Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 11

Teaching and Teacher Education 132 (2023) 104225

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Teaching and Teacher Education


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/tate

Research paper

Factors explaining teachers’ intention to implement inclusive practices


in the classroom: Indications based on the theory of planned
behaviour
Karolina Urton a, *, Jürgen Wilbert b, Johanna Krull c, Thomas Hennemann c
a
Faculty of Education and Social Sciences, University of Muenster, Germany
b
Department of Inclusive Education, University of Potsdam, Germany
c
Department of Special Education and Rehabilitation, University of Cologne, Germany

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: In terms of inclusive school development, a question is what factors enhance successful implementation
Received 10 June 2022 of inclusive practices. The present study applies the Theory of Planned Behaviour (TPB) as a whole and in
Received in revised form differentiated components related to the teacher intentions to implement inclusive practices. Data were
29 April 2023
collected from 585 primary school teachers in Germany. Multi-level multivariate regression models were
Accepted 10 June 2023
Available online 04 July 2023
computed to test the correlation of attitudes, norms and perceived behavioural control with teacher
intention to implement inclusion. All components investigated showed a significant relationship with
teacher intention. Differentiated examination provides indications of opportunities for the development
Keywords:
Theory of planned behaviour (TPB)
of an inclusive school system.
Inclusion © 2023 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Ltd. This is an open access article under the CC BY license
Teachers (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Germany

When Germany ratified the UN convention on the Rights of The theory of planned behaviour (TPB) has been successfully
Persons with Disabilities in 2009, the vast majority of German made use of to investigate the psychological factors which may
students with special educational needs (SEN) were still being predict and explain a person's intentions to act and behave in a
educated in separate schools. Despite efforts to move the education certain way in various contexts (e.g., McEachan et al., 2016;
system towards more inclusive teaching, the exclusion rate (the Steinmetz et al., 2016) including both health psychology and edu-
number of students not attending regular schools in proportion to cation research (Godin & Kok, 1996; Lee et al., 2010; McEachan
all students required to attend school full-time) remained constant et al., 2016). Therefore, it seems that the TPB may also be a prom-
at 4.2% (Autorengruppe, 2020). The reasons for this have been ising approach through which we can explain teachers' intentions
explored extensively on cultural levels (established attitudes and to practice inclusive education (Opoku et al., 2020). Thus, the pur-
practices held by educators and teachers) and structural levels pose of the present study is to explore the intention of primary
(constitution and organization of schools and related institutions) school teachers to arrange their lessons in an inclusive way; it
(Powell, 2015; Sharma & Mahapatra, 2007). Findings have sug- strives to develop an integrated understanding of this process by
gested that the willingness of teachers to support this change and applying the factors stated in the TPB to the context of inclusive
conduct their work accordingly is a key factor driving institutional teaching. This, in turn, can be expected to help us understand the
transformations towards inclusion. In particular, the intention of behaviour of teachers, since intention is an indicator that a person
teachers to support inclusive education and implement inclusive is willing to behave in a certain way (Ajzen, 2005, 2012; Ajzen &
practices in their teaching has been shown to be of central Kruglanski, 2019).
importance (Armstrong, 2014), which raises the question of how
intentions emerge.
1. The theory of planned behaviour

* Corresponding author. University of Muenster, Institute of Education, Bisping- The TPB (Ajzen, 1985; Ajzen, 1991; Ajzen, 2005; see Fig. 1) states
hof 5/6, 48143 Muenster. that behavioural intention is based on three factors: (a) a person's
E-mail address: karolina.urton@uni-muenster.de (K. Urton).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tate.2023.104225
0742-051X/© 2023 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Ltd. This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
K. Urton, J. Wilbert, J. Krull et al. Teaching and Teacher Education 132 (2023) 104225

Fig. 1. Theory of planned behavior (Ajzen, 2005).

attitude towards the behaviour, (b) their perception of a normative 3. Attitudes


urge (subjective norm) to behave in a certain way, and (c) their
conviction that they are able to perform the behaviour (perceived Several authors have stated that a teacher's positive attitude
behavioural control). Each of these three factors derives from an towards inclusion is a crucial prerequisite for its implementation
interaction between a belief and an evaluation component. A per- (Avramidis & Norwich, 2002; Boer et al., 2011; van Mieghem et al.,
son's attitude toward a behaviour is the result of the strength of 2018). Teachers' attitudes have been shown to predict their in-
their belief about the outcomes of that behaviour (behavioural be- tentions regarding the implementation of inclusive education
liefs) and the extent to which these outcomes are desirable for them (Gilor & Katz, 2019; Hellmich et al., 2019; Knauder & Koschmieder,
(outcome expectation). A person's subjective normative pressure 2019; MacFarlane & Woolfson, 2013; Opoku, Cuskelly, Rayner, &
derives from their beliefs about the opinions other people hold Pedersen, 2022; Yan & Sin, 2014, 2015). They have also been
about the behaviour (normative beliefs) and the extent to which shown to predict the intention of teachers to include students with
these other people are important to the person (motivation to disabilities or behavioural problems in regular schools (Ahmmed
comply). A person's perceived behavioural control emerges from et al., 2014; Malak et al., 2018). This aspect was addressed either
their belief about the accessibility of internal and external factors as attitudes towards inclusive education (Hellmich et al., 2019;
facilitating or hampering the execution of a behaviour (control be- Kuyini & Desai, 2007), or as attitudes towards students with SEN in
liefs) and the strength of the influence a person ascribes to each inclusive classes (Ahmmed et al., 2014; Gilor & Katz, 2019;
factor (perceived power). These aspects are considered predictors of MacFarlane & Woolfson, 2013; Yan & Sin, 2014; 2015), or also as
a person's intention, and, in turn, it is postulated that intention is attitudes towards the inclusion of students with inappropriate
the strongest determinant of future behaviour, along with behaviour (Malak et al., 2018), or, finally, as the subject's attitudes
perceived behavioural control “as a proxy for actual control” (Ajzen, towards the behaviour of including or teaching students with dis-
2012, p. 10). abilities in regular classrooms (Knauder & Koschmieder, 2019).
These differences indicate that the objective of inclusive education
is not only to teach academic learning content but to also promote
2. The intention to teach inclusively social-emotional development. These objectives are complemented
by the goal of promoting social participation (Koster et al., 2010).
In recent years, several studies have shown that the factors and
components assumed by the TPB are relevant predictors of in-
tentions and reported behaviour (Hellmich et al., 2019; Knauder & 3.1. Norms
Koschmieder, 2019; MacFarlane & Woolfson, 2013; Yan & Sin, 2014,
2015) and inclusive behaviour (Kuyini & Desai, 2007) of teachers Compared to attitudes and perceived behavioural control,
(Ahmmed et al., 2014; Batsiou et al., 2008; Hellmich et al., 2019; perceived norms have the weakest impact on a person's intentions
Knauder & Koschmieder, 2019; Kuyini & Desai, 2007; MacFarlane & (Armitage & Conner, 2001). With respect to inclusion, perceived
Woolfson, 2013; Malak et al., 2018), preservice teachers (Gilor & norms address the extent to which a teacher's intention to imple-
Katz, 2019) and principals (Yan & Sin, 2015). However, a closer ment inclusive practices is influenced by the conduct and attitude
examination of these studies reveals clear differences regarding (1) of significant others (Hellmich et al., 2019; Gilor and Katz, 2019; Yan
the definition and operationalization of the various factors and & Sin, 2014, 2015). A study by MacFarlane and Woolfson (2013)
components, (2) the examined outcome variables (i.e., interests or found that subjective norms did not have any significant effect on
actual behaviour), and (3) the data modelling and analyses. These intention. However, when the teacher's perceived support of the
differences and the research results are outlined in the following school was included as a predictor of inclusive teaching and his or
sections, which focus on attitudes, normative processes and self-ef- her intention to implement inclusive education, the teacher's
ficacy related to the intention to implement inclusion in the perceived norms became a significant predictor (Ahmmed et al.,
classroom. 2014).

