Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 8

Research

Original Investigation

Association of Lifetime Intellectual Enrichment


With Cognitive Decline in the Older Population
Prashanthi Vemuri, PhD; Timothy G. Lesnick, MS; Scott A. Przybelski, BS; Mary Machulda, PhD, LP;
David S. Knopman, MD; Michelle M. Mielke, PhD; Rosebud O. Roberts, MB, ChB; Yonas E. Geda, MD;
Walter A. Rocca, MD, MPH; Ronald C. Petersen, PhD, MD; Clifford R. Jack Jr, MD

Supplemental content at
IMPORTANCE Intellectual lifestyle enrichment throughout life is increasingly viewed as a jamaneurology.com
protective strategy against commonly observed cognitive decline in the older population.

OBJECTIVES To investigate the association of lifetime intellectual enrichment with baseline


cognitive performance and rate of cognitive decline in an older population without dementia
and to estimate the years of protection provided against cognitive impairment by these
factors.

DESIGN, SETTING, AND PARTICIPANTS Prospective analysis of individuals enrolled from


October 1, 2004, and in 2008 and 2009 in the Mayo Clinic Study of Aging, a longitudinal,
population-based study of cognitive aging in Olmsted County, Minnesota. We studied 1995
individuals without dementia (1718 cognitively normal individuals and 277 individuals with
mild cognitive impairment) who completed intellectual lifestyle enrichment measures at
baseline and underwent at least 1 follow-up visit.

MAIN OUTCOMES AND MEASURES We studied the effect of lifetime intellectual enrichment by
separating the variables into 2 nonoverlapping principal components: education/occupation
score and mid/late-life cognitive activity based on self-report questionnaires. A global
cognitive z score served as the summary cognition measure. Linear mixed-effects models
were used to investigate the associations of demographic and intellectual enrichment
measures with global cognitive z score trajectories.

RESULTS Baseline cognitive performance was lower in older individuals; individuals with lower
education/occupation score, lower mid/late-life cognitive activity, and APOE genotype; and
men (P < .001). The interaction between the 2 intellectual enrichment measures was significant
(P < .03) such that the beneficial effect of mid/late-life cognitive activity on baseline cognitive
performance was reduced with increasing education/occupation score. Only baseline age,
mid/late-life cognitive activity, and APOE4 genotype were significantly associated with
longitudinal change in cognitive performance from baseline (P < .05). For APOE4 carriers with
high lifetime intellectual enrichment (75th percentile of education/occupation score and midlife Author Affiliations: Department of
Radiology, Mayo Clinic, Rochester,
to late-life cognitive activity), the onset of cognitive impairment was approximately 8.7 years
Minnesota (Vemuri, Jack);
later compared with low lifetime intellectual enrichment (25th percentile of education/ Department of Health Sciences
occupation score and mid/late-life cognitive activity). Research, Mayo Clinic, Rochester,
Minnesota (Lesnick, Przybelski,
Mielke, Roberts, Rocca); Department
CONCLUSIONS AND RELEVANCE Higher education/occupation scores were associated with
of Psychology, Mayo Clinic,
higher levels of cognition. Higher levels of mid/late-life cognitive activity were also associated Rochester, Minnesota (Machulda,
with higher levels of cognition, but the slope of this association slightly increased over time. Geda); Department of Neurology,
Lifetime intellectual enrichment might delay the onset of cognitive impairment and be used Mayo Clinic, Rochester, Minnesota
(Knopman, Roberts, Rocca,
as a successful preventive intervention to reduce the impending dementia epidemic. Petersen); Department of Psychiatry,
Mayo Clinic, Scottsdale, Arizona
(Geda); Department of Neurology,
Mayo Clinic, Scottsdale, Arizona
(Geda).
Corresponding Author: Prashanthi
Vemuri, PhD, Department of
Radiology, Mayo Clinic and
Foundation, 200 First St SW,
JAMA Neurol. 2014;71(8):1017-1024. doi:10.1001/jamaneurol.2014.963 Rochester, MN 55905 (vemuri
Published online June 23, 2014. .prashanthi@mayo.edu).

1017

Copyright 2014 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: http://archneur.jamanetwork.com/ by a Purdue University User on 05/22/2015


