Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 267

Early Trauma as

the Origin of Chronic


Inflammation

A Psychoneuroimmunological
Perspective
Rainer H. Straub

123
Early Trauma as the Origin of Chronic
Inflammation
Rainer H. Straub

Early Trauma as the Origin


of Chronic Inflammation
A Psychoneuroimmunological Perspective
Rainer H. Straub
Division of Rheumatology
Department of Internal Medicine
University Hospital
Regensburg, Germany

ISBN 978-3-662-66750-7 ISBN 978-3-662-66751-4 (eBook)


https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-662-66751-4

© The Editor(s) (if applicable) and The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer-Verlag GmbH, DE, part
of Springer Nature 2023
This work is subject to copyright. All rights are solely and exclusively licensed by the Publisher, whether
the whole or part of the material is concerned, specifically the rights of translation, reprinting, reuse
of illustrations, recitation, broadcasting, reproduction on microfilms or in any other physical way, and
transmission or information storage and retrieval, electronic adaptation, computer software, or by similar or
dissimilar methodology now known or hereafter developed.
The use of general descriptive names, registered names, trademarks, service marks, etc. in this publication does
not imply, even in the absence of a specific statement, that such names are exempt from the relevant protective
laws and regulations and therefore free for general use.
The publisher, the authors, and the editors are safe to assume that the advice and information in this book
are believed to be true and accurate at the date of publication. Neither the publisher nor the authors or the
editors give a warranty, expressed or implied, with respect to the material contained herein or for any errors
or omissions that may have been made. The publisher remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in
published maps and institutional affiliations.

Responsible Editor: Christine Lerche


This Springer imprint is published by the registered company Springer-Verlag GmbH, DE, part of Springer
Nature.
The registered company address is: Heidelberger Platz 3, 14197 Berlin, Germany
Dedicated to my doctoral students and pupils
Foreword

Life events in childhood and adolescence have a lasting impact on a person's personal-
ity development, both in a positive and a negative sense. These functional connections
are not only postulated by the various psychological schools and psychotherapeutic
approaches from psychoanalysis to cognitive behavioral therapy, but are now also con-
firmed by many empirical studies.
As an experimental behavioral scientist and clinically active behavioral thera-
pist, even after more than 30 years of therapeutic work, I am always impressed by the
close connections between the current psychological problems my patients report and
the described experiences and events from their childhood and adolescence. It is not
always only traumatic experiences such as sexual abuse, physical abuse or emotional
neglect that are related to the acute psychological complaints such as anxiety, depressive
symptoms or somatoform dysfunctions. Often, divorce of the parents and the resulting
insecurity or high performance expectations of the parents cause the development of
psychological problems in later adult life. These stressful childhood experiences affect
not only mental health, but also physical processes. Since its beginnings, psychosomatic
medicine has postulated the connections between traumatic childhood experiences and
the development of physical illnesses such as chronic inflammatory processes. For a long
time, however, only speculation was possible about these connections, since too little
was known about the functions of the immune system and the corresponding connections
with the nervous and hormonal systems.
Through the new academic and research field of psychoneuroimmunology, the interac-
tions between systems have become known in increasing detail in recent years, and the
interplay between systems appears to be much closer and more complex than previously
thought. For example, a recent high-profile publication in the journal Cell showed that
information from a subsided inflammatory response in the gut is stored in a special brain
area called the insular cortex. When the neurons in the insular cortex are reactivated in
the mice using chemogenetic methods, a renewed inflammatory response in the intestinal
tissue occurs in the animals (Koren et al. 2022, Cell, 184 5902).

vii
viii Foreword

As a physician and one of the internationally recognized protagonists of psychoneuro-


immunology, Professor Rainer H. Straub has contributed groundbreaking basic scientific
findings on bidirectional communications between the nervous, endocrine, and immune
systems, which have significantly contributed to a better understanding of these interac-
tions between the systems for the development and course of immunologically related
diseases. In this book, Rainer H. Straub uses his profound knowledge accumulated over
many years to provide an excellent summary of previous knowledge and empirical find-
ings on the functional connections between traumatic experiences in childhood and ado-
lescence and the development of chronic inflammatory diseases in later adulthood.
Rainer H. Straub describes the psychological foundations just as competently as the
historical development, the evolutionary-biological background, the genetic-epigenetic
and, of course, the endocrinological and immunological correlations that prove to be
responsible for chronic inflammatory diseases after early traumas. The author does not
merely loosely string together individual empirical findings; rather, he understands how
to bring these sometimes complex interrelationships to the reader in an understandable
way in an entertaining narrative style.
Overall, this is an impressive book that presents this exciting topic in a unique way,
and I wish it a wide range of readers.

Essen Manfred Schedlowski


25 February 2022
Preface

The population on this earth is growing and growing, and this is the cause of many prob-
lems. New large populations are joining the globalized work force. Resources are being
consumed, the environment is being increasingly polluted and the world of work is being
seriously changed. The family with two working parents is the norm. This brings an
increase in stressful life situations, especially in families with precarious circumstances.
In addition, there are mental illnesses as a result of stress and environmental influences.
The migration of people seeking protection and the confrontation with the local popu-
lation is another stress factor. The difficulties cannot all be listed here in detail, but the
major victims are often the children and young people. The Corona pandemic also shows
this.
The famous American dancer Isadora Duncan (1877–1927) once wrote: “As long as
little children are allowed to suffer, there is no true love in this world.” Karl Menninger
(1893–1990), American psychiatrist, added: “What’s done to children, they will do to
society.”
Already around conception, during the time in utero, through childhood and in ado-
lescence, people are exposed to more and more stress factors. These stressful situa-
tions influence individual development in different time windows, which can be linked
to problems in adulthood. Throughout life, these stress factors accumulate, and this can
lead to lasting psychological and physical problems in later stages of life.
My goal in writing this book was, first, to raise public awareness of the important
long-term physical and psychological effects of early trauma in children and adoles-
cents. While this is a central task of psychotraumatologists, it also concerns the inter-
ested immunologist, as you will see in the next paragraph. It is not a question of “What
is wrong with this child?” but a question of “What happened to this child?” And another
question is, “Why can there be such long-term problems that extend into adulthood?”
My second goal as a psycho-neuro-endocrine oriented immunologist was, on the
other hand, to shed light on the often detectable increased inflammatory state after
early traumas. This chronic immune activation is shown by increased inflammation lev-
els in the blood. It may lead to an increased incidence of autoimmune diseases such as

ix
x Preface

rheumatoid arthritis (the most common form of chronic joint inflammation) later in life,
and this concerns the rheumatologist. However, it is not yet well understood why an
early traumatic event or stressful situation should cause a higher inflammatory situation
in the long run. There must be some kind of programming that originates in the brain and
subsequently affects the body periphery in the form of increased inflammation.
The reader is introduced to the topic of trauma through examples in the first chapter.
In the second chapter, the different trauma situations, the time of occurrence and the fre-
quencies of childhood adversities in the population are pointed out. Furthermore, those
factors that can protect against long-term consequences are mentioned. Indeed, it is by
no means just a “one-way street from trauma to late problems,” as there are many pro-
tective elements (the subject of resilience research, among others). This attitude reflects
the view of those affected who put the trauma behind them and yet look positively to the
future. This part is concluded by an evolutionary medical view.
In Chap. 3, I deal with the consequential problems found in the brain and body
periphery. Pathophysiological pathways are outlined, and this requires some medical
depth. This discussion transitions into the presentation of the elevated inflammatory
situation, with a focus on the chronic situation, also known as autoimmune disease.
Epidemiological studies show a clear link between childhood/adolescent trauma and
later onset of these autoaggressive diseases.
The fourth chapter now explains step by step how a problem in the brain can affect
the body periphery to cause chronic inflammation there. The brain and immune system
are intertwined via “connectors,” and these contribute to chronic immune activation. The
Chap. 5 looks into the future. On the basis of energy considerations, a hypothesis is put
forward which reads: Mild inflammation exists, but it does not decisively stimulate the
late problems. This last statement may be surprising for one or the other reader, because
he understands inflammation as the supporting pillar of all subsequent problems, particu-
larly in the periphery. This mild inflammation is for me only a concomitant phenomenon
in the context of the energy regulation of the body, a support of the immune system for
an overactive brain.
This book is intended for researchers already studying psychological trauma and late
effects, as well as those who wish to engage in this growing area of research, includ-
ing students, and individuals who wish to improve the lives of vulnerable children.
Professionals in medicine, psychology, psychiatry, social work, education, sociology,
nursing, pediatrics, public health, applied economics, humanitarian aid, and disaster
planning may find useful ideas and background for their work. A basic understanding of
medicine is beneficial for reading.
As in my previous books, I do not make any suggestions for therapy, because that is
presumptuous, because there are specialists in psychotraumatology for that. If you read
the book carefully, you can see approaches to therapy in abundance. Often these therapy
options consist of preventing the bad and stimulating the good.
Preface xi

A book like this is never created single-handedly, so a few very helpful people have
given good advice here. The book was critically read by my wife Verena Straub. Another
big thank you goes to Dr. Volker von Baehr, IMD Institute for Medical Diagnostics,
Berlin, who supported the printing of the book. From the Springer-Verlag, valuable help
came from Dr. Christine Lerche. If inclined readers provide further tips, I am grateful.
Improvements will be collected and added to a later edition.

Regensburg Rainer H. Straub


Summer 2022
Contents

1 The Long Shadow of Early Trauma—Look!. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1


1.1 Mental Illness. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1
1.2 Pain in Childhood—and What Comes Later?. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2
1.3 How can I Sleep While My Bed is Burning?. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3
1.4 Teeth Suffer. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4
1.5 Target Variable: Weight. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4
1.6 Cardiovascular Diseases . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 6
1.7 Chronic Lung Problems. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7
1.8 And then Chronic Inflammation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 8
1.9 To the Point . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 9
References. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 9
2 What is a Child’s Psychological Trauma? . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 13
2.1 Origins and Important Protagonists. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 13
2.1.1 Urie Bronfenbrenner, a Psychologist (1917–2005). . . . . . . . . . . . . 13
2.1.2 John Bowlby, a Child Psychiatrist (1907–1990). . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 14
2.1.3 Michael Rutter, Psychiatrist (1933–2021). . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 15
2.1.4 David Barker, Social Medicine and Epidemiologist
(1938–2013). . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 16
2.1.5 Vincent J. Felitti, an Internist (Born 1938) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 16
2.1.6 Robert J. Plomin, a Psychologist (Born 1948). . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 18
2.1.7 W. Thomas Boyce (Born 1943), a Pediatrician, and Jay
Belsky (Born 1952), a Psychologist. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 18
2.1.8 Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 19
2.2 The Different Types of Early Traumatic Experiences. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 19
2.3 Time Windows for Bad Childhood Experiences. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 22
2.4 Frequency of Bad Childhood Experiences. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 24
2.5 Compensating Positive Factors Against Trauma Experiences. . . . . . . . . . . 26
2.5.1 Resilience. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 26
2.5.2 Unfavorable Versus Favorable Childhood Experiences. . . . . . . . . . 29

xiii
xiv Contents

2.5.3 Orchid or Dandelion. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 32


2.5.4 Favorable or Unfavorable Genetic Predisposition. . . . . . . . . . . . . . 33
2.5.5 Favorable or Unfavorable Epigenetic Changes. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 41
2.6 Child Disadvantages and Steeling Effects. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 44
2.7 Transmission of Behavior from Generation to Generation. . . . . . . . . . . . . 47
2.7.1 Genetic Transmission. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 47
2.7.2 Epigenetic Transmission . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 48
2.7.3 Transmission—without Genetic and Epigenetic Explanations. . . . 51
2.8 An Evolutionary Medicine Perspective. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 52
2.8.1 Historical Development. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 52
2.8.2 Childhood Trauma and Evolutionary Medicine—the Results. . . . . 55
2.8.3 Gender Differences—an Evolutionary Medicine Perspective. . . . . 57
2.9 To the Point . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 58
References. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 59
3 Consequences of Early Traumatic Experiences. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 71
3.1 Many Places are Affected . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 71
3.2 The Brain Probably Suffers the Most . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 72
3.2.1 Alcohol, Nicotine and Drugs. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 72
3.2.2 Depression. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 75
3.2.3 Anxiety Disorders . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 79
3.2.4 Personality and Childhood Trauma. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 81
3.2.5 More Psycho- and Neuropathology. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 82
3.2.6 More Sleep Disorders as a Result of Previous Trauma. . . . . . . . . . 84
3.2.7 More Pain after Childhood Adversities. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 86
3.2.8 Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 89
3.3 The Body Periphery Suffers as Well . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 90
3.3.1 High Body Weight. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 91
3.3.2 Heart Attack and Co.. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 96
3.3.3 Chronic Lung Diseases . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 98
3.3.4 The Stomach Aches and the Stool Consistency Bothers. . . . . . . . . 100
3.3.5 And When We Get Old?. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 101
3.4 And Finally the Immune System is Activated. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 108
3.4.1 Early Trauma and Autoimmunity. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 109
3.5 To the Point . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 111
References. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 112
4 Chronic Immune System Activation. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 135
4.1 Egoistic Brain and Egoistic Immune System are Fourfold
Interconnected. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 135
4.1.1 Areas in the Brain that Stimulate Direct and Indirect
Connectors. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 137
Contents xv

4.2 Direct Connectors Chronically Activate the Immune


System (No. 1). . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 140
4.2.1 The Sympathetic Nervous System. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 141
4.2.2 The Parasympathetic nervous system . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 153
4.2.3 The HPA Axis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 154
4.2.4 Disruption of the Circadian Rhythm. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 164
4.2.5 Tone of the Pain Pathways. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 166
4.2.6 Tone of the Sex Hormone Axis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 169
4.3 Indirect Connectors Activate the Immune System
Chronically (No. 2) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 174
4.3.1 Adipose Tissue is Pro-inflammatory . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 174
4.3.2 The Insulin from the Pancreas Promotes Inflammation. . . . . . . . . . 177
4.3.3 Reduced Physical Activity—Anti-inflammatory Factors are
Missing. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 179
4.3.4 Microbiome and Inflammation. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 180
4.3.5 The Permeability of the Gut is Pro-inflammatory. . . . . . . . . . . . . . 182
4.3.6 The Skin Itches Chronically and is Inflamed. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 183
4.4 Environmental Factors Chronically Activate the Immune
System (No. 3). . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 184
4.4.1 Where There’s Smoke, There’s Fire. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 184
4.4.2 Where Fine Particles are Flying, It Gets Uncomfortable. . . . . . . . . 187
4.4.3 Asthma. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 189
4.4.4 Infections are Extra-corporeal Connectors . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 192
4.4.5 Risk Behavior. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 195
4.5 Gene Mutations as Pleiotropic Connectors (No. 4). . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 196
4.5.1 The Search in the Gene Databases Leads to Pleiotropic
Connectors. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 198
4.6 Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 201
4.7 To the Point . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 203
References. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 204
5 Energy, Early Traumatic Experiences and Chronic Immune System
Activation. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 233
5.1 Brain and Immune System Define Energy Expenditure . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 234
5.2 The Trauma Reaction can Use a Lot and a Little Energy . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 238
5.3 Is there a Trauma and Energy Provision Memory?. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 239
5.4 Energy and Chronic Immune System Activation. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 240
5.4.1 How High is Inflammation in People with Early Traumatic
Experiences? . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 240
5.4.2 How High is Energy Expenditure at Different Levels of
Inflammation? . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 241
xvi Contents

5.4.3 The Quintessence. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 243


5.5 To the Point . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 245
References. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 246

Glossary. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 249
About the Author

Prof. Dr. med. Rainer H. Straub is Professor of Experi­


mental Medicine and Internal Rheumatologist. He heads the
Laboratory of Experimental Rheumatology and Neuroen­
docrine Immunology, Internal Medicine, at the University
Hospital Regensburg. His non-fiction books “Understanding
Aging, Fatigue and Inflammation” (2018) and “Three Mem­
ories for the Body” (2020) have been published by Springer.

xvii
The Long Shadow of Early
Trauma—Look! 1

1.1 Mental Illness

Children are particularly vulnerable because everything can still take shape and change.
The problems affect a sensitive, constantly changing brain that is open to everything.
Scientists speak of plasticity of the brain. This plasticity can be bad and good, but in
any case it contributes significantly to the individual development of the person. Serious
stress-inducing constellation can be drug or alcohol abuse by parents, separation of par-
ents, physical and sexual abuse, death of close relatives (parents, siblings), mental illness
in one parent, emotional neglect or overburdening, deprivation of liberty of the child,
hospital stays, separation from the parental home, long-term unemployment of parents,
arrival of a new adult household member (step-parents), change of school and bullying
at school, low socio-economic status, observation of a criminal act or imprisonment of a
parent, etc. A detailed list will be presented in the next chapter. These mental and physi-
cal injuries lead to severe child stress, which affects children differently.
In comparison to control groups, children with such traumas attempt suicide 5 times
more often, injure themselves 3 times more often, have an attention deficit 2 times more
often, suffer from anxiety disorders 2 times more often and depressions 3 times as often,
almost 3 times as often from psychotic symptoms (loss of reality), are more addicted to
alcohol and drugs and stand out due to social behavior disorders (Danese 2020).
When scientists investigate mentally ill offenders of a forensic psychiatric depart-
ment, as was done, for example, in Scotland in 2013, almost 80% of the inmates reported
having had severe traumatic experiences in childhood. Often there were chaotic fam-
ily relationships because one parent left the family (32%) or took alcohol/drugs (25%).
In total, 23% of the affected were sexually abused, bullied at school (21%), or had to
change schools several times (21%). These stressed children and later offenders have a

© The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer-Verlag GmbH, DE, part of Springer 1
Nature 2023
R. H. Straub, Early Trauma as the Origin of Chronic Inflammation,
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-662-66751-4_1
2 1 The Long Shadow of Early Trauma—Look!

very high risk of suicide and death shortly after release from prison. The risk of death is
29 times higher than in the comparable group of healthy people (Karatzias et al. 2019).
The matter goes even deeper because transmission from one generation to the next
can be observed. If, for example, mothers were themselves exposed to stressful episodes
during childhood, their children also had more stressful circumstances during childhood
and psychological and emotional abnormalities later in life (Negriff et al. 2020).
The misconduct is programmed during childhood and continues into adulthood.
However, not all adults with psychiatric problems have childhood trauma. Childhood
stress is not an absolute prerequisite for psychiatric illness, but it can significantly favor
it.

1.2 Pain in Childhood—and What Comes Later?

We have neuronal pathways for exactly localized pain in the area of muscles,​​ bones,
ligaments, tendons and skin (we call them somatic pain pathways). Another group of
pain-sensing nerve fibers comes from the intestines such as lungs, heart, stomach, intes-
tine, bladder, etc. (we call them visceral pain pathways). The paths for somatic and vis-
ceral pain are different, but all paths end up in the brain, and so pain from different areas
comes into consciousness.
Interestingly, we can create a pain memory, which is most clearly seen when we think
of phantom pain. This form of pain exists in an amputated body part as if that region still
belonged to our body. Think of a soldier in a hospital whose foot had to be amputated
due to healing disorders and wound pain and who still suffers from these wound pains—
the phantom pains—many years later. This memory provides an unpleasant reminder,
and here we see a long-term programming, a long-term memory. What does it have to do
with childhood adversities?
Sometimes newborns have to be treated in an intensive care unit if, for example, they
were born too early or have a serious illness. Here they experience various pains during
blood withdrawal, when an infusion is applied, after surgery, in connection with wounds,
with dry and strained skin and some other painful situations. Up to 14 painful events
can occur per day (Maxwell et al. 2019). Newborns are particularly vulnerable, but can
hardly defend themselves. This is a special form of childhood stress that is comparable to
the early childhood stress situations already mentioned.
These pains lead to chronic changes in a slowly developing brain that functions differ-
ently in newborns than in adults. If longer-lasting pain exists during this phase, a mem-
ory for this pain can develop. So two-thirds of the children with a longer period on a
newborn intensive care unit have chronic pain at the age of 10 years (Bhatt et al. 2019).
They show anatomical changes in brain structures and disorders of brain function in later
childhood and young adulthood.
Adults who have experienced traumatic events more often suffer from fibromyalgia,
a disease with pain in the area of tendinous attachment points and bony prominences
1.3 How can I Sleep While My Bed is Burning? 3

(Ortiz et al. 2016), which is usually treated by rheumatologists. In addition, these chil-
dren more often suffer from migraine in adulthood, and there is a dose-response effect,
as more trauma leads to more migraine. Physical activity can reduce the problem
(Hammond and Colman 2020). In addition, the fear of pain increases in those affected,
and they are more likely to have depression and anxiety (You et al. 2019).
In a study of middle-aged women, pain in the pelvic area was examined, and this
problem was significantly more common in women with negative childhood experiences
(Krantz et al. 2019). Although women more often complain of pain, the link between
bad childhood experiences and chronic pain was also observed in men (Yamada et al.
2017). In addition to somatic pain, adults who have experienced childhood trauma also
suffer more from visceral pain, which manifests itself as irritable bowel syndrome or as
urogenital problems (Khandker et al. 2019; Berens et al. 2020; Epperson et al. 2020). It
looks as if those affected experience a kind of long-term increased pain sensitivity or
stronger pain transmission from the periphery to the brain.

1.3 How can I Sleep While My Bed is Burning?

This formulation comes from a song by Australian rock musicians who wanted to draw
attention to the fate of indigenous people. The group Midnight Oil sang in the original:
“How do we sleep while our beds are burning?” This question can also be asked when
we look at the sleep problems of abused children or adolescents. Because severe stress
constellations at this time lead to sleep disorders. This connection has already been
shown many times.
A systematic review presented the situation in 2015. The authors found a clear rela-
tionship between sexual abuse in childhood or family disputes on the one hand and
clinically relevant sleep disorders in adulthood on the other (Kajeepeta et al. 2015). So
it seems that the accumulation of stressful episodes, i.e. the increase in the number of
experiences over time, is more important for triggering sleep disorders than the time or
type of stressful event (Sheehan et al. 2020).
In a large-scale study, the authors examined 22403 adults with an average age of 47
years, and 14587 people had a normal sleep time of 7–9 hours, whereas 2069 people had
a very short sleep time of less than 6 hours. If the scientists now compared the people
with normal sleep with those with short sleep, the number of negative childhood experi-
ences was significantly higher in the short sleepers. The more negative experiences there
were, the more pronounced the phenomenon was (Sullivan et al. 2019). Here I think
involuntarily of Napoleon’s statement: “Four hours sleeps the man, five the woman, six
an idiot.” Probably Napoleon was brought up very harshly.
These analyses were confirmed in many other studies. These include the work of
Ketrell McWhorter, who works at the epidemiological center of the National Institute
of Health in Durham, North Carolina. She described results in 40082 women, and
those with a history of childhood trauma slept significantly shorter than those without
4 1 The Long Shadow of Early Trauma—Look!

comparable life circumstances. In addition, the affected people took longer to fall
asleep, woke up several times at night and showed short sleep episodes during the day
(McWhorter et al. 2019).
Since sleep quality is closely linked to subsequent problems such as cardiovascular
diseases, this results in an important factor for increased mortality in affected people.

1.4 Teeth Suffer

If children come from a household where financial constraints and low education are
present, they show more tooth decay, an increased serum levels of the stress hormone
cortisol and more typical tooth decay-inducing bacteria. The most severe caries findings
were found in those children who had the highest levels of the stress hormone cortisol
and the largest amounts of caries-inducing bacteria. In addition, a clear relationship
was found between the stress hormone cortisol and inflammatory changes in the gums
(Boyce et al. 2010).
For example, if children experienced more than four different adverse events, tooth
loss and -treatments were more common in them than in comparable control groups.
In addition, it bothers them throughout their lives, because tooth loss and dental treat-
ments were particularly present in older adults between the ages of 60 and 69 (Ford et al.
2020). This finding also speaks for a kind of long-term programming.
In primates, the researchers were able to establish a connection between tooth enamel
changes in older animals and stress situations in younger years based on dental examina-
tions, with things like separation from the mother, pregnancy of the mother, relocation to
a new enclosure, physical examination by humans or death of a sibling being of impor-
tance (Davis et al. 2020).
Thus, it affects animals and humans when they are exposed to early stress experi-
ences. Tooth problems can be accompanied by chronic inflammation phenomena, and so
they are often the starting point for chronic diseases (more on this in Chap. 4).

1.5 Target Variable: Weight

Children sometimes experience considerable stress even before birth (Fig. 1.1). Poor
nutrition among mothers during pregnancy can be problematic, as was the case in the
Dutch Hungerwinter of 1944–1945. Children of these mothers were heavier and more
prone to diabetes mellitus type 2 Altersdiabetes and metabolism problems later in life.
Similar studies on rats showed identical findings (Jackson et al. 1996). In an earlier
book (“Altern, Müdigkeit und Entzündung verstehen”), I already spoke of the Barker-
Hypothese (Straub 2018), which linked intrauterine stress due to malnutrition with later
increased weight development and increased mortality from heart attacks (see also in
Chap. 2, Origins and Important Protagonists) (Barker et al. 1989). In a similar way,
1.5 Target Variable: Weight 5

Life From Early Childhood Adolescence Time of the


of the fertilization childhood Adults
mother of an early A.
until oocyte perinatal (4–11 yrs) (11–14 yrs) early
fertilization until birth (0–2 mo) (21–35 yrs)
of an egg of the medium A.
(later child) child very early (15–18 yrs) Medium
childhood (35–65 yrs)
prenatal (0–12 mo) late A.
(18–21 yrs) higher
early (65–80 yrs)
childhood
(1–4 yrs) high
(>80yrs)

Timeline of mother and child

Fig. 1.1 The time of exposure to stress. This figure shows the different time windows when traumatic
events can occur (events and episodes are illustrated by the lightning bolt icon above the boxes). The
curved arrow below the boxes indicates the propagation of problems from one time window to the next.
(A: adolescence; yrs: years; mo: months)

children of mothers who were exposed to psychological stress during pregnancy suffer in
a very similar way (Burgueno et al. 2020). They are overweight, which points to a more
general stress mechanism.
The hormone of the adrenal gland released by stress—cortisol—is important for
weight development because it influences decisive neuronal pathways in the brain that
are important for long-term programming of eating behavior (Spencer 2013). Children
experience stress-inducing events and situations after birth, as reported in the preceding
subchapters. These children are often obese in later life with the corresponding meta-
bolic consequences such as metabolic syndrome and diabetes mellitus (Burgueno et al.
2020).
The important neuronal pathways for adjusting eating and reward behavior are still
very pliable before and after birth (Spencer 2013). The early programming of the corre-
sponding centers by exaggerated stress serves to protect against energy shortages (accu-
mulation of energy-rich fatty acids in fat tissue), but it leads to problems in the long term.
This is particularly the case when the accumulated energy reserves are no longer needed
in later life because physical activity decreases with age (Straub 2018). With regard to
addictive behavior, I refer to Chap. 3, Subchapter “Alcohol, nicotine and drugs”.
These early childhood influences also seem to be determined by a subsequent change
in the genetic makeup through so-called epigenetic processes (Explanation 1). In this
scenario, environmental influences in different time windows can permanently change
6 1 The Long Shadow of Early Trauma—Look!

the normally existing and stable genetic program. This creates an increased risk of
age-related diabetes and cardiovascular diseases (Hanson and Gluckman 2015).

1.6 Cardiovascular Diseases

In Sweden, a group of scientists conducted a very large study of more than 14,000 peo-
ple born in 1953. Data from people were stored in the databases of the Swedish health
system who were placed in institutions outside their family of origin or who had condi-
tions in the parental home that were contrary to the welfare of the child. Those children
who experienced these forms of childhood trauma had a higher mortality rate compared
to control persons who were not affected. The risk of higher mortality was particularly
high if the children grew up outside their family of origin (Jackisch et al. 2019). Does
death from cardiovascular disease play a role in these affected persons?
In a very large American study, the connection between childhood trauma and heart
attack was established. In total, more than 34,000 people took part in the studies, and
those with childhood abuse had almost twice the risk of a heart attack. The risk was
highest in people after sexual abuse and physical abuse (Chou and Koenen 2019).
This can be contributed by obesity, age-related diabetes, high blood uric acid or high
blood lipid levels. In fact, people who have been abused as children have more age-re-
lated diabetes, which typically occurs after the age of 40. There is also a direct relation-
ship between dose and effect, as a higher number of child abuses contributes to a higher
likelihood of age-related diabetes. In one study, it was shown how childhood trauma is
more often associated with severe alcohol abuse, smoking and high body weight (Lown
et al. 2019), further risk factors for cardiovascular disease.

Explanation 1: Epigenetics
By means of epigenetic changes of the genes the readability of the same is promoted
or impeded by chemical modifications. The composition of the genes (the sequence
of the nucleic bases) is not changed hereby. If for example a methyl group (a C-atom
with three hydrogen atoms, thus -CH3) is attached to certain sections of the DNA, the
associated gene cannot be read in the same way anymore. If an acetyl group (-C(O)
CH3) is attached to the known packaging proteins of the DNA (to the histones),
the DNA becomes better accessible and a gene can be read more easily. Thus an
extra-cellular stimulus can act on the cell and start the gene reading, but due to the
epigenetic changes by the methyl group the gene reading may be blocked. The attach-
ing of a methyl group or an acetyl group is controlled by different enzymes. All this
is very complex and we have only understood it in approaches. Thus the gene reading
can be manipulated retrospectively during the course of life. Besides these two men-
tioned possibilities of the retrospective change—methylation and acetylation—there
are further epigenetic mechanisms (Kegel 2015), which cannot be mentioned here
because this goes beyond the scope of the book. ◄
1.7 Chronic Lung Problems 7

Between 1959 and 1961 the “three-year great Chinese famine” was a disaster. In this
time under Mao Tse-tung in China between 20 and 40 million people died of malnutri-
tion. Wenqiang Zhang and Rongsheng Luan from Szechuan analyzed the data of a large-
scale health study of three different groups. They included persons who were born after
the famine (control) and persons who were affected as a fetus or in early childhood. The
persons affected in the fetal stage showed in comparison to the other groups in adulthood
high blood levels of uric acid. This connection remained when the physicians included
the risk factors smoking, high body weight, alcohol, hypertension and diabetes mellitus
additionally in the statistics (Zhang and Luan 2020). Since a high uric acid is a risk fac-
tor for the heart attack this study points to a significant problem.
Furthermore adults after early childhood traumas showed higher cholesterol levels,
which are a risk factor for the development of cardiovascular diseases (Cubbin et al.
2019). The different risks can occur independently from each other and reinforce each
other.