2
K. Urton, J. Wilbert, J. Krull et al. Teaching and Teacher Education 132 (2023) 104225

In the studies available so far, norms have been investigated in inclusive teaching practices.
relation to the expectations of different groups of people, i.e. in The second goal of this study is to provide a comprehensive
relation to teachers' perceptions of the opinions of “important operationalization of attitudes, subjective norms, and perceived
others” regarding inclusive education: the important others being, behavioural control in relation to inclusive behaviour. Based on
for example, the school principal, colleges or parents of students previous studies, we determined that attitudes towards inclusion
with and without SEN (e.g., Ahmmed et al., 2014; Hellmich et al., addressed three key aspects: (a) academic learning, (b) social
2019; Knauder & Koschmieder, 2019; Kuyini & Desai, 2007; development, and (c) social inclusion for students with and
MacFarlane & Woolfson, 2013; Yan & Sin, 2014, 2015). Other studies without SEN. Regarding normative beliefs, the various groups of
emphasized teachers' perceptions of the support and encourage- people determined to be relevant to the school context included (a)
ment they receive from the school administration, school com- the ministry, (b) the school inspectorate, (c) the school principal, (d)
munity and their social environment (Ahmmed et al., 2014; Batsiou colleagues, (e) parents, and (f) students. Regarding perceived
et al., 2008; Gilor and Katz, 2019; Knauder & Koschmieder, 2019; behavioural control, the predictors identified included (a) cooper-
Malak et al., 2018). Regarding the implementation of inclusion, we ation, (b) resources, and (c) knowledge.
can assume that all of the aforementioned groups influence the
teachers' willingness to implement inclusion to varying degrees. 5. Hypotheses and exploratory questions
Following the TPB (Ajzen, 2005, 2012), it is not only a teacher's
anticipation about the expectations of others that is important (e.g., In this study, with these goals in mind, we investigate the
‘The ministry expects me to support inclusion’) but also the level of following four hypotheses and the three exploratory questions
value the teachers personally attach to the opinions of these others corresponding to the each related hypothesis:
(e.g., ‘The expectations of the ministry are important to me.‘). Hypothesis 1: Teachers’ attitudes towards inclusion are corre-
lated to their intention to implement inclusive practices in the
3.2. Perceived behavioural control/self-efficacy classroom. Specifically, we distinguish between attitudes towards
(a) academic learning, (b) social development, and (c) the social
Perceived behavioural control has seldom been addressed in inclusion of students with and without SEN in an inclusive context.
research on teachers' intent to implement inclusive practices, Exploratory question 1: To what extent can we observe differ-
whereas the nearly identical concept of self-efficacy (Bandura, 1997) ences in the behavioural beliefs and outcome expectations of teachers
has been the focus of many such studies (Opoku et al., 2020). A for students with and without SEN in an inclusive context? Again,
review of research shows that teachers’ intentions regarding the we differentiate between (a) academic learning, (b) social devel-
implementation of inclusive education are significantly predicted opment, and (c) social inclusion.
by both their self-efficacy (Ahmmed et al., 2014; MacFarlane & Hypothesis 2: Teachers’ subjective norms (i.e., their perception of
Woolfson, 2013; Malak et al., 2018) and their collective efficacy the normative expectation to put inclusion into praxis) are corre-
beliefs (Hellmich et al., 2019). lated to their intention to implement inclusive practices in the
In addition to efficacy, previous research has also evaluated the classroom. Regarding normative expectations, we differentiate
significance of resources, collaboration, knowledge, and experience between (a) the ministry, (b) the school inspectorate, (c) the school
and has found that these are also important factors in supporting principal, (d) colleagues, (e) parents, and (f) students.
teachers and ensuring their willingness to implement inclusive Exploratory question 2: To what extent can we observe any dif-
practices in schools. More specifically, this significance has been ferences between teachers’ normative beliefs and their motivation to
attributed to professional development and experience, as well as comply in relation to various groups? Here, we differentiate be-
to opportunities for co-teaching and collaboration (Abegglen & tween (a) the ministry, (b) the school inspectorate, (c) the school
Hessels, 2018; Boer et al., 2011; van Mieghem et al., 2018). Thus, principal, (d) colleagues, (e) parents, and (f) students?
feeling competent to teach inclusively and having adequate in- Hypothesis 3: Teachers’ perceived behavioural control regarding
service training also explains the variance in one's readiness to the implementation of inclusive teaching is correlated to their
teach inclusively (Gilor and Katz, 2019; Yan & Sin, 2015). In terms of intention to implement inclusive practices in the classroom. We
the TPB, measuring perceived behavioural control should be done assume that perceived behavioural control is derived from (a) the
in a way that captures both the availability of each resource to intensity of cooperation, (b) the availability of personal and mate-
teachers and the degree of importance they place on each resource. rial resources, and (c) their knowledge and experience with respect
to inclusion.
4. Research goals Exploratory question 3: To what extent can we observe differ-
ences in teachers’ control beliefs and perceived power with respect to
An overview of the studies conducted so far indicates that the (a) cooperation, (b) personal, and material resources and (c)
TPB is well suited to predict the intention to teach inclusively. knowledge and experience in relation to inclusion?
However, it should be noted that, to date, only one study (by Gilor & Hypothesis 4: Attitudes, subjective norms and perceived behav-
Katz, 2019, which focused on preservice teachers), provides a ioural control mutually contribute to the intention of primary school
comprehensive application of all the aspects of the model (atti- teachers to implement inclusive practices in the classroom.
tudes, perceived norms and perceived behavioural control). Ac-
cording to our literature research, which is in line with the review 6. Methodology and methods
by Opoku et al. (2022), no other study has examined all factors of
the complete TPB in relation to predicting a teacher's intention to 6.1. Participants
implement inclusive teaching strategies. The present study ad-
dresses this gap. To achieve this, we measure attitudes, subjective We collected data from 585 primary school teachers. 504
norms and perceived behavioural control as the product of beliefs teachers (from 49 schools) returned valid responses, which were
(behavioural beliefs, normative beliefs, control beliefs) and value included in this study. The teachers’ averageage was M ¼ 41.9
(outcome expectation, motivation to comply, perceived power). We (SD ¼ 10.6) years and 94.1% were female. 90.8% were trained as
sought to determine the degree to which each of these dimensions general primary school teachers and 9.1% as special education
contribute to primary school teachers' intentions to implement teachers. On average, they had M ¼ 14.0 (SD ¼ 10.0) years of
3
K. Urton, J. Wilbert, J. Krull et al. Teaching and Teacher Education 132 (2023) 104225

