Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 5

Personality and Individual Differences 47 (2009) 758–762

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Personality and Individual Differences


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid

Empathy deficits and trait emotional intelligence in psychopathy and


Machiavellianism
Farah Ali, Ines Sousa Amorim, Tomas Chamorro-Premuzic *
Department of Psychology, Goldsmiths, University of London, London, UK

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: This study investigated the relationships between psychopathy (primary and secondary), Machi-
Received 6 April 2009 avellianism, trait emotional intelligence and empathy, using an image task that required an appropriate
Received in revised form 4 June 2009 empathic response to the emotional displays of others (happy, sad and neutral). Results indicated that
Accepted 12 June 2009
primary psychopathy and Machiavellianism were positively associated with the experience of positive
Available online 15 July 2009
affect from sad stimuli, while secondary psychopathy and Machiavellianism were positively associated
with the experience of negative affect in response to neutral stimuli, and the opposite pattern was found
Keywords:
for trait emotional intelligence. Regressional analyses demonstrated that secondary psychopathy, Machi-
Primary psychopathy
Secondary psychopathy
avellianism, trait emotional intelligence and state anxiety are important predictors when stimuli are
Machiavellianism ambiguous.
Empathy Ó 2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Emotional intelligence

1. Introduction (e.g., McHoskey et al., 1998; Mealey, 1995) and it has been sug-
gested that they denote a single construct (Lee & Ashton, 2005;
The study of aversive personality traits has received much McHoskey et al., 1998). However, Vernon et al. (2008) have dem-
attention in the past few years, particularly the non-clinical traits onstrated that Machiavellianism and psychopathy differ in herita-
of psychopathy and Machiavellianism (e.g., Lee & Ashton, 2005; bility, and research indicates that although overlapping, they are
Vernon, Villani, Vickers, & Harris, 2008). Within the personality- distinct constructs (Paulhus & Williams, 2002; Williams & Paulhus,
based approach to psychopathy there is a consensus that guiltless- 2004).
ness, callousness, dishonesty, egocentricity, failure to form close Unsurprisingly, psychopathy (Malterer, Glass, & Newman,
emotional bonds, low anxiety proneness, superficial charm and 2008) and Machiavellianism (Austin, Farrelly, Black, & Moore,
blame externalisation all represent core features (Hare, 1991). The- 2007) have been recently associated with lower trait emotional
oretically, psychopathy is regarded as a heterogeneous concept intelligence (also known as trait emotional self-efficacy). Individu-
consisting of primary psychopathy, which is characterised by fea- als high in trait emotional intelligence are good at managing their
tures such as cruelty and lack of affect and secondary psychopathy, stress levels and demonstrate enhanced psychosocial functioning,
which is characterised by features such as impulsivity, neuroticism such as better social relationships (e.g., Schutte et al., 2001).
and aggression (Levenson, Kiehl, & Fitzpatrick, 1995). Being able to empathise is an important part of emotional intel-
Machiavellianism involves interpersonal strategies that pro- ligence and a defining criterion of clinical psychopathy is reduced
mote the use of deception, manipulation and exploitation, and empathic responding to victims (Hare, 1991). The ability to repeat-
the Machiavellian individual can be described as cynical, domi- edly cause serious harm to others is an indicator of a profound dis-
neering, aloof and practical (McHoskey, Worzel, & Szyarto, 1998). turbance in an appropriate ‘‘empathic” response to the suffering of
According to Christie (1970) Machiavellian individuals are success- another (Blair, 2005). Empathy is known to inhibit and moderate
ful manipulators characterised by: (a) lack of interpersonal affect aggression (Richardson, Hammock, Smith, & Gardner, 1994;
in interpersonal relationships, (b) lack of concern with conven- Wheeler, George, & Dahl, 2002). Past research indicates that clini-
tional morality, (c) lack of gross psychopathology, and (d) low ideo- cal psychopathic individuals show a selective empathic deficit in
logical commitment. Various researchers have noted the that they are impaired in the recognition of sad and fearful facial
conceptual similarity between Machiavellianism and psychopathy expressions (e.g., Montagne et al., 2005).
Although investigators have recently begun to extend findings
* Corresponding author. Address: Department of Psychology, Goldsmiths,
from emotional deficits to psychopathy in normal populations,
University of London, New Cross, London SE146NW, UK. Tel.: +44 (0) 2079197885. only a few studies have looked at whether empathy deficits exist
E-mail address: pss02tc@gold.ac.uk (T. Chamorro-Premuzic). in non-clinical samples (e.g., Del Gaizo & Falkenbach, 2008;

