Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

Cultures and Selves: A Cycle of Mutual Constitution

Author(s): Hazel Rose Markus and Shinobu Kitayama


Source: Perspectives on Psychological Science , JULY 2010, Vol. 5, No. 4 (JULY 2010), pp.
420-430
Published by: Sage Publications, Inc. on behalf of Association for Psychological Science

Stable URL: https://www.jstor.org/stable/41613449

REFERENCES
Linked references are available on JSTOR for this article:
https://www.jstor.org/stable/41613449?seq=1&cid=pdf-
reference#references_tab_contents
You may need to log in to JSTOR to access the linked references.

JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide
range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and
facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at
https://about.jstor.org/terms

Sage Publications, Inc. and Association for Psychological Science are collaborating with JSTOR
to digitize, preserve and extend access to Perspectives on Psychological Science

This content downloaded from


103.133.254.25 on Wed, 14 Feb 2024 04:37:02 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
aps ЯШШЁШЁШШ I association for
ЯШШЁШЁШШ I association for
PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE

Perspectives on Psychological Science


5(4) 420-430
Cultures and Selves: A Cycle of Mutual © The Author(s) 2010
Reprints and permission:

Constitution sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 1 0.1 177/1745691610375557
http://pps.sagepub.com

®SAGE

Hazel Rose Markus1 and Shinobu Kitayama2


'Department of Psychology, Stanford University, CA and department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor

Abstract
The study of culture and self casts psychology's understanding of the self, identity, or agency as central to the ana
interpretation of behavior and demonstrates that cultures and selves define and build upon each other in an ongoing
mutual constitution. In a selective review of theoretical and empirical work, we define self and what the self does, d
culture and how it constitutes the self (and vice versa), define independence and interdependence and determine how
shape psychological functioning, and examine the continuing challenges and controversies in the study of culture an
propose that a self is the "me" at the center of experience - a continually developing sense of awareness and ag
guides actions and takes shape as the individual, both brain and body, becomes attuned to various environmen
incorporate the patterning of their various environments and thus confer particular and culture-specific form and fun
the psychological processes they organize (e.g., attention, perception, cognition, emotion, motivation, interpersonal rela
group). In turn, as selves engage with their sociocultural contexts, they reinforce and sometimes change the ideas, prac
institutions of these environments.

Keywords
culture, self, agency, independence, interdependence

Within psychology, the empirical study of the self asina the


cul-U.S., but by judgments made about both the self and
tural product and process is now almost three decades aboutold
one's mother in China (Zhu, Zhang, Fan, & Han, 2007).
(e.g., A. Fiske, Kitayama, Markus, & Nisbett, 1998; MarkusMoreover, in the last decade, the cultural comparisons stud-
& Kitayama, 1991; Shweder & Bourne, 1984; Triandis, ied are no longer just between people in North American and
1989). Hundreds of surveys, laboratory experiments, andEast Asian contexts; they now include comparisons across a
field
studies have bolstered earlier theories and ethnographic obser-of other significant social distinctions. Researchers also
variety
vations, drawing attention to powerful variation in self andfor example, that people in West African settings claim
know,
personhood. Researchers now have a good grasp of more why enemies
the and fewer friends than those in North American
nail that sticks out is likely to be hammered down insettings
Japan (Adams, 2005); that Western Europeans are less likely
whereas the squeaky wheel attracts grease and attentionthan North Americans to associate happiness with personal
in the
United States (for reviews, see Heine, 2008; Kitayama &
achievement (Kitayama, Park, Sevincer, Karasawa, & Uskul,
Cohen, 2007). They know, for example, that North American
2009); that Latino dyads talk, smile, and laugh more than do
Black
students can be expected to speak up in class more than and White dyads (Holloway, Waldrip, & Ickes, 2009);
their
Korean American counterparts (Kim, 2002); that parental
that Protestants are more likely than Jews to believe that people
expectations can have opposite motivational effects inhave
Asiancontrol over their thoughts (A.B. Cohen & Rozin, 2001);
American and European American families (Iyengar & Lep-
that people from the U.S. South respond with more anger to
per, 1999); that Japanese Olympic gold medalists, in compar-
insults than do Northerners (Nisbett, 1993); and that working
ison with American medalists, likely discuss their failures and
faults more than their successes and virtues (Markus, Uchida,
Omoregie, Townsend, & Kitayama, 2006); that helping others
is a moral obligation that holds whether or not one likes the
Corresponding Author
person in Indian contexts, but not in American contexts
Hazel Rose Markus, Department of Psychology, Jordan Hall, Building 420,
(Miller & Bersoff, 1998); and that the medial prefrontal cor- University, Stanford,
Stanford CA 94305
tex of the brain is activated by judgments made about E-mail:
the self
hmarkus@stanford.edu

This content downloaded from


103.133.254.25 on Wed, 14 Feb 2024 04:37:02 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
Culture and Self 42 1

