Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/236602262

Resilience of the Immune System in Healthy


Young Students to 30-Hour Sleep Deprivation
with Psychological Stress

Article in NeuroImmunoModulation · April 2013


DOI: 10.1159/000348698 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

11 288

10 authors, including:

Lee Shaashua Liat Sorski


Weizmann Institute of Science Tel Aviv University
36 PUBLICATIONS 151 CITATIONS 34 PUBLICATIONS 216 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Ilan Wald Yair Bar-Haim


Tel Aviv University Tel Aviv University
19 PUBLICATIONS 389 CITATIONS 151 PUBLICATIONS 7,019 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Suseptability to ptsd View project

Sleep study in children with BISQ View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Ilan Wald on 20 May 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Original Paper

Neuroimmunomodulation 2013;20:194–204 Received: November 15, 2012


Accepted after revision: January 30, 2013
DOI: 10.1159/000348698
Published online: April 27, 2013

Resilience of the Immune System in Healthy


Young Students to 30-Hour Sleep Deprivation
with Psychological Stress
Pini Matzner a Ofir Hazut a Reut Naim a Lee Shaashua a Liat Sorski a
Ben Levi a Avi Sadeh a Ilan Wald a Yair Bar-Haim a, b Shamgar Ben-Eliyahu a, b
a
School of Psychological Sciences and b Sagol School of Neuroscience, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel

Key Words levels, no effects were evident in any of the immunological/


Cytokines · Humans · Immune resilience · Immunity · endocrine indices. In contrast, expected sex differences in
Natural killer cells · Resilience · Sleep deprivation · Stress immune measures were found, including significantly high-
er NK cytotoxicity and monocyte counts in males, validating
the integrity of the measurements. Conclusions: The find-
Abstract ings suggest resilience of the immune system to a combined
Objective: Young adults often encounter sleep deprivation sleep deprivation and stressful exposure in young adults,
and stressful events. Both have been separately reported to while previous studies reported immune perturbations fol-
modulate immunity, and occasionally they occur simultane- lowing either of these conditions separately. These apparent
ously. We assessed the combined effects of these condi- contradictions might reflect differences in the study design
tions on immune competence in healthy students. Meth- or in the methodology used for immunological assessments,
ods: Twenty-three participants (mean age 24 years; SD 1.86; including the time of sample collection, the combination of
14 females) were exposed to 30 h of sleep deprivation during sleep deprivation with stress and our in vivo assessment of
which they conducted physiological, social and cognitive cytokine levels. Copyright © 2013 S. Karger AG, Basel
tasks. The control group consisted of 18 participants (mean
age 23.67 years; SD 1.46; 11 females). All participants under-
went cognitive and psychological evaluations at 10:00 AM,
followed by blood and saliva collection, 3 days before sleep Introduction
deprivation induction and on the morning following it. Im-
mune/endocrine measures included blood counts of lym- The lifestyle of adolescents and young people often
phocytes, granulocytes, monocytes and natural killer (NK) involves extensive deprivation of night sleep. Addition-
cells; levels of several cell surface markers; NK cytotoxicity; ally, epidemiological reports indicate that approximately
plasma levels of interleukin (IL)-6, IL-10, dehydroepiandros- one third of the general population in the United States
terone and neuropeptide Y, and plasma and salivary cortisol exhibits at least one symptom of insomnia [1]. Thus,
levels. Results: Although the experimental protocol signifi- studying the potential impacts of sleep deprivation on
Tel-Aviv University Lib. of Life Science and Med.

cantly elevated state anxiety and psychological dissociation health outcome is important.
132.66.156.41 - 9/10/2013 10:29:08 AM

© 2013 S. Karger AG, Basel Prof. Shamgar Ben-Eliyahu


1021–7401/13/0204–0194$38.00/0 School of Psychological Sciences
Tel Aviv University
Downloaded by:

E-Mail karger@karger.com
Tel Aviv 69978 (Israel)
www.karger.com/nim
E-Mail shamgar @ post.tau.ac.il
Sleep is a critical biological process for normal func- potentially through common proinflammatory responses.
tioning of all known mammals [2, 3] and has an impor- Indeed, some psychological and health disorders, includ-
tant role in learning and memory consolidation [4, 5], ing depression and cardiovascular diseases, seem to in-
regulation of hormonal levels [6] and immune activities volve irregularities in two or three of these processes [32,
[7]. Sleep and its associated circadian rhythms are hy- 33], suggesting the biological and clinical significance of
pothesized to fulfill a restorative role, preventing accu- these interactions. Importantly, under naturalistic cir-
mulated perceptual overload of the cerebral cortex [8], cumstances, sleep deprivation is often accompanied by
and ensuring stable and healthy cytokine rhythms [9]. stress or continuous activity. For example, students study-
Sleep and immunity have been suggested to mutually ing for examinations, professionals in demanding jobs,
affect each other. Specifically, sleep deprivation has been parents to newborns or soldiers during active service
associated with a tendency towards a proinflammatory may all experience sleep deprivation alongside maintain-
state, as reflected by a higher potential of monocytes to ing activity and/or being subjected to stressful circum-
produce interleukin (IL)-6 and tumor necrosis factor-α stances. Thus, in order to simulate such circumstances
upon in vitro activation [10, 11]. Sleep deprivation was and measure their impact on the immune system, we em-
also reported to modulate natural killer (NK) cell cyto- ployed a research protocol combining sleep deprivation
toxicity, with some studies showing reduction and some with potentially stressful activities.
enhancement in their activity [9, 12, 13]. Additionally, We compared young male and female students sub-
human systemic levels of prolactin, estradiol and cortisol jected to a 30-hour sleep deprivation alongside psycho-
were found to correlate with total sleep latency, REM logical stress, with a control group of participants. Spe-
sleep latency and the percentage of slow-wave sleep fol- cifically, we assessed multiple immune, endocrine, cog-
lowing either total sleep deprivation or selective REM de- nitive and psychological indices within participants
privation [14]. Inversely, the impact of immunity on before and after the sleep deprivation protocol. The bio-
sleep has long been examined, and many cytokines were logical indices studied were those previously reported to
found to regulate sleep patterns. For example, proinflam- be affected by either stress or sleep deprivation [14, 34–
matory cytokines, including IL-1, are known to be elevat- 36], as we herein assess the combined effect of these con-
ed in response to infections [15], and both intracerebro- ditions on immunity. The psychological and cognitive
ventricular and peripheral injections of proinflammatory outcomes of this study will be described in detail else-
cytokines were shown to impact sleeping patterns [16, where, while this paper focuses on the immunological
17]. Similarly, humans subjected to a viral infection re- and endocrine outcomes of the sleep deprivation and
ported a growing desire to sleep [18]. Indeed, some stud- stress paradigm.
ies suggest that proinflammatory cytokines increase
slow-wave sleep and sleep duration, while anti-inflam-
matory cytokines inhibit sleep [19–21]. However, other Materials and Methods
studies suggest a more complex association between Participants
sleep and immunity [22–24]. Forty-one young healthy undergraduate psychology students
Immunity was repeatedly shown to be affected by psy- (mean age = 24 years; SD = 1.86; 25 females) chose to participate
chological and physiological stressors through various in the study after being briefly informed about the paradigm. Par-
mechanisms. Some investigators suggested a dichotomy ticipants were randomly assigned into a stress group (23 partici-
pants, mean age 24 years; SD 1.86; 14 females) and a control group
between the impact of acute and chronic stress, namely (18 participants, mean age 23.67; SD 1.46; 11 females). Study par-
that chronic stress suppresses immunity and reduces re- ticipation fulfilled students’ requirements for participating in ex-
sistance to diseases, while acute stress enhances immu- periments. The study was conducted in 2 replicates (4 weeks
nity and disease resistance [25, 26]. However, several apart), each including about half of the participants in each group.
acute stressors were actually shown to suppress various The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board, and all
participants provided a written informed consent.
immune indices, including peripheral levels of NK acti-
vity and Th1 cytokine levels [27–30]. Acute stress was also Experimental Design and Specific Timelines
shown to increase proinflammatory cytokines, including The experimental design is illustrated in figure 1. All parti-
IL-6 [31]. cipants first reported to the laboratory on Monday morning
(09:00 AM), 3 days before the planned sleep deprivation night, to
Overall, it appears that immunity, stress and sleep are conduct a battery of psychological tests and to provide baseline
mutually interacting processes, and that sleep deprivation saliva and blood samples. Participants were then randomly as-
Tel-Aviv University Lib. of Life Science and Med.