0191-8869/$ - see front matter Ó 2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.paid.2009.06.016
F. Ali et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 47 (2009) 758–762 759

Mahmut, Homewood, & Stevenson, 2008), this is surprising, espe- 2.2. Materials
cially when research with non-clinical samples has found that de-
spite lower base-rates, there is evidence for diverse expressions of Levenson self-report psychopathy scale (LSRP): The LSRP scale
psychopathic traits across the population (Skeem, Poythress, (Levenson et al., 1995) is a 26-item self-report measure designed
Edens, Lilienfeld, & Cale, 2003). Del Gaizo and Falkenbach’s to assess psychopathic attributes in non-institutionalised samples
(2008) study indicated that primary psychopathic traits were pos- and to evaluate both the behavioural and personality traits com-
itively correlated with accuracy of perception of fearful faces, while monly associated with psychopathy in the literature. The primary
secondary psychopathic traits were not related to emotional recog- psychopathy scale consists of 16 items, ranging from disagree
nition. Mahmut et al.’s (2008) study indicated that individuals strongly (1) to agree strongly (4), designed to assess the core per-
scoring highly on psychopathy demonstrated low levels of empa- sonality features described by Cleckley (1988), such as being self-
thy on an emotional empathy questionnaire; the EEQ (Mehrabian ish, uncaring and manipulative. The secondary psychopathy scale
& Epstein, 1972). consists of 10 items assessing anti-social behaviour, a self-defeat-
Analogous to the psychopathic personality, conventional moral- ing lifestyle and impulsivity. Cronbach’s a in the current study
ity is not a concern to Machiavellian individuals; instead, they were .84 for the primary psychopathy scale and .72 for the second-
demonstrate shallow emotional involvement with others and ary psychopathy scale. The LSRP is both reliable and valid (McHos-
sanction behaviour which is emotionally-manipulative (Austin key et al., 1998).
et al., 2007). Because individuals high in Machiavellianism are Mach-IV: Machiavellianism was assessed with the Mach-IV
likely to exploit others and to view others in a goal-oriented man- (Christie & Geis, 1970), which has 20 items covering the use of de-
ner (they see people ‘‘as a means to an end”), unsurprisingly ceit in interpersonal relationships, a cynical attitude to human nat-
Machiavellianism has demonstrated negative correlations with ure and a lack of concern for conventional morality. Participants
empathy (e.g., Austin et al., 2007; Wastell & Booth, 2003). indicate their response on a seven-point scale ranging from dis-
The aim of the current study was to further explore the relation- agree strongly (1) to agree strongly (7), with higher scores indicat-
ships between non-clinical psychopathy, Machiavellianism, trait ing higher levels of Machiavellianism. The reliability and the
emotional intelligence and deficits in empathy using a purposely- validity of the Mach-IV are well documented (Fehr, Samsom, &
designed visual task to assess appropriateness of empathic re- Paulhus, 1992). Cronbach’s a in the current study was .77.
sponses to the emotional displays of others. Spielberger state-trait anxiety (STAI): The STAI (Spielberger, Gor-
Hoffman (2000) defined empathy as ‘‘feelings that are more such, & Lushene, 1970) consists of two 20-item scales: the state
congruent with another’s situation than with one’s own situation” and trait anxiety scales. In this study, only items from the SAI were
(p. 30), while Mehrabian and Epstein (1972) defined emotional used (STAI Form Y-1). This is a 20-item self-evaluation question-
empathy as a ‘‘vicariously emotional response to the perceived naire with responses to the statements about feelings at the pres-
emotional experiences of others” (p. 525). These definitions sug- ent time ranging from 1 (not at all) to 4 (very much so), with higher
