Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

Clinical Psychology Review 65 (2018) 163–174

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Clinical Psychology Review


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/clinpsychrev

Review

Mirror exposure therapy for body image disturbances and eating disorders: T
A review

Trevor C. Griffena, , Eva Naumannb, Tom Hildebrandtb
a
Department of Psychiatry, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, NY, USA
b
Eating and Weight Disorders Program, Department of Psychiatry, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, NY, USA

H I GH L IG H T S

• Body dissatisfaction is common and difficult to treat.


• Mirror exposure therapy is an effective treatment for body dissatisfaction.
• Mirror exposure has been used in eating disorders, BDD and non-clinical populations.
• Several styles of mirror exposure therapy are effective.

A R T I C LE I N FO A B S T R A C T

Keywords: Mirror exposure therapy is a clinical trial validated treatment component that improves body image and body
Body image satisfaction. Mirror exposure therapy has been shown to benefit individuals with high body dissatisfaction and
Mirror exposure therapy patients with eating disorders (ED) in clinical trials. Mirror exposure is an optional component of cognitive
Eating disorders behavioral therapy (CBT), an effective treatment for body dysmorphic disorder (BDD). However, most clinical
Body dysmorphic disorder
trials of mirror exposure therapy have been small or uncontrolled and have included few male subjects. Adverse
events have been reported during mirror exposure clinical trials. We discuss how individuals respond when
looking in a mirror and how mirrors can be used therapeutically, and we critically evaluate the evidence in favor
of mirror exposure therapy. We discuss clinical indications and technical considerations for the use of mirror
exposure therapy.

1. Introduction feature, share common pathological elements and have increased co-
morbidity with obsessive-compulsive disorder (American Psychiatric
The self-perception of and emotional valence attached to one's body Association [APA], 2013; Phillips & Kaye, 2007). Severity of body
affects many important aspects of life. Negative body image is asso- image disturbance correlates with ED symptom persistence, suggesting
ciated with low self-esteem, disordered eating, negative sexual experi- that specifically targeting body image disturbances could promote re-
ences, depression and anxiety, and is a risk factor for the development covery from ED (Stice & Shaw, 2002). Although several interventions
of ED (Cash & Szymanski, 1995; Davison & McCabe, 2005; Faith & have been designed to target body image dissatisfaction, a meta-ana-
Schare, 1993; Johnson & Wardle, 2005; Koch, Mansfield, Thurau, & lysis of randomized controlled trials of body image interventions, in-
Carey, 2005; Noles, Cash, & Winstead, 1985; Stice & Shaw, 2002). Body cluding fitness training, self-esteem enhancement, media literacy and
image influences the emotional responses people have while viewing psychoeducation, found only a small effect size for improving body
themselves in a mirror (Servián-Franco, Moreno-Domínguez, & Reyes image across these interventions (Alleva, Sheeran, Webb, Martijn, &
Del Paso, 2015; Svaldi, Zimmermann, & Naumann, 2012). Many in- Miles, 2015).
dividuals with ED and BDD have a problematic relationship with mir- Mirror exposure therapy, the systematic, repetitive viewing of
rors, often alternating between excessive mirror checking and mirror oneself in a mirror with specific guidance, has been proposed as a
avoidance (Beilharz, Castle, Grace, & Rossell, 2017; Grant & Phillips, treatment for body image dissatisfaction (Hilbert, Tuschen-Caffier, &
2005). Vögele, 2002; Rosen, Reiter, & Orosan, 1995). Exposure therapies have
Both ED and BDD include body image disturbance as a core clinical been found to be broadly effective, including for the treatment of


Corresponding author at: Department of Psychiatry, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, 1 Gustave L Levy Place, New York, NY 10029, USA.
E-mail address: trevor.griffen@mountsinai.org (T.C. Griffen).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2018.08.006
Received 8 February 2018; Received in revised form 25 August 2018; Accepted 27 August 2018
Available online 29 August 2018
0272-7358/ © 2018 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
T.C. Griffen et al. Clinical Psychology Review 65 (2018) 163–174

specific psychiatric illnesses such as obsessive-compulsive disorder (Foa amount of time looking at their self-reported most unattractive body
& McLean, 2016). Thus, mirror exposure therapy targeting body dis- parts after induction of negative mood, but are no different from peers
satisfaction is a rationally designed psychotherapeutic intervention. without ED after induction of positive mood (Svaldi et al., 2016). To-
Here, we examine where people direct their gaze during mirror gether, these findings suggest that individuals with ED spend more time
exposures, discuss the acute effects of mirror exposure on both clinical looking at their negative features than healthy individuals when
and non-clinical populations, review both controlled and uncontrolled looking in the mirror. Positive mood may be protective against this bias
trials of mirror exposure as therapy with attention to specific patholo- (Svaldi et al., 2016). When those with ED or who are overweight view
gies, explore technical differences in the implementation of mirror ex- pictures of themselves, they also tend to have bias towards their self-
posure, consider potential risks of and contraindications to mirror ex- reported more unattractive body parts (Jansen, Nederkoorn, &
posure therapy and attempt to develop a theoretical understanding of Mulkens, 2005; Kollei et al., 2017; Svaldi, Caffier, & Tuschen-Caffier,
the mechanisms of action of mirror exposure therapy. We also present 2011) although this result has not been consistent across all studies
novel hypotheses of pathologies that may be amenable to treatment (von Wietersheim et al., 2012; Warschburger, Calvano, Richter, &
with mirror exposure therapy and call for large scale, randomized Engbert, 2015).
controlled trials of mirror exposure therapy to more clearly elucidate
the risks, benefits and optimal techniques across different pathological 3. Responses to looking in the mirror
states.
Mirrors are ubiquitous in contemporary society; they are nearly
2. The locus of focus during mirror gazing impossible to avoid. Those with body image disturbances look at
themselves in the mirror differently than those with higher body sa-
2.1. Non-clinical populations tisfaction (Svaldi et al., 2016; Tuschen-Caffier et al., 2015). Mirrors
provide a source of repeated and possibly distressing exposure to and
Where does one look when presented with one's reflection in a reminder of one's self-perceived best features and worst flaws.
mirror? Excessive focus on an area that is perceived as flawed may
reinforce negative cognitions while focus on areas that are perceived 3.1. Non-clinical populations
positively may serve to improve self-esteem. Women without a history
of ED have been found to dedicate nearly equivalent amounts of time to Body dissatisfaction and distress increase in both men and women
looking at body parts that they identify as their most attractive and as after they briefly look at themselves in a mirror (Veale et al., 2016;
their least attractive when looking in a mirror (Tuschen-Caffier et al., Walker, Murray, Lavender, & Anderson, 2012; Windheim, Veale, &
2015). The even split between positively and negatively perceived body Anson, 2011). However, in mirror exposure tasks lasting longer than
parts is unmodified by mood as adolescent females without ED spend 30 min in which women are instructed to view body parts in a top down
nearly the same amount of time looking at their self-identified most and fashion, negative emotions have been found to remain unchanged from
least attractive body parts in a mirror after the induction of either po- baseline after the task (Shafran, Lee, Payne, & Fairburn, 2007; Vocks,
sitive or negative mood (Svaldi et al., 2016). Although not directly Legenbauer, Wächter, Wucherer, & Kosfelder, 2007) and a transient
tested during mirror gazing, women with body dissatisfaction recruited decrease in feelings of fatness occurs (Shafran et al., 2007). When
from non-clinical populations spend relatively more time looking at the women are directed to look at, touch and describe emotions related to
body parts that they feel most dissatisfied with compared to those they the body parts with which they are most dissatisfied while looking in a
are most satisfied with when looking at pictures of themselves mirror, body dissatisfaction acutely increases but then decreases to
(Glashouwer, Jonker, Thomassen, & de Jong, 2016; Janelle, below baseline 30 min later (Shafran et al., 2007). Thus, the length of a
Hausenblas, Ellis, Coombes, & Duley, 2009; Roefs et al., 2008). mirror viewing session and presence of specific instructions influences
the emotional response to viewing oneself in a mirror.
2.2. Body dysmorphic disorder Baseline body dissatisfaction also affects emotional responses to
mirror exposure. Females without ED or obesity but with high body
Most individuals with BDD report spending excessive amounts of dissatisfaction experience more negative emotions during brief mirror
time examining their perceived physical defects in the mirror (Phillips, exposure than those low in body dissatisfaction (Servián-Franco et al.,
Hollander, Rasmussen, & Aronowitz, 1997; Phillips, Menard, Fay, & 2015). Interestingly, females with high body dissatisfaction experience
Weisberg, 2005). When shown pictures of themselves, those with BDD increased body dissatisfaction after a negative but not after a positive
tend to bias their attention towards the areas that they are most dis- manipulation of self-esteem prior to the mirror exposure session (Svaldi
satisfied with (Greenberg, Reuman, Hartmann, Kasarskis, & Wilhelm, et al., 2012). Mirror exposure does not alter body satisfaction for those
2014; Grocholewski, Kliem, & Heinrichs, 2012; Kollei, Horndasch, with low baseline body dissatisfaction (Svaldi et al., 2012).
Erim, & Martin, 2017); however, when scanning is analyzed on a case- In summary, in non-clinical populations mirror exposure leads to
by-case basis, some individuals show a strong avoidance of the features distress and worsening of body dissatisfaction (Veale et al., 2016;
with which they are preoccupied (Toh, Castle, & Rossell, 2017). Thus, Walker et al., 2012). This population effect is likely driven by reactions
individuals with BDD may comprise a heterogeneous population, some from individuals with high baseline body dissatisfaction (Servián-
with enhanced visual attention to their perceived defects and some Franco et al., 2015; Svaldi et al., 2012). Manipulating the emotional
engaging in repetitive mirror gazing but avoiding their perceived de- content of a mirror exposure session, either through instruction in
fects. These results have not yet been replicated using eye tracking where to look or prior to exposure, may influence changes in mood and
while individuals with BDD look at themselves in a mirror. body satisfaction as well as their persistence (Shafran et al., 2007;
Svaldi et al., 2012; Vocks et al., 2007).
2.3. Eating and weight disorders
3.2. Body dysmorphic disorder
Similar to body dissatisfied women without ED, women with an-
orexia nervosa (AN) and bulimia nervosa (BN) spend more time looking Approximately 90% of individuals with BDD report spending ex-
at the body parts that they identify as their most ugly compared to those cessive amounts of time looking at themselves in the mirror (Phillips
they identify as their most beautiful (Tuschen-Caffier et al., 2015). et al., 1997; Phillips et al., 2005) and nearly 10% report having had a
Mood may interact with ED to influence where individuals look in the panic attack triggered by looking at themselves in the mirror (Phillips,
mirror as adolescent females with AN spend a significantly greater Menard, & Bjornsson, 2013). After looking in the mirror, individuals

