Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

Received: 18 November 2021 Revised: 26 November 2022 Accepted: 22 December 2022

DOI: 10.1111/asj.13809

RESEARCH ARTICLE

Effect of different odors on the fighting behavior of weaning


piglets after merging into a large pen

Weijie Zhao 1 | Na Yang 1 | Lei Zhu 2 | Yisheng Lin 3 | Qiman Zhang 3 |


Gang Shu 1 | Songbo Wang 1 | Ping Gao 1 | Xiaotong Zhu 1 | Lina Wang 1 |
Qingyan Jiang 1

1
Guangdong Provincial Key Laboratory of
Animal Nutrition Control, College of Animal Abstract
Science, South China Agricultural University,
In intensive pig production, the fighting behavior of weaning piglets after merging
Guangzhou, People’s Republic of China
2
Chuxiong Anyou Livestock Co., Chuxiong,
pens is relatively common. Fighting behavior not only easily causes injury in pigs but
People’s Republic of China also affects the production performance of pigs. To reduce fighting behavior in farms,
3
Guangdong Ruisheng Technology Group Co., this study aimed to explore the possible effect of odorous substances on piglet fight-
Ltd., Guangzhou, People’s Republic of China
ing behavior after merging into a large pen. Six different sprays were tested: original
Correspondence creamy, cheese flavor, orange flavor, truffle, vanilla and pigpen flavor. In each experi-
Xioatong Zhu, Lina Wang and Qingyan Jiang,
College of Animal Science, South China
ment, two groups were set (one odor-sprayed and no sprayed control), and 12 pigs
Agricultural University, Wushan Avenue, were used per group. After mixing, the frequency of occurrence of various piglet
Tianhe District, Guangzhou 510642, People’s
Republic of China.
behaviors in different pens was recorded. During this period, salivary cortisol levels
Email: xtzhu@scau.edu.cn, wanglina@scau.edu. and skin lesion scores were evaluated. As a result, the piglets sprayed with the origi-
cn and qyjiang@scau.edu.cn
nal creamy, cheese flavor and vanilla substances obtained significantly higher average
Funding information daily gain and feed intake and showed a significantly lower incidence of fighting
National Natural Science Foundation of China,
behavior, and the skin lesion score and salivary cortisol of piglets were also reduced
Grant/Award Numbers: 31672464, 31790411,
31828010 significantly. All the other odorous substances had no significant effects on the fight-
ing behavior and production performance of piglets.

KEYWORDS
cortisol, fighting behavior, odor substance, production performance, weaned piglets

1 | I N T RO DU CT I O N et al., 2019), which increases the risks of skin damage and stress (van
Nieuwamerongen et al., 2014).
In modern intensified pig raising farms, to save production costs, Piglet fighting behaviors usually include any one or combination
weaning piglets from two or three litters are normally merged into of pushes, head knocks and bites (Samarakone & Gonyou, 2009). The
larger pens to improve the usage efficiency of pig pens. However, this frequency and severity of aggression associated with the formation of
approach has resulted in a series of problems; one of the most promi- a dominance hierarchy appears to depend on a balance between
nent problems is the increased fighting behavior of piglets. Fighting social experience and individual differences, such as size (Hsu
behavior is the response to a conflict between individual animals for a et al., 2006). Piglets that had been exposed to anise prenatally showed
mating partner, food, space and other resource competition, which aggression compared with those that had not been exposed prenatally
usually consists of attack and escape (Weller et al., 2019). When (Oostindjer et al., 2010). Indeed, numerous studies have shown that
weaning piglets from different litters are mixed together, fighting is social mixing induces an increase in piglets’ aggressive behavior
significantly increased to establish a new hierarchy (Verdon (Hötzel et al., 2004).

Anim Sci J. 2023;94:e13809. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/asj © 2023 Japanese Society of Animal Science. 1 of 9


https://doi.org/10.1111/asj.13809
17400929, 2023, 1, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/asj.13809 by College Of Veterinary & Animal Sciences, Pookode, Kerala, Wiley Online Library on [16/10/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
2 of 9 ZHAO ET AL.