1.7 Chronic Lung Problems

Childhood stress experiences are more often associated with the onset of an asthma dis-
ease and allergy in adolescents. In one study, the observed persons were between 13 and
18 years old. They experienced blows from the educator, from other people, threats from
weapons, rape, sexual abuse, stalking (obsessive pursuit), violence in the family or they
observed severe injury or death events. All these things can lead to an increased risk of
an allergic disease or asthma (McLaughlin et al. 2016). This problem can persist into
adulthood, as was shown in a study in Tucson, Arizona (Oren et al. 2017).
It goes even further. If pregnant women experienced domestic violence, were afraid
during the pregnancy, suffered from financial constraints, showed symptoms of depres-
sion and had a generally higher stress level, their children—especially the male off-
spring—showed more asthma up to the age of 6. Here, a early time window from Fig.
1.1 is important. It does not stop at asthma, there’s more like allergic rhinitis, neuro-
dermatitis (atopic eczema), welts on the skin (urticaria) and other hypersensitivity reac-
tions are observed. This relationship has now been observed in 30 different studies and
recently summarized (Flanigan et al. 2018). However, paternal stress does not affect the
offspring’s risk of asthma.
And if you think we have reached the beginning of a causal chain, there is an esca-
lation of the problem. If mothers themselves were victims of domestic violence in their
childhood or were exposed to traumatic life events from an early age to adulthood and
then became pregnant, their male offspring more often had asthma. Interestingly, the
mothers often also had asthma during this pregnancy, smoked more often and were sig-
nificantly overweight (Rosa et al. 2018). A combination of factors increases the risk.
For example, the presence of chemicals together with the stress situations leads to an
8 1 The Long Shadow of Early Trauma—Look!

amplification of the problem. And again and again it is mainly boys who are affected by
the allergic disease (Rosa et al. 2018).

1.8 And then Chronic Inflammation

There are large epidemiological studies that show the connection between childhood
stress and the later onset of autoimmune diseases. If there were traumatic experiences
in childhood, one form of inflammatory joint disease—rheumatoid arthritis—can occur
more frequently (Kopec and Sayre 2004). Similar relationships have also been reported
for other autoimmune diseases (Feldman et al. 2019).
When the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) in Atlanta, Georgia, is
not busy with the consequences of the Corona infection, they investigate there, among
other things, the consequences of childhood psychological trauma. Shanta Dube of the
CDC is an epidemiologist with a focus on child welfare. Some of her informative studies
shed light on the connection between childhood trauma and later chronic inflammatory
diseases. For this she was able to access large CDC data sets. If only one type of child-
hood trauma is present, the risk of an inflammatory disease is still low, if several are
present at the same time, the risk of such a disease increases 2.5-fold. On average, the
diseases do not occur until after the 30th birthday (Dube et al. 2009).
Children with early trauma suffer more frequently from a childhood form of arthritis.
The risk of developing this chronic joint inflammation is up to 6 times higher in affected
children if they had to experience the death of a parent (Neufeld et al. 2013).
Likewise, in the less inflammatory osteoarthritis, which we typically attribute to wear
and tear (for example, due to occupational stress), the connection between childhood
trauma and illness was found (Fuller-Thomson et al. 2009). Although osteoarthritis is
relatively non-inflammatory, it is still associated with significant local inflammatory phe-
nomena and pain. Since pain is more often observed in adults who were under stress in
childhood, we are not surprised by increased osteoarthritis pain.
Even if it does not lead to an open chronic inflammatory disease, adults with child-
hood trauma have a generally higher inflammatory constellation. They have an increased
C-reactive protein in the serum, which is associated with various problems of the adult
such as osteoporosis, stroke, heart attack or depression (Danese and Lewis 2017). If the
investigators challenge depressive adults with previous childhood trauma to a stress situ-
ation, the already increased blood levels of inflammatory parameters increase even more.
The childhood stress, the consequences of which we would only suspect in the adult
brain (e.g. depression, anxiety, fatigue, etc.), is also transferred to many physical ail-
ments in the periphery. The brain and the body are in close interaction, and that is why
such problems arise in adulthood also outside the brain. A key theme of the book is the
connection between early childhood stress on the one hand (problem in the brain) and
chronic immune activation in adulthood (problem in the periphery). Since I have worked
in the field of chronic inflammatory diseases and psycho-neuro-endocrine immunology
References 9

for many years, I am particularly interested in this connection. The book will deal with
this transmission of problems from the brain to the body periphery and to the immune
system.

1.9 To the Point

• Prenatal, early childhood and childhood trauma often leads to psychiatric illnesses,
anxiety disorders and increased suicide rates.
• Consequences of stress-inducing experiences are transmitted by mothers to children,
who then suffer from this problem again and can pass it on.
• Pain in early childhood increases the likelihood of chronic pain later in life. Traumatic
life circumstances also increase the likelihood of chronic pain.
• Negative childhood experiences lead to sleep problems later in life. People sleep too
little, often wake up at night and need short naps during the day.
• Childhood stress episodes worsen dental health in later life.
• Child abuse is often associated with a high body weight. This often leads to an unfa-
vorable metabolic state, which in turn often leads to diabetes mellitus in old age.
• These metabolic disorders programmed in childhood can be the platform for cardio-
vascular diseases. The connection between childhood stress and heart attack has been
clearly demonstrated epidemiologically.
• In the same way, the connection between early traumatic experiences and allergic
diseases such as hay fever, asthma or neurodermatitis has been described. Here, the
problem of transmission from mother to child becomes clear.
• Finally, the link between bad childhood experiences and chronic inflammatory dis-
eases shows the extent of the problem. Since chronic activation of the immune system
is an important topic for this connection, it is dealt with in more detail in this book.

References

Barker DJ, Winter PD, Osmond C, Margetts B, Simmonds SJ (1989) Weight in infancy and death
from ischaemic heart disease. Lancet 2:577–580
Berens S, Banzhaf P, Baumeister D, Gauss A, Eich W, Schaefert R, Tesarz J (2020) Relationship
between adverse childhood experiences and illness anxiety in irritable bowel syndrome – The
impact of gender. J Psychosom Res 128:109846
Bhatt RR, Gupta A, Mayer EA, Zeltzer LK (2019) Chronic pain in children: structural and rest-
ing-state functional brain imaging within a developmental perspective. Pediatr Res 88:840–849
Boyce WT, Den Besten PK, Stamperdahl J, Zhan L, Jiang Y, Adler NE, Featherstone JD (2010)
Social inequalities in childhood dental caries: the convergent roles of stress, bacteria and disad-
vantage. Soc Sci Med 71:1644–1652
10 1 The Long Shadow of Early Trauma—Look!

Burgueno AL, Juarez YR, Genaro AM, Tellechea ML (2020) Systematic review and meta-analysis
on the relationship between prenatal stress and metabolic syndrome intermediate phenotypes.
Int J Obes 44:1–12
Chou PH, Koenen KC (2019) Associations between childhood maltreatment and risk of myocar-
dial infarction in adulthood: results from the national epidemiologic survey on alcohol and
related conditions. J Psychiatr Res 116:172–177
Cubbin C, Kim Y, Panisch LS (2019) Familial childhood adversity is associated with chronic dis-
ease among women: data from the geographic research on wellbeing (GROW) study. Matern
Child Health J 23:1117–1129
Danese A (2020) Annual research review: rethinking childhood trauma-new research directions for
measurement, study design and analytical strategies. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 61:236–250
Danese A, Lewis SJ (2017) Psychoneuroimmunology of early-life stress: the hidden wounds of
childhood trauma? Neuropsychopharmacology 42:99–114
Davis KA, Mountain RV, Pickett OR, Den Besten PK, Bidlack FB, Dunn EC (2020) Teeth as
potential new tools to measure early-life adversity and subsequent mental health risk: an inter-
disciplinary review and conceptual model. Biol Psychiatry 87:502–513
Dube SR, Fairweather D, Pearson WS, Felitti VJ, Anda RF, Croft JB (2009) Cumulative childhood
stress and autoimmune diseases in adults. Psychosom Med 71:243–250
Epperson CN, Duffy KA, Johnson RL, Sammel MD, Newman DK (2020) Enduring impact
of childhood adversity on lower urinary tract symptoms in adult women. Neurourol Urodyn
39:1472–1481
Feldman CH, Malspeis S, Leatherwood C, Kubzansky L, Costenbader KH, Roberts AL (2019)
Association of childhood abuse with incident systemic lupus erythematosus in adulthood in a
longitudinal cohort of women. J Rheumatol 46:1589–1596
Flanigan C, Sheikh A, DunnGalvin A, Brew BK, Almqvist C, Nwaru BI (2018) Prenatal mater-
nal psychosocial stress and offspring’s asthma and allergic disease: a systematic review and
meta-analysis. Clin Exp Allergy 48:403–414
Ford K, Brocklehurst P, Hughes K, Sharp CA, Bellis MA (2020) Understanding the association
between self-reported poor oral health and exposure to adverse childhood experiences: a retro-
spective study. BMC Oral Health 20:51
Fuller-Thomson E, Stefanyk M, Brennenstuhl S (2009) The robust association between childhood
physical abuse and osteoarthritis in adulthood: findings from a representative community sam-
ple. Arthritis Rheum 61:1554–1562
Hammond NG, Colman I (2020) The role of positive health behaviors in the relationship between
early life stress and migraine. Headache 60:1111–1123
Hanson MA, Gluckman PD (2015) Developmental origins of health and disease – global public
health implications. Best Pract Res Clin Obstet Gynaecol 29:24–31
Jackisch J, Brännström L, Almquist YB (2019) Troubled childhoods cast long shadows: childhood
adversity and premature all-cause mortality in a Swedish cohort. SSM Popul Health 9:100506
Jackson AA, Langley-Evans SC, McCarthy HD (1996) Nutritional influences in early life upon
obesity and body proportions. CIBA Found Symp 1996(201):118–129
Kajeepeta S, Gelaye B, Jackson CL, Williams MA (2015) Adverse childhood experiences are asso-
ciated with adult sleep disorders: a systematic review. Sleep Med 16:320–330
Karatzias T, Shevlin M, Pitcairn J, Thomson L, Mahoney A, Hyland P (2019) Childhood adver-
sity and psychosis in detained inpatients from medium to high secured units: results from the
Scottish census survey. Child Abuse Negl 96:104094
Kegel B (2015) Epigenetik – Wie unsere Erfahrungen vererbt werden. DuMont Buchverlag, Köln
Khandker M, Brady SS, Rydell SA, Turner RM, Schreiner PJ, Harlow BL (2019) Early-life
chronic stressors, rumination, and the onset of vulvodynia. J Sex Med 16:880–890
References 11

Kopec JA, Sayre EC (2004) Traumatic experiences in childhood and the risk of arthritis: a prospec-
tive cohort study. Can J Public Health 95:361–365
Krantz TE, Andrews N, Petersen TR, Dunivan GC, Montoya M, Swanson N, Wenzl CK, Zambrano
JR, Komesu YM (2019) Adverse childhood experiences among gynecology patients with
chronic pelvic pain. Obstet Gynecol 134:1087–1095
Lown EA, Lui CK, Karriker-Jaffe K, Mulia N, Williams E, Ye Y, Li L, Greenfield TK, Kerr WC
(2019) Adverse childhood events and risk of diabetes onset in the 1979 National longitudinal
survey of youth cohort. BMC Public Health 19:1007
Maxwell LG, Fraga MV, Malavolta CP (2019) Assessment of pain in the newborn: an update. Clin
Perinatol 46:693–707
McLaughlin KA, Basu A, Walsh K, Slopen N, Sumner JA, Koenen KC, Keyes KM (2016)
Childhood exposure to violence and chronic physical conditions in a national sample of US
adolescents. Psychosom Med 78:1072–1083
McWhorter KL, Parks CG, D’Aloisio AA, Rojo-Wissar DM, Sandler DP, Jackson CL (2019)
Traumatic childhood experiences and multiple dimensions of poor sleep among adult women.
Sleep 42:zsz108
Negriff S, Palmer Molina A, Hackman DA (2020) Parental exposure to childhood maltreatment
and offspring’s mental health: investigating pathways through parental adversity and offspring
exposure to maltreatment. Child Maltreat 25:422–432
Neufeld KM, Karunanayake CP, Maenz LY, Rosenberg AM (2013) Stressful life events antedating
chronic childhood arthritis. J Rheumatol 40:1756–1765
Oren E, Gerald L, Stern DA, Martinez FD, Wright AL (2017) Self-reported stressful life events
during adolescence and subsequent asthma: a longitudinal study. J Allergy Clin Immunol Pract
5:427–434
Ortiz R, Ballard ED, Machado-Vieira R, Saligan LN, Walitt B (2016) Quantifying the influence of
child abuse history on the cardinal symptoms of fibromyalgia. Clin Exp Rheumatol 34:S59–S66
Rosa MJ, Lee AG, Wright RJ (2018) Evidence establishing a link between prenatal and early-life
stress and asthma development. Curr Opin Allergy Clin Immunol 18:148–158
Sheehan CM, Li L, Friedman EM (2020) Quantity, timing, and type of childhood adversity and
sleep quality in adulthood. Sleep Health 6:246–252
Spencer SJ (2013) Perinatal programming of neuroendocrine mechanisms connecting feeding
behavior and stress. Front Neurosci 7:109
Straub RH (2018) Altern, Müdigkeit und Entzündungen verstehen – Wenn Immunsystem und
Gehirn um die Energie im Körper ringen. Springer, Berlin/Heidelberg
Sullivan K, Rochani H, Huang LT, Donley DK, Zhang J (2019) Adverse childhood experiences
affect sleep duration for up to 50 years later. Sleep 42:zsz087
Yamada K, Matsudaira K, Tanaka E, Oka H, Katsuhira J, Iso H (2017) Sex-specific impact of
early-life adversity on chronic pain: a large population-based study in Japan. J Pain Res
10:427–433
You DS, Albu S, Lisenbardt H, Meagher MW (2019) Cumulative childhood adversity as a risk fac-
tor for common chronic pain conditions in young adults. Pain Med 20:486–494
Zhang W, Luan R (2020) Early-life exposure to the Chinese famine of 1959–61 and risk of
Hyperuricemia: results from the China health and retirement longitudinal study. BMC Public
Health 20:15
What is a Child’s Psychological Trauma?
2

2.1 Origins and Important Protagonists

The connection between adversity in childhood and later problems in adulthood has been
known for a long time. Sigmund Freud generated great theories with the experiences of
his time, in which childhood conflicts and trauma had a significance in the formation of
personality and the development of neuroses. Although his considerations at that time
had no modern empirical platform, he earned the merit of popularizing the connection
between childhood trauma and difficulties in adulthood. However, his focus on psycho-
sexual aspects had a very exclusive character, which is now obsolete (Westen 1998). In
addition, he used case studies of individual sufferers, which are not suitable for a gener-
alized statement. Some called Freud’s theories and those of others “the grand theories of
personalities” (Westen 1998).
Before the Second World War, this view was very much shaped by Freud and his
students or followers, and only afterwards did different paths lead to a wide empiri-
cal investigation of a serious problem—that was and is the Anglo-American direction.
The number of scientists who have examined this connection since Sigmund Freud has
become innumerable today. Nevertheless, I would like to briefly mention a few important
representatives with different professional orientations. The chronologically determined
selection is subjective and does not claim to be complete.

2.1.1 Urie Bronfenbrenner, a Psychologist (1917–2005)

Bronfenbrenner (1917–2005) was born in the turmoil of the Russian Revolution in 1917
in Moscow. His family moved to the United States when he was six years old. He studied
psychology at Cornell University, Harvard and Ann Arbor. Immediately after the Second

© The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer-Verlag GmbH, DE, part of Springer 13
Nature 2023
R. H. Straub, Early Trauma as the Origin of Chronic Inflammation,
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-662-66751-4_2
14 2 What is a Child’s Psychological Trauma?

World War, he became interested in child development. So over the course of his life
he generated a model with the following content: The environment has an influence on
child development on different levels of society (norms, values), via relationships (e.g.
the mother’s workplace, the importance of the neighbors), and up to the immediate inter-
personal relationships of the child. The different levels are closely related to each other.
In addition, he recognized the enormous importance of socio-economic burden, and
he campaigned with Lady Bird Johnson (1934–1973)—the wife of President Lyndon B.
Johnson (1908–1973)—for a government support program for economically disadvan-
taged children. In this program, 20 million children were promoted over decades (Woo
2005). So he earned the merit of having drawn attention to the socio-economic bottle-
necks that represent a significant childhood burden.

2.1.2 John Bowlby, a Child Psychiatrist (1907–1990)

John Bowlby (1907–1990) was the son of a surgeon of the royal household under King
Edward VII of England. As was customary in this social class at that time, Bowlby’s
mother Mary hardly took care of her son’s education. This was the responsibility of a
nursery maid who took on the role of the mother. In Bowlby’s situation, this nanny left
the family in his fifth year of life, which he later described as “a tragic loss of a mother”
(van Dijken 1998). At the age of seven, he went to boarding school, a bad time for him,
which he later commented on as follows (Bowlby 1966): “At the age of seven, I wouldn’t
even send a dog to boarding school.” Finally, he lost a beloved godfather, another separa-
tion trauma for Bowlby.
We are now not surprised that John Bowlby became interested in the topic of “sepa-
ration” and “attachment” after he had completed his medical studies. During the Second
World War, he was confronted with children who, because of the war turmoil in London,
in Cambridge separated from their parents. In the local children’s clinic, he examined
boys who had been caught stealing. He found out from 17 of these 44 small thieves how
they had lived separated from their parents for a longer period of six and more months
before their fifth year of life. In the control group, this fate of separation was experi-
enced by only 2 of 44 children (Bowlby 1946). Separation seemed to lead to delinquent
behavior.
In the Tavistock Clinic northwest of London’s city center, he initiated the “Sanatorium
Study” in 1948, in which children between the ages of one and three were hospitalized
in a clinic due to illness or in an orphanage and long-term separated from their mother.
Together with James Robertson he described the severe consequences of early separa-
tion. The first two phases of separation—protest and despair—are characterized by a
very consuming struggle that only in the third phase leads to a seemingly less strenuous
situation of denial. Robertson made two films in the 1950s that soon afterwards led to
radical changes in the care of these children (Robertson and Bowlby 1952).
2.1 Origins and Important Protagonists 15

The concept of separation from the mother went down in the history of sci-
ence as “maternal deprivation”, which today is a recognized early childhood trauma.
Furthermore, these works were the platform for attachment research and for animal
experiments with similar forms of separation experiences. Another important representa-
tive on the topic of maternal deprivation was Michael Rutter (1933–2021) from London
(Rutter 1999).
Offshoots of this line of research are studies on children who have been exposed to
maternal deprivation for a long time. These include the studies on children evacuated
to the other places during the Second World War, e.g., in Finland (Eriksson et al. 2014)
and the studies on children from Romanian orphanages that made headlines in the early
1990s (e.g. Nelson 2013).

2.1.3 Michael Rutter, Psychiatrist (1933–2021)

Sir Michael Rutter (1933–2021) worked at King’s College in London. He was born in
Lebanon, as his father worked there as a doctor in the 1930s. During the Second World
War, at the age of seven, he lived in London, and there the German bombing took place.
In this context, Michael Rutter and his younger sister were evacuated from London to the
USA, and he experienced a painful separation experience, because the parents remained
in the war-torn London.
After attending school in New Jersey (USA) as well as Wolverhampton and York in
England, he attended the University of Birmingham, England, after the war, where he
graduated in 1955. After training as a neurologist, psychiatrist and pediatrician, Rutter
did a research stay at the Albert Einstein College of Medicine in New York, where he
dealt with child development (Kolvin 1999).
He was the first professor of child psychiatry in England, and he has been doing this
until recently. He received many awards for his scientific work in the fields of child
development, autism, separation experiences (deprivation), resilience (psychological
resistance after trauma), psychopathology as a result of traumatic childhood experiences,
genetic causes of behavior and long-term effects of bad childhood experiences in older
adults. In 1992 he was knighted by Queen Elizabeth II. (Kolvin 1999).
An important study was initiated by him and colleagues after the fall of the Iron Wall
in Romania. There he initiated an adoption program of Romanian children and adoles-
cents to England, who were accommodated in orphanages of Bucharest by the wrong
policy of the communist leader Ceauşescu at an early stage under unimaginable poor
conditions. These investigations were groundbreaking in many ways. He is the author
of several seminal books and textbooks (e.g. Rutter’s Child and Adolescent Psychiatry)
(Thapar et al. 2015). Various descriptions of the person of Sir Michael Rutter are exuber-
ant, and the interested reader may find out more there (Kolvin 1999).
16 2 What is a Child’s Psychological Trauma?

2.1.4 David Barker, Social Medicine and Epidemiologist


(1938–2013)

As the son of an engineer and a concert cellist, David Barker (1938–2013) was initially
more interested in natural sciences than in medical studies. Nevertheless, he studied
medicine in London, graduated in 1962 and started training as a social medical doctor
and epidemiologist at the University of Birmingham in England. Between 1969 and 1979
he worked in Uganda and discovered the cause of a nasty skin ulcer in a combination of
cuts with reed grass and colonization with mycobacteria (so-called Buruli ulcer).
In 1979 Barker was appointed professor of clinical epidemiology at the University of
Southampton, and there he remained for the rest of his life. In the role of epidemiolo-
gist, Barker observed in certain northwestern regions of England and Wales a correlation
between increased newborn and infant mortality in the early 1920s and increased mortal-
ity from heart attacks 75 years later in the same population (Barker and Osmond 1986).
The connection apparently resulted from the poor nutrition and living conditions in these
regions. That wasn’t the whole truth.
Soon Barker became aware of the connection between body weight after the first year
of life and the heart attack rate in middle-aged adults. If a child weighed little at the first
birthday (8.2 kg), it had a significantly higher risk of suffering from a heart attack many
decades later compared to children with a higher body weight of 12.3 kg. His hypoth-
esis was: There must be important factors for healthy aging to be found in newborns
and infants (Barker et al. 1989a). Since newborns were affected, Barker and his team
were finally able to point to a possible problem during pregnancy (Barker et al. 1989b).
The supply situation in the uterus sensitized people in old age to heart attacks, strokes,
high blood pressure, age-related diabetes and childhood infectious respiratory diseases
(Barker et al. 1993). The Barker phenomenon clearly points to epigenetic changes in the
uterus (explanation 1), which can cause a kind of adaptation reaction with negative long-
term consequences.
In an earlier book (“Altern, Müdigkeit und Entzündung verstehen”), I described the
Barker phenomenon (Straub 2018). Indeed, this finding was a clear indication of prenatal
stress factors and therefore of an early traumatic experience that can cause chronic prob-
lems in adulthood. Here, I further mention the harmful influence of alcohol and nicotine
during pregnancy, which also causes long-term problems in offspring (Stroud et al. 2018;
Wozniak et al. 2019).
Furthermore, animal experiments with prenatal stress by Ingeborg Ward were enlight-
ening, as they demonstrated a reduction in male sexual behavior in the offspring of
stressed mothers (Ward 1972).

2.1.5 Vincent J. Felitti, an Internist (Born 1938)

After studying medicine at the University of Minnesota in Minneapolis and at Johns


Hopkins University in Baltimore, which we got to know through the Corona Dashboard
2.1 Origins and Important Protagonists 17

in times of COVID-19, Felitti (born 1938) completed his medical studies in 1962. After
training as an internist, he came to Kaiser Permanente in San Diego in 1968. This organ-
ization is still a private special form of a certain health insurance and care model that is
subsidized by the US government and private sponsors. At Kaiser Permanente, Felitti
was responsible for prevention research, and one of the programs was used in the 1980s
to prevent obesity.
In the context of this work, he cared for a young woman who weighed more than 200
kg and lost 66 kg during controlled weight reduction. Unfortunately, this weight loss was
not of long duration, and shortly before the patient broke off contact with Felitti, she
had gained a lot of weight. She explained that she was a sleepwalker and a night-time
glutton. The sleepwalking-eating problem began when she was sexually abused by her
grandfather from the age of 10 for more than a decade (Felitti 2019).
Since a similar case was presented to the attentive Felitti shortly after this episode,
the prevention physicians of Kaiser Permanente began to systematically ask about sexual
abuse at his instigation. In a first study of 286 obese people, they were able to docu-
ment a history of sexual abuse in 55% of the people (Felitti 2019). That was an alarming
number.
Felitti presented the data of the 286 people at an American conference on obesity, but
the findings were not accepted or sharply attacked by those present. During a dinner, an
epidemiologist from the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention in Atlanta recom-
mended that he conduct a large epidemiological study and convince his opponents by
sheer numbers. During a visit to this research facility, he began a very fruitful interac-
tion with Robert Anda. Together they were able to start a large-scale study of men and
women in 1995 (Felitti 2019). The first results were published in 1998 as the Adverse
Childhood Experiences Study, which showed that among patients who experienced four
or more adverse stress events in their childhood, the risk of health problems such as alco-
holism, drug use, depression, suicide, nicotine abuse, promiscuity, sexually transmitted
diseases, physical inactivity, and obesity increased by a factor of 2–12 depending on the
follow-up problem (Felitti et al. 1998).
Thus, the first epidemiological study was published, which showed the connection
between childhood trauma and follow-up problems in adulthood in more than 17,000
people. In this survey, a questionnaire was developed that systematically recorded the
child abuse. However, all possible childhood burdens were not included in the afore-
mentioned questionnaire, and a revision of the questionnaire was necessary later (see
Sect. 2.2). Since the study was designed prospectively, Felitti and colleagues were able
to follow up with numerous follow-up studies to date. This resulted in a truly far-reach-
ing new view that left no doubt about this problem in children and adolescents.
This epidemiological study was not published until 1998, and that is already astonish-
ing. Were the people before that blind? No, of course they were not, as Bronfenbrenner,
Bowlby, Rutter and others showed, but the studies only illuminated individual aspects
and were small in scale. Felitti and his team of researchers were able to raise the issue to
another platform, and the public in the USA felt affected.
18 2 What is a Child’s Psychological Trauma?

2.1.6 Robert J. Plomin, a Psychologist (Born 1948)

When it comes to the heritability of behavior, the name of Robert Plomin comes first.
Plomin (born 1948) earned a bachelor’s degree from Vincent DePaul University in
Chicago in the state of Illinois in 1970. He then went to Austin, Texas, to earn a doc-
torate in psychology there in 1974. After several stops at various universities in the
USA—for example Boulder, Colorado and PennState University—he moved to King’s
College in London in 1994 to Sir Michael Rutter, a flagship of psychological research
in England. There he began his very successful studies of 10000 twin pairs, which were
observed from early childhood to adulthood over 25 years. This study was very impor-
tant for the field of behavioral genetics.
He published together with colleagues over 1000 articles, book chapters and books
and received many awards for his research work. He was the youngest president of the
International Society for Behavioral Genetics, which was only founded in 1970. By the
way, the year of foundation of a society often indicates when a scientific field really gets
going. Although in the early years of such societies turbulent finding phases are still typ-
ical, things begin to stabilize after about ten years. This was the case, for example, in the
field of behavioral genetics. In the presidential year 1989 of Robert Plomin, the propo-
nents had reached a stable platform. I take up the topic of genetics again in the Sect. 2.5
below.

2.1.7 W. Thomas Boyce (Born 1943), a Pediatrician, and Jay Belsky
(Born 1952), a Psychologist

In 2019, Thomas Boyce (born 1943) summarized his findings in a recommended book
entitled “Orchid or Dandelion” (Boyce 2019). From his own family history he learned
how he himself was a robust dandelion and his later schizophrenic sister was a sensitive
orchid in contrast.
He was until recently a pediatrician in California and taught at the universities of
San Francisco, Berkeley and Vancouver (University of British Columbia). Boyce distin-
guished children according to their sensitivity to trauma, and he called the sensitive “the
orchids” and the robust “the dandelions”. He recognized the practical clinical connection
between sensitivity and physical reactions of the stress axes to adverse life events, and
orchids showed a much stronger stress reaction than dandelions. The dandelion was so to
speak cool and did not get upset, no matter where it was planted. Orchids can fail under
strong child stress, and they are sick all their lives, whereas they can thrive very well
under caring stress-avoiding conditions—with higher and more creative performance
than dandelions (Boyce 2019).
The different sensitivities described by Boyce are not based solely on genetic differ-
ences in DNA, but also on so-called epigenetic influences (explanation 1). The geneticist
2.2 The Different Types of Early Traumatic Experiences 19

speaks of epigenetic influences when the reading of the genes is promoted or inhibited
by environmental changes at the DNA. These epigenetic influences begin during embry-
onic development in the womb and remain until adulthood. There they may lead to long-
term follow-up problems. This explanation is particularly important because it means
that the “offender” can be a victim of adverse circumstances in childhood.
Another important representative of the “different sensitivities” theory is Jay Belsky
(born 1952), who recognized this fact somewhat later than Boyce in the 1990s (Belsky
1997). In contrast to Boyce, Belsky had a much stronger connection to evolutionary the-
ory. So he can probably be credited with the authorship of a theory that brings together
the “different sensitivities” and the evolutionary theory. He first described this in a short
editorial from 1997 (Belsky 1997). At the end of Chap. 1, I will go into evolutionary
medicine aspects.

2.1.8 Summary

The list of these protagonists is incomplete and nevertheless enlightening, because we


recognize the entangled paths of different disciplines that aim at the childhood trauma
and its consequences. So please forgive the author if he does not list all the participants
at this point, because there was not enough space for that. A list of the most eminent psy-
chologists of the 20th century—and this is only a group of scientists—hardly lists any
representatives from the non-Anglo-American language area (Haggbloom et al. 2002).
We realize: The research has been mainly from the USA and England since 1945. In the
next subchapter, the different forms of traumatic experiences will now be addressed.