164
T.C. Griffen et al. Clinical Psychology Review 65 (2018) 163–174

with BDD experience more distress and anxiety than healthy controls estimation of their body size even more than those without ED (Norris,
(Buhlmann, Teachman, Naumann, Fehlinger, & Rief, 2009; Parsons, 1984; Vocks et al., 2007).
Straub, Smith, & Clerkin, 2017; Windheim et al., 2011). Additionally,
individuals with BDD have higher baseline disgust sensitivity and ex- 4. Mirror exposure therapy
perience more disgust in response to viewing themselves in a mirror
than healthy controls (Neziroglu, Hickey, & McKay, 2010). Thus, for The power of mirrors to elicit an emotional reaction to self-viewing
individuals with BDD mirror gazing is often a compulsive act that oc- has been used in a variety of therapeutic modalities to treat psychiatric
cupies a substantial amount of their time. Rather than relieving nega- disorders with symptomatic negative body image. Mirror exposure has
tive emotions, for individuals with BDD, looking in the mirror produces been incorporated into manualized, disease-focused CBT paradigms,
negative emotions. some having specific sessions dedicated to mirror exposure, some
having optional mirror exposure modules and some using exposure-
3.3. Eating and weight disorders response prevention with the means of exposure left to the discretion of
the therapist (Beilharz et al., 2017; Harrison, Fernández De La Cruz,
Like individuals with BDD, most women with ED engage in body Enander, Radua, & Mataix-Cols, 2016). Mirror exposure has also been
checking behaviors, including examining themselves in a mirror. Those used as an adjunctive, stand-alone therapy designed to augment on-
with more severe ED symptoms engage in body checking behaviors going, disease targeting interventions (Hildebrandt, Loeb, Troupe, &
more frequently (Shafran, Fairburn, Robinson, & Lask, 2004). Women Delinsky, 2012). Beyond interventions for those with diagnosable psy-
with ED also frequently engage in body avoidance behaviors, such as chiatric illnesses, mirror exposure therapy has been used to improve
covering mirrors, and many women with ED alternate between body image in non-clinical populations, including both individuals
checking and avoiding behaviors (Shafran et al., 2004). In summary, seeking treatment for body dissatisfaction and healthy individuals
women with ED, like individuals with BDD, spend a significant amount (those without psychiatric illness or severe body image concerns) re-
of time engaging in body checking and/or avoiding behaviors, often cruited to serve as experimental controls (Delinsky & Wilson, 2006;
mirror related, that lead to considerable distress. Moreno-Domínguez, Rodríguez-Ruiz, Fernández-Santaella, Jansen, &
Those with ED tend to experience even more distress and negative Tuschen-Caffier, 2012). We review the literature on controlled and
emotions than healthy individuals soon after being exposed to their uncontrolled trials using therapeutic mirror exposure in a variety of
reflection in a mirror, although this effect may be specific to BN and conditions.
BED and may not apply to those with AN (Cooper & Fairburn, 1992;
Naumann, Trentowska, & Svaldi, 2013; Vocks et al., 2007). Ad- 4.1. Search strategy
ditionally, for women with BED a brief mirror exposure increases both
salivation and their desire to binge eat (Naumann et al., 2013). To identify clinical trials examining the efficacy of mirror exposure
To cope with the negative emotions caused by looking at themselves therapy, a systematic search was performed in Pubmed, last updated on
in the mirror, individuals with ED report using similar cognitive self- April 27, 2018, using the search query: “mirror body image OR mirror
regulation skills to help overcome the elicited negative emotions as binge OR mirror nervosa OR mirror obese OR (mirror exposure therapy
healthy controls; however, those with ED report finding them less NOT stroke) OR mirror body dissatisfaction OR mirror body dys-
useful (Crino, Touyz, & Rieger, 2017). This perceived lack of self-effi- morphic.” This search returned 954 titles, which were screened at the
cacy may be at least partly unfounded: when undergoing prolonged level of title and abstract, and included if criteria were met after review
mirror exposure as 40 min of recording guided self-viewing, individuals of the text. Additionally, the reference lists of included studies as well as
with ED experience an increase in negative emotions at the start of the those of review articles identified that focused on treatment of eating,
mirror exposure that declines to near baseline by the end of the session feeding, body image or weight disorders were screened. Inclusion cri-
(Vocks et al., 2007). Thus, if they tolerate the initial distress of self- teria were: 1) Full length articles written in English 2) Articles indexed
viewing and continue for a prolonged period of time, they are able to on Pubmed as of April 27, 2018 3) Articles describing prospectively
regulate their emotions, at least with the help of a recording. designed clinical evaluations of mirror exposure therapy, independent
Body image disturbances in ED often go beyond dissatisfaction with of randomization or control, with at least 3 participants. A study was
one's appearance and may include profound disturbances in body self- determined to include mirror exposure if all subjects in a group were
perception. Early studies examining self-estimation of body size by required to look at their undistorted reflection in a mirror on separate
women with AN had heterogenous results (Farrell, Lee, & Shafran, occasions with therapeutic intent, even if additional therapeutic ele-
2005). Most contemporary studies have found that females with AN and ments were incorporated. Studies identified that included mirror ex-
BN overestimate their body size more than healthy controls (Gardner & posure as one required component of multisession therapy but with
Brown, 2014; Mölbert et al., 2017); however, Mölbert et al. (2018) ambiguity as to whether at least two separate mirror exposures were
recently found the opposite for individuals with AN when 3 dimen- required of all participants were excluded formally (but discussed in the
sional avatars were used to make body size estimations. Overestimation text) 4) Articles with outcomes related to body image, psychopathology
of body size by women with ED is not limited to situations when sen- and/or body size estimation 5) Studies of subjects with eating, feeding,
sory feedback is not directly available, as they overestimate body size body image or weight disorders and/or who reported dissatisfaction
more than those without ED even when looking in a mirror while es- with their body and/or who were not recruited based on the presence of
timating (Shafran & Fairburn, 2002). A single mirror exposure session a pathological condition. Studies of subjects with neurological disorders
where subjects are asked to describe themselves and touch their body (i.e. with discrete histopathological intracranial lesions such as tumors
contours leads most individuals, including those with AN, BN and no or stroke) were excluded.
ED, to decrease their body size estimations (Norris, 1984). Individuals 15 studies were identified through the primary search and no ad-
with AN decrease their estimations of body size after mirror exposure ditional studies were identified through the reference lists of those
significantly more than those without ED (Norris, 1984). studies. Five treatment focused review articles were found using the
These data show that although seeing at oneself in a mirror is a search query, but no additional studies fitting inclusion criteria were
common experience, looking in a mirror can induce distress and ne- identified through their reference lists.
gative emotions, especially in those with negative body image and
eating pathology. They also show that at the end of a single, prolonged 4.2. Non-clinical populations
and directed mirror exposure exercise, women with ED report only
baseline negative emotions and women with AN decrease their Four uncontrolled studies have examined whether repeated mirror

165
T.C. Griffen et al. Clinical Psychology Review 65 (2018) 163–174

exposure therapy might benefit healthy adult women without a history et al., 1995), CBT with optional mirror exposure (Mataix-Cols et al.,
of an ED. In one, healthy control women (n = 19) had very low baseline 2015; Veale et al., 2014), CBT with exposure-response prevention
negative thoughts, negative emotions and levels of distress, and they strategies with the means of exposure left to the discretion of the
had high baseline positive emotions. These parameters did not change therapist (Krebs, Turner, Heyman, & Mataix-Cols, 2012; Neziroglu,
between mirror exposure sessions (Trentowska, Bender, & Tuschen- McKay, Todaro, & Yaryura-Tobias, 1996) and CBT without any explicit
Caffier, 2013). However, two larger studies with healthy women description of mirror exposure (Enander et al., 2016; Veale et al.,
(n = 30 and n = 168, divided across 3 types of mirror exposure 1996). A single randomized controlled trial also found benefit of non-
therapy) found improvements in mood (Hilbert et al., 2002; Luethcke, disease specific metacognitive therapy for BDD without any description
McDaniel, & Becker, 2011), body satisfaction and ED symptoms fol- of mirror exposure (Rabiei, Mulkens, Kalantari, Molavi, & Bahrami,
lowing mirror exposure therapy (Luethcke et al., 2011). Another small 2012). Although mirror exposure has been included as a required or
(n = 13) study of healthy women found that negative thoughts asso- optional component of many CBT for BDD studies, no study has looked
ciated with watching a film of one's body were reduced after 3 sessions explicitly at the value of mirror exposure for the treatment of BDD.
of mirror exposure therapy (Trentowska, Svaldi, Blechert, & Tuschen- Studies of CBT for BDD with and without explicit descriptions of mirror
Caffier, 2017). Together, these studies suggest that for unselected, non- exposure have both reported clinically significant benefit and no study
clinical women, mirror exposure therapy may provide some benefit in has directly compared different paradigms. Whether mirror exposure
improving mood and possibly body image satisfaction. How persistent provides additive benefit beyond CBT without mirror exposure for in-
these changes are and whether the observed improvement in ED dividuals with BDD remains unexplored.
symptoms reduces risk of developing an ED remain unanswered.
Three small, randomized controlled trials have examined whether 4.4. Eating and weight disorders
mirror exposure is beneficial for women with body dissatisfaction re-
cruited from non-clinical populations. Moreno-Domínguez et al. (2012) 4.4.1. Mixed eating disordered populations
compared 2 types of mirror exposure therapy (n = 10 each) to imagery Individuals with ED have significant impairments in body image.
guided therapy (n = 11), during which subjects describe themselves One small, randomized controlled trial including individuals with all
without a mirror present. Delinsky and Wilson (2006) compared mirror ED, except for those who were underweight or obese, compared mirror
exposure therapy (n = 24) to supportive psychotherapy (n = 21). exposure therapy (n = 17) to supportive psychotherapy (n = 16;
Glashouwer et al. (2016) compared mirror exposure (n = 15) to no Hildebrandt et al., 2012). Mirror exposure therapy resulted in a sig-
intervention (n = 13). These studies excluded women with low or high nificantly greater decrease in body dissatisfaction and ED symptoms
BMI (composite range: 18.5–28) and 2 excluded women with a history compared to the control body image intervention (Hildebrandt et al.,
of ED (Delinsky & Wilson, 2006; Moreno-Domínguez et al., 2012); 2012). Two trials of a cognitive behavioral group therapies for mixed
however, with the exception of Glashouwer et al. (2016) who excluded ED that included at least 1 mirror exposure session, one waitlist con-
women with depression, other psychiatric illnesses, including BDD, trolled and one uncontrolled, found improvements in body image after
were not screened for or excluded. Mirror exposure therapy was found treatment (Bhatnagar, Wisniewski, Solomon, & Heinberg, 2013; Vocks,
to be superior to control interventions on most measures, including Wächter, Wucherer, & Kosfelder, 2008).
negative thoughts, feelings of ugliness, body checking and dissatisfac-
tion, ED symptoms and depression (Delinsky & Wilson, 2006; 4.4.2. Anorexia nervosa
Glashouwer et al., 2016; Moreno-Domínguez et al., 2012). Mirror ex- Mirror exposure therapy has not been trialed for low weight AN out
posure therapy did not, however, change the proportion of time that of concern for causing habituation to an underweight body (Morgan,
body dissatisfied women spent looking at their self-reported least at- Lazarova, Schelhase, & Saeidi, 2014; see below); however, 2 trials have
tractive compared to most attractive body parts during a picture used mirror exposure therapy to treat recently weight restored in-
viewing assessment (Glashouwer et al., 2016). Two additional studies dividuals with AN. Key et al. (2002) conducted a non-randomized trial
of women with body dissatisfaction compared 2 mirror exposure and compared group body image therapy with mirror exposure in 8
paradigms without control groups and found improvement in thought sessions (n = 9) to group body image therapy without mirror exposure
content, mood and body satisfaction across conditions (n = 22 and (n = 6) and found a significant improvement in body dissatisfaction
n = 35; Díaz-Ferrer, Rodríguez-Ruiz, Ortega-Roldán, Mata-Martín, & only in the mirror exposure therapy group. A larger, uncontrolled trial
Carmen Fernández-Santaella, 2017; Jansen et al., 2016). of group body image therapy (n = 55) that included mirror exposure in
most sessions found a significant decrease in shape and weight concerns
4.3. Body dysmorphic disorder and ED symptoms compared to baseline (Morgan et al., 2014). Ad-
ditionally, a case series of three individuals with weight restored AN
BDD is an extreme form of body dissatisfaction that includes ob- and persistent ED symptoms found benefit for acceptance and com-
sessions and/or compulsions and body image concerns that are grossly mitment therapy that included a mirror exposure component (Berman,
disproportionate with respect to how others perceive a purported flaw Boutelle, & Crow, 2009). A larger, randomized controlled trial is needed
(APA, 2013). Through our systematic search, we did not identify any to replicate these preliminary results.
studies investigating the efficacy of mirror exposure therapy for in-
dividuals with BDD as defined in our methods. BDD specific CBT has 4.4.3. Bulimia nervosa
been extensively studied and found to be an effective treatment Several small, uncontrolled trials have evaluated mirror exposure
(Beilharz et al., 2017; Harrison et al., 2016). Six randomized controlled therapy in women with BN or with ED-NOS whose frequency of binging
trials (and several uncontrolled trials) have found clinically significant and compensatory behaviors was insufficient to merit a diagnosis of BN
benefits of CBT for BDD and study populations have included male and (and who would be classified at BN of low frequency and/or limited
female adolescents and adults (Harrison et al., 2016); however, there duration in the DSM – 5; APA, 2013) with positive results (Díaz-Ferrer,
are variations of CBT for BDD with different usages of mirror exposure: Rodríguez-Ruiz, Ortega-Roldán, Moreno-Domínguez, & Fernández-
CBT with mirror exposure in at least one session (Fang, Schwartz, & Santaella, 2015; Trentowska et al., 2013; Trentowska et al., 2017;
Wilhelm, 2016; Greenberg et al., 2010; Greenberg, Mothi, & Wilhelm, Trentowska, Svaldi, & Tuschen-Caffier, 2014). Trentowska et al. (2014)
2016; Weingarden et al., 2011; Wilhelm et al., 2014; Wilhelm, treated subjects with ED-NOS (n = 14) with 5 mirror exposures sessions
Buhlmann, Hayward, Greenberg, & Dimaite, 2010; Wilhelm, Otto, Lohr, and subjects with BN (n = 13) with alternating video-of-self exposure
& Deckersbach, 1999; Wilhelm, Phillips, Fama, Greenberg, & Steketee, sessions (3) and mirror exposure sessions (2) and found improvement in
2011), group CBT with at home mirror exposure homework (Rosen body image dissatisfaction in both groups; however, improvement in