It has been reported that fighting behavior can be caused by were smeared on weaning piglets to make them smell the same.
genetic factors, individual differences and other external factors, such Creamy and cheese are by products of milk and have a smell similar to
as competition for food or a place of rest or play (Peden et al., 2018). milk. They are often used as attractants in the feed of weaned piglets
Fighting piglets usually make loud noises, have an elevated heart rate and improve feed palatability (Seabolt et al., 2010), so original creamy
(Geverink et al., 2003; Marchant-Forde et al., 2003), and show signifi- and cheese flavor were selected for testing. Additionally, pigs are sen-
cantly reduced average daily gain (D’Eath, 2002). Increased fighting sitive to the smell of truffles, and their sense of smell has been used
behavior reduces the feed conversion rate and has harmful effects on to find them in the forest (Lavelle et al., 2017). Vanilla was reported to
piglets (Colson et al., 2012). On the one hand, weaned piglets that are be used as an attractant for wild boar hunting (Campbell &
more aggressive have reduced performance, such as reduced food Long, 2008). Therefore, the flavors of truffles, vanilla and orange were
conversion efficiency and decreased weight gain (Stookey & used because they are all from natural plants or fruits. They were used
Gonyou, 1994; Turpin et al., 2017). On the other hand, as fighting as analogs to truffles. After mixing the groups, fighting, resting, feed-
increases, pig mortality and respiratory disease incidence increase, ing, drinking, excreting, exploring, manipulating and climbing behaviors
and backfat thickness decreases (Janczak et al., 2003). of piglets in different pens were recorded. During this period, salivary
Numerous studies have shown that mixing unfamiliar pigs at cortisol levels and skin lesion scores were evaluated.
weaning leads to immediate severe fights that are necessary to build a
new hierarchy (Hwang et al., 2016). Pigs recognize each other mostly
by olfactory mechanisms. Research on maternal behavior during nest 2 | M A T E R I A L S A N D M ET H O D S
leaving in free-ranging domestic pigs found that sows maintain con-
tact with newborn piglets by sniffing and humming (Ocepek & 2.1 | Ethics statement
Andersen, 2018). Nose-to-nose contact initiated by a sow or piglet
may represent the beginning of a mother-offspring relationship, and All animal experiments were conducted with the permission number
shortly after the piglets are born, the mother and individual offspring SYXK (Guangdong) 2014-0136. The experimental design and proce-
in the nest group will establish social relationships (Portele dures used in this study were reviewed and approved by Guangdong
et al., 2019). Additionally, it has been reported that piglets are most Provincial Key Laboratory of Animal Nutrition Control, College of
attracted to the odors associated with maternal feces and skin secre- Animal Science, South China Agricultural University, Guangzhou,
tions (Morrow-Tesch & McGlone, 1990a). Piglets with different sow Guangdong, People’s Republic of China. All experiments were
odors fight when mixed (Kanaan et al., 2012). Piglet sensory systems conducted in accordance with “The Instructive Notions with Respect
and odors from the sow’s udder were associated with piglet nipple to Caring for Laboratory Animals” issued by the Ministry of Science
attachment (Morrow-Tesch & McGlone, 1990b). The sense of smell and Technology of the People’s Republic of China.
mainly plays a priming role in eating behavior. It has been demon-
strated that odor exposure induces appetite specifically for food.
However, the influence of odors on food choice and intake is less 2.2 | Reagents
clear (Boesveldt & de Graaf, 2017). The recognition of each other by
piglets may influence fighting behaviors. Therefore, can interfering Sprays were synthetic and purchased from Guangdong Ruisheng
with olfactory recognition of the pig effectively reduce fighting behav- Technology Co., Ltd. (Guangzhou, China), which are packaged in
ior after weaning piglets? hydraulic spray bottles, 550 ml per bottle (Table 1). Cortisol kits were
Some studies have shown that salivary cortisol reflects purchased from Beijing North Institute of Biotechnology (Beijing,
hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal activity in response to different China).
stressors in pigs, and cortisol is a salivary biomarker associated with
aggression at weaning and stress in piglets (Escribano et al., 2019).
The skin lesion score is an important indicator of fighting after pigs 2.3 | Animals and housing
are mixed (Turner et al., 2006).
In the past, piglets were coated with sow feces or secretions dur- In this study, 144 Seghers piglets, with an initial weight of approxi-
ing the fostering process (Horrell, 1982). However, due to the smell of mately 9.23 ± 0.07 kg, half male and half female, were selected. Six
feces and urine, this process was not well accepted by operators. In different sprays were tested: original creamy, cheese flavor, orange
addition, after merging, the piglets may come from sows with three lit- flavor, truffle, vanilla and pigpen flavor. Each spray was tested sepa-
ters or more. Interference with foreign odors is a method worth rately. For each experiment, four litters of weaning piglets (six
exploring. To study the effect of different odor substances on the 35-day-old piglets from each litter) were selected for the experiment.
fighting behavior and performance of piglets, six synthetic odorous Before mixing the groups, the piglets in each litter were first divided
substances, including original creamy, cheese flavor, orange flavor, into two groups: three piglets were sprayed with odor on the head
truffle, vanilla and pigpen flavor, were used before mixing groups of and neck for 3 s (approximately 1 ml per piglet), and the other three
weaning piglets. Pigpen flavor was used to simulate the normal way piglets were not sprayed. Then, the piglets sprayed with odor from
that was done in the pig farm. Feces and urine from one of the sows four litters were merged into a large pen as the treatment group,
17400929, 2023, 1, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/asj.13809 by College Of Veterinary & Animal Sciences, Pookode, Kerala, Wiley Online Library on [16/10/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
ZHAO ET AL. 3 of 9