2.2 The Different Types of Early Traumatic Experiences

The important questionnaire on childhood trauma (ACE-Questionnaire:adverse child-


hood experiences) comes from the large epidemiological study by Vincent Felitti (Felitti
et al. 1998). There were similar questionnaires before Felitti’s publications (e.g. Masten
et al. 1988), but because of the epidemiological character of the mentioned study, I
would like to particularly highlight the ACE-Questionnaire. There are ten main catego-
ries mentioned there, which are listed in Table 2.1. Although this ACE-Questionnaire
covers many points, it is by no means complete, which was not the claim of Felitti and
colleagues. From the answers in the questionnaire, the investigator can assess the sever-
ity of the traumatic experiences.
Meanwhile, extensions of these questionnaires have been made, and many of them
can be accessed via relevant websites. An important German website is the Working
Group of Scientific Medical Societies (2019), and from there you can access helpers.
This website is supported by various German medical societies.
20 2 What is a Child’s Psychological Trauma?

Table 2.1  Childhood trauma according to Felitti (ACE-Questionnaire)


• Physical abuse (pushing, hitting, grabbing, throwing, cutting, injuring, etc.)
• Sexual abuse (at least 5 years older heterosexual person forces against their own wishes to
sexual acts or tries to force)
• Emotional abuse (insult, accuse, degrade, humiliate, scare)
• Physical neglect (little food/hunger, dirty clothes, nobody protects, nobody cares and takes care,
nobody provides medical care)
• Emotional neglect (lack of love, feeling of unimportance, family members do not take care of
each other, lack of proximity to family members)
• Violence against the mother (shove, hit, grab, throw, kick, bite, threaten with weapon)
• Substance abuse by a family member (alcohol, drugs, medication)
• Mental illness or suicide by a family member
• Separation of parents
• Incarceration of a family member
According to Vincent Felitti et al. (1998)

In addition to the elements mentioned in Table 2.1, further trauma has been recog-
nized in recent years that go far beyond the ACE questionnaire (Table 2.2). The points
of Table 2.2, for example, come from extensions of the Centers for Disease Control
and Prevention, Atlanta (Centers for disease control and prevention 2018) and many
working groups that regularly publish on this topic (Gest et al. 1999; Cohen et al.
2006; Burgermeister 2007; Tonmyr et al. 2011; Cronholm et al. 2015; Finkelhor et al.
2015; Ellis and Dietz 2017; Mersky et al. 2017; Oh et al. 2018; Anand et al. 2019).
Prenatal stress situations for the mother (Graignic-Philippe et al. 2014) and the unstable
socio-economic conditions in the parental home are particularly important in this context
(Poulton et al. 2002).
In addition, adverse childhood experiences often occur together and at the same time,
and so the same person is often exposed to multiple traumas. This phenomenon has been
called the “snowball effect” when one problem leads to the next (Masten 2014). In sci-
ence, a line of four childhood traumas has been established, above which the trauma
burden is considered to be severe. However, this line of demarcation depends on the
questionnaire and can therefore vary.
When children experience multiple childhood traumas, they often have so-called “best
friends” in young adulthood who have a high rate of mental problems. In addition, the
psychopathology of the “best friends” is a starting point for their own depressive symp-
toms. So the affected person chooses his environment according to his own needs and
thus takes damage himself (Raposa et al. 2015).
In addition, it is said that childhood traumas often go hand in hand with mental seque-
lae of the affected persons and that difficult economic conditions in the parental home
2.2 The Different Types of Early Traumatic Experiences 21

Table 2.2  Further traumas in childhood and adolescence that are outside the ACE questionnaire
• Stress of the mother during pregnancy or before (prenatal stress) (Graignic-Philippe et al. 2014;
Frasch et al. 2018)
• Prenatal toxins such as alcohol, smoking, hormone-active substances, heavy metals
• Early medication treatments (e.g. with glucocorticoids) of the mother during pregnancy or of
the child shortly afterwards (McGowan and Matthews 2018)
• Mother’s fear of the experience of childbirth (Rothenberger et al. 2011)
• Malnutrition of the mother during pregnancy (see David Barker)
• Low birth weight or premature birth (see David Barker)
• Malnutrition, hunger (Liu et al. 2003; Stickley et al. 2018; Jackson et al. 2019)
• Depression of the mother after childbirth (Murray et al. 1996)
• Young age of mother at first child
• Difficult operations and long-term hospital stays of the child
• Life as an orphan with institutionalization (example Romania) (see Michael Rutter)
• Life as an adoptive child with stepmother/stepfather
• Death of a parent or sibling
• Illnesses of parents and/or siblings with/without long-term separations
• Children who are left completely alone at home by their parents during the development phase
for hours (Wong et al. 2018)
• Loss of an important contact and reference person outside the parental home
• Loss of work by father or mother
• Single mother or father
• Low level of parental education
• Difficult economic conditions in the parental home, poverty (socio-economic disadvantages)
• Frequent change of place of residence and thus of the peer group (Anderson and Leventhal
2017)
• Living in homelessness (Patterson et al. 2014; Labella et al. 2019)
• Sexual abuse with same-age, same-sex persons (presses against one’s own wishes for sexual
acts or tries to press with/without the use of alcohol or drugs)
• Victims of child trafficking/human trafficking (Reid et al. 2017)
• Emotional abuse by schoolmates, for example for ethnic reasons (insult, accuse, denigrate,
humiliate, frighten)
• Problems at school with repeating a grade
• Life with intellectual disability, impairment of cognitive performance (cognitive disorder)
• Belonging to a group of people with a special sexual orientation (LGBT: lesbian, gay, bisexual
and transgender) (McLaughlin et al. 2012)
• Separation from a life partner
• Difficult neighborhood (criminal acts, no security or mutual assistance, etc.)
(continued)
22 2 What is a Child’s Psychological Trauma?

Table 2.2 (continued)


• Witnessing a fire in one’s own household
• Witnessing criminal acts against/by loved ones or strangers
• Witnessing of war actions and terrorism
• Separation of parents due to military service (Turner et al. 2017; Fear et al. 2018)
• Separation of asylum-seeking parents from their children (Miranda and Legha 2019)
• Natural disasters such as earthquakes, storms, tsunamis, etc. (Inoue et al. 2019)
• Expulsion, ethnic cleansing, crime against groups (e.g. Holocaust)

are more closely linked to physical sequelae in adulthood (Wickrama et al. 2015; Sheikh
et al. 2016). Whether this separation can be made so clearly is, in my opinion, uncertain.
If a parental home suffers from economic problems, other traumas also occur more fre-
quently. You remember the “snowball effect”.
A similar question that has been raised by scientists for a long time concerns the con-
nection between trauma subtype—for example, physical abuse as opposed to physical
neglect—on the one hand and subsequent problems in the future on the other. Does the
special trauma dimension play a role in future specific trauma sequelae? So the observer
can take the position: “All traumas trigger similar consequences.” In contrast, the fol-
lowing statement stands: “Each specific trauma triggers its own long-term effect.” With
regard to this distinction, the researchers are not in agreement, and I do not want to go
into it here (Smith and Pollak 2021).
Animals such as monkeys, pigs, sheep, guinea pigs, rats and mice can be exposed
to similar stress factors under natural conditions and have to fight similar sequelae
(sequelae in the next chapter). There are therefore animal models in which animals are
“exposed to prenatal or childhood stress” in order to uncover important factors for the
development of disease in later life and to find therapeutic approaches. If, for example,
scientists do not provide enough nest material to rat mothers, the rat mothers take care of
their offspring less well, which in turn leads to anxiety in the growing and adult young
animals (Ivy et al. 2008). This is an emotional and physical neglect.
Most questionnaires for the assessment of trauma are based on only one survey. These
are usually retrospective and do not allow a study of the connection between trauma and
time windows of brain development. Therefore, there have been good suggestions for the
assessment of trauma over a longer period of time in order to bring together trauma sub-
type and time of exposure (Teicher and Parigger 2015).

2.3 Time Windows for Bad Childhood Experiences

Due to the sensitivity of structures in the brain and in the body periphery (e.g. immune
system), long-term programming of embryos, fetuses, infants, children and adolescents
can occur at different times (Nederhof and Schmidt 2012; Slopen et al. 2013; Strøm
2.3 Time Windows for Bad Childhood Experiences 23

et al. 2013; Schalinski et al. 2019; Luby et al. 2020). Even the time before or during fer-
tilization of the egg is considered by some authors to be a sensitive phase in which this
programming takes place (Shachar-Dadon et al. 2009; Li et al. 2010; Strata et al. 2015;
Fleming et al. 2018).
We have known for a long time how smoking and alcohol can have a significant neg-
ative effect on the child during pregnancy (Stroud et al. 2018; Wozniak et al. 2019).
During pregnancy, mothers’ psychotraumatic life experiences can have an adverse effect
on the child. Other toxins can cause harm. These prenatal toxins can have long-term
effects into adulthood, as we saw in Table 2.2. Different time windows of exposure are
important for the subsequent problems.
To illustrate the principle of time windows, the view of the development of language
is helpful (Fig. 2.1). With regard to the uptake of information, on the one hand the brain
must be prepared by appropriate maturity, i.e. the corresponding nerve cell networks
must exist, and on the other hand the corresponding experiences must be offered by the
environment (Werker and Hensch 2015). In the brain, different regions have the highest
degree of sensitivity at different times (Luby et al. 2020).
There are limits to the various functions, after which the time windows are closed
and learning a function is either not possible or hardly possible at all. For example, the
time window for language comprehension closes after 15 months, for feelings of security
and attachment (Bowlby!) after 24 months, and for later reading after 24 months (Nelson
et al. 2019). Time windows are important when acquiring normal functions.
Typical time windows are prenatal (first, middle and last third of pregnancy), around
birth (0–2 months), infancy (2–12 months), early childhood (13 months to 4 years),
childhood (4–11 years), adolescence and adulthood (Fig. 1.1) (Hambrick et al. 2019).
Not always are these time windows well defined for later behavior because the
functions under consideration are complex and learned in different time windows.

phonological categories

Formation of word forms in the mother tongue

native phonetic categories

Preference for the


mother tongue
differentiation
of foreign
speech sounds

Fig. 2.1 Time windows of language development in years. There are critical time windows in early
brain development for sensitive, cognitive and affective dimensions. Early trauma can disrupt develop-
ment and cause lasting disorders. (Information from Werker and Hensch 2015)
24 2 What is a Child’s Psychological Trauma?

Disruptions within the time windows through various trauma situations, as mentioned
in Table 2.1 and 2.2, can strongly influence and affect normal behavior in the future
(Nelson et al. 2019). Scientists find similar time windows in animal models (Mueller and
Bale 2006; Matsumoto et al. 2009; Nishi et al. 2013; Andersen 2015).
Perhaps you yourself have witnessed such events. However, if you are one of the peo-
ple to whom none of this has happened, you may have no idea how common trauma is in
children, adolescents and young adults. In the next section, I would like to briefly illus-
trate the frequency of child abuse.

2.4 Frequency of Bad Childhood Experiences

Absolute numbers of abuse vary widely when looking at both the actually reported cases
and a dark field. In 2019, the German Federal Criminal Police Office recorded 13670
definitive cases of sexual abuse of persons under 14 years of age. The offenders are
almost 94% male and more than 60% are adult. The crimes mainly take place in small
towns with up to 20000 inhabitants and medium-sized towns (up to 100000 inhabitants).
The other consolidated cases of abuse of children under 14 years of age amounted to
3430 definitive cases. The offenders are predominantly adults (97.4%) and 45.5% of the
offenders are female. The numbers for the different years can be seen on the official web-
site of the German Federal Criminal Police Office (Federal Criminal Police Office 2019).
In a book chapter in 2013, Becker and Schulz described the dark field (Becker and
Schulz 2013):

The official statistics can only give an indication of the actual prevalence of traumatization
in childhood. The clarification of the dark field is possible to a certain extent by collecting
empirical data [note by the author: mostly retrospective].

In the following text, I refer to such retrospective studies on the dark field from three dif-
ferent countries. In addition, these publications further break down the individual forms
of abuse. The most common are the separation of parents or carers and economic hard-
ship in the family mentioned in large population-based studies (Table 2.3).
Children are particularly affected if they belong to an ethnic minority, are disabled or
sick, are in economic difficulties, are raised by one parent, or live in rural areas (Crouch
et al. 2019). Parents most often divorce when the children are between 13 and 17 years
old, just at a time of maximum orientation. If two partners live together but are not mar-
ried, there is more violence compared to a situation with married couples (Crouch et al.
2019).
The study conducted in Germany of students in Table 2.3 also shows a very dark
picture. The numbers are approximately comparable in all analyses, although the study
groups and survey periods differ and, therefore, a direct comparison is not allowed.
In the German study, 24.6% of respondents had 4 or more bad childhood experiences
2.4 Frequency of Bad Childhood Experiences 25

Table 2.3  Frequencies of bad childhood experiences


Type of abuse* USA1. N = 45,287. Germany2. N = 1466. Australia3. N = 7432.
Physical abuse 3.3–17.9 % 3.9 % 5.2–8.2 %
Sexual abuse 10.6–22.0 % 12.3 % 1.1 %
Emotional abuse 3.3–34.4% 19.6% 6.5%
Physical neglect 24.2% 4.6% 1.6%
Emotional neglect 9.4% 19.1% 1.6%
Physical violence by 5.0–17.5% 34.0% 5.5%
family members
Substance abuse in the 8.1–27.6% 12.5% 5.7–18.4%
family
Mental illness or suicide 7.1–18.8% 32.1% 17.8–23.8%
in the family
Separation/divorce of 21.9–27.6% 28.1% 14.4%
parents
Parent was incarcerated 3.4–7.9% not examined not examined
Death of a parent 2.9% 18.5% not examined
Economic difficulties of 22.5% 9.8% 11.6%
the parents
*When “parents” is used here, the same applies to protective/caregiving adults who are not the par-
ents. N is the number of people studied. 1) (Felitti et al. 1998; Afifi et al. 2011; Merrick et al. 2018;
Crouch et al. 2019); 2) (Wiehn et al. 2018) of students (!); 3) (Rosenman and Rodgers 2004)

(Wiehn et al. 2018). In other words, one in 4 students had serious experiences in his
childhood/youth.
This immediately leads to health-damaging behavior, because 18.9% of the students
examined drank alcohol regularly, 11.2% smoked daily, 17.9% took drugs, 17.5% had
early sexual experiences, 5.3% had many sexual partners and 11.4% showed signs of sui-
cidality (Wiehn et al. 2018). The conclusions of the German study on students make the
problem clear: There is a need for much more education and protection for those (espe-
cially children) who cannot speak for themselves. Political and therapeutic consequences
are discussed elsewhere (e.g. Spitzer and Grabe 2013; Masten 2014; Berens and Nelson
2015; Black et al. 2017).
In the third, Australian study of more than 7000 people, 15.3% of those examined had
four or more bad childhood experiences (Rosenman and Rodgers 2004). In this analysis,
the age of the respondents—whether 20 to 24 years, 40 to 44 years or 60 to 64 years—
had an influence on the reported frequencies, as the 40- to 44-year-olds had higher num-
bers than the other groups. The causes of this phenomenon are unknown according to the
authors, but it may be due to the higher stress levels of people in middle age. Because
26 2 What is a Child’s Psychological Trauma?

current stress worsens the assessment of the burden made when looking back (retrospec-
tive) (Colman et al. 2016).
Many publications are retrospective and unique in the sense of a cross-sectional study,
in which the adult respondents only look back on their childhood/adolescence at one sin-
gle point in time. In this regard, there may be distortions of the statements because the
problem is either increased or reduced in memory. For example, if the respondents are
suffering from a current depression or other stressful episodes, the hardships of child-
hood are often presented more dramatically (Colman et al. 2016). Some prospective and
retrospective studies show encouragingly similar results with regard to the statements
(Patten et al. 2015; Reuben et al. 2016; Jivraj et al. 2020). Accordingly, scientists can
rely on a good data base when using retrospective data.
This subchapter made it clear how common the problems are. One fifth to one quar-
ter of the population experienced negative childhood experiences, and we must ask
ourselves whether there are positive elements in childhood that can offset the negative
factors.

2.5 Compensating Positive Factors Against Trauma


Experiences

2.5.1 Resilience

The definition of resilience is diverse, since the word is used in different places. So the
engineer speaks of resilience when a system returns to its original state despite massive
disruptions. Resilience in energy management is the ability to not completely fail in the
event of breakdown and to return to the starting point before the collapse. In dental med-
icine, the doctor means the suppleness of the mucous membranes under stress. The mate-
rial scientist means the elastic property after deformation, which allows a return to the
original form (rubber as opposed to dough).
The German dictionary Duden essentially assigned resilience to psychology and
defined it as the ability to overcome difficult life situations without lasting impairment.
Since this is a somewhat long explanation, most people in German use the term “psy-
chological resistance” instead of resilience. Resilience is a process to find a functional
balance after trauma. An excellent book on resilience was written by Ann S. Masten, pro-
fessor at the Institute of Child Development at the University of Minnesota, Minneapolis
(Masten 2014). Ann Masten divides the systematic research work on resilience into four
temporally separate episodes (explanation 2):

Explanation 2: The four episodes of resilience research


1. Descriptive episode: Search for a definition of resilience, measurement of child-
hood trauma (variables), observation of the consequences of trauma and recogni-
tion of mental resistance
2.5 Compensating Positive Factors Against Trauma Experiences 27

2. Episode of clarification of processes leading to resilience and recognition of pro-


tective factors
3. Episode of therapeutic resilience promotion: Therapeutic approaches have been
tested that promote resilience in the affected person. They are based on the meas-
urement methods from episode 1 and the recognition of the protective factors of
episode 2.
4. Episode of integrated considerations (from the 2000s): With the help of many new
techniques from neuroscience and elsewhere such as imaging, genetics, epigenet-
ics, omics1 and statistics, an integrated view of resilience is taken. Furthermore, the
dynamics of events, the entire system including many body functions (including
immune system and metabolism), genes and the environment—the context—play a
major role in the multidisciplinary analysis. ◄

Resilience research recognizes childhood trauma and wonders why, despite adverse
sometimes unspeakable childhood experiences, often surprisingly gratifying develop-
ments in childhood , during adolescence, in young adulthood and later are still to be
observed. It recognizes the many follow-up problems after childhood trauma and tries
to promote resilience in a therapeutic sense. Resilience research was mainly interested in
the causes of the favorable development of a person affected, whereas risk research was
more concerned with the risks for the unfavorable child development and the follow-up
problems. If these things were strictly separated in the early years of both research lines,
the two camps come together in particular with the new multidisciplinary approach of
episode 4 (explanation 2).
A key statement from the resilience research is as follows: Mental resistance is often
present, very strongly developed and based on basic protective mechanisms that were
positively selected during evolution. In other words, we can say: During the evolution
mechanisms were retained and developed to provide protection in the event of adverse
early life conditions. These mechanisms are “adaptive”, like everything that was posi-
tively selected during evolution to maintain the adaptation of the individual to the envi-
ronment. Here they are, “the compensating positive factors against trauma experiences”
in the sense of the subchapter heading. What are the favorable factors in detail? See
Table 2.4.
There the focus is on competence, which is very important for the development
after bad childhood experiences. Because the more competent an affected person is, the
more gratifying is the later development (Masten 2014). Competence and resilience are
dynamic processes that change over time. Some people experience an early favorable

1 Omics: The scientific disciplines informally known as omics are various disciplines in biology
and medicine whose names end in the suffix -omics, such as genomics, proteomics, metabolomics,
microbiomics, glycomics, etc. There typically very many—sometimes all—variables are measured
and analyzed at the same time.
28 2 What is a Child’s Psychological Trauma?

Table 2.4  Factors for resilience and importance for an evolutionarily positively selected, adaptive
system
Resilience factors (if present, then favorable) Adaptive systems
Effective care and good parenting by parents or Attachment system; attachment to parents, car-
caregivers egivers, mentors, family, fellow human beings
Close relationships with other caring adults Attachment system; attachment to fellow
(mentor) human beings and creating social networks
Close friends and romantic loving relationships Attachment system; attachment to family and
fellow human beings
High intelligence and problem-solving skills Learning and thinking systems of the brain,
(competence) executive functions*
High self-control, emotion regulation, planning, Self-regulation systems of the brain, executive
good working memory and good conscious functions*
attention control (competence)
Highly success-oriented motivation Motivation and reward systems
(competence)
High self-efficacy expectation, high self-confi- Motivation and reward systems
dence (competence)
Strong belief (religiosity), great hope and Spiritual and cultural social systems
enormous trust in the meaningfulness of one’s
own life
Effective schools Educational systems
Effective neighborhoods; good collective con- Systems of fellow human beings in a social
trol of behavior within a community network
Personality traits such as openness to experi- Is associated with multiple adaptive systems
ence (openness), conscientiousness, agreea-
bleness, and low emotional instability or low
vulnerability (no neuroticism)
List according to Ann Masten (Masten 2014). * Executive functions include self-control, emotion
regulation, planning ability, working memory, and conscious attention control

development (early bloomers) after the traumatic time, and others go only relatively late
in a favorable direction (late bloomers are more often women). In competent and resil-
ient people, the positive aspects of one adaptive system can cascade to the next and cre-
ate a positive overall picture.
As Table 2.4 suggests, personality—formerly called temperament—is of great impor-
tance. By the way, temperament was already recognized by Immanuel Kant as a fun-
damental element of human nature (Kant 1833). Openness to experience (openness and
curiosity), conscientiousness (perfectionism), agreeableness (consideration, willingness
to cooperate, empathy) and low emotional lability or vulnerability (opposite of neuroti-
cism) are very favorable factors for competence and resilience. Children and adolescents
with these properties are more stable and have a high emotional, social and motivational
2.5 Compensating Positive Factors Against Trauma Experiences 29

control over a long period of time (Shiner and Masten 2012). In addition, there seem
to be reciprocal effects between personality and competence. Competence problems
lead over time to negative personality traits, which are unfavorable in the further course
(snowball effects). On the other hand, favorable personality traits can lead to high com-
petence and stabilize the personality (Masten 2014).
In the assessment of personality traits, the researchers found strong cultural differ-
ences. So children with positive traits like agreeableness and friendliness were not the
preferred children in studies of Masai groups because the Masai valued higher the ”diffi-
cult“ children with a strong perseverance and intensive self-assertion. Interestingly, chil-
dren with the latter properties survived a drought period in 1974 better than the agreeable
and friendly children (deVries 1984). Is it with the Masai like with the aggressive bird
children who by their appearance snatch the food away from the others in the nest?
Maybe, but the number of Masai children studied was small, and reliable findings can
only be gained with caution.
Table 2.4 also lists self-control, emotion regulation, planning ability, working mem-
ory and conscious attention control, which are all important elements to plan and
control one’s own behavior while taking into account environmental conditions. In neu-
roscience, the term ”executive functions“” has established itself for this range of tasks.
Furthermore, self-motivation, will formation and action initiative belong to this block of
executive functions. The better the executive functions are, the higher the competence
and resilience. In the brain, this is essentially the frontal lobe, which is in close interac-
tion with other areas (Masten 2014).
Furthermore, children learn stress management and avoid the stressor, actively seek
support, plan problem solutions, seek balance in religion (culturally dependent), and
control the stress reaction. Motivation and reward are important factors (Geschwind et al.
2010), which are located in the dopaminergic reward areas in the brain, such as the core
areas of the Nucleus accumbens and other places. Motivation/reward, resilience, and the
personality trait “openness to experience” are closely related. However, the incorrect or
maladaptive use of this system is associated with addiction—reward and addiction are
causally linked (Masten 2014).
Thus, every human being has “compensatory positive factors” that were developed
and maintained during the course of evolution. Why are most studies only investigating
the risk factors for poor development and not the positive factors for favorable devel-
opment? Only in the last 5–10 years, work groups have begun to take both factors into
account equally under the strong influence of resilience research.

2.5.2 Unfavorable Versus Favorable Childhood Experiences

First, I want to give a concrete example of how bad childhood experiences (ACE,
adverse childhood experiences, Table 2.1) can lead to a later dilemma. Girls with hard-
ships in childhood have early menstrual periods and are often pregnant as teenagers. This
30 2 What is a Child’s Psychological Trauma?

is an important topic because these teenagers often do not graduate from high school,
are disadvantaged throughout their lives, and pass similar behavior on to their own chil-
dren (Hillis et al. 2010). The working group of Vincent Felitti and Robert Anda wanted
to examine whether parallel positive family experiences regarding early unwanted
pregnancy in teenage years protect these girls with early traumatic experiences (Hillis
et al. 2010). Family/parents/caregivers are important for resilience, as we have seen in
Table 2.4.
To do this, the authors used a questionnaire in the well-known way, which examined
the following positive family factors: family cohesion, family support, family cares for
its members, family protects members, family gives self-confidence and mental strength,
family members like each other and family cares for the health of members. These ques-
tions were examined together with the ACE questions from Table 2.1. The focus was
therefore on negative and at the same time compensating positive factors in childhood.
In the survey of more than 3500 participants, the risk of a trauma-related early preg-
nancy was lower in those women with a large number of positive family factors, even
if they had previously made bad childhood experiences. In addition, the women with a
positive family situation had fewer job problems, less family and financial problems, less
intense stress experiences, and outbursts of anger (Hillis et al. 2010). For girls without
bad childhood experiences, the positive family factors are unimportant for the frequency
of pregnancies during adolescence. This is illustrated in Fig. 2.2.
In the years 2015–2018, another questionnaire emerged from resilience research,
which was similar to the ACE questionnaire by Vincent Felitti (Table 2.1) (Narayan et al.
2018). The authors called it BCE, which stood for benevolent childhood experiences
(Chung et al. 2008).
If you look at the factors in Table 2.5, you will see the similarity with the resilience
factors from Table 2.4. The first author had a lot of experience with resilience, as she
had previously worked with Ann Masten. Similar questionnaires developed in different
places (Chung et al. 2008). This provided an opportunity to test the favourable and unfa-
vourable influence factors at the same time.
A study of pregnant women demonstrates this possibility. Expectant mothers with
previous adversities in childhood did not reveal the typical symptoms of post-traumatic
stress disorder and had less stress experiences if they also had positive childhood expe-
riences. The unfavourable experiences were offset by favourable childhood experiences,
and pregnancy was less stressful (Narayan et al. 2018). Since stress factors during preg-
nancy affect the child, favourable childhood experiences can serve both the mother and
the unborn child.
Similar studies of more than 1200 college students with unfavorable childhood expe-
riences showed how protective factors had positive effects on physical and psychological
health on the one hand and social interactions on the other (Powell et al. 2020). Further
studies confirm this view (Atzl et al. 2019; Bethell et al. 2019; Crandall et al. 2019).
Regardless of abuse in childhood, positive experiences in childhood lead to a lower car-
diovascular risk in middle adulthood (54 years) (Slopen et al. 2017).
2.5 Compensating Positive Factors Against Trauma Experiences 31

50 With bad childhood experiences (+ACE)

Frequency of pregnancy during Without bad childhood experiences (–ACE)

40
adolescence (%)

30

20

10

0
0–1 2–3 4–5 5–6
Number of positive family factors in childhood

Abb. 2.2 Pregnancy in adolescence. This figure shows the relationship between positive family factors
in childhood and the frequency of pregnancies in adolescence. The red bars show the situation with bad
childhood experiences and the white columns the constellation without unfavorable childhood experien-
ces.—Abbreviation: ACE, adverse childhood experience (bad childhood experience). The more positive
family factors there were in children with trauma (red bars), the less likely it was to become pregnant in
young years

Table 2.5  Questions about benevolent childhood experiences (BCE questionnaire)


As you grew up (during the first 18 years of your life)
• did you have at least one carer (e.g. parents) who made you feel safe?
• did you have at least one good friend?
• did you feel like you were growing up in a safe environment?
• did you like school?
• did you have at least one teacher who cared about you?
• did you have a pleasant neighbourhood?
• was there an adult (not a parent or carer) who could support or advise you (e.g. teacher, mentor,
coach, neighbour or friend)?
• did you feel that you were living a “good life” overall?
• did you like yourself or find yourself pleasant?
• were there typical routine activities at home, such as regular meals and a regular bedtime?
32 2 What is a Child’s Psychological Trauma?

In another study, an impressively better result was observed with regard to life expec-
tancy in adulthood when children grew up in an environment with a low socioeconomic
status (and were disadvantaged as a result), however, a lot was invested in their educa-
tion, and they achieved a high level of education (Montez and Hayward 2014).
In this section we were able to see how favorable experiences in childhood can offset
the hardships in the same life phase to some extent. In recent years, in addition to resil-
ience and positive childhood experiences, another favorable aspect has been considered
that scientists described with child sensitivity to negative environmental experiences. It’s
about orchids and dandelions.

