranged one-to-one play with a peer. How- lations, the interventionist could encourage
ever, such activities may not be as straight- highly directive parents to focus more on
forward to arrange as it would first appear. supporting and expanding the initiations of
Parents of children with disabilities may have the child. Emphasizing the importance of
difficulty finding opportunities in which fam- providing their child with opportunities to
ilies in the general community are willing co regulate his or her emotions in this context
make adjustments to the needs of individual and to support any attempts at compliance
children ( Bailey & Winton, 1989). Parents to parentai requests may proviqe a basis for
mav also have concerns about their child subsequent interventions related to this sen-
being rejected or ignored an·d may need to sitive area of development. An advantage to
work through their own reactions to possible promoting parent-child play is that it is gen-
stigmatization as a result of having a child erally an enjoyable activity that is likely to be
with a disability now facing the challenges of mutually satisfying and may have generally
interacting with peers and establishing positive repercussions on the parent-child
friendships. relationship as a whole. In addition, parents
Similarly, parents may need assistance in may be more open to learning new play styles
the context of the child's play with peers in than altering other aspects of the relationship
fostering productive interactions. This part with their child.
o f the process should be coordinated with
the child's educational program (see next Early Efforts
major section ) and should attempt to mini- To complete this discussion of possible inter-
mize didactic or structured activities while ventions, early child-caregiver relationships .
emphasizing the parents· role as facilitator should be con_s idered in view of their impor-
by adapting the play setting to maximize suc- tance to the developm~nt of peer social com-
cess. As the process unfolds, high-priority petence. By emphasizing these early rela-
concerns can be identified, such as improv- tionships, it is hoped that the extensive array
ing the child's conflict resolution skills or ex- of interventions just described for preschool
panding a dramatic play repertoire to enable children can be minimized.
more extensive shared exchanges with peers. As reviewed previously, our earliest efforts
The final potentially fruitful strategy that might best focus on supporting the develop-
follows from our approach is to explore op- ment of a secure attachment between parent
portuni ties to enhance parent-child interac- and child, particularly for specific sub-
tions. As noted in the previous section, groups. Although most early intervention
parent-child play can be an important set- programs are consistent with this general
ting in which children learn social skills nec- goal, surprisingly few attempts have specifi-
essary for effective peer relations and can cally targeted attachment (when appropri-
ser\'e as an excellent vehicle for encouraging ate) and measured intervention effectiveness
broader aspects of parent-child interac- with an attachment rating. An exception is
tions. Consequently. although compliance the recent work of van den Boom (1994). In
problems also may be apparent, perhaps a carefully designed intervention study, 100
even directly expressed as a concern by mother-infant dyads, who were at risk for de-
PRO\ltOTI~G CHILDRE~'s Soc1AL Co\ltPETE:-.:cE 599

\·eloping insecure attachments as a result of group became more comparable to the


low socioeconomic status and high infant secure-co ntrol than the insecure-control
irritability at binh. were randomly assigned group on these measures. :\o differences.
to intervention and control groups. The howe\·er, were found in ratings of securitv of
foc us of the intervention was to assist moth- attachment. \(ore time mav be needed after
ers (in their homes ) in increasing sensitive an insecure attachment has alreadv been es-
responsi\·eness to their babies. The training tablished for parents and children to de-
cook place when the babies were 6-9 months velop the level of trust and comfort with each
old, after which mother-infant interaction other that secure attachment demands.
was observed. Intervention mothers were Additional studies are needed to establish
found to be more responsive, stimulating, vi- the reliability of these inter\'ention methods,
sually attentive, and controlling. Their ba- to evaluate longer term changes. and to ex-
bies were observed to display more soc_iabil- tend the approach to families of children
ity and a greater capability to self-soothe, to with disabilities. It does appear, however, that
engage in more cognitively sophisticated lev- these approaches can be useful for many
els of exploration, and to cry less than con- families of children with disabilitie·s. By
trol babies. At 12 months the dyads were ob- providing highly individualized guidance,
served in the Strange Situation. Significant support, and information relevant to attach-
differences between the groups were found ment quality-including, for example, con-
in attachment classifications, with only 28% cerns about the relative absence of emcr
of control infants classified as secure com- tional expressiveness seen in many children
pared with 62% of the intervention. group. with Down syndrome-the foundation for
This study nicely documents the efficacy of adaptive parent-'-child relationships and sub-
individualized assistance in developing the sequent social competence can be bolstered.
skills and feelings of effectiveness that lead It would also appear, based on these two
to secure attachments. studies, that prevention efforts are more
A more traditional clinical approach was likely to be effective with parents at very high
adopted by Lieberman, Weston, and Pawl risk, rather than waiting until an insecure at-
( 1991 ). They provided services to mothers at tachment has been formed.
risk for attachment problems as a result of
sociodemographic factors, in which "clini-
INTERVENTIONS IN
cians sought co alleviate the mothers' psy-
THE PRESCHOOL CONTEXT
chological conflicts about their children and
to provide developmental information that Despite our best efforts during the first 3
was clinically timed and tailored to the child's years ofa child's life, it is likely chat preschool-
temperament and individual style" (p. 202). age children at risk and those with estab-
In this study, insecurely attached mother- lished disabilities will manifest many of the
child dyads were randomly assigned to either difficulties in establishing relationships with
an intervention or a control group at 12 their peers and developing friendships de-
months of age. Comparisons were made at scribed earlier (see Guralnick, 1990a). As
24 months of age among three groups: noted, many of the intervention approaches
insecure-intervention, insecure-control, and emphasized in the previous section were in-
secure-control. The results supported the ef- tended for families of preschool-age children.
fectiveness of the intervention in enhancing In fact, one component related to expanding
the affective quality of mothers' interaction or sustaining the child's peer social network
styles and decreasing child avoidance, resis- consisted of parenc-orchestrated strategies
tance, and anger, thus improving the overall designed specifically to facilitate their
quality of mother-child interactions and the child's social skills in informal peer play situ-
negotiation of conflict. The intervention ations.
600 Gt.:RAL:'-JICK AND NEVILLE
- - - - - ·- -- - - - - - -- - -- - - ·-- - -

~e,·ertheless . it is educators and clinicians and the often restricted peer social networks
in preschool and child care settings who gen- found for children with disabilities.
erally ta ke primary responsibility for directly ..\!though intervention tec hniques de-
fostering children's peer-related social com- signed to be applied in the peer context are
petence. Indeed, numerous educational and continually being refined and revised to im-
therapeutic techniques have been developed prove their effectiveness, three limitations to
o\·er the years to promote young children's the general approaches that have been taken
peer interactions, many of which are best can be identified. First, thorough asses~
carried out with the assistance of, or in a con- ments consistent with contemporary concel"
text that includes, children without disabili- tualizations of children's peer-related social
ties. Approaches involving modeling and olr competence are not typically found. Absent
servational learning, coaching, prompting, as well are linkages between a comprehensive
rehearsal. direct teaching of social strategies, assessment and the design of intervention
and reinforcement procedures, as well as the s.trategies. Second, interventions tend to
application of peer-mediated techniques, focus only on altering the surface features of
have been used extensively. Moreover, there children's peer-related social behavior, with
has been a n increased recognition of the virtually no consideration of the underlying
fact that environmental and social context processes that may be influencing those so-
factors can have a major impact on children's cial interaction patterns. Of importance here
peer interactions. These factors include the are attentional processes as well as the ability
number and familiarity of the children in the of the child to regulate his or her emotions in
social setting, the types of toys available, and the peer comext or to arrive at a mutual or
the physical arrangement of the classroom shared understanding regarding what to do
environment. Detailed reviews of these tech- when engaging in specific play themes (e.g.,
niques and their effectiveness are available rough and tumble play, cooking sequences).
elsewhere ( \.tcEvoy. Odom, & McConnell, Third, an unusually large proportion of in-
1992; Odom & Brown, 1993; Sainato & Carta, tervention techniques have not been cast
1992). within a developmental framework.
