Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 50

Infancy & Toddlerhood

Book Chapters Papalia (4, 5, & 6) ; Santrock (4, 5, & 6)

Initial @02/01/2024

Progress Done

Physical Development
The Birth Process

Parturition → act or process of giving birth

typically begins about 2 weeks before delivery

uterine contractions begin typically about 266 days after conception and it is
characterized by the tightening of the uterus

more frequent, rhythmic, and painful, and they increase in frequency and intensity

Braxton-Hicks contractions

false contractions during the final months of pregnancy or even as early as the
second trimester, when the uterus tightens for up to 2 minutes

relatively mild and irregular

Stages of Childbirth

Stage 1 — dilation of the cervix

this is the longest, lasting 12-14 hours for a woman having her first child

this is shorter in subsequent births

regular and increasingly frequent uterine contractions (15-20 minutes apart) cause
the cervix to shorten and dilate, or widen

at the end of this stage, contractions are every 2-5 minutes

lasts until the cervix is fully open (10 cm or 4 inches)

Stage 2 — descent and emergence of the baby

Infancy & Toddlerhood 1


lasts up to an hour or two

begins when the baby’s head begins to move through the cervix into the vaginal
canal

ends when the baby emerges completely from the mother’s body

the baby is born but is still attached to the placenta by the umbilical cord → this
must be cut and clamped

Stage 3 — expulsion of the placenta

lasts between 10 minutes and 1 hour

the placenta and the remainder of the umbilical cord are expelled from the mother

Electronic Fetal Monitoring

used to track the fetus’s heartbeat during labor

uses sensors attached to the woman’s midsection and held in place with an electric belt

continuous EFM use in a healthy, low-risk pregnancy is associated with an increased


risk for mother and baby

it has a high false-positive rate—suggesting that fetuses are in trouble when they
are not

Vaginal vs Cesarean Delivery

cesarean delivery

performed when labor progresses too slowly, when fetus is in the breech (feet or
buttocks first) or transverse (lying crosswise in the uterus), or when the mother is
bleeding vaginally

benefit:

reduced risk of urinary incontinence and pelvic organ prolapse (weakened


muscles allow one or more of the pelvic organs to drop into or press out of the
vagina)

risks:

can lead to bleeding, uterine rupture, and heightened risks of problems in


future pregnanices

Infancy & Toddlerhood 2


associated with placental abnormalities in subsequent pregnancies

deprive the baby of important benefits of normal birth

surge of hormones that clears the lungs of excess fluid, mobilization of


stored fuel to nourish cells, and the movement of blood to the heart and
brain

may put the child at risk for future obesity and asthma by negatively affecting
the stress response system

vaginal birth after cesarean (VBAC)

for women who had a previous low transverse uterine incision and who were low
risk otherwise

associated with greater risk of complications

uterine rupture, unplanned hysterectomy, and maternal death

not recommended for home births or women who have conditions that make it less
likely to be successful

advanced age

obesity

diabetes

high blood pressure

very large baby

previous cesarian that was the result of a failure of the cervix to dilate

repeat cesarian deliveries

risks:

may cause postpartum endometriosis

uterine cells are found outside of the uterus

complications related to the use of anesthesia

bladder or bowel injury

hysterectomy

Infancy & Toddlerhood 3


Medicated vs Nonmedicated Delivery

natural childbirth

seeks to prevent pain by eliminating the mother’s fear through education about the
physiology of reproduction and training in breathing and relaxation during delivery

prepared childbirth

method of childbirth that uses instruction, breathing exercises, and social support to
induce controlled physical responses to uterine contractions and reduce fear and
pain

Note: both aforementioned minimize or eliminate the use of drugs that may pose risks
for babies and enable both parents to participate in a natural, empowering experience

local (vaginal) anesthesia

also call pudendal block

given usually during the second stage of labor

analgesic

a painkiller

reduces the perception of pain by depressing the activity of the central nervous
system

may slow labor, cause maternal complications, and make the baby less alert after
birth

regional anesthesia

epidural

injected into a space in the spinal cord between the vertebrae in the lumbar (lower)
region

blocks the nerve pathways that would otherwise carry the sensation of pain to the
brain

high-dosage epidurals may slow the rate of labor, although lower doses do not
appear to be as disruptive

doula

Infancy & Toddlerhood 4


experienced mentor, coach, and helper who can furnish emotional support and
information and can stay at a woman’s bedside throughout labor

The Newborn Baby

Size and Appearance

neonatal period → first 4 weeks of life

neonate → newborn

boys tend to be slightly longer and heavier than girls

in their first few days, neonates lose as much as 10% of body weight because of loss of
fluids

they gain weight at the 5th day and generally back to birth weight by the 10th to
14th day

distinctive features:

large head and receding chin

fontanels

areas on their heads where bones of the skill do not meet

covered by a tough membrane that allows for flexibility in shape

first 18 months of life, the plates of the skull gradually fuse together

pinkish cast

skin is so thin that it barely covers the capillaries through which blood flows

during the first few days, they are very hairy because some of the lanugo (fuzzy prenatal
hair) has not yet fallen off

almost all new babies are covered with vernix caseosa (cheesy varnish), an oily
protection against infection that dries within the first few days

“witch’s milk”

secretion that sometimes leaks from the swollen breasts of newborn boys and girls
around the 3rd day of life

Infancy & Toddlerhood 5


whitish or blood-tinged vaginal discharge of some newborn girls

results from high levels of estrogen secreted by placenta just before birth and goes
away within a few days or weeks

a newborn, especially if premature, may have swollen genitals

Body Systems

upon birth, a newborn must start breathing for itself

not breathing within about 5 minutes may result to anoxia (lack of oxygen) or hypoxia
(reduced oxygen supply)

may occur as a result of repeated compression of the placenta and umbilical cord
with each contraction

can leave permanent brain damage, causing intellectual disability, behavior


problems, or even death

babies are born alert and have strong sucking reflex

they secrete meconium

stringy, greenish-black waste matter formed in the fetal intestinal tract

when bowels and bladder are full, the sphincter muscles automatically open; they do not
have full control of this yet

neonatal jaundice

skin and eyeballs look yellow

due to the immaturity of the liver and failure to filter out bilirubin → by-product of
breakdown of red blood cells

usually not serious and does not need treatment, and no long-term effects

Medical and Behavioral Assessment

Apgar Scale

Dr. Virginia Apgar

assessed one minute after delivery and 5 minutes after birth

five subtests:

Infancy & Toddlerhood 6


appearance → color

pulse → heart rate

grimace → reflex irritability

activity → muscle tone

respiration → breathing

rated 0, 1, or 2 in each measure

7-10 → good to excellent condition

below 5 to 7 → baby needs help to establish breathing

below 4 → needs immediate lifesaving treatment

scores of 0 to 3 at 10, 15, and 20 minutes after birth

associated with cerebral palsy (muscular impairment) and other neurological


problems

Brazelton Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale

used to assess neonates’ responsiveness to their environment, to identify strengths


and vulnerabilities in neurological functioning, and to predict future development

suitable for infants up to 2 months old

subscales:

motor organization → activity level

reflexes

changes in state → irritability, excitability

attention and interactive capacities → general alertness and response to visual


and auditory stimuli

indications of central nervous system instability → tremors and changes in skin


color

takes 30 minutes to administer

scores are based on a baby’s best performance

Neonatal Screening for Medical Conditions

Infancy & Toddlerhood 7


Recommended Uniform Screening Panel

35 core conditions and 26 secondary conditions for which it recommends


screening all newborns

States of Arousal

infant’s physiological and behavioral status at a given moment in the periodic daily
cycle of wakefulness, sleep, and activity

youngest babies sleep the most and wake up the most frequently

sleep alternates between quiet (regular) and active (irregular) sleep

active sleep accounts for up to 50% of a newborn’s total sleep time

Complications of Childbirth

Low Birth Weight

weight of less than 2500 grams (5 pounds) at birth

preterm (premature) infants → born before the 37th week of gestation

small-for-date infants

born at or around their due dates but are smaller than would be expected

weigh less than 90% of babies of the same gestational age

they are small most commonly because of inadequate prenatal nutrition, which
slows fetal growth

birth weight and length of gestation are the two most important predictors of an infant’s
survival and health

treatment and outcomes

feeding intravenously

administering surfactant to high-risk newborns for respiratory distress syndrome

placing in an isolette → an antiseptic, temperature-controlled crib

kangaroo care

Infancy & Toddlerhood 8


method of skin-to-skin contact in which a newborn is laid face down between
the mother’s breasts for an hour or so at a time after birth

reduce stress on the central nervous system and help with self-regulation of
sleep and activity

Postmaturity

Postmature

fetus not yet born as of 2 weeks after the due date or 42 weeks after the mother’s
last menstrual period

tend to be long and thin because they have kept growing in the womb but have had
an insufficient blood supply toward the end of gestation

possibly due to aged placenta and less efficiency, providing less oxygen

mother risks cesarian delivery, perineal tears, and postpartum hemorrhage

neonate risks should dystocia → baby’s shoulders become stuck behind the
mother’s pelvic bone during delivery

other risks: meconium aspiration, low Apgar scores, brain damage, and death

Stillbirth

sudden death of a fetus at or after the 20th week of gestation

preventable with high-quality interventions and health care

rates are highest for women aged 40 and older

Survival and Health

Sudden Infant Death Syndrome (SIDS)

crib death

sudden death of an infant under age 1

“triple risk” model

infant who is vulnerable

critical period during which an infant is at risk

Infancy & Toddlerhood 9


exogenous stressor

summary: SIDS will only occur if a vulnerable infant is exposed to a stressor


during the critical period; all factors must co-occur

environmental trigger: babies sleeping on their stomachs during the critical first year of
age

risk reduction: sleeping in parent’s room, but on a separate surface, using breathing
monitors and pacifier, and avoiding tobacco smoke

Early Physical Development

Principles of Development

cephalocaudal principle

growth from the top down

infants learn to use the upper parts of the body before the lower parts

proximodistal principle

inner to outer

babies first learn to control their arms when reaching, then use their hands in a
scooping motion, then finally learn to use their thumb and pointer finger in a pincer
grip

Physical Growth

children grow faster during the first 3 years, especially during the first few months, than
they ever will again

a 3 year old is typically slender than a chubby, potbellied 1 year old

teething

begins around 3 or 4 months

infants begin grabbing almost everything in sight to put into their mouths

first tooth may not actually arrive until sometime between 5-9 months or even later

1st birthday → 6-8 teeth

Infancy & Toddlerhood 10


2 1/2 years old → 20 teeth

Nutrition

Breastfeeding

benefits:

less likely to contract infectious diseases such as diarrhea, respiratory


infections, otitis media (infection of the middle ear), and staphylococcal,
bacterial, and urinary tract infections

have lower risk of SIDS and of postneonatal death

less likely to develop obesity, diabetes, or childhood cancer

perform better on IQ and cognitive tests

have fewer cavities

inadvisable if:

baby has been diagnosed with galactosemia → genetic metabolic disorder

mother is infected with HIV, Ebola, or any other infectious diseases

mother has been exposed to radiation

mother has been taking any drug that would not be safe for the baby

Solid Foods

babies should consume nothing but breast milk or iron-fortified formula for the first
6 months

iron-enriched solid foods must be introduced gradually during the second half of
the 1st year

water may be introduced at this time as well

children should be offered 2-3 healthy snacks a day and can be encouraged to feed
themselves and drink from a cup

Obesity

infants having a weight for height in the 95th percentile

Malnutrition

Infancy & Toddlerhood 11


caused by factors such as poverty, low-quality foods, poor dietary patterns,
contaminated water, unsanitary conditions, insufficient hygiene, inadequate
healthcare, and diarrheal diseases and other infections

protein is important for growth

Building the Brain

Brain Anatomy and Development

beginning about 3 weeks after conception, the brain gradually develops from a long,
hollow tube into a spherical mass of cells

by birth, the growth spurt of the spinal cord and brain stem has nearly run its course

cerebellum grows fastest during the first year of life

lateralization

specialization of left and right hemispheres

left hemisphere → language and logical thinking

right hemisphere → visual and spatial functions

corpus callosum

joins the two hemispheres

giant switchboard of fibers connecting the hemispheres and allowing them to share
information and coordinate commands

four lobes:

occipital → smallest; visual processing

parietal → integrating sensory information with the body; helps us move our bodies
through space and manipulate objects in our world

temporal → interpret smells and sounds and is involved with memory

frontal → involved with a variety of higher-order processes

cerebral cortex → outer surface of cerebrum

Infancy & Toddlerhood 12


regions that govern sensory information grow rapidly in the first few
months after birth and are mature by 6 months

regions that govern abstract thought, mental associations, remembering,


and deliberate motor responses grow very little during this period and
remain immature

Brain Cells

2nd month of gestation

estimated 250,000 immature neurons are produced every minute through mitosis

at birth, most of more than 100 billion neurons in a mature brain are already formed but
are not yet fully developed

most of the neurons in the cortex are in place by 20 weeks of gestation, ad the cortex’s
structure becomes fairly well-defined during the next 12 weeks

integration → neurons that control various groups of muscles coordinate their activities

differentiation → each neuron takes on a specific, specialized structure and function

cell death

begins during prenatal period and continues after birth

normal elimination of excess brain cells to achieve more efficient functioning

Myelination

begins about halfway through gestation

accelerates at 12 to 16 months and then slowing again from 2-5 years of age

at 5 years, the myelinated white matter volume in the brain is approximately 80 percent
of that found in adults

myelin development is proximodistal

sensory pathways → somatosensory, visual, and auditory pathways, are generally


myelinized before motor pathways

occipital pole (posterior end of the occipital lobe) is myelinized before the temporal
and frontal poles