Research Original Investigation Intellectual Enrichment and Cognitive Decline

T
he older population in the United States is expected to Educational attainment is self-reported and based on the num-
more than double from 35 million in 2000 to 72 mil- ber of years of school completion. The job level score is based
lion in 2030.1 Commonly observed cognitive decline on the participants’ primary occupation during most of their
in the older population due to the pathologic aging of the adult life. All the occupations were then assigned to 1 of 6
brain will have a significant effect on public health. Intellec- groupings based on similar attributes and complexity of the
tual lifestyle enrichment throughout life is increasingly jobs. Details about the cognitive activity questionnaires used
viewed as a protective strategy against cognitive decline in for recording are available in the eAppendix in the Supple-
the older population. Numerous studies have found that all ment. The same form was used to record their cognitive ac-
components of intellectual enrichment, including higher tivities during the past 12 months (late life) and cognitive ac-
lifetime nonleisure learning components, such as education tivities at 50 to 65 years of age (midlife). Each component score
and primary occupation, 2-6 and cognitively stimulating is weighted based on the amount of activity participation. The
activities,7-10 are protective against cognitive decline and Alz- 10 cognitive activities are added to determine the cognitive ac-
heimer disease (AD)–related dementia. Intellectual enrich- tivity score. Television, which is the 11th component that is cap-
ment may succeed as a preventive intervention if we under- tured, is not included in the final cognitive activity score.17
stand the relative influence of intellectual enrichment factors Using principal components applied to these 4 measures
on baseline cognitive performance and rate of decline and (ie, educational attainment, occupational score, midlife cog-
estimate the years of protection provided against cognitive nitive activity, and late-life cognitive activity), we separated
impairment by these factors. the uncorrelated components of early-life nonleisure activity
Lifetime intellectual enrichment can be grouped into 2 and mid/late-life cognitive activity. The first 2 principal com-
major components: early life and midlife noncognitive activi- ponents explained 84% of the variance, and after a varimax
ties, such as educational attainment and major occupation, rotation, the data were consolidated into 2 distinct composite
and mid/late-life cognitive activity. In this study, we separate measures of intellectual enrichment: education/occupation
these 2 components and closely examine their effects on score (ie, lifelong nonleisure intellectual learning) assessed
baseline cognition and the subsequent rate of cognitive from years of education/occupation score (weighted contri-
decline in a population-based sample of older individuals bution was 0.688 for years of education and 0.725 for occupa-
without dementia. The number of years of protection pro- tional score) and mid/late-life cognitive activity from a self-
vided by each component was estimated for subsequent report of cognitive activities in the previous 12 months and at
onset of cognitive impairment. 50 to 60 years of age (weighted contribution was 0.708 for
midlife and 0.700 for cognitive activity in the previous 12
months).

Methods
Global Cognition Measure
Selection of Participants The neuropsychological battery of tests was constructed as pre-
This study was approved by the Mayo Clinic Institutional Re- viously described.13-15 Four cognitive domains were assessed
view Board, and written informed consent was obtained from from 9 tests: executive (Trail Making Test Part B and Wechs-
all participants or their surrogates. Study participants were in- ler Adult Intelligence Scale–Revised Digit Symbol), language
dividuals from the Mayo Clinic Study of Aging (MCSA), an epi- (Boston Naming Test and category fluency), memory (Wechs-
demiologic study of the prevalence, incidence, and risk fac- ler Memory Scale–Revised Logical Memory II [delayed re-
tors of mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and dementia among call], Wechsler Memory Scale–Revised Visual Reproduction II
Olmsted County, Minnesota, residents 70 to 89 years of age. [delayed recall], and Auditory Learning Verbal Test delayed re-
The study participants consisted of the original sampled co- call), and visuospatial performance (Wechsler Adult Intelli-
hort from October 1, 2004, and replenishment sampling co- gence Scale–Revised Picture Completion and Block Design).
horts that occurred in 2008 and 2009. We included all 1995 par- Individual test scores were first converted to z scores using
ticipants without dementia at baseline (1718 cognitively normal means (SDs) from the MCSA 2004 enrollment cohort that con-
participants and 277 participants with MCI) with the APOE sisted of individuals without dementia (n = 1969). A global cog-
genotype, intellectual enrichment variables (described be- nitive summary score was estimated from the z transforma-
low), complete neuropsychological assessments, and at least tion of the mean of the 4 domain z scores and was used to assess
1 additional clinical follow-up with complete neuropsycho- cognitive impairment in our study participants. The baseline
logical assessments. The MCSA uses the Rochester Epidemi- global z score and rate of decline were the primary outcomes
ology Project records linkage system infrastructure,11,12 and of interest. Of the 1995 study participants, 1675 had not un-
complete details of the MCSA design have been published dergone testing at the time of enrollment into this study, and
elsewhere.13-15 320 had previously completed the neuropsychological bat-
tery of tests as part of an earlier study. We controlled for the
Intellectual Enrichment Variables number of times the participant underwent the battery of tests
The primary intellectual enrichment variables, assessed at before enrollment into the MCSA using a variable named base-
baseline, included education/occupation score and mid/late- line visit number because practice effects influence the mea-
life cognitive activity.16 The intellectual enrichment data were sured outcome variable (global cognition over time).18,19 A total
recorded for all study participants at the MCSA enrollment visit. of 1675 patients had a baseline visit number of 1 (ie, the first

1018 JAMA Neurology August 2014 Volume 71, Number 8 jamaneurology.com

Copyright 2014 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: http://archneur.jamanetwork.com/ by a Purdue University User on 05/22/2015