teaching experience and had worked at their current school for differentiated between six relevant persons/groups: (a) the minis-
M ¼ 7.6 (SD ¼ 7.3) years. On an average, each teacher taught try, (b) the school inspectorate, (c) the school principal, (d) col-
M ¼ 21.2 (SD ¼ 6.6) lessons per week. Teachers rated their expe- leagues, (e) parents, and (f) students.
rience in teaching students with and without SEN together on a Perceived behavioural control. To identify the teachers' perceived
five-point Likert scale (0 ¼ no experience to 4 ¼ a lot of experience) behavioural control, both the control beliefs and the perceived power
with an average of M ¼ 3.5 (SD ¼ 1.0). 26.4% of all teachers reported that the person attributes to each of the different factors were
having an additional qualification related to teaching students with ascertained. Perceived behavioural control was assessed using
SEN (for further information see supplement material A). twenty-two items related to control beliefs and perceived power. The
items related to the areas of (a) cooperation (the school principal,
6.2. Procedure and materials colleagues, parents, social networks), (b) resources (personnel,
material, rooms), and (c) knowledge of the subject being taught and
6.2.1. Procedure expertise/experience in SEN.
Data collection took place from April to June 2017 in the first Intention to implement inclusion. The intention to implement
year of a three-year longitudinal research project (see Hennemann inclusive practices in the classroom was carried out on the basis of
et al., 2018; Urton et al., 2018). All schools were informed about the two items. The scales described above showed acceptable to very
project through the inspectorate and then we mailed the ques- good internal consistencies (0.83  Cronbach's a  0.89). See
tionnaires to the interested schools. All teachers were able to Table 1 and Supplement C for more information on the items and
choose to participate in the study on a voluntary basis. They item analyses of the TPB scales.
received a letter of information explaining the data collection and Through testing the construct validity of the scales, the assumed
management. Data collection and transmission were anonymous. It structure (see Fig. 2) could be corroborated. For attitudes including
took about 10 min to complete the questionnaire. Behavioural Beliefs and the Outcome-Expectations, a four-factor so-
lutions showed to best reflect the data, accounting for 71%e80% of
the variance. Regarding Subjective Norms, which include Normative
6.2.2. Operationalizing the TPB Beliefs and Motivation to Comply, the parallel analyses revealed a
By adhering to the TPB, we sought to explain the intention of three-factor structure of the data that explained 61%e73% of the
primary-school teachers to bring inclusion into practice through variance. For Perceived Behavioural Control - including Control Be-
their attitudes towards the behaviour, subjective norms, and perceived liefs and Perceived Power - the parallel analyses showed the data to
behavioural control. These three factors were each ascertained be best fit by a three-factor solution with explained variances of
through a belief component, an evaluation component, and the 51%e59%. For more details on the analyses see Supplements D and
interaction between these two components. Each teacher's attitude E.
toward a behaviour was calculated by multiplying the rating for the
strength of the beliefs about the outcomes of the behaviour
(behavioural beliefs) by the extent to which those outcomes are 6.2.4. Design and analysis strategy
desirable to the person (outcome expectation). Each teacher's sub- To investigate hypotheses 1 to 4 and exploratory questions 1 to
jective norms were calculated by multiplying their rating for the 3, we applied multilevel multivariate regression models. This was
strength of their beliefs about other peoples' opinions of a behav- necessary to take the nested data structure into account (teachers
iour (normative beliefs) by the extent to which these other people nested in schools). Following Snijders and Bosker (2012, p. 2), we
matter to the person (motivation to comply). Teachers' perceived added a random slope factor (component) at the lowest level (level
behavioural control was derived from their rating of the strength of 1: component; level 2: teachers; level 3: schools). For example, the
their beliefs about the accessibility of factors that facilitate or items on the attitude scale were interpreted as six responses to one
impede a behaviour (control beliefs) multiplied by the level of attitude variable (this is very similar to modelling repeated mea-
importance they ascribe to each factor (perceived power). surements with multilevel models). Additionally, we allowed for
different variances of all variables and different correlations be-
6.2.3. Questionnaire tween variables. The dependent variable was standardized (mean
The questionnaire began with a description of the aim of the zero and standard deviation one) and the intercept was set to zero.
study and an assurance that the anonymity of the participant would The resulting model now provides estimators for each component
be guaranteed. This was followed by questions about age, gender, that depicts the difference in standard deviations from the overall
length of employment at the present school, weekly teaching mean (which is zero due to the standardization). To estimate the
hours, and the experience of teaching students with and without association of each component to the teacher's intention we added
SEN together. Next, teachers were asked to respond to 52 questions an interaction term, component x intention, to the model. This
about their attitudes, subjective norms, and perceived behavioural interaction depicts the strength of the association between the
control on a five-point Likert scale (the exact wording of the poles respective component and the intention in standard deviations
were adapted for each scale; for more details see Supplement B). (which have the same scale as a correlation coefficient).
The items were developed by the authors specifically for the pre- To investigate hypothesis 4, we first built sum scores for the
sent study. See Fig. 2 for more information on the TPB model. A full items of the attitudes scale as well as for the items of the subjective
list containing all items can be found in Supplement B. norms scale and those of the perceived behavioural control scale.
Attitudes towards the behaviour. The assessment of the compo- Secondly, we calculated a random intercept multilevel regression
nent attitudes towards the behaviour was based on behavioural be- (teachers nested in schools) with intention regressed for attitudes,
liefs and a corresponding outcome expectation scale. Each of the subjective norms, and perceived behavioural control.
scales consists of six items. Attitudes towards the behaviour were All hypotheses were tested against models assuming no statical
assessed for (a) academic learning, (b) social development, and (c) differences in the respective aspects of the compared distributions
the social inclusion of students with and without SEN. (null hypotheses).
Subjective norms. Subjective norms were assessed using 14 items All regression models were calculated using the nlme package in
related to normative beliefs and motivation to comply. We R (Pinheiro, Bates & Core Team, 2022). Regression tables were

4
K. Urton, J. Wilbert, J. Krull et al. Teaching and Teacher Education 132 (2023) 104225

Fig. 2. Application of the TPB in the present study.

created with the help of the sjPlot package (Lüdecke, 2021). Details 7.1.2. Outcome expectations
on the analysing syntax and reproducible analyses are available in Outcome expectations revealed higher average values than
the supplement to this paper. behavioural beliefs. Accordingly, teachers rated academic learning
for students with SEN as M ¼ 3.77 (SD ¼ 0.85) and without SEN as
M ¼ 4.16 (SD ¼ 0.88, Cohen's d for this mean difference is 0.45). For
7. Results social development, the average for students with SEN was
M ¼ 4.30 (SD ¼ 0.74) and without SEN M ¼ 4.36 (SD ¼ 0.71). Cohen's
Following the logic of the theory of planned behaviour, we now d for this mean difference is 0.08. Furthermore, they emphasized
present the results related to the exploratory questions followed by the social inclusion of students with SEN (M ¼ 4.31, SD ¼ 0.73) and
those related to the hypotheses. positive social inclusion in classes (M ¼ 4.54, SD ¼ 0.65). Cohen's
d for this mean difference is 0.33. Therefore, the outcome expec-
7.1. Components of attitudes towards inclusion (exploratory tation was marginally higher for the students without SEN. The
question 1) correlation matrix shows moderate to high correlations between all
dimensions (0.36 < r < 0.86, ps < .001, see Supplement D, Table 2).
7.1.1. behavioural beliefs
On average, teachers either partially or strongly agreed that 7.1.3. Attitudes
inclusive teaching can be conducted in such a way that academic Each attitude component was calculated by multiplying the
learning, social development, and social inclusion show positive behavioural belief and outcome evaluation. The resulting scale was
results among students both with and without SEN. The average linearly transformed to a theoretical range from 0 to 1. The attitude
agreement was strongest for the factor social development of stu- was lowest for the academic learning of students with SEN
dents without SEN (M ¼ 3.92, SD ¼ 0.83) and weakest for academic (M ¼ 0.35, SD ¼ 0.19) and highest for social learning of students
learning of students with SEN (M ¼ 2.96, SD ¼ 0.82). Cohen's d for without SEN (M ¼ 0.62, SD ¼ 0.24) and social inclusion of students
this mean difference is 1.16. All components were moderately to without SEN (M ¼ 0.62, SD ¼ 0.23). Cohen's d for this mean dif-
highly correlated (0.34 < r < 0.69, ps < .001, see Supplement D, ferences are 1.25 and 1.27. The correlation between components
Table 1). ranged from r ¼ 0.39 to r ¼ 0.76 (Table 2).