TABLE 1 Composition of sprays TABLE 2 Composition and nutrient levels of basal diets

Sprays Composition (%) Items Percentage (%)


Original Ethyl vanillin ≥1.0, vanillin ≥1.5, δ-Decalactone ≥1.0, Ingredients
creamy Ethyl Maltol ≥7.0, γ-Nonanolactone ≥6.0, Corn 52.84
Cinnamaldehyde ≥2.0, γ-Decalactone ≥2.0, butyric
acid ≥6.0 Soybean meal 14.71

Cheese Ethyl vanillin ≥1.5, γ-Octalactone ≥1.8, γ- Wheat bran 23.53


Nonanolactone ≥3.0, vanillin ≥10.0, δ-Decalactone Soybean 3.92
≥2.0, benzyl alcohol ≥8.0, butyric acid ≥7.0, ethyl Vitamin E 1
butyrate ≥3.0
Premix 4
Orange Sweet orange oil ≥0.5, β-ionone ≥0.5, Citral ≥1.0,
Ethyl butyrate ≥3.0, Octanal ≥3.0, decanal ≥6.0, Nutrient levels
Ethyl vanillin ≥3.0, ethyl Maltol ≥6.0 Digestible energy (DE, MJ) 13.69
Vanilla Fennel oil ≥0.5, cinnamon oil ≥0.1, Litsea cubeba oil Crude protein 20.6
≥0.5 Lysine 0.91
Benzyl alcohol ≥0.4, ethyl butyrate ≥3.0, β-ionone
≥0.5, thymol ≥3.0 Calcium 0.88

Truffle Benzaldehyde ≥2.8, α-Terpineol ≥0.1, ethyl vanillin Phosphorus 0.74


≥1.0, benzyl alcohol ≥10, oil of eucalyptus ≥0.1,
Cinene ≥0.1,
2-Undecanone ≥1.0
TABLE 3 Description of the behaviors of piglets
Pigpen Octanoic acid ≥0.6, Caproic acid ≥0.6, butyric acid
≥1.5, Type Description
Ethyl vanillin ≥5, vanillin ≥10, Fighting Reciprocal pushing, biting, head knocking, and
4-Methyl-5-beta-hydroxyethyl thiazole ≥0.5 chasing
Feeding Head in the feeder

whereas the other piglets without odor sprayed were also mixed in a Drinking Drinking water from waterer

large pen as the control group. The weaned piglets were housed in Excreting Defecating or urinating
4 m  4 m slatted floor pens with one feeder and two drinking nipples Resting Lying down, eyes are open or closed
in each pen. Light/dark cycles are provided by natural light. The tem- Exploring Standing, walking or not, sniffing the ground and/or
perature was controlled at 22–24 C. Ventilation is performed by a walls
ventilator at both ends of the pig house. The relative humidity of the Manipulating Nosing or chewing feeder, drinker, floor etc.
pigpen is 60%–75%. The diameter of the feeder is 60 cm. Climbing Forelegs on the back of another pig
For all treatments, food (Table 2) and water were supplied ad libi- Lying on Body weight not supported by the legs, at least 3
tum. All piglets were weighed when the groups were mixed and side legs extended on the side
10 days after mixing the groups. The weight of the feed was mea- Lying on Body weight is not supported by the legs, at least 3
sured every day. We calculated feed intake by weighing the feeder belly legs “crossed” under the piglet

before and after the piglets were fed. Sitting Body weight supported by buttocks and forelimbs

2.4 | Behavioral observations


to reduce the residual food in the mouth of piglets. The piglet chewed
Piglets were continuously video-taped during the 3 days after merging a piece of gauze until it was drenched. The wettest part was cut and
into a large pen. Scan sampling was performed every 10 min between placed into a glass syringe, and the extruded saliva was collected into
08:00 and 18:00 to assess piglet posture and activity (60 scans/day/ 2 ml centrifuge tubes. Experimenters use tweezers to hold the gauze
treatment), as described in Table 3 (Colson et al., 2012). Afterwards, and put it to the pig’s mouth to wait for chewing. In addition, experi-
we calculated the percentage of piglets engaged in one behavioral cat- menters did not touch the piglets. Experimenters wore gloves when
egory relative to the total number of observations. Cameras were pur- handling the gauze. After centrifugation (10,000 rpm, 10 min), the
chased from Sony China Corporate (HDR-XR160E). saliva was collected and stored at 20 C until analysis. Then, saliva
was used for further analysis of cortisol according to the instructions
of the manufacturer (radioimmunoassay). According to the manufac-
2.5 | Cortisol measurement turer’s instructions, the cortisol kit passed ISO 9001 quality manage-
ment system certification. Because the amount of saliva collected by
Saliva was collected from three piglets in each pen at 10:00 on Days each pig was similar, the concentration of cortisol was not corrected
1–3. Before saliva collection, the feeders were removed from the pen for the amount of saliva that was collected.
17400929, 2023, 1, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/asj.13809 by College Of Veterinary & Animal Sciences, Pookode, Kerala, Wiley Online Library on [16/10/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
4 of 9 ZHAO ET AL.