Whether focused on individual children Advances in the field of peer-related social
(Odom & Strain, 1986) or larger gro.ups competence have begun to address each of
(Antia & Kreimeyer, 1987, 1988; Hundert & these limitations. Specifically, assessments in
Houghton, 1992; Poresky & Hooper, 1984), which peer-related social competence is con-
these potentially important techniques can ceptualized as the appropriateness and effec-
be readily employed by educators and clini- tiveness with which children solve important
cians in preschool contexts. Yet, it is also the social tasks noted earlier (see Dodge et al.,
case that these techniques have not yielded 1986; Guralnick, 1990b), such as entry into
social interaction skills that generalize to peer groups or resolving conflicts, have now
other contexts over time in the majority of been developed (Guralnick, l992b). Al-
circumstances (see Guralnick, 1994). In part, though additional information is needed to
this generally acknowl~dged problem can be bridge assessment and intervention, proce-
attributed to the difficulties encountered in dures for this are now becoming available
altering behavioral patterns that are unusu- (see Guralnick, 1994). Similarly, the adapta-
ally resistant to change. Apparently, numer- tions and special considerations at the indi-
ous factors contribute to this tendency vidual child level required as part of an inter-
toward stability, including constraints associ- vention program to accommodate to a range
ated with the child's developmental charac- of information-processing, emotional regula-
teristics, reputational factors, the existence tion, or shared understanding processes that
of social status hierarchies in the classroom govern the selection of children's strategies
context, family-child interaction patterns, within social tasks are also being developed.
PRo:v1on :--;c CHILDRE:-.; 's SocIAL C o:-..trETE:"CE 601

Finallv. the appropriateness of the de\'elop- that the sequences of pla~· of typical I\' de\'el-
mental perspective applied to children at risk oping children that could be identified as
and those with developmental disabilities and scripted were associated with longer episodes
the compatibility of associated interventions of play than nonscripted sequences. Simi-
to improve children's peer relations within larly, Furman and Walden ( 1990) created
the framework of developmentally appropri- dyads of 3-, 4-. and 5-year-olds, matching
ate practices has been established (see Gu- children on the basis of chronological age.
ralnick, 1993a). gender, and specific knowledge of four dif-
ferent scripts. Their analysis revealed that
Process Approaches communication failures occurred propor-
This general contemporary approach that tionally less often when scripts were more fa-
emphasizes the importance of underlying miliar but only for older children. With re-
processes can be illustrated by focusing on gard to the length of play episodes, results
''shared understanding," a process that is cen- revealed that more interactive turns oc-
tral to virtually all social tasks. This process curred in the context of the more familiar
refers to a .set of mutually agreed on interac- scripts but only for the younger children.
tion patterns and expectations in the peer This latter finding suggests that scripts may
context that become established, usually re- be particularly valuable in extending social
garding thematic play, social roles, or social exchanges between peers for those children
rules (Guralnick, 1992a). Shared understand- whose social/ communicative skills are most
ing, a process of particular concern for chil- fragile, a potentially important finding when
dren with developmental disabilities, may Ofr considering intervention programs for chil-
erate to facilitate social play interactions dren with disabilities.
among children by evoking "scripts" or struc- Comprehensive assessments focusing on
tures that can help guide sequences of peer- the various dimensions of shared understand-
related social interactions (Schank & Abel- ing have been developed (Guralnick. 1992a.
son, 1977 ). Generally speaking, scripts 1992b). From an intervention perspective, it
constitute a shared understanding of a con- may be possible to facilitate interactions be-
ceptual structure and typically represent com- tween children experiencing difficulties by
mon events or routines such as those that establishing shared scripts in some struc-
occur in circle time or in specific themes tured, even didactic, manner (see DeKlyen
found in sociodramatic play. ln essence, these & Odom, 1989). This is precisely the ap-
scripts also constitute goals that are repre- proach taken by one group of investigators
sented by a particular theme. Of special im- in an effort to improve the peer interactions
portance is that scripts are part of children's of young children with behavior disorders
memory structures, help define roles and ex- and language delays (Goldstein, Wickstrom,
pectations, and provide the basis for inter- Hoyson, Jamieson, & Odom, 1988). Script
preting and sequencing events across an ex- training focusing on a sociodramatic play se-
te nded social interaction. Potentially of most quence took place during 15-minute lesson
value from a social development perspective periods over the course of approximately 2
is that scripts allow a smoother flow of ex- weeks. Triads of children were involved con-
change to occur yet offer participants con- sisting of one child with developmental prob-
siderable flexibility and opportunities to elab- lems and two typically developing children
orate on thei.r social exchanges. or composed entirely of children with devel-
Available research is in fact consistent with opmental problems. Clinicians direcclv
the notion that a shared understanding in taught the sequence of events for scripts by
the form of scripts can assist young children modeling appropriate interactions and
to interact more competently wich one an- prompting children to participate. Children
other. Nelson and Seidman (1984) observed were encouraged to assume different roles
602 C L"RAL'.\:ICK A'.\:D '.'JE\'I LLE

established in the sociodramatic script. and from a cogniti\·e perspecti\e . .-.\ltho ug h the
all reached a minimum criterion of perfor- results of this study were equ irncal and d id
mance . E\"aluations of the effects of this not individualize training to a significant ex-
training protocol during structured free tent, the study ne,·ertheless represents an im-
play re\·ealed only a modest increase of the- portant effort to consider core processes
matically appropriate social interactions. (e.g., higher order processes related to mon-
Howe\'er, when clinicians began to prompt itoring social interactions) when de\'eloping
children to remain in their roles or to adopt intervention programs . ..\ major challenge
others, t~e number of thematic interactions for the future will be to evaluate the effec-
did increase. Other evidence suggested that tiveness of these newly emerging approaches
role prompting was in fact necessary for these in the peer context and to integrate them
interactions to he maintained. with strategies that involve families and com-
We cannot be certain of the extent to which munities in connection with expanding c;hil-
the social interactions of the children with dren's peer social networks.
developmental problems were appropriate
and effective in this context. The reliance on
CONCLUSION
clinicians to prompt interactions is of con-
cern, but it may be seen as a beginning point This chapte_r has attempted to provide a ra-
for building extended social exchanges. Of tionale for establishing social competence as
importance, there was evidence that some of a central feature of early intervention pro-
the children exhibited spontaneity in their grams. As described, social competence is
interactions, because a number of non- conceptualized as a valued outcome of earlv
scripted but thematically consistent social intervention, analogous to facilitating a
exchanges were observed. [n general, it may child's cognitive or communicative compe-
well be that some form of direct training tence. Cnquestionably, this is an essential
within a script framework will constitute a message of this chapter, one that is consis-
core component of intervention efforts for tent with conducting second-generation
children with established developmental research in the field of early intervention
problems. (Guralnick, 1989, 1993b). However, it was
Similarly, beyond processes related to also suggested that a focus on children's so-
shareci understanding, investigators working cial competence_can be useful as a means of
with typically developing children have rec- organizing and providing coherence to the
ognized the importance of a more process- many seemingly disparate activities found
oriented approach to intervention in the area under the rubric of early intervention. .