166
T.C. Griffen et al. Clinical Psychology Review 65 (2018) 163–174

ED symptoms was only observed in the ED-NOS group. The difference mirror exposure and included control groups. Of these, 1 trial was not
in ED symptom improvement could be secondary to the baseline illness randomized and included only 15 participants in total (Key et al.,
severity or to the difference in treatment protocols. Three other studies 2002). One trial included only 16 total participants, reported only a
of women with BN (n = 29, n = 19 and n = 13) found that mirror ex- trend towards a benefit, and did not include validated outcome mea-
posure reduces body dissatisfaction, distress and negative thoughts sures of body image satisfaction, body image related behaviors and/or
(including those associated with watching videos of one's body) and eating disorder symptoms (Jansen et al., 2008). Four randomized
increases positive thoughts (Díaz-Ferrer et al., 2015; Trentowska et al., controlled trials had between 10 and 24 participants per treatment
2013; Trentowska et al., 2017). A larger uncontrolled trial (n = 67) condition (total n = 137, 97% female) and all found significant benefit
found that manualized CBT including an unspecified number of mirror of mirror exposure therapy over control treatment (Delinsky & Wilson,
exposure sessions is effective at reducing binging, body dissatisfaction 2006; Glashouwer et al., 2016; Hildebrandt et al., 2012; Moreno-
and depressive symptoms in women with BN (Tuschen-Caffier, Pook, & Domínguez et al., 2012). Additionally, one randomized controlled trial
Frank, 2001). found an effect of time in treatment for exposure therapy including
mirror exposure that was not better than cognitive restructuring
4.4.4. Binge eating disorder therapy (Hilbert & Tuschen-Caffier, 2004). The studies that found
One study has examined the effects of mirror exposure therapy in benefit for mirror exposure therapy over the control condition all
binge eating disorder (BED). Hilbert et al. (2002) found improvement in provided individual mirror exposure and excluded individuals that
mood and appearance related self-esteem during a second mirror ex- were underweight or obese (Delinsky & Wilson, 2006; Glashouwer
posure session in a group of women (n = 30) with BED in an un- et al., 2016; Hildebrandt et al., 2012; Moreno-Domínguez et al., 2012).
controlled trial. Additionally, in a small, randomized, controlled trial of In the study by Hilbert and Tuschen-Caffier (2004), mirror exposure
19 sessions of group CBT with 4 body exposure sessions (including an was provided in a group context and the average BMI of participants
unspecified number of mirror exposures) or with 4 cognitive re- was in the obese range. All subjects were treated with CBT, either with
structuring sessions focused on body image for women with BED or ED- a body exposure component or with a cognitive restructuring compo-
NOS (and who would qualify for BED of low frequency and/or limited nent. The body exposure component included mirror and video ex-
duration in the DSM – 5; APA, 2013; n = 14 per condition), body dis- posure and exposure to “avoided body-related situations” and the re-
satisfaction, ED symptoms (including binging) and depression improved lative amount of each component was not specified (Hilbert & Tuschen-
over treatment, but not differentially between conditions (Hilbert & Caffier, 2004). Therefore, because the experimental manipulation did
Tuschen-Caffier, 2004). It is not clear how much of the exposure ses- not explicitly require multiple mirror exposure sessions it did not meet
sions were dedicated to mirror exposure in this trial and whether the the strict definition for inclusion. This study also had a more robust,
benefit of mirror exposure may have been attenuated or enhanced by its evidence-based control therapy condition compared to the other con-
occurrence within a therapy group. Additionally, whether cognitive trolled studies mentioned above, making detecting superiority of the
restructuring and mirror exposure might have additive benefit was not exposure intervention less likely (Hilbert & Tuschen-Caffier, 2004). The
tested. discrepant results of Hilbert and Tuschen-Caffier (2004) may therefore
be secondary to inclusion criteria (e.g. BMI), experimental intervention
4.4.5. Obesity (group exposure therapy not restricted entirely to mirror exposure) or
Body dissatisfaction is prevalent among obese individuals to the presence of a robust control intervention.
(Weinberger, Kersting, Riedel-Heller, & Luck-Sikorski, 2016); however, Other stand-alone interventions designed to improve body image
most trials of mirror exposure therapy have excluded obese individuals. have only a small effect (Alleva et al., 2015). We calculated modified
A small randomized, controlled trial for male and female adolescents in Cohen's d effect sizes as d+ according the method proposed by Morris
a residential obesity treatment program compared mirror exposure for pretest-posttest experimental designs (Morris, 2008) for the 4 ran-
therapy added on to treatment as usual (n = 8) to treatment as usual domized, controlled studies that matched inclusion criteria, directly
alone (n = 8) and found a non-significant improvement in body dis- manipulated individual mirror exposure sessions and had validated
satisfaction and anxiety (Jansen et al., 2008). This trend towards a measures of body image satisfaction, body image related behaviors
positive result was accompanied by significantly less weight loss in the and/or eating disorder symptoms as primary outcomes (Delinsky &
mirror exposure group compared to the control group (Jansen et al., Wilson, 2006; Glashouwer et al., 2016; Hildebrandt et al., 2012;
2008). A small case series of adults with obesity (1 male, 2 female) Moreno-Domínguez et al., 2012). Briefly, the difference between the
undergoing a comprehensive weight loss program that included pre- and posttest mean of the mirror exposure group was subtracted
watching oneself eat in a mirror when deviating from pre-planned from the difference between the pre- and posttest mean of the control
meals reported promising results for weight loss (Rosen, 1981); this group and divided by the pooled pretest standard deviation and then
unique exposure strategy, which did not meet our criteria for mirror multiplied by a sample size correction factor (Morris, 2008). We then
exposure therapy, has not been followed up with further published calculated the mean effect size (d) as the mean effect size of all body
research. As these studies were uncontrolled or underpowered, larger image and eating related validated primary outcome measures
trials are needed to determine whether mirror exposure therapy or weighted by sample size. Baseline and last available follow-up data
using a mirror during deviation from a planned diet provides benefit for were used for effect size calculations.
the treatment of obesity and associated body image dissatisfaction. In the study by Delinsky and Wilson (2006), we found a medium
effect of mirror exposure on shape and weight concerns (Eating Dis-
4.5. Comparison of evidence across conditions order Examination-Questionnaire (EDE-Q), Shape and Weight Con-
cerns, d+ = 0.50) and small effects on body image avoidance (Body
Studies of mirror exposure therapy have yielded generally positive Image Avoidance Questionnaire, d+ = 0.33) and body checking (Body
results for disorders of body image perception, eating and weight and Checking Questionnaire (BCQ), d+ = 0.48). There was no significant
for non-clinical populations. Enthusiasm for mirror image therapy, treatment effect on dissatisfaction with body parts (Satisfaction with
however, must be tempered by the limitations of these studies. Trials of Body Parts Scale, d+ = −0.05) or dieting (Dutch Restrained Eating
mirror image therapy for body dissatisfaction, BN and BED are limited Scale, d+ = 0.03). In the study by Moreno-Domínguez et al. (2012), we
by having only female subjects, and very few male subjects were in- found a large effect of “pure mirror exposure” and a medium effect of
cluded in studies of mixed ED populations and AN. For BDD, no trial has “guided mirror exposure” on body shape concerns (Body Shape Ques-
tested the benefit of mirror exposure alone or as an isolated component tionnaire (BSQ), d+ = 1.67, 0.57). In the study by Glashouwer et al.
of CBT. Most importantly, only 6 small trials have directly manipulated (2016), we found a medium effect on eating disorder symptoms (EDE-