gest an affective reaction (‘‘affective/emotional empathy”; see Lo- scores indicative of greater state anxiety. In the current study, six
soya & Eisenberg, 2001), not just a cognitive ability to read items of the SAI were used due to time constraints. Participants
another person’s thoughts or feelings correctly (‘‘cognitive empa- were given the SAI to complete before and after viewing the
thy”). Following this line of thought, this study is concerned with images. Cronbach’s a in the current study was .82. for time one
whether an individual experiences an appropriate empathic re- and .80 for time two.
sponse rather than how capable he or she is of such an experience, Trait emotional intelligence questionnaire – short form (TEIQue–
hence the use of an empathy task that is not limited to emotion SF): The TEIQue–SF (Petrides & Furnham, 2006) is a 30-item self-re-
recognition. port scale that yields a global measure of trait emotional intelli-
Although psychopathy is considered a heterogeneous concept, gence and emotional self-efficacy, namely, the ability to identify
many emotional deficit studies have approached it as homoge- and manage one’s own emotions and the emotions of others. The
neous, potentially missing important differences between the short form is based on the 153-item TEIQue and has demonstrated
two. An important difference between primary and secondary psy- adequate reliability and validity (Petrides & Furnham, 2006). Par-
chopathy is that negative affect is notably absent from primary ticipants are asked to rate their degree of agreement with each
psychopathy, whereas secondary psychopathy is associated with item on a seven-point Likert-type scale with responses ranging
negative affect (Del Gaizo & Falkenbach, 2008). For this reason, from completely disagree (1) to completely agree (7). Cronbach’s
the current study included a measure of state anxiety in order to a in this study was .90.
measure the levels of anxiety experienced by participants at partic- Empathy image task using the self-assessment manikin (SAM):1 To
ular points during the study. Following on from findings from pre- create the image set we initially selected 45 images; 15 were classi-
vious research it was hypothesised that primary and secondary fied as representing sadness, 15 representing happiness and 15 rep-
psychopathy and Machiavellianism would be associated with low resenting neutral emotion. The stimuli were selected from the
emotional intelligence and empathic deficiencies, i.e. the expres- gettyimagesÒ database, which depicts individuals of various ages,
sion of inappropriate affect in response to images of positive and gender and ethnicity showing a range of emotions. Each image con-
negative affective content. sisted of one individual who was the focal point of the image. Three
independent raters (all psychology students) then rated the emo-
2. Method tions displayed by the individual in each picture. Subsequently, nine
images were discarded due to inter-rater disagreement over the
2.1. Participants emotion displayed (i.e. disagreement between sadness and fear
and happiness and surprise). The resulting image set consisted of
Participants were 84 undergraduates (67 female) who partici- 36 images of male and female individuals expressing sad, happy
pated in the study in exchange for course credits. Their ages ranged and neutral emotional expressions, namely, 12 images of sadness
from 18 to 46 years (M = 20.7, SD = 4.1); 30 were White British (overtly sad expressions; a = .81) and 12 images of happiness
(36%), 23 White Other (27%), six Black Caribbean (7%) and five
Black African (6%). The remaining 20 participants (24%) were of
Mixed, South Asian, East Asian and Arab ethnicity. Participants
all had normal, or corrected to normal vision. 1
All images are available on request.
760 F. Ali et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 47 (2009) 758–762