2.5.3 Orchid or Dandelion

When scientists look at their data, they often see a large variation. This situation is illus-
trated in the completely fictional example in Fig. 2.3. The scattering is enormous in the
left subfigure, and the scientist does not see any connection between the two variables on
the X and Y axes.
If it is possible to find an important characteristic or several important characteris-
tics to form two clearly different groups from a single group (Fig. 2.3, middle and left),
the previously large variation can be eliminated and relationships between the variables
can be observed. However, these things are rare in science, but sometimes there is a big
“aha” moment. Thomas Boyce had such an “aha” event.
In terms of child trauma, Boyce early on pointed to a different sensitivity of children
(Boyce et al. 1977). If an affected person is very sensitive (orchid), the child trauma will
have a stronger effect than if the person has a low sensitivity (dandelion).
Boyce has defined the feature SENSITIVITY with different criteria, and in this way
he was able to divide a total group like in Fig. 2.3 into a subgroup A and B with different
sensitivity. In fact, he found clear clinical and laboratory evidence for a different sen-
sitivity (Boyce 2019). Sensitive children reacted much more strongly to stress stimuli
than non-sensitive boys and girls, because they showed a stronger stress response in the
form of a cortisol release by the adrenal gland and an increased sympathetic reaction
with adrenaline release. The two increased reaction patterns are very important for the
chronic immune activation that is at the center of the book (Chap. 4).
For Boyce, in the 1980s and 1990s, genetic factors were most likely responsible for
the different sensitivity. At that time, the focus was on DNA, the genetic substance, with
fixed and unchangeable information. At the same time, behavioral genetics experienced a
strong boom (Plomin and Colledge 2001). Boyce, Plomin, Belsky suspected genetic var-
iants in the genome that were supposed to make the difference. From father and mother,
we each receive a copy of the DNA of a gene, and this copy can differ in some aspects. It
is not fundamentally different—no completely different gene product (=protein) is pro-
duced—but there are variants that change the function of a protein. In these variants, the
2.5 Compensating Positive Factors Against Trauma Experiences 33

Group total = Group A only Group B only


Group A + Group B
Reaction to stress (points)

Stress (points) Stress (points) Stress (points)

Fig. 2.3 Variation in scientific studies between stress and stress reaction. In this example, the relation-
ship between stress on the X axis and stress reaction on the Y axis is shown. All the people studied are
represented in a single graph on the far left (total group), and the correlation between the two variables is
almost zero. We do not see any connection between stress and stress reaction. If it is possible to distingu-
ish the entire group based on an important characteristic into group A (with characteristic) and group B
(without characteristic), the situation can look completely different, as shown in the two other subfigures
in the middle and on the right. Now there is a positive linear relationship for group A and a completely
reversed, negative linear relationship for group B. Unfortunately, in reality it is not so easy to find a dis-
tinguishing characteristic

observers saw the key to explaining different sensitivity at that time. But before investi-
gating a single gene product, twin and adoption studies came first.

2.5.4 Favorable or Unfavorable Genetic Predisposition

When the structure of the genetic substance was published by Watson and Crick in 1953,
it took almost 50 years to decode the human genome in 2000. The Human Genome
Project involved 12 American, 3 German, 2 Japanese, 1 English and 1 Chinese institute,
and between 2001 and 2003, the key aspects of the genome were published in the journal
Nature (summarized in: U.S. Department of Energy—Office of Science and Office of
Biological and Environmental Research 2019). Nevertheless, genetic influences on dis-
eases or behavior were already well known before these important publications. In the
field of behavioral genetics, primarily twin studies and adoption studies contributed to
findings.
34 2 What is a Child’s Psychological Trauma?

2.5.4.1 Phase 1—Twin and Adoption Studies


Robert Plomin, whom we already know, wrote the following in an article from 2001 with
a retrospective view on 40 years of research (Plomin and Colledge 2001):

Many diseases, such as schizophrenia, were explained on the basis of environmental influ-
ences only in the 1960s [AU: although it is different]. (…) Even in the 1970s, there was
a great controversy in psychology about behavioral genetics. (…) While in the 1980s and
especially in the 1990s psychology accepted the genetic influence on individual behavior
(…), today any behavior is determined genetically in some way.

I quote Arthur Schopenhauer on the subject of truth (Schopenhauer 1819), “which is


only granted a short victory celebration between the two long periods when it is con-
demned as paradoxical and underestimated as trivial”. This way to truth in behavioral
genetics was experienced by twin and adoption researchers like Robert Plomin.
In these studies, the study leaders chose the child participants in order to clearly sep-
arate genetic from environmental influences. Typically, these are long-term studies in
which children are observed for many years or decades. For example, if the researchers
looked at identical twins, these children had a 100% match of their genes. If they grow
up with their own parents and differ later in their adolescence or adulthood in their indi-
vidual behavior, this is due to the environmental share that they did not experience at the
same time (this is called non-shared environment). These environmental influences may
have been exerted, for example, in kindergarten, at school, in everyday life, or in differ-
ent circles of friends, etc. (Plomin 2018).
In contrast, when the researchers looked at genetically different adoptees who grew
up in the same family with genetically unrelated parents, they were able to make a state-
ment about the identical environmental influence of this family. You can imagine how
further comparison groups can be formed (e.g. identical twins who grow up in separate
families and are brought together later, etc.), and in fact the studies with twins and adop-
tees are very sophisticated and well thought out (Plomin 2018).
The results from these studies are based on a solid foundation because thousands of
children were included in these analyses. Under such circumstances, positive and neg-
ative influence variables could be worked out that have a positive or negative effect on
behavior. For example, parents may prefer one child and negatively influence the other
child. The favored are later prosocial and sympathetic, whereas the disadvantaged are
often antisocial and depressed (Neiderhiser et al. 1999). In this way, parents can act in a
compensatory or divisive way, and thus attenuate or amplify a childhood trauma.
With these experiences of twins or adoptees, no statements can be made about indi-
vidual genes because these things were not investigated. However, the statements of
behavioral geneticists are very clear due to the large epidemiological approach with
many children (Plomin 2018). Plomin says: “50% of behavior is inherited when consid-
ering different dimensions.” The consideration of individual genes could only be carried
out after their systematic discovery, starting in the late 1990s. The consideration of gene
2.5 Compensating Positive Factors Against Trauma Experiences 35

variants experienced a strong upswing with the publication of the human genome at the
beginning of the 2000s.

2.5.4.2 Phase 2—The Candidate Genes


In the early phase of this research approach (1990s and 2000s), certain genes identified
as candidates were chosen because they fit well to a disease mechanism or to a behavior
for biological reasons. The candidates were biologically plausible. The most important
candidate genes that fit to behavioral aspects are summarized in Table 2.6, and the mean-
ing of the respective factor is listed there.
Often, the receptors for neurotransmitters or hormones came into the center of inves-
tigations. In part, their transport proteins were considered, which are important for the
uptake of neurotransmitters or hormones into cells. In addition to the neurotransmitter
candidates that are associated with brain performance, the scientists examined hormonal
factors from the area of cortisol
​​ control and cortisol action.
Cortisol is the stress hormone of the adrenal gland. In the brain, a cascade is set in
motion by psychological stress, which begins in the hypothalamus with corticotropin-re-
leasing hormone (CRH), which in turn releases adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH)
from the pituitary gland, which stimulates cortisol from the adrenal glands. Cortisol
inhibits its own release by a negative feedback mechanism at the level of the hypothala-
mus (blockade of CRH) and at the level of the pituitary gland (blockade of ACTH). This
hormone axis was called the HPA axis (hypothalamus, pituitary gland, adrenal gland),
at the end is the hormone cortisol, which in turn has a inhibitory effect on the other hor-
mones (negative feedback). The feedback is important because otherwise an excessive
cortisol level would damage the brain and the rest of the body (inhibition of brain func-
tion and negative influence on the immune system).
A high activity of the HPA axis has often been related to depression and anxiety dis-
orders, because the lack of negative feedback does not lead to an inhibition of CRH in
the hypothalamus, therefore cortisol remains constantly elevated and thus causes fol-
low-up problems (Dwyer et al. 2020). The matter will be picked up in more detail below.
The first significant study of these candidate genes in the context of childhood adver-
sity from Table 2.6 appeared in the scientific journal Science in 2003 (Caspi et al. 2003).
There the serotonin transporter from Table 2.6 (top) was examined in more detail for the
first time. But there is a history to it, which is briefly summarized in the explanation 3
together with the results.
The variant of the serotonin transporter was a big thing at the beginning, a fantastic
find, an egg of Columbus. If scientists believe they have found something great, the rav-
ages of time often damage the results because there are always other scientifically active
groups that confirm or refute things. This is good scientific practice, which often seems
strange to the lay observer.
Our virologists have demonstrated this in the corona crisis because the statements
depended on and changed with new studies. The confirmation/refutation is a ­necessary
36 2 What is a Child’s Psychological Trauma?

Table 2.6  Candidate genes*, which were chosen for the studies


Candidate genes (observable Significance with biological plausibility
changes)
Neurotransmitters
Serotonin transporter (with Serotonin is an important neurotransmitter that is significant in
lower serotonin function) depression. Less released serotonin means more depression.
Monoamine oxidase type A Important enzyme that breaks down serotonin, noradrenaline,
(with low or high activity) and dopamine. Little released serotonin and noradrenaline
means more depression. Too much noradrenaline means
aggression.
Catechol-O-methyltransferase Important enzyme that breaks down noradrenaline, adrena-
(with low or high activity) line, and dopamine. Little released noradrenaline means more
depression. Too much noradrenaline means aggression.
Dopamine transporter (with Dopamine is an important neurotransmitter that is important for
lower dopamine function) reward behavior, motivation, and attention. A lot of dopamine
released means high attention, motivation, and reward experi-
ence. Little of this neurotransmitter makes the opposite.
Dopamine receptor type D2 Dopamine is an important neurotransmitter that plays a role
(with lower dopamine function) in reward behavior, motivation, and attention. Little dopamine
action means less of these functions.
Dopamine receptor type D4 Dopamine is an important neurotransmitter, is relevant to
(with lower dopamine function) reward behavior, motivation and attention. Less dopamine
action means less of these functions.
Nerve growth factors
BDNF (with lower function) BDNF is an important nerve growth and survival factor for
nerve cells. A reduced function would lead to disturbances of
nerve growth and survival—with the consequence of nerve cell
death.
Hormones
CRH receptor (with increased CRH releases adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH) from the
activity) pituitary, which in turn stimulates cortisol from the adrenal
glands. Much CRH function means high cortisol. Too much
cortisol is unfavorable because it contributes to depression,
anxiety disorders and brain dysfunction and negatively affects
the immune system.
Glucocorticoid receptor (with The glucocorticoid receptor is the receptor for the stress hor-
high or low sensitivity to mone cortisol of the adrenal glands. If there is a low function,
cortisol) the negative feedback is low and more cortisol is released (and
vice versa with strong function). Too much cortisol is unfavora-
ble because it contributes to depression, anxiety disorders and
brain dysfunction and negatively affects the immune system.
(continued)
2.5 Compensating Positive Factors Against Trauma Experiences 37

Table 2.6 (continued)


Candidate genes (observable Significance with biological plausibility
changes)
FK506 binding protein 5–1 Cortisol must bind to a cortisol receptor in the cell for its effect,
(FKBP51) (with low or high which is composed of various subunits. FKBP51 is part of the
amount) complex of the cortisol receptor and is decisive for its effect in
the cell. If a lot of FKBP51 is present in the cell, cortisol is less
effective (and vice versa if there is less FKBP51).
Oxytocin receptor (with low or Oxytocin is a hormone that initiates the birthing process and
high function) activates the milk glands. In addition, it has a supportive role in
the relationship between mother and child, the sexual partners
and in the interaction between people. If a lot of oxytocin is
present, these things are more pronounced (and vice versa if
there is less oxytocin).
*From (Strauss et al. 2004; Bakermans-Kranenburg and van Ijzendoorn 2011; McQuaid et al.
2013; Zhang and Belsky 2020). Abbreviations: BDNF, brain-derived neurotrophic factor; CRH,
corticotropin-releasing hormone.

process that only stabilizes a result when it has been confirmed many times. The observ-
ers must be very patient until something is confirmed, and many do not have this
patience. If the politician has a legislative period of 4 or 5 years ahead of him, he nec-
essarily has less patience, which is why science and the promotion of science must be
kept out of the usual time frame of a legislative period. Those who do not understand this
damage science.

Explanation 3: Serotonin Transporter, Childhood Trauma and Depression


At scientific congresses, a few chosen ones are allowed to give a presentation, most
only show a poster with contents of their own research work. The poster is typically
attached to a display board. At certain times, interested scientists gather in front of
this poster. Being chosen as a speaker does not have to be an advantage. The interac-
tion with other scientists is much better at the poster than around a lecture, because
only the truly interested meet at the poster. I found my best friends at posters and
hardly after lectures. The lecturer, however, floats on a cloud and doesn’t really find
any contact with the audience, even if listeners come to the lectern after the lecture. I
know both perspectives and summarize: The egocentric concentration on the lecture
blocks the social interaction that is possible at the poster.
This type of happy, poster-dependent contact took place with the later couple of
Avshalom Caspi from a kibbutz in the Negev desert in Israel and Terrie Moffitt from
North Carolina. The two newly minted doctors had a scientific poster side by side in
St. Louis in 1987. Today they are two widely known psychologists who deal with the
influence of genes on behavior and who also take into account the influence of envi-
ronmental factors.
38 2 What is a Child’s Psychological Trauma?

In the 1990s, they started these considerations together with a New Zealand group
from Dunedin and an English team at King’s College in London (Caspi et al. 2003).
But how did it come about? In 1972–1973, Phil Silva from New Zealand examined
1037 children who were born in a certain hospital in Dunedin. After the initial exami-
nation, he and the further team followed the history of the children at regular intervals
at the ages of 3, 5, 7, 9, 11, 13, 15, 18, 21 and 26. The examined group remained
constant in the relatively isolated Dunedin, as 96% of the original group was still
available at the age of 26. This is a highlight of a longitudinal study that was highly
interesting for Caspi and Moffitt and others.
In a study, Caspi, Moffitt and colleagues then showed how the rate of depressive
episodes and suicidal thoughts increased with the number of early traumatic experi-
ences. It increased especially in those who had a serotonin transporter with less ser-
otonin function. Since serotonin is an important “mood-enhancing” neurotransmitter,
less serotonin released from the nerve cell means more depression. This was the first
study to show a link between a gene and an environmental factor in the field of child-
hood trauma. ◄

Back to the candidate gene of the serotonin transporter by Caspi and Moffitt (explanation
3), which was put to a 10-year patience test. Despite numerous attempts at repetition,
some studies were unable to reproduce the same effect. Sometimes the study leaders
observed no or even opposite results!
In so-called meta-analysis, which summarizes many studies on the same subject, the
matter remained unclear and led to considerable criticism of the investigation of individ-
ual candidate genes (Manuck and McCaffery 2014; Sharpley et al. 2014; Plomin 2018;
Zhang and Belsky 2020). This is true for all of the factors listed in Table 2.6, which went
through a similar history. There are no clear relationships between a gene variant and
adverse childhood experiences, both of which influence a later health problem in adult-
hood, for the large number of affected individuals (Plomin 2018).
However, there may well be candidate genes that are essential for a certain disease or
for a particular behavior. There are examples of this with the so-called monogenic dis-
eases. A genetic change with a deficiency of the monoamine oxidase type A mentioned
in Table 2.6, for example, leads to aggressive and autistic behavior with more frequent
crime (Brunner syndrome) (Kniffin and McKusick 2018). This is a clear connection
between a single gene and a behavior.
In most cases, more than just one gene is behind complex behavior. Many genetic
factors influence behavior (Plomin 2018) or, in the sense of Boyce, sensitivity (orchids
versus dandelions), which is why the search for the sole genetic factor is always difficult.
This knowledge has now become more and more established, which is why the people in
this research landscape use two other methods that take into account the combination of
several genes (Plomin 2018). These are described in the next subchapters as phases 3 and
4. Phase 3 describes the candidate approach with a hypothesis and phase 4 the hypothe-
sis-free genome-wide human approach.
2.5 Compensating Positive Factors Against Trauma Experiences 39

2.5.4.3 Phase 3—Combination of Candidate Genes


If you look at Table 2.6, you will get the idea of how to combine several gene variants in
a combined risk factor. If a child with adverse living conditions has several unfavorable
gene variants at the same time, it has a high combined risk of suffering from an unfa-
vorable consequence later in life. Or, conversely: A person can be protected if he or she
has several compensating factors (=protective gene variants) at the same time. With the
genes mentioned in Table 2.6, polygenic risk measures have been developed and applied
(Belsky and Beaver 2011; Davies et al. 2020; Huang und Starr 2020; Starr et al. 2020;
Zhang und Belsky 2020).
As has already been done with the candidate genes, there are now the first meta-anal-
yses for the polygenic risk measures. The matter has been more successful here than
with the meta-analyses of individual candidate genes. For example, if we look at sev-
eral genes that regulate the amount of the neurotransmitter dopamine in the brain, those
traumatized persons with a low polygenic risk have a favorable development course
(Bakermans-Kranenburg and van Ijzendoorn 2011).
Certainly, it is more promising to combine several gene variants than to look at a sin-
gle candidate gene. For this, those gene variants have to be examined in combination
for which there is a biological plausibility. By this I mean the combination of gene vari-
ants in a cumulative risk factor, which contribute to a common biological function with a
corresponding phenotype such as aggression (lots of noradrenaline) or depression (little
noradrenaline or little serotonin) (Zhang und Belsky 2020).
In addition to this approach, there is another way of investigation that is not initially
directed at any biological plausibility, but which will be the most promising of all. By
this I mean the hypothesis-free human genome-wide association studies.

2.5.4.4 Phase 4—Human Genome-Wide Association Studies


While decoding the human genome in the 1990s, human genome-wide association stud-
ies were designed. In these studies, thousands of people with a certain disease on one
side are compared with healthy control persons on the other side with regard to thou-
sands of gene variants across the entire genome—hence the term “human genome-wide”.
Here the researchers examined all gene variants of the two groups without a specific
hypothesis, which was possible after the development of corresponding powerful tech-
niques for the analysis of many genes.
With these association studies, individual gene variations can be found completely
without hypothesis, which are significantly associated with a disease. The first prelim-
inary study of this kind dates back to 1994 in patients with psoriasis (Tomfohrde et al.
1994). Psychiatrists were very quick in this respect, and as early as 1995 an international
group found the first risk genes for schizophrenia (Moises et al. 1995).
Whether these first, non-hypothesis-based, human genome-wide studies were use-
ful in the further course was examined 20 years later by means of meta-analyses, and
I can take it in advance: Not all found gene variants were confirmed, but many were
very useful. Further hypothesis-free gene search took place for the following diseases:
40 2 What is a Child’s Psychological Trauma?

inflammatory bowel diseases, diabetes mellitus, obesity, heart attack, macular degener-
ation, rheumatoid arthritis, prostate cancer, schizophrenia, depression and many more.
Here, there were partly fantastic new findings, which brought us very many important
insights into the pathogenesis and cause of the disease.
In neuroscience, the focus was first on diseases of psychiatry (depression, schizo-
phrenia, anxiety disorders and bipolar disorders) and neurology (Parkinson, Alzheimer,
epilepsy) and less on behavior, disturbed behavior or stress sensitivity in the sense of
orchids and dandelions by Thomas Boyce. The patients’ diseases from psychiatry and
neurology were better definable, at least the doctors believed. The investigation of behav-
ior is difficult because it depends on many influencing variables and diagnostic tests,
which are not always uniform and clear. The definition of prostate cancer is much more
clear.
Therefore, it took a long time in this field of behavioral research for the first useful
human genome-wide association studies on the sensitivity of children to appear. A first
survey brought together childhood trauma and later depression. The study consisted of
several parts. In a first step, typical risk variants for depression were defined in the context
of a human genome-wide association study with more than 15,000 people. In a second
step, on the basis of the found genetic variants, the researchers formed a polygenic risk
score. In a third step, they found the following after the analysis. If an unfavorable poly-
genic risk score was present, those children with a traumatic experience in their young
years had a much higher probability of later suffering from depression than those children
with a favorable risk score and traumatic experiences. In other words, there are compen-
sating factors (=favorable genetic variants) that make the occurrence of depression less
likely despite a bad childhood experience (Peyrot et al. 2014; Colodro-Conde et al. 2018).
These are the predicted sensitivity factors that are decisive for orchids and dandelions.
Whether the polygenic risk score was oriented towards a biological plausibility was
not important, because the approach was initially hypothesis-free. The genetic vari-
ants found in this way were often provided with a biological function by other working
groups only in the course of further research work, and biological plausibility post hoc
shows up. This is painstaking work that can take many years (e.g. Straub et al. 2018;
Straub et al. 2021).
In another genome-wide study, the authors examined 1026 identical twins (Keers
et al. 2016). The twins had a different degree of anxiety (=phenotype). With the help of
this phenotypic difference between the twins, a human genome-wide association study
was carried out, which defined a polygenic risk score for anxiety. Now the authors exam-
ined a further group of 1406 children in a second step, in which they related the parental
education, the newly found polygenic risk score and the emotional problems of the chil-
dren. Here the polygenic risk score was able to make a prediction between unfavorable
education and emotional problems.
In a third approach, the same authors examined whether the polygenic risk score
could predict whether cognitive behavioral therapy is effective in children with neg-
ative emotions. In fact, those children with a high risk score benefited most from the
2.5 Compensating Positive Factors Against Trauma Experiences 41

individual therapy. Those traumatized and at-risk children—the orchids—achieved the


highest gain after an improvement in environmental conditions through individual ther-
apy. The dandelions with a low risk score did not benefit and were not as emotionally
affected (Keers et al. 2016). Fortunately, this work could be repeated with a correspond-
ing study approach with a similar result by other authors (Lemery-Chalfant et al. 2018).
At this point we now recognize the risk variants that make children sensitive like
orchids or insensitive like dandelions. This is a wonderful discovery because we can
diagnose and treat sensitive children to put them on a better path in life. It looks like
those highly sensitive children would benefit most from the therapy methods. However,
these therapy studies require further confirmation.
In addition to the pure psychological/psychiatric meaning, candidate genes (phase 2),
combined consideration of several candidate genes (phase 3) and combined genetic var-
iants from genome-wide association studies (phase 4) can give us clues why the at-risk
individuals have a more active immune system that can contribute to chronic inflamma-
tion (more on this later). In addition, the findings from the four phases of genetics can be
used to better classify and plausibly approach epigenetic questions.

2.5.5 Favorable or Unfavorable Epigenetic Changes

The term epigenetics I have already illuminated in explanation 1. If the scientist speaks
of an epigenetic change, he means a subsequent modification of genes or gene-related
sections of DNA or of DNA packaging proteins, without changing the sequence of DNA
building blocks. Although biologists have known such epigenetic principles since the
1960s (McClintock 1961; Allfrey et al. 1964), the phenomenon only came into focus of
the broader biomedical science from the 2000s onwards.
Starting around 2010, studies with epigenetic content are now being published reg-
ularly. This environmentally induced modification of the reading of genes leads to an
increase or inhibition of intracellular protein production, and this principle is by no
means engraved in DNA. The image of the engraving is appropriate because after the
production of the material (DNA like metals, stone or glass), subsequent changes (chem-
ical groups like ornaments, writings and decorations) can be added.
This subsequent influence on gene reading came into the focus of new research
interests. The phenomenon helped to understand how environmental influences can
cause long-term changes to genes and thus functions. The emphasis is here on long-
term, because they can be passed on from generation to generation (more on this in the
Sect. 2.7). Such a platform was necessary because people needed an explanation for why
there are more than 200 different cell types in the body, even though all cells have the
same set of genes. If all the cells have the same genetic starting point, why are there 200
different cell types? Shouldn’t they all look the same?
No, because cells develop depending on their internal autonomous and external con-
ditions—that is, through the “local environmental influences” in the neighborhood of
42 2 What is a Child’s Psychological Trauma?

the cells—in one direction or the other, namely from fertilization of the egg cell. This
was already the topic of Hans Spemann (1869–1941) in his experiments with amphibi-
ans, when he was able to prove how tissue transplanted early in embryonic development
behaved in a location-specific manner. The neighborhood influences create epigenetic
changes that lead to the reading of a certain set of genes, but not to the simultaneous
reading of all genes. In this way, cell diversity can arise.
Epigenetic modification is the starting point for plasticity (malleability), which
allows adaptation to environmental conditions. Others spoke of programming (Barker
1998), when it came to early influence in the uterus (embryonic and/or fetal), around
birth (perinatal) or in childhood and adolescence. Today we recognize the importance
of early years in the process of development of a living being, in which this program-
ming is the origin of health and disease (developmental origin of health and disease, e.g.
Barker 1998, 2007; Gabory et al. 2011; Rinaudo and Wang 2012; Breton 2013; Hanson
and Gluckman 2014, 2015). I subscribe to this idea of early programming. It is taken up
again in the Sect. 2.8. These long-term effective epigenetic influences are important for
the malleability of the brain and the immune system. But which genes are epigenetically
modified?

2.5.5.1 Phase A—The Candidate Genes are Epigenetically Modified


Above, we had mentioned the candidate genes when we spoke of the different sensitiv-
ities of orchids and dandelions (Table 2.6). However, variations in the candidate genes
were not well coupled with this sensitivity, although early studies in the 2000s suggested
this connection (Caspi et al. 2003). Meta-analyses of many studies brought the exclu-
sive consideration of individual candidate genes into difficulty, and therefore modern
researchers turned to polygenic risk measures or whole-genome considerations (Phase 3
and 4).
The friends of epigenetics went a similar way because they first included the candi-
date genes in their considerations. Indeed, most of the candidate genes in Table 2.6 were
checked to see if there were epigenetic changes (Park et al. 2019). For this purpose, typ-
ically leukocytes or lymphocytes from blood or DNA samples from saliva were tested.
In fact, the researchers identify epigenetic markers in these peripheral blood leu-
kocytes, for example in the gene of the glucocorticoid receptor, the FKBP51, the ser-
otonin transporter and the nerve growth factor BDNF, all factors from Table 2.6. For
these genes, an increased methylation of susceptible DNA sections is observed (Park
et al. 2019), which reduces the readability of the genes (methylation see explanation 1).
Consider Table 2.6 again and you will recognize what the reduced or increased function
means. The reduced function can be unfavorable and contribute to depression. Others
showed demethylations at other sections of the same genes—that is, the removal of
methyl groups (see explanation 1)—as a result of traumatic life circumstances, which
can also have unfavorable effects (Klengel et al. 2013). Whether methylation or demeth-
ylation is important remains very vague for one or the other gene. Here, there are only
2.5 Compensating Positive Factors Against Trauma Experiences 43

relatively few basic research studies that show the way to the goal in a causal sense. With
regard to the transfer of all these results to humans, there is a catch.
If the telescope is aimed at depression or other psychopathological states in adulthood
after previous childhood traumas, it is surprising when one looks at the conditions in
white blood cells or in the DNA from saliva (Thompson et al. 2013; Park et al. 2019;
Szyf 2019). We learned that epigenetic differences are relevant for the differentiation of
cell types. Wouldn’t the doctors rather have to examine local material from brain areas
that are directly related to the respective psychopathology?
Difficult, if not impossible, because the researchers do not have easy access to
brain material for ethical reasons. Here, studies on human brains can help, which were
removed from deceased persons. Some groups have actually done this, but the number of
cases is very small (Lutz et al. 2017). Furthermore, animal experiments can lead to suc-
cess because tissue can be removed at will (Jensen Peña et al. 2012; Schraut et al. 2014;
Wu et al. 2014; Massart et al. 2016; Cao-Lei et al. 2017).
Due to the short time period since the start of epigenetic surveys, the number of stud-
ies on candidate genes is small. The first meta-analyses do exist, but we do not yet have a
final picture of the epigenetic changes of the candidate genes. Certainly we need a com-
prehensive view of many genes, as is done in a polygenic approach (see phases 3 and 4
in the previous subchapter). In any case, the removal of DNA from leukocytes and saliva
is problematic for this question.
The study of leukocytes can lead us to another important clue, namely chronic
immune activation (which will be reported below). The studies on children with trauma
say the following for the moment: Instead of the favorable epigenetic changes, the unfa-
vorable modifications are observed. Conversely, if a child is resistant to the methyla-
tions of the critical candidate genes, it has a compensating positive factor in terms of the
subchapter heading. A child thus protected may be a dandelion.

2.5.5.2 Phase B—Polygenic Approaches in Epigenetics


For this approach, the observers need, as in the human genome-wide association studies
for gene variants, a human genome-wide investigation of the methylations of gene sec-
tions. Methylation was an important aspect of epigenetic change that leads to the inhi-
bition of gene reading (explanation 1). In fact, corresponding tools have been developed
with which the scientists can investigate the methylations of many gene sections on a
large scale (Bibikova et al. 2006; Bibikova et al. 2011). The second important aspect
of epigenetic regulation, acetylation (explanation 1), is, however, much less in focus
because assignment to a defined gene are more difficult to obtain.
In addition, the first studies already appeared in 2011, which applied the technique
to a small number of abused children (Naumova et al. 2012; Yang et al. 2013; Cicchetti
et al. 2016). However, the interpretability of these small surveys is still low. More recent
studies were carried out on a maximum of 200–300 people, but unfortunately the value
is still unclear because leukocytes were used as a source of DNA again. I summarize:
In these human genome-wide methylation studies there are no really hard data so far.
44 2 What is a Child’s Psychological Trauma?

In addition, the other possibilities of epigenetic influence (e.g. acetylation, explanation 1)


would have to be considered in parallel.

2.5.5.3 Summary—Epigenetics
The first meta-analyses of candidate genes show positive relationships between methyla-
tion at certain gene loci on the one hand and childhood maltreatment on the other (Phase
A). The current findings point to a promotion of circumstances that favor depression
in adulthood. This research approach is relatively new, and we are all eagerly awaiting
basic research that includes an adequate number of affected individuals and goes beyond
mere associative considerations. Important methodological considerations include, for
example, the analysis of the right cell material. In the end, epigenetics will shed new
light on the connection between childhood trauma and later diseases in adulthood.
Epigenetics offers explanations of how early childhood episodes stimulate long-term bio-
logical changes.