5
K. Urton, J. Wilbert, J. Krull et al. Teaching and Teacher Education 132 (2023) 104225

Table 1
Items and Item analyses of TPB scales.

Scale Example n Alpha (CI95%) Homo- Discrimi- |Loading|


items geneity nation

Behavioural Beliefs I am convinced that inclusive education can be organized to ensure that the subject 6 .86 (.84-.88) .51 .55e.74 .60e.82
matter is successfully learned by students with SEN
Normative Beliefs How strongly do you perceive the expectations of the following people/institutions 6 .67 (.63-.71) .26 .28e.51 .34e.63
regarding how well inclusive education is implemented in schools? My colleagues
have …
Control Beliefs How much do the following facilitate/support successful inclusive teaching: … the 11 .79 (.77-.82) .26 .38e.54 .39e.62
special education knowledge of the teacher?
Outcome With regard to inclusive teaching, ‘How important are the following things to you in 6 .89 (.88-.90) .59 .57e.82 .59e.89
Expectations relation to inclusive education: The successful learning of the subject matter by
students with special educational needs.
Motivation to How important is it to you in general (not only with regard to inclusion) to meet the 6 .83 (.80-.85) .45 .49e.72 .54e.82
Comply expectations of the following people/institutions … my colleagues?
Perceived Power How much can you draw on the following when implementing inclusive teaching … 11 .89 (.87-.90) .42 .50e.72 .53e.78
special education knowledge?
Intention How committed will you be in the future to ensuring that inclusive education is 2 .67 (.62e.72) .52 .52e.52 .73e.73
successfully implemented at your school?

Table 2
Correlation matrix and descriptive statistics for components of attitudes (behavioural beliefs x outcome expectations).

M SD 2 3 4 5 6

1 Academic Learning SEN 0.35 0.19 .47*** .59*** .45*** .50*** .45***
2 Academic Learning w/o SEN 0.54 0.22 .47*** .64*** .39*** .42***
3 Social Development SEN 0.53 0.25 .76*** .64*** .66***
4 Social Development w/o SEN 0.62 0.24 .55*** .64***
5 Social Inclusion SEN 0.51 0.24 .73***
6 Social Inclusion w/o SEN 0.62 0.23

Note. *** ¼ p < .001; w/o ¼ without.

7.2. Attitudes towards inclusion and intention to support inclusion lowest for the academic learning of students without SEN (b ¼ 0.20,
(hypothesis 1) p < .001).

Table 3 depicts the results of a multivariate-multilevel regres-


sion model. The attitude score was lowest for academic learning of 7.3. Components of subjective norms in the context of inclusion
students with SEN (b ¼ 0.71; p < .001) and highest for social (exploratory question 2)
development of students without SEN (b ¼ 0.40; p < .001) and
social inclusion of students without SEN (b ¼ 0.39; p < .001). All six 7.3.1. Normative beliefs
attitudes showed a significant positive interaction with the inten- The normative beliefs (the normative expectation perceived by a
tion to put inclusion into practice. The correlation was highest for group that inclusion will be put into practice) were highest for the
the social inclusion of students with SEN (b ¼ 0.36, p < .001) and ministry (M ¼ 4.39, SD ¼ 0.86) and lowest for students (M ¼ 2.71,
SD ¼ 1.01). Cohen's d for this mean difference is 1.79. Correlations
ranged between r ¼ 0.04 (between the ministry and colleagues)
and r ¼ 0.67 (between the ministry and the school inspectorate).
Table 3
See Supplement D, Table 3 for details.
Regression model predicting the strength of an attitude by the attitudes’ component
parts and the interaction of these components with the teachers’ intention.

Predictors ß se t p
7.3.2. Motivation to comply
Academic Learning SEN 0.71 0.03 23.05 <.001 The motivation to comply was highest regarding students
Academic Learning w/o SEN 0.05 0.04 1.30 .193
(M ¼ 4.05, SD ¼ 0.82) and lowest regarding the ministry (M ¼ 2.59,
Social Development SEN 0.03 0.05 0.63 .528
Social Development w/o SEN 0.40 0.05 8.94 <.001 SD ¼ 0.98). Cohen's d for this mean difference is 1.61. Correlations
Social Inclusion SEN 0.05 0.04 1.25 .212 ranged between r ¼ 0.13 (between the ministry and students) and
Social Inclusion w/o SEN 0.39 0.04 8.95 <.001 r ¼ 0.78 (between the ministry and the school inspectorate).
Intention  Academic Learning SEN 0.32 0.03 10.21 <.001 See Supplement D, Table 4 for details.
Intention  Academic Learning w/o SEN 0.20 0.04 5.19 <.001
Intention  Social Development SEN 0.36 0.04 9.04 <.001
Intention  Social Development w/o SEN 0.25 0.04 6.32 <.001
Intention  Social Inclusion SEN 0.30 0.04 7.85 <.001 7.3.3. Subjective norms
Intention  Social Inclusion w/o SEN 0.28 0.04 7.25 <.001 The subjective norms (the product of normative beliefs and the
Random Effects
motivation to comply) were highest regarding the school principal
s2 0.50
ICC .04 SEN (M ¼ 0.47, SD ¼ 0.19) and lowest regarding students (M ¼ 0.33,
N school 49 SD ¼ 0.23). Cohen's d for this mean difference is 0.66. Correlations
Observations 3144 ranged between r ¼ 0.20 (between the ministry and colleagues)
Marginal R2/Conditional R2 .30/.33 and r ¼ 0.78 (between the ministry and the school inspectorate).
Note. All degrees of freedom are 3048; w/o ¼ without. See Table 4 for details.
6
K. Urton, J. Wilbert, J. Krull et al. Teaching and Teacher Education 132 (2023) 104225

Table 4 perceived power showed them to be reciprocally correlated


Correlation matrix and descriptive statistics for components of subjective norms (0.22 < r < 0.84, p < .001, see Supplement D, Table 6).
(normative beliefs x motivation to comply).

M SD 2 3 4 5 6 7.5.3. Perceived behavioural control


1 Ministry 0.34 0.23 .78*** .42*** .20*** .30*** .28*** For perceived behavioural control (the product of control beliefs
2 Inspectorate 0.40 0.21 .52*** .30*** .40*** .32*** and perceived power) the highest value revealed was for the
3 Principal 0.47 0.19 .61*** .51*** .37*** cooperation of the educators (M ¼ 0.66, SD ¼ 0.25) and the lowest
4 Colleagues 0.39 0.19 .49*** .42***
for staff resources (M ¼ 0.31, SD ¼ 0.25). Cohen's d for this mean
5 Parents 0.41 0.22 .61***
6 Students 0.33 0.23 difference is 1.40. (see Table 6 for details). No significant to high
correlations were found between any of the dimensions
Note. *** ¼ p < .001.
(0.05 < r < 0.76, see Table 6).