and stress can often lead to similar immune consequences, signed to either the stress or the control group. Three days later,
132.66.156.41 - 9/10/2013 10:29:08 AM

Immune Resilience in Students following Neuroimmunomodulation 2013;20:194–204 195


Stress and Sleep Deprivation DOI: 10.1159/000348698
Downloaded by:
Control group Stress group

Blood and saliva Blood and saliva


09:00– Mon 09:00–
collection, actigraphy collection, actigraphy
11:00 AM 11:00 AM
distribution distribution

Thu Early wake-up:


04:00 AM beginning of sleep
deprivation period

09:00 PM Reporting to the lab


Normal daily routine

Fri
09:15 PM Psychological tests,
to cognitive and social
06:30 AM stress tasks

Blood and saliva Blood and saliva


09:00– 09:00–
collection, actigraphy collection, actigraphy
11:00 AM 11:00 AM
collection collection

Fig. 1. Participants from both groups first reported to the labora- laboratory at 09:00 PM. Batteries of psychological tests, and cogni-
tory on Monday morning (09:00 AM) to conduct a battery of psy- tive and social stress tasks were employed throughout the night
chological tests followed by the collection of saliva and blood sam- period. Participants of the control group reported to the labora-
ples. Three days later (Thursday), participants from the sleep-de- tory on Friday 09:00 AM, and both groups underwent a final bat-
prived group were awakened at 04: 00 AM and reported to the tery of psychological tests, and provided saliva and blood samples.

on the day of the experiment (Thursday), participants of the stress scale, continuous performance test and text presentation task; (ii)
group were awakened at 04:00 AM and allowed to maintain their analog mood scale, dot probe while putting the nondominant
daily routine, while continuously being monitored for staying hand in ice water and continuous performance test; (iii) arithme-
awake (by wearing an Actigraph, Mini Motionlogger, Ambula- tic task, analog mood scale and social stress task, and (iv) group
tory Monitoring Inc. and scheduled phone calls). At 09: 00 PM, text presentation.
these participants reported to the laboratory and were fully active
throughout the night until 10:00 AM Friday morning. At 4 time Saliva Collection
points during the night [(i) 09:15–11:25 PM, and (ii) 00:00–02:25, Saliva was collected using Salivatte swabs (Sarstedt, Germany)
(iii) 02:40–06:00 and (iv) 06:30–08:25 AM], different batteries of that were chewed for 60 s and placed back in the saliva tube. To
psychological tests and social stress tasks were employed (see be- overcome potential circadian-related cortisol alterations, the ex-
low). Participants of the control group reported to the laboratory act timing of saliva collection from each participant was matched
on Friday 09:00 AM, when participants of both the stress and the along the 2 collection days and counterbalanced between groups.
control groups underwent a final battery of psychological tests, Salivattes were then centrifuged at 1,000 g for 2 min, and the ex-
and provided the second set of saliva and blood samples. The time tract was collected and stored at –32 ° C for later analyses.
points of saliva and blood collection were counterbalanced be-
tween groups and maintained constant per participant for the Blood Collection
2 collection days in each replicate. Ten milliliter of blood were drawn by a certified nurse from
The following composition of tests and tasks were given the cubital vein into a heparinized syringe (50 units/ml) and
Tel-Aviv University Lib. of Life Science and Med.

through the nightly time points indicated above: (i) analog mood were kept at room temperature for a maximum of 2 h before be-
132.66.156.41 - 9/10/2013 10:29:08 AM