2.6 Child Disadvantages and Steeling Effects

In the previous chapters, we met important compensating positive factors that protect the
affected with early traumatic experiences from consequences. We wonder if these posi-
tive factors must be stimulated by adversity, if resilience must be induced, if epigeneti-
cally not only the bad, but also the good things are set in motion. Thus, some scientists
assume a steeling effect induced by adverse childhood experiences. Others call such a
form of induced insensitivity to adversity “stress vaccination”. The proponents of the
theory speak of a kind of vaccination, in which an early administration of the unfavora-
ble factor generates psychological resistance in later life.
In case-reports, preferably in competitive sports, among creative artists and people with
outstanding performance, early traumatic experiences are repeatedly brought together
with a favorable course in later life (Joseph and Linley 2006; Seery et al. 2010a; Shrira
et al. 2010; Cheng et al. 2015; Damian und Simonton 2015; Mittal et al. 2015; Sarkar
et al. 2015; Standing et al. 2015; Simonton 2016; Sarkar und Fletcher 2017; Thomson
und Jaque 2018). Steve Jobs, the founder of Apple, is a good example. Another impressive
model is Édith Piaf, the very successful French chanson singer in the 1950s, etc.
In recent studies, so-called bell-shaped curves have been found, which correspond to
those in Fig. 2.4. A Swiss working group examined 270 older people with different num-
bers of adverse childhood experiences (Höltge et al. 2019). Approximately 8–9 unfavora-
ble childhood experiences were associated with optimal quality of life and best possible
mental health. Zero or few bad experiences are associated with lower quality of life and
poorer mental health. On the other hand, too many early traumatic experiences are bad
for both quality of life and mental health (right in Fig. 2.4) (Höltge et al. 2019). Here
we enter the field of psychopathology, which we will discuss in more detail later in the
2.6 Child Disadvantages and Steeling Effects 45

a b
Individual quality of life

Mental health

Number of bad childhood experiences Number of bad childhood experiences

Fig. 2.4 Steeling effect, stress vaccination. This schematic figure shows the relationship between the
cumulative number of adverse childhood experiences on the one hand and quality of life (a) or mental
health (b) on the other. The blue and red function resembles a bell-shaped curve (shape of a bell). The
vertical line shows the mean number of adverse childhood experiences for the entire group of subjects.
That is where the optimal range lies. In the literature, the average number of childhood traumatic expe-
riences is not always 8–9, as in this example. Depending on the measuring method, depending on the
trauma experiences queried, depending on the age of the examined and depending on the culture, the
location of the optimal range is different. Elsewhere, I identified the number 4 as the critical unfavora-
ble number when investigators used the Felitti questionnaire of Tab. 2.1. The information comes from
Höltge et al. (2019)

book. These studies support the idea of stress vaccination, but the number of subjects is
small.
There it is again, the bell-shaped curve, which I had reported on extensively in my last
book on memory (Straub 2020). A bell-shaped curve occurs when a variable represents a
favorable response and is plotted as the Y coordinate, and higher values indicate
​​ a more
favorable result (see Fig. 2.4). Inversely, U-curves arise when higher values on the Y-axis
show an unfavorable result. Bell-shaped curves or U-curves always occur when there
are optimum ranges, and optimum ranges are often the result of a balanced development
during the course of evolution (Sect. 2.8). Further studies by other groups confirm the
bell-shaped curve or the U-curve (Edge et al. 2009; Seery et al. 2010a, b; Howell and
Sanchez 2011; Boyce 2016; Shakiba et al. 2020).
In another impressive example, the steeling effect of previous traumatic expe-
riences in childhood is shown. You may remember the tsunami of March 11, 2011 in
the Fukushima region, which led to a nuclear disaster in the local nuclear power plant
Fukushima-Daiichi and to the nuclear power plant shutdown promise of Chancellor
Merkel in Germany. This tsunami, caused by the Tōhoku earthquake of magnitude 7,
46 2 What is a Child’s Psychological Trauma?

not only devastated the nuclear power plant, but also other regions of the Fukushima and
Miyagi prefectures, for example the 2 million city of Iwanuma. The death rate was 2% of
the population, and the devastation was enormous with a flooding rate of 48% of the total
area. In the Tamaura district, 68% of the houses were completely or partially destroyed.
Such events often provide an opportunity to study the stress response of the affected
residents, which was done in Iwanuma with 580 participants over the age of 65 in a
large-scale study (Inoue et al. 2019). Natural disasters can trigger post-traumatic stress
disorder, and this was related to these old people’s unfavorable childhood experiences.
The tsunami triggered post-traumatic stress in 56 people, 524 showed no such signs.
Triggers for post-traumatic stress disorder were the destruction of the house, the loss of a
relative or the death of people from the circle of friends.
A total of 40% of the respondents had no traumatic childhood experiences, 39%
remembered a negative episode, and 21% reported two or more events. The participants
remembered things like economic hardship, the loss of a parent, the divorce of their par-
ents, paternal violence against their mother, physical punishment of themselves, and
emotional abuse (insults). Thus, similar to what we know from the ACE questionnaire
from Table 2.1.
If unfavorable childhood experiences are protective in the sense of steeling, those
people without childhood experiences should suffer from post-traumatic stress disor-
der more often than those with early childhood trauma. This is exactly the result of the
Japanese tsunami study, because only in participants without a childhood trauma did the
devastation of the tsunami trigger a post-traumatic stress disorder (Inoue et al. 2019). In
this study, no search was made for a bell-shaped or U-shaped relationship between the
number of childhood adversities and post-traumatic stress disorder.
Should children therefore be deliberately burdened for the purpose of steeling effects?
Disgusting idea! However, this has happened again and again over the centuries in var-
ious cultures. The already reported example of the Masai in the subchapter Resilience
shows in the same direction. However, we know a clear connection between childhood
trauma and psychopathological sequelae such as depression, anxiety disorders, post-trau-
matic stress disorder, and many other physical problems such as cardiovascular diseases,
diabetes mellitus, tooth problems, etc. How would we know in targeted steeling attempts
of our children where to set the limit of adversity?
The connection between childhood trauma and the occurrence of post-traumatic stress
disorder after deployment in a crisis area has been investigated in soldiers. After a war
deployment, people are more at risk if they experienced childhood trauma (Pratchett
and Yehuda 2011). Where is the steeling effect here, the stress vaccination? Where is
the bell-shaped curve here? Such steeling attempts or stress vaccinations are dangerous
because you do not know which child you have in front of you.
Finally, we must remember the remarks of Thomas Boyce, who described sensitive
Orchids and robust Dandelions. The properties of the two types depend on genetic traits.
In natural disasters, Dandelions may fairly well survive, while Orchids suffer. The dis-
cussion of “steeling by adversity” without considering the genetically/epigenetically
2.7 Transmission of Behavior from Generation to Generation 47

determined sensitivity of those affected is one-sided, misleading and dangerous. It could,


for example, incite researchers to expose all children to a program of moderate adver-
sity without regard to their sensitivity, in order to vaccinate them against stress. We have
had such programs in Germany before (Reichsarbeitsdienst, youth organizations in the
National Socialism), but we certainly do not want them back.
So what do we learn from this subchapter? The phenomenon of steeling or stress vac-
cination exists, but we by no means know how a researcher can implement this princi-
ple in a meaningful way with which child using which methods. For ethical and legal
reasons, experiments on children using systematically applied adversity fortunately are
prohibited anyway.

2.7 Transmission of Behavior from Generation to Generation

Sometimes investigators find psychological abnormalities in grandparents, parents, chil-


dren, etc. over several generations. They may be favorable and positive or unfavorable
and negative, if you think, for example, of creativity and tolerance or addiction and psy-
chopathology. Obviously, behavior is passed on from one generation to the next.

2.7.1 Genetic Transmission

Remember the statement of Robert Plomin, who classified the degree of heritability of
behavior at 50%. So it’s no wonder that there is a transmission of behavior. If on average
50% of behavior is passed on genetically to the next generation, we can infer in return an
environmental share of also 50%. Plomin recognizes after many population-based stud-
ies, which have a high degree of validity, that these environmental influences are largely
independent of the family (Plomin 2018). Who would have expected that?
Plomin explains how children with the same family environment are different because
they make decisive experiences outside the family (non-shared environment). Usually we
think here of experiences elsewhere, in daycare centers, kindergarten, primary schools,
secondary schools, peer groups, sports clubs, with friends and partners, in the job, etc.
However, it has so far been difficult to define such factors outside the family well (Plomin
2018). Individual randomly appearing experiences set the course in one direction or the
other. I can remember events of this kind that changed me a lot because they were nodes
in my life path (e.g. explanation 4). Nodes are present when, at a time (node), there are at
least two possible life paths and a backward movement is not possible.
Plomin also says: “We would be just as similar to our parents and siblings (50%
genetically determined) whether we grew up in our own family or were adopted by
another genetically foreign family.” This is the case if both families have a normal—that
is, average—form of education without severe childhood traumas. Plomin focuses on the
totality of all children (therefore population-based).
48 2 What is a Child’s Psychological Trauma?

However, there are a few exceptions among the many behavior characteristics stud-
ied, where family is significant, namely in religiosity, political views, intelligence and
school performance, where the shared environment in a family is only responsible for
10–20% and decreases significantly with age (Plomin 2018). For example, with intelli-
gence and performance, the non-genetic familial influence disappears completely after
the 20th year of life. For most of the behavior characteristics studied, the non-genetic
family share is insignificant.

Explanation 4: Randomly occurring events


Randomly occurring experiences from the environment sustainably influence behav-
ior, as I once experienced. In December 1972, FC Freiburg had to play at home
against Borussia Mönchengladbach in the soccer cup competition. You remember:
Berti Vogts, Günther Netzer, Jupp Heynckes, Rainer Bonhof, Herbert Wimmer and
Co. That was a big sensation for me, because FC Freiburg was only in the south-
ern regional league and not significant nationwide. In order to be able to see this foot-
ball game, I wanted to go to the ticket office, but unfortunately I fell so badly with the
bicycle and got a kidney contusion. The urine was bloody—into the clinic.
In the early 1970s, there were no ultrasound examinations, and so during one
night I was monitored hourly by measuring the waist circumference on the surgical
intensive care unit of the University Hospital Freiburg. If the waist circumference
had increased by a certain percentage, the doctors would have operated immediately.
Although this did not happen, I experienced a longer hospital stay with lasting effects:
1. I had to repeat the grade because of the long sick leave and the resulting poor per-
formance. 2. This episode impressed me intensely, because it promoted the decision
in my 13th year of life to become a doctor in order to be able to help in the same way.
A lot depended on these decisions, because they led to a different life, the life of a
doctor and later a scientist (there were other nodes for the direction of science).
By the way: FC Freiburg won 3–1 against Mönchengladbach without me, lost 7–1
in the return leg, and Mönchengladbach was able to continue. That was not a node for
the FC Freiburg. ◄

2.7.2 Epigenetic Transmission

In addition to the purely genetically determined transmission we know another form of


transmission. I have made an example of this with the sentences to David Barker (Barker
et al. 1989). You may remember: Children with low birth weight have a significantly
higher risk of later heart attack. Since newborns were already affected, Barker and his
team were finally able to point to a possible problem in the uterus during pregnancy
(Barker et al. 1989). The nutritional situation in the uterus sensitized people in old age
to heart attacks, strokes, hypertension, age-related diabetes and for childhood infectious
2.7 Transmission of Behavior from Generation to Generation 49

respiratory diseases (Barker et al. 1993). The “Barker phenomenon” clearly points to
epigenetic changes in the child during the time in the uterus (explanation 1).
Such epigenetic changes can be passed on from generation to generation. Various
ways of passing from generation F0 to F1, F1 to F2, F2 to F3, etc. have been described,
which are either called intergenerational (F0-F2) or transgenerational (from F3 onwards)
(Fig. 2.5) (Heard and Martienssen 2014). The inheritance can only take place if the DNA
of the germ cells undergoes an epigenetic change, because only then can the information
be passed on to the next generation via either the oocyte or the sperm. Some called this
process a re-programming of the germline, because the germ cells are influenced (Heard
and Martienssen 2014).
Practically, this often happens in plants, and so an epigenetic trait can be maintained
over generations and hundreds of years (Cubas et al. 1999). With animals, it has rarely
been observed over a longer period in the sense of transgenerational transmission in Fig.
2.5, for example with the nematode (C. elegans) (Heard and Martienssen 2014). If we
can only currently surmise the situation with humans, intergenerational transmission as
in the “Barker phenomenon” from the first section is relevant. Whether there is trans-
generational transmission over several generations in humans is unlikely (Heard and
Martienssen 2014), because after the fusion of the germ cells, a complete demethylation
takes place—so to speak a “reset” (Heard and Martienssen 2014). If the methylations
are important for epigenetic transmission, the reset is necessarily linked to the deletion
of epigenetic markers. The bypassing of a reset has not been described in humans so far.
You certainly remember Jean-Baptiste Lamarck (1744–1829), who at the end of
the 18th century founded the lamarckism. He said: If the organism uses an organ fre-
quently, it will be strengthened, developed and enlarged, whereas non-use leads to atro-
phy. According to Lamarck, this is the “inheritance of acquired traits”. They are acquired
because they are used intentionally. Lamarck speculated how giraffes, in order to feed,
intentionally stretched their necks to high leaves and, because of this desired process, got
long necks over the course of evolutionary history.
Darwin’s theory of evolution tells us something different: For random reasons (muta-
tions in the genome), a giraffe got a longer neck, and because this was favorable for eat-
ing high leaves and for survival and reproduction, this long-necked giraffe was positively
selected. The information for this feature was retained in the giraffe’s genome over the
course of evolutionary history. This is the inheritance of a trait not caused by intentional
action. After Darwin, Lamarckism was pushed back (exception in the Soviet Union in
communist times under the Soviet agronomist Trofim Denissowitsch Lyssenko, because
he taught how desired traits could be passed on to the next generation; this fit in with the
planned economy in communism).
With the information we have received in the last decades in the field of epigenet-
ics, Lamarckism is experiencing a kind of rebirth. Lamarckism and Darwinism exist
side by side. As Fig. 2.5 shows, a noxa can lead to an epigenetic modification of the
DNA, and this change can be transmitted to the offspring generation. And as with the
giraffes that want to reach the high leaves lamarckistically intentionally, this noxa could
50 2 What is a Child’s Psychological Trauma?

Fig. 2.5 Inter- and intergenerational


transgenerational inheritance
of epigenetic information. In
DNA of the
generation F0, an epigenetic
germ cell
change can occur in the DNA changed
of the germ cells of F0 and in
DNA of the
the DNA of the germ cells of germ cell
an unborn child F1 through an changed
influence such as stress, toxins,
nutrition, smoking, alcohol, etc.
F1 then passes on this epigenetic
change and becomes pregnant.
Since the germ cells of F2 are DNA of the
already laid down in F1 during germ cell
the pregnancy of F0 with F1, changed
the epigenetic change in F1 is
already taken over by F2. Up
to this point, scientists speak
of intergenerational inheritance
of the trait.—From generation
DNA of the
F3, scientists speak of a true germ cell
transgenerational inheritance of changed
the trait if F3 was not exposed to
the above-mentioned influence
at any time.—The offspring
generation F2 does not pass the transgenerational
DNA of the
epigenetic change on to F3 in germ cell
most cases. If this did happen, changed
it is very often not permanent
(left). Very rarely does a
stable inheritance take place
(middle), and occasionally the
epigenetic trait is lost in further No transfer stable transmission Transfer & Loss
generations (right).—The order (frequent) (rare) (occasional)
of the DNA building blocks is DNA of the
germ cell Loss
not changed by this influence.
changed of DNA
This is the special feature of the
modification
epigenetic change (explanation
1). (Information from: Heard and
Martienssen 2014)

be administered intentionally. If we look at the orange area of Fig. 2.5 in the lower part,
Lamarck’s form of transmission is very rare and not stable over many generations, at
least not in humans. Lamarckian inheritance is far less significant in animals and humans
than Darwinian inheritance, but it exists in plants (Heard and Martienssen 2014). At the
end of this subchapter, I would like to make an example of epigenetic transmission from
generation to generation.
2.7 Transmission of Behavior from Generation to Generation 51

2.7.2.1 The Dutch Hunger Winter—An Example of Intergenerational


Transmission (F0 to F2)
During the German occupation of the Netherlands in the Second World War, a severe
famine occurred between October 1944 and the end of April 1945, which people in
Holland called the Hongerwinter. A German blockade prevented the Dutch from receiv-
ing food and fuel from the southern and rural regions of the country from September
1944 onwards. Around 18,000 to 22,000 people lost their lives, and calorie intake fell to
1000 kcal/day.
As expected, there were many pregnant mothers (F0 in Fig. 2.5) and affected fathers
(F0) who were exposed to hunger during this critical period. Daughters (F1 in Fig. 2.5)
and sons (F1) from this Hungerwinter birth cohort and their children (F2), the grandchil-
dren of the starving mothers (F0) and fathers (F0), were subsequently studied in medi-
cal long-term studies up to the present day (Veenendaal et al. 2013). As a control group,
daughters/sons (F1) and their children (F2) whose parents/grandparents (F0) were not
affected by the Hongerwinter were used.
After decades of scientific observation, the focus is now on the grandchildren, that
is, the F2 generation according to Fig. 2.5. Affected grandchildren (F2) whose grand-
fathers had starved showed higher body weight and a higher body mass index (body
weight in kg divided by the square of body height in m, unit: kg/m2) than control chil-
dren. However, grandchildren whose grandmothers had starved did not show this differ-
ence (Veenendaal et al. 2013). At the time of the study in 2013, the grandchildren (F2)
were on average 37 years old and showed no health problems such as hypertension,
diabetes mellitus, hypercholesterolemia, asthma, allergic eczema or cardiovascular dis-
eases, which may still come (the studies are ongoing). Similar studies were carried out
in Sweden with the same results (Bygren et al. 2014). This is a true intergenerational
transmission.

2.7.3 Transmission—without Genetic and Epigenetic Explanations

Many authors in the field of psychology and sociology use the terms “intergenerational”
and “transgenerational” incorrectly to describe the transmission of behavior (see Fig.
2.5), because they neither look at genetics nor epigenetics. For example, they describe
mothers who themselves suffered from childhood adversities and now have toddlers who
show conspicuous emotional behavioral reactions, as if this were passed on from genera-
tion to generation (Hipwell et al. 2019). It is unclear how the process of transmitting the
conspicuous behavior from the mother to the child takes place.
Other authors studied mothers who were traumatized in childhood, who were socially
poorly integrated, had high anxiety, showed depressive symptoms, more fatigue, sleep
disorders and more pain compared to a control group. Now the children of these moth-
ers also showed more depressive symptoms at the age of 8–12 years. Here it is said that
52 2 What is a Child’s Psychological Trauma?

intergenerational transmission takes place, although neither genetics, epigenetics nor


anything else was included (Dennis et al. 2019).
Mothers who have experienced childhood trauma are often depressed, and this can
intensify during pregnancy and shortly afterwards. For example, one study showed how
negative emotionality and conspicuous behavior in children of traumatized mothers are
closely linked to the mothers’ symptoms of depression (Bouvette-Turcot et al. 2020).
Nothing is reported about genetics or epigenetics.
Others showed that mothers who were exposed to adversities and harsh methods of
upbringing repeated the harsh methods with their own children. This transmission of
rough motherhood leads to a similar style of upbringing over generations (Lomanowska
et al. 2017). The authors speculate about genetic and epigenetic influences, nothing of
the sort was properly investigated.
Many similar studies and review articles can be mentioned here (Bifulco et al. 2002;
Galler and Rabinowitz 2014; Widom et al. 2015; Cowan et al. 2016; Bowers et al. 2018;
Kane et al. 2018; Najman et al. 2018; Schoon und Melis 2019; Zipple et al. 2019).
Traumatized mothers and fathers pass on negative elements to their children, but there
are no reliable genetic or epigenetic findings. I am not saying that these data do not exist
in these studies. But up to the time of discovery, the technical terms should be used cor-
rectly. Why don’t the authors write “transmission from generation to generation” more
correctly?

2.8 An Evolutionary Medicine Perspective

I had already reported in detail on biological evolution in my earlier books, and I do not
want to deepen the theory here again (Straub 2018, 2020). The evolutionary psychology
is a discipline that combines aspects of biological evolution and psychology. “Adaptation
to the environment” and “reproductive success” are central in Darwinian terms.

2.8.1 Historical Development

As early as Charles Darwin, he integrated aspects of biological evolution with behav-


ior when he reported on “the evolutionary basis of emotional expression in mammals”
(Darwin 1872; Snyder et al. 2010). He recognized some parallels in the forms of emo-
tional expression between humans and animals, and he took this similarity as evidence of
common ancestors. He considered behaviors such as anger, fear, surprise, disgust, happi-
ness, and sadness.
These early considerations were long forgotten, and it was not until the boom in
Ethology (comparison of humans and animals) by Nobel laureates Niko Tinbergen,
Konrad Lorenz, and Karl von Frisch that a new area emerged within psychology in
2.8 An Evolutionary Medicine Perspective 53

the early 1970s, the “evolutionary psychology” (Ghiselin 1973; Wood-Gush 1963).
Sometimes it takes a long time for something to break through.
After that, there was a phase of pros and cons against evolutionary psychology
because the idea of mind and body as separate entities still prevailed (Bunge 1979).
The cons said: Mind and thus psychology are not influenced by evolutionary processes
because evolution only affects the material part of the body (e.g. the DNA).
Increasing progress in comparative anatomy and physiology in the 1980s found neu-
ral structures in humans and animals that were seen as a prerequisite for similar behavior
(e.g. Kaas 1989). At the same time, behavioral genetics experienced a boom. Plomin and
colleagues said: 50% of behavior is genetically determined (Plomin and Colledge 2001).
Thus, any positively selected behavior received a stable genetic anchoring.
At the same time, scientists developed animal models that led to problems in later
life following previous traumatic adversities during early development. A good exam-
ple is “maternal deprivation”, which we learned about from John Bowlby and is associ-
ated with attachment disorder and other consequences in adulthood. Animal experiments
using maternal deprivation showed similar consequences in monkeys, rats, mice, and
guinea pigs as studies in children after separation from caretakers. The similarities
between animals and humans in terms of behavior and thus a common ancestry were
obvious.
Later, the theory of evolution was applied to psychiatric disorders such as anxiety
disorders and others (Nesse 1990; Glover 2011). Anxiety was interpreted as an adap-
tive advantage that protects the individual or group in dangerous situations because the
anxious warn and protect themselves in time. Anxiety is therefore basically normal, but
excessive and recurring anxiety can make one sick. The expression of anxiety is, like all
behavioral expressions, a continuous variable with a distribution with values of zero, lit-
tle, medium, much, or very much (Fig. 2.6).
Furthermore, evolutionary psychologists recognized many “adaptive advantages”
of personality traits, for example in social exchange (in relation to extroversion and
agreeableness), in the formation of coalitions (in relation to agreeableness), in altruis-
tic behavior (in relation to conscientiousness and agreeableness), with regard to physical
attractiveness (in relation to extroversion), the number of sexual partners (in relation to
extroversion), the testing of social behavior (in relation to emotional instability and vul-
nerability; neuroticism) and the detection of fraudulent behavior (in relation to emotional
instability and vulnerability; neuroticism) (Buss and Penke 2015). Adaptive advantages
were positively selected, and so the adapted individual is better equipped for life.
The first theories that brought biological evolution, early positive and negative child-
hood experiences and fertility/reproduction into direct connection date back to the
early 1990s by Draper and Belsky (Draper and Belsky 1990; Belsky et al. 1991). The
authors discussed the following: individuals from a protected childhood become sexually
mature in later ages, have their first sexual intercourse later, form stable relationships,
take less risks, develop a positive relationship with the opposite sex and take better care
of their own children. Conversely, it is the case with children who have early traumatic
54 2 What is a Child’s Psychological Trauma?

Fig. 2.6 The expression of


anxiety is a continuous variable
with a positively skewed
normally distributed

Number of people affected


distribution. On the x-axis, positive skew
the anxiety variable is plotted. negative skew
People can have little or much
anxiety. On the y-axis, the
number of people affected
is plotted. For anxiety, a
positively skewed distribution
function typically results from
surveys of people (red line).
For other continuous behavioral
variables, normal distributions
(blue) and negatively skewed
distributions (green) can result
0 little medium much a great deal

Anxiety variable
(points on a scale)

experiences, who mature sexually early, have many partners and do not live healthily
(Belsky et al. 2010). Meanwhile, these relationships have been confirmed in many stud-
ies with animals and humans.
If people experience adversity in childhood, it is adaptive in the sense of evolutionary
medicine if the different body systems are set to a shorter life span. For example, after
traumatic childhood experiences, girls experience early physical maturity and they have
early sexual intercourse, as a recent large-scale meta-analysis found in 43 studies (Zhang
et al. 2019). Intriguingly, this adaptive program transfers to decision-making processes,
as people with previous adversity make decisions faster than people without this history
(Knowles et al. 2019). The acceleration of life is reflected in an accelerated aging pro-
cess (a subchapter in Chap. 3 Accelerated Aging).
Today we think that only the protected variant has an adaptive advantage because
these people are healthier and live longer. This does not have to be the case under other
circumstances—for example in war situations, in countries with unstable conditions or
in cultures with other orientations (e.g. Masai!). The adaptive advantage of children with
negative and traumatic experiences is significant because they find their way around in
a tough environment, have children in the same way, and do so earlier and more often.
From a perspective of evolutionary medicine, they must reproduce successfully, and
that’s all that counts. They are, so to speak, programmed for the adverse conditions and
pass on these properties. These considerations were developed by Jay Belsky in the
1990s (Belsky 1997).
If we look at the connection between childhood/adolescent stress experiences and
later psychopathological or other physical ailments (see examples in Chap. 1), there
are very strong relationships between the one and the other, as is the case in the above
2.8 An Evolutionary Medicine Perspective 55

example of the protected and the lifestyle. With this knowledge, some authors have given
positive and negative childhood experiences an evolutionary medicine significance since
the mid-2000s (Ellis and Bjorklund 2005; Bakermans-Kranenburg and van Ijzendoorn
2007; Belsky et al. 2007; Belsky and Pluess 2009; Del Giudice et al. 2011; Glover 2011;
Bakermans-Kranenburg and van Ijzendoorn 2015; Hanson and Gluckman 2015; Boyce
2016; Szepsenwol and Simpson 2019; Shakiba et al. 2020).
If this connection between the early childhood situation and the later situation dur-
ing life is so strong, then—according to the considerations of various groups—a sound
theory must exist that can integrate these connections. So some others come to a simi-
lar conclusion as Draper and Belsky, and they developed it further (Ellis and Bjorklund
2005; Bakermans-Kranenburg and van Ijzendoorn 2007; Belsky et al. 2007; Belsky
and Pluess 2009; Del Giudice et al. 2011; Glover 2011; Hostinar and Gunnar 2013;
Bakermans-Kranenburg and van Ijzendoorn 2015; Hanson and Gluckman 2015; Boyce
2016; Szepsenwol and Simpson 2019; Shakiba et al. 2020).
The model says roughly the following. In the course of evolution, mechanisms were
positively selected to set switches in regulatory systems of the brain and in other parts of
the body (e.g. immune system) during early development in utero, around birth, in early
childhood up to puberty, which allow adaptation to later life situations. Put a little sim-
pler: Harshly raised children then fit into a rough environment and gently raised children
fit into a peaceful and supportive world. Everyone fits into their niche.
This theory explains how possibilities for change were maintained during the course
of evolution (positively selected), which still allow later modification of the individ-
ual after the fusion of the germ cells. This modification is plasticity (Belsky and Pluess
2009). When I speak of plasticity, I mean subsequent changes by epigenetic processes,
where information is amplified or not read from the existing gene (explanation 1). Here
the environment can have an influence, and this possibility of modification was posi-
tively selected during the course of evolution to allow for plasticity.
Not reading means setting the switch to NULL (e.g. methylation = DNA reading pre-
vented), and amplified reading means pushing the switch to ONE (e.g. demethylation).
Possibly whole networks of genes are turned on or off. Otherwise we would not see the
continuous changes in the observed variables as in Fig. 2.6, but a drastic ON or a clear
OFF. This machinery serves during the extremely plastic development to better prepare
individuals for their own future.
This first consideration is now linked to the idea of Boyce and Belsky of the “different
sensitivities” of the dandelions and orchids, and already a very impressive synthesis has
arisen. How does this theory work out?

2.8.2 Childhood Trauma and Evolutionary Medicine—the Results

Children from the same family can be quite different, like Thomas Boyce and his sis-
ter! Parents unconsciously set their children up for at least two different life plans, and
56 2 What is a Child’s Psychological Trauma?

that’s why the children in a family are often different (Dunn and Plomin 1990; Plomin
2018). Life plan 1 always results in promoted and cared for life and life plan 2 results
in readiness for rough and combative environment (Boyce 2019). One of the children
is often handled carefully, it is sensitive or delicate, an orchid, and the educators have
to be gentle and supportive with this child in order to create something good. The other
child is brought up rougher, does not receive the same care and gentleness, rather a strict
upbringing, and yet everything goes well, a dandelion. How does all this work?
On the one hand, children with their genetically determined 50% behavior programs
create the conditions for this careful/supportive or rough/combative result because they,
through their nature, trigger the respective supportive behavior patterns of the parents.
Plomin calls this “nature of nurture” and he means by it the way the children understand,
interpret, search for and actively shape their environment (nurture) influenced by genes
(nature) (Plomin 2018). Children actively shape their environment and also their parents,
and this action is genetically determined in the children. An orchid triggers something
different than a dandelion.
On the other hand, the environment changes the children through the setting of the
epigenetic switches (explanation 1) (Boyce 2016, 2019). At the beginning, the genetic
predisposition for an orchid or a dandelion is laid down, but only the corresponding envi-
ronmental influences create the real orchid and the true dandelion. Genetic and epige-
netic processes are responsible at the same time, according to the protagonists (Belsky
and Pluess 2009; Boyce 2016, 2019). The children from the two different camps are
themselves involved in this by shaping their environment—keyword “nodes of life”
(Plomin 2018). Why do we need something like this?
From an evolutionary medicine perspective, this parent-controlled difference is
favorable because at least one child in a supportive and peaceful environment can often
achieve high performance and be healthy (orchid) and because the other child can be
successful and healthy in a rough but also normal world. In the respective niche, one and
the other can develop and—what is evolutionarily central—survive and reproduce.
However, adverse childhood experiences, as described in the Sect. 2.2, are often of
a strong nature and go far beyond the aforementioned peaceful or harsh upbringing.
Nevertheless, the theory should be applicable, whereby the situation may lead to a strong
shift in the stress reaction. Figure 2.7 shows the relationship in the form of a U-shaped
curve (Shakiba et al. 2020), which is the mirror image of the bell-shaped curve. A bell-
shaped curve is created when a favorable reaction is plotted on the Y-axis and higher val-
ues show a more favorable result (see Fig. 2.4). Conversely, U-curves arise when higher
values on the Y-axis show an unfavorable result (Fig. 2.7, blue line).
If I come to this U-curve on the X-axis from left to low childhood stress and from
right to high childhood stress, the reactivity is high and the quality of life and mental or
physical health are often endangered or poor. This is called maladaptive reaction because
the normal, expected reaction ranges of the organism are exceeded with regard to vari-
ous functions. We say: The answer is outside the reaction norm of Fig. 2.7. This happens
much more easily with orchids because the niche is smaller.
2.8 An Evolutionary Medicine Perspective 57

Stress reactivity of the HPA axis


and the sympathetic nervous high = outside the
unfavorable reaction
norm
The niche of the dandelions
system

is large, actually not


a niche anymore,
because they can
get along anywhere. reaction
The niche of orchids is norm
small, because they can
low = only manage under
favorable conditions.
favourable
low medium high
family stress

Fig. 2.7 Relationship between bad childhood experiences—here family stress—and stress reactivity.
For stress reactivity of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis (HPA axis) or the sympathetic nervous
system on the Y-axis, I can enter the frequency of psychopathological and physical sequelae as equiva-
lent. The small niche for orchids is yellow and the large area for dandelions is green. Orchids feel com-
fortable in the small yellow niche, dandelions in the large green area. If stress reactivity remains within a
normal range, the reaction norm, this is favorable (gray area). High stress reactivity outside the reaction
norm is unfavorable (red area). Schematic drawing according to the following literature: Ellis and Bjor-
klund 2005; Howell and Sanchez 2011; Schweizer et al. 2016; Shakiba et al. 2020

There is an evolutionary advantage on the left or right side of the U-curve in Fig. 2.7,
because a high level of stress in children often means earlier reproduction (Belsky et al.
2015). This leads to the emergence of an individual in the next generation who, through
the aforementioned epigenetic reset (text at Fig. 2.5—general demethylation) in the ferti-
lized egg, has a new chance for a positive life plan without the epigenetic baggage of the
parents.