class Americans react less strongly than middle-class Ameri- current and fu
cans to having their choices denied (Snibbe & Markus, 2005). & Scheier, 1998
These striking differences in behavior, as well as hundredssituated and, a
of others like them, are important in their own right. They texts in signifi
markedly expand the range of the normal, or of the "good" Just as one ca
or "right way to be," by revealing patterns of thinking, feeling, cannot be a sel
and acting that have not been part of mainstream psychology. lically mediat
Understanding these differences has significant practical appli-the social env
cations for intergroup relations, education, health, well-being, Markus & Kita
business, and peaceful coexistence in an increasingly diverse or constitution
and interconnected world. The study of culture and self, how-"how" and "when." Cultural variation across selves arises
ever, has two other highly significant consequences for the from differences in the images, ideas (including beliefs, values,
field of psychology, and they are the focus here. and stereotypes), norms, tasks, practices, and social interac-
tions that characterize various social environments and reflects
First, the study of culture and self has renewed and extended
psychology's understanding of the self, identity, or agency differences
and in how to attune to these environments.
casts it as central to the analysis and interpretation of behavior.
Theorists use a family of overlapping terms for the nexus of
Experience is socioculturally patterned, and the self reflects the
the biological, psychological, and sociocultural: self, self-
individual's engagement with the world that is the source concept,
of self-schema, self-construal, selfway, self-narrative,
this patterning: The array of contrasting behavioral differences
ego, psyche, mind, identity, personal identity, social identity,
and agency. Agency is the most general or global term and
described in the opening paragraph can all be illuminated with
a focus on what it means to be a self or agent in a particular
refers to acting in the world. Self is usually interchangeable
sociocultural context. with agency but is sometimes used to refer more specifically
Second, the study of culture and self has led to the realiza-
to how the person thinks or believes him or herself to be. Iden-
tity is typically used when the emphasis is on how others, be
tion that people and their sociocultural worlds are not separate
they individuals or groups, influence the person. All of the
from one another. Instead they require each other and complete
terms are similar in purpose. They attempt to index the
one another. In an ongoing cycle of mutual constitution, people
dynamic and recursive process of organizing and integrating
are socioculturally shaped shapers of their environments; they
make each other up and are most productively analyzed through which the individual, the biological entity, becomes
together (Shweder, 2003). The comparative method of socio-a meaningful entity - that is, a person.
cultural psychology reveals that although feeling, thinking, and
acting can take particular, culture-specific forms, the capacity
What Does a Self Do?
to continually shape and to be shaped by the context is a pow-
erful human universal. Selves are implicitly and explicitly at work in all aspects o
In the sections below, we examine these two consequencesbehavior: attention, perception, cognition, emotion, motivat
relationships, and group processes. More specifically, o
of the study of culture and self in detail. In the course of a selec-
ongoing sense of self functions as a foundational schema t
tive review of some of the major empirical and theoretical con-
tributions, we will define self and what the self does, define
recruits and organizes more specific self-regulatory schém
culture and how it constitutes the self (and vice versa), define
including cognitive, emotional, motivational, somatic, and b
independence and interdependence and determine how they vioral schémas. Some of the compelling evidence for selve
shape psychological functioning, and examine the continuing work can be seen in studies in U.S. contexts with American
challenges and controversies in the study of culture and self.ticipants. People hear their own name across a noisy crow
room (Wood & Cowan, 1 995), remember their own contribut
to a project better than they remember the contributions
What Is a Self?
their coworkers (Ross & Sicoly, 1979), and are motivated b
A self is the "me" at the center of experience - a continuallyself-interest and self-concern across a wide variety of dom
developing sense of awareness and agency that guides(Greenwald, action 1980). In broad strokes, people in North Ameri
and takes shape as the individual, both brain andcontexts body, are smarter, kinder, healthier, and happier when t
becomes attuned to the various environments it inhabits. Selves selves are affirmed or when situations are self or identity con
are thus psychological realities that are both biologically
ent than when selves are threatened or when situations are iden
(LeDoux, 1996; Northoff et al., 2006) and socioculturally
incongruent (e.g., Oyserman, 2008; Steele, Spencer, & Arons
(Markus & Kitayama, 1991) rooted. Selves develop as individ-2002; Taylor, Lerner, Sherman, Sage, & McDowell, 2003).
uals attune themselves to contexts that provide different solu-Researchers have now moved beyond the traditional conf
of research within North America and have observed contexts
tions to the universal questions of "Who or what am I?",
"What should I be doing?", and "How do I relate to others?"like those in East Asia and South Asia. These contexts are quite
(Kitayama & Uchida, 2005; Markus & Hamedani, 2007). They
differently arranged than North American ones and are ani-
are simultaneously schémas of past behavior and patterns for
mated by different ontological understandings of what a person

This content downloaded from


103.133.254.25 on Wed, 14 Feb 2024 04:37:02 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
422 Markus and Kitayama

Fig. I. The mutual constitution of cultures and selves. Figure adapted from Markus and Kitayama ( 1 994) and
Fiske et al. (1998).

is. These comparisons


such as world, environment,
among contexts, cultural
people
systems, soc
world have systems, social
revealed structures, institutions, practices,
differences polic
in
terns of attuning
meanings, to norms,contexts,
and values, that give form and that
direction
As a result, behavior. Culture
many is not a stable set of beliefs or values-
processes that perc
motivation, relational
reside inside people. Instead,andculture is located
intergr
in the world,
thought to be basic,
in patterns of ideas,universal, and
practices, institutions, products, and arti-
ing, have been facts (e.g., Adams & Markus,
found to 2004; vary Atran, Medin,dram & Ross,
parisons, the influence
2005; Chui & Hong, 2006; Kroeber &of the
Kluckhohn, 1952; s
becomes even more
Shweder, 2003). apparent.
With this definition, the emphasis in the study of culture and
What Is Culture and How Does It Constitute self is not on studying culture as collections of people - the
Japanese, the Americans, the Whites, the Latinos - but is instead
the Self (and Vice-Versa)? on how psychological processes may be implicitly and explicitly
shaped by the
Just as the word self is used to index a family of overlapping butworlds, contexts, or sociocultural systems that
people
not identical terms, the word culture is a stand-in for inhabit. As illustrated in Figure 1, the self (i.e., body,
a similarly
brain,
untidy and expansive set of material and symbolic and psychological tendencies) and the sociocultural
concepts,