7.4. Subjective norms and the intention to support inclusion 7.5.4. Perceived behavioural control and the intention to support
(hypothesis 2) inclusion (hypothesis 3)
The results of the multivariate multilevel regression model for
In the results of the multivariate multilevel regression model perceived behavioural control proved to be lowest in relation to
regarding the subjective norm score was lowest for the students knowledge of the subject matter (b ¼ 0.46, p < .001) and highest
(b ¼ 0.26, p < .001) and highest for the school principal (b ¼ 0.38, in relation to cooperation with parents (b ¼ 0.92, p < .001). For all
p < .001). For all six subjective norms, there was a significant eleven domains of perceived behavioural control, significant posi-
positive interaction with the intention to put inclusion into prac- tive correlations were shown for the intention to put inclusion into
tice. The correlation was highest for students and colleagues practice. The correlation was highest for the cooperation with ed-
(b ¼ 0.29, p < .001) and lowest for the ministry (b ¼ 0.17, p < .001). ucators and with the principal (b ¼ 0.36, p < .001) and lowest for
See Table 5 for details. cooperation with the support system (b ¼ 0.19, p < .001). See
Table 7 for details.
7.5. Components of perceived behavioural control with respect to
7.5.5. Attitudes, subjective norms, perceived behavioural control
inclusion (exploratory question 3)
and the intention to implement inclusion (hypothesis 4)
To test for the complete model, we predicted the strength of
7.5.1. Control beliefs
intention by evaluating the strength of attitudes, subjective norms,
The teachers' ratings for control beliefs varied from partially
and perceived behavioural control (see Table 8). Therefore, the
agree to fully agree. The mean agreement was strongest for coop-
different attitude components were condensed by calculating an
eration with special educators (M ¼ 4.03, SD ¼ 0.80) and weakest
average attitude score for each participant. The average subjective
for staff resources (M ¼ 2.52, SD ¼ 1.08). Cohen's d for this mean
norms and average perceived behavioural control were calculated
difference is 1.59. There were no significant to high correlations
in a similar fashion.
between the teachers' control beliefs and any of the various areas
Perceived behavioural control was the strongest predictor of
(0.03 < r < 0.72, see Supplement D, Table 5).
intention (b ¼ 0.33, p < .001), followed by attitudes (b ¼ 0.25,
p < .001) and subjective norms (b ¼ 0.11, p < .01). The variance of
7.5.2. Perceived power intention between schools was low (t2 ¼ 0.02) resulting in a low ICC
The teachers' perceived power in the different areas was of 3%.
consistently rated as high. This was strongest for cooperation with Extended regression tables are found in the Supplement F.
special educators (M ¼ 4.40, SD ¼ 0.76) and room-resources
(M ¼ 4.40, SD ¼ 1.00) and weakest for cooperation with principal 8. Discussion
(M ¼ 3.46, SD ¼ 1.16). Cohen's d for this mean difference is 0.96 and
0.87. The correlation matrix indicated that the dimensions of The purpose of the present study was to examine the extent to
which the intention of primary teachers to implement inclusive
practices in the classroom is associated with their attitudes, sub-
Table 5
Regression model predicting intention by components of subjective norms. jective norms, and perceived behavioural control, in relation to
inclusion. Following Ajzen (2005), the three major TPB dimensions
Predictors ß se t p
were operationalized as the product of beliefs (behavioural beliefs,
Ministry 0.21 0.05 4.17 <.001 normative beliefs, and control beliefs) and values (outcome
Inspectorate 0.04 0.04 0.86 .392
expectation, motivation to comply, and perceived power). Each
Principal 0.38 0.05 8.21 <.001
Colleagues 0.02 0.04 0.45 .651 dimension was then divided further into more detailed compo-
Parents 0.10 0.05 2.05 .041 nents, in the attempt to determine which aspects are most relevant
Students 0.26 0.05 5.38 <.001 to the implementation of inclusive classroom practices.
Intention  Ministry 0.17 0.05 3.74 <.001 Consistent with hypothesis 1, an in-depth examination revealed
Intention  Inspectorate 0.18 0.04 4.41 <.001
Intention  Principal 0.19 0.04 5.11 <.001
that teachers' attitudes toward (a) academic learning, (b) social
Intention  Colleagues 0.29 0.04 7.88 <.001 development, and (c) the social inclusion of students with and
Intention  Parents 0.20 0.04 4.58 <.001 without SEN in an inclusive context correlated with their intention
Intention  Students 0.29 0.05 6.17 <.001 to implement inclusion in the classroom. Considering all di-
Random Effects
mensions (academic learning, social development, and social in-
s2 1.12
ICC .01 clusion), teachers’ attitudes were less positive with respect to the
N school 49 implementation of inclusive education practices for students with
Observations 3074 SEN. This was particularly pronounced in relation to their academic
Marginal R2/Conditional R2 .08/.09 learning. Overall, the results suggest that attitudes toward inclusive
Note. All degrees of freedom are 3014. teaching are associated with both the domain (academic learning,
7
K. Urton, J. Wilbert, J. Krull et al. Teaching and Teacher Education 132 (2023) 104225

Table 6
Correlation matrix and descriptive statistics for components of perceived behavioural control (control beliefs x perceived power).

M SD 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11

1 Coop-Colleagues 0.53 0.27 .59*** .48*** .38*** .34*** .31*** .22*** .18*** .24*** .16*** .17***
2 Coop-Educators 0.66 0.25 .50*** .40*** .38*** .32*** .24*** .21*** .34*** .19*** .22***
3 Coop-Principal 0.37 0.28 .44*** .36*** .25*** .24*** .22*** .21*** .05 .07
4 Coop-Parents 0.34 0.22 .43*** .24*** .30*** .22*** .30*** .23*** .23***
5 Coop-Support System 0.37 0.22 .35*** .28*** .28*** .33*** .28*** .20***
6 Res-Rooms 0.44 0.27 .60*** .58*** .23*** .32*** .27***
7 Res-Materials 0.36 0.23 .76*** .28*** .28*** .29***
8 Res-Staff 0.31 0.25 .23*** .28*** .24***
9 Know-Special Ed 0.47 0.22 .41*** .33***
10 Know-Teaching Subject 0.38 0.26 .66***
11 Know-Experience 0.45 0.25

Note. *** ¼ p < .001.