167
T.C. Griffen et al. Clinical Psychology Review 65 (2018) 163–174

Q, d+ = 0.70). Finally, in the study by Hildebrandt et al. (2012), we versus sequential body parts) and the language that the individual is
found medium effects of mirror exposure on body checking (BCQ, instructed to use (describing emotions that arise versus non-judgmental
d+ = 0.68), eating-related obsessions (Yale-Brown-Cornell Eating Dis- descriptions of appearance). Either or both of those parameters could
order Scale (YBC-EDS), obsessions subscale, d+ = 0.77) and eating-re- account for the benefit seen with the pure mirror exposure technique.
lated rituals (YBC-EDS, rituals subscale, d+ = 0.72), and a small effect
of mirror exposure on body shape concerns (BSQ, d+ = 0.21). After 5.2. Adding an emotional focus to mirror exposure
weighting the validated ED and body image symptom primary out-
comes from these four studies for sample size, we found an overall Jansen et al. (2016) modulated where subjects with body dis-
medium effect of mirror exposure compared to control conditions, satisfaction directed their attention during mirror exposure therapy to
d = 0.67. determine whether focusing on either subject-defined attractive or
unattractive body parts alters the efficacy of mirror exposure therapy.
5. Technical considerations for mirror exposure therapy They instructed participants to focus on viewing their self-defined most
attractive body parts and use language with positive valence exclusively
Many variations of mirror exposure therapy have been reported. (positive focus group) or to focus on viewing their self-defined least
Most studies of mirror exposure therapy utilize a cognitive restructuring attractive body parts and attend to their thoughts and feelings as they
approach: the subject is instructed to describe their reflection using arise (negative focus group; Jansen et al., 2016). Both styles of mirror
non-judgmental language, typically starting with their head and pro- exposure led to similar improvements in body satisfaction, body
gressing down to their toes, followed by a whole-body description, avoidance behaviors and mood; however, body avoidance continued to
while a therapist is present and ensures that the subject adheres to the improve after completion of the intervention only in the negative focus
instructions (Delinsky & Wilson, 2006; Harrison et al., 2016; Phillips & group. The negative focus group also had a greater improvement in
Rogers, 2011). This approach is known as “guided non-judgmental perceived attractiveness of their least attractive body part (Jansen et al.,
mirror exposure therapy.” Another approach, “pure mirror exposure 2016). These benefits did not come without cost. The negative focus
therapy,” involves the subject looking at their whole body and obser- group experienced more negative thoughts during early therapy ses-
ving and commenting on evoked emotions as they arise (Moreno- sions than the positive focus group; however, this difference dis-
Domínguez et al., 2012). Mirror exposure has also been used within appeared in later sessions (Jansen et al., 2016).
group therapy and as homework to be completed outside of the direct To isolate the effect of language valence on mirror exposure,
supervision of a therapist (Key et al., 2002; Rosen et al., 1995). Luethcke et al. (2011) compared instructing participants to describe
A small number of randomized trials have compared different their body parts using language with positive valence to using non-
technical approaches to individual mirror exposure therapy, discussed judgmental language. They found that for both treatment styles, ED
below. Except for a single study of women with BN (Díaz-Ferrer et al., symptoms and mood improved similarly, but body satisfaction im-
2015), all trials comparing mirror exposure modalities used body dis- proved only in the positive valence condition (Luethcke et al., 2011).
satisfied women as subjects (Díaz-Ferrer et al., 2017; Jansen et al., This study has two important limitations: only 2 mirror exposure ses-
2016; Luethcke et al., 2011; Moreno-Domínguez et al., 2012). No trial sions were used, compared to 3–6 sessions in other studies (Delinsky &
has compared mirror exposure therapy performed under the guidance Wilson, 2006; Díaz-Ferrer et al., 2015, 2017; Hildebrandt et al., 2012;
of a therapist to mirror exposure therapy performed alone or in the Jansen et al., 2016; Moreno-Domínguez et al., 2012), and the sessions
context of a therapy group, and no trial has empirically determined the were guided by a list of body parts to describe, not a trained therapist.
ideal length of mirror exposure sessions or the ideal length or frequency Given the results of Jansen et al. (2016) described above, it is possible
of mirror exposure treatment. A recent study found that after a single that had Luethcke et al. (2011) extended their treatment to include
mirror exposure session, body dissatisfied women whose posture was more mirror exposure sessions, they would have found a positive effect
manipulated to be more upright experienced more positive emotions of using non-judgmental language on body image.
than those whose posture was manipulated to be contracted (Miragall
et al., 2018). Whether manipulations that increase positive emotions 6. Risks of and relative contraindications to mirror exposure
after mirror exposure sessions early in therapy lead to larger and more therapy
sustained improvements is unknown.
6.1. Adverse events in clinical trials of mirror exposure therapy
5.1. Pure mirror exposure versus guided non-judgmental mirror exposure
Psychotherapies can have harmful side effects and well controlled
The technique applied during a mirror exposure session as well as clinical trials with adequate monitoring and reporting of adverse events
the length of the session can alter the emotional response during and are critical to quantifying the risk of harm (Barlow, 2010; Bystedt,
after the exposure session (Díaz-Ferrer et al., 2015; Díaz-Ferrer et al., Rozental, Andersson, Boettcher, & Carlbring, 2014; Crown, 1983).
2017; Jansen et al., 2016; Luethcke et al., 2011; Moreno-Domínguez Uncontrolled trials that show symptomatic improvement might re-
et al., 2012; Shafran et al., 2007; Vocks et al., 2007). Guided non- present a slowing of normal recovery relative to no treatment and
judgmental mirror exposure therapy has been compared to pure mirror provide no baseline of adverse events for comparison. As looking at
exposure therapy in 3 randomized trials. For women with body dis- oneself in a mirror can lead to significant distress and worsening of
satisfaction, both therapeutic techniques achieve similar improvement negative emotional states (Veale et al., 2016; Walker et al., 2012;
over baseline in positive and negative thoughts and feelings of ugliness Windheim et al., 2011), mirror exposure therapy could be destabilizing
(Díaz-Ferrer et al., 2017; Moreno-Domínguez et al., 2012), but pure and dangerous for certain individuals.
mirror exposure therapy is superior for reducing distress both within Many of the uncontrolled trials of mirror exposure therapy or CBT
and between sessions (Moreno-Domínguez et al., 2012). Similarly, for that may or may not have included mirror exposure reported that at
women with BN both pure and guided non-judgmental mirror exposure least one participant dropped out or missed enough treatment sessions
paradigms lead to equal improvements in positive and negative to mandate exclusion after starting therapy (Díaz-Ferrer et al., 2015;
thoughts, but pure mirror exposure therapy is superior for reducing Greenberg et al., 2016; Trentowska et al., 2014; Tuschen-Caffier et al.,
body dissatisfaction (Díaz-Ferrer et al., 2015). Together, these findings 2001; Vocks et al., 2008; Wilhelm et al., 1999). Greenberg et al. (2016)
suggest that pure mirror exposure therapy may have some benefit over reported that 2 subjects in their trial of CBT with mirror exposure for
the guided non-judgmental technique. These therapies differ in two BDD dropped out due to needing a higher level of care. If some parti-
important ways: where the individual is instructed to look (whole body cipants dropped out due to clinical deterioration secondary to therapy,

168
T.C. Griffen et al. Clinical Psychology Review 65 (2018) 163–174

this may have led to overestimation of benefit. underweight body.


In randomized controlled trials of CBT with either optional or no A controlled trial of a psychoeducational group therapy focused on
explicit mention of mirror exposure therapy, 2 subjects receiving CBT body image compared to waitlist was well tolerated and led to im-
compared to 5 control subjects dropped out (Veale et al., 2014) and 3 provement in shape and weight concerns for underweight individuals
adverse events were reported: 1 suicide attempt by a subject receiving with AN (Mountford et al., 2015). If mirror exposure can be tolerated
CBT and 1 sleep disturbance in and 1 suicide attempt by control sub- by underweight individuals with AN and decreases self-perceived body
jects (Enander et al., 2016; Mataix-Cols et al., 2015). In those rando- size overestimation (Noris, 1984) and body dissatisfaction (as seen in
mized controlled trials where mirror exposure was included in CBT for with other ED), it could reduce the desire to lose weight at the core of
all subjects, 5 receiving CBT compared to 6 controls dropped out AN pathology. This hypothesis remains to be tested and should be done
(Hilbert & Tuschen-Caffier, 2004; Wilhelm et al., 2014). Thus, in ran- so with caution given concerns of experienced clinicians (Morgan et al.,
domized controlled trials of CBT with or without mirror exposure, drop- 2014).
out and adverse event rates are similar in study and control interven-
tions. One cannot determine the specific impact of mirror exposure in 7. Mechanism of action of mirror exposure therapy
these trials given that mirror exposure was not manipulated in isola-
tion. How mirror exposure therapy confers therapeutic benefit is un-
In randomized controlled trials examining mirror exposure therapy known. Those high in body image concerns and with ED spend more
as an isolated manipulation adverse events were also reported. In one time looking at their less attractive body parts when they look in the
study of an ED population, 2 subjects receiving mirror exposure therapy mirror and experience more distress after looking in the mirror
were withdrawn after acute increases in non-suicidal self-injurious be- (Buhlmann et al., 2009; Svaldi et al., 2016; Tuschen-Caffier et al., 2015;
haviors; both had prior histories of these behaviors (Hildebrandt et al., Veale et al., 2016; Vocks et al., 2007; Walker et al., 2012; Windheim
2012). No adverse events were reported in the control condition et al., 2011) with no clear disease specific responses thus far identified
(Hildebrandt et al., 2012). In a study of females with body images that might differentiate distinct mechanisms of action in distinct
disturbance, 3 subjects in the mirror exposure condition and none in the pathologies. The one exception is that mirror exposure reduces body
control condition dropped out of the study; the dropouts had sig- size overestimation in underweight AN more than in other groups
nificantly higher baseline depression scores than other participants (Norris, 1984). A more realistic evaluation of self-body size could lead
(Delinsky & Wilson, 2006). Larger, more inclusive trials isolating mirror to reduced drive to lose weight; however, this is a speculative me-
exposure therapy are warranted to more fully characterize the safety of chanism and it is unknown whether mirror exposure therapy is effective
this intervention and to extend our understanding of whether there may or safe in underweight individuals with AN (see above). Beyond un-
be gender differences in treatment risk. Given that the total number of derweight AN, there is insufficient empirical evidence to assign possible
adverse events that has been reported in mirror exposure conditions is mechanisms of action to specific underlying pathological states.
numerically greater than for control interventions, caution is warranted Appearance related cognitive biases, both attentional and inter-
when treating individuals with a history of self-injurious behavior or pretive, have been described in ED (Bauer et al., 2017; Cardi et al.,
current clinical depression. 2017; Cooper, 1997; Glashouwer et al., 2016; Smeets, Jansen, & Roefs,
2011) and BDD (Greenberg et al., 2014). Individuals with ED tend to
6.2. Underweight and obese populations interpret ambiguous negative situations as being related to their shape
or weight (Bauer et al., 2017; Cooper, 1997; Cooper, Cohen-Tovée,
Mirror exposure therapy for individuals at weight extremes is con- Todd, Wells, & Tovée, 1997) and those with BDD interpret negative
troversial (Morgan et al., 2014) and most randomized controlled trials social cues as relating to their appearance (Buhlmann et al., 2002).
of mirror exposure therapy have excluded underweight and obese in- Interventions targeting negative interpretation biases have had some
dividuals (Delinsky & Wilson, 2006; Glashouwer et al., 2016; success in BDD and AN (Summers & Cougle, 2016; Turton, Cardi,
Hildebrandt et al., 2012; Moreno-Domínguez et al., 2012). Fostering Treasure, & Hirsch, 2018). Mirror exposure therapy may normalize
body acceptance in individuals for whom weight loss or gain would interpretive biases by training individuals to interpret their bodies in an
confer medical benefit could in theory lead to a decrease in motivation objective, affectively neutral or affectively positive manner (Delinsky &
to change weight. During a small randomized controlled trial of mirror Wilson, 2006; Luethcke et al., 2011).
exposure for adolescents in an obesity treatment program, those in the Individuals with body dissatisfaction also have attentional biases
mirror exposure group lost significantly less weight than those in the that likely play both etiological and maintaining roles in their pa-
control condition (Jansen et al., 2008). However, if mirror exposure for thology. These attentional biases take the form of both own-body spe-
obese individuals were to be shown to lead to more gradual weight loss cific and generalized visual processing deficits. Individuals with AN, BN
and a reduction in psychiatric symptoms, it could possibly lead to more and BDD spend more time visually attending to fine details at the ex-
sustained weight loss and well-being (Pasanisi, Contaldo, de Simone, & pense of global features relative to healthy controls, resulting in defi-
Mancini, 2001). This is an enticing hypothesis that could be in- cient performance on the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Task
vestigated in a future larger and longer clinical trial. (Deckersbach et al., 2000; Lang et al., 2016). The biases of body dis-
Vocks et al. (2008) included 3 subjects with AN who were pre- satisfied for processing of own-body are characterized by an increase in
sumably underweight by diagnosis in their uncontrolled trial of group general self-focused attention with a specific focus on their self-defined
CBT in which 3 sessions included mirror exposure. Four subjects were least attractive body parts. Increased self-focused attention has been
excluded from this trial after missing 2 or more treatment sessions. observed in many psychiatric disorders (Ingram, 1990), including BDD,
Neither the ED of nor the sessions missed by the excluded subjects were BED and weight-restored individuals with history of AN (Sawaoka,
reported (Vocks et al., 2008); therefore, it is not clear how well the Barnes, Blomquist, Masheb, & Grilo, 2012; Windheim et al., 2011;
treatment was tolerated by the individuals in the study with AN. No Zucker et al., 2015). Interestingly, underweight individuals with AN
trial has explicitly tested mirror exposure for AN before weight re- report less self-focused attention than healthy controls (Zucker et al.,
storation; however, a single ME in this group does result in a reduction 2015). In BDD, it has been suggested that repeated mirror gazing may
of body size estimation (Norris, 1984). Whether the reduction in body lead to cognitive distortions and over representation of the perceived
dissatisfaction and negative emotions seen after mirror exposure defect (Veale & Riley, 2001). Hypothetically, this could lead to an over-
therapy in other ED would occur in AN is not known. Similarly, it is not representation within sensory cortex dedicated to processing perceived
known whether mirror exposure therapy would change the patient's defect-related stimuli.
resistance to changing weight or a lead to habituation to an Acute mirror exposure increases self-focused attention, both in