196 Neuroimmunomodulation 2013;20:194–204 Matzner/Hazut/Naim/Shaashua/Sorski/


DOI: 10.1159/000348698 Levi/Sadeh/Wald/Bar-Haim/Ben-Eliyahu
Downloaded by:
ing processed. Blood samples were drawn following the saliva lysing solution (Becton Dickinson) was added, and 12 min later,
collection, between 09: 00–11: 00 AM, and the exact timing of samples were centrifuged at 670 g for 5 min and the lysate was
blood sampling from each participant was also matched along aspirated. Cells were rewashed twice with 1 ml PBS++ (5 min
the 2 collection days and counterbalanced between groups (to centrifugation, 670 g) and resuspended in 500 μl of PBS++ for
overcome potential circadian-related hormonal and cytokine flow cytometry using a FACScan (Becton Dickinson).
alterations). A blood aliquot from each sample was centrifuged Identification of Leukocyte Subsets. Lymphocytes, granulo-
for 20 min at 931 g at 4 ° C, and plasma was collected and stored cytes and monocytes were identified based on forward and side
at –32 ° C. scatters. Within the lymphocytes, NK cells were identified as
CD3– (using PE-Cy5-conjugated anti-human-CD3, Biolegend)
Assessment of NK Cytotoxicity and CD16+ (APC-Cy7-conjugated anti-human-CD16, Bioleg-
Radiolabeling of K562 Target Cells. Standard NK-sensitive end) and/or CD56+ (PE-conjugated anti-human-CD56, Serotec).
[37] K562 erythromyeloid tumor cells were used as targets in the Evaluating Cell Marker Expression Levels. Expression levels of
NK cytotoxicity assay. Cells were grown in complete medium the following activation markers were assessed on relevant cell
[CM; RPMI 1640 medium supplemented with 10% heat-inacti- types: HLA-DR (FITC-conjugated anti-human-HLA-DR, Bioleg-
vated fetal calf serum, 50 μg/ml of gentamycin, 2 mM of L-gluta- end), NKG2D (APC-conjugated anti-human-CD314, Serotec),
mine, 0.1 mM of nonessential amino acids and 1 mM sodium Fas ligand (FITC-conjugated anti-human-CD178, Serotec) and
pyruvate (Biological Industries, Beit Haemek, Israel)] at 37 ° C CD11a (APC-conjugated anti-human-CD11a, Serotec).
in 100% humidity and 5% CO2. For radiolabeling, 4 × 107 cells Evaluating Leukocyte Numbers. To assess the absolute num-
were incubated for 1.5 h with 200 μCi of 51Cr, 200 μl of fetal calf bers of cells per microliter of blood (for each specific leukocyte
serum and 150 μl of CM. After incubation, cells were washed subset and for the total number of leukocytes), we added 300
3 times with CM (300 g, for 10 min) and adjusted to the desirable polystyrene microbeads (20 μm; Duke Scientific, Palo Alto, Ca-
concentrations. lif., USA) per microliter of sample studied. Following cytometry,
NK Cytotoxicity Assay. The cytotoxic activity of NK cells was the formula: CD3+ (n) / microbeads (n) × 300 was used to calcu-
assessed using a standard whole-blood 4-hour 51Cr release assay. late the number of CD3+ (T cells) per microliter of sample. The
This procedure assesses antitumor cytotoxicity of NK cells per same formula was used for each other leukocyte subset and for
milliliter of blood without removing any cell subtypes or serum the total number of leukocytes. The coefficient of variation for
content. Eight aliquots of 200 μl of heparinized whole blood this method was found in our laboratory to be 6% for identical
were placed in a microtiter plate. To achieve 8 different effector- samples [27, 39, 40].
to-target (E:T) ratios, 2 × 105 radiolabeled K562 cells/well, in
50 μl of CM, were added on top of the blood aliquot for the low- Assessment of Cytokines, Cortisol, Dehydroepiandrosterone
est E:T ratio, and half of these target cells (in 50 μl of CM, using and Neuropeptide Y
serial dilutions) were used for each additional higher E:T ratio. Based on manufacturer’s instructions, enzyme-linked im-
To determine spontaneous and maximal 51Cr release, whole munosorbent assays (ELISA) were used to measure salivary and
blood was substituted with CM or Triton X-100 (Sigma, Rehov- plasma cortisol levels (Cortisol Parameter assay kit, R&D), plas-
ot, Israel), respectively. Plates were centrifuged at 500 g for ma IL-10 levels (Human IL-10 HS ELISA, Bender MedSystems),
10 min and were then incubated for 4 h in 100% humidity, 5% plasma dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA) levels (DHEA-ELISA,
CO2 at 37 ° C. Following incubation, plates were centrifuged Biosource) and plasma IL-6 levels (Human IL-6 Quantikine HS
again at 500 g for 10 min at 4 ° C, and 100 μl of the supernatant ELISA, R&D).
were recovered from each well for assessment of radioactivity in For the quantification of neuropeptide Y (NPY) levels, peptide
a γ-counter in order to calculate specific target cell lysis. Earlier extraction from plasma was conducted based on the manufactur-
studies indicated that cytotoxicity measured using this proce- er’s instructions using C18 separation columns (Phoenix Pharma-
dure is attributable to NK cells rather than to other cell types or ceuticals Inc.) followed by lyophilization and freezing. The extract
soluble factors [38]. Cytotoxicity was calculated as (sample re- was then reconstituted and assayed for NPY levels using competi-
lease × correction – spontaneous release)/(total release – spon- tive ELISA (Phoenix Pharmaceutical Inc.). Based on the manufac-
taneous release), where correction is the percentage of the super- turer’s report, the intra-assay coefficient of variation for all ELISA
natant within the volume of the well (i.e. excluding the hemato- kits is 6.9–10%, as was indeed observed in our samples.
crit volume). This correction is necessary because the 51Cr
released by the labeled target cells is found only in the superna- Psychological and Cognitive Assessments and Stress
tant above the red blood cells. Manipulations
The psychological stress paradigm continued throughout the
Flow Cytometry sleep-deprived night and included physiological, social and cog-
FACS analysis was used to assess the number of leukocyte sub- nitive tasks, which will be described in detail in a separate paper
populations (including lymphocytes, NK cells, monocytes and focusing on the psychological and cognitive outcomes of the
granulocytes), as well as expression levels of different activation study. Shortly, participants had to complete computerized tasks
markers (see below). while putting their nondominant hand in ice-cold water; were
Sample Preparation. A 50-μl aliquot of whole blood was added engaged in a social stress procedure, which was based on the
to 10 μl of PBS supplemented with 2% FCS and 0.1% NaN3 Leaderless Group Discussion [41] and included elements of com-
(PBS++) containing a full set of conjugated antibodies for identi- petition, cooperation, responsibility and social status, and were
fication. Samples were kept in the dark at room temperature subjected to several cognitive tasks, such as mental arithmetic
Tel-Aviv University Lib. of Life Science and Med.

thereafter. Following a 15-min incubation period, 1 ml of FACS tasks [42].


132.66.156.41 - 9/10/2013 10:29:08 AM

Immune Resilience in Students following Neuroimmunomodulation 2013;20:194–204 197


Stress and Sleep Deprivation DOI: 10.1159/000348698
Downloaded by:
50 12
Monday Monday
Friday Friday
45 10

Depression (PHQ-9)
40 8
State anxiety

35 6

30 4

25 2

20 0
a Stress Control b Stress Control

Fig. 2. a Comparison of state anxiety levels between the control and levels, as indicated by PHQ-9 results, between the control and
sleep-deprived groups before (Monday) and after (Friday) the par- sleep-deprived groups before (Monday) and after (Friday) the par-
adigm. Significant differences were evident within the sleep-de- adigm. Significant differences were evident within the sleep-de-
prived group participants, but not within the control group par- prived group participants, but not within the control group par-
ticipants. Means ± SEM, p < 0.05. b Comparison of depression ticipants. Means ± SEM, p < 0.01.