Infancy & Toddlerhood 13


projection fibers (nerve tracks that connect the cortex with lower parts of the brain
and spinal cord) are myelizined before association fibers (nerve tracts that connect
cortical areas within the cerebral hemisphere)

this sequence exists because before higher cortical areas can use information, they
must be able to access stable inputs

Early Reflexes

automatic, innate response to stimulation

controlled by the lower brain centers

Primitive Reflexes

related to instinctive needs for survival and protection

Moro

stimulation: baby is dropped or hears a loud noise

behavior: extends legs, arms, and fingers, arches back, draws back head

Darwinian (grasping)

stimulation: palm of baby’s hand is stroked

behavior: makes strong fist; can be raised to standing position if both fists
are closed around a stick

Tonic Neck

stimulation: baby is laid down on back

behavior: turns head to one side, assumes “fencer” position, flexes


opposite limbs

Babinski

stimulation: sole of baby’s foot is stroked

behavior: toes fan out, foot twists in

Rooting

stimulation: baby’s cheek or lower lip is stroked with finger or nipple

behavior: head turns; mouth opens; sucking movements begin

Infancy & Toddlerhood 14


Postural Reflexes

reactions to changes in position or balance

Parachute reflex

instinctive attempt to break a fall (similar to Moro reflex)

Locomotor Reflexes

resemble voluntary movements that do not appear until months after the reflexes
have disappeared

Walking

stimulation: baby is held under arms, with bare feet touching flat surface

behavior: makes steplike motions that look like well-coordinated walking

Swimming

stimulation: baby is put into water face down

behavior: makes well-coordinated swimming movements

most early reflexes disappear during the first 6-12 months

reflexes that continue to serve protective functions remain

blinking, yawning, coughing, gagging, sneezing, shivering, and dilation of pupils in


the dark

disappearance of reflexes signal that motor pathways in the cortex have been partially
myelinated, enabling a shift to voluntary behavior

Brain Plasticity

range of modifiability of performance

molding of the brain through experience

plasticity enables learning

during the formative period of early life when the brain is most plastic, the brain is
especially vulnerable

Early Sensory Capacities

Infancy & Toddlerhood 15


Touch and Pain

embryos will respond to touch as early as 8-9 weeks of pregnancy, however these
responses do not involve any conscious awareness

by 32 weeks of gestation, all body parts are sensitive to touch, and this sensitivity
increases during the first 5 days of life

infants show a decreased pain response when they are held or cuddled, especially with
skin-to-skin contact

Smell and Taste

flavors from food the mother has consumed are found in the amniotic fluid

from this, a preference for certain tastes and smells can be developed in the utero

flavors from foods that the mother eats are also transmitted via breast milk

newborns much prefer sweet tastes to sour, bitter, or salty tastes

newborns strongly dislike bitter tastes as a survival mechanism given the toxic nature of
bitter substances

Hearing

even in the womb, fetuses respond to sound

they respond differently to familiar vs unfamiliar faces, live vs recorded maternal voice,
and native vs nonnative language

by 4 months, infants’ brains are showing lateralization for language, as occurs in adults

Sight

vision is the least developed sense at birth, perhaps because there is so little to see in the
womb

retinal structures are incomplete and optic nerve is underdeveloped

neonate’s eyes focus best from about 1 foot away

they blink at bright lights

their field of peripheral vision is very narrow

binocular vision (use of both eyes to focus) usually does not develop until 4 or 5 months

Infancy & Toddlerhood 16


infants prefer to look at and are able to discriminate between human faces more than
almost any other stimuli

infants should be examined by 6 months for visual fixation preference, ocular


alignment, and signs of eye disease

formal vision screening should begin by age 3

Motor Development

Milestones of Motor Development

systems of action → increasingly complex combinations of motor skills, which permit a


wider or more precise range of movement and more control of the movement

Denver Developmental Screening Test

used to chart progress between 1 month and 6 years and to identify children who
are not developing normally

test measures:

gross motor skills → those that use large muscles

fine motor skills → those that use small muscles

language development

personality and social development

Head Control

at birth → infants can turn their heads from side to side when lying on their backs
and when lying chest down, they can lift their head enough to turn them

2-3 months → they can lift their heads higher and higher—sometimes to the point
that they lose their balance

4 months → infants can keep their heads erect while being held or supported in a
sitting position

Hand Control

babies are born with a grasping reflex (Darwinian)

3 months → bat at objects and grasp an object of moderate size

Infancy & Toddlerhood 17


4 months → keep their hands open the majority of the time and will deliberately
hold and shake a rattle

6 months → grasp objects with one hand and transfer them to the other

7-11 months → hands become coordinated enough to pick up a tiny object →


pincer grasp

Locomotion

3 months → roll over deliberately, instead of accidentally

6 months → can sit without support

8 months → can assume a sitting position

can crawl

crawling helps babies learn to better judge distances and perceive depth

learn social referencing → looking to caregivers for clues as to whether a


situation is secure or frightening

7 months → can stand while holding onto a helping hand or a piece of furniture

11 1/2 months → can stand alone well

all these developments lead up to the major motor achievement of infancy →


walking

Motor Development and Perception

depth perception

ability to perceive objects and surfaces in three dimensions

depends on several kinds of cues:

binocular coordination

motor control

kinetic cues → produced by movement of the object or by the observer

to find out whether an object is moving, a baby might hold their head still for a
moment, an ability that is well established by about 3 months

haptic perception

Infancy & Toddlerhood 18


ability to acquire information by handling objects rather than just looking at them

babies born early as 28 weeks of gestation were bale to recognize and remember
features of objects that were placed in their hands

Theories of Motor Development

Ecological Theory of Perception

Eleanor and James Gibson

stemmed from the experiment involving a visual cliff

apparatus designed to give an illusion of depth and used to asses depth perception
in infants

in this approach, locomotor development depends on infants’ increasing sensitivity to


the interaction between their changing physical characteristics and new and varied
characteristics of the environment

instead of relying on new solutions that previously worked, with experience, babies
learn to continually gauge their abilities and adjust their movements to meet the
demands of their current environment

the process of “learning to learn” is an outcome of both perception and action

in here, the baby is somewhat like a small scientist testing out new ideas in each
situation

what worked at one time may not work now, and what worked in one environment
may not work well in another