2.8.3 Gender Differences—an Evolutionary Medicine Perspective

With regard to, the reactions to prenatal stress episodes differ between boys/men and
girls/women. Men have a more aggressive variant of psychopathology with rule viola-
tions, and they are more often affected by attention deficit and hyperactivity or autistic
behaviors. Women, on the other hand, suffer more from anxiety disorders and depression
(Glover and Hill 2012).
Even in animal experiments, females show more anxiety reactions and depressive fea-
tures than males. Males tend to show more learning and memory disorders. The stress
reactivity increases in girls/women compared to boys/men if the birth weight—a sign of
intrauterine stress—is low. In boys/men, an increased peripheral vascular resistance is
observed, which can contribute to chronic hypertension (Glover and Hill 2012).
58 2 What is a Child’s Psychological Trauma?

Early intrauterine stress situations can lead to adaptive phenomena because, for exam-
ple, in the case of malnutrition and growth retardation, the body switches on programs
that lead to a rapid normalization of the situation after birth (Barker 2007; Hanson and
Gluckman 2015; Barker 2018). This can contribute to a very rapid weight gain up to
puberty. In this respect, these reactions can be seen as adaptive switch positions, i.e. plas-
ticity, in order to counteract the growth retardation. Mechanisms for the emergence of
plasticity have been preserved in the course of evolution (positively selected).
Likewise, prenatal growth retardation can lead to more psychopathology such as
depression or schizophrenia (Glover and Hill 2012). These reactions are similar to obe-
sity, diabetes mellitus and cardiovascular diseases a maladaptive response to an actually
adaptive regulation, which only has negative effects in a modern world of abundance and
increased stimulus offers. Just maladaptation.
From an evolutionary medicine perspective, increased aggressiveness and the increase
in rule violations are beneficial for men in competition in order to have a large number
of sexual partners and to increase reproductive success. For them, it makes sense if they
show a low stress reactivity in this game, which can contribute to a lower behavioral
inhibition (Glover and Hill 2012; Sutherland and Brunwasser 2018). In contrast, a pro-
gram of increased attention to child-rearing is switched on in women, where risk-taking
behavior is suppressed by anxiety (Glover and Hill 2012; Sutherland and Brunwasser
2018). A higher stress reactivity in women can contribute to an increased defensive reac-
tion, as found, for example, in female rhesus monkeys (Maestripieri 2011).

2.9 To the Point

• Since the World War II, Anglo-American protagonists from different disciplines
developed an understanding of early traumatic events in childhood and chronic fol-
low-up problems.
• Questionnaires were created to find adverse childhood experiences.
• Events can have an impact on the sensitive brain in different time windows. This can
lead to functions of the organs being permanently changed in different ways in later
life. The follow-up problems are discussed in the next chapter.
• Bad childhood experiences are common (adverse childhood experiences). In a
German study of students, about 25% of those surveyed had made four or more bad
childhood experiences.
• The balancing positive factors that counter childhood trauma include resilience (psy-
chological resistance), benevolent childhood experiences occurring simultaneously,
reduced sensitivity to impacts (Dandelions better than Orchids), and favorable genetic
and epigenetic factors. Epigenetic mechanisms inhibit or promote gene reading in dif-
ferent cells in various ways.
References 59

• Resilience (psychological resistance) is often present, very strongly developed, and


based on fundamental protective mechanisms that were positively selected during
evolution.
• In terms of favorable genetic factors, scientists distinguish between variations in can-
didate genes, a combination of variations of different candidate genes, or hypothe-
sis-free gene variants found in human genome-wide association studies. Favorable
factors are the mirror image of the risk factors for late consequences that are more
often investigated by scientists.
• Regarding favorable epigenetic changes, the field of science is only at the beginning.
• Some authors think that “steeling” can be favorable in childhood (stress vaccination).
However, we do not know how an examiner can implement this principle in a mean-
ingful way for which child with which methods. Ethical and legal considerations for-
tunately prohibit experiments on children using systematically imposed hardships.
• To some extent, the effects of early traumatic experiences can be passed on to the next
generation, and then the offspring generation suffers from similar consequences as the
parental generation. The transfer can take place epigenetically over 1–2 generations.
A long-term transfer over many generations is unlikely in humans because of a typi-
cal deletion of epigenetic markings (demethylation of the entire genome = reset).
• The theory of evolution arrived earlier in the field of psychology than in medicine.
During evolution, mechanisms were preserved to allow for switch positions in brain
regulatory systems during early development in utero, around the time of birth, dur-
ing early childhood up to puberty, that allow for adaptation to later life situations.
This environment-changeable plasticity has been positively selected. Dandelions and
orchids have their niches.

References

Afifi TO, Mather A, Boman J, Fleisher W, Enns MW, Macmillan H, Sareen J (2011) Childhood
adversity and personality disorders: results from a nationally representative population-based
study. J Psychiatr Res 45:814–822
Allfrey VG, Faulkner R, Mirsky AE (1964) Acetylation and methylation of histones and their pos-
sible role in regulation of RNA synthesis. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 51:786–794
Anand KJS, Rigdon J, Rovnaghi CR et al (2019) Measuring socioeconomic adversity in early life.
Acta Paediatr 108:1267–1277
Andersen SL (2015) Exposure to early adversity: points of cross-species translation that can lead
to improved understanding of depression. Dev Psychopathol 27:477–491
Anderson S, Leventhal T (2017) Residential mobility and adolescent achievement and behavior:
understanding timing and extent of mobility. J Res Adolesc 27:328–343
Atzl VM, Grande LA, Davis EP, Narayan AJ (2019) Perinatal promotive and protective factors for
women with histories of childhood abuse and neglect. Child Abuse Negl 91:63–77
AWMF Portal der wissenschaftlichen Medizin (2019) Kindesmisshandlung, – missbrauch, -ver-
nachlässigung unter Einbindung der Jugendhilfe und Pädagogik (Kinderschutzleitlinie). https://
www.awmf.org/leitlinien/detail/ll/027-069.html. Accessed 21 Oct 2020
60 2 What is a Child’s Psychological Trauma?

Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van Ijzendoorn MH (2015) The hidden efficacy of interventions:


gene×environment experiments from a differential susceptibility perspective. Annu Rev
Psychol 66:381–409
Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van Ijzendoorn MH (2007) Research review: genetic vulnerabil-
ity or differential susceptibility in child development: the case of attachment. J Child Psychol
Psychiatry 48:1160–1173
Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van Ijzendoorn MH (2011) Differential susceptibility to rearing
environment depending on dopamine-related genes: new evidence and a meta-analysis. Dev
Psychopathol 23:39–52
Barker DJ (1998) In utero programming of chronic disease. Clin Sci 95:115–128
Barker DJ (2007) The origins of the developmental origins theory. J Intern Med 261:412–417
Barker DJ, Osmond C (1986) Infant mortality, childhood nutrition, and ischaemic heart disease in
England and Wales. Lancet 1:1077–1081
Barker DJ, Osmond C, Law CM (1989a) The intrauterine and early postnatal origins of cardiovas-
cular disease and chronic bronchitis. J Epidemiol Community Health 43:237–240
Barker DJ, Winter PD, Osmond C, Margetts B, Simmonds SJ (1989b) Weight in infancy and death
from ischaemic heart disease. Lancet 2:577–580
Barker DJ, Hales CN, Fall CH, Osmond C, Phipps K, Clark PM (1993) Type 2 (non-insulin-de-
pendent) diabetes mellitus, hypertension and hyperlipidaemia (syndrome X): relation to
reduced fetal growth. Diabetologia 36:62–67
Barker ED (2018) Epigenetics, early adversity and child and adolescent mental health.
Psychopathology 51:71–75
Becker M, Schulz A (2013) Epidemiologie von Kindesmissbrauch. In: Spitzer C, Grabe HJ (Hrsg)
Kindesmisshandlung – Psychische und körperliche Folgen im Erwachsenenalter. Suttgart, W.
Kohlhammer, S 13–21
Belsky J (1997) Variation in susceptibility to environmental influence: an evolutionary argument.
Psychol Inq 8:182–186
Belsky J, Beaver KM (2011) Cumulative-genetic plasticity, parenting and adolescent self-regula-
tion. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 52:619–626
Belsky J, Pluess M (2009) Beyond diathesis stress: differential susceptibility to environmental
influences. Psychol Bull 135:885–908
Belsky J, Steinberg L, Draper P (1991) Childhood experience, interpersonal development, and
reproductive strategy: and evolutionary theory of socialization. Child Dev 62:647–670
Belsky J, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van Ijzendoorn MH (2007) For better and for worse: differ-
ential susceptibility to environmental influences. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 16:300–304
Belsky J, Steinberg L, Houts RM, Halpern-Felsher BL, NICHD Early Child Care Research
Network (2010) The development of reproductive strategy in females: early maternal harshness
--> earlier menarche --> increased sexual risk taking. Dev Psychol 46:120–128
Belsky J, Ruttle PL, Boyce WT, Armstrong JM, Essex MJ (2015) Early adversity, elevated
stress physiology, accelerated sexual maturation, and poor health in females. Dev Psychol
51:816–822
Berens AE, Nelson CA (2015) The science of early adversity: is there a role for large institutions
in the care of vulnerable children? Lancet 386:388–398
Bethell C, Jones J, Gombojav N, Linkenbach J, Sege R (2019) Positive childhood experiences and
adult mental and relational health in a statewide sample: associations across adverse childhood
experiences levels. JAMA Pediatr 173:e193007
Bibikova M, Lin Z, Zhou L et al (2006) High-throughput DNA methylation profiling using univer-
sal bead arrays. Genome Res 16:383–393
References 61

Bibikova M, Barnes B, Tsan C et al (2011) High density DNA methylation array with single CpG
site resolution. Genomics 98:288–295
Bifulco A, Moran PM, Ball C, Jacobs C, Baines R, Bunn A, Cavagin J (2002) Childhood adversity,
parental vulnerability and disorder: examining inter-generational transmission of risk. J Child
Psychol Psychiatry 43:1075–1086
Black MM, Walker SP, Fernald LCH et al (2017) Early childhood development coming of age: sci-
ence through the life course. Lancet 389:77–90
Bouvette-Turcot AA, Fleming AS, Unternaehrer E, Gonzalez A, Atkinson L, Gaudreau H, Steiner
M, Meaney MJ (2020) Maternal symptoms of depression and sensitivity mediate the relation
between maternal history of early adversity and her child temperament: the inheritance of cir-
cumstance. Dev Psychopathol 32:605–613
Bowers K, Ding L, Gregory S, Yolton K, Ji H, Meyer J, Ammerman RT, Van Ginkel J, Folger A
(2018) Maternal distress and hair cortisol in pregnancy among women with elevated adverse
childhood experiences. Psychoneuroendocrinology 95:145–148
Bowlby J (1946) Forty-four juvenile thieves: their character and home-life. Bailliere, Tindall &
Cox, London
Bowlby J (1966) Maternal care and mental health. Schocken Books, New York
Boyce WT (2016) Differential susceptibility of the developing brain to contextual adversity and
stress. Neuropsychopharmacology 41:142–162
Boyce WT (2019) Orchidee oder Löwenzahn? – Warum Menschen so unterschiedlich sind und wie
sich alle gut entwickeln können. Droemer Knaur, München
Boyce WT, Jensen EW, Cassel JC, Collier AM, Smith AH, Ramey CT (1977) Influence of life
events and family routines on childhood respiratory tract illness. Pediatrics 60:609–615
Breton C (2013) The hypothalamus-adipose axis is a key target of developmental programming by
maternal nutritional manipulation. J Endocrinol 216:R19–R31
Bundeskriminalamt (2019) PKS Jahrbuch. https://www.bka.de/DE/AktuelleInformationen/
StatistikenLagebilder/PolizeilicheKriminalstatistik/PKS2019/PKSJahrbuch/pksJahrbuch_node.
html. Accessed 03 Nov 2020
Bunge M (1979) The mind-body problem in an evolutionary perspective. CIBA Found Symp
69:53–77
Burgermeister D (2007) Childhood adversity: a review of measurement instruments. J Nurs Meas
15:163–176
Buss DM, Penke L (2015) Evolutionary personality psychology. In: Mikulincer M, Shaver
PR, Cooper ML, Larsen RJ (Hrsg) APA handbook of personality and social psychology, Bd
4. Personality processes and individual differences. American Psychological Association,
Washington, DC, S 3–29
Bygren LO, Tinghög P, Carstensen J, Edvinsson S, Kaati G, Pembrey ME, Sjöström M (2014)
Change in paternal grandmothers‘ early food supply influenced cardiovascular mortality of the
female grandchildren. BMC Genet 15:12
Cao-Lei L, de Rooij SR, King S, Matthews SG, Metz GAS, Roseboom TJ, Szyf M (2017) Prenatal
stress and epigenetics. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 18:30726
Caspi A, Sugden K, Moffitt TE et al (2003) Influence of life stress on depression: moderation by a
polymorphism in the 5-HTT gene. Science 301:386–389
Centers for disease control and prevention (2018) Family health history. von https://www.cdc.gov/
violenceprevention/index.html. Accessed 21 Oct 2020
Cheng L, Tan M, Liu Z (2015) Adverse environments and children‘s creativity development:
transforming the notion of „success in adversity“ in China. New Dir Child Adolesc Dev
2015:93–100
62 2 What is a Child’s Psychological Trauma?

Chung EK, Mathew L, Elo IT, Coyne JC, Culhane JF (2008) Depressive symptoms in disadvan-
taged women receiving prenatal care: the influence of adverse and positive childhood experi-
ences. Ambul Pediatr 8:109–116
Cicchetti D, Hetzel S, Rogosch FA, Handley ED, Toth SL (2016) Genome-wide DNA meth-
ylation in 1-year-old infants of mothers with major depressive disorder. Dev Psychopathol
28:1413–1419
Cohen RA, Hitsman BL, Paul RH et al (2006) Early life stress and adult emotional experience: an
international perspective. Int J Psychiatry Med 36:35–52
Colman I, Kingsbury M, Garad Y, Zeng Y, Naicker K, Patten S, Jones PB, Wild TC, Thompson
AH (2016) Consistency in adult reporting of adverse childhood experiences. Psychol Med
46:543–549
Colodro-Conde L, Couvy-Duchesne B, Zhu G et al (2018) A direct test of the diathesis-stress
model for depression. Mol Psychiatry 23:1590–1596
Cowan CS, Callaghan BL, Kan JM, Richardson R (2016) The lasting impact of early-life adversity
on individuals and their descendants: potential mechanisms and hope for intervention. Genes
Brain Behav 15:155–168
Crandall A, Miller JR, Cheung A et al (2019) ACEs and counter-ACEs: how positive and negative
childhood experiences influence adult health. Child Abuse Negl 96:104089
Cronholm PF, Forke CM, Wade R, Bair-Merritt MH, Davis M, Harkins-Schwarz M, Pachter LM,
Fein JA (2015) Adverse childhood experiences: expanding the concept of adversity. Am J Prev
Med 49:354–361
Crouch E, Probst JC, Radcliff E, Bennett KJ, McKinney SH (2019) Prevalence of adverse child-
hood experiences (ACEs) among US children. Child Abuse Negl 92:209–218
Cubas P, Vincent C, Coen E (1999) An epigenetic mutation responsible for natural variation in flo-
ral symmetry. Nature 401:157–161
Damian RI, Simonton DK (2015) Psychopathology, adversity, and creativity: diversifying experi-
ences in the development of eminent African Americans. J Pers Soc Psychol 108:623–636
Darwin C (1872) The expression of the emotions in animals. John Murray, London
Davies PT, Cicchetti D, Thompson MJ, Bascoe SM, Cummings EM (2020) The interplay of poly-
genic plasticity and adrenocortical activity as sources of variability in pathways among fam-
ily adversity, youth emotional reactivity, and psychological problems. Dev Psychopathol
32:587–603
Del Giudice M, Ellis BJ, Shirtcliff EA (2011) The adaptive calibration model of stress responsiv-
ity. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 35:1562–1592
Dennis CH, Clohessy DS, Stone AL, Darnall BD, Wilson AC (2019) Adverse childhood expe-
riences in mothers with chronic pain and intergenerational impact on children. J Pain
20:1209–1217
deVries MW (1984) Temperament and infant mortality among the Masai of East Africa. Am J
Psychiatry 141:1189–1194
Draper P, Belsky J (1990) Personality development in the evolutionary perspective. J Pers
58:141–161
Dunn J, Plomin R (1990) Separate lives: why siblings are so different. Basic Books, New York
Dwyer JB, Aftab A, Radhakrishnan R, Widge A, Rodriguez CI, Carpenter LL, Nemeroff CB,
McDonald WM, Kalin NH (2020) Hormonal treatments for major depressive disorder: state of
the art. Am J Psychiatry 177:686–705
Edge MD, Ramel W, Drabant EM, Kuo JR, Parker KJ, Gross JJ (2009) For better or worse? Stress
inoculation effects for implicit but not explicit anxiety. Depress Anxiety 26:831–837
Ellis BJ, Bjorklund DF (2005) Origins of the social mind: evolutionary psychology and child
development. The Guilford Press, New York
References 63

Ellis WR, Dietz WH (2017) A new framework for addressing adverse childhood and community
experiences: the building community resilience model. Acad Pediatr 17:S86–S93
Eriksson M, Räikkönen K, Eriksson JG (2014) Early life stress and later health outcomes – find-
ings from the Helsinki Birth Cohort Study. Am J Hum Biol 26:111–116
Fear NT, Reed RV, Rowe S et al (2018) Impact of paternal deployment to the conflicts in Iraq and
Afghanistan and paternal post-traumatic stress disorder on the children of military fathers. Br J
Psychiatry 212:347–355
Felitti VJ (2019) Origins of the ACE study. Am J Prev Med 56:787–789
Felitti VJ, Anda RF, Nordenberg D, Williamson DF, Spitz AM, Edwards V, Koss MP, Marks JS
(1998) Relationship of childhood abuse and household dysfunction to many of the leading
causes of death in adults. The Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACE) Study. Am J Prev Med
14:245–258
Finkelhor D, Turner HA, Shattuck A, Hamby SL (2015) Prevalence of childhood exposure to vio-
lence, crime, and abuse: results from the national survey of children‘s exposure to violence.
JAMA Pediatr 169:746–754
Fleming TP, Watkins AJ, Velazquez MA et al (2018) Origins of lifetime health around the time of
conception: causes and consequences. Lancet 391:1842–1852
Frasch MG, Lobmaier SM, Stampalija T et al (2018) Non-invasive biomarkers of fetal brain devel-
opment reflecting prenatal stress: an integrative multi-scale multi-species perspective on data
collection and analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 117:165–183
Gabory A, Attig L, Junien C (2011) Developmental programming and epigenetics. Am J Clin Nutr
94:1943S–1952S
Galler J, Rabinowitz DG (2014) The intergenerational effects of early adversity. Prog Mol Biol
Transl Sci 128:177–198
Geschwind N, Peeters F, Jacobs N, Delespaul P, Derom C, Thiery E, van Os J, Wichers M (2010)
Meeting risk with resilience: high daily life reward experience preserves mental health. Acta
Psychiatr Scand 122:129–138
Gest SD, Reed MG, Masten AS (1999) Measuring developmental changes in exposure to adver-
sity: a Life Chart and rating scale approach. Dev Psychopathol 11:171–192
Ghiselin MT (1973) Darwin and evolutionary psychology: Darwin initiated a radically new way of
studying behavior. Science 179:964–968
Glover V (2011) Annual research review: prenatal stress and the origins of psychopathology: an
evolutionary perspective. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 52:356–367
Glover V, Hill J (2012) Sex differences in the programming effects of prenatal stress on psychopa-
thology and stress responses: an evolutionary perspective. Physiol Behav 106:736–740
Graignic-Philippe R, Dayan J, Chokron S, Jacquet AY, Tordjman S (2014) Effects of prena-
tal stress on fetal and child development: a critical literature review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev
43:137–162
Haggbloom SJ, Warnick R, Warnick JE et al (2002) The 100 most eminent psychologists of the
20th century. Rev Gen Psychol 6:139–152
Hambrick EP, Brawner TW, Perry BD (2019) Timing of early-life stress and the development of
brain-related capacities. Front Behav Neurosci 13:183
Hanson MA, Gluckman PD (2014) Early developmental conditioning of later health and disease:
physiology or pathophysiology? Physiol Rev 94:1027–1076
Hanson MA, Gluckman PD (2015) Developmental origins of health and disease – global public
health implications. Best Pract Res Clin Obstet Gynaecol 29:24–31
Heard E, Martienssen RA (2014) Transgenerational epigenetic inheritance: myths and mecha-
nisms. Cell 157:95–109
64 2 What is a Child’s Psychological Trauma?

Hillis SD, Anda RF, Dube SR, Felitti VJ, Marchbanks PA, Macaluso M, Marks JS (2010) The pro-
tective effect of family strengths in childhood against adolescent pregnancy and its long-term
psychosocial consequences. Perm J 14:18–27
Hipwell AE, Tung I, Northrup J, Keenan K (2019) Transgenerational associations between mater-
nal childhood stress exposure and profiles of infant emotional reactivity. Dev Psychopathol
31:887–898
Höltge J, Mc Gee SL, Thoma MV (2019) The curvilinear relationship of early-life adversity and
successful aging: the mediating role of mental health. Aging Ment Health 23:608–617
Hostinar CE, Gunnar MR (2013) The developmental effects of early life stress: an overview of
current theoretical frameworks. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 22:400–406
Howell BR, Sanchez MM (2011) Understanding behavioral effects of early life stress using the
reactive scope and allostatic load models. Dev Psychopathol 23:1001–1016
Huang M, Starr LR (2020) Interpersonal childhood adversity and stress generation in adolescence:
moderation by HPA axis multilocus genetic variation. Dev Psychopathol 32:865–878
Inoue Y, Stickley A, Yazawa A, Aida J, Kawachi I, Kondo K, Fujiwara T (2019) Adverse childhood
experiences, exposure to a natural disaster and posttraumatic stress disorder among survivors of
the 2011 Great East Japan earthquake and tsunami. Epidemiol Psychiatr Sci 28:45–53
Ivy AS, Brunson KL, Sandman C, Baram TZ (2008) Dysfunctional nurturing behavior in rat
dams with limited access to nesting material: a clinically relevant model for early-life stress.
Neuroscience 154:1132–1142
Jackson DB, Chilton M, Johnson KR, Vaughn MG (2019) Adverse childhood experiences and
household food insecurity: findings from the 2016 national survey of children‘s health. Am J
Prev Med 57:667–674
Jensen Peña C, Monk C, Champagne FA (2012) Epigenetic effects of prenatal stress on 11β-hy-
droxysteroid dehydrogenase-2 in the placenta and fetal brain. PLoS One 7:e39791
Jivraj S, Goodman A, Ploubidis GB, de Oliveira C (2020) Testing comparability between retro-
spective life history data and prospective birth cohort study data. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc
Sci 75:207–217
Joseph S, Linley PA (2006) Growth following adversity: theoretical perspectives and implications
for clinical practice. Clin Psychol Rev 26:1041–1053
Kaas JH (1989) The evolution of complex sensory systems in mammals. J Exp Biol 146:165–176
Kane JB, Harris KM, Siega-Riz AM (2018) Intergenerational pathways linking maternal early life
adversity to offspring birthweight. Soc Sci Med 207:89–96
Kant I (1833) Anthropologie in pragmatischer Hinsicht: Drittes Buch vom Begehrungsvermögen.
Immanuel Müller, Leipzig
Keers R, Coleman JR, Lester KJ et al (2016) A genome-wide test of the differential susceptibility
hypothesis reveals a genetic predictor of differential response to psychological treatments for
child anxiety disorders. Psychother Psychosom 85:146–158
Klengel T, Mehta D, Anacker C et al (2013) Allele-specific FKBP5 DNA demethylation mediates
gene-childhood trauma interactions. Nat Neurosci 16:33–41
Kniffin CL, McKusick VA (2018) Brunner syndrome. https://omim.org/entry/300615. Accessed 28
Oct 2020
Knowles JP, Evans NJ, Burke D (2019) Some evidence for an association between early life adver-
sity and decision urgency. Front Psychol 10:243
Kolvin I (1999) The contribution of Michael Rutter. Br J Psychiatry 174:471–475
Labella MH, Narayan AJ, McCormick CM, Desjardins CD, Masten AS (2019) Risk and adversity,
parenting quality, and children‘s social-emotional adjustment in families experiencing home-
lessness. Child Dev 90:227–244
References 65

Lemery-Chalfant K, Clifford S, Dishion TJ, Shaw DS, Wilson MN (2018) Genetic moderation of
the effects of the Family Check-Up intervention on children‘s internalizing symptoms: a longi-
tudinal study with a racially/ethnically diverse sample. Dev Psychopathol 30:1729–1747
Li J, Olsen J, Vestergaard M, Obel C, Baker JL, Sørensen TI (2010) Prenatal stress exposure
related to maternal bereavement and risk of childhood overweight. PLoS One 5:e11896
Liu J, Raine A, Venables PH, Dalais C, Mednick SA (2003) Malnutrition at age 3 years and lower
cognitive ability at age 11 years: independence from psychosocial adversity. Arch Pediatr
Adolesc Med 157:593–600
Lomanowska AM, Boivin M, Hertzman C, Fleming AS (2017) Parenting begets parenting: a neu-
robiological perspective on early adversity and the transmission of parenting styles across gen-
erations. Neuroscience 342:120–139
Luby JL, Baram TZ, Rogers CE, Barch DM (2020) Neurodevelopmental optimization after ear-
ly-life adversity: cross-species studies to elucidate sensitive periods and brain mechanisms to
inform early intervention. Trends Neurosci 27:30175–30172
Lutz PE, Tanti A, Gasecka A et al (2017) Association of a history of child abuse with impaired
myelination in the anterior cingulate cortex: convergent epigenetic, transcriptional, and mor-
phological evidence. Am J Psychiatry 174:1185–1194
Maestripieri D (2011) Emotions, stress, and maternal motivation in primates. Am J Primatol
73:516–529
Manuck SB, McCaffery JM (2014) Gene-environment interaction. Annu Rev Psychol 65:41–70
Massart R, Nemoda Z, Suderman MJ, Sutti S, Ruggiero AM, Dettmer AM, Suomi SJ, Szyf M
(2016) Early life adversity alters normal sex-dependent developmental dynamics of DNA meth-
ylation. Dev Psychopathol 28:1259–1272
Masten AS (2014) Ordinary magic – resilience in development. The Guilford Press, New York/
London
Masten AS, Garmezy N, Tellegen A, Pellegrini DS, Larkin K, Larsen A (1988) Competence and
stress in school children: the moderating effects of individual and family qualities. J Child
Psychol Psychiatry 29:745–764
Matsumoto M, Yoshioka M, Togashi H (2009) Early postnatal stress and neural circuit underlying
emotional regulation. Int Rev Neurobiol 85:95–107
McClintock B (1961) Some parallels between gene control systems in maize and in bacteria. Am
Nat 95:265–277
McGowan PO, Matthews SG (2018) Prenatal stress, glucocorticoids, and developmental program-
ming of the stress response. Endocrinology 159:69–82
McLaughlin KA, Hatzenbuehler ML, Xuan Z, Conron KJ (2012) Disproportionate exposure to
early-life adversity and sexual orientation disparities in psychiatric morbidity. Child Abuse
Negl 36:645–655
McQuaid RJ, McInnis OA, Stead JD, Matheson K, Anisman H (2013) A paradoxical association of
an oxytocin receptor gene polymorphism: early-life adversity and vulnerability to depression.
Front Neurosci 7:128
Merrick MT, Ford DC, Ports KA, Guinn AS (2018) Prevalence of adverse childhood experiences
from the 2011–2014 behavioral risk factor surveillance system in 23 states. JAMA Pediatr
172:1038–1044
Mersky JP, Janczewski CE, Topitzes J (2017) Rethinking the measurement of adversity. Child
Maltreat 22:58–68
Miranda J, Legha R (2019) The consequences of family separation at the border and beyond. J Am
Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 58:139–140
66 2 What is a Child’s Psychological Trauma?