(overtly happy expressions; a = .84) and 12 images of emotionally tral images. State anxiety at time one and time two correlated neg-
neutral expressions (a = .78). atively with valence to the neutral images.
The SAM (Bradley & Lang, 1994), a non-verbal pictorial assess-
ment methodology of a human-like stimulus, was used to assess 3.2. Multiple regressions
affective responding after participants viewed each image. The
SAM permits the assessment of the core dimensions of emotional Two standard multiple regressions were performed next; the
experience: valence (scale 1), arousal (scale 2) and control/domi- first with valence to sad images as the criterion variable and
nance (scale 3). SAM ratings are made along five figures for each Machiavellianism and primary psychopathy as predictors and the
scale. Participants select their current level of affective state along second regression with valence to neutral images as the criterion
these dimensions on a nine-point scale by placing an ‘‘X” over any variable and Machiavellianism, secondary psychopathy, trait emo-
of the five figures in each scale, or between any two figures. Total tional intelligence and state anxiety (time one) entered as predic-
scores range from zero to nine for each dimension. In the current tors. State anxiety time two was excluded from the regressions
study, the valence scale of the SAM was used. Valence refers to as it was highly correlated with state anxiety time one. Results of
whether a stimulus is viewed as pleasant or unpleasant (Lang, these analyses are presented in Table 2. In the first regression, pri-
1995). Figures depicting valence range from a low-spirited, frown- mary psychopathy and Machiavellianism did not significantly pre-
ing manikin to a widely smiling one, going through a middle neu- dict the variability in valence to sad images (F(2,81) = 2.61,
tral stance. The SAM has sound psychometric properties and has adjusted R2 = .04, p > .05). In the second regression, secondary psy-
been successfully employed to measure affective responses (Brad- chopathy, Machiavellianism, trait emotional intelligence and state
ley & Lang, 1994). The SAM scale was chosen for the current study, anxiety significantly predicted the variability in valence to neutral
rather than a simple emotion identifying task, because it allows an images (F(4,79) = 3.62, adjusted R2 = .11, p < .01).
insight into the emotions felt vicariously by participants in re- Sobel’s tests of mediation (Sobel, 1982) were then performed in
sponse to the emotions of others. It has also previously been used three cases where mediation conditions were met (Baron & Kenny,
alongside other measures in empathy studies (e.g. Brown, Bradley, 1986), with one model proving to be significant. The effect of sec-
& Lang, 2006; Danziger, Prkachin, & Willer, 2006). Cronbach’s a for ondary psychopathy on valence to neutral images was fully medi-
the valence scale in the current study was .80 for valence to the sad ated by trait emotional intelligence (Z = 2.30, p < .05), with
images, .87 for valence to the happy images and .60 for valence to secondary psychopathy negatively affecting trait emotional intelli-
the neutral images. gence. State anxiety did not significantly mediate the effect of sec-
ondary psychopathy on valence to neutral images (Z = 1.64,
2.3. Procedure p > .05) and trait emotional intelligence did not significantly medi-
ate the effect of Machiavellianism on valence to neutral images
Every participant was tested individually in a quiet university (Z = 1.73, p > .05).
room (lab cubicle). Each participant was seated in a comfortable
chair behind a small table approximately 2 m from the screen on 4. Discussion
which the slide stimuli were to be displayed. Participants com-
pleted the questionnaire battery and were then presented with The present study set out to examine whether non-clinical psy-
each image sequentially. The 36 slides were projected individually chopathy (primary and secondary) and Machiavellianism were
onto a screen at the front of the room, with the order of presenta- associated with inappropriate empathic responses to the emo-
tion of the pictures randomised for each participant. Participants tional displays of others. Our results suggest that primary psychop-
were instructed to look at each image and rate their affect in re- athy and Machiavellianism are positively associated with
sponse to the image on the valence scale (scale 1) of the SAM. experiencing positive affect (valence) when looking at sad images.
Although no limits were issued for viewing the images, all partic- This finding is consistent with the idea that individuals high in pri-
ipants were told to ‘‘study the image carefully” before responding. mary psychopathy do not experience negative emotions (e.g.,
Once they had completed the valence scale for that particular im- Cleckley, 1988; Mealey, 1995) and therefore images of sadness
age, the next image was displayed. are unlikely to cause them any sort of distress. Interestingly, in
the current study, individuals high in primary psychopathy actu-
ally responded with positive affect (rather than simply not
3. Results responding with negative affect), to negative images. Research in
clinical samples has demonstrated that psychopathic individuals
3.1. Correlations show reduced autonomic responses to stimuli associated with
the distress and sadness of another individual (e.g., Blair, 1999;
Intercorrelations among primary psychopathy, secondary psy- House & Milligan, 1976); therefore it is possible that individuals
chopathy, Machiavellianism, state anxiety (time one and time who score highly in primary psychopathic traits would actually
two), trait emotional intelligence and valence (to the happy, sad experience, or at least report, pleasurable affect from the sadness
and neutral images) are shown in Table 1. Gender correlated with of others. However, a replication of this finding is needed before
primary psychopathy and Machiavellianism, with males scoring such conjectures are made, especially when considering the lack
higher on both traits. Primary psychopathy positively correlated and contradictory nature of research addressing the relationship
with Machiavellianism and valence to the sad images. The correla- between psychopathy and sadism (e.g., Kirsch & Becker, 2007).
tional analyses indicated that secondary psychopathy positively The apparent dysfunction of an appropriate empathic response
correlated with Machiavellianism, state anxiety time one and state in primary psychopathy in this sample lends support to research
anxiety time two. Secondary psychopathy correlated negatively indicating that psychopathic individuals show a selective emo-
with trait emotional intelligence and valence to the neutral images, tional empathy dysfunction by not processing sad (and fearful)
while Machiavellianism negatively correlated with trait emotional expressions appropriately (Blair, 1995).
intelligence and with valence to the neutral images, but correlated When ambiguous (i.e. neutral) images were presented, second-
positively with valence to the sad images. Trait emotional intelli- ary psychopathy, Machiavellianism and state anxiety (time one
gence correlated negatively with state anxiety time one and state and time two) were positively associated with the experience of
anxiety time two and correlated positively with valence to the neu- negative affect. The association between secondary psychopathy
F. Ali et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 47 (2009) 758–762 761