Table 7 social learning, or social interaction) and the group of students


Regression model predicting intention by components of perceived behavioural (with or without SEN) being surveyed.
control. Regarding question 1, comparing behavioural beliefs with
Predictors ß se t p outcome expectations reveals that the latter are somewhat higher,
Coop-Colleagues 0.37 0.07 5.25 <.001
especially for students with SEN. This indicates that teachers place
Coop-Educators 0.17 0.05 3.35 .001 a high value on academic and social learning for all students in
Coop-Principal 0.10 0.05 2.14 .033 inclusive classrooms although their beliefs about whether this can
Coop-Parents 0.92 0.05 16.85 <.001 be done successfully are weaker. This finding is also consistent with
Coop-Support System 0.22 0.08 2.68 .007
the reviews of Avramidis and Norwich (2002) and Boer et al., 2011,
Res-Rooms 0.29 0.05 5.65 <.001
Res-Materials 0.18 0.05 3.98 <.001 who found that teachers were positive about implementing in-
Res-Staff 0.03 0.06 0.43 .668 clusion in principle, but whether they believed it was possible to
Know-Special Ed 0.27 0.05 5.06 <.001 achieve depended on the particular students in the class and their
Know-Teaching Subject 0.46 0.06 7.20 <.001 specific needs.
Know-Experience 0.18 0.04 4.29 <.001
Intention * Coop-Colleagues 0.23 0.04 5.38 <.001
In support of hypothesis 2, the opinions of all relevant groups
Intention * Coop-Educators 0.36 0.04 9.12 <.001 (the ministry, the school inspectorate, the principal, colleagues,
Intention * Coop-Principal 0.36 0.04 9.31 <.001 parents, and students) were significantly correlated to the teachers'
Intention * Coop-Parents 0.20 0.04 5.03 <.001 intention to implement inclusion in the classroom. This was espe-
Intention * Coop-Support System 0.19 0.04 4.50 <.001
cially pronounced for the groups with whom teachers have
Intention * Res-Rooms 0.28 0.03 8.25 <.001
Intention * Res-Materials 0.20 0.03 5.94 <.001 frequent contact in everyday school life: their colleagues and
Intention * Res-Staff 0.26 0.04 6.26 <.001 students.
Intention * Know-Special Ed 0.25 0.03 7.21 <.001 Regarding explanatory question 2, it appears that the normative
Intention * Know-Teaching Subject 0.22 0.04 5.94 <.001 beliefs are strongest at the administrative level (the ministry and
Intention * Know-Experience 0.21 0.04 5.85 <.001
Random Effects
the school inspectorate), although the motivation to comply is
s2 0.81 lowest for the same groups. A contrary picture emerges for those
ICC .10 with whom teachers frequently come into contact in everyday
N school 49 school life (colleagues and students). Here, expectations that in-
Observations 5739
clusive practices will be implemented are perceived as lower, while
Marginal R2/Conditional R2 .18/.27
teachers' motivation to comply is higher in relation to these groups.
Note. All degrees of freedom are 5669. Thus, the results provide a first indication that the more distant
administration has less of an impact on teachers' motivation to
implement inclusive teaching practices than it does for the groups
Table 8
Multilevel model predicting intention by perceived behavioural control, subjective of people with whom teachers have daily contact in school.
norms, and attitudes. Hypothesis 3, which stated that perceived behavioural control
related to the implementation of inclusive education would
Predictors Intention
correlate with the intention to implement inclusion in the class-
ß se t p room, was also confirmed for cooperation, availability of resources,
Intercept 0.00 0.04 0.10 .917 and knowledge and experience. Consistent with the literature, our
Attitudes 0.25 0.04 6.33 <.001 results point to the centrality of cooperation (Abegglen & Hessels,
Subjective Norms 0.11 0.04 2.87 <.01
2018; Boer et al., 2011; van Mieghem et al., 2018). This is espe-
Perceived Behavioural Control 0.33 0.04 8.27 <.001
cially true for the possibility to cooperate with special education
Random effects
teachers and principals.
s2 0.68 When considering control beliefs in comparison to perceived
t2 0.02 power (exploratory question 3), all areas (cooperation, the avail-
ICC .03
ability of human and material resources, and knowledge and
N schools 49
Observations 559 experience regarding inclusion) are relevant to the teacher's level
Marginal R2/Conditional R2 .29/.31 of perceived power. However, the control beliefs are strongest for
Note. All degrees of freedom are 50. the different cooperation's and special education expertise.
In line with hypothesis 4 and consistent with previous research,

8
K. Urton, J. Wilbert, J. Krull et al. Teaching and Teacher Education 132 (2023) 104225

the overall TPB model (Ajzen, 2005) provided a suitable theoretical this area. Secondly, the survey was conducted entirely within the
basis for explaining the intention to implement inclusive teaching federal state of North Rhine-Westphalia (Germany). Accordingly, it
(e.g. Hellmich et al., 2019; Knauder & Koschmieder, 2019; Malak would be of interest for future studies to investigate how teachers
et al., 2018). Accordingly, all dimensions, albeit to varying de- who work in inclusive schools differ from those who have had no
grees, showed a unique association with the intention to imple- experience with inclusive education. It might also be of interest to
ment inclusive teaching. Supporting previous research, the consider differential effects between teachers with and without
association was strongest for perceived behavioural control and training in SEN. A further suggestion would involve investigating
attitudes, while normative beliefs are comparatively less pro- whether the results can be replicated in studies carried out in other
nounced (e.g. Hellmich et al., 2019; Opoku et al., 2022). school types as well as in other (international) regions.
Our results clearly show that different dimensions of attitudes, Opoku et al. (2022) point out that studies that have investigated
norms, and perceptions about behavioural control impact teachers' teacher intentions based on interviews have, in part, arrived at
intention to implement inclusive teaching practices. Therefore, as different and more nuanced results than questionnaire studies. A
suggested by (Gilor & Katz, 2019; Yan & Sin, 2015), providing mixed-method approach might have revealed a more subtle pic-
teachers with appropriate professional development opportunities ture of the relevant dimensions of attitudes, normative beliefs, and
is key to encouraging the implementation of inclusion. perceived levels of behavioural control. Thus, it must also be noted
Furthermore, it seems crucial to not only examine the attitudes about our study that our statements are to be interpreted only in
and experiences of efficacy of individual teachers, but also to relation to the aspects we surveyed, and that these make no claim
evaluate the processes at the school level (Urton et al., 2014). The of being exhaustive, since we did not, as suggested by Sutton et al.
results of our study indicate that the groups of people with whom (2003), previously ascertain to what degree the formulated beliefs
teachers have daily contact are crucial for significantly influence for are supported, accepted or have been taken into consideration
their intention to implement inclusive education. Indeed, in terms altogether by the teachers.
of attitudes, norms, and collaborative processes, this relates to the Furthermore, we have only examined teacher intention through
concept of inclusive school climate (Hennemann et al., 2018). Tar- personal reports. To be able to make statements about whether and
geted school development activities are important for ensuring the how inclusion is implemented in practice, classroom observations
long-term development of inclusive schools that go beyond could be more rigorous. However, this also raises the fundamental
accepting the ‘placement’ (Go € ransson & Nilholm, 2014, p. 265) of question of how to define inclusive teaching. An article by Sharma's
students with SEN and function as truly collaborative schools which research group (2019) demonstrated that the question of the
develop classrooms and that also promote interaction within the characteristics of inclusive teaching has received little attention.
social context. Specifically, this means developing practices at the These observation categories must be operationalized before
school level which support both academic and social-emotional further research in this direction can be undertaken.
learning and at the same time ensure the participation of all stu- Further limitations of our study are also related to the limita-
dents. According to the theory of the ecological environment of tions of the TPB. Even though the TPB represents one of the central
students, the quality of an inclusive school that supports this can be theories for explaining behaviour, the limited predictive power of
further developed in a school - development model for inclusive the theory represents one of the central criticisms of the TPB. For
schools on the following five levels: (1) ‘students and their indi- example, research suggests that the variability of intentions and
vidual needs’, (2) ‘inclusive teaching’, (3) ‘multiprofessional coop- observed behaviors can only be partially explained by the pre-
eration’, (4) ‘inclusive school concept and school life’, and (5) dictors of the TPB (Sniehotta, et.al, 2014). Thus, studies have
‘external support and community networking’ (Schurig et al., 2020, repeatedly shown that individuals have expressed an intention but
p. 1). At level two, inclusive teaching, this means improving the then fail to act on it (Orbell & Sheeran, 1998). Furthermore, the
practice of additional support from peers and additional support theory's reduction to attitudes, norms, and perceived behavioural
from teachers (van Mieghem et al., 2018). This is the case, for control has also been criticized because it ignores an individual's
example, when teachers implement peer-assisted learning strate- behavioural history and the role which broader social influences
gies in the classroom which allow students to practice the learned may also play in a person's behaviour (Conner & Sparks, 2015).
content in a cooperative way (Bowman-Perrott et al., 2013; Another criticism is that the theory is limited to rational aspects
Moeyaert et al., 2021). Teacher support can be implemented and does not consider other affective/emotional, unconscious, or
through co-teaching arrangements in which “several professionals irrational determinants of human behavior (Sheeran et al., 2013;
work together in a co-teaching team, based on a shared vision, in a Sniehotta, et.al, 2014). Regarding the intended purpose of our
structured way, over an extended period of time, during which they study, however, it is important to keep in mind that we used the
are equally responsible for good teaching and good learning for all TPB as a theoretical basis for investigating the factors which may
students in their classroom” (Fluijt et al., 2016, p. 197). Furthermore, explain a teacher's intention to implement inclusive practices in the
collaboration in an inclusive school should not only refer to classroom; we did not intend to test the TPB as such.
collaboration between teachers but also to multi-professional
cooperation (e.g., teaching assistants, school social work, school 9. Conclusion
psychology) which not only includes collaboration within the
school but involves external support and community networking as Even now, after more than ten years since the ratification of the
well (Chapman and Fullan, 2007; Muijs et al., 2010). UN Convention on the Rights of Persons with Disabilities, what
motivates teachers to exhibit the intention to put inclusion into
8.1. Limitations practice remains a central question that we need to understand
better. The present study examined this question using a differen-
The findings of the current study must be considered against the tiated operationalization of the TPB (Ajzen, 2005) for inclusive
backdrop of the following limitations. teaching. The results indicate that different content areas may
In terms of the generalizability of the results, there are limita- indeed be mapped using a differentiated approach. In turn, this
tions regarding the selection of the sample. Firstly, the survey confirms that further studies utilizing differentiated indications
exclusively involved teachers who worked in primary schools with may well be suitable for establishing indications for the continuing
inclusive education and who had already had some experience in development of the inclusive school systems.
9
K. Urton, J. Wilbert, J. Krull et al. Teaching and Teacher Education 132 (2023) 104225