Dynamic Systems Theory

Esther Thelen

behavior emerges in the moment from the self-organization of multiple components

infant and environment form an interconnected, dynamic system

takes into account the infant’s physical characteristics, motivation, energy level, motor
strength, and position in the environment at a particular moment in time

these all affect whether and how an infant achieves a goal

Infancy & Toddlerhood 19


ultimately, a solution emerges as the baby explores various combinations of movements
and assembles those that most efficiently contribute to that end

the maturing brain is but one component of a dynamic process

no one factor determines the pace of development, and no predetermined timetable


specifies when a particular skill will emerge

Cognitive Development
Behaviorist Approach
Classical and Operant Conditioning

Classical Conditioning

a person learns to make a reflex or involuntary response to a stimulus that


originally did not bring about the response

enables infants to anticipate an event before it happens

learning will become extinct if not reinforced by repeated association

Operant Conditioning

focuses on the consequences of behaviors and how they affect the likelihood of that
behavior occurring again

behaviors may be reinforced and become more likely to occur

behaviors may be punished and become less likely to occur

Psychometric Approach
Intelligence → enables people to acquire, remember, and use knowledge; to understand
concepts and relationships; and to solve everyday problems

intelligent behavior → presumed to be goal-oriented and adaptive

IQ tests → consist of questions or tasks that are supposed to show how much of the
measured abilities a person has by comparing that person’s performance with norms

Infancy & Toddlerhood 20


Testing Infants and Toddlers

development tests

assess infants’ behavior on tasks and compare their performance with norms
established on the basis of what large numbers of infants and toddlers can do at
particular ages

delayed → child is unable to perform a task that the “average baby” can do by a
particular age

ahead of the curve → child performs better than same-age peers

Bailey Scales of Infant and Toddler Development (Bailey-III)

developmental test designed to assess children from 1 month to 3 1/2 years

scores indicate a child’s competencies in each of five developmental areas

cognitive

language

motor

social-emotional

adaptive behavior

separate scores, called developmental quotients (DQs) are calculated for each scale

DQs are used to detect emotional disturbances and sensory, neurological, and
environmental deficits

Assessing the Early Home Environment

Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment (HOME)

trained observers interview the primary caregiver and rate on a yes-or-no checklist
the intellectual stimulation and support observed in a child’s home

six subscales:

number of books and appropriate play materials in the home

parents’ involvement with the child

parental emotional and verbal responsiveness

Infancy & Toddlerhood 21


acceptance of the child’s behavior

organization of the environment

opportunities for daily and varied stimulation

scores are significantly correlated with cognitive function, language ability, and
academic achievement

Early Intervention

systematic process of planning and providing therapeutic and educational services for
families that need help in meeting infants’, toddlers’, and pre-school children’s
developmental needs

programs involve full-day, year-round early childhood education from infancy through
the preschool years as well as family-oriented social services, early childhood
education, medical care and services, and family education on child development

participants show positive outcomes on cognitive developmental outcomes, including


reading, math scores, IQ, and school progress

Piagetian Approach
Substages of the Sensorimotor Stage

sensorimotor stage

first of four cognitive stages of development

infants learn about themselves and their world through their developing sensory
and motor activity

six substages that flow from one to another as the baby’s schemes become more
elaborate

during the first five substages → coordinate input from senses and organize
activities in relation to the environment

during the sixth substage → progress to using symbols and concepts to solve
simple problems

circular reactions

Infancy & Toddlerhood 22


an infant learns to reproduce events originally discovered by chance

repetition of behavior produces pleasure, which motivates the baby to do it yet


again

Substages

Substage 1 — Use of Reflexes

birth to about 1 month

practice their reflexes

example:

infants suck by reflex when their lips are touched

they soon learn to find the nipple even when they are not touched

infants modify and extend the scheme for sucking

they do not coordinate information from their senses

Substage 2 — Primary Circular Reaction

1-4 months

learn to purposely repeat a pleasurable bodily sensation first achieved by


chance

activities focus on the body rather than the external environment

Substage 3 — Secondary Circular Reaction

4-8 months

intentionally repeats an action not for its own sake, but to get rewarding results
beyond the infant’s own body

examples:

cooing repeated because it makes the parent smile

rattling the toy because it produces a pleasurable sound

actions are intentional but not goal-directed

Substage 4 — Coordination of Secondary Schemes

Infancy & Toddlerhood 23


8-12 months

built upon the few schemes they were born with

behavior is more intentional and purposeful, and they can anticipate events

learned to generalize from past experiences to solve new problems

they try out, modify, and coordinate previous schemes to find one that works

marks the development of complex, goal-directed behavior

Substage 5 — Tertiary Circular Reactions

12-18 months

babies begin to experiment to see what will happen

vary a behavior to see what might happen

by trial and error, toddlers try behaviors until they find the best way to attain a
goal

Substage 6 — Mental Combinations

18 months to 2 years

transition to the preoperational stage of early childhood

representational ability

capacity to store mental images or symbols of objects and events

ability to mentally represent objects and actions in memory, largely


through symbols such as words, numbers and mental pictures

it frees toddlers from immediate experience

they can thinking about actions before taking them and try out solutions in
their mind

they no longer have to go through trial and error in the real world

Object Concept

object permanence → realization that something continues to exist when out of sight

18 to 24 months

Infancy & Toddlerhood 24


babies understand that objects have independent existences and will reliably search
for hidden objects

two-step sequence of actions

methods only based on babies’ looking behavior eliminate the need for
coordination with motor activity

example: they just look for the object (one step) but not grasp them (two step)

A-not-B error

infants continue to look for an object in the place where they first found it after
seeing it hidden, even if they were later shown the object being moved to a new
location

Imitation

visible imitation

uses body parts such as hands or feet that babies can see

develops first

invisible imitation

involves parts of the body that babies cannot see (e.g. mouth)

follows after visible imitation

deferred imitation

more complex ability requiring long-term ability which children under 18 months
cannot engage in

reproduction of an observed behavior after the passage of time

requires that a stored representation of the action be recalled

children could not engage in deferred imitation because they lacked the ability to
retain mental representations

the ability to hold material in memory over a longer time span increases with age,
so does the ability to remember a sequence of steps in order

toddlers are more likely to imitate conventional actions (cuddling a teddy bear) over
unconventional actions (cuddling a photo of a teddy bear)

Infancy & Toddlerhood 25


children’s imitation depends on their goals

when trying to communicate similarly or forge social bonds → imitate other


children

when trying to learn → imitate adults

overimitation

tendency to copy any action performed by an adult, even if that action is clearly
purposeless or inefficient

Symbolic Development

pictorial competence

ability to understand the nature of pictures

example: suns are represented in books or drawings as a circle with radiating spires.
the child understands that the drawing or graphic is representative of a big ball of
light in the sky (real sun)

infants are still unable to apply real-world knowledge to a symbolic representation