2.6 | Skin lesion score vanilla substances had significantly difference in the proportion of
behaviors (Figure 1). In these group, the piglets fighting behavior
The skin lesion score was analyzed as previously described (Nicolaisen decreased significantly (Table 4).
et al., 2019). On the fourth day after mixing the groups, three piglets
were selected from every pen, 36 piglets in total were observed, and
the number of new wounds and new scars on the head, ears, neck, 3.2 | Feeding behaviors and production
shoulders, buttocks, tail and other parts of the body was recorded performance
during the test. Each pig was evaluated by three people. Observers
were blinded to the treatment of the piglets. The severity of the No significant difference in the frequency of occurrence of eating,
lesions was scored using a categorical classification ranging from 0 to drinking and excreting was observed in piglets in different pens
4 as the severity increased: a score of 0 corresponded to no lesions, (Figure 2a–c). However, compared with control piglets, the piglets
1 corresponded to ≤5 superficial lesions (scratches), 2 corresponded sprayed with the original creamy, cheese flavor and vanilla substances
to 6–10 superficial lesions or ≤5 deep lesions (evidence of hemor- had significantly increased average daily gain (control 154.34 ± 4.59 g/
rhage), 3 corresponded to 11–15 superficial lesions or 6–10 deep day vs. original creamy 216.00 ± 5.11 g/day, control 152.84 ± 2.17 g/
lesions, and 4 corresponded to ≥16 superficial lesions or >10 deep day vs. cheese 181.33 ± 1.36 g/day, control 152.08 ± 5.93 g/day
lesions. vs. vanilla 170.64 ± 4.95 g/day, Figure 2d) and feed intake (control
389.23 ± 5.05 g/day vs. original creamy 412.13 ± 8.57 g/day, control
344.72 ± 5.10 g/day vs. cheese 403.61 ± 7.14 g/day, control 389.15
2.7 | Statistical analysis ± 8.89 g/day vs. vanilla 396.68 ± 10.68 g/day, Figure 2e) and signifi-
cantly decreased feed-gain ratio (control 2.52 ± 0.12 vs. original
GraphPad Prism 8.0 was used for statistical analysis. Each experiment creamy 1.91 ± 0.14, control 2.26 ± 0.11 vs. cheese 1.95 ± 0.2, control
was independent, which means that only the control group and one of 2.56 ± 0.22 vs. vanilla 2.32 ± 0.08).
the treatment groups were compared each time, so comparisons were
performed using t tests and chi-square tests, with p < 0.05 represent-
ing a significant difference. 3.3 | Resting behaviors and posture

The frequency of occurrence of resting behavior in piglets from differ-


3 | RESULTS ent pens was not significantly different (Figure 3a). There was no
impact of treatment on the proportion of sitting (Figure 3b), lying on
3.1 | Observed behavior in each group the back (Figure 3c) or lying on the belly (Figure 3d).

The videos of piglet behaviors were analyzed, and the data analysis
were conducted with chi square test (Table 4 and Figure 1). The per- 3.4 | Aggressive and stress
centage of piglets engaged in one behavioral category relative to the
total number of observations were calculated. Compared with control Observations of piglet fighting behavior showed that after the pig-
piglets, the piglets sprayed with the original creamy, cheese flavor and lets were sprayed with the original creamy, cheese flavor and vanilla

TABLE 4 Contingency table of observed behavior in each group

Fighting Eating Drinking Excreting Resting Exploring Manipulating Climbing


Control 34 100 16 10 481 55 14 10
Original creamy 10 107 20 11 495 52 16 9
Control 30 93 17 10 492 55 14 9
Cheese 9 118 19 11 494 45 18 6
Control 25 105 17 11 471 46 17 10
Orange 25 111 19 11 477 42 19 9
Control 35 115 17 6 479 40 18 10
Vanilla 11 111 15 12 496 46 20 9
Control 24 106 14 10 476 59 19 10
Truffle 24 97 14 11 476 62 20 11
Control 25 106 13 12 481 54 15 11
Pigpen 26 106 13 10 492 47 15 10
17400929, 2023, 1, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/asj.13809 by College Of Veterinary & Animal Sciences, Pookode, Kerala, Wiley Online Library on [16/10/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
ZHAO ET AL. 5 of 9