Author contributions health behaviour: Research and practice with social cognition models (3rd ed., pp.
142e188). Open University Press.
Fluijt, D., Bakker, C., & Struyf, E. (2016). Team-reflection: The missing link in co-
KU and JW conceived and designed the study; KU and JW wrote teaching teams. European Journal of Special Needs Education, 31(2), 187e201.
the paper; JW performed the analyses; JW curated the data; JK and https://doi.org/10.1080/08856257.2015.1125690
KU organized and supervised the data collection; TH, JK and KU Gilor, O., & Katz, M. (2019). Pre-service teachers’ willingness to engage in inclusive
teaching: An explanatory model. Journal of International Special Needs Educa-
administrated the project; TH raised the funding. tion, 77e89. https://doi.org/10.9782/17-00011
Godin, G., & Kok, G. (1996). The theory of planned behavior: A review of its ap-
plications to health-related behaviors. American Journal of Health Promotion,
Declaration of competing interest
87e98. https://doi.org/10.4278/0890-1171-11.2.87
Go€ ransson, K., & Nilholm, C. (2014). Conceptual diversities and empirical
The authors declare that they have no known competing shortcomingsea critical analysis of research on inclusive education. European
financial interests or personal relationships that could have Journal of Special Needs Education, 265e280. https://doi.org/10.1080/
08856257.2014.933545
appeared to influence the work reported in this paper Hellmich, F., Lo €per, M. F., & Go€ rel, G. (2019). The role of primary school teachers'
attitudes and self-efficacy beliefs for everyday practices in inclusive classrooms
Data availability e a study on the verification of the ‘theory of planned behaviour’. Journal of
Research in Special Educational Needs, 36e48. https://doi.org/10.1111/1471-
3802.12476
Data will be made available on request. Hennemann, T., Hillenbrand, C., Fitting-Dahlmann, K., Wilbert, J., & Urton, K. (2018).
Auf dem Weg zum inklusiven Schulsystem im Kreis Mettmann e konzeption
der wissenschaftlichen Begleitevaluation" [Towards an inclusive school system
Appendix A. Supplementary data in the district of Mettmann - conception of the scientific evaluation]. Zeitschrift
für Heilpa€dagogik, 4e16.
Supplementary data to this article can be found online at Knauder, H., & Koschmieder, C. (2019). Individualized student support in primary
school teaching: A review of influencing factors using the theory of planned
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tate.2023.104225. behavior (TPB). Teaching and Teacher Education, 66e76. https://doi.org/10.1016/
j.tate.2018.09.012
References Koster, M., Pijl, S. J., Nakken, H., & Van Houten, E. (2010). Social participation of
students with special needs in regular primary education in The Netherlands.
International Journal of Disability, Development and Education, 59e75. https://
Abegglen, H. J., & Hessels, M. G. P. (2018). Measures of individual, collaborative and
doi.org/10.1080/10349120903537905
environmental characteristics predict Swiss school principals’, teachers’ and
Kuyini, A. B., & Desai, I. (2007). Principals' and teachers' attitudes and knowledge of
student teachers’ attitudes towards inclusive education. Psychoeducational
inclusive education as predictors of effective teaching practices in Ghana.
Assessment, Intervention and Rehabilitation, 1e24. https://doi.org/10.30436/
Journal of Research in Special Educational Needs, 104e113. https://doi.org/
PAIR18-01
10.1111/j.1471-3802.2007.00086.x
Ahmmed, M., Sharma, U., & Deppeler, J. (2014). Variables affecting teachers' in-
Lee, J., Cerreto, F. A., & Lee, J. (2010). Theory of planned behavior and teachers’
tentions to include students with disabilities in regular primary schools in
decisions regarding use of educational technology. Journal of Educational
Bangladesh. Disability & Society. https://doi.org/10.1080/
Technology & Society, 152e164.
09687599.2013.796878
Lüdecke, D. (2021). sjPlot: Data visualization for statistics in social science. https://
Ajzen, I. (1985). From intentions to actions: A theory of planned behavior. In J. Kuhl,
strengejacke.github.io/sjPlot/.
& J. Beckmann (Eds.), Action control (pp. 11e39). Springer. https://doi.org/
MacFarlane, K., & Woolfson, L. M. (2013). Teacher attitudes and behavior toward the
10.1007/978-3-642-642-69746-3 2.
inclusion of children with social, emotional and behavioral difficulties in
Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and human
mainstream schools: An application of the theory of planned behavior. Teaching
decision processes. https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-T
and Teacher Education, 46e52. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tate.2012.08.006
Ajzen, I. (2005). Attitudes, personality and behavior. In Mapping social psychology.
Malak, M. S., Sharma, U., & Deppeler, J. M. (2018). Predictors of primary school-
Open Univ. Press.
teachers' behavioural intention to teach students demonstrating inappropriate
Ajzen, I. (2012). The theory of planned behavior. In P. A. M. Lange, A. W. Kruglanski,
behaviour in regular classrooms. Cambridge Journal of Education, 495e514.
& E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of theories of social psychology (Vol. 1, pp.
https://doi.org/10.1080/0305764X.2017.1364698
438e459). Sage. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781446249215.n22.
McEachan, R., Taylor, N., Harrison, R., Lawton, R., Gardner, P., & Conner, M. (2016).
Ajzen, I., & Kruglanski, A. W. (2019). Reasoned action in the service of goal pursuit.
Meta-Analysis of the reasoned action approach (RAA) to understanding health
Psychological Review, 774e786. https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000155
behaviors. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 592e612. https://doi.org/10.1007/
Armitage, C. J., & Conner, M. (2001). Efficacy of the theory of planned behaviour: A
s12160-016-9798-4
meta-analytic review. British Journal of Social Psychology, 471e499. https://
van Mieghem, A., Verschueren, K., Petry, K., & Struyf, E. (2018). An analysis of
doi.org/10.1348/014466601164939
research on inclusive education: A systematic search and meta review. Inter-
Armstrong, D. (2014). Educator perceptions of children who present with social,
national Journal of Inclusive Education, 1e15. https://doi.org/10.1080/
emotional and behavioural difficulties: A literature review with implications for
13603116.2018.1482012
recent educational policy in england and internationally. International Journal of
Moeyaert, M., Klingbeil, D. A., Rodabaugh, E., & Turan, M. (2021). Three-level meta-
Inclusive Education, 731e745. https://doi.org/10.1080/13603116.2013.823245
analysis of single-case data regarding the effects of peer tutoring on academic
Autorengruppe, B. (2020). Bildung in Deutschland 2020. Ein indikatorengestützter
and social-behavioral outcomes for at-risk students and students with dis-
Bericht mit einer Analyse zu Bildung in einer digitalisierten Welt. In [Education
abilities. Remedial and Special Education, 42(2), 94e106. https://doi.org/10.1177/
in Germany 2020. An indicator-based report with an analysis of education in a
0741932519855079
digitalized world.]. wbv Media. https://doi.org/10.3278/6001820gw.
Muijs, D., West, M., & Ainscow, M. (2010). Why network? Theoretical perspectives
Avramidis, E., & Norwich, B. (2002). Teachers' attitudes towards integration/inclu-
on networking. School Effectiveness and School Improvement, 21(1), 5e26.
sion: A review of the literature. European Journal of Special Needs Education,
https://doi.org/10.1080/09243450903569692
129e147. https://doi.org/10.1080/08856250210129056
Opoku, M., Cuskelly, M., Rayner, C., & Pedersen, S. (2022). The impact of teacher
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. Freeman.
attributes on intentions to practice inclusive education in secondary schools in
Batsiou, S., Bebetsos, E., Panteli, P., & Antoniou, P. (2008). Attitudes and intention of
Ghana.International Journal of Disability (pp. 382e398). Development and Ed-
Greek and Cypriot primary education teachers towards teaching pupils with
ucation. https://doi.org/10.1080/1034912X.2020.1731434
special educational needs in mainstream schools. International Journal of In-
Orbell, S., & Sheeran, P. (1998). ‘Inclined abstainers’: A problem for predicting
clusive Education, 201e219. https://doi.org/10.1080/13603110600855739
health-related behaviour. British Journal of Social Psychology, 37, 151e165.
Boer, A. de, Pijl, S. J., & Minnaert, A. (2011). Regular primary schoolteachers' atti-
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8309.1998. tb01162.x
tudes towards inclusive education: A review of the literature. International
Pinheiro, J., Bates, D., & Core Team, R. (2022). nlme: Linear and nonlinear mixed
Journal of Inclusive Education, 331e353. https://doi.org/10.1080/
effects models. R package version, 3, 1e157. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package
13603110903030089
¼nlme.
Bowman-Perrott, L., Davis, H., Vannest, K., Williams, L., Greenwood, C., & Parker, R.
Powell, J. J. (2015). Barriers to inclusion: Special education in the United States and
(2013). Academic benefits of peer tutoring: A meta-analytic review of single-
Germany. Routledge.
case research. School Psychology Review, 42(1), 39e55. https://doi.org/10.1080/
Schurig, M., Weiß, S., Kiel, E., Heimlich, U., & Gebhardt, M. (2020). Assessment of the
02796015.2013.12087490
quality of inclusive schools A short form of the quality scale of inclusive school
Chapman, C., & Fullan, M. (2007). Collaboration and partnership for equitable
development (QU! SS)ereliability, factorial structure and measurement
improvement: Towards a networked learning system? School Leadership &
invariance. International Journal of Inclusive Education, 1e16. https://doi.org/
Management, 27(3), 207e211. https://doi.org/10.1080/13632430701379354
10.1080/13603116.2020.1862405
Conner, M., & Sparks, P. (2015). The theory of planned behaviour and reasoned
Sharma, K., & Mahapatra, B. C. (2007). Emerging trends in inclusive education. IVY
action approach. In M. Conner, & P. Norman (Eds.), Predicting and changing