A battery of both written and computerized tests was used to analysis of variance with a predetermined significance level of
assess trait and state anxiety, depression symptoms, dissociation, 0.05 was conducted. The independent between-participant vari-
behavior in stress situations, and coping styles and resilience. Ad- ables were group (stress/control) and sex (male/female). Time
ditionally, public presentations and social stress tasks were con- (before and after stress exposure) served as a repeated within-sub-
ducted during the night hours. All of the tests and tasks were given ject factor. Specific data observations that deviated from their
in their validated Hebrew versions and included the State-Trait group mean by more than 3 SDs were excluded from statistical
Anxiety Inventory-Trait Scale (STAI-T), State-Trait Anxiety In- analysis. These are identified in the Results section. Provided sig-
ventory-State Scale (STAI-S), patient health questionnaire (PHQ- nificant group differences were found, Fisher’s protected least sig-
9), Clinician Administered Dissociative State Scale, analog mood nificant difference analyses were performed to compare specific
scales, dot probe, continuous performance test, text presentation pairs of groups based on a priori hypotheses. All statistical analy-
and social stress tasks. ses were conducted using StatView software (SAS Institute, San
Data from the STAI-T, STAI-S and PHQ-9 were also used for Francisco, Calif., USA).
validating the psychological effect of the stressor, and thus these
tests are described in detail below:
The STAI [43] consists of two 20-item scales measuring situ- Results
ational variations in levels of perceived anxiety (state anxiety).
Likert scales ranged from 1 (not at all/almost never) to 4 (extreme-
ly/almost always) and scores ranged from 20 to 80, with higher Stress Manipulation Check
scores reflecting higher anxiety. The translation of these scales to Significant group-by-time interaction effects were
Hebrew was done according to the instructions of Spielberger and found for state anxiety (F1, 39 = 11.99, p < 0.01; fig. 2a) and
Sharma [44] and was found to be reliable and valid for use with depression (as indicated by PHQ-9 results; F1, 39 = 17.96,
Israelis [45]. p < 0.01; fig. 2b). Additionally, no differences were found
The PHQ-9 [46] is a self-reported depression rating scale con-
sisting of 9 items on which the diagnosis of DSM depressive dis- between the two experimental groups for trait anxiety.
order is based (designed to assess the severity of depressive These findings indicate a robust psychological impact of
symptoms). Reliability and diagnostic validity of the PHQ-9 the experimental procedure, as they indicate an immedi-
were established in several studies [46, 47]. ate change in state anxiety and depression within the
stress group only, which was not evident at all in the trait
Statistical Analysis
Data from both replicates were combined for the analysis, as anxiety data. We further examined these findings using
no noticeable differences between the two replicates were ob- paired Student’s t tests between the two experimental
Tel-Aviv University Lib. of Life Science and Med.

served. For each index examined, a 2 × 2 × 2 repeated-measure groups, which indicated no differences before the para-
132.66.156.41 - 9/10/2013 10:29:08 AM

198 Neuroimmunomodulation 2013;20:194–204 Matzner/Hazut/Naim/Shaashua/Sorski/


DOI: 10.1159/000348698 Levi/Sadeh/Wald/Bar-Haim/Ben-Eliyahu
Downloaded by:
Table 1. Results of psychological measures assessed 3 days before the paradigm and on the morning following it

Index Sex Stress Control Statistical


n n outcome
Monday Friday Monday Friday

State anxiety male 9 30.15±2.49 46.82±5.58 7 38.43±3.98 38.71±5.76 *


female 14 31.78±2.77 39.56±2.34 11 33.98±2.43 31.45±3.60
Trait anxiety male 9 41.07±4.72 33.86±4.27 7 38.86±3.37 47.00±5.33 NS
female 14 40.53±2.14 33.56±3.19 11 43.55±2.51 36.91±2.42
PHQ-9 male 9 4.93±1.84 9.79±2.21 7 9.14±1.93 6.14±1.68 *
female 14 4.44±0.82 8.81±1.49 11 6.05±1.04 5.82±1.08

Means ± SEM of the different psychological measures before (Monday) and after (Friday) sleep deprivation, and statistical outcomes
indicating significant differences. * These measures showed significant interaction between group and time point, resulting in elevated
anxiety and PHQ-9 in the stress group following the paradigm. NS = No significant outcome.

Table 2. Results for white blood cell counts assessed 3 days before the paradigm and on the morning following it

Cell population Sex Stress, cells/ml Control, cells/ml Statistical


outcome
n Monday Friday n Monday Friday

Lymphocytes male 8 1,613±120 1,718±151 7 2,023±331 2,075±383 NS


female 14 1,750±150 2,261±251 11 1,918±103 2,199±184
Monocytes male 3 304±67 103±28 4 395±122 137±23 *
female 5 181±15 88±17 3 109±20 52±5
Granulocytes male 8 2,142±306 2,529±259 6a 2,216±350 2,056±361 NS
female 14 2,101±266 1,798±222 11 2,139±147 2,096±283
NK male 8 96±17 74±15 7 117±19 98±20 NS
female 14 82±11 70±11 11 74±9 66±9

Means ± SEM of white blood cell subpopulation count before (Monday) and after (Friday) sleep deprivation, and statistical outcomes
indicating significant differences. * Monocyte counts were significantly higher in males than in females regardless of group and time
point. NS = No significant outcome. a One male deviant subject was removed from the control granulocyte group.

digm and significant differences following it (p < 0.01). lem with one set of tubes, IL-6 and monocytes were only
All findings are presented in table 1. assessed in approximately half of the samples.
Leukocyte Counts. The experimental paradigm had no
Immunological Indices effect on the total leukocyte numbers or on the number
Assessments of NK cytotoxicity and FACS analysis of any leukocyte subtype. Further examination of cellular
were conducted on all 23 stress and 18 control partici- marker expression levels (see Methods) on all related leu-
pants, excluding 1 male subject from the stress group in kocyte subtypes yielded also no effect for sleep depriva-
whom the second blood withdrawal was unsuccessful. tion, although baseline levels were within the detection
For all cytokine and hormone measurements, we ran- limits and indicated individual differences. The number
domly and a priori chose 19–22 participants from the of lymphocytes, monocytes and granulocytes did not sig-
stress group, and matched those to 13–17 participants nificantly differ between the groups, nor between baseline
from the control group, enforcing similar representation values (Monday) and values after sleep deprivation (Fri-
for each replicate and sex. We conducted this random day) in both groups. When examined for gender differ-
selection in order to keep within the available number of ences, males had a significantly higher number of mono-
Tel-Aviv University Lib. of Life Science and Med.