F I G U R E 1 Observed behavior in
each group. Asterisk (*) means the two
group had significantly difference in the
proportion of behaviors. p < 0.05, versus
the control group

F I G U R E 2 Effect of spraying piglets with different substances on feeding behaviors and production performance after merging into a large
pen. (a–c) Proportion of eating, drinking and excreting behavior after piglets were sprayed with different substances. (d) Average daily gain of
piglets sprayed with different substances. (e) Average daily food intake of piglets sprayed with different substances. (f) Feed-gain ratio of piglets
sprayed with different substances. The figure shows the results of six independent experiments. In each experiment two treatments were
compared (one odor-sprayed and no sprayed control), and 12 pigs were used per treatment. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01 versus the control group

substances, their fighting behavior decreased significantly decreased in the original creamy group, cheese group and vanilla
(Figure 4a). The skin lesion score of the piglets in these groups was group compared with the control group (Figure 4c). The other
also reduced significantly compared with that of piglets in the con- behaviors were not significantly different among the groups
trol group (Figure 4b). Additionally, salivary cortisol was significantly (Figure 4d–f).
17400929, 2023, 1, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/asj.13809 by College Of Veterinary & Animal Sciences, Pookode, Kerala, Wiley Online Library on [16/10/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
6 of 9 ZHAO ET AL.

F I G U R E 3 Effect of spraying piglets


with different substances on resting
behaviors and posture after merging into
a large pen. (a) Proportion of resting
behavior after piglets were sprayed with
different substances. (b–d) Proportion of
different resting behaviors (siting, lying on
the belly and lying on the side) after
piglets were sprayed with different
substances. The figure shows the results
of six independent experiments. In each
experiment, two treatments were
compared (one odor-sprayed and no
sprayed control), and 12 pigs were used
per treatment.

F I G U R E 4 Effect of spraying piglets with different substances on aggression and stress after merging into a large pen. (a) Proportion of
fighting behavior after piglets were sprayed with different substances. (b) Skin lesion score after piglets were sprayed with different substances.
(c) Salivary cortisol levels after piglets were sprayed with different substances. (d–f) Proportion of exploring, manipulating and climbing behaviors
after piglets were sprayed with different substances. The figure shows the results of six independent experiments. In each experiment, two
treatments were compared (one odor-sprayed and no sprayed control), and 12 pigs were used per treatment. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01 versus the
control group
17400929, 2023, 1, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/asj.13809 by College Of Veterinary & Animal Sciences, Pookode, Kerala, Wiley Online Library on [16/10/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
ZHAO ET AL. 7 of 9

4 | DISCUSSION performance under stressing conditions (Calvi et al., 2020). In sum-