of children·speer-related social competence. With regard to this latter point, it is well
For example, ~lize and Ladd (1990) devel- recognized that selecting an array of princi-
oped adult-mediated coaching techniques for ples and practices that provide a framework
children who held positions of low social sta- for early intervention programs tends to be a
tus designed to address underlying informa- difficult process. Numerous models and
tion-processing difficulties, including social approaches compete for the attention of pro-
knowledge, the ability to translate that knowl- gram developers. Nevertheless, considera-
edge into actual social interactions with tions of a child's developing social compe-
peers, and the ability to monitor social inter- tence can provide a systematic basis for
actions during exchanges with peers. The organizing and structuring the core compo-
training technique involved the use of hand nents of an early intervention program. For
puppets to provide direct instruction and example, the list below represents key princi-
feedback to children and to assist in rehearsal ples and practices that emerge from or are
of social interactions as well as in role play- consistent with the social competence frame-
ing. However, the materials were demanding work described in this chapter. In accor-
PRO.\.I OTI'.'G CH!LDRE:--;'s SocIAL Co\.tPETE>.CE b03

dance \,·irh this framework, early inten·en- 1. Emphasize strategies and adaptation..;
tion programs would be guided to do the fol- in context rather than spec ific beha\·iors.
lowing: 8. Establish the \'alue and contributions
of participation in natural e n\'iron-
I. Focus o n longer term goals. ·
ments to a child's de\'elopment.
2. ...\ddress the ways skills and abilities as-
9. Oe\'elop support systems for families
sociated with more basic de,.·elopmen-
that emphasize both close relation-
tal dmains are integrated in a social
ships and inrnlvemem of the broader
context in a meaningful way (i.e., in the
community.
service of a social task).
10. Encourage not on ly the creation o f
3. Recognize the value of conceptualizing
stimulating and interesting environ-
assessment and intervention activities
ments but also the ways in which knowl-
within a developmental model that
edge obtain~d from those environ-
considers the influence of various con-
ments can be incorporated into shared
texts.
experiences that form the foundations
4. Emphasize parent-child soc ial and
for socially competent exchanges.
emotional relationships rather than
parent-child instructional or didactic \ilany or eve n most of these principles or
types of relationships. practices can be derived from other frame-
5. L'tilize the construct of social compe- works in the field of early intervention. In
tence to provide a framework for orga- addition, there are important aspects of
nizing discrete curricular activities early intervention programs that benefit lit-
often associated with individual devel- tle or not at all from considerations of chil-
o pmental domains. dren's social competence. :\"evenheless, a
6. Require consideration of the impor- refined and well~articulated construct of
tance of underlying processes such as social competence may well have the un-
those related to attention, shared un- usual capacity for encouraging new ap-
derstanding, and emotion regulation, proaches and organizing perhaps rhe most
a nd develop any adaptations and ad- important features of the early intervention
justments that may be required. enterprise.

REFERENCES
Abidin, R.R. (1990). Parenting Stress Index (3rd .-\rend, R .. Gove. F., & Sroufe, L.A. (1979). Conti-
ed.). Charlottesville, VA: Pediatric Psychology nuity of individual adaptation from infancy to
Press. kindergarten: .-\ predictive study of ego-
Affleck, G., McGrade, BJ., McQueeney, M., & resiliency and curiosity in preschoolers. Child
Allen, D. (1982). Promise of relationship- Development, 50, 950-959.
focused early intervention in developmental dis- .-\sher, S.R. (1983). Social competence and peer
abilities. Journal of special Education, 16, 413- 430. status: Recent advances and future directions.
.\inswonh, M.D.S. (1979). Infant-mother attach- Child Development, 54, 1427-1434.
ment. ,-\merican Psychologist, 34, 932-937. Bailey. D.S. ( 1987). Collaborative goal-setting with
Ainsworth, M.D.S., Blehar, M.C., Waters, E., & families: Reso lving differences in values and
\.\'all, S. ( 1978). Patterns of attachment. Hillsdale, priorities for services. Topics in Early Childhood
:--;J : Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Special Education, 7, 59-71.
Amia, S.D.. & Kreimeyer, K.H. (1987). The effect Bailey. D.B.. Jr., & Winton, PJ. (1989). Friendship
of social skill training on the peer interaction of and acquaintance amon g families in a main-
preschool hearing-impaired children. Journal of streamed day care center. Education and Train-
the Division for Early Childhood, 11, 206- 216. ing in ,Wental Retardation, 2 4, 107-113.
Amia, S.D.. & Kreimeyer. K.H . ·(1988). Mainte· Bailey, D.B.. Jr., & Wolery, M. (Eds. ). (1989). .-\s-
nance of positive peer interaction in preschool sessing infants and prescho<Jl.ers with . handicaps.
hearing-impaired children. Volta Review, 90(7), Columbus, OH: Charles E. Merrill.
325-337. Barnard, K.E., & Kelly, J.F. (1990). Assessment of
604 GL:RALNICK AND NEVILLE

parent-child interaction. In SJ. \feisels & J.P. opment among mothers of preschoolers with disabili-
Shonkoff (Eds.), Handbook of earl_v chiUl.hood in- ties. Paper presented at the meecing of the In-
tervention (pp. 278-302). Cambridge. England: ternational Society for the Scudy of Behavioral
Cambridge L"niversity Press. Disorders, .-\mscerdam, Netherlands.
Bates, E. (1975). Peer relations and the acquisi- Booth, C.L., Rose-Krasnor, L., & Rubin, K.H.
tion of language. In \(. Lewis & L..-\. Rosen- (1991). Relating preschoolers' social compe-
blum (Eds.). The origins ofbehavior: Vol. 4. Friend- tence and their mothers' parenting behaviors
ship and peer relations (pp. 259-292). New York: to early attachment security and high-risk sta-
John Wiley & Sons. tus. journal ofSocial and Personal Rel.a.tionships, 8,
Baumrind. D. ( 1971). Current patterns of parental 363- 382.
authority. Developmental Psychology .'vfonograph, Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol. 2. Sepa-
4(1, Pt. 2). ration. New York: Basic Books.
Beck.man, PJ., & Lieber,J. (1994). The Social Strat- Bowlby, J. ( 1980). Attachment and loss: Vol. 3. Loss.
egy Rating Scale: An approach to evaluating so- New York: Basic Books.
cial competence. Journal of Early Intervention, Boyum, L. (1991. April). Family emotional expres-
18, 1-11. siveness: A possible a.ntecedent of chil,dren's social
Beckwith, L. (1990). Adaptive and maladapcive competence. Poster presented at the Biennial
parenting-implications for intervention. In SJ. ~eeting of the Society for Research in Child
~eisels &J.P. Shonkoff (Eds.), Handbook of early Dt;velopment, Seattle, WA.
chiU/.hood intervention (pp. 53-77). Cambridge, Bredekamp, S. (Ed.). (1987). Developmentally a~
England: Cambridge University Press. propriate practice in early childhood programs sero-
Beeghly, M., Weiss-Perry, B., & Cicchetti, D. ( 1989). i ng chil,dren from fJirth through age 8. Washington,
Scructural and affettive dimensions of play DC: National Association for the Education of
development in young children wich Down Young Children.
syndrome. International Journal of Behavioral De- Brethercon, I. (1985). Attachment theory: Retro-
velopment, 12, 257-277. spect and prospect. Monographs of the Society for
Belsky.]. (1984). The determinants of parenting: Research in Chil,d Development, 50(1-2, Serial No.