169
T.C. Griffen et al. Clinical Psychology Review 65 (2018) 163–174

individuals with BDD and healthy controls (Wilhelm et al., 2011. Ad- conditioned anxiety, conditioned disgust is more effectively modified
ditionally, experimentally increasing self-focused attention prior to by counterconditioning (e.g. novel pairing of the CS (body image) with
mirror exposure worsened body satisfaction in a non-clinical sample of positive or neutral emotions via classical conditioning) than by ma-
women (Veale et al., 2016). These findings appear paradoxical given nipulation of the original CS (body image) ➔ CR (disgust) association
the association between elevated self-focused attention and psycho- (i.e. operant conditioning; Engelhard, Leer, Lange, & Olatunji, 2014). In
pathology and the efficacy of mirror exposure therapy; however, they this model, mirror exposure may improve body image dissatisfaction
may explain the clinical worsening that a small number of vulnerable through counterconditioning rather than weakening the net negative
individuals have experienced early in treatment with mirror exposure valence of the CR (Klimek et al., 2016). Evaluative conditioning (EC),
therapy. Mirror exposure therapy may act in part by redirecting the the basis for counterconditioning, transfers valence between a CS and
focus of attention away from negative body parts to the more balanced an unconditioned stimulus (US) without utilizing a contingency (CS-
focus of those low in body image concerns (Glashouwer et al., 2016; US). The neurocircuitry of EC, particularly in the context of disgust,
Smeets et al., 2011), thereby globally reducing self-focused attention involves greater insula processing of a CS-US association and less robust
after completion of a full course of treatment. Mirror exposure might activation in the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex and nucleus ac-
also act in part through a cognitive retraining mechanism, whereby the cumbens compared to reward learning (Schweckendiek et al., 2013).
individual is trained to view their body more globally rather than fo- Therefore, body image disturbances may be resistant to change through
cusing on fine details. Individuals who dedicate excessive attention to a traditional exposure therapies because they originate from the acqui-
particular area and engage in excessive mirror gazing may benefit from sition of a disgust response to one's body. Classic exposure models rely
learning to spread their attention across their body. on the creation of a new, less threatening contingency and consolida-
In support of the hypothesis that cognitive biases towards negatively tion of this new memory (Craske, Hermans, & Vervliet, 2018), a
perceived body parts may perpetuate body dissatisfaction, a single memory that would not form if the exposure led consistently to an
computer training session that focused attention on self-defined most aversive (i.e. disgust) response. Counterconditioning does not rely on
attractive body parts, but not a training session that focused attention the conscious formation of a new memory (Sweldens, Corneille, &
evenly across the body, was found to increase body satisfaction for Yzerbyt, 2014) and may change valence independent of awareness of
subjects with high body dissatisfaction (Smeets et al., 2011). Un- the perceived causal relationship between body exposure and negative
fortunately, both training paradigms decreased mood acutely and the consequences. For example, during guided non-judgmental mirror ex-
persistence of efficacy was not tested (Smeets et al., 2011). Glashouwer posure therapy, the repeated pairing of a negatively valanced CS (body
et al. (2016) found that 4 sessions of mirror exposure therapy in which image) with a neutrally valanced US (the descriptive procedure) may
subjects were asked to focus on their self-defined most attractive body act by transferring valence from the US to the CS over time and redu-
parts had no effect on where they looked when viewing pictures of cing its negativity.
themselves even though the therapy improved body satisfaction. Fur- Mirror exposure therapy may also act through the creation of cog-
ther research is required to determine whether other styles or longer nitive dissonance, discomfort arising through a conflict between belief
durations of mirror exposure therapy act via redirecting gaze pre- and behavior (Festinger, 1957; Jansen et al., 2016; Klimek et al., 2016;
ference, reducing self-focused attention or modifying other attentional Luethcke et al., 2011). The creation of discomfort is thought to drive
biases. behavioral modification to align belief and behavior and decrease dis-
In addition to benefits for anxiety, obsessive-compulsive and post- comfort (Festinger, 1957; Jansen et al., 2016; Klimek et al., 2016;
traumatic stress disorders, exposure response prevention therapy has Luethcke et al., 2011). Although cognitive dissonance is most clearly
been shown to have some benefit in both AN and BN and is explicitly created when a subject is asked to describe body parts exclusively using
incorporated into CBT for BDD (Bulik, Sullivan, Carter, McIntosh, & language with positive valence (Jansen et al., 2016; Luethcke et al.,
Joyce, 1998; Foa & McLean, 2016; Harrison et al., 2016; Leitenberg, 2011), even the use of neutral language could create dissonance for an
Rosen, Gross, Nudelman, & Vara, 1988; Levinson et al., 2015; McKay individual with strongly negative body image related beliefs sufficient
et al., 2015; Steinglass et al., 2014; Wilson, Eldredge, Smith, & Niles, to drive a behavioral change.
1991). During mirror exposure, one's own body image in the mirror In addition to change induced while an individual confronts their
serves as a conditioned stimulus (CS), which may elicit a conditioned body in a mirror, mirror exposure therapy paradigms may effect change
response (CR) of anxiety (Klimek, Grotzinger, & Hildebrandt, 2016). outside of the therapist's office. Most interventions implicitly or ex-
Mirror exposure therapy may act via mechanisms similar to other ex- plicitly ask patients to reduce body-checking and avoidance outside of
posure therapies by enhancing extinction learning, through formation sessions (Delinsky & Wilson, 2006; Glashouwer et al., 2016; Harrison
of a new safety memory that attenuates the negative response and/or et al., 2016; Hildebrandt et al., 2012). These behavioral changes may
through habituation (Craske et al., 2008; Foa & McLean, 2016). Several play a role in driving improvements in body image during mirror ex-
studies on mirror exposure therapy suggest that habituation to the posure therapy; however, they are likely not the only mechanism of
discomfort and negative affect associated with visual exposure to one's change in mirror exposure therapy as Delinsky and Wilson (2006) in-
own body occurs within and between sessions (Díaz-Ferrer et al., 2017; cluded similar homework assignments in their mirror exposure and
Trentowska et al., 2013; Trentowska et al., 2017). Thus, the total ne- control conditions and still found a benefit to mirror exposure therapy.
gative valence of the CR, anxiety, diminishes over the course of the Mirror exposure in the therapeutic context, however, may act to drive
mirror exposure therapy (Jansen et al., 2008; Key et al., 2002). While behavioral changes outside of sessions beyond instructive interventions.
some degree of anxiety response attenuation often occurs within ses- By training one to look at themselves in the mirror differently than they
sions, the precise learning mechanism (habituation, extinction learning had been, mirror exposure therapy might provide benefit both to in-
and/or new safety memory) is unclear. Further, within session at- dividuals who engage in excessive body checking and to those who
tenuation of negative affect does not necessarily correlate with treat- avoid mirrors. No study has examined whether individuals who engage
ment outcomes in other exposure paradigms (Craske et al., 2008), and in a particular mirror related pathological behavior, either avoidance or
Díaz-Ferrer et al. (2017) found that although within session attenuation excessive use, might be more or less likely to benefit from mirror ex-
of negative emotion occurs during pure mirror exposure therapy but not posure therapy. Importantly, the proposed mechanisms of action dis-
during guided non-judgmental mirror exposure therapy, both types of cussed here are not mutually exclusive and some or all may contribute
mirror exposure are therapy effective. to therapeutic benefit in any given patient (Klimek et al., 2016).
Alternatively, it has been proposed that disgust towards one's own
body may contribute to body image disturbances by serving as the
primary CR, not anxiety (Klimek et al., 2016). Differently from

170
T.C. Griffen et al. Clinical Psychology Review 65 (2018) 163–174

8. Mirror exposure therapy beyond eating, weight and body image 2006; Hildebrandt et al., 2012; Mataix-Cols et al., 2015). We re-
disorders commend screening prior to initiation and monitoring during treatment
for: suicidal ideation, self-harm behaviors and depressive symptoms.
Body image disturbances occur in pathologies beyond eating, Significant worsening in any of these domains should prompt termi-
weight and body dysmorphic disorders and exposure to mirrors occurs nation of mirror exposure therapy and referral to a higher level of care
frequently in every-day life. Responses to single mirror exposure ses- if indicated.
sions have been examined in women with post-traumatic stress disorder Over and underweight individuals comprise special populations for
(PTSD) and impairments in sexual arousal (Borgmann, Kleindienst, mirror exposure therapy. Due to their exclusion from most clinical
Vocks, & Dyer, 2014; Seal & Meston, 2007). As we hypothesize that trials, we believe that mirror exposure therapy for over or underweight
mirror exposure therapy for eating, weight and body dysmorphic dis- individuals should be restricted to expert clinicians whose practices are
orders likely acts via generalized mechanisms, we will review these focused on these populations and to randomized, controlled clinical
experiments and propose the testable hypotheses that mirror exposure trials to assess efficacy within these populations.
therapy could provide benefit for individuals with these pathologies. Several variations of mirror exposure therapy have been reported to
have benefit, and clinicians may wish to tailor therapy to the individual
8.1. Post-traumatic stress disorder patient. Focusing on a patient's most positively perceived body parts
and encouraging the use of language with positive valence may be more
PTSD and childhood trauma are both associated with low body sa- tolerable (Jansen et al., 2016; Luethcke et al., 2011) and thus could be
tisfaction (Dyer et al., 2013; Scheffers et al., 2017). Although there is a selected for a patient with poor distress tolerance or when only a few
very high comorbidity between ED and PTSD with history of childhood mirror exposure sessions are feasible. Pure mirror exposure therapy and
sexual abuse, the body image disturbance seen in women with PTSD mirror exposure focusing exclusively on the body parts that a patient is
and history of childhood sexual abuse cannot be fully accounted for by most dissatisfied with may be the most effective forms of mirror ex-
the comorbid ED (Dyer et al., 2013). Unsurprisingly, women with posure therapy tested (Díaz-Ferrer et al., 2017; Jansen et al., 2016;
trauma histories associate negative emotions with body areas related to Moreno-Domínguez et al., 2012). Focusing on negatively perceived
the traumas they have experienced (Dyer, Feldmann, & Borgmann, body parts exclusively has not been trialed in a clinical population.
2015). During a standardized mirror exposure paradigm lasting Two important considerations should be given attention prior to
~10 min, women with PTSD and a history of childhood sexual trauma selection of the mirror exposure therapist: gender matching between
experience a significantly greater worsening of negative emotions and patient and therapist and whether mirror exposure therapy should be
cognition compared to healthy controls (Borgmann et al., 2014), similar conducted by the same therapist providing the patient with other psy-
to what has been observed for women with BDD (Buhlmann et al., chotherapies. During mirror exposure therapy, patients are challenged
2009; Windheim et al., 2011) and ED (Crino et al., 2017). Given the to wear revealing clothing, often undergarments. Thus, care should be
high prevalence of body dissatisfaction in women with sexual trauma taken to ensure that local practice is considered when deciding whether
and their similar response to a single mirror exposure as women for to undertake therapy with a non-gender matched patient. Our clinical
whom mirror exposure therapy is an effective treatment, mirror ex- practices differ in this regard based on experiences with local popula-
posure therapy could be empirically tested as a novel therapeutic in- tions.
tervention for individuals with PTSD and a history of sexual trauma Mirror exposure has been successfully incorporated into manualized
(Borgmann et al., 2014). CBT with the same therapist conducting all components of therapy
(Beilharz et al., 2017; Harrison et al., 2016; Tuschen-Caffier et al.,
8.2. Sexual arousal impairment 2001). We support the use of comprehensive, manualized therapy with
a single therapist; however, our clinical experience is that in non-re-
Negative body image is correlated with poor sexual functioning search settings, indications for mirror exposure are often revealed
(Davison & McCabe, 2005; Faith & Schare, 1993; Koch et al., 2005), during ongoing psychotherapy. In this common scenario, we re-
although the relationship between body image and sexual functioning commend that the therapist consider the possible complications of
may be moderated through self-esteem (Davison & McCabe, 2005). unwanted transference-countertransference development and/or the
Interestingly, Seal and Meston (2007) found that after a body awareness appearance of impropriety. A clinician with little experience in mirror
exercise using mirror exposure, women with sexual dysfunction report exposure therapy, who is in a private office without other staff, who is
an increase in subjective arousal while listening to audiotapes of erotic gender mismatched with their patient or who has any other concern
stories compared to those receiving a control intervention without that mirror exposure therapy could disrupt their therapeutic alliance
mirror exposure. In this mirror exposure paradigm, women were asked with the patient should feel comfortable referring the patient to a col-
to use a mirror to place electrocardiogram electrodes on their bodies league for the mirror exposure component of treatment with whom
and the mirror remained present while they subsequently listened to they can collaborate. We typically provide mirror exposure therapy as
erotica (Seal & Meston, 2007). Future studies could investigate mirror an adjunct to ongoing psychotherapy by the treating therapist when
exposure therapy for sexual arousal impairments and would benefit indicated. A typical course of treatment involves approximately 6 one-
from testing whether mirror exposure therapy has a beneficial and hour sessions, including preparation, exposure and debriefing in each
persistent effect on sexual arousal and performance with a partner. session.