Table 1
Intercorrelations: Bivariate Pearson correlation coefficients.

M SD a 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
* **
1 Gender .12 .25 .01 .29 .05 .12 .09 .20 .21 .15
2 Age 20.62 4.12 .13 .01 .14 .06 .17 .03 .03 .03 .12
3 Primary psychopathy 30.88 7.68 .84 .33** .70** .17 .01 .01 .18 .22* .11
4 Secondary psychopathy 22.00 5.00 .72 .36** .48** .28** .31** .02 .02 .23*
5 Machiavellianism 95.30 7.68 .77 .23* .01 .06 .15 .23* .22*
6 Trait emotional intelligence 143.93 24.26 .90 .60** .64** .01 .06 .35**
7 State anxiety (time 1) 11.67 3.71 .82 .77** .16 .07 .28*
8 State anxiety (time 2) 12.13 3.57 .80 .20 .03 .34**
9 Valence happy images 7.08 .94 .87 .48** .19
10 Valence sad images 3.16 .96 .80 .20
11 Valence neutral images 5.17 .56 .60

n = 84.
*
p < .05 level (2-tailed).
**
p < .01 level (2-tailed).

Table 2
Summary of regression analyses.

Criterion Predictors B SE B b t p
Valence to sad images Constant 1.82 0.64 2.85 0.01
Machiavellianism 0.01 0.01 0.16 1.07 0.29
Primary psychopathy 0.01 0.02 0.10 0.69 0.49
Valence to neutral images Constant 5.36 0.88 6.12 0.00
Secondary psychopathy 0.01 0.01 0.04 0.28 0.78
Machiavellianism 0.01 0.01 0.16 0.14 0.16
Trait emotional intelligence 0.01 0.01 0.21 1.45 0.15
State anxiety (time one) 0.02 0.20 0.14 1.08 0.28

Note: R2 = .06 and DR2 = .04 (p > .05) for valence to sad images; R2 = .16 and DR2 = .11 (p < .01) for valence to neutral images.