mary, spraying the piglets with odorous substances probably elimi-
Fighting behavior is common in swine husbandry, especially during nated the differences in their individual odors and increased the
regrouping after weaning, which may be one of the most serious and mutual recognition and familiarity of piglets with the new environ-
common causes. Piglets have to adapt to the new environment and ment and other piglets, thereby effectively reducing the occurrence of
new feeding regimes and establish new social hierarchies (Fels fighting behaviors and improving production performance. Therefore,
et al., 2014). Therefore, the piglets will exhibit some behavioral our research results can be applied to production to reduce weaning
changes. For example, after regrouping, piglets may show fighting or piglet fighting behavior.
submissive behavior to cope with stress (Sun et al., 2017). Addition- In conclusion, the original creamy, cheese flavor and vanilla sub-
ally, salivary cortisol is often used to assess animal welfare (Morrison stances can significantly reduce the fighting behavior of piglets after
et al., 2007). In another study, piglets with low salivary cortisol levels merging into a large pen and improve production performance.
were less aggressive than those with high salivary cortisol levels
(Clouard et al., 2016). In our study, salivary cortisol was significantly ACKNOWLEDG MENT
decreased in the original creamy group, cheese group and vanilla This work was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation
group compared with the control group; at the same time, piglet fight- of China (31672464, 31790411 and 31828010).
ing behavior was decreased significantly in these groups. Fighting
behavior among piglets is an important factor leading to skin damage. CONFLICTS OF INTEREST
Other studies have shown that scented toys can reduce skin damage The authors declare no conflicts of interest.
and fighting behavior in weaned piglets (Guy et al., 2009). Our results
showed that the skin lesion score of the piglets in these groups was ORCID
also reduced significantly compared with that of piglets in the control Weijie Zhao https://orcid.org/0000-0002-7373-350X
group. In other studies, the rest behavior of the piglets in the control Lina Wang https://orcid.org/0000-0001-8133-3349
group was approximately 70%, which was consistent with our study,
and the proportion of other behaviors was relatively small, which was
RE FE RE NCE S
roughly the same as our results (Colson et al., 2012; D’Eath, 2002).
Boesveldt, S., & de Graaf, K. (2017). The differential role of smell and taste
In our study, as we expected, milk-related original creamy and
for eating behavior. Perception, 46(3–4), 307–319. https://doi.org/
cheese flavor showed a significant effect. This suggests that the famil- 10.1177/0301006616685576
iar milk-related smell during sucking can reduce weaning stress and Brennan, P. A., & Keverne, E. B. (2004). Something in the air? New insights
fighting between piglets. It has been reported that neonatal rats pri- into mammalian pheromones. Current Biology, 14(2), R81–R89.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2003.12.052
marily use olfaction for attachment and prefer the smell of milk
Calvi, E., Quassolo, U., Massaia, M., Scandurra, A., D’Aniello, B., &
(Zimmerberg et al., 2009). In another study, pain was relieved in D’Amelio, P. (2020). The scent of emotions: A systematic review of
human newborns when they were exposed to odors from their human intra- and interspecific chemical communication of emotions.
mother’s milk (Nishitani et al., 2009). Other studies have shown that a Brain and Behavior, 10(5), e1585. https://doi.org/10.1002/brb3.1585
Campbell, T. A., & Long, D. B. (2008). Mammalian visitation to candidate
few essential oils are currently in use as aromatherapy agents to
feral swine attractants. The Journal of Wildlife Management, 72(1),
relieve anxiety, stress, and depression (Herz, 2009). Popular anxiolytic 305–309. https://doi.org/10.2193/2007-227
oils include lavender, rose, orange, bergamot, lemon, sandalwood, Clouard, C., Gerrits, W. J. J., Kemp, B., Val-Laillet, D., & Bolhuis, J. E.
clary sage, Roman chamomile, and rose-scented geranium (2016). Perinatal exposure to a diet high in saturated fat, refined
sugar and cholesterol affects behaviour, growth, and feed intake in
(Setzer, 2009). However, there are no reports on the effects of vanilla
weaned piglets. PLoS ONE, 11(5), e154698. https://doi.org/10.1371/
flavor. Its mechanism needs further study. journal.pone.0154698
Olfactory information is essential for a wide range of animal Colson, V., Martin, E., Orgeur, P., & Prunier, A. (2012). Influence of housing
behaviors, including navigation, foraging, predator aversion, kin recog- and social changes on growth, behaviour and cortisol inpiglets at
weaning. Physiology & Behavior, 107(1), 59–64. https://doi.org/10.
nition, bond formation, mate selection, and sexual and parenting
1016/j.physbeh.2012.06.001
behaviors (Brennan & Keverne, 2004). In another study, piglets were D’Eath, R. B. (2002). Individual aggressiveness measured in a resident-
released an olfactory cue (perhaps adrenal in origin) toward the end of intruder test predicts the persistence of aggressive behaviour and
a fight to signal submission (McGlone, 1985). In addition, the influence weight gain of young pigs after mixing. Applied Animal Behaviour Sci-
of smell on fighting behavior is not only manifested in pigs. Other ence, 77(4), 267–283. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0168-1591(02)
00077-1
studies have shown that the main olfactory epithelium signaling path-
Escribano, D., Ko, H., Chong, Q., Llonch, L., Manteca, X., & Llonch, P.
way was injured, and mice failed to mate or fight (Mandiyan (2019). Salivary biomarkers to monitor stress due to aggression after
et al., 2005). Male hamsters appear to use familiarity with a former weaning in piglets. Research Veterinary Science, 123, 178–183.
opponent’s odors to adaptively regulate their responses to variations https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rvsc.2019.01.014
Fels, M., Hartung, J., & Hoy, S. (2014). Social hierarchy formation in piglets
in social threat (Petrulis et al., 2004). In recent years, evidence has
mixed in different group compositions after weaning. Applied Animal
emerged that chemosignals affect humans’ social interactions, danger Behaviour Science, 152, 17–22. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.
detection and risk-taking behavior, social aspects of eating, and 2014.01.003
17400929, 2023, 1, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/asj.13809 by College Of Veterinary & Animal Sciences, Pookode, Kerala, Wiley Online Library on [16/10/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
8 of 9 ZHAO ET AL.