Intellectual Enrichment and Cognitive Decline Original Investigation Research

Table 1. Characteristics of the Study Participants

Median (25th-75th Percentile)a


All Participants Cognitively Normal Participants With MCI
Characteristic (N = 1995) Participants (n = 1718) (n = 277)
Women, No. (%) 960 (48.1) 846 (49.2) 114 (41.2)
APOE4 carriers, No. (%) 539 (27.0) 438 (25.5) 101 (36.5)
Age at visit, mean (range), y 78.9 (74.3 to 82.8) 78.4 (74.1 to 82.5) 81.5 (76.7 to 84.2)
Educational attainment, y 13 (12 to 16) 13 (12 to 16) 12 (12 to 15)
Job level score 4 (3 to 6) 4 (3 to 6) 3 (3 to 5)
Education/occupation score −0.27 (−0.76 to 0.73) −0.25 (−0.76 to 0.97) −0.51 (−1.02 to 0.24)
Cognitive activity
Midlife 20 (14 to 28) 20.5 (14 to 28.5) 18.5 (13 to 25.5)
Late life 21.5 (15.5 to 28.5) 22.5 (16 to 29.0) 18 (14 to 25)
Mid/late life −0.10 (−0.76 to 0.67) −0.04 (−0.73 to 0.72) −0.42 (−0.97 to 0.39)
Short test of mental status 34 (32 to 36) 35 (33 to 36) 30 (28 to 32)
Global z score 0.24 (−0.44 to 0.85) 0.38 (−0.15 to 0.95) −1.07 (−1.73 to −0.53)
Memory z score 0.17 (−0.56 to 0.85) 0.35 (−0.28 to 0.96) −1.24 (−1.66 to −0.66)
Language z score 0.22 (−0.44 to 0.78) 0.34 (−0.25 to 0.87) −0.76 (−1.52 to −0.11)
Attention z score 0.29 (−0.35 to 0.82) 0.40 (−0.19 to 0.90) −0.77 (−1.68 to 0.01)
Visuospatial z score 0.21 (−0.51 to 0.77) 0.31 (−0.34 to 0.85) −0.65 (−1.28 to 0.05)
Nonamnestic MCI, No. (%) 66 (3.3) NA 66 (23.8)
Baseline visit number, No. (%)
1 1932 (96.8) 1672 (97.3) 260 (93.9)
2 35 (1.8) 24 (1.4) 11 (4.0)
Abbreviations: MCI, mild cognitive
3 20 (1.0) 14 (0.8) 6 (2.2)
impairment; NA, not applicable.
4 8 (0.4) 8 (0.5) 0 a
Data are presented as median (25th
Follow-up time, 3.6 (1.6 to 5.2) 3.9 (1.9 to 5.2) 2.6 (1.4 to 4.0) to 75th percentile) unless otherwise
mean (range), y indicated.

time they took the test was at baseline of the study), 34 pa- random terms were significant (P < .001). The final models in-
tients had a baseline visit number of 2 (ie, tested once before corporated continuous first-order autoregressive correlation
baseline), 39 patients had a baseline visit number of 3 (ie, tested structures (estimated correlation for values 1 year apart = 0.41,
twice before baseline), and 247 patients had a baseline visit P < .001).
number of 4 or more. No significant 3-way interactions were found, so we re-
moved them from further consideration. We then used a back-
Statistical Analysis ward elimination procedure, respecting the need to retain
We examined the intellectual enrichment measures and demo- nested terms, to remove predictors and to form the most re-
graphic variables as predictors of longitudinal global cogni- strained model. The final model contained baseline age (in
tive z scores using linear mixed-effects models fit by maxi- years), sex, APOE4 carrier status, time from baseline (in years),
mum likelihood. In these models, the coefficients that are not baseline visit number, education/occupation score, and mid/
associated with time from baseline or any interactions that in- late-life cognitive activity. The model also included 6 two-
clude time from baseline estimate shifts in global z scores, way interactions: baseline age with time from baseline, mid/
which are consistent over time from baseline. The coeffi- late-life cognitive score with time from baseline, APOE4 carrier
cients associated with time from baseline and any interac- status with time from baseline, baseline visit number with time
tions that include time from baseline estimate deviations in from baseline, baseline visit number with education/
the rates of global z score change. A significant interaction in- occupation score, and an interaction of the 2 intellectual en-
dicates that shifts in global z scores vary with time rather than richment variables. The random intercepts (P < .001) and ran-
remaining constant. The initial model included baseline age dom slopes (P < .001) were deemed necessary.
(in years), sex, APOE carrier status, time from baseline (in
years), the intellectual enrichment variables, baseline visit
number, all 2-way interactions of these variables, and all 3-way
interactions that contain time. The models were fit with ran-
Results
dom participant-specific intercepts and slopes. We tested for The demographic, clinical, and intellectual enrichment vari-
the statistical significance of these random terms using like- ables of the participants without dementia included in this
lihood ratio tests. We also used likelihood ratio tests to com- analysis are given in Table 1. The results of the linear mixed-
pare independence (in which the within-participant errors are effects models are presented in Table 2. Baseline global z scores
independent) and continuous first-order autoregression. The were lower in men, older participants, APOE4 carriers, those

jamaneurology.com JAMA Neurology August 2014 Volume 71, Number 8 1019

Copyright 2014 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: http://archneur.jamanetwork.com/ by a Purdue University User on 05/22/2015