and negative affect experienced in the presence of ambiguous secondary psychopathy negatively affected trait emotional intelli-
stimuli is consistent with research findings on the positive link be- gence when considering that secondary psychopathy is character-
tween negative emotions and this trait (e.g., Del Gaizo & Falken- ised by features such as aggression and impulsivity.
bach, 2008). Although Machiavellianism is related to both Males were associated with higher scores of primary psychopa-
primary and secondary psychopathy, there is an important differ- thy and Machiavellianism. This is consistent with self-report evi-
ence between Machiavellianism and primary psychopathy; the for- dence that indicates that males score higher than females in
mer trait is linked to heightened experience of anxiety (McHoskey psychopathy (e.g., Lilienfeld & Hess, 2001) and Machiavellianism
et al., 1998), something which is notably absent from the latter (e.g., McHoskey, 2001).
(Cleckley, 1988; Lykken, 1995). Consequently, in a similar vein to The results of the regressions highlight the importance of trait
individuals high in secondary psychopathy, our findings showed emotional intelligence when it comes to explaining why individu-
that individuals high in Machiavellianism seem to have also expe- als higher in secondary psychopathy interpret emotionally neutral
rienced negative affect from the neutral images. images in terms of negative affect; thus when individuals are
To date, we are aware of only one other study that has indicated matched in trait emotional intelligence secondary psychopathy
an inappropriate response to neutral faces. In a study by Dadds no longer affects responses to neutral images. On the other hand,
et al. (2006), children scoring highly in psychopathic traits had dif- Machiavellianism and state anxiety affect these responses inde-
ficulty in recognising neutral faces in an emotion recognition task; pendently of trait emotional intelligence.
neutral faces where often mistakenly rated as angry. To further There are limitations to the current study, including the use of a
understand this apparent misattribution to neutral faces more re- small, majority female, undergraduate sample, which may make
search into facial emotion processing/recognition should use neu- generalisability to other non-clinical groups difficult. Nevertheless,
tral expressions alongside other emotions in samples with undergraduate samples have the advantage of being relatively free
psychopathic traits. of severe Axis I disorders (e.g., major depressive disorder, alcohol
Trait emotional intelligence was associated with attributing po- dependence), which could distort the reporting of enduring per-
sitive affect to the neutral images and negatively associated with sonality traits and personality disorder features (Lilienfeld & Pen-
secondary psychopathy, Machiavellianism and state anxiety at na, 2001).
time one and time two. This finding is consistent with studies In the current study, only two of the basic emotions were used,
showing that individuals higher in trait emotional intelligence this study could be extended by also using surprised, disgusted, an-
have a more positive outlook in general (Austin, Saklofske, & Egan, gry, and fearful images. It appears that ambiguous images work the
2005) and experience greater psychological well-being (Mavroveli, best when predicting individual differences in the traits examined
Petrides, Rieffe, & Bakker, 2007). No association was found be- in the current study. The use of static images and the low emo-
tween primary psychopathy and trait emotional intelligence; this tional involvement of impersonal images may have limited the
is in contrast to previous research as Malterer et al. (2008) found validity of empathic responsiveness on behalf of participants; be-
a negative association between primary psychopathy and a mea- cause emotions displayed in the real world vary in intensity and
sure of emotional intelligence, however, this association was quite content. Future studies could use less artificial means for invoking
small. In relation to the mediation effect, it was not surprising that emotional empathy such as video footage. To date, no studies have
762 F. Ali et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 47 (2009) 758–762