A process model. Child Development, 55, 83-96. 209), 3-35.
Belsky, J., Robins, E., & Gamble, W. (1984). The Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human
determinants of parental competence: Toward dl!velopmmt: Experiments by nature and dl!sign.
a contextual theory. In ~- Lewis (Ed.), Beyond Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
the dyad (pp. 251-279). New York: Plenum. Bryant, B. (1985). The neighborhood walk:
Belsky, J., Ravine, M., & Taylor, D.G. (1984). The Sources of support in middle childhood. Maner
Pennsylvania Infam and Family Development r;raph.s of the Society for Research in Child DevelOf>-
Project III: The origins of individual differ- ment, 50(3, Serial No. 210).
ences in infant-mother attachment: Maternal Buck, R. (1975). Nonverbal communication of af-
and infant contributions. Chif,d Development, 55, fect in children. Journal of Personality and Social
718-728. Psychology, 31, 644-653.
Bennecc, F.C.. & Guralnick, MJ. (1992). Promot- Buium, N., Rynders.J., &: Turnure,J. (1974). Early
ing development and integration of infants ex- maternal linguistic environment of normal and
periencing neonatal intensive care. In K.A. Har- Down's syndrome language-learning children.
ing, D.L. Lovett, & NJ. Haring (Eds.), Integrated AmericanJournal of Mental Deficimcy, 79, 52-58.
life cycle services for persons with disabilities: A therr Burks, V.M., & Parke, R.D. (1991,July). Parent and
retical and empirical perspecti~ (pp. 198-219). child representations of social relationships: Media-
~ew York: Springer-Verlag. tors of chil,dren' social adjustment. Paper pre-
Berg, \l., & ~edrich, E.A. (1980). Children in sented at the International Society for the
four neighborhoods: Physical environment and Study of Behavioral Development, Minneapo-
its effects on play and play patterns. Environ- lis, MN. .
ment and Behavior, 12, 320-348. Capps, L., Sigman, M., & Mundy, P. (1994). At-
Bhamagri, N.P.. & Parke, R.D. (1991). Parents as tachment security in children with autism. De-
direcc facilitators of children's peer relation- velopment and Psychopathowgy. 6, 249-261.
ships: Effects of age of child and sex of parent. Carson, J.L. (1991, April). In search of mediating
journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 8, processes: Emotional cius as linlcs between family and
-123-440. peer systems. Poster presented at the Biennial
Blacher.]., & Mevers, C.E. (1983). A review of at- Meecing of the Society for Research in Child
tachment formation and disorder of handi- Development, Seattle, WA.
capped children . .J..mericanjournal of Mental De- Cassidy,J. (1988). Child-mother attachment and
ficiency, 87, 359-371. the self in 6-year-olds. Chif,d Development, 59,
Booch, C.L. (1994). Beliefs abou.l social-skills dl!vel- 121-134.
PROMOTl~G CHILDREN'S SOCIAL CO\.IPETE'.'.'CE 605
- - -- --- - -- - -- - - - --
Casto. G .. & \lastropieri. \.l.A. (1986). The effi- Dodge. K..\ .. \lcClaske\·, C.\ I.. & Feldman. E.
cacy of earl~· inter\'ention programs: .\ meta- (1985 ). A situati o nal ·approach to the assess-
anal~·sis. Exceptional Children, n, 41 i--124. ment of social competence in children. Journal
Cicchetti, D. , & Beeghly, M. (1990). :\.n organiza- of Consulting an.d Clinical Psycholog_v, 33, 3H-:353.
tional approach to the studv of Down syndrome: Dodge. K.:\., Pettit, G.S., \.kClaskev, C.L. &
Contributions to an integrative theory of de- Brown, \.1..\-l. (1986). Social compete~ce in chil-
\'elopment. In D. Cicchetti & M. Beeghly (Eds. ), dren . .'vfonographs of the Society for Research in
Children with Down syndrow: A. developmental per- Child Development, 51 (2. Serial :\'o. 213).
spective (pp. 29-62). Cambridge, England: Cam- Dumas, J.E., Lafreniere, Pj.. Beaudin, L., & \er-
bridge L'niversity Press. laan, P. (1992). \fother-child interactions in
Cicchetti, D.. & Serafica, F.C. (1981 ). Interplay competent and aggressi\'e dvads: Implications
among behavioral systems: Illustrations from of relationship stress for beha\'iour therap~· with
the study of attachment, affiliation, and wari- families. :'vew Zealand journal of Psychology. 21.
ness in young children with Down's syndrome. 3-13.
Developmental Psychology, 17, 36-49. Dunst, CJ., Trivette, C.\.l., & Deal. :\ .G. ( 1988).
Cohn, D.A. (1990). Child-mother attachment in Enabling and empowering families: Principles and
six-year-olds and social competer:ice a·t school. guidelines for practice. Cambridge. \IA: Brook-
Child Developwnt, 61, 152-162. line Books.
Collin, M.F., Halsey, C.L., & Anderson, C.L. (1991). Eheart, S..K. (1982). Mother-child interactions
Emerging deveiopmental sequelae in the "nor- with nonretarded and mentallv retarded
mal" extremely low birth weight infant. Pedi- preschoolers. American Journal of \Iental Defi-
atrics, 88, 115-120. · ciency, 87, 20-25.
Comfort, M.. & Farran, D.C. (1994). Parent-child Elicker.]., Englund. ~l.. & Sroufe. LA. (1992). Pre-
interaction assessment in family-centered inter- dicting peer competence and peer relationships
\'ention. Infants and Young Children, 6, 33-45. in childhood from early parent-child relation-
Crick, N.R .. & Dodge, K.A. (1994). A review and ships. In R.D. Parke & G.W. Ladd (Eds.), Family-
reformulation of social information-processing peer relationships: ,'vlodes of linkage (pp. 77- 106).
mechanisms in children's social adjustment. Psy- Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
chological BuUetin, 115, 74-101. Emde, R.N .. Katz, E.L., & Thorpe. J.K. (l9i8).
Crnic, K.A.. Greenberg, M.T.. Ragozin, A.S .. Emotional expression in infancy: II. Early devi-
Robinson, N.M., & Basham, R.B. (1983). Effects ations in Down's svndrome. In \.l. Lewis & L..\.
of stress and social support on mothers and Rosenblum (Eds.), The development of affect
premature and full-term infants. Child Devel<>f>- (pp. 351-360). New York: Plenum.
ment, 54, 209-217. Erickson, M.F., Sroufe, L.A., & Egeland. B. (1985).
Crockenberg, S. (1981). Infant irritability, mother The relationship between quality of attachment
responsiveness, and social support influences and behavior problems in preschool in a high-
on the security of infant-mother attachment. risk sample. ,'vfonographs of the Society for Research
Child Development, 52, 857-865. · in Child Development, 50(1-2, Serial No. 209),
Cunningham, C.E., Reuter, E .. Blackwell, j.. & 147-166.
Deck. J. ( 1981 ). Behavioral and linguistic devel- Erwin, E.J. (1994). Social competence in .young
opments in the interactions of normal and re- children with visual impairments. Infants and
tarded children with their mothers. Child Devel- Young Children, 6, 26-33.
opment, 52, 62-70. Field, T.M. (1980). Self, teacher. toy. and peer-
DeKlyen, M.. & Odom, S.L. (1989). Activity struc- directed behaviors of handicapped preschool
ture and social interaction with peers in devel- children. In T.M. Field, S. Goldberg, D. Stern, &
opmentally integrated play groups. journal of A.M. Sostek ( Eds.), High-risk infants and chil-
Earl_y Intervention, 13, 342-352. dren: Adult and peer interactions (pp. 313-326).