This content downloaded from


103.133.254.25 on Wed, 14 Feb 2024 04:37:02 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
Culture and Self 423

content (i.e., proliferation


ideas, practices
continually ecological
constitute fact
one
the of
mediating philosophy
self and psyc
As indicated Notably,
in Figure the
1 , i
cu
ual; it is
a independence
product of hum
activity as Every
well as context
the thou
individuals of
who both.
have In all
come
context transactions)
shapes the self w
thro
tacit ences
categories ofand goal
culture
input from friend relatiom
sociocultural
is the center and
of relationsh
awareness
reflects carries
these elemen
sociocultural
feelings, anding degrees
actions (A
(i.e.,
change, the 1995). Neverth
sociocultural fo
cycle of these
mutual two sch
constitutio
As a sidered dominant or foundational.
consequence of this
dynamic In an early paper on culture and the self 2000
(Kashima, (Markus &
dynamic in Kitayama,
that 1991), we proposed
the that if one of these schémas
socioc
products, becomes foundational - guiding how cultural ideas,
artifacts, econo practices,
that institutions, and products
comprise it of a culture
are are evaluated, selected,
con
changed and deselectedtime.
over or weeded out - there will be Selves
widespread and
the various important
cultural conte
differences in the nature and functioning of the self
tion, a and in the
focus on psychologicaltheprocesses that aresociocu
rooted in these
deny schémas. Figure 2 is an adaptation and amplification ofand
the
individuality an
even earlier figure representing
most the tight-knitindependent and interdependent
vidual selves (Markus & Kitayama, 1991). Thein
participates diagram reflects
athe- var
texts that orizing and empirical work since this time (see
constitute theHeine, 2008; se
might Markus & Kitayama,
include 2003) and depicts two different patternscol
specific
gin, such of
as attuning to
the the social world and two different senses of
family
defined by self or
gender,agency. ethnic
class, birth As shown Figure 2, when an independent
cohort, and schema of self se
ing similar organizes behavior, the primary referent is the individual's own
configurations
spaces will thoughts, feelings, and actions. Alternatively, when
obviously an interde-
diver
experiences pendent
and schema of self organizes
will behavior, the immediate
differ refer-
experiences
ent is the(e.g.,
thoughts, feelings, and actions Markus
of others with whom the
person is in relationship. With an independent self (i.e., an inde-
pendent way of attuning to the social environment or indepen-
What Is dentIndepende
mode of being), interaction with others (actual, imagined,
One or implied) produces a sense of
particularly self as separate, distinct, or inde-
powerful
which pendent from others. These the
prescribe interactions are guided by culturally
norm
the self prescribed individual)
(the tasks that require and encourage the development and an
scientists in various
reification of individual preferences, goals, beliefs, and field
abilities
1858/1973; (as indicated by the Xs in the independent
Mead, 1934; self-schema) and the Tr
rized two use of these attributes as referents and
distinct guides for action. The
types of
be linked to large dotted circle separates close relations from moremod
divergent distant
of sociality relations,
assumes suggesting that people have a sense that
thatthey can move so
of between ingroup and outgroup
instrumental relatively easily.
interests a
Labels for such With an interdependent
socialself (i.e., an interdependent way of
relat
dent, attuning to the social environmentand
egocentric, or interdependent modeindiof
assumes being), interaction
that with others produces a sense of self as con-
individuals a
meaningful nected to, related to, or interdependent with others.relati
through These inter-
social actions are guided by include
relations culturally prescribed tasks that require ge
centric, and encourage fitting in with others (as indicated
communal, and by the Xs co
Shweder & in the overlap between self and others in the
Bourne, interdependent
1984;
The origins self-schema
of in Fig. 2),these
taking the perspective of others,
two reading
contested. the expectations of others,researcher
Some adjusting to others, and using others

This content downloaded from


103.133.254.25 on Wed, 14 Feb 2024 04:37:02 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
424

Fig. 2. Independent a
Kitayama (1991) and

as referents for
Further,action.
the
are dotted (those
with a deline
solid l
solid), and they
tion represen
is signif

This content downloaded from


103.133.254.25 on Wed, 14 Feb 2024 04:37:02 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
Culture and Self 425