Research Original Investigation Intellectual Enrichment and Cognitive Decline

with lower education/occupation scores, and those with lower


Table 2. Lifetime Intellectual Enrichment, Baseline Cognition,
and Cognitive Declinea mid/late-life cognitive activity. Participants who had previ-
ous exposure to the neuropsychological battery of tests per-
Variable Coefficient (SE) P Value
formed better, and this effect diminished as time progressed.
Intercept 6.05 (0.29) <.001
Better education/occupation score and mid/late-life cog-
Baseline age −0.07 (0.004) <.001
nitive activity were associated with better cognitive perfor-
Male sex −0.18 (0.04) <.001 mance. Mid/late-life cognitive activity also had a significant
Time 0.70 (0.06) <.001 interaction with time from baseline (P = .045), where the slope
Education/occupation score 0.33 (0.03) <.001 of this association increased over time. Qualitatively, the
Mid/late-life cognitive activity 0.17 (0.02) <.001 change in slope of this association was small relative to the mag-
APOE4 genotype −0.20 (0.04) <.001 nitudes of the shifts in cognition associated with intellectual
Baseline visit number 0.05 (0.01) <.001 activity. In addition, a significant interaction was found be-
Baseline age × time −0.01 (0.001) <.001 tween the 2 intellectual enrichment variables (P = .03). Within
the observed follow-up period, higher mid/late-life cognitive
Mid/late-life cognitive activity × time 0.01 (0.003) .04
activity was associated with higher baseline global z scores, but
APOE4genotype × time −0.04 (0.01) <.001
the association was slightly attenuated as the education/
Baseline visit number × time −0.01 (0.002) .02
occupation score increased (Figure 1). We separated the plots
Baseline visit number × education/ −0.02 (0.01) .04
occupation score by sex and APOE4 carrier status because the baseline cogni-
Education/occupation score × mid/late-life −0.04 (0.02) .03 tive performance differed between these groups. Low to high
cognitive activity mid/late-life cognitive activity was related to better cognitive
a
Time refers to time from baseline. The terms without interaction with time performance if the education/occupation score was low, thus
indicate the variables that were significantly associated with baseline global shifting the low education/occupation score curve higher. This
cognition, and the terms with interaction with time refer to variables that were
shift in the cognitive performance was smaller if the education/
significantly associated with annual change in cognition over time.
occupation score was high.

Figure 1. Predicted Cognitive Global z Scores as a Function of Time From Baseline for Different Levels
of Intellectual Enrichment Measures

Educational Attainment and Occupational Score/Mid/Late-Life Cognitive Activity


High/High High/Low Low/High Low/Low

Female APOE4 Noncarriers Male APOE4 Noncarriers


1.0 1.0

0.5 0.5
Predicted Global z Score

Predicted Global z Score

0.0 0.0

–0.5 –0.5

–1.0 –1.0

–1.5 –1.5

–2.0 –2.0
0 1 2 3 4 5 0 1 2 3 4 5
Time From Baseline, y Time From Baseline, y

Female APOE4 Carriers Male APOE4 Carriers


1.0 1.0

0.5 0.5
Predicted Global z Score

Predicted Global z Score

0.0 0.0

–0.5 –0.5

–1.0 –1.0

–1.5 –1.5
The graphs illustrate the interaction
between education/occupation score
–2.0 –2.0
and mid/late-life cognitive activity.
0 1 2 3 4 5 0 1 2 3 4 5 Low and high intellectual enrichment
Time From Baseline, y Time From Baseline, y were defined by 25th and 75th
percentiles.

1020 JAMA Neurology August 2014 Volume 71, Number 8 jamaneurology.com

Copyright 2014 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: http://archneur.jamanetwork.com/ by a Purdue University User on 05/22/2015


Intellectual Enrichment and Cognitive Decline Original Investigation Research

To estimate the number of additional years remaining


Figure 2. Differences in the Predicted Times to Reach a Cognitive
cognitively normal that was associated with high intellectual Threshold Associated With Subtle Cognitive Impairment
activity, we used the fitted model to predict when these
curves cross a threshold of −0.74, which is the 10th percentile
of z scores in cognitively normal individuals. We chose this

Educational Attainment and Occupational Score


Women
cut point because it was previously used for the operational- L 3.3 H
Men

High
ization of the National Institute on Aging and the Alzheimer’s L 3.2 H
Association preclinical criteria for AD to indicate cognitive
impairment in cognitively normal individuals.20 This cut
L 3.6 H

Medium
point is also close to the mean global z score of −0.8 seen in
incident MCIs in the MCSA. Because the predicted times to L 3.4 H
reach the threshold sometimes exceeded the follow-up in our
study, requiring extrapolation, we limited our example of
L 3.7 H
prediction to 80-year-old APOE4 carriers. We had 142 partici-