Mittal C, Griskevicius V, Simpson JA, Sung S, Young ES (2015) Cognitive adaptations to stressful
environments: when childhood adversity enhances adult executive function. J Pers Soc Psychol
109:604–621
Moises HW, Yang L, Kristbjarnarson H et al (1995) An international two-stage genome-wide
search for schizophrenia susceptibility genes. Nat Genet 11:321–324
Montez JK, Hayward MD (2014) Cumulative childhood adversity, educational attainment, and
active life expectancy among U.S. adults. Demography 51:413–435
Mueller BR, Bale TL (2006) Impact of prenatal stress on long term body weight is dependent on
timing and maternal sensitivity. Physiol Behav 88:605–614
Murray L, Fiori-Cowley A, Hooper R, Cooper P (1996) The impact of postnatal depression and
associated adversity on early mother-infant interactions and later infant outcome. Child Dev
67:2512–2526
Najman JM, Bor W, Ahmadabadi Z, Williams GM, Alati R, Mamun AA, Scott JG, Clavarino AM
(2018) The inter- and intra- generational transmission of family poverty and hardship (adver-
sity): a prospective 30 year study. PLoS One 13:e0190504
Narayan AJ, Rivera LM, Bernstein RE, Harris WW, Lieberman AF (2018) Positive childhood
experiences predict less psychopathology and stress in pregnant women with childhood adver-
sity: a pilot study of the benevolent childhood experiences (BCEs) scale. Child Abuse Negl
78:19–30
Naumova OY, Lee M, Koposov R, Szyf M, Dozier M, Grigorenko EL (2012) Differential patterns
of whole-genome DNA methylation in institutionalized children and children raised by their
biological parents. Dev Psychopathol 24:143–155
Nederhof E, Schmidt MV (2012) Mismatch or cumulative stress: toward an integrated hypothesis
of programming effects. Physiol Behav 106:691–700
Neiderhiser JM, Reiss D, Hetherington EM, Plomin R (1999) Relationships between parenting
and adolescent adjustment over time: genetic and environmental contributions. Dev Psychol
35:680–692
Nelson CA 3rd, Zeanah CH, Fox NA (2019) How early experience shapes human development:
the case of psychosocial deprivation. Neural Plast 2019:1676285
Nelson CA (2013) Biological embedding of early life adversity. JAMA Pediatr 167:1098–1100
Nesse RM (1990) The evolutionary functions of repression and the ego defenses. J Am Acad
Psychoanal 18:260–285
Nishi M, Horii-Hayashi N, Sasagawa T, Matsunaga W (2013) Effects of early life stress on brain
activity: implications from maternal separation model in rodents. Gen Comp Endocrinol
181:306–309
Oh DL, Jerman P, Purewal Boparai SK, Koita K, Briner S, Bucci M, Harris NB (2018) Review of
tools for measuring exposure to adversity in children and adolescents. J Pediatr Health Care
32:564–583
Park C, Rosenblat JD, Brietzke E, Pan Z, Lee Y, Cao B, Zuckerman H, Kalantarova A, McIntyre
RS (2019) Stress, epigenetics and depression: a systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev
102:139–152
Patten SB, Wilkes TC, Williams JV, Lavorato DH, El-Guebaly N, Schopflocher D, Wild C, Colman
I, Bulloch AG (2015) Retrospective and prospectively assessed childhood adversity in associa-
tion with major depression, alcohol consumption and painful conditions. Epidemiol Psychiatr
Sci 24:158–165
Patterson ML, Moniruzzaman A, Somers JM (2014) Setting the stage for chronic health prob-
lems: cumulative childhood adversity among homeless adults with mental illness in Vancouver,
British Columbia. BMC Public Health 14:350
References 67

Peyrot WJ, Milaneschi Y, Abdellaoui A, Sullivan PF, Hottenga JJ, Boomsma DI, Penninx BW
(2014) Effect of polygenic risk scores on depression in childhood trauma. Br J Psychiatry
205:113–119
Plomin R (2018) Blueprint. The MIT Press, Cambridge, MA/London
Plomin R, Colledge E (2001) Genetics and psychology: beyond heritability. Eur Psychol
6:229–240
Poulton R, Caspi A, Milne BJ, Thomson WM, Taylor A, Sears MR, Moffitt TE (2002) Association
between children‘s experience of socioeconomic disadvantage and adult health: a life-course
study. Lancet 360:1640–1645
Powell KM, Rahm-Knigge RL, Conner BT (2020) Resilience protective factors checklist
(RPFC): buffering childhood adversity and promoting positive outcomes. Psychol Rep
124:11437–11461
Pratchett LC, Yehuda R (2011) Foundations of posttraumatic stress disorder: does early life trauma
lead to adult posttraumatic stress disorder? Dev Psychopathol 23:477–491
Raposa EB, Hammen CL, Brennan PA (2015) Close friends’ psychopathology as a pathway
from early adversity to young adulthood depressive symptoms. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol
44:742–750
Reid JA, Baglivio MT, Piquero AR, Greenwald MA, Epps N (2017) Human trafficking of minors
and childhood adversity in Florida. Am J Public Health 107:306–311
Reuben A, Moffitt TE, Caspi A et al (2016) Lest we forget: comparing retrospective and prospec-
tive assessments of adverse childhood experiences in the prediction of adult health. J Child
Psychol Psychiatry 57:1103–1112
Rinaudo P, Wang E (2012) Fetal programming and metabolic syndrome. Annu Rev Physiol
74:107–130
Robertson J, Bowlby J (1952) Responses of young children to separation from their mothers II:
observations of the sequences of response of children aged 18 to 24 months during the course
of separation. Courrier du Centre International de l’Enfance 2:131–142
Rosenman S, Rodgers B (2004) Childhood adversity in an Australian population. Soc Psychiatry
Psychiatr Epidemiol 39:695–702
Rothenberger SE, Moehler E, Reck C, Resch F (2011) Prenatal stress: course and interrelation of
emotional and physiological stress measures. Psychopathology 44:60–67
Rutter ML (1999) Psychosocial adversity and child psychopathology. Br J Psychiatry 174:480–493
Sarkar M, Fletcher D (2017) Adversity-related experiences are essential for Olympic success:
additional evidence and considerations. Prog Brain Res 232:159–165
Sarkar M, Fletcher D, Brown DJ (2015) What doesn‘t kill me…: adversity-related experiences are
vital in the development of superior Olympic performance. J Sci Med Sport 18:475–479
Schalinski I, Breinlinger S, Hirt V, Teicher MH, Odenwald M, Rockstroh B (2019) Environmental
adversities and psychotic symptoms: the impact of timing of trauma, abuse, and neglect.
Schizophr Res 205:4–9
Schoon I, Melis G (2019) Intergenerational transmission of family adversity: examining constella-
tions of risk factors. PLoS One 14:e0214801
Schopenhauer A (1819) Die Welt als Wille und Vorstellung—Vorrede zur ersten Auflage. F. A.
Brockhaus, Leipzig
Schraut KG, Jakob SB, Weidner MT et al (2014) Prenatal stress-induced programming of genome-
wide promoter DNA methylation in 5-HTT-deficient mice. Transl Psychiatry 4:e473
Schweizer S, Walsh ND, Stretton J, Dunn VJ, Goodyer IM, Dalgleish T (2016) Enhanced emotion
regulation capacity and its neural substrates in those exposed to moderate childhood adversity.
Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 11:272–281
68 2 What is a Child’s Psychological Trauma?

Seery MD, Holman EA, Silver RC (2010a) Whatever does not kill us: cumulative lifetime adver-
sity, vulnerability, and resilience. J Pers Soc Psychol 99:1025–1041
Seery MD, Leo RJ, Holman EA, Silver RC (2010b) Lifetime exposure to adversity predicts func-
tional impairment and healthcare utilization among individuals with chronic back pain. Pain
150:507–515
Shachar-Dadon A, Schulkin J, Leshem M (2009) Adversity before conception will affect adult
progeny in rats. Dev Psychol 45:9–16
Shakiba N, Ellis BJ, Bush NR, Boyce WT (2020) Biological sensitivity to context: a test of
the hypothesized U-shaped relation between early adversity and stress responsivity. Dev
Psychopathol 32:641–660
Sharpley CF, Palanisamy SK, Glyde NS, Dillingham PW, Agnew LL (2014) An update on the
interaction between the serotonin transporter promoter variant (5-HTTLPR), stress and depres-
sion, plus an exploration of non-confirming findings. Behav Brain Res 273:89–105
Sheikh MA, Abelsen B, Olsen JA (2016) Clarifying associations between childhood adversity,
social support, behavioral factors, and mental health, health, and well-being in adulthood: a
population-based study. Front Psychol 7:727
Shiner RL, Masten AS (2012) Childhood personality as a harbinger of competence and resilience
in adulthood. Dev Psychopathol 24:507–528
Shrira A, Palgi Y, Ben-Ezra M, Shmotkin D (2010) Do Holocaust survivors show increased vulner-
ability or resilience to post-Holocaust cumulative adversity? J Trauma Stress 23:367–375
Simonton DK (2016) Reverse engineering genius: historiometric studies of superlative talent. Ann
N Y Acad Sci 1377:3–9
Slopen N, Kubzansky LD, McLaughlin KA, Koenen KC (2013) Childhood adversity and inflam-
matory processes in youth: a prospective study. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38:188–200
Slopen N, Chen Y, Guida JL, Albert MA, Williams DR (2017) Positive childhood experiences and
ideal cardiovascular health in midlife: associations and mediators. Prev Med 97:72–79
Smith KE, Pollak SD (2021) Rethinking concepts and categories for understanding the neurode-
velopmental effects of childhood adversity. Perspect Psychol Sci 16:67–93
Snyder PJ, Kaufman R, Harrison J, Maruff P (2010) Charles Darwin‘s emotional expression
„experiment“ and his contribution to modern neuropharmacology. J Hist Neurosci 19:158–170
Spitzer C, Grabe HJ (2013) Kindesmisshandlung. W. Kohlhammer, Stuttgart
Standing LG, Aikins S, Madigan B, Nohl W (2015) Exceptional achievement and early parental
loss: the phaeton effect in American writers, presidents, and eminent individuals. J Psychohist
42:188–199
Starr LR, Stroud CB, Shaw ZA, Vrshek-Schallhorn S (2020) Stress sensitization to depression
following childhood adversity: moderation by HPA axis and serotonergic multilocus profile
scores. Dev Psychopathol 20:1–15
Stickley A, Koyanagi A, Inoue Y, Leinsalu M (2018) Childhood hunger and thoughts of death or
suicide in older adults. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 26:1070–1078
Strata F, Giritharan G, Sebastiano FD, Piane LD, Kao CN, Donjacour A, Rinaudo P (2015)
Behavior and brain gene expression changes in mice exposed to preimplantation and prenatal
stress. Reprod Sci 22:23–30
Straub IR, Janer A, Weraarpachai W, Zinman L, Robertson J, Rogaeva E, Shoubridge EA (2018)
Loss of CHCHD10-CHCHD2 complexes required for respiration underlies the pathogenicity of
a CHCHD10 mutation in ALS. Hum Mol Genet 27:178–189
Straub IR, Weraarpachai W, Shoubridge EA (2021) Multi-OMICS study of a CHCHD10 variant
causing ALS demonstrates metabolic rewiring and activation of endoplasmic reticulum and
mitochondrial unfolded protein responses. Hum Mol Genet 30:687–705
References 69

Straub RH (2018) Altern, Müdigkeit und Entzündungen verstehen – Wenn Immunsystem und
Gehirn um die Energie im Körper ringen. Springer, Berlin/Heidelberg
Straub RH (2020) Drei Gedächtnisse für den Körper. Springer Nature, Berlin
Strauss J, Barr CL, George CJ, King N, Shaikh S, Devlin B, Kovacs M, Kennedy JL (2004)
Association study of brain-derived neurotrophic factor in adults with a history of childhood
onset mood disorder. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 131B:16–19
Strøm IF, Thoresen S, Wentzel-Larsen T, Hjemdal OK, Lien L, Dyb G (2013) Exposure to life
adversity in high school and later work participation: a longitudinal population-based study. J
Adolesc 36:1143–1151
Stroud LR, McCallum M, Salisbury AL (2018) Impact of maternal prenatal smoking on fetal to
infant neurobehavioral development. Dev Psychopathol 30:1087–1105
Sutherland S, Brunwasser SM (2018) Sex differences in vulnerability to prenatal stress: a review
of the recent literature. Curr Psychiatry Rep 20:102
Szepsenwol O, Simpson JA (2019) Attachment within life history theory: an evolutionary perspec-
tive on individual differences in attachment. Curr Opin Psychol 25:65–70
Szyf M (2019) The epigenetics of perinatal stress. Dialogues Clin Neurosci 21:369–378
Teicher MH, Parigger A (2015) The ‚Maltreatment and Abuse Chronology of Exposure‘ (MACE)
scale for the retrospective assessment of abuse and neglect during development. PLoS One
10:e0117423
Thapar A, Pine DS, Leckman JF, Scott S, Snowling MJ, Taylor E (2015) Rutter‘s child and adoles-
cent psychiatry, 6th Edition, Wiley, Chichester/West Sussex
Thompson TM, Sharfi D, Lee M, Yrigollen CM, Naumova OY, Grigorenko EL (2013) Comparison
of whole-genome DNA methylation patterns in whole blood, saliva, and lymphoblastoid cell
lines. Behav Genet 43:168–176
Thomson P, Jaque SV (2018) Childhood adversity and the creative experience in adult professional
performing artists. Front Psychol 9:111
Tomfohrde J, Silverman A, Barnes R et al (1994) Gene for familial psoriasis susceptibility mapped
to the distal end of human chromosome 17q. Science 264:1141–1145
Tonmyr L, Draca J, Crain J, Macmillan HL (2011) Measurement of emotional/psychological child
maltreatment: a review. Child Abuse Negl 35:767–782
Turner HA, Finkelhor D, Hamby S, Henly M (2017) Victimization and adversity among children
experiencing war-related parental absence or deployment in a nationally representative US
sample. Child Abuse Negl 67:271–279
U.S. Department of Energy – Office of Science and Office of Biological and Environmental
Research (2019) Human genome project information archive 1990–2003. https://web.ornl.gov/
sci/techresources/Human_Genome/index.shtml. Accessed 24 Oct 2020
van Dijken S (1998) John Bowlby: his early life: a biographical journey into the roots of attach-
ment theory. Free Association Books, London/New York
Veenendaal MV, Painter RC, de Rooij SR, Bossuyt PM, van der Post JA, Gluckman PD, Hanson
MA, Roseboom TJ (2013) Transgenerational effects of prenatal exposure to the 1944–45 Dutch
famine. BJOG 120:548–553
Ward IL (1972) Prenatal stress feminizes and demasculinizes the behavior of males. Science
175:82–84
Werker JF, Hensch TK (2015) Critical periods in speech perception: new directions. Annu Rev
Psychol 66:173–196
Westen D (1998) The scientific legacy of Sigmund Freud: toward a psychodynamically informed
psychological science. Psychol Bull 124:333–371
70 2 What is a Child’s Psychological Trauma?

Wickrama KK, O‘Neal CW, Lee TK, Wickrama T (2015) Early socioeconomic adversity, youth
positive development, and young adults’ cardio-metabolic disease risk. Health Psychol
34:905–914
Widom CS, Czaja SJ, DuMont KA (2015) Intergenerational transmission of child abuse and
neglect: real or detection bias? Science 347:1480–1485
Wiehn J, Hornberg C, Fischer F (2018) How adverse childhood experiences relate to single and
multiple health risk behaviours in German public university students: a cross-sectional analysis.
BMC Public Health 18:1005
Wong KK, Raine A, Venables P (2018) The effect of being left home alone at age 3 years on
schizotypy and antisocial behavior at ages 17 and 23 years. J Psychiatr Res 105:103–112
Woo E (2005) Urie Bronfenbrenner, 88 – Co-founder of Head Start urged closer family ties Los
Angeles, Los Angeles Times
Wood-Gush DG (1963) Comparative psychology and ethology. Annu Rev Psychol 14:175–200
Wozniak JR, Riley EP, Charness ME (2019) Clinical presentation, diagnosis, and management of
fetal alcohol spectrum disorder. Lancet Neurol 18:760–770
Wu Y, Patchev AV, Daniel G, Almeida OF, Spengler D (2014) Early-life stress reduces DNA meth-
ylation of the Pomc gene in male mice. Endocrinology 155:1751–1762
Yang BZ, Zhang H, Ge W, Weder N, Douglas-Palumberi H, Perepletchikova F, Gelernter J,
Kaufman J (2013) Child abuse and epigenetic mechanisms of disease risk. Am J Prev Med
44:101–107
Zhang L, Zhang D, Sun Y (2019) Adverse childhood experiences and early pubertal timing among
girls: a meta-analysis. Int J Environ Res Public Health 16:2887
Zhang X, Belsky J (2020) Three phases of Gene × Environment interaction research: theoretical
assumptions underlying gene selection. Dev Psychopathol 3:1–12
Zipple MN, Archie EA, Tung J, Altmann J, Alberts SC (2019) Intergenerational effects of early
adversity on survival in wild baboons. elife 8:e47433
Consequences of Early Traumatic
Experiences 3

3.1 Many Places are Affected

The resulting follow-up problems are manifold. They primarily affect the brain and,
secondly, the periphery such as the heart, vessels, kidneys and urinary tract, genitals
and fertility, lungs, teeth, stomach, intestine, bones and muscles, body weight, metabo-
lism, growth and the immune system. The susceptibility to cancer is also significantly
increased after previous traumatic experiences. This multimorbidity was illuminated in a
large meta-analysis.
This meta-analysis included a total of 96 articles, all of which used the ACE ques-
tionnaire by Vincent Felitti (Petruccelli et al. 2019), which we have learned about in
Table 2.1 The risk of developing a subsequent problem thus increases with the number of
early traumatic episodes. The highest risk is for the development of an alcohol problem,
a depression with suicide or an anxiety disorder. With regard to the internal problems,
the risk of overweight, a heart attack, a lung disease and for gastrointestinal disorders is
highest.
The increased activation of the immune system was not treated separately in the
aforementioned meta-analysis. This is surprising because in 2019 this was actually a
clear thing and because with 96 studies surely not all of them refrained from consider-
ing this follow-up problem. Therefore: For many people, the problem of inflammation
or rather chronic inflammation is still not properly described in 2019. I will present this
point at the end of the chapter in order to lead into the actual core of the book (Chap. 4).
We have already heard of the “snowball effect” when additional traumas are added to
an adverse childhood burden. Similarly, we can speak of a snowball effect with regard
to the follow-up problems of alcoholism or nicotine abuse, for example, because they
often lead to other follow-up problems such as heart attack, stroke, tumor, etc. Mental

© The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer-Verlag GmbH, DE, part of Springer 71
Nature 2023
R. H. Straub, Early Trauma as the Origin of Chronic Inflammation,
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-662-66751-4_3
72 3 Consequences of Early Traumatic Experiences

and internal follow-up problems cannot be considered in isolation from each other, they
promote each other and often lead to multimorbidity.

3.2 The Brain Probably Suffers the Most

In the aforementioned Felitti study, patients who had four or more adverse stress expe-
riences in their childhood had an increased risk of health problems such as alcoholism,
drug use, depression, suicide, nicotine abuse, promiscuity and sexually transmitted dis-
eases, physical inactivity and obesity by a factor of 2 to 12 depending on the follow-up
problem (Felitti et al. 1998). The brain is thus without a doubt affected. The brain has
always been in the focus of attention of most scientists, and consequently we have the
most data on this.

3.2.1 Alcohol, Nicotine and Drugs

Addictive behavior is a frequently described consequence of early traumatic experiences.


Alcohol in particular was in the crosshairs of the researchers, for example of the Centers
for Disease Control and Prevention in Atlanta, Georgia. A publication from 2018 sum-
marizes the situation after questioning almost 70000 people: Both excessive binge drink-
ing and other forms of drinking are more common in people after traumatic childhood
experiences than in control subjects. Men and people with a higher education are more
affected (Loudermilk et al. 2018). However, people do not ask about the dangerous side
effects and the social consequences. If two negative elements such as alcohol abuse in
the family and early traumatic experiences come together, the problem of dangerous
alcoholism is particularly high (Sorocco et al. 2015).
Soldiers of the US armed forces who were stationed in Iraq or Afghanistan as part
of Operation Enduring Freedom from 2003 to 2006 showed alcohol problems and a
risk-taking behavior if they had experienced trauma at a young age. Sexual abuse was a
strong risk factor (Clarke-Walper et al. 2014).
Similarly, we find a higher number of smokers—often in combination with alcohol
abuse—in the group of traumatized people (Hsu and Kawachi 2019). Abused women
compared to controls have more difficulty quitting smoking (Smith et al. 2016). Authors
of a longitudinal study of women in Ukraine described how early childhood stress stimu-
lates the onset of smoking in adolescence (Iakunchykova et al. 2015). In the large British
Child Development Study, which began in 1958 and is still providing data, adult individ-
uals with economic constraints in childhood showed a much higher willingness to smoke
heavily (Bartley et al. 2012).
Some scientists trace a cascading course that begins with trauma in childhood, leads
to less self-control and thus leads to an addictive behavior (Otten et al. 2019), an idea
that is supported by others (Sorocco et al. 2015). In addition, personality traits such as
3.2 The Brain Probably Suffers the Most 73

impulsivity (tendency to rash spontaneous actions with reduced self-control), sensa-


tion-seeking (strong desire for new experiences) and aggressive behavioral components
drive addiction (Kim et al. 2018). This is quite the opposite of “executive control”, which
we met in Table 2.4.
Generally, men are more at risk of addiction after previous traumatic childhood expe-
riences than women. If the number of negative experiences is very high (i.e. greater than
4 and more), the gender difference disappears. This is true for alcoholism and especially
for drugs, where there is even a reversal of the problem and women are more addicted
than men, as a study showed (Evans et al. 2017).
The large American study on “alcohol dependence and related problems of addiction
(NESARC)” started in 2001–2002 with a first wave of observation of more than 26,000
people to systematically uncover mental problems in the population. In this first wave of
observation, no person was addicted to drugs, but this changed dramatically 3 years later,
when 1145 people were addicted to drugs. With regard to the reasons for this change,
the researchers found, among other things, early traumatic experiences (Harrington et al.
2011).
I can continue this list indefinitely, the scientific literature is full of it. Things repeat
themselves from different perspectives. Animal experiments show similar difficulties in
mice, rats and monkeys (Pastor et al. 2017, 2018; Walker et al. 2018). They drink, smoke
or become more drug-dependent if they are stressed before birth or in their early years.
But what mechanisms are behind these clear phenomena?

3.2.1.1 Where in the Brain is the Problem?


The regions for reward, motivation, well-being and motivational movement control are
well known in the brain. Figure 3.1 shows these regions in detail by looking at the inside
of a brain hemisphere. These regions are at the center of considerations when it comes to
addiction, which can be triggered by adverse childhood experiences.
A study on humans showed how the wakefulness-promoting drug amphetamine
released significantly more dopamine from important reward centers (dorsST in Fig. 3.1)
in those persons with previous early childhood trauma than in persons without trauma
(Oswald et al. 2014). If dopamine is responsible for rewards, motivation, and well-being,
the dopamine increase caused by amphetamine is a pleasantly experienced signal. This
study showed exactly this positive feeling after amphetamine, and in people with adverse
childhood experiences this feeling was significantly increased. This opens the door to
addiction.
In another consideration of humans, the authors carried out a stress test in which
the test person had to calculate quickly, and not everyone is a quick mental calculator.
During the calculation tasks, the calculation errors were immediately displayed, which
corresponded to an additional unpleasant negative stimulus. In persons without early
trauma, the dopamine release from the reward center did not change during the stress test
(dorsST in Fig. 3.1). However, dopamine release was significantly reduced in those per-
sons with adversities in childhood. When these people were stressed, they had to feel the
74 3 Consequences of Early Traumatic Experiences

top

SNpc

occipital
frontal

VTA Brainstem

Fig. 3.1 Dopamine pathways of the reward system. We look from the midline of the brain at one hemis-
phere. The brown-red pathways of the neurotransmitter dopamine originate in two important regions in
the midbrain, namely in the VTA (ventral tegmental area) and in the SNpc (substantia nigra pars com-
pacta). From there, these pathways reach four different regions: 1) The long arrows lead into the cortex
(mesocortical pathway; for motivation, cognition and emotional response), 2) a short arrow leads to the
nucleus accumbens (NAc, reward, motivation, well-being), 3) three arrows lead to the dorsal striatum
(dorsST, motivational movement control), and 4) three arrows lead to the yellow nuclei in the brainstem
(serotonergic area, noradrenergic area; relevant in depression, etc.). The cauliflower-like structure on the
lower right is the cerebellum (movement control)

opposite of reward. Low dopamine release means sparse reward, low motivation, and lit-
tle well-being. So these people get the positive stimulus elsewhere, for example through
drugs like amphetamines. The platform for addiction is thus created.
In explanation 1 and in the Chap. 2 I pointed to epigenetic changes that can be
engraved by early traumatic experiences. Such engraving of DNA was observed in ani-
mal models with young rats when they were separated from their mother for a longer
period of time (maternal deprivation). They saw epigenetic changes in an impor-
tant reward center in the brain, the nucleus accumbens (lat. accumbere = to lie down)
(Fig. 3.1). Rats with these changes took more amphetamines, the wakefulness-promoting
drug (Lewis et al. 2016).
In a similar way, another team of scientists demonstrated in rats a preference for nico-
tine after early trauma. This preference of the animals was accompanied by an increased
number of a receptor for dopamine in the Nucleus accumbens (here: D2, one of 5 differ-
ent dopamine receptors) (Fig. 3.1). Since dopamine is a reward neurotransmitter, it has
a stronger effect on the reward center due to the higher number of these receptors (Said
et al. 2015). Nicotine therefore stimulates dopamine effects that make you happy, and
the animals loved to reward themselves repeatedly. Similar findings were found in other
regions of the reward system (Brenhouse et al. 2013; Hausknecht et al. 2013).
3.2 The Brain Probably Suffers the Most 75

Finally, in the animal experiment after stress in the uterus, the researchers found a
reduction in the formation of synapses in the nucleus accumbens (Fig. 3.1) and in the
hippocampus, which is responsible for memory, among other things (Martínez-Téllez
et al. 2009). Both findings contribute to disturbed reward behavior.
In summary, we recognize manifold changes on a functional and morphological level
that create a platform for addiction. Early childhood trauma leads to a more sensitive
reward system, and this functions differently under stressful conditions. Stress means
punishment, bad feeling and by no means motivation, which can only be compensated by
drugs. These mechanisms create the starting point for addictive behavior in traumatized
people.

3.2.2 Depression

Stressful events can already affect the future mother before fertilization of an egg, which
can lead to depression during pregnancy. If more than 4 childhood traumas were pres-
ent in the future mother’s childhood, the risk of pregnancy depression is significantly
increased. The risk of depression is additionally increased if social support is lacking or
the relationship with the partner is strained (Wajid et al. 2020).
Traumatic life situations can already have an impact on mother and child in the
womb. An example would be war experiences, which represent a particularly drastic
example with a clear beginning and end, making cause and effect better definable. Israel
is a country with a long tradition in psychology and psychiatry, and so, for example,
Israeli scientists were interested in these questions around war events (Kleinhaus et al.
2013). Since its existence in 1948, the state of Israel has constantly been confronted with
internal and external conflicts. In addition to the early independence war, the so-called
Six-Day War in 1967 and the Yom Kippur War in 1973 were the most impressive epi-
sodes in Israeli history, because there the neighbors threatened with violent war words,
“to throw the state of Israel into the sea”.
Imagine the mothers whose husbands were drafted into the wars while their unborn
children were growing up in their womb (Israel always had a large reserve army and
many husbands were affected). The Six-Day War began on June 5, 1967, and during the
first two days, West Jerusalem was heavily bombarded (Kleinhaus et al. 2013). In the
following days after the Israeli mobilization, no further evacuations and food shortages
were to be expected because the war was over within a short time. So there was a clear
beginning and end of the stressful constellation. The unborn children of the mothers liv-
ing in West Jerusalem were observed over the next 35 years.
The children confronted with the war in the first third of pregnancy showed a three- to
fourfold higher risk of being treated for depression in the follow-up period (they were
compared with a control group with non-stressed pregnancies). The children from the
other thirds of pregnancy were not affected (Kleinhaus et al. 2013). Consequently, the
76 3 Consequences of Early Traumatic Experiences

first third of pregnancy is critical because that is where an important window of brain
development lies (time window see Sect. 2.3).
Sometimes children are evacuated from crisis areas without the accompaniment
of their parents. Do you remember how President Trump treated the children from
Guatemala at the Mexican-American border? He put them in cages—unspeakable!
My protagonist Prof. Michael Rutter was displaced to the USA with his sister during
the Second World War, without experiencing any additional Trump-like hardships.
Evacuation is a severe trauma for children, and all too often it is the starting point for
depressive development, which can lead to an open depression or anxiety disorders dur-
ing adolescence or adulthood. Even the children of displaced people suffer from similar
problems in the next generation.
This is exactly what Finnish scientists had in mind when they questioned the second
generation (F1) of the girls displaced during the Second World War (F0). They found in
the second generation (F1) under 45955 affected women a doubled risk of a psychiatric
illness and almost a fivefold increased risk of depression (Santavirta et al. 2018). Here
the term “intergenerational” is appropriate because the influence of the F0 generation is
directly passed on to the F1 generation (Fig. 2.5).
In Scandinavia, large population-based studies are often conducted. Such a study took
place in Denmark, involving almost one million people born in Denmark between 1980
and 1998. The scientists examined, among other things, early traumatic experiences in
the age group 0 to 15 years. The risk of depression in adulthood increased significantly
with the number of negative experiences (snowball effect). The risk was particularly high
for those people for whom the family structure had broken down between the ages of 0
and 4.
Similarly, the risk was increased in people who, between the ages of 10 and 14, grew
up outside the family due to adverse domestic conditions (Dahl et al. 2017). In addition,
it should be reported that if this latter group of adolescents grew up under private condi-
tions with another family and not in a public institution, they were more effectively pro-
tected from depression (Kessler et al. 2008).
A meta-analysis from 2012 brought together 16 publications with a total of 23544
participants. The authors find a clear connection between childhood trauma and later epi-
sodic or chronic depression. In addition, these affected people have a lower chance of
benefiting from antidepressant therapy (Nanni et al. 2012).
For older people, the symptoms of depression typically decrease around the time of
retirement (Airagnes et al. 2016), because occupational stress factors are eliminated, new
meaningful activities are added, or a better ability to regulate mood during uninterrupted
aging is present. However, it was long unknown whether early traumatic experiences in
childhood affect this improvement in depressive mood.
This question was examined in 2016 on almost 10,000 employees of the French
national gas and electricity company. For men and women with a high number of child-
hood adversities, there was little or no improvement in chronic depression around the
time of retirement. The more adversities there were, the less protective the effect of the
3.2 The Brain Probably Suffers the Most 77

beginning retirement was (Airagnes et al. 2016). The influence of childhood adversities
therefore extends far into the future.
In addition, people with depression are at risk of committing suicide in later life
(suicide) (Wan et al. 2019; O‘Neill and O‘Connor 2020). The risk of a suicide attempt
increases by a factor of 2.5 to 3.5 (Angelakis et al. 2019). The more complex the sit-
uation of child abuse, the higher the risk of suicide (Goldney 1981; Dube et al. 2001;
Björkenstam et al. 2017a; Angelakis et al. 2019). If you need help as a reader, I refer
here to very good offers on the Internet—take the step in time (for Germany: German
Society for Suicide Prevention 2021).