line, frequently
In contrast, when the schemaresulting
for self is interdependent with
and outgroup members
others and this schema organizes agency, people will have a (se
It is sense of themselves as partto
important of encompassing social relation-
note th
form of sociality or
ships. People are likely to reference others, of
and to understand in
relationshipstheir individual
are actions as contingent
understo
on or organized by the
choice. actions of others and their relations with
Likewise, these others. Actions
interde
types of independence
rooted in this schema will have different meanings and conse- in
by quences than actions rooted in a with
identification independent schema. Thus, or a
in a
relationship.
lack of speech does not imply aAlthough
lack of thinking, performing well
are likely to
on a taskbeselected by one's
responsive
mother does not imply a preference
mony or affection among
for having choices usurped or a lack of self-efficacy, and attend-
relationshipsing to one's(Kitayama
shortcomings does not imply low self-esteem or et
depression (Iyengar & Lepper, 1999; Kim, 2002; Markus
et al., 2006). Such tendencies instead can reflect an acknowl-
How Do Independence and Interdependence
edgement of one's role or obligations in a particular situation
Shape Psychological Functioning? and an awareness of the significant others with whom one is
The distinction between independence and interdependence as interdependent and who define the self. Similarly, fostering
foundational schémas for the self has proved to be a powerful good relations (Holloway et al., 2009), having concerns about
heuristic for demonstrating how sociocultural contexts canone's enemies (Adams, 2005), experiencing a heightened sensi-
shape self-functioning and psychological functioning (fortivity to others' evaluations (Nisbett, 1993), having greater con-
detailed reviews, see A. Fiske et al., 1998; Heine, 2008; cern for others' actions than for thoughts (A.B. Cohen & Rozin,
Kitayama & Cohen, 2007). Returning to the selection of find- 2001), and exhibiting relatively little concern with getting to
ings described in the opening paragraphs of this article, all of choose (Snibbe & Markus, 2005) are also consistent with a sense
the differences cited can be explained in some important partof one's self as being related to others and with an awareness of
by the independent and interdependent patterns of sociality.the relatively larger role of others in influencing who you are and
Across all of these examples, the ideas and/or practices in onewhat you should be doing. Moreover, even the same region of
setting place relatively more emphasis on the attributes of thethe brain is activated by both significant others (mother) and the
individual and their expression as the form of agency, whereasself for people in Chinese contexts (Zhu et al., 2007), which
the ideas and practices of the comparison setting place rela- serves as yet another type of evidence for the psychological real-
tively more emphasis on relationships and social responsive- ity of this interdependent sense of agency.
ness and the maintenance of these relationships as the form Together these findings, and hundreds more like them,
of agency. powerfully demonstrate that independence and interdepen-
When the schema for self is independent from others and dence have significant psychological consequences - for
this schema organizes agency, people will have a sense of cognition, emotion, motivation, morality, relationships, inter-
themselves as separate and will be relatively likely to focus group processes, health, and well-being - and the field's view
on, reference, and express their own thoughts, feeling, and of these concepts is broadening. For example, viewing aspects
goals. For example, people in North American settings are of the world and one's self as distinct objects and attributes that
likely to speak out and emphasize their good qualities, are separate from their contexts (e.g., Masuda et al., 2005), per-
because in doing so they can express their defining prefer- ceiving one's self to be consistent across situations (e.g., Suh,
ences or attributes (Kim, 2002). Highlighting one's successes 2002), and experiencing well-being in the pursuit of fun and
after a performance functions similarly by drawing attention enjoyment (e.g., Oishi & Diener, 2001) derive from and con-
to one's positive, defining attributes (Markus et al., 2006). tribute to a sense of independence. Alternatively, paying atten-
In addition, people in North American settings decide tion to the context, others, role obligations, and duties; taking
whether or not to help someone based on their preferences, the other's perspective; and cultivating feelings of balance or
and normatively good actions follow from the expression of calm in relations with others derive from and serve to further
these preferences (Miller & Bersoff, 1998). Similarly, choice realize a sense of interdependence (e.g., D. Cohen &
enhances the performance of middle-class Americans, and Hoshino-Browne, 2005; Mesquita, 2001; Tsai, Louie, Chen,
they seek out and construct their actions in terms of choice & Uchida, 2007).
because choice allows the expression of these preferences
and thus serves to affirm the self (Iyengar & Lepper, 1999;
Snibbe & Markus, 2005). Lastly, individual achievement and What Are the Continuing Challenges
success are associated with happiness in independent settings
and Controversies in the Study of Culture
because achievement signals positive internal attributes
and Self?
(Kitayama et al., 2009). In all cases, these actions reflect set-
tings that foster the sense that the individual is the source of We now know considerably more about cultural variation in
thought, feeling, and action. the self and, further, have gained numerous insights into the

This content downloaded from


103.133.254.25 on Wed, 14 Feb 2024 04:37:02 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
426 Markus and Kitayama