Low
pants with follow-up that extended more than 6 years and L 3.5 H
602 participants with follow-up that extended more than 5
years. Predicted times within a few years of these values are
0 2 4 6 8 10 12 14
likely fairly accurate, whereas times farther away could be
Predicted Time, y
subject to increasing inaccuracies from nonlinearity and
other unmeasurable factors. Predicted times for 80-year-old
Figure depicts the results for an 80-year-old APOE4 carrier who never
APOE4 noncarriers exceeded 10 years and are therefore not underwent the neuropsychological battery of tests before baseline. Low and
given. high intellectual enrichment were defined by 25th and 75th percentiles.
Figure 2 illustrates the differences in the predicted times H indicates high mid-late cognitive; L, low mid-late cognitive.

for an 80-year-old APOE4 carrier who never received the neu-


ropsychological battery of tests before baseline to reach a cog- threshold) for shallow declines (young ages) than for steep
nitive threshold associated with subtle cognitive impairment declines (old ages).
depending on sex, education/occupation score, and mid/late-
life cognitive activity. We stratified low, medium, and high in-
tellectual enrichment scores by the 25th, 50th, and 75th per-
centiles. A sample interpretation of the data are as follows: in
Discussion
participants with medium education/occupation scores, en- The major conclusions of the study are that the protective ef-
gaging in high mid/late-life activity will have an associated later fect of intellectual enrichment is primarily manifested as a rela-
onset of cognitive impairment of 3.4 years for male APOE4 car- tively consistent higher cognitive performance over time. Mid/
riers and 3.6 years for female APOE4 carriers. Overall, going late-life cognitive activity had an increasing effect over time,
from low lifetime intellectual enrichment (low education/ but qualitatively the magnitude of this effect relative to the
occupation score and low mid/late-life cognitive activity) to overall shift in cognitive performance was minor. High life-
high lifetime intellectual enrichment (high education/ time intellectual enrichment (75th percentile) may delay the
occupation score and high mid/late-life cognitive activity) could onset of cognitive impairment by approximately 8.7 years in
delay the onset of cognitive impairment by approximately 8.7 male APOE4 carriers and 8.8 years in female APOE4 carriers
years for male APOE4 carriers and 8.8 years for female APOE4 compared with a low lifetime intellectual enrichment (25th per-
carriers. centile). The protective effect of mid/late-life cognitive activ-
Among all the intellectual enrichment and demographic ity on baseline cognitive performance decreases with increas-
variables tested, only older age at baseline visit, baseline visit ing education/occupation score.
number, mid/late-life cognitive activity, and APOE4 genotype Higher levels of educational, occupational, and cognitive
had significant interactions with time from baseline. An inter- activity are independently associated with a lower risk of de-
action was also found between baseline visit number and mentia consistent with earlier studies.2-10 The contribution of
education/occupation score, indicating that the learning education/occupation score (model coefficient = 0.33) was
effect that was provided as participants had more exposures larger than the contribution of mid/late-life cognitive activity
to the battery of tests was attenuated in those with higher (model coefficient = 0.17). This result is logical and consis-
education/occupation scores. The cognitive z-score trajecto- tent with a previous finding.17 Intellectual development due
ries for different baseline ages separated by sex and APOE4 to educational attainment and occupation exerts an effect dur-
carrier status are illustrated in Figure 3. Older participants had ing the entire adult lifespan, whereas mid/late-life cognitive
lower global z scores and declined more rapidly after baseline. activities refer to a more limited portion of an individual’s life.
The steeper decrease in cognitive z scores at older ages would The negative interaction between mid/late-life cognitive
affect the number of years to a cognitive threshold associated activity and education/occupation score was intriguing. We
with intellectual activity. For example, the constant vertical found that an individual with a low education/occupation score
shift over time in z scores associated with education/occu- benefited more by engaging in high mid/late-life cognitive ac-
pation score would have a larger horizontal shift (time to tivity than an individual with a high educational education/

jamaneurology.com JAMA Neurology August 2014 Volume 71, Number 8 1021

Copyright 2014 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: http://archneur.jamanetwork.com/ by a Purdue University User on 05/22/2015


Research Original Investigation Intellectual Enrichment and Cognitive Decline

Figure 3. Predicted Cognitive Global z-Score Trajectories for Different Baseline Ages Separated by APOE Carrier
Status and Sex

Baseline Age, y
75 85 95

Female APOE4 Noncarriers Male APOE4 Noncarriers


1 1
Predicted Global z Score

Predicted Global z Score


0 0

–1 –1

–2 –2

–3 –3
0 1 2 3 4 5 0 1 2 3 4 5
Time From Baseline, y Time From Baseline, y
Female APOE4 Carriers Male APOE4 Carriers
1 1
Predicted Global z Score

Predicted Global z Score

0 0

–1 –1

–2 –2

–3 –3
0 1 2 3 4 5 0 1 2 3 4 5
Time From Baseline, y Time From Baseline, y The mean trajectories are shown for
participants 75, 85, and 95 years old.