15 months → use hands to explore pictures as if they were real objects — do not
understand it is a representation

19 months → able to point at a picture while saying its name — understanding that
a picture is a symbol of something else

as children’s ability to recognize and understand visual features improves, so too


does their ability to draw those same features

scale error

momentary misperception of the relative sizes of objects

example: sitting in a miniature chair that is too tiny to hold them

dual representation hypothesis

an object has two potential representations:

an object in its own right (a miniature chair)

symbol for a class of things (chairs—for sitting)

Infancy & Toddlerhood 26


it is difficult for toddlers to simultaneously mentally represent both the actual
object and the symbolic nature of what it stands for

Information-Processing Approach
Habituation

type of learning in which repeated or continuous exposure to a stimulus reduces


attention to that stimulus

researched by presenting repeatedly a stimulus and monitoring responses

example:

when presented with a new sound, a baby will stop sucking or slow down their
sucking as opposed to initially vigorous sucking

when the stimulus is presented repeatedly (it loses its novelty), the baby
generally resumes sucking vigorously

dishabituation

response to a new stimulus

example:

a new sound is presented to the baby which is different from the initial sound

the baby repeats behavior of stopping or slowing down sucking

this is dishabituation because the baby shifted their attention to another


stimulus

Tools of Infant Research

visual preference

tendency to spend more time looking at one sight rather than another

used by researchers to ask babies which of the two objects they prefer

novelty preference

infants prefer new sights to familiar ones

Infancy & Toddlerhood 27


because the novel stimulus is new and babies like new things, it is more interesting
and thus warrants a better look than the previously seen, more boring, stimulus

visual recognition memory

ability to distinguish a familiar visual stimulus from an unfamiliar one when show
both at the same time

depends on the capacity to form and refer to mental representation

Perceptual and Attentional Processes

generally assessed by how quickly infants habituate to a new stimulus and how well
they remember previously encountered stimuli

joint attention

shared attentional focus, typically initiated with eye gaze or pointing

when babies follow an adults’ gaze by looking or pointing in the same direction

Cross-Modal Transfer

ability to use information gained from one sense to guide another

ex. navigating through the dark by feeling the location of familiar objects

if the objects used as stimulus were either smooth or nubby (differently textured
objects), the infants could transfer vision to touch and touch to vision equally well

Information Processing and Piagetian Abilities

Categorization

newborns distinguish between closed and open shapes but are unable to categorize
different types of closed shapes until about 3-4 months

example:

babies can distinguish a triangle from a cross, but cannot distinguish and
isosceles triangle to an equilateral one

at 3-4 months, they can distinguish between a dog and a cat or between a chair and
a bed (specific categorizations within broad ones) by looking longer at items in a
new category

Infancy & Toddlerhood 28


infants at first seem to categorize on the basis of perceptual features, such as shape,
color, and pattern

12-14 months → categories become conceptual, based on real-world knowledge,


particularly of function

example: they recognize that chairs with zebra-print are furniture—not animals

Causality

principle that one event causes another

4-6 months → they begin to recognize that they can act on their environment but
they did not yet know that causes must come before effects and that forces outside
of themselves can make things happen

8 months → infants make causal attribution for simple events even when they
cannot see the actual moment of contact between the two objects

Object Permanence

violation-of-expectations

begins with a familiarization phase in which infants see an event happen


normally

after habituation, the event is changed in a way that conflicts with normal
expectations

the baby tends to look longer at the changed event, meaning additional interest

Cognitive Neuroscience Approach


examines the hardware of the central nervous system to identify what brain structures are
involved in specific areas of cognition

implicit memory

remembering that occurs without effort or even conscious awareness

pertains to habits and skills

more automatic and early appearing, little affected by culture

Infancy & Toddlerhood 29


explicit memory

declarative memory

conscious or intentional recollection, usually of facts, names, events, or other things that
can be stated or declared

involves a conscious and deliberative process and thus is subject to cultural influences

working memory

short-term storage of information the brain is actively processing or working on

appears relatively late in development and may be responsible for the slow development
of object permanence

Social-Contextual Approach
influenced by Vygotsky’s sociocultural theory

interested in how cultural context affects early social interactions

guided participation

mutual interactions with adults that help structure children’s activities and bridge the
gap between a child’s understanding and an adult’s

occurs in shared play and in ordinary, everyday activities in which children informally
learn the skills, knowledge, and values important to their culture

Language Development
language

communication system based on words and grammar

Language Milestones

Birth → perceive speech, cry, make some response to sound

2 months → makes sounds other than crying

4 months → responds to sound, coos, and turns head towards others’ voices

Infancy & Toddlerhood 30


6 months → blows raspberries and makes squealing noises

9 months → babbling

12 months → calls parents “mama” or “papa” or other special names; understands “no”

15 months → uses 1-2 words other than mama or papa, follows directions with gesture
and words, looks at familiar objects, points to ask for help

18 months → follows directions without gestures

24 months → says at least 2 words together

30 months → says around 50 words; says at least 2 words with one action word

36 months → talks in conversations with at least 2 back-and-forth exchanges

Nature vs Nurture

Skinner

language learning is based on experience and learned associations through operant


conditioning

babies utter sounds at random which caregivers reinforce when they make sounds
that resemble adult speech

infants repeat reinforced sounds and language is gradually shaped

Social Learning Theory

babies imitate the sounds they hear adults make and are reinforced for doing so

Naom Chomsky

nativism

human brain has an innate capacity for acquiring language; babies tend to talks as
naturally as they learn to walk

language acquisition device (LAD)

inborn mechanism that programs children’s brains to analyze the language they
hear and to figure out its rules

almost all children master their native language in the same age-related sequence
without formal teaching

Infancy & Toddlerhood 31


does not tell us why some children acquire language more rapidly and efficiently
than others, why children differ in linguistic skill and fluency, or why speech
development appears to depend on having someone to talk with, not merely on
hearing spoken language

language depends on an intertwining of nature and nurture

children have an inborn capacity to acquire language, which may be activated or


constrained by experience

Sequence of Early Language Development

prelinguistic speech

forerunner of linguistic speech

utterance of sounds that are not words

includes crying, cooing, babbling, and accidental and deliberate imitation of sounds
without understanding their meaning

Early Vocalization

crying

a newborn’s first means of communication

cooing

starts between 6 weeks and 3 months

squealing, gurgling, and making vowel sounds

babbling

repeating consonant-vowel strings

between ages 6 and 10 months

often mistaken for a baby’s first word

it is language-general until about 9 months

although initially nonsensical, becomes more wordlike over time

Perceiving Language Sounds and Structure

Infancy & Toddlerhood 32


infants’ brains seem to be preset to discriminate basic linguistic units, perceive
linguistic patterns, and categorize them as similar or different