ways in which that


the cultural variation
self assessed in terms
is of collective
shaped artifacts b
time, shapes culture.
is bound to be far greater thanA number
the corresponding cultural var-
unresolved, iation as assessed in terms of self-reported
unaddressed, or beliefsotherw
and values
sely debated. (Morling & Lamoreaux, 2008). Another approach to assessing
culture is a situation sampling method in which participants
generate situations that are associated with particular thoughts
Measurement of Culture and feelings (i.e., feeling good, feeling in control). Researchers
Numerous researchers have assumed that at least some ele- then give these situations to another group of respondents to see
ments of culture should be measurable in a self-report format
if envisioning these particular situations produces the psycho-
logical tendencies that gave rise to them (e.g., Kitayama,
and have administered a variety of cultural value question-
naires. One most prominent example is a large-scale cross-
Markus, Matsumoto, & Norasakkunkit, 1997).
cultural survey Hofstede administered on IBM employees Both the personal, explicit aspects and the more collective,
across the world (Hofstede, 1980). Schwartz and colleagues
tacit aspects are important in understanding and, thus, measur-
have tested cultural variations in self-reported value priorities
ing culture. One important step for the field is, on the one hand,
(e.g., Schwartz, 1992). Also notable are some scales assessing
to articulate exactly how the two aspects of culture might be
individualism and collectivism, tightness and looseness, or
dynamically related and, on the other, to specify how collective
independent and interdependent self-construal (e.g., Gelfand,
cultural environments that structure a person's life might inter-
Nishii, & Raver, 2006; Singelis, 1994; Triandis, 1995). act
Onewith the person's personal beliefs and values to determine
his or her psychological behaviors (D. Cohen, 2007). Effort
strength of this approach is that measurement is relatively
along this line would require simultaneous examinations of
straightforward and involves evaluating attributes or items
along rating scales. Cultures can be quantified on different
groups that vary systematically in terms of collective artifacts
dimensions and can be readily compared. One potential prob-
and individuals within each group who vary systematically in
terms of their personal beliefs and values.
lem is that it is not always obvious whether and to what extent
culture can be reduced to each individual's beliefs, values, or
behavioral observations. Another important challenge stems
Measurement
from the fact that it is not known whether one's beliefs or val- of Self
Parallel issues of measurement can be raised for the self as
ues are always accessible to one's conscious reflection. If not,
the validity of self-report questionnaires may be called into
well. In recent decades, research and theorizing about the self
question. has been anchored on particular methods that assess how peo-
Other researchers have, instead, taken the fact that culture is
ple consciously think about themselves. This is necessary and
actually quite tacit and taken for granted as a starting point important work because in settings like those in North Amer-
(Markus & Hamedani, 2007). These researchers also assume ica, which focus on and encourage an explicit understanding
that beliefs and values such as individualism and collectivism of the self, the explicit self-concept can be shown to mediate
are important components of culture. How they differ from the and regulate much of behavior (e.g., Oyserman, 2008). Within
first group of researchers stems from an observation that cul- this tradition of work, the most face-valid measure of self is
tural beliefs and values - especially those that are important how people describe themselves. One most commonly used
and, thus, have constituted each culture's practices, institu- research tool in this school of thought is the 20 statements test,
tions, and its ways of life - are, by definition, inscribed into
wherein participants are asked to describe themselves in 20 dif-
these practices, institutions, and ways of life. These beliefs ferent
and ways (e.g., Cousins, 1989).
values are externalized and materialized in the world An equally robust and time-honored tradition of research on
(D' Andrade, 1995) and, thus, no longer need to be the packed in emphasized the crucial role of unconscious self-
self has
the head of each individual member of the cultural regulation. group. ForThe self, as we have noted, encompasses not only
example, contemporary American society as a whole may
what thebe
person regards himself or herself to be, but also how
described as individualistic, not so much because many peoplemem-
regulate their behavior in somewhat specific and char-
bers of this society strongly endorse individualistic values
acteristic fashions. This view suggests that there are many ways
(although this could also be true), but rather because ofthis
beingsoci-
or senses of the self that are not represented in one's
ety is composed of interpersonal routines, situations, explicit beliefs. Such aspects are likely to be implicit in the
practices,
social institutions, and social systems that are fundamentally sense that they do not directly index thoughts and feelings
individualistic. about the self, but instead reflect differences in attending, per-
On the basis of this reasoning, some researchers haveceiving, feeling, thinking, and acting that arise as people attune
assessed collective artifacts of culture, such as ads in TV or themselves to contexts that provide different solutions to the
popular magazines, children's books, religious texts, and news existential questions of who or what am I and what should I
coverage of sporting events (Kim & Markus, 1999; Markus be doing. These implicit psychological tendencies are most
et al., 2006; Tsai et al., 2007; Tsai, Miao, & Seppala, 2007).likely to be unconscious and may be equally consequential in
An extensive review of this literature has concluded that cul-
organizing one's psychological behaviors. Moreover, there is
tures do differ in terms of collective artifacts and, moreover,
no reason to assume that the explicit and the implicit aspects

This content downloaded from


103.133.254.25 on Wed, 14 Feb 2024 04:37:02 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
Culture and Self 427

of the self are closely


independence or interdependence is associated with different link
2009). cues (such as singular vs. plural pronouns).
One crucial question for the dynamic social constructivist
view is to specify what particular knowledge might be lined
What Does Cultural Priming Mean? to different cultural icons. For example, Chinese icons may
One important development of cultural psychological work in well call out behaviors that are common in China. Although
the last decade was the proliferation of priming work. This lit- this might be true in a general, abstract sense, it might also
erature highlights two related, but theoretically distinct, meth- be the case that within any given cultural context, specific icons
odologies. One approach assumes that cultures carry icons that might be associated with, and could thus be used to call out,
are associated with commonly available meanings and prac- particular aspects of Chinese culture. A parallel question can
tices. These icons may then be used to "call out" mental repre- be raised for the situated cognition approach. Although the gen-
sentations of relevant cultural meanings and practices. For eral concepts of independence and interdependence are likely
example, one set of pioneering studies tested bicultural Hong to be commonly available across many, and perhaps all, cul-
Kong Chinese and showed that they either exhibit a prototypi- tures, it is far from clear whether independence and interdepen-
cally East Asian response or a prototypically Western response dence mean the same thing across cultures - most theorizing on
depending on the cultural icons used in the priming manipula- the topic suggests that they do not. Think about a Chinese adult
tion (Hong, Morris, Chiù, & Benet-Martinez, 2000). When par- who regards himself as very independent and self-reliant
ticipants were exposed to Chinese scenes, such as dragons and because he is capable of providing financial assistance for his
the Great Wall, bicultural Hong Kong Chinese showed more ailing parents. Even though this behavior is regarded as an
prototypically interdependent behaviors, but when exposed to instance of independence in one cultural community, the same
American scenes, such as the Statue of Liberty or Liberty Bell, behavior may easily be reconstrued as an instance of interde-
they showed prototypically independent behaviors. Because pendence in another. It seems quite clear that the priming
the pertinent cultural knowledge is considered to construct psy- approaches will be enriched substantially when supplemented
chological experience in dynamic interaction with certain per- with an in-depth analysis of the nature of cultural knowledge
sonality characteristics of the actor, such as the need for that is called out by specific priming stimuli.
cognitive closure, this approach is called the dynamic social Another important question that must be addressed is
constructivist approach. whether knowledge is always a mediating element in all
Another approach is based on the assumption that the sché- forms of cultural influence. That is to say, can culture's influ-
mas of independence and interdependence are, in large part, ences be most fully understood in terms of the ability of cul-
universal and shared across cultures (Oyserman & Lee, tural contexts to activate key psychological constructs such as
2007). With this assumption, one might suppose that cultures independence and interdependence? An alternative perspec-
are very different in terms of availability of cues that call out tive, and the one we have assumed here, is that that sociocul-
one or the other schema. Within this theoretical framework, a tural contexts afford cultural practices that become
number of researchers have investigated potential effects of a incorporated into the behavioral routines of daily life (see
variety of priming manipulations designed to call out either Fig. 1). These practices often reflect and foster orientations
independence or interdependence. For example, participants toward and values of independence and interdependence.
may be presented with a paragraph describing the behaviors From the very beginning of one's life, then, individuals are
of a single individual who was referred to as "I" or a paragraph encouraged to be engaged in such practices, initially only
in which the same set of behaviors was attributed to a group passively but gradually more and more actively. Repeated
described as "we" (Brewer & Gardner, 1996). Frequent refer- and continuous engagement in some select set of practices
ence to the personal self ("I") may be assumed to call out inde- or situations involving certain features, such as self-
pendence, whereas frequent reference to the relational self expression in an independent cultural context or adjustment
("we") may be assumed to call out interdependence. Because or conformity in an interdependent cultural context, may lead
this approach implies that the generic schémas of independence to some characteristic patterns of psychological responses.
and interdependence are embedded in specific social situations These responses may be initially deliberate and effortful, but
that carry different sets of cues that call out the generic sché- they will eventually be highly practiced and thus automa-
mas, it is called the situated cognition approach. tized. In fact, recent neuroscience evidence suggests that
These priming methods have been highly instrumental in repeated engagement in certain tasks, including cultural tasks
advancing our understanding about a proximate mechanism such as self-expression or conformity, is likely to cause cor-
by which culturally specific behaviors may be induced. Once responding changes in brain pathways (see Han & Northoff,
culturally relevant knowledge is activated, this knowledge 2008; Kitayama & Park, 2009, for reviews). It is evident,
mediates the effect of culture on behavior. The two approaches then, that culture may influence psychological processes not
vary in the nature of this knowledge. Whereas the dynamic only by providing priming stimuli that bias one's responses
social constructivist approach assumes that culture-specific in one way or another, but also by affording a systematic
knowledge is closely linked to cultural icons, the situated cog- context for development in general and the establishment
nition approach hypothesizes that generic knowledge of of systematic response tendencies in particular.