occupation score. These findings suggest that the effect of late- Among the demographic variables (other than intellec-
life cognitive training programs to delay the onset of AD may tual enrichment variables), older age, male sex, and APOE4
be reduced in those with high education/occupation scores, genotype were predictors of lower baseline global z scores;
which implies the need to account for this interaction when older age and APOE4 genotype were significantly associated
designing preventive trials based on cognitive training. How- with future cognitive decline. The fact that men had lower cog-
ever, a significant protection can be gained from engagement nitive performance at baseline is consistent with the litera-
in high mid/late-life cognitive activity irrespective of the in- ture reporting that men are at higher risk of MCI, particularly
dividual’s lifelong nonleisure activity through educational at- at younger ages, because of elevated cardiovascular risk
tainment and occupation. factors.15 Age25 and APOE4 genotype26,27 are the strongest risk
The education/occupation score was not associated with factors for sporadic AD. Because the proportion of individu-
the rate of cognitive decline, but mid/late-life cognitive activ- als without dementia who develop dementia increases in older
ity was slightly associated with the rate of cognitive decline. individuals and those with the APOE4 genotype and because
However, the effect of mid/late-life cognitive activity on the the rate of cognitive decline increases as a person moves closer
rate of cognitive decline (model coefficient = 0.01; P = .04) to a dementia diagnosis, it is logical that higher baseline age
was minimal compared with its effect on baseline cognitive and APOE genotype may be associated with faster cognitive
performance (model coefficient = 0.17; P < .001). The lack of decline28 and worse performance in a population without de-
association between education/occupation score and rate of mentia. Multivariate analysis enabled us to isolate the signifi-
cognitive decline is consistent with an earlier longitudinal cant associations after accounting for all other demographic
study21 that followed up more than 9000 people semiannu- and intellectual enrichment variables, which strengthen our
ally for 15 years. The association between mid/late-life cogni- findings of the association of APOE4 genotype and age with
tive activity and rate of cognitive decline in older individuals rate of cognitive decline. Although practice effects were not a
without dementia is consistent with earlier studies.22,23 How- focus of this study, the finding that greater past exposure to
ever, the weaker association of mid/late-life cognitive activity the test resulted in better performance and a reduced effect
with rate of decline compared with its larger effect on baseline of educational attainment is consistent with the literature.18,19
cognitive performance is important to consider. 24 These A report29 by the Alzheimer’s Association projected that
results support that the protection provided by lifetime intel- a treatment breakthrough that can delay the onset of AD by 5
lectual enrichment is largely driven by its effect on baseline years means reducing the expected number of patients with
cognitive performance and marginally due to its effect on the AD by approximately 43% in the United States by the year 2050.
rate of cognitive decline. The estimation of the years of protection against cognitive im-

1022 JAMA Neurology August 2014 Volume 71, Number 8 jamaneurology.com

Copyright 2014 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: http://archneur.jamanetwork.com/ by a Purdue University User on 05/22/2015


Intellectual Enrichment and Cognitive Decline Original Investigation Research

pairment provided by educational attainment and occupa- in our observational, population-based sample in which par-
tion and mid/late-life cognitive activity in a population- ticipants self-reported information about their mid/late-life
based sample in this study (Figure 2) provides guidelines that cognitive activities. Second, when estimating delay in dis-
can be used to understand the public health effect of using in- ease onset due to higher levels of enrichment, we assumed that
tellectual enrichment as a preventive intervention. cognitive decline is linear over time, which, although prob-
For the education/occupation score, we found that the ably true for short intervals (ie, several years), is likely not true
number of years of protection provided by higher educa- for longer periods of observation. However, by limiting esti-
tional attainment (keeping cognitive activity constant) is at least mation of time to only APOE4 carriers, we are not extrapolat-
5 years irrespective of sex and APOE4 carrier status. The de- ing much more than the follow-up time. Increasing pathologi-
crease in the risk of dementia with increasing educational at- cal burden with age may cause an acceleration of the decline.
tainment in the past century30 supports these findings and Third, the study results are pertinent to individuals without
highlights the importance of intervening early for larger pub- dementia in the population and may be different in individu-
lic health effect. Specifically, future reduction in the epi- als with dementia. Fourth, we did not have measurements for
demic of dementia will come from public investments to in- early-life cognitive activities and assumed that educational at-
crease access to education and better jobs for the young adults tainment and occupation are the major components of the in-
in our population. tellectual enrichment in early life.
For mid/late-life cognitive activity, although the effect of The study also has major strengths. First, the population-
the education/occupation score was larger than mid/late-life based nature of the sample makes the results of the study gen-
cognitive activity, the years of protection provided by high mid/ eralizable and enhances their external validity. Second, the use
late-life cognitive activity vs low mid/late-life cognitive activ- of principal components aided us in separating 2 major intel-
ity was at least 3.2 years for APOE4 carriers (7.3 years for non- lectual enrichments in life, educational attainment and occu-
carriers [data not shown]). Although the optimal intervention pation and mid/late-life cognitive activities, into 2 uncorre-
time may be intellectual enrichment in early life, there are sub- lated variables. Third, the multivariate analysis enabled us to
stantial benefits of using a public health campaign by provid- isolate the independent significant associations of the mul-
ing intellectual enrichment to midlife to late-life individuals. tiple components.
In this study, high mid/late-life engagement in cognitively
stimulating activities (75th percentile) corresponded to en-
gaging in several cognitively stimulating activities at least 3
times a week during midlife to late life. Examples of these ac-
Conclusions
tivities include reading books and magazines, playing games Higher education/occupation scores were associated with
and music, participating in artistic activities, participating in higher levels of cognition. Higher levels of mid/late-life cog-
crafts, participating in group activities, participating in social nitive activity were also associated with higher levels of cog-
activities, and participating in computer activities. nition, but the slope of this association slightly increased
The study has some limitations. First, the results do not over time. Lifetime intellectual enrichment might delay the
preclude the possibility that active lifestyle intervention might onset of cognitive impairment and be used as a successful
prospectively alter the rate of cognitive decline in an active in- preventive intervention to reduce the impending dementia
terventional study. However, we did not find evidence of this epidemic.