Geverink, N. A., Schouten, W. G. P., Gort, G., & Wiegant, V. M. (2003). Nicolaisen, T., Risch, B., Luhken, E., van Meegen, C., Fels, M., & Kemper, N.
Individual differences in behaviour, physiology and pathology in (2019). Comparison of three different farrowing systems: Skin
breeding gilts housed in groups or stalls. Applied Animal Behaviour lesions and behaviour of sows with special regard to nursing behav-
Science, 81(1), 29–41. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0168-1591(02) iour in a group housing system for lactating sows. Animal, 13(11),
00253-8 2612–2620. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1751731119000661
Guy, J. H., Burns, S. E., Barker, J. M., & Edwards, S. A. (2009). Reducing Nishitani, S., Miyamura, T., Tagawa, M., Sumi, M., Takase, R., Doi, H.,
post-mixing aggression and skin lesions in weaned pigs by applica- Moriuchi, H., & Shinohara, K. (2009). The calming effect of a maternal
tion of a synthetic maternal pheromone. Animal Welfare, 18(3), 249– breast milk odor on the human newborn infant. Neuroscience Research,
255. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1751731109004480 63(1), 66–71. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neures.2008.10.007
Herz, R. S. (2009). Aromatherapy facts and fictions: A scientific analysis of Ocepek, M., & Andersen, I. L. (2018). Sow communication with piglets
olfactory effects on mood, physiology and behavior. International while being active is a good predictor of maternal skills, piglet sur-
Journal of Neuroscience, 119(2), 263–290. https://doi.org/10.1080/ vival and litter quality in three different breeds of domestic pigs (Sus
00207450802333953 scrofa domesticus). PLoS ONE, 13(11), e206128. https://doi.org/10.
Horrell, R. I. (1982). Immediate behavioural consequences of fostering 1371/journal.pone.0206128
1-week-old piglets. Journal of Agricultural Science, 99(2), 329–336. Oostindjer, M., Bolhuis, J. E., van den Brand, H., Roura, E., & Kemp, B.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0021859600030100 (2010). Prenatal flavor exposure affects growth, health and behavior
Hötzel, M. J., Pinheiro Machado, F., Wolf, F. M., & Dalla Costa, O. A. of newly weaned piglets. Physiology & Behavior, 99(5), 579–586.
(2004). Behaviour of sows and piglets reared in intensive outdoor or https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2010.01.031
indoor systems. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 86(1), 27–39. Peden, R. S. E., Turner, S. P., Boyle, L. A., & Camerlink, I. (2018). The trans-
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2003.11.014 lation of animal welfare research into practice: The case of mixing
Hsu, Y., Earley, R. L., & Wolf, L. L. (2006). Modulation of aggressive behav- aggression between pigs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 204, 1–9.
iour by fighting experience: Mechanisms and contest outcomes. Bio- https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2018.03.003
logical Reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society, 81(1), 33–74. Petrulis, A., Weidner, M., & Johnston, R. E. (2004). Recognition of competi-
https://doi.org/10.1017/S146479310500686X tors by male golden hamsters. Physiology & Behavior, 81(4), 629–638.
Hwang, H. S., Lee, J. K., Eom, T. K., Son, S. H., Hong, J. K., Kim, K. H., & https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2004.03.001
Rhim, S. J. (2016). Behavioral characteristics of weaned piglets mixed Portele, K., Scheck, K., Siegmann, S., Feitsch, R., Maschat, K., Rault, J. L., &
in different groups. Asian-Australasian Journal of Animal Sciences, Camerlink, I. (2019). Sow-piglet nose contacts in free-farrowing pens.
29(7), 1060–1064. https://doi.org/10.5713/ajas.15.0734 Animals (Basel), 9(8), 513. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani9080513
Janczak, A. M., Pedersen, L. J., & Bakken, M. (2003). Aggression, fearful- Samarakone, T. S., & Gonyou, H. W. (2009). Domestic pigs alter their social
ness and coping styles in female pigs. Applied Animal Behaviour Sci- strategy in response to social group size. Applied Animal Behaviour Sci-
ence, 81(1), 13–28. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0168-1591(02) ence, 121(1), 8–15. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2009.08.006
00252-6 Seabolt, B. S., van Heugten, E., Kim, S. W., Ange-van, H. K., & Roura, E.
Kanaan, V. T., Lay, D. C. J., Richert, B. T., & Pajor, E. A. (2012). Increasing (2010). Feed preferences and performance of nursery pigs fed diets
the frequency of co-mingling piglets during the lactation periodalters containing various inclusion amounts and qualities of distillers
the development of social behavior before and after weaning. Journal coproducts and flavor. Journal of Animal Science, 88(11), 3725–3738.