6-7 months → learned to recognize the phonemes used in their native language

8 months → lose sensitivity to phonemes not used in their native language

end of 1st year → lose sensitivity to sounds that are not part of the language or
languages they usually hear spoken

hypotheses:

infants mentally compute the relative frequency of particular phonetic


sequences in their language and learn to ignore sequences they infrequently
hear

early language experience modifies neural structures, facilitating detection of


word patterns in the native language while suppressing attention to nonnative
patterns that would slow native language learning

between 6-12 months → begin to become aware of the phonological rules of their
language

Gestures

by using gestures, babies show an understanding that symbols refer to specific


objects, events, desires, and conditions

12 months → learned some conventional social gestures

waving bye bye and shaking head for “no”

13 months → use more elaborate representational gestures

holding out empty cup to indicate thirst

symbolic gestures (blowing for “hot”) often emerge around the same time that
babies say their first words

gesture-word combinations serve as a signal that a child is about to begin using


multiword sentences

First Words

linguistic speech

Infancy & Toddlerhood 33


verbal expression that conveys meaning

average baby says their first work between 10 and 14 months

13 months → understand that a word stands for a specific thing or event, and they
can quickly learn the meaning of a new word

may use a simple syllable to mean more than one thing depending on the
context

entire sentence expressed with one word → holophrase

10 months to 2 years → shift from simple associations to following social cues

10 months → assume a new word they hear refers to whatever object they find
interesting, whether or not it is correct

12 months → pay attention to cues from adults; still learning words only for
interesting objects

18-24 months → follow social cues in learning words, regardless of intrinsic


interest

24 months → quickly recognize the names of familiar objects in the absence of


visual cues

receptive vocabulary

what infants understand

continues to grow as verbal comprehension gradually becomes faster and more


accurate and efficient

expressive vocabulary → spoken

nouns are the easiest type of word to learn while verbs are more difficult

First Sentences

next important linguistic breakthrough → toddler puts two words together to


express one idea

happens between 18-24 months

first sentences typically deal with everyday events, things, people, or activities

telegraphic speech → consists of only a few essential words to form sentences

Infancy & Toddlerhood 34


children show implicit understanding of the fundamental rules for putting sentences
together → syntax

20-30 months → children show increasing competence in syntax

become increasingly aware of the communicative purpose of speech and of


whether their words are being understood

age 3 → speech is fluent, longer, and more complex

code mixing

in bilingual children

using elements of both languages in the same utterance

code switching

ability to shift from one language to another

overregularization

occurs when children inappropriately apply a syntactical rule

“Daddy goed to the store” “I drawed that”

underextension

use words in too narrow a category

restricting the word “doggy” to one’s own pet

overextension

using words in too broad of a category

calling a stray “Cholo” (as in the child’s pet’s name) because his dog is black
and an aspen, all dogs of the same nature can be called “Cholo”

Influences of Early Speech

Brain Development

brain activation in left temporal and parietal lobes in toddlers with large
vocabularies

brain activation is scattered for those with small vocabularies

Social Interaction and the Linguistic Environment

Infancy & Toddlerhood 35


interaction with a live communicative partner

Child-Directed Speech

a.k.a. parentese, motherese, or baby talk

form of speech often used in talking to babies or toddlers

includes slow, simplified speech, a high-pitched tone, exaggerated vowel


sounds, and much repetition

Psychosocial Development
Early Emotional Development

Emotions → subjective reactions to experience that are associated with physiological and
behavioral changes

Crying

primary way in which infants communicate their needs and is considered to be an


honest signal of need

adults find the sound of crying unpleasant and are wired to respond with caregiving
behaviors

crying is related to their physiological state

a higher pitch and a more monotonic vocalization is associated with autonomic system
activity during stressful procedures in infants

cries of preterm infants who generally have higher needs

as children age, they begin to realize that crying serves a communicative function

5 months → they monitor caregivers’ expressions

if ignored will first cry harder and then stop crying if still unsuccessful in getting
attention

Smiling and Laughing

social smiling → when newborn infants gaze and smile at their parents begins in the
2nd month

Infancy & Toddlerhood 36


12 weeks → smile at others more or less frequently depending on the responses of
adults around them

laughter

smile-linked vocalization

becomes more common in 4-12 months

it may signify the most intense positive emotion

clowning

silly, nonverbal behaviors that parents do to elicit laughs from young children

anticipatory smiling

rises between 8-10 months

infants smile at an object and then gaze at an adult while continuing to smile

among the first types of communication in which the infant refers to an object or
experience

Self-Conscious Emotions

self-awareness

cognitive understanding that they have a recognizable identity, separate and


different from the rest of their world

gives rise to self-conscious emotions → embarrassment, empathy, and envy

emerge between 15-24 months

necessary before children can be aware of being the focus of attention, identify with
what other “selves” are feeling, or wish they had what someone else has

age 3 → demonstrating self-evaluative emotions → pride, guilt, and shame

Altruistic Helping and Empathy

altruistic behavior → acting out of concern with no expectation of reward

1, 3, 6, and 9 months → respond to the cries of other infants with cries of their own and
facial expressions of distress

Infancy & Toddlerhood 37


6 and 12 months → respond to other infants’ expressions of anger and distress with
pupillary dilation—sign consistent with emotional arousal

12 months → spontaneously will help an adult reach for or find a toy that has fallen out
of reach

15 months → expectations about fairness as illustrated by their tendency to stare longer


at an unfair distribution of goods than an equal distribution

2 years → likely to help others, share belongings and food, and offer comfort at the
distress of others

reflect empathy → ability to imagine how another person might feel in a particular
situation

mirror neurons

may underlie empathy and altruism

these fire when a person does something but also when they observe someone else
doing the same thing

Collaborative Activities and Cultural Transmission

collaborative activities increase during the 2nd year of life as toddlers become more
adept at communication

they have the ability and motivation to engage in socially coordinated actions with
shared goals

overimitation

out universal propensity to overimitate accounts for our impressive creation of


cultural artifacts and institutions

Temperament

early-appearing, biologically based tendency to respond to the environment in predictable


ways

Temperament Patterns and Development

types:

easy children → generally happy, rhythmic in biological functioning, and accepting


of new experiences

Infancy & Toddlerhood 38


difficult children → more irritable and harder to please, irregular in biological
rhythms, wary of new experiences, and more intense in expressing emotion

slow to warm up children → mild but slow to adapt to new people and situations

stable in temperament from 2-13 months of age is quite high

temperament is relatively stable individual differences, perhaps because it is largely


inborn and strongly influenced by genetics

does not mean that temperament is fully formed at birth

develops as various emotions and self-regulatory capacities appear, and it can change in
response to parental treatment and other life experiences