ARTICLE INFORMATION National Institute on Aging and the Alzheimer’s Family Alzheimer's Disease Research Professorship
Accepted for Publication: April 2, 2014. Association. Dr Knopman reported serving as of the Mayo Foundation. No other disclosures were
deputy editor for Neurology, serving on a data reported.
Published Online: June 23, 2014. safety monitoring board for Lilly Pharmaceuticals,
doi:10.1001/jamaneurol.2014.963. Funding/Support: This work was supported by
serving as an investigator in clinical trials sponsored grants K99/R00-AG37573, R01-AG011378, R01-
Author Contributions: Dr Vemuri had full access to by Janssen Pharmaceuticals, and receiving research AG041851, P50-AG16574, and U01-AG06786 from
all the data in the study and takes responsibility for support from the National Institutes of Health. Dr the National Institutes of Health, the Alexander
the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the Mielke reported serving as a consultant for Eli Lilly Family Alzheimer’s Disease Research Professorship
data analysis. and receiving funding from the National Institute of of the Mayo Foundation, and grant C06-RR018898
Study concept and design: Vemuri, Lesnick, Geda, Aging. Drs Rocca and Roberts reported receiving from the National Institutes of Health for the Opus
Jack. research funding from the National Institutes of Building.
Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: Health. Dr Petersen reported receiving consulting
Vemuri, Lesnick, Przybelski, Machulda, Knopman, fees from Roche Inc, Merck, and Genentech, Role of the Sponsors: The funding sources had no
Mielke, Roberts, Rocca, Petersen, Jack. receiving royalties from Oxford University Press, role in the design and conduct of the study;
Drafting of the manuscript: Vemuri, Lesnick. serving as chair of data monitoring committees for collection, management, analysis, and
Critical revision of the manuscript for important Pfizer Inc and Janssen Alzheimer Immunotherapy, interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or
intellectual content: All authors. and receiving research support from the National approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit
Statistical analysis: Lesnick, Przybelski, Rocca. Institute on Aging. Dr Jack reported serving as a the manuscript for publication.
Obtained funding: Vemuri, Roberts, Jack. consultant for Siemens; receiving research funding
Administrative, technical, or material support: from grants R01-AG011378, R01-AG041851, R01- REFERENCES
Vemuri, Mielke, Jack. AG037551, U01-HL096917, U01-AG032438, and 1. Administration on Aging. Projected future
Study supervision: Vemuri, Jack. U01-AG024904 from the National Institutes of growth of the older population. http://www.aoa
Conflict of Interest Disclosures: Dr Vemuri Health; and receiving funding from the Alexander .gov/Aging_Statistics/future_growth/future_growth
reported receiving research funding from the .aspx. Accessed June 14, 2013.

jamaneurology.com JAMA Neurology August 2014 Volume 71, Number 8 1023

Copyright 2014 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: http://archneur.jamanetwork.com/ by a Purdue University User on 05/22/2015