wells in the ELISA plates. Also, due to a technical prob- cytes than females, independently of group condition
132.66.156.41 - 9/10/2013 10:29:08 AM

Immune Resilience in Students following Neuroimmunomodulation 2013;20:194–204 199


Stress and Sleep Deprivation DOI: 10.1159/000348698
Downloaded by:
80

70

60

K562 cytotoxicity (%)


50

40

30

20
Control Fri.
Stress Mon.
10 Control Mon.
Stress Fri.
0
E:T 1 E:T 2 E:T 3 E:T 4 E:T 5 E:T 6 E:T 7 E:T 8
a E:T ratio (whole blood:K562)

80

70

60
K562 cytotoxicity (%)

50

Fig. 3. a Comparison of NK cytotoxicity 40


against K562 cells between the sleep-de-
prived and the control group before (Mon- 30
day) and after (Friday) the paradigm. No
significant differences were evident be- 20
tween groups or time points. Means ± SEM. Male Fri.
Male Mon.
b Comparison of NK cytotoxicity against 10 Female Fri.
K562 cells between males and females be- Female Mon.
fore (Monday) and after (Friday) the sleep 0
deprivation paradigm. Significant differ- E:T 1 E:T 2 E:T 3 E:T 4 E:T 5 E:T 6 E:T 7 E:T 8
ences were evident between males and fe- b E:T ratio (whole blood:K562)
males, but not between the two different
time points. Means ± SEM, p < 0.01.

(F1, 11 = 10.89, p < 0.01). No additional gender differences blood NPY levels between the groups, nor between levels
were found among the other leukocyte subpopulations at baseline and after sleep deprivation. Additionally, no
examined. All data are presented in table 2. gender differences were found among these hormone lev-
NK Cell Counts and Cytotoxic Activity. The experi- els. All levels are presented in table 3, including minimal
mental paradigm neither affected NK numbers or cyto- detection levels.
toxicity between the groups nor between both time points Cytokines. The experimental paradigm had neither an
(at baseline and after sleep deprivation). When examined effect on IL-6 and IL-10 levels between the groups, nor
for gender differences, males had significantly higher kill- between levels at baseline and after sleep deprivation. No
ing activity levels than females, independent of group significant sex differences were found in these indices. All
condition (F1, 36 = 8.49, p < 0.01; fig. 3). levels are presented in table 4, including minimal detec-
Hormone Levels. The experimental paradigm had no tion levels.
Tel-Aviv University Lib. of Life Science and Med.

effect on DHEA levels, blood and saliva cortisol levels and


132.66.156.41 - 9/10/2013 10:29:08 AM

200 Neuroimmunomodulation 2013;20:194–204 Matzner/Hazut/Naim/Shaashua/Sorski/


DOI: 10.1159/000348698 Levi/Sadeh/Wald/Bar-Haim/Ben-Eliyahu
Downloaded by:
Table 3. Results of hormonal levels (in ng/ml) assessed 3 days before the paradigm and on the morning following it

Hormone Minimal Sex Stress Control Statistical


detection level n Monday Friday n Monday Friday outcome

DHEA 0.37 male 8 5.34±0.92 7.39±3.42 7 5.28±1.13 4.37±0.81 NS


female 14 2.85±0.31 4.19±0.79 9 2.81±0.47 4.33±1.09
Plasma cortisol 0.156 male 7 13.74±2.03 14.06±2.04 5 10.87±2.69 12.59±1.96 NS
female 14 13.75±2.29 12.69±1.66 8 10.61±3.54 17.77±2.56
Saliva cortisol 0.156 male 8 1.14±0.61 0.72±0.12 7 0.58±0.07 1.29±0.46 NS
female 14 0.60±0.09 0.53±0.07 10 0.92±0.19 0.92±0.21
NPY 0.01 male 8 0.26±0.14 0.25±0.06 7 0.24±0.08 0.21±0.05 NS
female 14 0.34±0.10 0.26±0.04 10 0.20±0.06 0.31±0.03

Means ± SEM of hormonal levels before (Monday) and after (Friday) sleep deprivation, and statistical outcomes indicating signifi-
cant differences. NS = No significant outcome.

Table 4. Results of cytokine levels (in pg/ml) assessed 3 days before the paradigm and on the morning following it

Cytokine Minimal Sex Stress Control Statistical


detection level n Monday Friday n Monday Friday outcome

IL-6 0.156 male 4 0.86±0.24 0.38±0.19 4 1.19±0.86 0.64±0.53 NS


female 5 0.82±0.28 0.71±0.39 4 2.15±0.75 1.78±1.18
IL-10a 0.39 male 7 1.00±0.67 0.76±0.53 5 0.17±0.08 0.12±0.04 NS
female 12 0.21±0.04 0.15±0.04 8 0.15±0.04 0.14±0.08

Means ± SEM of cytokine levels before (Monday) and after (Friday) sleep deprivation and statistical outcomes indicating significant
differences. NS = No significant outcome.a Most, but not all, readings of IL-10 levels were below the minimal detection level provided
by the manufacturer.

Discussion pected physiological levels and individual differences.


For example, the NK cytotoxicity assay yielded signifi-
The combination of 30-hour sleep deprivation and so- cant killing, increasing specific lysis from 6 to 70% along
cial stress tasks was shown to be an effective psychologi- the different E:T ratios, and hormone levels are within
cal manipulation, as reflected by elevated levels of state normal and detectable levels (as detailed in table 3).
anxiety and depression in the sleep deprivation group on To further validate the methodological integrity of the
the morning following the paradigm. However, this po- data, we analyzed our findings for sex differences, as some
tent manipulation had no detectable influence on any of were previously reported. Specifically, and as was previ-
the neuroendocrine or immunological indices studied ously shown by us and others [27, 48], we found a sig-
on the morning following the paradigm. Specifically, no nificantly higher NK cytotoxic activity in males than in
effects were evident regarding (i) levels of NK activity; females. Additionally, higher numbers of monocytes
(ii) numbers of leukocyte subsets (including monocytes, were observed in males than in females, as was also re-
granulocytes, lymphocytes and NK cells); (iii) expression ported by others [49, 50]. Importantly, these differences
levels of various activation markers (including NKG2D, did not interact with sleep deprivation or with the time
MHC-II, CD11a and FasL) on leukocytes subsets, and points tested (before and after the experimental para-
(iv) cytokine and hormone levels (IL-6, IL-10, NPY, digm). Overall, these findings suggest resilience of the im-
DHEA, and saliva and plasma cortisol). Importantly, the mune system of young students to our sleep deprivation/
great majority of the indices tested were within the detec- stress paradigm. This apparent resilience concurs with
Tel-Aviv University Lib. of Life Science and Med.