Dodge, K.A. (1986). A social information proc- New York: Academic Press.
essing model of social competence in children. Field, T. (1983). High-risk infants "have less fun"
In M. Perlmutter (Ed.), Minnesota Symposia on during early interactions. Topics in Early Child-
Child Ps_vchology: Cognitive perspectives on children s hood Special Education, 3, 77-87.
social and behavioral development (Vol. 18, pp. Field, T.M., & Walden, T.A. ( 1982). Production
77-126). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum As- and discrimination of facial expressions b~:
sociates. preschool children. Child Development, 53,
Dodge. K.A. (1991). Emotion and social informa- 1299-1311.
tion processing. In J. Garber & K.A. Dodge Finnie, V., & Russell , A. (1988). Preschool chil-
( Eds. ), The development of emotion regulation and dren's social status and their mothers' behavior
dysregulntion (pp. 159-181). Cambridge, Eng- and knowledge in the supervisory role. Devel~
land: Cambridge University Press. mental Psychology, 24, 789-801.
606 GCRAL:-.: ICK A:--;O NEVILLE

FJo,·d. FJ.. & Phillippe. !\. ...\. ( 1993). Parental in- Guralnick . .\l.j. ( 1990a i. Peer interactio ns and the
teractio ns with children with ;,ind without de,·elo pment of handicapped children·s social
mental retardation: Behavior management. and communicati\'e competence. In H. Foot.
coerci\·eness. and positi\'e exchange. ;\merican '.'vl. '.'vforgan. & R. Shute ( Eds. ). Childre_n helping
Journal on .\(ental Retardation. 97, 673-684. children (pp. 275-305 ). :\'ew fork: John Wilev &
Frodi ...\., & Thompson. R. (1985). Infants' affec- Sons. .
ti\'e responses in the strange situation: Effects Guralnick. \il.j. (1990b). Social competence and
of prematurit\' and of qualitv of attachment. early inter\'ention. journal of Early Intervention ,
Child Dei1elopment. 56, 1280-1291. 14, 3-14.
Furman. L.:'-i .. & Walden, T.:\.. (1990). Effect of Guralnick . .\1.j. (l 992a). ..\ hierarchical model for
script knowledge on preschool children's com- understanding children·s peer-related social
municati,·e interactions. Developmental Psychol- competence. In S.L. Odom, S.R. \lcConnell , &
ogy. 26, 227-233. .\1.A. McEvoy (Eds.). Social competence of _young
Garve~-, C. ( 1986). Peer relations and the growth of children with disabilities: Issues and strategies for 1n-
communication. In E.C. .\1ueller & C.R. Cooper lervention (pp. 37-64). Baltimore: Paul H:
( Eds.), Process and O'Utcome in peer relationships Brookes Publishing Co.
(pp. 329-345). Orlando; FL Academic Press. Guralnick, M.J. ( l 992b) . ..hsessment ofpeer relations.
Goffman. E. (1963). Stigma. Englewood Cliffs, '.'Jj: Seattle: Cniversity of Washington, Center on
Prentice Hall. Human Development and Disability.
Goldstein, H., \.Vickstrom , S.. Hovson. .\1., Guralnick, .\1.j. (1993a). Developmentally appro-
Jamieson , B., & Odom, S.L. (1988). ·Effects of priate practice in the assessment and interven-
script training on social and communicative in- tion of children's peer relations. Topics in Early
teractions. Education and Treatment of Children, Childhood Special Education, 13(3 ), 3-14-371.
1 J, 97-177. Guralnick, .\1J. (1993b). Second generation re-
Conman, J.~l. (1986). The observation of social search on the effectiveness of earlv intervention.
process. In j.\ll. Cottman & J.G. Parker (Eds.), Early Education and Development, 4: 366- 378.
Conversations of friends: Speculations on affective Guralnick, MJ. ( 199-t ). Social competence with
development (pp. 53-100). :\few York: Cambridge peers: Outcome and process in early childhood
Cni\'ersitv Press. special education. In P.L. Safford ( Ed.), }ear-
Gottman .j.\ll.. & Katz. L. (1989). Effects of mari- book in early childhood education : Ear(y childhood
tal discord on voung children's peer interac- special education ( Vol. 5. pp. 45-7 L). :'llew York:
tion and health. Developmental Psychology. 25, Teacher's College Press.
373-381. Guralnick, .\1.j., & Bennett, f.C. (Eds.). (1987).
Greenberg. \ll.. & Crnic, R. (1988). Longitudinal The effectiveness ofearly intervention for at-risk and
predictors of de,·elopmental status and social handicapped children. :-.iew York: ..\cademic Press.
interaction in preterm and full-term infants at Guralnick, .\1j.. Connor, R., & Hammond, .\1.
age 2. Child Development, 59, 554-5 70. ( 1995 ). Parent perspectives of peer relations
Grossman. K.E.. & Grossman, K. (1991). Attach- and friendships in integrated and specialized
ment qualit~' as an organizer of emotional programs. American journal on Mental Retarda-
and beha,·ioral responses. In C. Parkes, J. tion, 99, 457-476.
Ste\'enso n-Hinde, & P. Marris (Eds.). ..wach- Guralnick, M.J., Connor, R., Hammond. .\1.,
ment across the life cycle (pp. 93-114). New York: Conman, J.M., & Kinnish, K. (in press). The
Routledge. peer relations of preschool children with com-
Guralnick, ~lJ. ( 1986). The peer relations of munication disorders. Child Development.
rnung handicapped and nonhandicapped chil- Guralnick. \il.j.. & Groom.JM. (1985). Correlates
d ren. In P.S. Strain, MJ. Guralnick, & H.M. of peer-related social competence of develop-
\\'alker (Eds.), Children's social behavior: Develop- mentally delayed preschool children. ,\merican
ment, assessment, and modification (pp. 93-140). Journal of Mental Deficienc.v, 90, 140-150.
:S-ew York: ..\cademic Press. Guralnick, M.J.. & Groom,J.M. (1987). The peer
Guralnick. '.'vl.J. (1988). Efficacy research in .early relations of mildly delaved and nonhandi-
childhood inten·ention programs. In S.L. Odom capped preschool children in mainstreamed
& \il.B. Karnes ( Eds.), Early intervention for in- playgroups. Child Development, 58, 1556-1572.
fants and children with handicaps: ,-\n empirical Guralnick, M.J., & Groom,J.M. (1988). Friendships
base (pp. 75-88). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes of preschool children in mainstreamed play-
Publishing Co. groups. Devel.opmental Psychology, 24. 595- 604.
Guralnick, '.'v1.J. ( 1989). Recent developments in Guralnick, .\1J., & Weinhouse, E.M. (1983).
earlv intervention efficacy research: Implica- Child-child social interactions: An analysis of
tions for familv involvement in P.L. 99-457. Top- assessment instruments for young children. Ex-
ics in Early Childhood Special Education, 9(3), 1-17. ceptional Children, 50, 268-27 l.