of Applied Animal Welfare Science: JAAWS, 15(2), 163–180. https:// https://doi.org/10.2527/jas.2009-2640
doi.org/10.1080/10888705.2012.658333 Setzer, W. N. (2009). Essential oils and anxiolytic aromatherapy. Natural
Lavelle, M. J., Snow, N. P., Fischer, J. W., Halseth, J. M., Vannatta, E. H., & Product Communications, 4(9), 1305–1316. https://doi.org/10.1177/
Vercauteren, K. C. (2017). Attractants for wild pigs: Current use, 1934578X0900400928
availability, needs, and future potential. European Journal of Wildlife Stookey, J. M., & Gonyou, H. W. (1994). The effects of regrouping on
Research, 63(6), 86. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10344-017-1144-z behavioral and production parameters in finishing swine. Journal of
Mandiyan, V. S., Coats, J. K., & Shah, N. M. (2005). Deficits in sexual and Animal Science, 72(11), 2804–2811. https://doi.org/10.2527/1994.
aggressive behaviors in Cnga2 mutant mice. Nature Neuroscience, 72112804x
8(12), 1660–1662. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1589 Sun, Y., Lian, X., Bo, Y., Guo, Y., & Yan, P. (2017). Effects of 20-day litter
Marchant-Forde, J. N., Bradshaw, R. H., Marchant-Forde, R. M., & weight on weaned piglets’ fighting behavior after group mixing and
Broom, D. M. (2003). A note on the effect of gestation housing envi- on heart rate variability in an isolation test. Asian-Australasian Journal
ronment on approach test measures in gilts. Applied Animal Behaviour of Animal Sciences, 30(2), 267–274. https://doi.org/10.5713/ajas.16.
Science, 80(4), 287–296. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0168-1591(02) 0215
00229-0 Turner, S. P., Farnworth, M. J., White, I. M. S., Brotherstone, S., Mendl, M.,
McGlone, J. J. (1985). Olfactory cues and pig agonistic behavior: Evidence Knap, P., Penny, P., & Lawrence, A. B. (2006). The accumulation of
for a submissive pheromone. Physiology & Behavior, 34(2), 195–198. skin lesions and their use as a predictor of individual aggressiveness
https://doi.org/10.1016/0031-9384(85)90105-2 in pigs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 96(3), 245–259. https://doi.
Morrison, R. S., Johnston, L. J., & Hilbrands, A. M. (2007). The behaviour, org/10.1016/j.applanim.2005.06.009
welfare, growth performance and meat quality of pigs housed in a Turpin, D. L., Langendijk, P., Plush, K., & Pluske, J. R. (2017). Intermittent
deep-litter, large group housing system compared to a conventional suckling with or without co-mingling of non-littermate piglets before
confinement system. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 103(1–2), weaning improves piglet performance in the immediate post-
12–24. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2006.04.002 weaning period when compared with conventional weaning. Journal
Morrow-Tesch, J., & McGlone, J. J. (1990a). Sensory systems and nipple of Animal Science and Biotechnology, 8, 14. https://doi.org/10.1186/
attachment behavior in neonatal pigs. Physiology & Behavior, 47(1), s40104-017-0144-x
1–4. https://doi.org/10.1016/0031-9384(90)90034-2 van Nieuwamerongen, S. E., Bolhuis, J. E., van der Peet-
Morrow-Tesch, J., & McGlone, J. J. (1990b). Sources of maternal odors Schwering, C. M., & Soede, N. M. (2014). A review of sow and piglet
and the development of odor preferences in baby pigs. Journal of Ani- behaviour and performance in group housing systems for lactating
mal Science, 68(11), 3563–3571. https://doi.org/10.2527/1990. sows. Animal, 8(3), 448–460. https://doi.org/10.1017/
68113563x S1751731113002280
17400929, 2023, 1, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/asj.13809 by College Of Veterinary & Animal Sciences, Pookode, Kerala, Wiley Online Library on [16/10/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
ZHAO ET AL. 9 of 9

Verdon, M., Morrison, R. S., & Rault, J. L. (2019). Group lactation from 7 or
14 days of age reduces piglet aggression at weaning compared to How to cite this article: Zhao, W., Yang, N., Zhu, L., Lin, Y.,
farrowing crate housing. Animal, 13(10), 2327–2335. https://doi.org/
Zhang, Q., Shu, G., Wang, S., Gao, P., Zhu, X., Wang, L., &
10.1017/S1751731119000478
Weller, J. E., Camerlink, I., Turner, S. P., Farish, M., & Arnott, G. (2019).
Jiang, Q. (2023). Effect of different odors on the fighting
Socialisation and its effect on play behaviour and aggression in the behavior of weaning piglets after merging into a large pen.
domestic pig (Sus scrofa). Scientific Reports, 9(1), 4180. https://doi. Animal Science Journal, 94(1), e13809. https://doi.org/10.
org/10.1038/s41598-019-40980-1 1111/asj.13809
Zimmerberg, B., Foote, H. E., & Van Kempen, T. A. (2009). Olfactory asso-
ciation learning and brain-derived neurotrophic factor in an animal
model of early deprivation. Developmental Psychobiology, 51(4), 333–
344. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.20373

You might also like