tion levels of the different bioassays and showed the ex- the ability of young people to cope with occasional sleep
132.66.156.41 - 9/10/2013 10:29:08 AM

Immune Resilience in Students following Neuroimmunomodulation 2013;20:194–204 201


Stress and Sleep Deprivation DOI: 10.1159/000348698
Downloaded by:
deprivation and temporary stressful events, maintaining measurement of plasma levels that was used herein. It is
their health and resistance to opportunistic diseases, as possible that higher levels of sleep deprivation and stress
would be evolutionarily sound in young adults. exposure may lead to an actual state of immune pertur-
When comparing our study design to common sleep bation, which is reflected by altered plasma levels of cy-
deprivation studies, in which significant immunological tokine and hormones, and by changes in NK activity and
impacts were reported [12, 51, 52], one can notice four other immune functions.
major differences that may account for the apparent in- Most sleep deprivation studies are conducted in the
congruence in results. In our study we: (i) measured in setting of a well-controlled sleep laboratory, in which par-
vivo plasma cytokine levels, as opposed to assessing in vi- ticipants are continuously present from the beginning of
tro ‘induced production’ levels; (ii) allowed participants to the study to its end [10, 12, 57]. In the current study, how-
maintain normal daily routine following baseline assess- ever, participants were allowed to maintain their normal
ment and up until the beginning of the sleep deprivation daily routine from the day of baseline measurements
period, rather than restricting them to the laboratory; (iii) (Monday) to the beginning of the sleep-deprived night
added social and physiological activities during the sleep (Thursday). Depriving participants from their daily rou-
deprivation period, as opposed to keeping participants tine combined with the exposure to the stringent labora-
under conditions of social isolation and minimal physical tory setting during the sleep deprivation period may have
activity during the sleep deprivation period, and (iv) as- substantial psychological impacts. These psychological
sessed immune indices following completion of the entire factors may interact with or underlie some of the report-
paradigm, when participants were ready to resume their ed effects of sleep deprivation, effects that were absent in
daily activity rather than assessing immunity at specific the current study.
time points along the night period and in close conjunc- Another major difference characterizing our study was
tion with the potentially acute stress responses. the social setting along the sleep-deprived night. In most
In the current study, we directly measured plasma lev- studies, participants are isolated in the sleep laboratory,
els of cytokines and hormones. This approach is different with some studies even maintaining minimal movement
from most of the studies addressing cytokine levels under policy [12, 53]. In the current study, participants spent
stress [10, 53], which use the in vitro ‘induced production’ their sleep-deprived night together, interacting socially,
approach that is based on in vitro activation of white blood and were allowed to move freely in the laboratory area. As
cells (or of a specific subpopulation) employing substanc- social isolation, even for short periods of time, was report-
es that mimic bacterial or viral infections (such as lipo- ed to decrease immune activity [58, 59], it is possible that
polysaccharides or CpG-C). This in vitro activation leads the experience of social isolation, which characterizes
to the secretion of cytokines from target cells, hence its most sleep deprivation studies, modulated immunologi-
advantage, but it is also biased since it reflects the specific cal outcomes, leading to higher inflammation markers
leukocyte composition of the sample at the exact time of and lower NK cytotoxic activity [60]. Inversely, social in-
blood withdrawal and its potential to secrete specific cyto- teractions in our study may have buffered or counteracted
kines upon activation. It does not indicate plasma levels of the impacts of sleep deprivation and stress.
the measured cytokines, which are derived from many Additionally, some researchers suggested that in con-
sources along an extended period. It is noteworthy that trast to the known immunosuppressive effects of chronic
plasma baseline levels of most cytokines are commonly stress, short-term (acute) stress actually leads to immu-
very low or below detection levels, and thus the ‘induced noenhancement [61]. For example, it was shown that a
production’ approach is commonly used. Importantly, short paradigm of acute stress in mice prior to UVB ra-
studies by others and us have shown that plasma cytokine diation exposure improved anti-tumor activity com-
levels may show different patterns of effects (even oppo- pared to control conditions [25]. As our paradigm in-
site) from those observed following ‘induced production’ cluded exposure to several acute stressors during the
in the context of some manipulations [54, 55]. sleep-deprived night, it is possible that these tasks in-
It can also be proposed that the current paradigm of duced immunoenhancing effects, which in turn counter-
sleep deprivation combined with psychological, cogni- acted immunosuppressive effects of sleep deprivation.
tive and physiological stress might have only provoked a Last, it is also possible that the herein lack of sleep de-
priming effect on the immune system [56]. Such priming privation-/stress-related immunological changes results
effects may only be revealed by approaches such as in from the specific time points that were chosen for blood
Tel-Aviv University Lib. of Life Science and Med.

vitro cytokine ‘induced production’, but not by direct collection. Unlike other studies, in which blood samples
132.66.156.41 - 9/10/2013 10:29:08 AM

202 Neuroimmunomodulation 2013;20:194–204 Matzner/Hazut/Naim/Shaashua/Sorski/


DOI: 10.1159/000348698 Levi/Sadeh/Wald/Bar-Haim/Ben-Eliyahu
Downloaded by:
were collected during the night period [62], in our study morning following it rather than studying periodical
blood and saliva were collected in the morning hours fol- acute alterations during the night. Cytokine levels were
lowing the sleep-deprived night. The blood and saliva col- only measured in the plasma, as we believe this approach
lection time point was chosen in this study to reflect the better reflects in vivo levels and their biological ramifica-
overall cumulative immunological and psychological ef- tions. Also, the study was limited to young healthy stu-
fect of the paradigm, in contrast to assessing acute and dents, without comparing them to more susceptible pop-
potentially transient perturbations. Thus, it is possible ulations. Last, only one level of sleep deprivation and
that in the current study sleep deprivation had also led stress exposure was used, and the two manipulations were
to immunological perturbations, but these were short imposed simultaneously and not studied separately.
lasting and no longer present at the time of sample collec- However, the findings are sufficient to suggest a resil-
tion. Specifically, the leukocyte sample composition, ience of the immune system in young students to a com-
which markedly determines in vitro cytokine induced bination of sleep deprivation and a stressful situation.
production levels and NK activity, constantly fluctuates The evident resilience is demonstrated herein when test-
based on momentary changes in stress hormone levels ing immunity following the completion of the experi-
[63]. Engaging in normal social activity within the labora- mental paradigm at an active daytime period. Although
tory before the second blood sampling may have masked fluctuations in immune indices could have occurred dur-
these previous temporary perturbations, reflecting only ing the night period, they do not seem to have long-last-
long-lasting effects with potential biological significance. ing effects in the current study and may thus bear mini-
Clearly, this study has several shortcomings, especially mal biological significance. That said, studies have also
given its lack of significant results. Immune indices were reported greater susceptibility to stress in older adults [64,
not assessed during the night period, as is common in 65], and in such populations both the ongoing fluctua-
most studies. The rationale for this approach was our aim tions and the long-term effects on immune measures
to study the cumulative overall effects of the sleep-de- should be further studied.
prived night along with stressful tasks evident on the