Research Original Investigation Intellectual Enrichment and Cognitive Decline

2. Stern Y, Gurland B, Tatemichi TK, Tang MX, Epidemiology Project: half a century of medical 21. Schneider AL, Sharrett AR, Patel MD, et al.
Wilder D, Mayeux R. Influence of education and records linkage in a US population. Mayo Clin Proc. Education and cognitive change over 15 years: the
occupation on the incidence of Alzheimer’s disease. 2012;87(12):1202-1213. atherosclerosis risk in communities study. J Am
JAMA. 1994;271(13):1004-1010. 12. St Sauver JL, Grossardt BR, Yawn BP, Melton LJ Geriatr Soc. 2012;60(10):1847-1853.
3. Cobb JL, Wolf PA, Au R, White R, D’Agostino RB. III, Rocca WA. Use of a medical records linkage 22. Wilson RS, Bennett DA, Bienias JL, Mendes de
The effect of education on the incidence of dementia system to enumerate a dynamic population over Leon CF, Morris MC, Evans DA. Cognitive activity
and Alzheimer’s disease in the Framingham Study. time: the Rochester Epidemiology Project. Am J and cognitive decline in a biracial community
Neurology. 1995;45(9):1707-1712. Epidemiol. 2011;173(9):1059-1068. population. Neurology. 2003;61(6):812-816.
4. Ott A, Breteler MM, van Harskamp F, et al. 13. Petersen RC, Roberts RO, Knopman DS, et al; 23. James BD, Wilson RS, Barnes LL, Bennett DA.
Prevalence of Alzheimer’s disease and vascular The Mayo Clinic Study of Aging. Prevalence of mild Late-life social activity and cognitive decline in old
dementia: association with education: the cognitive impairment is higher in men. Neurology. age. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2011;17(6):998-1005.
Rotterdam study. BMJ. 1995;310(6985):970-973. 2010;75(10):889-897. 24. Mitchell MB, Cimino CR, Benitez A, et al.
5. Qiu C, Karp A, von Strauss E, Winblad B, 14. Roberts RO, Geda YE, Knopman DS, et al. The Cognitively stimulating activities: effects on
Fratiglioni L, Bellander T. Lifetime principal Mayo Clinic Study of Aging: design and sampling, cognition across four studies with up to 21 years of
occupation and risk of Alzheimer’s disease in the participation, baseline measures and sample longitudinal data. J Aging Res. 2012;2012:461592.
Kungsholmen project. Am J Ind Med. 2003;43(2): characteristics. Neuroepidemiology. 2008;30(1): 25. Evans DA, Funkenstein HH, Albert MS, et al.
204-211. 58-69. Prevalence of Alzheimer’s disease in a community
6. Karp A, Kåreholt I, Qiu C, Bellander T, Winblad B, 15. Roberts RO, Geda YE, Knopman DS, et al. The population of older persons: higher than previously
Fratiglioni L. Relation of education and incidence of MCI differs by subtype and is higher in reported. JAMA. 1989;262(18):2551-2556.
occupation-based socioeconomic status to incident men: the Mayo Clinic Study of Aging. Neurology. 26. Corder EH, Saunders AM, Strittmatter WJ, et al.
Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Epidemiol. 2004;159(2): 2012;78(5):342-351. Gene dose of apolipoprotein E type 4 allele and the
175-183. 16. Geda YE, Topazian HM, Roberts LA, et al. risk of Alzheimer’s disease in late onset families.
7. Scarmeas N, Levy G, Tang MX, Manly J, Stern Y. Engaging in cognitive activities, aging, and mild Science. 1993;261(5123):921-923.
Influence of leisure activity on the incidence of cognitive impairment: a population-based study 27. Saunders AM, Strittmatter WJ, Schmechel D,
Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology. 2001;57(12): [published correction appears in J Neuropsychiatry et al. Association of apolipoprotein E allele epsilon 4
2236-2242. Clin Neurosci. 2012;24(4):500]. J Neuropsychiatry with late-onset familial and sporadic Alzheimer’s
8. Fabrigoule C. Do leisure activities protect against Clin Neurosci. 2011;23(2):149-154. disease. Neurology. 1993;43(8):1467-1472.
Alzheimer’s disease? Lancet Neurol. 2002;1(1):11. 17. Vemuri P, Lesnick TG, Przybelski SA, et al. Effect 28. Salmon DP, Ferris SH, Thomas RG, et al. Age
doi:10.1016/S1474-4422(02)00010-8. of lifestyle activities on Alzheimer disease biomarkers and apolipoprotein E genotype influence rate of
9. Crowe M, Andel R, Pedersen NL, Johansson B, and cognition. Ann Neurol. 2012;72(5):730-738. cognitive decline in nondemented elderly.
Gatz M. Does participation in leisure activities lead to 18. Duff K, Beglinger LJ, Moser DJ, Paulsen JS, Neuropsychology. 2013;27(4):391-401.
reduced risk of Alzheimer’s disease? a prospective Schultz SK, Arndt S. Predicting cognitive change in 29. Alzheimer’s Association. Changing the
study of Swedish twins. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. older adults: the relative contribution of practice trajectory of Alzheimer’s disease. http://www.alz.org
2003;58(5):249-255. effects. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2010;25(2):81-88. /alzheimers_disease_trajectory.asp. Accessed May 19,
10. Sattler C, Toro P, Schönknecht P, Schröder J. 19. Dodge HH, Wang CN, Chang CC, Ganguli M. 2014.
Cognitive activity, education and socioeconomic Terminal decline and practice effects in older adults 30. Rocca WA, Petersen RC, Knopman DS, et al.
status as preventive factors for mild cognitive without dementia: the MoVIES project. Neurology. Trends in the incidence and prevalence of Alzheimer’s
impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Psychiatry Res. 2011;77(8):722-730. disease, dementia, and cognitive impairment in the
2012;196(1):90-95. 20. Jack CR Jr, Knopman DS, Weigand SD, et al. An United States. Alzheimers Dement. 2011;7(1):80-93.
11. Rocca WA, Yawn BP, St Sauver JL, Grossardt BR, operational approach to NIA-AA criteria for preclinical
Melton LJ III. History of the Rochester Alzheimer's disease. Ann Neurol. 2012;71(6):765-775.

1024 JAMA Neurology August 2014 Volume 71, Number 8 jamaneurology.com

Copyright 2014 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: http://archneur.jamanetwork.com/ by a Purdue University User on 05/22/2015

You might also like