· PRov1on:-.:c C HILDRE~'s SocIA L C o:'l.1PETE:"CE

G uralnick. \l.J .. & \\"einh ouse. E.\I. 11984). Peer- Jmong preschoo lers: Relat ion t11 .111 ,1rhmc:-m
related socia l imeractinns nf de\'elo pm'entally history. .\'nt• Directions for Chtltl o,,.,rfop111r111. .J .J.
dela\'ed \·o ung children: De\'elopment and char- 51 - 64.
acteristics. Dev11lopmen1al P5_\'chology. 20. 815- Kochanska, G. ( 1992). Chi ldren's interpersonal
~ 27. influence with mothers and peers. Developmen-
Hart. C.H .. De\\'o lf. D.\.I.. \fozniak. P.. & Burts, tal P5ycholog)" 28. 491-499.
D.C. ( 1992l. \(aternal and paternal d isci plinar~· Krauss. \.f.\V., & Jacobs. F. (19901. Famil\' assess-
·m ·les: Relat ions \\'ith preschoolers' pla~·gro und ment: Purposes and techniques. In SJ. \(eisels
heha,·ioral orientations and peer status. Child & J.P. Sho nko ff {Eds.). Handbook of early child-
DPv11lopmenl, 63. 879-892. hood intervention (pp. 303-325). Cambridge.
Hart. C.H .. Ladd. G.W.. & Burleso n. B.R. (1990). England: Cambridge l'ni,·ersitv Press.
Children's expectations of the outcomes of Ladd. G.W. (1991). Family-peer relations during
social strategies: Relations with sociometric childhood: Pathways to competence and pathol·
status and maternal disciplinary styles. Chil.d ogy? journal of Social and Personal Relationships.
Developmenl, 61, 127-137. 8, 307- 314.
Hartup. \V.W. (1983). Peer relations. In E.M. Heth- Ladd, G.W.. & Goiter, B.S. ( 1988 ). Parents· man-
erington {Ed.). P.H. \.Cussen (Series Ed.), Hand- agement ofpreschooler's peer relatio ns: Is it re·
book of child psychology: \ol. -I. Sociali:ation, per- · lated to children's soc ial competence: Det•el<>jJ-
5onalit_v. and social devel<>fJment (pp. 103-196). men1al Ps.vchology, 14, 109-117.
~ew York: John v\'iley & Sons. Ladd. G.W.. & Hart, C.H. (1992). Creating infor·
Higgenbotham, j .. & Baker, B.\.t. (1981). Social mal play opportunities: .\re parents' and
participation and cogniti\'e play differences in preschoolers' initiations related to children 's
hearing-impaired and normally hearing competence with peers? Devel<>jJmental Psychol-
preschoolers. \ 'olta Review, 83, 135-149. ogy, 28, 1179-1187.
Hoga n, .-\.E.. Scott. K.G.. & Bauer. C.R. (1992). Lafreniere. Pj.. & Dumas. J.E. (1 9Y2)..\ transac-
The Adaptive Social Behavior Inventory (.-\SBI): tional analysis of early childhood anxiety and
A. new assessment of social competence in high- social withdrawal. Devel<>jJment and Psych<>jJathol-
risk three-year- o lds. Journal of Ps_~choeducational ogy, 4, 385-402.
:\ ssessment, JO, 230-239. Lafreniere. P.J.. Dumas. J.E .. Capuano. F.. &
Howes, C. (1988). Peer interaction of young chil- Dubeau, D. (1992). The de\'elopment and rnli·
dren . ,l,fonographs of lhe Societ)' for Research in dation of the Preschool Socioaffecti\'e Profile.
Chil.d Development, 53(1. Serial No. 217). Psychological Assessment: ,-\Journal of Consulting
Howes. C .. & Stewart. P. (1987). Child's play with . and Clinical Ps)•chology. 4, H 2- 450.
adults. toys, and peers: .-\n examination of fam- Lafreniere, P.J.. & Sroufe. L..\ . ( 1985). Profiles of
ily and child-care influences. Devel<>jJmmtal Psy- peer competence in the preschool: Interrela-
chology, 23. 4 23-4 30. tions between measures, influence of social
Hundert, j .. & Houghton. A. (1992). Promoting ecology, and relation to attachment history. De-
social interaction of children with disabilities vel<>jJmental Ps_vchology, 21. 56- 69.
in integrated preschools: .-\ failure to general- Landry. S.H .. Chapieski, M.L.. Richardson. M.A ..
ize. Exceptional Chil.dren, 58, 311-320. Palmer.J., & Hall. S. (1990). The social compe·
Isabella , R. (1993). Origins of attachment: Mater- tence of children born prematurely: Effects of
nal interactive behaviors across the first year. medical complications and parent behaviors.
Chil.d Devel<>jJment, 64, 605-621 . Chil.d Devel<>flment, 61, 1605-1616.
Jennings. K.D.. Stagg. V., & Connors, R.E. (1991 ). Landry, S.H .. Garner. P.W.. Pirie, D.. & Swank,
Social networks and mothers' interaClions with P.R. (1994 ). Effects of social context and moth-
their preschool children. Chil.d Devel<>jJment, 62. ers' requesting strategies on Down's syndrome
966-978. children's social responsiveness. Devel<>jJmeiital
Jones. O.H.M. (1980). Prelinguistic communica- Psychology, 30, 293-302.
tion skills in Down's svndrome and normal in- Lederberg, A.R .. & Mobley, C.E. (1990). The ef·
fants. ln T.\1. Field, S. Goldberg, D. Stern, & feet of hearing impairment on the quality of
.\ .\IL Sostek ( Eds.), High-risk infants and chil- attachment and mother-toddler interaction .
dren: .-\dult and peer interactions (pp. 205-225 ). Chil.d Devel<>jJment, 61, 1596-1604.
:--.;ew York: Academic Press. Lewis, M.. Feiring, C .. & Brooks-Gunn.J. (1987).
Katz, L.F.. & Cottman, J.M. (1993). Patterns of The social networks of children with and with-
marital conflic t predict children's internalizing out handicaps: A developmental perspective. In
and externalizing behaviors. Devel<>fJmental Psy· S. Landesman & P. Vietze (Eds.). Living enui-
chology. 29, 940-950. ronmmt.s and mental retardation (pp. 377-400).
Kestenbaum, R.. Farber. E.A., & Sroufe, L.A. Washington, DC: American .\ssociation on
(1989). Individual differences in empathy Mental Retardation.
608 Gt..:RAL:--JICK .-\~D NEVILLE

Lieber.J.. Beckm an. P.J.. &: Strong. BS. ( 1993). :\ deaf and hearing preschoolers. journn./ ,,l 'ipnrh
lo ngitudinal s tud ~· of the social exchanges of and Hearing Research, 25. 48/-499.
\·oung children with disabilities. journal of Ear(v ~tedrich . E.:\ .. Ro izen. J.A .. Rubin. V.. & Buckle,·.