This content downloaded from


103.133.254.25 on Wed, 14 Feb 2024 04:37:02 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
428

Concluding
References
Rem
In the Adams, G. three
last (2005). The cultural grounding of personal
deca relationship:
cultural Enemyship in North American and West African worlds. Journal
psychologic
classNorth of Personality and Social Psychology , 88, 948-968.
Americans
AmericansAdams, G.,as
& Markus, H.R.one(2004). Toward a conception
of of culture t
of this suitable for a social psychology of culture. In M. Schaller &
Euro-Americ
learned С. S. Crandall
a great(Eds.), The psychological foundations
deaof culture
humans - those from middle-class North American and West- (pp. 335-360). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
ern European contexts (Arnett, 2008; Markus, Kitayama, &Adams, G. (2005). The cultural grounding of personal relationship:
Heiman, 1997). People engaging in these contexts are likely Enemyship in North American and West African worlds. Journal
to reveal relatively high levels of self-esteem, self-efficacy, of Personality and Social Psychology , 88, 948-968.
optimism, or intrinsic motivation and express a desire for mas-Arnett, J.J. (2008). The neglected 95%: Why American psychology
tery, control, achievement, choice, self-expression, or unique- needs to become less American. American Psychologist , 63,
602-614.
ness. They also like to feel happy, upbeat, and successful,
and their agency often takes the form of influencing others orAtran, S., Medin, D.L., & Ross, N.O. (2005). The cultural mind: Envi-
the world. We can now confidently say that this robust set of ronmental decision making and cultural modeling within and
psychological tendencies - with its many world-making and across populations. Psychological Review , 112 , 744-776.
world-maintaining consequences - is not, however, anBanaji, M., & Prentice, D. (1994). The self in social contexts. Annual
expression of a universal human nature. Instead, it reflects the Review of Psychology , 45, 297-332.
particular worlds in which these people engage. These well- Brewer, M.B., & Gardner, W. (1996). Who is this "we"? Levels of
documented self-serving and self-interested tendencies are cre- collective identity and self representations. Journal of Personality
ated, fostered, and maintained by widely distributed ideas, such and Social Psychology , 71, 83-93.
as the importance of individual achievement, that have been Bruner, J. (1990). Acts of meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
reinforced and instituted by dense networks of everyday prac- University Press.
tices, such as complimenting and praising one another for indi-Carver, C.S., & Scheier, M.F. (1998). On the self regulation of beha-
vidual performance, frequently distributing awards and honors vior. New York: Cambridge University Press.
in classrooms and workplaces, and promoting the self in situa-Cohen, A.B., & Rozin, P. (2001). Religion and the morality of mental-
tions like applying for jobs. These tendencies for self- ity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 81, 697-710.