9. Clinical indications and pearls 10. Conclusions

Based on the available evidence reviewed here, we recommend Mirror exposure therapy is a transdiagnostic treatment for in-
mirror exposure therapy for treatment of body image disturbances, both dividuals with body image disturbances and ED; however, larger, ran-
in the presence and absence of ED, and as an optional component of domized controlled trials are needed to further validate the efficacy of
CBT for BDD. Mirror exposure therapy should be carried out under the and more completely characterize the side effects of mirror exposure
supervision of an experienced clinician who can screen for contra- therapy. The greatest areas of need for further clinical trials are: trials
indications and monitor for adverse events. Particular caution should with equal gender distributions or entirely male populations; trials
be exercised if mirror exposure therapy is used to treat individuals with testing the specific value of mirror exposure in BDD; trials of mirror
a history of self-harm, suicidality or with current clinical depression exposure in underweight and overweight populations with attention
based on adverse events observed in clinical trials (Delinsky & Wilson, paid to weight gain/loss during the trial; and trials in specific ED groups

171
T.C. Griffen et al. Clinical Psychology Review 65 (2018) 163–174

(AN, BN and BED). Additionally, every clinical trial explicitly including belief questionnaire: Preliminary development. Behaviour Therapy and Research,
a mirror exposure component that we have referenced was conducted 35(4), 381–388. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(96)0115-5.
Cooper, M. J., & Fairburn, C. G. (1992). Thoughts about eating, weight and shape in
in Europe or the United States of America. Whether mirror exposure anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. Behavior Research and Therapy, 30(5),
therapy has the same benefits and risks in other countries and cultures 501–511. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(92)90034-E.
is unknown. With these caveats in mind, we recommend mirror ex- Craske, M. G., Hermans, D., & Vervliet, B. (2018). State-of-the-art and future directions
for extinction as a translational model for fear and anxiety. Philosophical Transactions
posure therapy as an effective and generally well tolerated treatment of of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 373(1742), https://doi.org/10.
body image dissatisfaction to be administered by an experienced clin- 1098/rstb.2017.0025.
ician. Craske, M. G., Kircanski, K., Zelikowski, M., Mystkowski, J., Chowdhury, N., & Baker, A.
(2008). Optimizing inhibitory learning during exposure therapy. Behavior Research
and Therapy, 46(1), 5–27. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2007.10.003.
Role of funding sources Crino, N., Touyz, S., & Rieger, E. (2017). How eating disordered and non-eating dis-
ordered women differ in their use (and effectiveness) of cognitive self-regulation
strategies for managing negative experiences. Eating and Weight Disorders, 1–8.
The research did not receive any specific grant from funding
https://doi.org/10.1007/s40519-017-0448-z.
agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for profit sectors. Crown, S. (1983). Contraindications and dangers of psychotherapy. British Journal of
Psychiatry, 143(5), 436–441. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.143.5.436.
Contributors Davison, T. E., & McCabe, M. P. (2005). Relationships between men's and women's body
image and their psychological, social, and sexual functioning. Sex Roles, 52(7–8),
463–475. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-005-3712-z.
TCG wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors contributed Deckersbach, T., Savage, C. R., Phillips, K. A., Wilhelm, S., Buhlmann, U., Rauch, S. L., ...
to revising the manuscript and all authors have approved the final Jenike, M. A. (2000). Characteristics of memory dysfunction in body dysmorphic
disorder. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 6, 673–681.
manuscript. Delinsky, S. S., & Wilson, G. T. (2006). Mirror exposure for the treatment of body image
disturbance. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 39(2), 108–116. https://doi.org/
Conflict of interest 10.1002/eat.20207.
Díaz-Ferrer, S., Rodríguez-Ruiz, S., Ortega-Roldán, B., Mata-Martín, J. L., & Carmen
Fernández-Santaella, M. (2017). Psychophysiological changes during pure vs guided
All authors declare they have no conflicts of interest. mirror exposure therapies in women with high body dissatisfaction: What are they
learning about their bodies? European Eating Disorders Review, 25(6), 562–569.
https://doi.org/10.1002/erv.2546.
References Díaz-Ferrer, S., Rodríguez-Ruiz, S., Ortega-Roldán, B., Moreno-Domínguez, S., &
Fernández-Santaella, M. C. (2015). Testing the efficacy of pure versus guided mirror
Alleva, J. M., Sheeran, P., Webb, T. L., Martijn, C., & Miles, E. (2015). A meta-analytic exposure in women with bulimia nervosa: A combination of neuroendocrine and
review of stand-alone interventions to improve body image. PLoS One, 10(9), https:// psychological indices. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 48,
doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0139177. 1–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2015.01.003.
American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental dis- Dyer, A., Borgmann, E., Kleindienst, N., Feldmann, R. E., Vocks, S., & Bohus, M. (2013).
orders (5th ed.). Arlington, AV: American Psychiatric Publishing. Body image in patients with posttraumatic stress disorder after childhood sexual
Barlow, D. H. (2010). Negative Effects from Psychological Treatments: A Perspective. abuse and co-occurring eating disorder. Psychopathology, 46(3), 186–191. https://doi.
American Psychologist, 65(1), 13–20. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015643. org/10.1159/000341590.
Bauer, A., Schneider, S., Waldorf, M., Braks, K., Huber, T. J., Adolph, D., & Vocks, S. Dyer, A. S., Feldmann, R. E., & Borgmann, E. (2015). Body-related emotions in post-
(2017). Selective visual attention towards oneself and associated state body sa- traumatic stress disorder following childhood sexual abuse. Journal of Child Sexual
tisfaction: An eye-tracking study in adolescents with different types of eating dis- Abuse. https://doi.org/10.1080/10538712.2015.1057666.
orders. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 45(8), 1647–1661. https://doi.org/10. Enander, J., Andersson, E., Mataix-Cols, D., Lichtenstein, L., Alström, K., Andersson, G., ...
1007/s10802-017-0263-z. Rück, C. (2016). Therapist guided internet based cognitive behavioural therapy for
Beilharz, F., Castle, D. J., Grace, S., & Rossell, S. L. (2017). A systematic review of visual body dysmorphic disorder: Single blind randomised controlled trial. BMJ Clinical
processing and associated treatments in body dysmorphic disorder. Acta Psychiatrica Research Edition, 352, i241. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.i241.
Scandinavica. https://doi.org/10.1111/acps.12705. Engelhard, I. M., Leer, A., Lange, E., & Olatunji, B. O. (2014). Shaking that icky feeling:
Berman, M. I., Boutelle, K. N., & Crow, S. J. (2009). A case series investigating acceptance Effects of extinction and counterconditioning on disgust-related evaluative learning.
and commitment therapy as a treatment for previously treated, unremitted patients Behavior Therapy, 45(5), 708–719. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2014.04.003.
with anorexia nervosa. European Eating Disorders Review, 17(6), 426–434. https://doi. Faith, M. S., & Schare, M. L. (1993). The role of body image in sexually avoidant behavior.
org/10.1002/erv.962. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 22(4), 345–356. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01542123.
Bhatnagar, K. A., Wisniewski, L., Solomon, M., & Heinberg, L. (2013). Effectiveness and Fang, A., Schwartz, R. A., & Wilhelm, S. (2016). Treatment of an adult with body dys-
feasibility of a cognitive-behavioral group intervention for body image disturbance in morphic disorder. In E. Storch, & A. Lewin (Eds.). Clinical Handbook of Obsessive-
women with eating disorders. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 69(1), 1–13. https://doi. Compulsive and Related Disorders. Cham: Springer.
org/10.1002/jclp.21909. Farrell, C., Lee, M., & Shafran, R. (2005). Assessment of body size estimation: A review.
Borgmann, E., Kleindienst, N., Vocks, S., & Dyer, A. S. (2014). Standardized mirror European Eating Disorders Review, 13(2), 75–88. https://doi.org/10.1002/erv.622.
confrontation: Body-related emotions, cognitions and level of dissociation in patients Festinger, L. (1957). A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance. Stanford, CA: Stanford University
with Posttraumatic stress Disorder after childhood sexual abuse. Borderline Personality Presshttps://doi.org/10.1037/10318-001.
Disorder and Emotion Dysregulation, 1(1), 10. https://doi.org/10.1186/2051-6673- Foa, E. B., & McLean, C. P. (2016). The efficacy of exposure therapy for anxiety-related
1-10. disorders and its underlying mechanisms: The Case of OCD and PTSD. Annual Review
Buhlmann, U., Teachman, B. A., Naumann, E., Fehlinger, T., & Rief, W. (2009). The of Clinical Psychology, 12(1), 1–28. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-
meaning of beauty: Implicit and explicit self-esteem and attractiveness beliefs in body 021815-093533.
dysmorphic disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 23(5), 694–702. https://doi.org/ Gardner, R. M., & Brown, D. L. (2014). Body size estimation in anorexia nervosa: A brief
10.1016/j.janxdis.2009.02.008. review of findings from 2003 through 2013. Psychiatry Research. https://doi.org/10.
Buhlmann, U., Wilhelm, S., McNally, R. J., Tuschen-Caffier, B., Baer, L., & Jenike, M. A. 1016/j.psychres.2014.06.029.
(2002). Interpretive biases for ambiguous information in body dysmorphic disorder. Glashouwer, K. A., Jonker, N. C., Thomassen, K., & de Jong, P. J. (2016). Take a look at
CNS Spectrums, 7(6), 435–436. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1092852900017946. the bright side: Effects of positive body exposure on selective visual attention in
Bulik, C. M., Sullivan, P. F., Carter, F. A., McIntosh, V. V., & Joyce, P. R. (1998). The role women with high body dissatisfaction. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 83, 19–25.
of exposure with response prevention in the cognitive-behavioural therapy for bu- https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2016.05.006.
limia nervosa. Psychological Medicine, 28(3), 611–623. https://doi.org/10.1017/ Grant, J. E., & Phillips, K. A. (2005). Recognizing and treating body dysmorphic disorder.
S0033291798006618. Annals of Clinical Psychiatry.. https://doi.org/10.1080/10401230500295313.
Bystedt, S., Rozental, A., Andersson, G., Boettcher, J., & Carlbring, P. (2014). Clinicians' Greenberg, J. L., Markowitz, S., Petronko, M. R., Taylor, C. E., Wilhelm, S., & Wilson, G. T.
perspectives on negative effects of psychological treatments. Cognitive Behaviour (2010). Cognitive-behavioral therapy for adolescent body dysmorphic disorder.
Therapy, 43(4), 319–331. https://doi.org/10.1080/16506073.2014.939593. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 17(3), 248–258. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cbpra.
Cardi, V., Turton, R., Schifano, S., Leppanen, J., Hirsch, C. R., & Treasure, J. (2017). 2010.02.002.
Biased interpretation of ambiguous social scenarios in anorexia nervosa. European Greenberg, J. L., Mothi, S. S., & Wilhelm, S. (2016). Cognitive-behavioral therapy for
Eating Disorders Review, 25(1), 60–64. https://doi.org/10.1002/erv.2493. adolescent body dysmorphic disorder: A pilot Study. Behavior Therapy, 47(2),
Cash, T. F., & Szymanski, M. L. (1995). The development and validation of the body- 213–224. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2015.10.009.
image ideals questionnaire. Journal of Personality Assessment, 64(3), 466–477. Greenberg, J. L., Reuman, L., Hartmann, A. S., Kasarskis, I., & Wilhelm, S. (2014). Visual
https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa6403_6. hot spots: An eye tracking study of attention bias in body dysmorphic disorder.
Cooper, M. (1997). Bias in interpretation of ambiguous scenarios in eating disorders. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 57(1), 125–132. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35(7), 619–626. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005- 2014.06.015.
7967(97)00021-01. Grocholewski, A., Kliem, S., & Heinrichs, N. (2012). Selective attention to imagined facial
Cooper, M., Cohen-Tovée, E., Todd, G., Wells, A., & Tovée, M. (1997). The eating disorder ugliness is specific to body dysmorphic disorder. Body Image, 9(2), 261–269. https://