Intervention. II. 116-1 28. S. ( 1982). The serious business of growing up: .-\
Lieberman ...\.F. (1977). Preschoolers' compe- · study of children's lives outside school. Berkeley:
tence with a peer: Relations with . attachment L'niversitv of California Press.
a~d peer experience. Child Development, 48. \-tel son. G.f.. Ladd , G. W.. & Hsu. H.C. ( 1993 ). Ma-
1277-1 287. ternal support networks, maternal cognitions,
Lieberman . .-\.F.. \\"eston. D.R .. & Pawl,J.H. (1991). and young children's soc ial and cogn iti\'e de-
Pre,·enti\'e intervention and outcome with anx- velopment. Child Development. 64. 1401-1-117.
iouslv attached dyads. Child Development, 62, ~tills, R.S.L. , & Rubin, K.H. (1990). Parental be-
199-209. liefs about problematic social behaviors in early
MacDonald. K.. & Parke. R.D. (1984). Bridging childhood. Child Development, 61, 138-151.
the gap: Parent-c hild play interaction and peer Mills, R.S.L., & Rubin, K.H. (1992). :\ longitudi-
interactive competence. Child Development, 55, nal study o f maternal beliefs about children's
1265-1277. social behaviors. .'Werrill-Paliner Quarterly, 38,
Maho ney, G.J., Fors. S.. & Wood , S. ( 1990). Mater- 494-512.
nal directive behavior revisited. ,-\merican Jour- Mize, J. , & Ladd, G.W. (1990). A cognitive-social
nal on .\ifental Retardation, 94, 398-406. learning approach to social skill training with
Mahonev, G., Robinson, C .. & Powell. A. (1992). low-status preschool children . Developmental Psy-
Focusing o n parent-child interaction: The chology, 26, 388-397.
bridge to developmentally appropriate prac- Mize,J., Pettit, G.S., & Brown, E.G. (1995). Moth-
tices. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, ers' supervision of their children's peer play:
12. 105-120. Relations with beliefs, perceptio ns, and knowl-
Main, \-t, & Solomon. J. (1986). Discovery of an edge. Developmental Psychology, 31, 311-321.
insecure disorganized/ disoriented attachment Mueller. E., & Brenner. J. ( 1977). The origins of
pattern: Procedures, findings and implications social skills and interaction among playgroup
fo r the classification of behavior. In M. Yogman toddlers. Child Develop~nt, 48, 854- 861.
& T.B. Brazelto n ( Eds.), Affective development in Nelson, K., & Seidman, S. (1984). Playing with
infancy (pp. 94-124). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. scripts. In I. Bretherton (Ed.), Symbolic play: The
Marfo, K. ( Ed. ). (1988 ). Parent-child interaction development of social understanding (pp. 45-71 ).
and developmental disabilities: Theory, research, New York: Academic Press.
and intervention . New York: Praeger. Odom, S.L., & Brown, W.H. (1993). Social inter-
~tarfo. K. ( 1990). ~taternal directiveness in inter- action skills interventions fo r young children
actions with mentally handicapped children: with disabilities in integrated settings. In C.A.
.-\n analytical co mmentary. journal of Child Psy- Peck, S.L. Odom, & D.O. Bricker (Eds.), 'Inte-
chology and Psychiatry, 31, 531-549. grating young children with disabilities into commu-
Marfo. K. (1991 ). The maternal directiveness nity programs: E~ological pmpectives on research
theme in mother-child interaction research: and implementation (pp. 39-64 ). Baltimore: Paul
Implications for early intervention. In K. Marfo H . Brookes Publishing Co .
( Ed.), Earl_v in tervention in transition (pp. 177- Odom. S.L.. McConnell, S.R .• & McEvoy, M.A.
203). New York: Praeger. ( Eds.). ( 1992). Social competence ofyoung children
~tarkovits, H .. & Strayer, F.F. (1982). Toward an with disabilities: Issues and strategies for interven-
applied social ethology: A case study of social tion. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co.
skills among blind children. In K.H. Rubin & Odom, S.L., & Strain, P.S. (1986). A comparison
H.S. Ross ( Eds.), Peer relationships and social skills of peer-initiation and teacher-antecedent inter-
in childhood (pp. 301-322). New York: Springer- ventions for promoting reciprocal social inter-
Verlag. · action of autistic preschoolers. Journal of A~
\fartinez, ~L\ . ( 1987). Dialogues among children plied &haviOT Analysis, 19, 59-71 .
and between children and their mothers. Child Park, K.A .. & Waters, E. (1989). Security of at-
Devel~ent, 58, 1035-1043. tachment and preschool friendships. Child De-
'.\-kEvoy, ~L\. , Odom, S.L.. & McConnell, S.R. ve!.opment, 60, 1076-1081.
(1992). Peer s_oc ial competence intervention for Parke, R.D.. Burks, V.M., Carson,J.L., Neville, 8 .,
young children with disabilities. In S.L. Odom, & Boyum, L. (1994). Family-peer relationships:
S.R. McConnell. & M.A. McEvov (Eds.), Social A tripartite model. In R.D. Parke & S.G. Kellam
competence ofyoung children with disabilities: issues ( Eds.), Explaring family relationships with other scr
and strategies for intervention (pp. 113-133). Bal- cial contexts (pp. 115-145). Hillsdale, NJ:
timore: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
McKirdy, L.S., & Blank, M. (1982). Dialogue in Parke, R.D., Cassidy, J ... Burks, V.M.. Carson, J.L..
PRO\.IOTI ~G CHILDRE~'s SOCIAL Co\.1PETE'.':C E 6\)Q

& Bornm. L. ( 1992). Familial comribu lions LO nal of Developmental nnd 81'hat·ioml PPdintnr~. 4.
peer competence among young children: The 73-78:
role of interacti,·e and affecLive processes. ln Rice. \.LL.. Sell. '.\I..\ .. & Hadle'" P..\. \199 1). So-
R.D. Parke & G.W. Ladd ( Eds.). Family-peer rela- cial interactions of speech~ and language-
twnships: .\(odes of linkage lpp. 107-1 3-l ). Hills- impaired children.journal of Speech and Hearing
dale. :'\j: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Research, 34. 1299-1307.
Parke. R.D.. & Ladd. G.W. ( Eds.). (1992). Familv- Ross, G .. Lipper, E.G .. & Auld. P..\.\.I. ( 1990). 5<>-
peer relationships: .Hodes of linkage. Hillsdale. :--ij: cial competence and beha,·ior problems in pre-
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. mature children at school age. Pediatrics, 86.
Parker.JG .. & Asher, S.R. (1987). Peer relations and 391-397.
later personal adjustment: Are low-accepted chil- Rubin. K.H., & Coplan, RJ. ( 1992). Peer relatio n-
dren at risk? Psychowgical Bulktin, 102, 357- 389. ships in childhood. In \.l.H. Bornstein & \.I.E.
Pastor. D.L. (1981). The qualitv of mother-infant Lamb (Eds.), Developmental psychology: .-\n ad-
attachment and its relationship to toddlers' ini- vanced textbook (3rd ed., pp. 519-578). Hillsdale.
tial sociability with peers. Developmental fsychol- NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
ogy, 17. 326-335. Rubin, K.H ., & Krasnor, LR. (1986). Social-
Pettit. G.S., Dodge, K.A. , & Brown, M.M. (1988). cognitive and social behavioral perspectives on
Early family experience, social problem solving problem solving. ln M. Perlmutter ( Ed.), .\lin-
patterns, and childrens' social competence. nesota Symposia on Child Psychology: ~al. l 8. Cog-
Child Development, 59, 107-120. nitive perspectives on children ssocial and behavioral
Pianta. R.C., & Ball, R.M. (1993). Maternal social development (pp. 1-68). Hillsdale. '.\:j: Lawrence
support as a predictor of child adjustment in Erlbaum Associates.
kindergarten. Journal of Applied Developmental Rubin, K.H .. Le Mare, L., & Lollis, S. ( 1990). Ser
Psychology, 14, 107-120. cial withdrawal in childhood: Deve!Opmemal
Poresky. R.D.. & Hooper, DJ. (1984). Enhancing pathways to peer rejection. In S.R . .\sher &JD.
prosocial play between handicapped and non- Coie (Eds.), Children's status in the peer group
handicapped preschool children. Psychological (pp. 217-249). New York: Cambridge Cniver-
Reports, 54, 391-402. sity Press.