Goodness of Fit

match between temperament and the environmental demands and constraints the child
must deal with

if temperament does not match the environment, tensions may occur

easy child is expected to sit still for long periods

difficult child constantly taken away from absorbing projects

slow to warm up children constantly pushed into new situations

Behavioral Inhibition

how boldly or cautiously a child approaches unfamiliar objects and situations

most clearly seen when babies are presented with novel stimuli

high → physiologically aroused and may even be overaroused that would be


unpleasant for them

they have an unusually excitable amygdala

low → relaxed; show little distress or motor activity; often calmly stare at new
stimuli, sometimes smiling at it

behaviorally inhibited children are more likely to outgrow their inhibition if parents do
not completely shield them from new situations and instead provide gentle support and
encouragement during anxiety-provoking situations

Infancy & Toddlerhood 39


Gender

what it means to be male or female

Sex Differences in Infants and Toddlers

Physical

boys are a bit longer and heavier and may be slightly stronger and more active, but
are physically more vulnerable from conception on

girls are less reactive to stress and more likely survive infancy

boys’ brains at birth are about 10% larger than girls’

Empathy

girls are more likely than boys to cry, and cry longer when they hear another baby
crying

girls show more concern and are more likely to respond prosocially to the distress
of others

Aggression

illustrated within the context of play

boys are more likely to express anger than girls

boys as young as 17 months tend to play more actively and aggressively than girls

girls play with girls and boys play with boys, not because they necessarily want to
to but because most children enjoy playing with someone who plays like they do

Gender-typed toy preferences

emerge at 3 months before infants could have formed an understanding of


masculine or feminine conceptual categories

testosterone levels in infancy predict later gender-typed toy preferences

socialization influences such as family, peers, and media can have a strong effect on
children’s toy preferences

Visual information

boys show an advantage for spatial information

Infancy & Toddlerhood 40


boys are better at recognizing mirror image of a previously presented stimulus

boys are more likely to be able to predict the path of an object hidden behind a
screen

boys are better at using the geometry of a room as a cue to orient themselves when
other information is not available

Parental Influences on Gender Differences

gender-typing → process by which children learn behavior their culture considers


appropriate for each sex

delivered implicitly through the ways in which parents interact with baby boys and
girls

gendered differences in parental treatment

girls are generally allowed a bit more latitude in the expression of negative
emotions

fathers treat boys and girls more differently than mothers do

fathers talk more and spend more time with sons than with daughters

fathers, overall, play with their children more than mothers do and play more
roughly with sons and show more sensitivity to daughters

mothers talk more, and more supportively, to daughters than to sons

Developmental Issues in Infancy

Developing Trust

Basic Trust vs Mistrust

Erik Erikson

successful → develop a sense of reliability of people and objects in our world;


feeling safe and loved

risk → develop sense of mistrust and feel that those around us cannot be counted
on in times of need

begins in infancy and continues until about 18 months

Infancy & Toddlerhood 41


babies develop a balance between trust and mistrust

trust predominates → develop hope → belief that they can fulfill their needs
and obtain their desires

too much trust → maladaptation of sensory distortion (being gullible)

mistrust predominates → view the world as unfriendly and unpredictable and


have trouble forming quality relationships

too much mistrust → malignancy of withdrawal

Developing Attachments

attachment → reciprocal, enduring emotional tie between an infant and a caregiver,


each of whom contributes to the quality of the relationship

Harry Barlow

rhesus monkey experiment where there were two “surrogate” mothers:

plain, cylindrical wire mesh → offered food

terry-cloth → offered nothing but a cuddly figure to cling to

baby monkeys spent majority of their time clinging to the terry-cloth mother

conclusion:

feeding is not the only, or even the most important, thing babies get from their
mothers

mothering includes the comfort of close bodily contact and the satisfaction of
an innate need to cling

John Bowlby

attachment styles are the result of repeated interactions with a caregiver

every time a baby cries, the mother responds quickly and sensitively to that bid of
comfort → over time the baby comes to expect it

if a mother responds inconsistently to crying, babies form a different set of


expectations

set of expectations → working models

Infancy & Toddlerhood 42


because the working model emerges as a result of interactions between both
partners in the relationship, babies can have different working models (and
attachment styles) with different people

Stages of Attachment

Asocial or Pre-attachment Stage

Birth to 3 months

stage that baby is forming bonds, but relationship with objects and humans
are similar

Indiscriminate Attachment

6 weeks to 7 months

babies display more observable social behavior

preference for people over objects

prefer familiar adults, however do not show stranger or separation anxiety

Specific or Discriminate Attachment

7-11 months

stage where majority of babies display stranger anxiety and separation


anxiety from one particular adult

Multiple Attachments

24 months above

attachment to one adult usually extend to multiple attachments with other


adults who they regularly spend time with

Stranger and Separation Anxiety

John Bowlby believed this was an adaptive process

stranger anxiety → wariness of a person they do not know

separation anxiety → distress when a familiar caregiver leaves them

Studying Patterns of Attachment

Strange Situation

Infancy & Toddlerhood 43


Mary Ainsworth

laboratory-based technique designed to assess attachment patterns between an


infant and an adult

consists of three sequence of episodes and takes less than half an hour

episodes are designed to trigger the emergence of attachment-related behaviors

mother twice leaves the baby in an unfamiliar room, the first time with a
stranger

second time, mother leaves and the stranger comes back before the mother
does

mother then encourages the baby to explore and play again and gives comfort
if the baby seems to need it

what the baby does during the caregiver’s absence is not diagnostic of attachment
categorization, but what the babies do when the caregiver returns

secure attachment

flexible and resilient in the face of stress

sometimes cry when caregiver leaves, but quickly obtain comfort once they return

some are comfortable being with a stranger for a short period of time

they clearly indicate they prefer the caregiver to the stranger in the reunion
episode, often smiling at, greeting, or approaching the caregiver

avoidant attachment

outwardly unaffected by a caregiver leaving or returning

generally continue to play in the room and frequently interact with the stranger

upon return of the caregiver, they ignore or reject them, sometimes deliberately
turning away

tend to show little emotion, either positive or negative

ambivalent (resistant) attachment

generally anxious even before the caregiver leaves, sometimes approaching the
caregiver for comfort when the stranger looks at or approaches them for interaction

Infancy & Toddlerhood 44


extremely reactive to the caregiver’s departure from the room and generally
become very upset

upon return, they tend to remain upset for long periods of time

they show a mix of proximity-seeking and angry behaviors and are very difficult to
settle

disorganized-disoriented attachment

Main & Solomon

seem to lack a cohesive strategy to deal with the stress of the Strange Situation

show contradictory, repetitive, or misdirected behavior

seeking closeness to the stranger instead of the mother or showing a fear


response upon the caregiver’s entry

they seem confused and afraid

occur in babies whose mothers are insensitive, intrusive, or abusive

who are fearful

who have suffered unresolved loss or have unresolved feelings about their
childhood attachment to their own parents

more common in infants who have undergone major or repeated separations form
primary caregivers