measured autonomic responses to affective material in a non-clin- House, T. H., & Milligan, W. L. (1976). Autonomic responses to modeled distress
in prison psychopaths. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34,
ical sample with psychopathic traits; this may be worthwhile be-
556–560.
cause clinical psychopathic samples have demonstrated reduced Kirsch, L. G., & Becker, J. V. (2007). Emotional deficits in psychopathy and sexual
autonomic responses to emotional stimuli. sadism: Implications for violent and sadistic behavior. Clinical Psychology
It should also be noted that while copious research has investi- Review, 27, 904–922.
Lang, P. J. (1995). The emotion probe: Studies of motivation and attention. American
gated deficits in facial emotion processing in psychopathy, to date, Psychologist, 50, 372–385.
apart from the current study, no study has investigated this in rela- Lee, K., & Ashton, M. C. (2005). Psychopathy, Machiavellianism, and narcissism in
tion to Machiavellianism. This may be worthwhile, not only when the five-factor model and the HEXACO model of personality structure.
Personality and Individual Differences, 38, 1571–1582.
taking into account the results of the current study, but also previ- Levenson, M. R., Kiehl, K. A., & Fitzpatrick, C. M. (1995). Assessing psychopathic
ous research which has demonstrated that Machiavellianism is attributes in a noninstitutionalised population. Journal of Personality and Social
associated with empathic deficits. Psychology, 68, 151–158.
Lilienfeld, S. O., & Hess, T. H. (2001). Psychopathic personality traits and
Despite these limitations, the current results suggest that even somatization: Sex differences and the mediating role of negative
in a small student sample, brief self-reports of constructs related to emotionality. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 23, 11–24.
emotional-processing deficits are predictive of individual differ- Lilienfeld, S. O., & Penna, S. (2001). Anxiety sensitivity: Relations to psychopathy,
DSM-IV personality disorder features, and personality traits. Journal of Anxiety
ences in emotional reactions to images of neutral emotional Disorders, 15, 367–393.
valence. Losoya, S. H., & Eisenberg, N. (2001). Affective empathy. In J. A. Hall & F. J. Bernieri
We hope that the current study will encourage further research (Eds.), Interpersonal sensitivity: Theory and measurement (pp. 21–43). Mahwah,
NJ: Erlbaum.
into aversive personality traits and emotional deficits in non-clin-
Lykken, D. T. (1995). The antisocial personalities. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
ical populations and the use of studies that do not rely solely on Mahmut, M. K., Homewood, J., & Stevenson, R. J. (2008). The characteristics of non-
self-report inventories. Continuing research into the emotional criminals with high psychopathy traits: Are they similar to criminal
deficits manifested in aversive personality traits such as psychop- psychopaths? Journal of Research in Personality, 42, 679–692.
Malterer, M. B., Glass, S. J., & Newman, J. P. (2008). Psychopathy and trait emotional
athy and Machiavellianism may facilitate a greater understanding intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 44, 735–745.
of both non-clinical and clinical psychopathology. Mavroveli, S., Petrides, K. V., Rieffe, C., & Bakker, F. (2007). Trait emotional
intelligence, psychological well-being, and peer-rated social competence in
adolescence. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 25, 263–275.
References McHoskey, J. W. (2001). Machiavellianism and personality dysfunction. Personality
and Individual Differences, 31, 791–798.
Austin, E. J., Farrelly, D., Black, C., & Moore, H. (2007). Emotional intelligence, McHoskey, J. W., Worzel, W., & Szyarto, C. (1998). Machiavellianism and
Machiavellianism and emotional manipulation: Does EI have a dark side? Psychopathy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 192–210.
Personality and Individual Differences, 43, 179–189. Mealey, L. (1995). The sociobiology of sociopathy: An integrated evolutionary
Austin, E. J., Saklofske, D. H., & Egan, V. (2005). Personality, well-being and health model. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 18, 523–599.
correlates of trait emotional intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, Mehrabian, A., & Epstein, N. (1972). A measure of emotional empathy. Journal of
38, 547–558. Personality, 40, 525–543.
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable distinction in Montagne, B., van Honk, J., Kessels, R. P. C., Frigerio, E., Burt, M., van Zandvoort, M. J.