References
1 Ohayon MM: Epidemiology of insomnia: 10 Irwin MR, Wang M, Campomayor CO, et al: 16 Shoham S, Davenne D, Cady AB, et al: Re-
what we know and what we still need to learn. Sleep deprivation and activation of morning combinant tumor necrosis factor and inter-
Sleep Med Rev 2002;6:97–111. levels of cellular and genomic markers of in- leukin 1 enhance slow-wave sleep. Am J
2 Staunton H: Mammalian sleep. Naturwissen- flammation. Arch Intern Med 2006;166:1756– Physiol 1987;253:R142–R149.
schaften 2005;92:203–220. 1762. 17 Krueger JM, Walter J, Dinarello CA, et al: Sleep-
3 Laposky AD, Bass J, Kohsaka A, et al: Sleep 11 Aldabal L, Bahammam AS: Metabolic, endo- promoting effects of endogenous pyrogen (in-
and circadian rhythms: key components in crine, and immune consequences of sleep de- terleukin-1). Am J Physiol 1984; 246:R994–
the regulation of energy metabolism. FEBS privation. Open Respir Med J 2011;5:31–43. R999.
Lett 2008;582:142–151. 12 Matsumoto Y, Mishima K, Satoh K, et al: To- 18 Roger J, Broughton RDO (eds): Sleep, Arous-
4 Frank MG, Benington JH: The role of sleep in tal sleep deprivation induces an acute and al, and Performance. Boston, Birkhäuser,
memory consolidation and brain plasticity: transient increase in NK cell activity in 1992.
dream or reality? Neuroscientist 2006;12:477– healthy young volunteers. Sleep 2001;24:804– 19 Majde JA, Krueger JM: Links between the in-
488. 809. nate immune system and sleep. J Allergy Clin
5 Maquet P: The role of sleep in learning and 13 Shakhar K, Valdimarsdottir HB, Guevarra JS, Immunol 2005;116:1188–1198.
memory. Science 2001;294:1048–1052. et al: Sleep, fatigue, and NK cell activity in 20 Patel SR, Zhu X, Storfer-Isser A, et al: Sleep
6 Morselli L, Leproult R, Balbo M, et al: Role of healthy volunteers: significant relationships duration and biomarkers of inflammation.
sleep duration in the regulation of glucose revealed by within subject analyses. Brain Be- Sleep 2009;32:200–204.
metabolism and appetite. Best Pract Res Clin hav Immun 2007;21:180–184. 21 Krueger JM, Obál FJ, Fang J, et al: The role of
Endocrinol Metab 2010;24:687–702. 14 Martins RC, Andersen ML, Garbuio SA, et al: cytokines in physiological sleep regulation.
7 Bollinger T, Bollinger A, Oster H, et al: Sleep, Dopamine transporter regulation during four Ann NY Acad Sci 2001;933:211–221.
immunity, and circadian clocks: a mechanis- nights of REM sleep deprivation followed by 22 Gamaldo CE, Shaikh AK, McArthur JC: The
tic model. Gerontology 2010;56:574–580. recovery – an in vivo molecular imaging sleep-immunity relationship. Neurol Clin
8 Muzur A: Toward an integrative theory of study in humans. Sleep 2010;33:243–251. 2012;30:1313–1343.
sleep and dreaming. J Theor Biol 2005; 233: 15 Julkunen I, Melén K, Nyqvist M, et al: Inflam- 23 Motivala SJ: Sleep and inflammation: psycho-
103–118. matory responses in influenza A virus infec- neuroimmunology in the context of cardio-
9 Irwin M: Effects of sleep and sleep loss on im- tion. Vaccine 2000;19(suppl 1):S32–S37. vascular disease. Ann Behav Med 2011; 42:
munity and cytokines. Brain Behav Immun 141–152.
Tel-Aviv University Lib. of Life Science and Med.

2002;16:503–512.
132.66.156.41 - 9/10/2013 10:29:08 AM

Immune Resilience in Students following Neuroimmunomodulation 2013;20:194–204 203