expression, feeling good about the self, and controlling the Cohen, D. (2007). Methods in cultural psychology. In S. Kitayama &
environment are further encouraged by products such as coffee D. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of cultural psychology (pp. 196-236).
New York: Guilford.
mugs, bumper stickers, self-help books, automobile advertise-
Cohen, D., & Hoshino-Browne, E. (2005). Insider and outsider per-
ments, medications, perfume, and cleaning products that exhort
people to "Be a star," "Take control," "Never follow," and spectives on the self and social world. In R.M. Sorrentino,
"Get happy." Notably, when people inhabit many other kinds D. Cohen, J.M. Olson & M.P. Zanna (Eds.), Culture and social
of worlds that are configured with ideas, practices, and institu- behavior : The Ontario symposium (Vol. 10, pp. 49-76).
tions that do not construct the self as the primary source of Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

action, strikingly different psychological tendencies areCousins, S.D. (1989). Culture and self-perception in Japan and the
revealed. United States. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 56,
124-131.
Although a vast amount of both theoretical and empirical
work remains before researchers can more fully specify the D' Andrade, R.G. (1995). The development of cognitive anthropology.
cycles of mutual constitution between cultures and selves, this Cambridge, United Kingdom: Cambridge University Press.
work is steadily changing the way psychology understands theDumont, L. (1977). From Mandeville to Marx: The genesis and tri-
person. Psychologists and all behavioral scientists are less cer- umph of economic ideology. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
tain about what can be designated as basic or universal psycho-Fiske, A., Kitayama, S., Markus, H.R., & Nisbett, R.E. (1998). The
logical process and more certain that it is not possible to cultural matrix of social psychology. In D. Gilbert, S. Fiske, &
develop a comprehensive human psychology by focusing G. Lindzey, The handbook of social psychology, vol. 2 (4th ed.,
solely on the individual and on what is inside that individual pp. 915-981). San Francisco: McGraw-Hill.
(e.g., Barrett, Mesquita, & Smith, in press; Bruner, 1990). Such Fiske, S.T., & Taylor, S.E. (1994). Social cognition. (2nd ed.).
a psychology will require a focus on humans' remarkable Reading, MA: Addison- Wesley.
capacity to create cultures and then to be shaped by them. Gelfand, M.J., Nishii, L.H., & Raver, J.L. (2006). On the nature and
importance of cultural tightness-looseness. Journal of Applied Psy-
chology, 91, 1225-1244.
Declaration of Conflicting Interests Greenfield, P.M. (2009). Linking social change and developmental
The authors declared that they had no conflicts of interest with respect change: Shifting pathways of human development. Developmental
to their authorship or the publication of this article. Psychology, 45, 401-418.

This content downloaded from


103.133.254.25 on Wed, 14 Feb 2024 04:37:02 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
Culture and Self 429

Greenwald, Markus,
A.G. (1980).
H.R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implica- Th
revision of tions
personal histo
for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological
603-618. Review , 98, 224-253.
Han, S., & Northoff, G. (2008). Culture-sensitive neural substrates
Markus, of
H.R., & Kitayama, S. (1994). A collective fear of the collec-
human cognition: A transcultural neuroimaging approach. Nature
tive: Implications for selves and theories of selves. Personality and
Reviews Neuroscience , 9, 646-654. Social Psychology Bulletin , 20, 568-579.
Heine, S.J. (2008). Cultural psychology. New York: Norton.Markus, H.R., & Kitayama, S. (2003). Models of agency: Sociocul-
Hofstede, G. (1980). Culture's consequences: International differ-
tural diversity in the construction of action. In V.M. Berman &
ences in work-related values. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. J.J. Berman (Eds.), Nebraska symposium on motivation: Cross-
Holloway, R.A., Waldrip, A.M., & Ickes, W. (2009). Evidence that a
cultural differences in perspectives on the self (Vol. 49, pp.
1-58). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
simpático self-schema accounts for differences in the self-concepts
and social behavior of Latinos versus Whites (and Blacks).Markus,
JournalH.R., Kitayama, S., & Heiman, R. (1997). Culture and
of Personality and Social Psychology , 96 , 1012-1028. "basic" psychological principles. In E.T. Higgins &
Hong, Y., Morris, M.W., Chiù, С., & Benet-Martinez, V. (2000).A.W.
Mul-Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic
principles
ticultural minds: A dynamic constructivist approach to culture and (pp. 857-913). New York: Guilford.
cognition. American Psychologist , 55, 709-720. Markus, H.R., & Moya, P. (Eds.). (2010). Doing race: 21 essays for
Iyengar, S.S., & Lepper, M. (1999). Rethinking the value of choice: A century. New York: W.W. Norton.
the 21st
Markus,
cultural perspective on intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality H.R., Uchida, Y., Omoregie, H., Townsend, S., &
and Social Psychology , 76, 349-366. Kitayama, S. (2006). Going for the gold: Models of agency in Japa-
Kashima, Y. (2000). Conceptions of culture and person for psychol-
nese and American contexts. Psychological Science, 17, 103-1 12.
ogy. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology , 31, 14-32. Marx, K. (1973). Grundrisse: Foundations of the critique of political
economy
Kim, H.S. (2002). We talk, therefore we think? A cultural analysis of (M. Micolaus, Trans.). New York: Random House.
the effect of talking on thinking. Journal of Personality and(Original
Social work published 1857-1858).
Psychology , 83 , 828-842. Masuda, T., Ellsworth, P.C., Mesquita, В., Leu, J., Tañida, S., & Van
Kim, H., & Markus, H.R. (1999). Deviance or uniqueness, harmony or
de Veerdonk, E. (2005). Placing the face in context: Cultural dif-
conformity? A cultural analysis. Journal of Personality and Social
ferences in the perception of facial emotion. Journal of Personality
Psychology , 77, 785-800. and Social Psychology, 94, 365-381.
Kitayama, S., & Cohen, D. (2007). Handbook of cultural psychology.
Mead, G.H. (1934). Mind, self and society. Chicago: University of
New York: Guilford. Chicago Press.
Kitayama, S., Duffy, S., & Uchida, Y. (2007). Self as cultural mode Mesquita,
of В. (2001). Emotions in collectivist and individualist con-
being. In S. Kitayama & D. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of cultural texts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 68-74.
psychology (pp. 136-173). New York: Guilford Press. Mesquita, В., Barrett, L.F., & Smith, E.R. (2010). The mind in context.
Kitayama, S., Markus, H.R., Matsumoto, H., & Norasakkunkit, V. New York: Guilford Press.
(1997). Individual and collective processes in the construction of the
Miller, J.G., & Bersoff, D.M. (1998). The role of liking in perceptions
self: Self-enhancement in the United States and self-criticism in of the moral responsibility to help: A cultural perspective. Journal
Japan .Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72 , 1245-1267. of Experimental Social Psychology, 34, 443-469.
Kitayama, S., Park, H., Sevincer, A.T., Karasawa, M., & Uskul, A.K.
Morling, В., & Lamoreaux, M. (2008). Measuring culture outside the
(2009). A cultural task analysis of implicit independence: Compar- head: A meta-analysis of individualism - collectivism in cultural
ing North America, Western Europe, and East Asia. Journal of products. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 12, 199-221 .
Personality and Social Psychology , 97, 236-255. Nisbett, R.E. (1993). Violence and U.S. regional culture. American
Kitayama, S., & Park, J. (2009). The social self and the social brain: A Psychologist, 48, 441^49.
Northoff, G., Heinzel, A., de Greek, M., Bermpohl, F.,
perspective of cultural neuroscience. Manuscript submitted for
publication. Dobro wolny, H., & Panksepp, J. (2006). Self-referential process-
Kitayama, S., & Uchida, Y. (2005). Interdependent agency: An alter- ing in our brain: A meta-analysis of imaging studies on the self.
native system for action. In R.M. Sorrentino, D. Cohen, Neurolmage, 1, 440-457.
J.M. Olson, & M.P. Zanna (Eds.), Cultural and social behavior:Oishi, S., & Diener, E. (2001). Goals, culture, and subjective well-
The Ontario symposium (Vol. 10, pp. 137-164). Mahwah, NJ: being. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 1674-1682.
Erlbaum. Oyserman, D. (2008). Racial-ethnic schémas: Multidimensional iden-
Kroeber, A.L., & Kluckhohn, C.K. (1952). Culture: A critical review tity based motivation. Journal of Research in Personality, 42,
of concepts and definitions. New York: Random House. 1186-1198.