Mutual Regulation

ability of both infant and caregiver to respond appropriately and sensitively to each other’s
mental and emotional states

they have high interactional synchrony

both unconsciously coordinate their behavior and affect in a rhythmic back and forth
manner, responding appropriately and effectively to each other’s signals in an
interactive dance

infants take an active part in this by sending behavioral signals that influence the way
caregivers behave toward them

Infancy & Toddlerhood 45


in 2-9 months, it is measured using still-face paradigm

mother interacts in a normal fashion in order to establish a baseline of behaviors

in a still-face episode, the mother suddenly becomes stone-faced, silent, and


unresponsive, avoiding eye contact

the baby attempts to draw the mother’s attention and express more negative affect

in essence, they become dysregulated

when interaction is highly synchronous, the baby tends to be joyful or interested

when a mother or caregiver is not synchronous in her interaction with the baby, the baby
becomes stressed or physiologically aroused

from this process, babies learn how to send signals and what to do when their signals
are not effective

Social Referencing

children look at their caregivers on encountering an ambiguous event

seeking emotional information to guide behavior

one person forms an understanding of how to act in an ambiguous, confusing, or unfamiliar


situation by seeking and interpreting another person’s perception of it

as children age, their use of social referencing becomes more sophisticated, and they become
less dependent on facial expression

12 months → toddlers can use either a facial expression or vocal tone as a cue to the
safety of an action but respond more quickly to voice

while younger infant tend to check in with adults regardless of what type of stimulus they
encounter, older infants tend to check in only when a stimulus or situation is ambiguous

Developmental Issues in Toddlerhood

Emerging Sense of Self

self-concept → sense of self; describes what we know and feel about ourselves and
guides our actions

Infancy & Toddlerhood 46


3 months → infants pay more attention to their mirror image than to images of
others

4 months → begin to show more interest in images of others than themselves

recognition of own vs other-race faces is not just a perceptual process but is linked
with emotional content and gradually refined over time

the emergence of self-awareness builds on the dawning of perceptual distinction


between self and others

a sign of self-recognition is the use of first-person pronouns, such as me and mine,


usually at 20-24 months

between 19-30 months, children begin to apply descriptive terms (big, little) and
evaluative terms (good, naughty) to themselves

they demonstrate self-understanding through acknowledging objects that belong to them


and those that belong to others

Development of Autonomy

autonomy vs shame and doubt

18 months to 3 years

shift from external control to self-control

virtue → will

malignancy (too much shame and doubt) → impulsivity

maladaptation (too much autonomy) → compulsivity

toilet training is an important step toward autonomy and self-control

toddlers need adults to set appropriate limits, and shame and doubt help them
recognize the need for those limits

Moral Development and Socialization

socialization → process by which children develop habits, skills, values, and motives
that make them responsible, productive members of society

compliance with parental expectations can be seen as a first step toward compliance
with societal standards

Infancy & Toddlerhood 47


socialization rests on internalization of these standards

children no longer obey rules or commands just to get rewards and avoid
punishment, but because they believe them to be right and true

Developing Self-Regulation

self-regulation → control of behavior to conform to a caregiver’s demands or


expectations, even when the caregiver is not present

it is the foundation of socialization

cognitive awareness is not enough; it also requires emotional control and


understanding as children continually absorb information about what conduct their
parents approve of

before then can control their own behavior, children need to be able to regulate
their attentional processes and to modulate negative emotions

attentional regulation enables children to develop willpower and cope with


frustration

Developing a Conscience

conscience → eventual goal of socialization is for the development of a conscience

involves both the ability to refrain from certain acts as well as to feel emotional
discomfort for failing to do so

situational compliance → children need the extra assistance provided by their


parents’ reminder and prompts to complete the task

committed compliance → children are committed to following requests and could


do so without their parents’ direct intervention

tends to increase with age, whereas situational compliance decreases

receptive cooperation → child’s eager willingness to cooperate harmoniously with


a parent, not only in disciplinary situations but also in a variety of daily
interactions, including routines, chores, hygiene, and play

constructive conflict (one that involves negotiation, reasoning, and resolution) over
a child’s misbehavior can help children develop a conscience by enabling them to
see another point of view

Infancy & Toddlerhood 48


Relationships with Other Children

Siblings

earliest, most frequent, and most intense disputes among siblings are over property
rights or access to the mother

despite the frequency of conflict, prosocial and play-oriented behaviors are more
common than rivalry, hostility, and competition

because older siblings tend to dominate younger ones, the quality of the relationship is
more affected by the emotional and social adjustment of the older child than the
younger one

quality of sibling relationships tends to carry over to relationships with other children

siblings are generally a secondary attachment and babies prefer the attention of a
primary caregiver when in distress

Peers

preschoolers usually like to play with children of the same age, sex, and gender

they also prefer prosocial playmates who can provide them with positive experiences
and who are advanced in theory of mind (knowledge that others’ beliefs, desires,
intentions, emotions, and thoughts may be different from one’s own)

they reject disruptive, demanding, intrusive, or aggressive children

Child Maltreatment

forms:

physical abuse → injury to the body through punching, beating, kicking, or burning

neglect → failure to meet a child’s basic needs, such as food, clothing, medical care,
protection, and supervision

sexual abuse → any sexual activity involving a child and an older person

emotional maltreatment → including rejection, terrorization, isolation, exploitation,


degradation, ridicule, or failure to provide emotional support, love, and affection

Infancy & Toddlerhood 49


sex trafficking → recruitment, harboring, transportation, provision, or obtaining of a
person for the purpose of a commercial sex act

Maltreatment in Infancy and Toddlerhood

nonorganic failure to thrive

experienced by babies who do not receive nurturance and affection or who are
neglected

slowed or arrested physical growth with no known medical cause, accompanied by


poor developmental and emotional functioning

symptoms:

lack of appropriate weight gain

irritability

excessive sleepiness and fatigue

avoidance of eye contact

lack of smiling or vocalizing

delayed motor development

can result from a combination of inadequate nutrition, difficulties in breastfeeding,


improper formula preparation or feeding techniques, and disturbed interactions with
parents

shaken baby syndrome

found mainly in children under 2 years old, most often in infants

because the baby has weak neck muscles and a large, heavy head, shaking makes
the brain bounce back and forth inside the skull

this causes bruising, bleeding, and swelling and can lead to permanent and severe
brain damage, paralysis, and even death

damage is typically worse if the baby is thrown into bed or against a wall

Infancy & Toddlerhood 50

You might also like