10
K. Urton, J. Wilbert, J. Krull et al. Teaching and Teacher Education 132 (2023) 104225

Publication House. the theory of planned behaviour: The effect of question wording. Current Psy-
Sheeran, P., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Bargh, J. A. (2013). Nonconscious processes and chology, 22(3), 234e251. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-003-1019-1
health. Health Psychology, 32, 460e473. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029203 Urton, K., Bo€rnert-Ringleb, M., Krull, J., Wilbert, J., & Hennemann, T. (2018). Ink-
Sniehotta, F. F., Presseau, J., & Araújo-Soares, V. (2014). Time to retire the theory of lusives Schulklima: Konzeptionelle Darstellung eines Rahmendmodells [Inclu-
planned behaviour. Health Psychology Review, 8(1), 1e7. https://doi.org/10.1080/ sive school climate: Conceptual presentation of a framework model]. Zeitschrift
17437199.2013.869710 für Heilpa€dagogik, 40e52.
Sniehotta, F. F., Presseau, J., & Araújo-Soares, V. (2014). Time to retire the theory of Urton, K., Wilbert, J., & Hennemann, T. (2014). Attitudes towards inclusion and self-
planned behaviour. Health Psychology Review, 8, 1e7. https://doi.org/10.1080/ efficacy of principals and teachers. Learning Disabilities: A Contemporary Journal,
17437199.2013.869710 151e168.
Snijders, T. A. B., & Bosker, R. J. (2012). Multilevel analysis: An introduction to basic Yan, Z., & Sin, K.-F. (2014). Inclusive education: Teachers' intentions and behaviour
and advanced multilevel modeling. Sage. analysed from the viewpoint of the theory of planned behaviour. International
Steinmetz, H., Knappstein, M., Ajzen, I., Schmidt, P., & Kabst, R. (2016). How effective Journal of Inclusive Education, 72e85. https://doi.org/10.1080/
are behavior change interventions based on the theory of planned behavior? A 13603116.2012.757811
three-level meta-analysis. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 216e233. https://doi.org/ Yan, Z., & Sin, K.-F. (2015). Exploring the intentions and practices of principals
10.1027/2151-2604/a000255 regarding inclusive education: An application of the theory of planned behav-
Sutton, S., French, D. P., Hennings, S. J., Mitchell, J. O., Wareham, N. J., Griffin, S., iour. Cambridge Journal of Education, 205e221. https://doi.org/10.1080/
Hardeman, W., & Kinmonth, A. L. (2003). Eliciting salient beliefs in research on 0305764X.2014.934203

11

You might also like