Stress and Sleep Deprivation DOI: 10.1159/000348698
Downloaded by:
24 Krueger JM, Toth LA: Cytokines as regula- 38 Ottenhof PC, Morales A, Baines MG: Quanti- 52 Ruiz FS, Andersen ML, Martins RC, et al: Im-
tors of sleep. Ann NY Acad Sci 1994;739:299– tation of a whole blood assay for human natu- mune alterations after selective rapid eye
310. ral killer cell activity. J Immunol Methods movement or total sleep deprivation in he-
25 Dhabhar FS, Saul AN, Daugherty C, et al: 1981;42:305–318. althy male volunteers. Innate Immun 2012;
Short-term stress enhances cellular immunity 39 Benish M, Bartal I, Goldfarb Y, et al: Periop- 18:44–54.
and increases early resistance to squamous erative use of beta-blockers and COX-2 inhib- 53 Irwin MR, Carrillo C, Olmstead R: Sleep loss
cell carcinoma. Brain Behav Immun 2010;24: itors may improve immune competence and activates cellular markers of inflammation:
127–137. reduce the risk of tumor metastasis. Ann Surg sex differences. Brain Behav Immun 2010;24:
26 Frick LR, Arcos ML, Rapanelli M, et al: Oncol 2008;15:2042–2052. 54–57.
Chronic restraint stress impairs T-cell immu- 40 Goldfarb Y, Benish M, Rosenne E, et al: CpG- 54 Greenfeld K, Avraham R, Benish M, et al: Im-
nity and promotes tumor progression in mice. C oligodeoxynucleotides limit the deleterious mune suppression while awaiting surgery and
Stress 2009;12:134–143. effects of beta-adrenoceptor stimulation on following it: dissociations between plasma cy-
27 Ben-Eliyahu S, Shakhar G, Page GG, et al: Sup- NK cytotoxicity and metastatic dissemina- tokine levels, their induced production, and
pression of NK cell activity and of resistance tion. J Immunother 2009;32:280–291. NK cell cytotoxicity. Brain Behav Immun
to metastasis by stress: a role for adrenal cat- 41 Thornton GC III, Mueller-Hanson RA: Devel- 2007;21:503–513.
echolamines and beta-adrenoceptors. Neuro- oping Organizational Simulations: A Guide 55 Dorresteijn MJ, Draisma A, van der Hoe-
immunomodulation 2000;8:154–164. for Practitioners and Students., Mahwah, Erl- ven JG, et al: Lipopolysaccharide-stimulated
28 Melamed R, Rosenne E, Shakhar K, et al: Mar- baum, 2004. whole blood cytokine production does not
ginating pulmonary-NK activity and resis- 42 Wright RA, Martin RE, Bland JL: Energy re- predict the inflammatory response in human
tance to experimental tumor metastasis: sup- source depletion, task difficulty, and cardio- endotoxemia. Innate Immun 2010; 16: 248–
pression by surgery and the prophylactic use vascular response to a mental arithmetic 253.
of a beta-adrenergic antagonist and a prosta- challenge. Psychophysiology 2003; 40: 98– 56 McEwen BS: Stress, adaptation, and disease.
glandin synthesis inhibitor. Brain Behav Im- 105. Allostasis and allostatic load. Ann NY Acad
mun 2005;19:114–126. 43 Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene RD: Sci 1998;840:33–44.
29 Calcagni E, Elenkov I: Stress system activity, STAI: Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety In- 57 Dinges DF, Douglas SD, Zaugg L, et al: Leu-
innate and T helper cytokines, and suscepti- ventory. Palo Alto, Consulting Psychologists kocytosis and natural killer cell function par-
bility to immune-related diseases. Ann NY Press, 1970. allel neurobehavioral fatigue induced by 64
Acad Sci 2006;1069:62–76. 44 Spielberger CD, Sharma S: Cross-cultural hours of sleep deprivation. J Clin Invest 1994;
30 Shaashua L, Sominsky L, Levi B, et al: In vivo measurement of anxiety; in Spielberger CD, 93:1930–1939.
suppression of plasma IL-12 levels by acute Diaz-Guerrero R (eds): Cross-Cultural 58 Cacioppo JT, Hawkley LC, Norman GJ, et al:
and chronic stress paradigms: potential medi- Anxiety. Washington, Hemisphere, 1976, Social isolation. Ann NY Acad Sci 2011;1231:
ating mechanisms and sex differences. Brain pp 13-25. 17–22.
Behav Immun 2012;26:996–1005. 45 Teichman Y, Melineck C: Manual for the He- 59 Miller G: Social neuroscience. Why loneliness
31 Elenkov IJ, Iezzoni DG, Daly A, et al: Cytokine brew State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Tel Aviv, is hazardous to your health. Science 2011;331:
dysregulation, inflammation and well-being. Tel Aviv University Press, 1978. 138–140.
Neuroimmunomodulation 2005;12:255–269. 46 Spitzer RL, Kroenke K, Williams JB: Valida- 60 Wu W, Yamaura T, Murakami K, et al: Social
32 Nagai M, Hoshide S, Kario K: Sleep duration tion and utility of a self-report version of isolation stress enhanced liver metastasis of
as a risk factor for cardiovascular disease – a PRIME-MD: the PHQ primary care study. murine colon 26-L5 carcinoma cells by sup-
review of the recent literature. Curr Cardiol Primary Care Evaluation of Mental Disor- pressing immune responses in mice. Life Sci
Rev 2010;6:54–61. ders. Patient Health Questionnaire. JAMA 2000;66:1827–1838.
33 Hamazaki Y, Morikawa Y, Nakamura K, et al: 1999;282:1737–1744. 61 Dhabhar FS: Stress, leukocyte trafficking, and
The effects of sleep duration on the incidence 47 Spitzer RL, Williams JB, Kroenke K, et al: Va- the augmentation of skin immune function.
of cardiovascular events among middle-aged lidity and utility of the PRIME-MD patient Ann NY Acad Sci 2003;992:205–217.
male workers in Japan. Scand J Work Environ health questionnaire in assessment of 3000 62 Redwine L, Hauger RL, Gillin JC, et al: Effects
Health 2011;37:411–417. obstetric-gynecologic patients: the PRIME- of sleep and sleep deprivation on interleu-
34 Thorsell A: Brain neuropeptide Y and corti- MD Patient Health Questionnaire Obstetrics- kin-6, growth hormone, cortisol, and melato-
cotropin-releasing hormone in mediating Gynecology Study. Am J Obstet Gynecol 2000; nin levels in humans. J Clin Endocrinol Metab
stress and anxiety. Exp Biol Med (Maywood) 183:759–769. 2000;85:3597–3603.
2010;235:1163–1167. 48 Penschow J, Mackay IR: NK and K cell activ- 63 Dhabhar FS, Miller AH, McEwen BS, et al: Ef-
35 Azpiroz A, De Miguel Z, Fano E, et al: Rela- ity of human blood: differences according to fects of stress on immune cell distribution.
tions between different coping strategies for sex, age, and disease. Ann Rheum Dis 1980; Dynamics and hormonal mechanisms. J Im-
social stress, tumor development and neuro- 39:82–86. munol 1995;154:5511–5527.
endocrine and immune activity in male mice. 49 Bain BJ: Ethnic and sex differences in the total 64 Hawkley LC, Cacioppo JT: Stress and the ag-
Brain Behav Immun 2008;22:690–698. and differential white cell count and platelet ing immune system. Brain Behav Immun
36 Goel N, Dinges DF: Behavioral and genetic count. J Clin Pathol 1996;49:664–666. 2004;18:114–119.
markers of sleepiness. J Clin Sleep Med 2011; 50 Aulock SV, Deininger S, Draing C, et al: Gen- 65 Heffner KL: Neuroendocrine effects of stress
7(suppl 5):S19–S21. der difference in cytokine secretion on im- on immunity in the elderly: implications for
37 Lozzio BB, Lozzio CB: Properties and useful- mune stimulation with LPS and LTA. J Inter- inflammatory disease. Immunol Allergy Clin
ness of the original K-562 human myeloge- feron Cytokine Res 2006;26:887–892. North Am 2011;31:95–108.
nous leukemia cell line. Leuk Res 1979;3:363– 51 Reis ES, Lange T, Köhl G, et al: Sleep and cir-
370. cadian rhythm regulate circulating comple-
ment factors and immunoregulatory proper-
ties of C5a. Brain Behav Immun 2011; 25:
1416–1426.
Tel-Aviv University Lib. of Life Science and Med.
132.66.156.41 - 9/10/2013 10:29:08 AM

204 Neuroimmunomodulation 2013;20:194–204 Matzner/Hazut/Naim/Shaashua/Sorski/


DOI: 10.1159/000348698 Levi/Sadeh/Wald/Bar-Haim/Ben-Eliyahu
Downloaded by:

View publication stats

You might also like