172
T.C. Griffen et al. Clinical Psychology Review 65 (2018) 163–174

doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2012.01.002. Moreno-Domínguez, S., Rodríguez-Ruiz, S., Fernández-Santaella, M. C., Jansen, A., &
Harrison, A., Fernández De La Cruz, L., Enander, J., Radua, J., & Mataix-Cols, D. (2016). Tuschen-Caffier, B. (2012). Pure versus guided mirror exposure to reduce body dis-
Cognitive-behavioral therapy for body dysmorphic disorder: A systematic review and satisfaction: A preliminary study with university women. Body Image, 9(2), 285–288.
meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Clinical Psychology Review, 48, 43–51. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2011.12.001.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2016.05.007. Morgan, J. F., Lazarova, S., Schelhase, M., & Saeidi, S. (2014). Ten session body image
Hilbert, A., & Tuschen-Caffier, B. (2004). Body image interventions in cognitive-beha- therapy: Efficacy of a manualised body image therapy. European Eating Disorders
vioural therapy of binge-eating disorder: A component analysis. Behaviour Research Review, 22(1), 66–71. https://doi.org/10.1002/erv.2249.
and Therapy, 42(11), 1325–1339. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2003.09.001. Morris, S. B. (2008). Estimating effect sizes from pretest-posttest-control group designs.
Hilbert, A., Tuschen-Caffier, B., & Vögele, C. (2002). Effects of prolonged and repeated Organizational Research Methods, 11(2), 364–386 (doi: 10.177/1094428106291059).
body image exposure in binge-eating disorder. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, Mountford, V. A., Brown, A., Bamford, B., Saeidi, S., Morgan, J. F., & Lacey, H. (2015).
52(3), 137–144. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-3999(01)00314-2. BodyWise: Evaluating a pilot body image group for patients with anorexia nervosa.
Hildebrandt, T., Loeb, K., Troupe, S., & Delinsky, S. (2012). Adjunctive mirror exposure European Eating Disorders Review, 23(1), 62–67. https://doi.org/10.1002/erv.2332.
for eating disorders: A randomized controlled pilot study. Behaviour Research and Naumann, E., Trentowska, M., & Svaldi, J. (2013). Increased salivation to mirror exposure
Therapy, 50(12), 797–804. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2012.09.004. in women with binge eating disorder. Appetite, 65, 103–110. https://doi.org/10.
Ingram, R. E. (1990). Self-focused attention in clinical disorders: Review and a conceptual 1016/j.appet.2013.01.021.
model. Psychological Bulletin, 107(2), 156–176. Neziroglu, F., Hickey, M., & McKay, D. (2010). Psychophysiological and self-report
Janelle, C. M., Hausenblas, H. A., Ellis, R., Coombes, S. A., & Duley, A. R. (2009). The time components of disgust in body dysmorphic disorder: The effects of repeated exposure.
course of attentional allocation while women high and low in body dissatisfaction International Journal of Cognitive Therapy, 3(1), 40–51. https://doi.org/10.1521/ijct.
view self and model physiques. Psychology and Health, 24(3), 351–366. https://doi. 2010.3.1.40.
org/10.1080/08870440701697367. Neziroglu, F., McKay, D., Todaro, J., & Yaryura-Tobias, J. A. (1996). Effect of cognitive
Jansen, A., Bollen, D., Tuschen-Caffier, B., Roefs, A., Tanghe, A., & Braet, C. (2008). behavior therapy on persons with body dysmorphic disorder and comorbid axis II
Mirror exposure reduces body dissatisfaction and anxiety in obese adolescents: A diagnoses. Behavior Therapy, 27(1), 67–77. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7894(96)
pilot study. Appetite, 51(1), 214–217. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2008.01.011. 80036-0.
Jansen, A., Nederkoorn, C., & Mulkens, S. (2005). Selective visual attention for ugly and Noles, S. W., Cash, T. F., & Winstead, B. A. (1985). Body image, physical attractiveness,
beautiful body parts in eating disorders. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43(2), and depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 53(1), 88–94. https://
183–196. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2004.01.003. doi.org/10.1037//0022-006X.53.1.88.
Jansen, A., Voorwinde, V., Hoebink, Y., Rekkers, M., Martijn, C., & Mulkens, S. (2016). Norris, D. L. (1984). The effects of mirror confrontation on self-estimation of body di-
Mirror exposure to increase body satisfaction: Should we guide the focus of attention mensions in anorexia nervosa, bulimia and two control groups. Psychological
towards positively or negatively evaluated body parts? Journal of Behavior Therapy Medicine, 14(4), 835–842. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291700019802.
and Experimental Psychiatry, 50, 90–96. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2015.06. Parsons, E. M., Straub, K. T., Smith, A. R., & Clerkin, E. M. (2017). Body dysmorphic,
002. obsessive-compulsive, and social anxiety disorder beliefs as predictors of in vivo
Johnson, F., & Wardle, J. (2005). Dietary restraint, body dissatisfaction, and psycholo- stressor responding. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 205(6), 471–479.
gical distress: A prospective analysis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114(1), https://doi.org/10.1097/NMD0000000000000656.
119–125. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.114.1.119. Pasanisi, F., Contaldo, F., de Simone, G., & Mancini, M. (2001). Benefits of sustained
Key, A., George, C. L., Beattie, D., Stammers, K., Lacey, H., & Waller, G. (2002). Body moderate weight loss in obesity. Nutrition, Metabolism, and Cardiovascular Diseases:
image treatment within an inpatient program for anorexia nervosa: The role of mirror NMCD, 11(6), 401–406.
exposure in the desensitization process. International Journal of Eating Disorders, Phillips, K. A., Hollander, E., Rasmussen, S. A., & Aronowitz, B. R. (1997). A severity
31(2), 185–190. rating scale for body dysmorphic disorder: Development, reliability, and validity of a
Klimek, P., Grotzinger, A., & Hildebrandt, T. (2016). Using acceptance to improve body modified version of the Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale. Psychopharmacology
image among individuals with eating disorders. In A. F. Haynos, E. M. Forman, M. L. Bulletin, 33(1), 17–22.
Butryn, & J. Lillis (Eds.). Mindfulness & acceptance for treating eating disorders & weight Phillips, K. A., & Kaye, W. H. (2007). The relationship of body dysmorphic disorder and
concerns: Evidence based interventions (pp. 121–142). Oakland, CA: New Harbinger eating disorders to obsessive-compulsive disorder. CNS Spectrums, 12(5), 347–358.
Publications. Phillips, K. A., Menard, W., & Bjornsson, A. S. (2013). Cued panic attacks in body dys-
Koch, P. B., Mansfield, P. K., Thurau, D., & Carey, M. (2005). “Feeling frumpy”: The morphic disorder. Journal of Psychiatric Practice, 19(3), 194–203. https://doi.org/10.
relationships between body image and sexual response changes in midlife women. 1097/01.pra.0000430503.16952.f0.
Journal of Sex Research, 42(3), 215–223. https://doi.org/10.1080/ Phillips, K. A., Menard, W., Fay, C., & Weisberg, R. (2005). Demographic characteristics,
00224490509552276. phenomenology, comorbidity, and family history in 200 individuals with body dys-
Kollei, I., Horndasch, S., Erim, Y., & Martin, A. (2017). Visual selective attention in body morphic disorder. Psychosomatics, 46(4), 317–325. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.
dysmorphic disorder, bulimia nervosa and healthy controls. Journal of Psychosomatic psy.46.4.317.
Research, 92, 26–33. https://doi.org/10.1016/jpsychores.2016.11.008. Phillips, K. A., & Rogers, J. (2011). Cognitive-behavioral therapy for youth with body
Krebs, G., Turner, C., Heyman, I., & Mataix-Cols, D. (2012). Cognitive behaviour therapy dysmorphic disorder: Current status and future directions. Child and Adolescent
for adolescents with body dysmorphic disorder: A case series. Behavioural and Psychiatric Clinics of North America. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chc.2011.01.004.
Cognitive Psychotherapy, 40(4), 452–461. https://doi.org/10.1017/ Rabiei, M., Mulkens, S., Kalantari, M., Molavi, H., & Bahrami, F. (2012). Metacognitive
S1352465812000100. therapy for body dysmorphic disorder patients in Iran: Acceptability and proof of
Lang, K., Roberts, M., Harrison, A., Lopez, C., Goddard, E., Khondoker, M., ... Tchanturia, concept. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 43(2), 724–729.
K. (2016). Central coherence in eating disorders: A synthesis of studies using the Rey https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2011.09.013.
Osterrieth complex figure task. PLoS One, 11(11), e0165467. https://doi.org/10. Roefs, A., Jansen, A., Moresi, S., Willems, P., van Grootel, S., & van der Borgh, A. (2008).
1371/journal.pone.016547. Looking good. BMI, attractiveness bias and visual attention. Appetite, 51(3), 552–555.
Leitenberg, H., Rosen, J. C., Gross, J., Nudelman, S., & Vara, L. S. (1988). Exposure plus https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2008.04.008.
response-prevention treatment of bulimia nervosa. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Rosen, L. W. (1981). Self-control program in the treatment of obesity. Journal of Behavior
Psychology, 56(4), 535–541. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.56.4.535. Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 12(2), 163–166. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-
Levinson, C. A., Rodebaugh, T. L., Fewell, L., Kass, A. E., Riley, E. N., Stark, L., ... Lenze, E. 7916(81)90011-2.
J. (2015). D-cycloserine facilitation of exposure therapy improves weight regain in Rosen, J. C., Reiter, J., & Orosan, P. (1995). Cognitive-behavioral body image therapy for
patients with anorexia nervosa: A pilot randomized controlled trial. Journal of Clinical body dysmorphic disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 63(2),
Psychiatry, 76(6), e787–e793. https://doi.org/10.4088/JCP.14m09299. 263–269. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.63.2.263.
Luethcke, C. A., McDaniel, L., & Becker, C. B. (2011). A comparison of mindfulness, Sawaoka, T., Barnes, R. D., Blomquist, K. K., Masheb, R. M., & Grilo, C. M. (2012). Social
nonjudgmental, and cognitive dissonance-based approaches to mirror exposure. Body anxiety and self-consciousness in binge eating disorder: Associations with eating
Image, 8(3), 251–258. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2011.03.006. disorder psychopathology. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 53(6), 740–745. https://doi.
Mataix-Cols, D., Fernández De La Cruz, L., Isomura, K., Anson, M., Turner, C., Monzani, org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2011.10.003.
B., ... Krebs, G. (2015). A pilot randomized controlled trial of cognitive-behavioral Scheffers, M., van Busschbach, J. T., Bosscher, R. J., Aerts, L. C., Wiersma, D., &
therapy for adolescents with body dysmorphic disorder. Journal of the American Schoevers, R. A. (2017). Body image in patients with mental disorders:
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 54(11), 895–904. https://doi.org/10. Characteristics, associations with diagnosis and treatment outcome. Comprehensive
1016/j.jaac.2015.08.011. Psychiatry, 74, 53–60. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2017.01.004.
McKay, D., Sookman, D., Neziroglu, F., Wilhelm, S., Stein, D. J., Kyrios, M., ... Veale, D. Schweckendiek, J., Klucken, T., Merz, C. J., Kagerer, S., Walter, B., Vaitl, D., & Stark, R.
(2015). Efficacy of cognitive-behavioral therapy for obsessive-compulsive disorder. (2013). Learning to like disgust: Neuronal correlates of counterconditioning. Frontiers
Psychiatry Research, 227(1), 104–113. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2015.02. in Human Neuroscience, 7, 346. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00346.
004. Seal, B. N., & Meston, C. M. (2007). The impact of body awareness on sexual arousal in
Mölbert, S. C., Klein, L., Thaler, A., Mohler, B. J., Brozzo, C., Martus, P., ... Giel, K. E. women with sexual dysfunction. The Journal of Sexual Medicine, 4(4), 990–1000.
(2017). Depictive and metric body size estimation in anorexia nervosa and bulimia https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1743-6109.2007.00525.x Pt 1.
nervosa: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 57, Servián-Franco, F., Moreno-Domínguez, S., & Reyes Del Paso, G. A. (2015). Body dis-
21–31. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2017.08.005. satisfaction and mirror exposure: Evidence for a dissociation between self-report and
Mölbert, S. C., Thaler, A., Mohler, B. J., Streuber, S., Romero, J., Black, M. J., ... Giel, K. E. physiological responses in highly body-dissatisfied women. PLoS One, 10(4), https://
(2018). Assessing body image in anorexia nervosa using biometric self-avatars in doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0122737.
virtual reality: Attitudinal components rather than visual body size estimation are Shafran, R., & Fairburn, C. G. (2002). A new ecologically valid method to assess body size
distorted. Psychological Medicine, 48(4), 642–653. https://doi.org/10.1017/ estimation and body size dissatisfaction. International Journal of Eating Disorders,
S0033291717002008. 32(4), 458–465. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.10097.