LeDoux, J.E. (1996). The emotional brain. New York: Simon & Oyserman, D., & Lee, W. (2007). Priming "culture": Culture as situ-
Schuster. ated cognition. In S. Kitayama & D. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of
Markus, H.R., & Hamedani, M.G. (2007). Sociocultural psychology: cultural psychology. New York: Guilford.
The dynamic interdependence among self-systems and social sys-Ross, M., & Sicoly, F. (1979). Egocentric biases in availability and
tems. In S. Kitayama & D. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of cultural attribution. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37,
psychology (pp. 3-46). New York: Guilford. 322-336.

This content downloaded from


103.133.254.25 on Wed, 14 Feb 2024 04:37:02 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
430 Markus and Kitayama

Schwartz, S.H. Taylor, S.E., Lerner,Universals


(1992). J.S., Sherman, D.K., Sage, R.M., & in t
ues: TheoreticalMcDowell,
advancesN.K. (2003). Are self-enhancingand
cognitions associated
emp
M.P. Zanna (Ed.),
with healthyAdvances in
or unhealthy biological profiles? Journal of Personal- exp
(Vol. 25, pp. ity and Social Psychology,
1-65). New 85, 605-615. York: Ac
Shweder, R. Tönnies, F. (1988).
(2003). Why Community and society
do (C.P. Loomis,
men Trans.). ba
chology. New York: Harper & Row.
Cambridge, MA: (Original work published
Harvard 1887).
Shweder, R.A., Triandis,
& H.C. (1989). The self and social behavior
Bourne, L. in differing
(1984).cul-
vary tural contexts. Psychological Review,
cross-culturally? In93, R.A.
506-520. Shw
ture theory : Triandis, H.C. (1995). on
Essays Individualism and collectivism. Boulder,
mind, CO:
self,
York: Westview.
Cambridge University Press.
Singelis, T.M. Tsai, J.L., Louie, J., Chen,
(1994). The E.E., & Uchida, Y. (2007). Learning what
measurem
pendent feelings to desire: Socialization of ideal
self-construals. affect through children's
Personalit
tin , 20 , storybooks. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33,
580-591.
Snibbe, A.C., & 17-30.
Markus, H.R. (2005
want: Tsai, J.L., attainment,
Educational Miao, F., & Seppala, E. (2007). Good feelings in Christian-
agenc
sonality and ity and Buddhism:
Social Religious differences in ideal affect. Personal- ,
Psychology 8
Steele, C.M., ity and Social Psychology
Spencer, Bulletin, 33, 409-421.
S.J., & Aron
group image: Wood, N., & Cowan,
The N. (1995). The cocktail party phenomenon revis-
psychology of s
threat. In M.P. Zanna (Ed.),
ited: How frequent are attention Advanc
shifts to one's name in an irrele-
chology (Vol. vant auditory
34, pp. channel? Journal of Experimental Psychology:
379-440). Sa
Suh, E.M. (2002).
Learning Culture,
Memory, and Cognition, 21, 255-260. identi
well-being. Personality and
Zhu, Y., Zhang, L., Fan, J., & Han, S. (2007). Neural Soci
basis of cultural
1378-1391. influence on self-representation. Neurolmage, 34, 1310-1316.

This content downloaded from


103.133.254.25 on Wed, 14 Feb 2024 04:37:02 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms

You might also like