Putallaz, M. (1983). Predicting children's soci<>- Rubin, K.H .. & Lollis, S.P. (1988). Origins and
metric status from their behavior. Child Develop- consequences of social withdrawal. ln J Belskv
ment, 54, 1417-1426. & T. Nezworski (Eds.), Clinical implications of at-
Putallaz, M. (1987). Maternal behavior and chil- tachment (pp. 219-252). Hillsdale, :-.ij: Lawrence
dren's sociometric status. Child Development, 58, Erlbaum Associates.
324-340. Rubin , K.H .. & Mills, R.S.L. ( 1990). Maternal be-
Putallaz, M., Costanzo, P.R., & Smith, R.B. (1991). liefs about adaptive and maladaptive social
:Vlaternal recollections of childhood peer rela- behaviors in normal, aggressive, and with-
tionships: Implications for their children's s<>- drawn preschoolers. journal of .-\bnormal Child
cial competence. Journal of Social and Personal Ps_ychology, 18, 419-435.
Relationships, 8, 403-422. Rubin, Z., & Slomin.J. (1984). How parents influ-
PuLallaz, M., & Heflin, A.H. (1990). Parent-child ence their children's friendships. In :Vt. Lewis
interaction. In S.R. Asher & J.D. Coie (Eds.). (Ed.). Beyo11d the dyad (pp. 223-250). :-,;ew York:
Peer rejection in childhood (pp. 189-216). Cam- Plenum.
bridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Russell, A .. & Finnie, V. (1990). Preschool chil-
Quay, LC .. &Jarrett, Q.S. (1984). Predictors of so- dren's social status and maternal instructions
cial acceptance in preschool children. Develop- LO assist group entry. Developmental Psychology.
mental Psychology, 20, 793-796. 26, 603-611.
Quirk, M., Sexton, M., Ciottone, R., Minami. H., Sainato, D.M .. & Cana,JJ. (1992). Classroom in-
& Wapner, S. (1984). Values held by mothers for fh..iences on the development of social compe-
handicapped and nonhandicapped preschool- tence in young children with disabilities. ln S.L.
ers . •Werrill-Palmer Quarterly, 30, 403-418. Odom, S.R. McConnell, & M.A. McEvoy ( Eds.),
Rauh , VA., Achenbach, T.M .. Nurcombe, B., How- Social competence ofyoung children with disabilities:
ell, C.T.. & Teti, D.M. (1988). Minimizing ad- Issues and strategies for intervention (pp. 93-109).
verse effects of low binhweight: Four-year re- Baltimore: Paul H . Brookes Publishing Co.
sults of an early intervention program. Child Schank, R.C .. & Abelson , R.P. (1977). Scripts, plans,
Development, 59, 544-553. goals and understanding: An inquiry into human
Resnick, M.B.. Armstrong. S., & Carter, R.L. knowledgt structures. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Er I-
(1988). Developmental intervention program baum Associates.
for high-risk premature infants: Effects on de- Spieker, SJ., & Booth, C.L. (1988). Maternal an-
velopment and parent-infant interactions. four- tecedents of attachment quality. ln J Belsky &
610 G C RAL~ICK A :'-i D NEV IL LE

T. :"\ezwo rski ( Eds.). (.'finical 1mplicatzons of at- Vande ll. D.L.. & George. L.B. (198 11. Social incer-
tachment (pp. 95-135). Hillsdale. :"\j: Lawrence act ion in hearing and deaf presc hoolers: Suc-
Erlbaum ..\ssociales. cesses and failures in initiations. Child Develop-
Spiker. D.. Ferguson .].. & Brooks-Gunn .J. (l993). ment, 52. 627-635.
Enhancing male rn al interacci\'e beha,·io r and \'an den Boom . D.C. (1994 ). The influence of tem-
child soc ial competence in low birch weighl. perament and mmhering o n attachment and
premature infants. Child Develt>pment, 64, explo ration: An experime ntal manipulation
754-768. of sensitive responsi\'eness among lower-class
Sroufe, L.A. ( 1983). Infant-caregiver auachmem mothers and irritable infants. Child Develt>pment,
and panerns o f adaplation in preschool: The 65, 1457-1477.
roots of maladaptation and competence. In Vaughn , B.E.. Goldberg, S., Atkinson. L.. \.larco\'-
M. Perlmutter (Ed.), The :vtinnesota Symposia itch, S., MacGregor, D.. & Seifer. R. ( 1994 ).
on Child Psychology: Vol. 16. Develt>pment and policy Quality of toddler-mother altachment in chil-
concerning children with special needs (pp. 41-83). dren with Down syndro me: Lim its to interpre-
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. tatio n o f strange situation behavior. Child De-
Sroufe. L.A .. & Fleeson.J. (1986). Attachment and velt>pment, 65, 95-108.
the constructio n of relationships. In W. Hartup Wasserman, G.A .. Lennon, M.C .. Allen , R.. & Shi-
& Z. Rubin (Eds.). Relationships within families lanskv, M. (1987). Contributors to attachment
(pp. 57-71 ). Hillsdale, '.'lj: Lawrence Erlbaum in no.rmal and physically handicapped infants.
Associales. journal of the American Academy of Child and A.d<>-
Stahlecker, J.E .. & Cohen, ~LC . (1985 ). Applica- lescent Psychiatry, 26, 9-15.
tion of the Strange Situation attachment para- Waters, E., & Deane. K .E. (1985). Defining and as-
digm co a neurologically impaired population. sessing individual differences in attachment
Child Develt>pment, 56, 502-507. relatio nships: Q-methodology and the organi-
Strain. P.S.. Guralnick. MJ., & Walker, H.M. ( Eds.). zation o f behavior in infancy and early ch ild-
(1986). Children ssocial behavior: Develt>pment, as- hood. Monographs of tht Society for Research in
sessment, and modification. :-Jew York: Academic Child Develt>pmtnt, 50 (1-2, Se rial :--Jo. 209).
Press. 41-65.
Taft, L.A. (1983). Critique of early intervention Waters, E., Wippman. J., & Sroufe, L.A. ( 19i9).
for cerebral palsy. In T.B. Brazelton & B.M. Attachment, positive affect, and competence in
Lester ( Eds.) . .Yew approaches to develt>pmental the peer gro up: Two studies in construct vali-
screening of infants (pp. 2 19-228). New York: El- dation. Child Develt>pment, 50, 821-829.
sevier. Wright, M.J. (1980). Measuring the social compe-
Ta~'lor. SJ. (1988). Caught in the co minuum: A tence of preschool children. Canadianjournal of
critical analysis of the principle of the least re- Behavioral Science, 12, 17-32.
strictive environment. joumal of The Association Youngblade, L.M.. & Belsky. J. (1992).
for Persons with Severe Handicaps, JJ, 41-53. Parent-child antecedents of 5-year-olds' close
Terdal. L.,Jackson. R.H .. & Garner, A.M. (1976). friendships: A longitudinal analysis. Develop-
\.fother- child interactions: A co mparison be- mental Psychology, 28, 700-713.
tween no rmal and developmentally delayed Zigler. E.. & Trickett, P.K. (1978). IQ, social com-
groups. In E.J. Mash, L.A. Hamerlynck, & L.C. petence, and evaluation of early childhood in-
Handy ( Eds.), Behavior modification and families tervention programs. ,\merican Psychologist. 33,
(pp. 249-264 ). New York: Brunner/ Maze!. 789-798.

You might also like