173
T.C. Griffen et al. Clinical Psychology Review 65 (2018) 163–174

Shafran, R., Fairburn, C. G., Robinson, P., & Lask, B. (2004). Body checking and its Veale, D., & Riley, S. (2001). Mirror, mirror on the wall, who is the ugliest of them all?
avoidance in eating disorders. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 35(1), 93–101. The psychopathology of mirror gazing in body dysmorphic disorder. Behavior
https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.10228. Research and Therapy, 39(12), 1381–1393.
Shafran, R., Lee, M., Payne, E., & Fairburn, C. G. (2007). An experimental analysis of body Vocks, S., Legenbauer, T., Wächter, A., Wucherer, M., & Kosfelder, J. (2007). What
checking. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45(1), 113–121. https://doi.org/10.1016/ happens in the course of body exposure? Emotional, cognitive, and physiological
j.brat.2006.01.015. reactions to mirror confrontation in eating disorders. Journal of Psychosomatic
Smeets, E., Jansen, A., & Roefs, A. (2011). Bias for the (un)Attractive self: On the role of Research, 62(2), 231–239. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychores.2006.08.007.
attention in causing body (dis)satisfaction. Health Psychology, 30(3), 360–367. Vocks, S., Wächter, A., Wucherer, M., & Kosfelder, J. (2008). Look at yourself: Can body
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022095. image therapy affect the cognitive and emotional response to seeing oneself in the
Steinglass, J. E., Albano, A. M., Simpson, H. B., Wang, Y., Zou, J., Attia, E., & Walsh, B. T. mirror in eating disorders? European Eating Disorders Review, 16(2), 147–154. https://
(2014). Confronting fear using exposure and response prevention for anorexia ner- doi.org/10.1002/erv.825.
vosa: A randomized controlled pilot study. International Journal of Eating Disorders, Von Wietersheim, J., Kunzl, F., Hoffmann, H., Glaub, J., Rottler, E., & Traue, H. C. (2012).
47(2), 174–180. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.22214. Selective attention of patients with anorexia nervosa while looking at pictures of their
Stice, E., & Shaw, H. E. (2002). Role of body dissatisfaction in the onset and maintenance own body and the bodies of others: An exploratory study. Psychosomatic Medicine,
of eating pathology: A synthesis of research findings. Journal of Psychosomatic 74(1), 107–113. https://doi.org/10.1097/PSY.0b013e31823ba787.
Research. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-3999(02)00488-9. Walker, D. C., Murray, A. D., Lavender, J. M., & Anderson, D. A. (2012). The direct effects
Summers, B. J., & Cougle, J. R. (2016). Modifying interpretation biases in body dys- of manipulating body checking in men. Body Image, 9(4), 462–468. https://doi.org/
morphic disorder: Evaluation of a brief computerized treatment. Behaviour Research 10.1016/j.bodyim.2012.06.001.
and Therapy, 87, 117–127. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2016.09.005. Warschburger, P., Calvano, C., Richter, E. M., & Engbert, R. (2015). Analysis of atten-
Svaldi, J., Bender, C., Caffier, D., Ivanova, V., Mies, N., Fleischhaker, C., & Tuschen- tional bias towards attractive and unattractive body regions among overweight males
Caffier, B. (2016). Negative mood increases selective attention to negatively valenced and females: An Eye-Movement Study. PLoS One, 10(10), https://doi.org/10.1371/
body parts in female adolescents with anorexia nervosa. PLoS One, 11(4), https://doi. journal.pone.0140813.
org/10.1371/journal.pone.0154462. Weinberger, N. A., Kersting, A., Riedel-Heller, S. G., & Luck-Sikorski, C. (2016). Body
Svaldi, J., Caffier, D., & Tuschen-Caffier, B. (2011). Attention to ugly body parts is in- dissatisfaction in individuals with obesity compared to normal-weight individuals: A
creased in women with binge eating disorder. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, systematic review and meta-analysis. Obesity Facts, 9(6), 424–441. https://doi.org/
80(3), 186–188. https://doi.org/10.1159/000317538. 10.1159/000454837.
Svaldi, J., Zimmermann, S., & Naumann, E. (2012). The impact of an implicit manip- Weingarden, H., Marques, L., Fang, A., LeBlanc, N., Buhlmann, U., Phillips, K. A., &
ulation of self-esteem on body dissatisfaction. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Wilhelm, S. (2011). Culturally adapted cognitive behavioral therapy for body dys-
Experimental Psychiatry, 43(1), 581–586. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2011.08. morphic disorder: Case examples. International Journal of Cognitive Therapy, 4(4),
003. 381–396. https://doi.org/10.1521/ijct.2011.4.4.381.
Sweldens, S., Corneille, O., & Yzerbyt, V. (2014). The role of awareness in attitude for- Wilhelm, S., Buhlmann, U., Hayward, L. C., Greenberg, J. L., & Dimaite, R. (2010). A
mation through evaluative conditioning. Personality and Social Psychology Review, cognitive-behavioral treatment approach for body dysmorphic disorder. Cognitive and
18(2), 187–209. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868314527832. Behavioral Practice, 17(3), 241–247. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cbpra.2010.02.001.
Toh, W. L., Castle, D. J., & Rossell, S. L. (2017). How individuals with body dysmorphic Wilhelm, S., Otto, M. W., Lohr, B., & Deckersbach, T. (1999). Cognitive behavior group
disorder (BDD) process their own face: A quantitative and qualitative investigation therapy for body dysmorphic disorder: A case series. Behaviour Research and Therapy,
based on an eye-tracking paradigm. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 22(3), 213–232. 37(1), 71–75. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(98)00109-0.
https://doi.org/10.1080/13546805.2017.1300090. Wilhelm, S., Phillips, K. A., Didie, E., Buhlmann, U., Greenberg, J. L., Fama, J. M., ...
Trentowska, M., Bender, C., & Tuschen-Caffier, B. (2013). Mirror exposure in women with Steketee, G. (2014). Modular cognitive-behavioral therapy for body dysmorphic
bulimic symptoms: How do thoughts and emotions change in body image treatment? disorder: A randomized controlled trial. Behavior Therapy, 45(3), 314–327. https://
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 51(1), 1–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2012.03. doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2013.12.007.
012. Wilhelm, S., Phillips, K. A., Fama, J. M., Greenberg, J. L., & Steketee, G. (2011). Modular
Trentowska, M., Svaldi, J., Blechert, J., & Tuschen-Caffier, B. (2017). Does habituation cognitive-behavioral therapy for body dysmorphic disorder. Behavior Therapy, 42(4),
really happen? Investigation of psycho-biological responses to body exposure in 624–633. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2011.02.002.
bulimia nervosa. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 90, 111–122. https://doi.org/10. Wilson, G. T., Eldredge, K. L., Smith, D., & Niles, B. (1991). Cognitive-behavioral treat-
1016/j.brat.2016.12.006. ment with and without response prevention for bulimia. Behaviour Research and
Trentowska, M., Svaldi, J., & Tuschen-Caffier, B. (2014). Efficacy of body exposure as Therapy, 29(6), 575–583.
treatment component for patients with eating disorders. Journal of Behavior Therapy Windheim, K., Veale, D., & Anson, M. (2011). Mirror gazing in body dysmorphic disorder
and Experimental Psychiatry, 45(1), 178–185. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2013. and healthy controls: Effects of duration of gazing. Behaviour Research and Therapy,
09.010. 49(9), 555–564. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2011.05.003.
Turton, R., Cardi, V., Treasure, J., & Hirsch, C. R. (2018). Modifying negative inter- Zucker, N., Wagner, H. R., Merwin, R., Bulik, C. M., Moskovich, A., Keeling, L., & Hoyle,
pretation bias for ambiguous social scenarios that depict the risk of rejection in R. (2015). Self-focused attention in anorexia nervosa. International Journal of Eating
women with anorexia nervosa. Journal of Affective Disorders, 227, 705–712. https:// Disorders, 48(1), 9–14. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.22307.
doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2017.11.089.
Tuschen-Caffier, B., Bender, C., Caffier, D., Klenner, K., Braks, K., & Svaldi, J. (2015). Trevor C. Griffen, MD, PhD is a Resident Psychiatrist at the Mount Sinai Hospital, where
Selective visual attention during mirror exposure in anorexia and bulimia nervosa. he divides his time between practicing medicine and clinically oriented research. He
PLoS One, 10(12), https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0145886. completed both medical and graduate school at Stony Brook University.
Tuschen-Caffier, B., Pook, M., & Frank, M. (2001). Evaluation of manual-based cognitive-
behavioral therapy for bulimia nervosa in a service setting. Behaviour Research and
Therapy, 39(3), 299–308. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(00)00004-8. Tom Hildebrandt, PsyD, FAED is the Chief of the Division of Eating and Weight
Veale, D., Anson, M., Miles, S., Pieta, M., Costa, A., & Ellison, N. (2014). Efficacy of Disorders (Center of Excellence) at Mount Sinai and an Associate Professor of Psychiatry
at the Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai. He completed his graduate training at
cognitive behaviour therapy versus anxiety management for body dysmorphic dis-
order: A randomised controlled trial. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 83(6), Rutgers University and his post-doctoral fellowship at the Icahn School of Medicine at
341–353. https://doi.org/10.1159/000360740. Mount Sinai. He is an active clinician and currently oversees the development and ex-
Veale, D., Gournay, K., Dryden, W., Boocock, A., Shah, F., Willson, R., & Walburn, J. ecution of both the clinical and research programs.
(1996). Body dysmorphic disorder: A cognitive behavioural model and pilot rando-
mised controlled trial. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 34(9), 717–729. https://doi. Eva Naumann, PhD is a Visiting Research Fellow at the Eating and Weight Disorders
org/10.1016/0005-7967(96)00025-3. Program at Mount Sinai. She completed her graduate training at the University of
Veale, D., Miles, S., Valiallah, N., Butt, S., Anson, M., Eshkevari, E., ... Baldock, E. (2016). Marburg and worked as a research assistant at the University of Freiburg. She received
The effect of self-focused attention and mood on appearance dissatisfaction after her doctorate in Clinical Psychology from the University of Tuebingen and her license as a
mirror-gazing: An experimental study. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental psychotherapist from FAVT in Freiburg, Germany.
Psychiatry, 52, 38–44. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2016.03.002.

174

You might also like