social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic and statistical E., et al. (2005). Reduced efficiency in recognising fear in subjects scoring high
considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182. on psychopathic personality characteristics. Personality and Individual and
Blair, R. J. R. (1995). A cognitive developmental approach to morality: Investigating Differences, 38, 5–11.
the psychopath. Cognition, 57, 1–29. Paulhus, D. L., & Williams, K. M. (2002). The dark triad of personality: Narcissism,
Blair, R. J. R. (1999). Responsiveness to distress cues in the child with psychopathic Machiavellianism and psychopathy. Journal of Research in Personality, 36,
tendencies. Personality and Individual Differences, 27, 135–145. 556–563.
Blair, R. J. R. (2005). Responding to the emotions of others: Dissociating forms of Petrides, K. V., & Furnham, A. (2006). User manual of the trait emotional intelligence
empathy through the study of typical and psychiatric populations. questionnaire (TEIQue). University of London: Institute of Education.
Consciousness and Cognition, 14, 698–718. Richardson, D. R., Hammock, G. S., Smith, S. M., & Gardner, W. (1994). Empathy as a
Bradley, M., & Lang, P. (1994). Measuring emotion: The Self-Assessment Manikin cognitive inhibitor of interpersonal aggression. Aggressive Behavior, 20,
and the semantic differential. Journal of Behavioral Therapy and Experimental 275–289.
Psychiatry, 1, 49–59. Schutte, N. S., Malouff, J. M., Bobik, C., Conston, T., Greeson, C., & Jedlicka, C. (2001).
Brown, L. M., Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (2006). Affective reactions to pictures of Emotional intelligence and interpersonal relations. Journal of Social Psychology,
ingroup and outgroup members. Biological Psychology, 71, 303–311. 141, 523–536.
Christie, R. (1970). Why Machiavelli? In R. Christie & F. Geis (Eds.), Studies in Skeem, J. L., Poythress, N., Edens, J. F., Lilienfeld, S. O., & Cale, E. M. (2003).
Machiavellianism (pp. 1–9). New York: Academic Press. Psychopathic personality or personalities? Exploring potential variants of
Christie, R., & Geis, F. (1970). Studies in Machiavellianism. New York: Academic Press. psychopathy and their implications for risk assessment. Aggression & Violent
Cleckley, H. (1988). The mask of sanity (5th ed.). Augusta, GA: Emily S. Cleckley Behavior, 8, 513–546.
(Original work published 1941). Sobel, M. E. (1982). Asymptotic intervals for indirect effects in structural equations
Dadds, M. R., Perry, Y., Hawes, D. J., Merz, S., Riddell, A. C., Haines, D. J., et al. (2006). models. In S. Leinhart (Ed.), Sociological methodology 1982 (pp. 290–312). San
Attention to the eyes and fear-recognition deficits in child psychopathy. The Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
British Journal of Psychiatry, 189, 280–281. Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., & Lushene, R. D. (1970). STAI manual for the state-
Danziger, N., Prkachin, K. M., & Willer, J. (2006). Is pain the price of empathy? The trait anxiety inventory (‘‘self evaluation questionnaire”). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting
perception of others’ pain in patients with congenital insensitivity to pain. Psychologists Press.
Brain, 129, 2494–2507. Vernon, P. A., Villani, C. V., Vickers, L. C., & Harris, J. A. (2008). A behavioral genetic
Del Gaizo, A. L., & Falkenbach, D. M. (2008). Primary and secondary psychopathic- investigation of the Dark Triad and the Big 5. Personality and Individual
traits and their relationship to perception and experience of emotion. Differences, 44, 445–452.
Personality and Individual Differences, 45, 206–212. Wastell, C., & Booth, A. (2003). Machiavellianism: An alexithymic perspective.
Fehr, B., Samsom, D., & Paulhus, D. L. (1992). The construct of Machiavellianism: Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 22, 730–744.
Twenty years later. In C. D. Spielberger & J. N. Butcher (Eds.). Advances in Wheeler, J. G., George, W. H., & Dahl, B. J. (2002). Sexually aggressive college males:
personality assessment (Vol. 9, pp. 77–116). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Empathy as a moderator in the ‘‘Confluence Model” of sexual aggression.
Hare, R. D. (1991). The Hare Psychopathy Checklist – Revised. North Tonawanda, NY: Personality and Individual Differences, 33, 759–775.
Multi-Health Systems. Williams, K. M., & Paulhus, D. L. (2004). Factor structure of the self report
Hoffman, M. L. (2000). Empathy and moral development: Implications for caring and psychopathy scale (SRP-II) in non-forensic samples. Personality and Individual
justice. New York: Cambridge University Press. Differences, 37, 765–778.

You might also like