3.2.2.1 Where is the Problem in the Brain?


In Fig. 3.2 important regions are shown which exhibit abnormalities in functional mag-
netic resonance imaging after childhood trauma and subsequent depression. The amyg-
dala is morphologically changed, becoming smaller on both sides after the trauma
(Favaro et al. 2015; Luby et al. 2019; Gray et al. 2020; Nogovitsyn et al. 2020).
Similarly, the region of the ACC is smaller, a region for emotional control and evalu-
ation, especially after social stimuli (Fig. 3.2) (Carballedo et al. 2012; Jensen et al. 2015;
Kuhn et al. 2016; Bromis et al. 2018; Luby et al. 2019; Gray et al. 2020). Furthermore,
the ACC no longer takes on its typical important role as a neuronal hub (Cisler et al.

top

frontal occipital

frontal brain
regions

Uncinate
fasciculus
Amygdala
Hippocampus

Brainstem

HPA axis
cortisol

Fig. 3.2 Brain areas involved in depression. We look at a hemisphere from the midline. The green areas
become smaller after childhood trauma and subsequent depression. The tract between the amygdala and
the hippocampus as well as the frontal lobe in the front part of the brain is shown by red lines (uncinate
fasciculus). This tract can be disturbed and the inhibitory function of the frontal lobe on the amygdala
and hippocampus may be absent. Important areas in the frontal lobe are the ventral and dorsal medial
prefrontal cortex. Abbreviations: ACC, anterior cingulate gyrus (emotional control and evaluation); NAc,
nucleus accumbens (reward center)
78 3 Consequences of Early Traumatic Experiences

2013). In addition, the important memory region of the hippocampus decreases in size
(Rao et al. 2010; Carballedo et al. 2012; Frodl et al. 2014; Calem et al. 2017; Bromis
et al. 2018; Humphreys et al. 2019; Luby et al. 2019). In the animal model, reductions
in the branching of nerve cells or cell loss can be clearly seen in these regions (Brunson
et al. 2001, 2005; Danielewicz and Hess 2014; Lajud and Torner 2015). Cell loss cor-
responds to a situation with insufficient nerve development and increased breakdown.
Finally, some areas in the frontal lobe are smaller (Carballedo et al. 2012).
In animal experiments, a special function of nerve cell groups, long-term potentiation,
can also be studied. I discussed it in a previous book as an indicator of memory function
(Straub 2020). During the investigation of long-term potentiation, nerve cell groups in
the hippocampus are electrically stimulated, and a resulting voltage change in the nerve
cells an be observed. If the investigator caries out this electrical stimulation over several
days, one notices an increase in the voltage change in the nerve cells.
I wrote at the time: “As if by magic, the voltage change” increases in the multiply
stimulated nerve cells, and this is a sign of memory formation (Straub 2020). This long-
term potentiation was studied in animals in the hippocampus after previous stress dur-
ing pregnancy or stress after birth. These animals later showed a reduction in long-term
potentiation as an indication of reduced memory formation (Yeh et al. 2012; Grigoryan
and Segal 2013).
When an experimenter offers emotionally stimulating faces to the test person during
the stay in the tube of the magnetic resonance tomograph, various regions in the brain
react differently. In people with early childhood trauma before puberty and later depres-
sion, the amygdala reaction was weakened, while later traumatic effects after puberty
resulted in an increased reaction (Zhu et al. 2019). Others showed an increased reaction
of the amygdala to sad faces, indicating an overactivity of the limbic system (Suzuki
et al. 2014). In parallel, an increased reaction of the ACC is observed, which is responsi-
ble for the evaluation of the faces (Suzuki et al. 2014; Peters et al. 2019).
When children and adolescents grow up, the observer sees a clear increase in activ-
ity in the reward center, the nucleus accumbens (Fig. 3.1). If an early childhood trauma
preceded and a depression is present, this maturation process is attenuated, and signifi-
cantly lower activity is observed in this nucleus (Goff et al. 2013). This change means a
smaller reward experience for the depressive.
The hook-shaped nerve fiber bundle, the uncinate fasciculus (uncinate, lat. hook-
shaped) (Fig. 3.2) is noticeably changed. This fasciculus is a central connecting tract in
the limbic system, which connects the amygdala and the hippocampus on the one hand
with the frontal lobe on the other. After early childhood trauma with later depression, the
function of this important tract is disturbed, which impairs the tasks of the limbic system
and disturbs emotional reactions (Kircanski et al. 2019). Similarly, the second major lim-
bic tract, the fornix, which is not shown in Fig. 3.2, is also disturbed (Frodl et al. 2012).
A critical point is the HPA axis. This axis is often overactive in depression and there-
fore too much CRH is produced in the brain and too much cortisol is produced in the
3.2 The Brain Probably Suffers the Most 79

periphery (van Bodegom et al. 2017). We will come back to this topic in more detail later
(Chap. 4, Sect. 4.2.3).
We therefore clearly recognize changes in the morphological structure of the brain
and in functional aspects. The regions can be very well defined by means of functional
magnetic resonance imaging. Often parts of the limbic system (amygdala, hippocam-
pus, ACC, uncinate fasciculus, fornix), the memory system (hippocampus, ACC) and the
reward system (nucleus accumbens) are affected. It changes the processing and storage
of emotional stimuli, and depressive and anxious reactions are more often observed.

3.2.3 Anxiety Disorders

Anxiety is a normal thing if we go through the pitch-dark forest at night, stand on a high
rock without security, climb a high tree, drive too fast with the car, jump out of an air-
plane for the first time, etc. Anxiety has a protective function because it protects us from
the dangers of everyday life. For some people, however, anxiety is more pronounced and
for some it even takes on an exaggerated form. We speak of anxiety disorders.
Although the sight of a snake like the ringed snake is not exactly a feeling of comfort,
after a relatively short assessment of the situation, the first fear has turned into curiosity
and amazement. Some people, however, react phobically to snakes, and then there is an
anxiety disorder from the field of phobias. Other phobic reactions can occur on a large
square, in confined spaces, in front of objects, in front of animals (e.g. snake, spider,
wasp) or other situations. A phobia is an excessive fear reaction to defined triggers.
In contrast, there are other anxiety disorders such as panic disorders (sudden anxiety
attacks lasting a few minutes without focus on object or situation), generalized anxiety
disorder (diffuse anxiety about everyday events lasting at least 6 months), and anxiety
disorders that are coupled with depression. Anxiety disorders are often associated with
physical symptoms of the fight-or-flight response (sweating, heart palpitations, trem-
bling, dry mouth, etc.), and they typically impair the lives of those affected. An anxiety
disorder can intensify in a vicious circle and lead to social withdrawal.
The connection between childhood trauma and anxiety disorders in adulthood was
summarized in a meta-analysis (Carr et al. 2013). Furthermore, more than 100,000 peo-
ple in Sweden were included in a large population-based study in which childhood trau-
mas were systematically recorded. In total, 42% of the people had at least one severe
trauma in childhood (the most common being the parents’ divorce). People with child-
hood trauma had more psychiatric disorders than control persons, most often anxiety dis-
orders. If several adversities occurred at the same time, the risk of an anxiety disorder
increased by a factor of 2 (Björkenstam et al. 2016).
In a large British longitudinal study of a group of people born in 1958 (Child
Development Study), the researchers found that more than 9000 people had anxiety
80 3 Consequences of Early Traumatic Experiences

disorders as adults at the age of 45 years. This anxiety disorder was associated with a
smaller social network and interpersonal relationship problems (Ford et al. 2011). Others
showed the connection between early traumatic experiences and social phobia, in which
the affected persons were afraid of situations in which they were the center of attention
(DeWit et al. 2005). The traumas can have an effect during pregnancy, as is the case,
for example, with stressed pregnant women (Phillips et al. 2005). One thing the publica-
tions on anxiety pointed out was that more women than men were affected (Wanner et al.
2012).
In 2013, there was a severe earthquake of magnitude 6.6 in Sichuan Province, China
(sometimes called Ya’an), and the situation lasted for about 2 days with aftershocks. A
total of 196 people died, 24 were missing, and 11826 were injured (968 seriously). Three
years after the earthquake, Chinese scientists conducted a study of 6132 children and
adolescents aged 9 to 18 years. Children/adolescents were more likely to be affected by
excessive fear if they were exposed to the earthquake, left alone, poor, or experienced
hostility and harassment from the peer group, and women were more affected than men
(Tang et al. 2020).
In animal models with mice and rats, something similar to anxiety can be quite
well determined by behavioral studies. The expert speaks more of a defensive behav-
ior because animals take a defensive position. Obviously, early stressful life experiences
lead to increased defensive behavior in these animals (Fride et al. 1986; Wang et al.
2012; Wilson et al. 2013; Rau et al. 2015). With animals, the observer can also look into
brain regions, and this has been done, for example, with regard to the amygdala, the hip-
pocampus (memory region) and various frontal brain regions (Kraszpulski et al. 2006;
Eiland and McEwen 2012; Laloux et al. 2012; Rau et al. 2015). In rats, the memory of
fear is less erasable (Green et al. 2011), and female animals are more affected than male
animals (Bowman et al. 2004).
If you ask at this point where all this takes place in the brain, I can again refer to
Fig. 3.2, because that is where the most important regions are basically shown. In
humans, these are the amygdala, the hippocampus, the frontal brain region and the ACC
(Williams et al. 2009; Kuhn et al. 2016; Lange et al. 2019; Murthy and Gould 2020).
The amygdala is central here, which after a trauma shows inadequate increased activ-
ity (Kraaijenvanger et al. 2020). The increased activity arises from the disturbed connec-
tion between the amygdala and frontal brain parts via the impaired uncinate fasciculus
(Fig. 3.2) and the fornix (Graham et al. 2015; Scheinost et al. 2017; Kaiser et al. 2018;
Herzberg and Gunnar 2020). In animals, these regions are similarly affected (Murthy and
Gould 2020).
The appearance of similar regions as in depression shows us the close connection
between anxiety disorder and depression. Part of the connection between important areas
such as the amygdala and hippocampus and the ACC is disturbed (Kim et al. 2019), and
the nerve activity is changed in the respective regions. In the animal model, the observer
sees how density of neuronal synapses decreases in these regions (Lemaire et al. 2000;
Brunson et al. 2005; Yang et al. 2006; Derks et al. 2016).
3.2 The Brain Probably Suffers the Most 81

3.2.4 Personality and Childhood Trauma

A person’s personality is usually constant over a long period of time. We know five dis-
tinct personality traits (Explanation 5). Personality disorders are “extreme manifestations
of a personality with inflexible, rigid and inappropriate personality traits that impair
the quality of life of the affected person and lead to suffering and/or frequent conflicts
with the environment” (Professional associations and societies for psychiatry; child and
adolescent psychiatry; psychotherapy; psychosomatics; neurology and neurology from
Germany and Switzerland 2021).

Explanation 5 Personality—The Big Five


In the so-called Five Factor Model five essential factors for a personality with weak
compared to strong expression are observed:

1. Openness to experience (“conservative, cautious” compared to “inventive,


curious”)
2. Conscientiousness (“carefree, negligent” compared to “effective, organized”)
3. Extraversion (“reticent, reserved” compared to “sociable”)
4. Agreeableness (“competitive, antagonistic” compared to “cooperative, friendly”)
5. Neuroticism (“emotional, vulnerable” compared to “self-confident, calm”)

In alternative models, “honesty-humility” is sometimes considered as a sixth factor


that is included in “agreeableness” under the Big Five. These description factors are
very stable, independent of each other and existent in different cultural circles, and
they can change slightly during aging (towards less openness, with more conscien-
tiousness and agreeableness). The heritability of personality traits is very high at 50%.
Only 40–45% are determined by environmental influences (Nettle 2009). ◄

Adverse childhood experiences influence these stable personality traits by exacerbating


or bringing them out more. In a large Australian study, the authors found a clear rela-
tionship between early trauma and a higher degree of neuroticism, a negative mood, a
stronger loss of self-control, and pronounced anti-social behavior, regardless of age or
gender (Rosenman and Rodgers 2006).
In addition, we often see personality disorders in people with previous trauma, for
example narcissistic personality disorders with drastic emotional behavior and those with
anti-social or eccentric behavior. Abuse and neglect in early years promote the exacerba-
tion of personality traits up to personality disorders, as shown by large population-based
studies (Afifi et al. 2011).
In a Finnish study over 30 years, the following was observed: A higher number of
personality disorders is present in people aged 30 if the mothers experienced subjec-
tive stress during pregnancy. The higher the perceived stress, the more often personality
82 3 Consequences of Early Traumatic Experiences

disorders occurred. The result is therefore significant because this analysis controlled
for the influence of important confounding factors such as maternal and paternal mental
illness, nicotine exposure, and pregnancy depression (Brannigan et al. 2020). A similar
English study found a relationship between stress during pregnancy and personality dis-
order (Winsper et al. 2012).
In a similar way, a Swedish longitudinal study from the Stockholm area often finds a
personality disorder in young adulthood if the children were exposed to a higher num-
ber of adverse childhood experiences. At the same time, the school performance of these
people was lower than that of normal controls (Björkenstam et al. 2017b). A Swiss study
confirmed the relationship between trauma and personality disorders (Winsper et al.
2012). Thus, the special thing is a sharper shaping of the personality, which can develop
into a real personality disorder.

3.2.5 More Psycho- and Neuropathology

Above, only the most common problems were described extensively. In this book, there
is also not enough space to describe all psychopathological consequences in detail. This
is left to the specialized literature (Thapar et al. 2015). Therefore, in a short table, further
psycho- and neuropathological consequences are summarized briefly and the important
literature is demonstrated (Table 3.1).
If the trauma expert observes children after adversities over a very long period of
time, the psychopathological disorders remain—they do not weaken very much (Clark
et al. 2010). Furthermore, bad childhood experiences can lead to an increased rejection
of antipsychotic drugs by those affected, which can fuel the disease process (Ajnakina
et al. 2018). However, trauma experiences in childhood and adolescence are not solely
responsible for the onset of psychopathology. There are other factors in the sense of
“double hits”. Such double hits can be events in childhood and later in adolescence (e.g.
drugs or new traumas and stress experiences) (Peh et al. 2019). We will come back to the
double hits in several places in the book.
The development of psychoses is assumed to be caused by trauma-induced neuronal
developmental disorders, in which there are structural and functional brain disorders that
we have already learned about in the previous chapters. A dopaminergic dysregulation,
atrophy of brain areas (e.g. amygdala, frontal lobe), damage to the hippocampus and
hyperactivity of the HPA axis as in depression are of importance (Longden and Read
2016).
Furthermore, the observer often sees different response patterns in women and men. I
have already dealt with this in the Sect. 2.8.3. So women show accelerated sexual matu-
rity more often, with central nervous system effects playing an important role (Bath
2020). Men, on the other hand, have delayed sexual maturity.
Female animals have an early-maturing development in terms of emotional stimuli
and fear learning. The amygdala undergoes faster maturation, whereas in male animals
3.2 The Brain Probably Suffers the Most 83

Table 3.1  Further psycho- and neuropathological follow-up problems after early traumatic expe-
riences outside of addiction, depression and anxiety disorders
Psycho- and Neuropathology Literature
Schizophrenia (Wicks et al. 2005; Matheson et al. 2013;
Schroeder et al. 2016; Sideli et al. 2020)
Paranoid psychoses (Sideli et al. 2020)
Bipolar Disorders (manic-depressive) (Palmier-Claus et al. 2016; Post et al. 2016)
Autism Spectrum Disorder (Kinney et al. 2008; Berg et al. 2016)
Attention Deficit-/Hyperactivity Disorder (Zhu et al. 2015; Østergaard et al. 2016;
(ADHD) Björkenstam et al. 2018; MacKinnon et al.
2018); in animals (Bock et al. 2017)
Adjustment Disorder (Reaction to Stressful (Shaw et al. 1994; Kiff et al. 2012)
Life Events)
Attachment disorder (Shaw and Vondra 1993; van Dijken 1998;
Opendak et al. 2017; Perry et al. 2017; Venta
2020)
Disorder of the executive function (Richards and Wadsworth 2004; Laplante et al.
This includes self-control, emotion regula- 2008; Hackman et al. 2010; Hostinar et al. 2012;
tion, planning ability, working memory, and Schwabe et al. 2012; Li et al. 2015; Mittal et al.
conscious attention control. 2015; Lambert et al. 2017; Sheridan et al. 2017;
Chen et al. 2019; Goodman et al. 2019; Lovallo
et al. 2019)
Disorder of social behavior (conduct disorder), (Beck and Shaw 2005; Provençal et al. 2015;
aggression MacKinnon et al. 2018)
Disorder regarding the parental role (Barrett 2009)
Dementia (Alzheimer’s disease) (Persson und Skoog 1996; Norton et al. 2011;
Ravona-Springer et al. 2012; Pesonen et al.
2013; Donley et al. 2018; Lemche 2018; Tani
et al. 2020)
Fatigue (chronic fatigue) (Cho et al. 2012; Crawley et al. 2012; Bower
et al. 2014)
Epilepsy (Edwards et al. 2002; Jones et al. 2014; van
Campen et al. 2014); in animals (Ali et al. 2013;
Dubé et al. 2015)
Motor disorders and reduced muscle strength (Buitelaar et al. 2003; Cheval et al. 2019); in
animals (Schneider et al. 1999)
Sexual orientation; men become more fem- (Ellis and Cole-Harding 2001; Hines et al. 2002;
inine and women become more masculine; McLaughlin et al. 2012; Suglia et al. 2020);
faster sexual development in women, slowed in animals (Ward 1972, 1984; Bowman et al.
sexual development in men 2004; Meek et al. 2006; Morgan and Bale 2011;
Popova et al. 2011)
Sleep disorders and pain Its own subchapter below and in the introduction
84 3 Consequences of Early Traumatic Experiences

accelerated development of the hippocampus and amygdala can be seen (Bath 2020). In
general, there is accelerated maturation of the limbic system and delayed development of
cortical areas in both sexes (Bath 2020).
Women are characterized by increased depression and anxiety, whereas men are more
likely to suffer from autistic disorders, attention deficit/hyperactivity and aggressive
behavior (see 2.8.3) (Wainwright and Surtees 2002; Davis and Pfaff 2014).

3.2.6 More Sleep Disorders as a Result of Previous Trauma

I mentioned the topic of sleeping in a introductory text in the first chapter, and I ask
again: How can I sleep while my bed is burning? Children from families with lower
incomes go to bed late (on average at 10:36 p.m.) and have a high variability of sleep
time of plus/minus 3.68 h. The parents have hardly any knowledge of official recommen-
dations on the topic of sleep in children, and 65% of the parents slept with their children
in one bed (Ordway et al. 2020). Other authors see the same connection between the
financial situation of the family and the sleep problems (El-Sheikh et al. 2015).
Childhood adversities often lead to problems falling asleep and other forms of sleep
deterioration in teenagers later (April-Sanders et al. 2020). The more childhood traumas
there were, the stronger the sleep problem in later life (Sullivan et al. 2019; Sheehan
et al. 2020).
If the investigator asks women of middle age about the quality of sleep, he often rec-
ognizes in those with childhood adversities a short sleep duration, a longer time to fall
asleep, frequent nocturnal awakenings and more often short daytime sleep (2019). Others
observed in adults with previous bad childhood experiences numerous nightmares, sleep-
walking and sleep spindles. The latter are typically observed in electroencephalography
in the deep sleep phases of the eye movement-free sleep (Non-REM-sleep) (Nielsen et al.
2019).
Here is the principle of the “double hit,” because bad sleep often appears when there
have been previous adverse childhood experiences and another trauma situation arises
later in life. This was observed in students who were just starting their studies at the uni-
versity and had experienced emotional neglect in their childhood (2018).
Thomas Boyce conducted an important study on children. He showed how children
with adverse childhood experiences showed different sleep behavior depending on the
activation capacity of the sympathetic nervous system. For this purpose, he offered four
slightly stress-inducing events in quick succession and tested the reaction of the sympa-
thetic nervous system. If children with trauma had a weak sympathetic nervous system
response, they had more sleep problems than those with a good sympathetic nervous sys-
tem response. He interpreted the weakened sympathetic nervous system response as a
dulling to the stress-inducing stimuli as a sign of a severe disorder (Alkon et al. 2017).
Finally, two large epidemiological studies are to be mentioned, in which 22000 and
almost 10000 people were interviewed. In both studies, the risk of a sleep disorder was
3.2 The Brain Probably Suffers the Most 85

dependent on the number of adverse childhood experiences: the more traumas, the worse
the sleep (Wang et al. 2016; Sullivan et al. 2019).

3.2.6.1 Where in the brain is the problem?


Previously, we suspected the cause of insomnia (falling asleep and sleeping through the
night) in people affected mainly in the brain centers responsible for the circadian rhythm
and regular sleep/wakefulness (Van Someren 2021). According to newer concepts, these
brain areas are not involved alone (Van Someren 2021), but rather those areas that we
already learned about in the context of depression in Fig. 3.2. These new considerations
came about after the influence of stressors on sleep behavior was examined more closely
(Van Someren 2021). Stressors lead to short-term and long-term overexcitability in some
brain centers (e.g. amygdala, Locus coeruleus).
Doctors distinguish between different sleep phases, and one phase is called REM
sleep, in which we move our eyes rapidly back and forth (Rapid Eye Movement).
During the REM sleep phase, we have a high plasticity for the formation of synapses,
which is why this interval is important for the development of memory (Van Someren
2021). In this phase, the topmost noradrenaline area (the locus coeruleus from Fig. 3.3)
must be switched off, because noradrenaline otherwise interferes with the formation of
synapses. A reasonable REM sleep is absolutely essential in order to protect the brain at
least in these intervals from an excess of noradrenaline (Van Someren 2021).
Stressors can disrupt this shutdown of the locus coeruleus, and restless or fragmented
sleep is the result. People react very differently to stressors. Some can still sleep great
and process stress during sleep. Others, however, experience restless sleep, which can
be dangerous because recovery is lacking and because memory formation is disturbed.
Here, too, genetic factors are important (Van Someren 2021).
This is typically associated with an activation of the HPA axis (Van Someren 2021).
This increased readiness to release hormones of the HPA axis—for example, CRH from
the hypothalamus and cortisol from the adrenal gland—we have already encountered in
depression. This phenomenon is associated with increased responsiveness and lack of
nocturnal shutdown of the amygdala (Amygdala) and the locus coeruleus (Van Someren
2021). The amygdala and locus coeruleus are controlled and braked by the frontal lobe
and ACC (Fig. 3.2). Since the frontal lobe and ACC and others perceive their function of
control poorly, the brake of the amygdala and the locus coeruleus is lacking.
Somehow it looks as if the affected people are constantly in a state of increased vig-
ilance, and so these responsible regions are typically changed after childhood traumas.
The affected person has, so to speak, learned to be constantly attentive. He is more sen-
sitive to negative information and he becomes “hyperaroused”. However, we cannot
be constantly wide awake because this is extremely exhausting—energy-consuming.
Finally, repeated negative events can stabilize this misalignment in the long term.
86 3 Consequences of Early Traumatic Experiences

3.2.7 More Pain after Childhood Adversities

In the introductory chapter, it was already reported about pain in newborns. There it was
pointed out that early pain situations can have long-term consequences: fibromyalgia,
migraine, chronic pelvic pain, irritable bowel syndrome and chronic urethral and bladder
pain, which can all occur more frequently after early traumatic experiences. The connec-
tion between childhood adversities and chronic pain has been known for quite some time
(Davis et al. 2005). Furthermore, the connection between the number of hardships and
the severity of functional limitations caused by pain was described (Nelson et al. 2018;
Groenewald et al. 2020).
In a large epidemiological study of more than 6000 adolescents, a connection was
shown between back pain, neck pain and headache on the one hand and previous experi-
ences of violence on the other. The risk of these chronic pain problems was increased by
2–2.5 times in those affected. At the same time, these people had mental disorders that
partly explained the pain (McLaughlin et al. 2016).
Some scientists speak of an increased pain sensitivity, which is caused by central
nervous system factors. Our brain has excellent possibilities to block pain from the
body periphery, so to speak to block the inputs. Central to the suppression of pain are
mechanisms in the brainstem and in the spinal cord. The inhibition takes place through
central areas of the brain, among other things through descending pain-inhibiting path-
ways (Middlebrook et al. 2021) (blue path in Fig. 3.3). Although there is no perfect
test to investigate these functions, various possibilities of testing have been proposed
(Middlebrook et al. 2021). In one type of examination, increased pain sensitivity is tested
as a reduced pain threshold to various stimuli. Descending pathways can also amplify the
pain inputs, which gives the brain a controlled possibility of amplified pain perception
(blue path in Fig. 3.3).
After the experience of childhood trauma, doctors found increased pain sensitiv-
ity more often in those affected (e.g. (Tesarz et al. 2016; You and Meagher 2016, 2018;
Atlas et al. 2018)). Since pain receptors are always slightly stimulated in the periphery,
lower pain thresholds easily switch on pain perception. The person affected experiences
the normally subliminal stimuli as painful or different. A mild touch of the skin maybe
perceived as a burn. A slight pressure on a bone protrusion is noticed as pressure pain,
etc. So patients with fibromyalgia often experienced childhood adversities (Low and
Schweinhardt 2012; Jones 2016). Harmless-looking pressure stimuli are perceived by
those affected as painful—this is the nature of neuropathy.
There are several animal models—mostly on rodents—in which exactly this type of
chronic, centrally mediated pain hypersensitivity can be investigated (e.g. (Alvarez et al.
2013; Prusator and Greenwood-Van Meerveld 2016)). Early traumatic experiences are
coupled with increased pain sensitivity. In the animal experiment on the rat with previ-
ous separation experience (maternal deprivation), those animals with high estrogen levels
had more pain than animals with low levels (Moloney et al. 2016), which speaks for the
3.2 The Brain Probably Suffers the Most 87

top
postcentral gyrus
conscious pain processing

insular cortex
frontal occipital
Cingulum

medial ACC
prefrontal medial NAc
thalamus
cortex

PAG
Raphe
Amygdala
Hippocampus LC

HPA axis sympathetic


cortisol noradrenaline

descending ascending
pathway pathways
report pain

Fig. 3.3 Central nervous components of pain processing. We look from the midline of the brain at
one hemisphere. The green and blue areas are significantly involved in pain processing (there are more
areas involved, but not shown because of complexity). The red track from the periphery reports the pain
via the thalamus to the cortex (postcentral gyrus), where we become aware of the pain and where it is
assigned to a location. The blue descending pathway picks up information in the frontal lobe, anterior
cingulate gyrus (ACC), nucleus accumbens (NAc), periaqueductal gray (PAG) and in the raphe nuclei
(serotonin) and forwards it to the spinal cord in order to inhibit or increase pain inputs there (blue line).
The insular cortex, the amygdala, hippocampus, LC (locus coeruleus = topmost noradrenaline center)
and HPA axis are interconnected with all regions (not shown). This activates the topmost centers of hor-
mones and the sympathetic nervous system during pain. At the same time, emotions arise in the limbic
system (amygdala) and a memory of the pain in the hippocampus. The frontal lobe is responsible for
the evaluation of the pain. Further aspects are illustrated in Fig. 3.1. Abbreviations: LC, locus coeruleus
(noradrenaline center); NAc, nucleus accumbens (reward center); PAG, periaqueductal gray

influence of biological sex. Estrogens can increase the feeling of pain, and that affects
women more than men (Straub 2007).
A special brain area is the nucleus accumbens (NAc), which we have learned about
in the reward system (Fig. 3.1). Why does the reward system have something to do with
pain? The answer is: The occurrence of pain is experienced as punishment and the alle-
viation of pain as a reward. Sounds trivial, is well researched, and endogenous opioids
are of great importance for this positive feeling (Navratilova et al. 2015). So if we are
rewarded (opposite: punished), we learn more easily to avoid the action leading to pain
88 3 Consequences of Early Traumatic Experiences

and to carry out the right measure to combat pain, because a reward memory is created
(opposite: a punishment memory).
The inputs for these pains can also come from the gastrointestinal tract or from the
regions of the bladder and genital organs (red line in Fig. 3.3). This can result in stom-
ach pain, irritable bowel syndrome or chronic urethral and bladder complaints (Miranda
2009; O’Mahony et al. 2009). The insular cortex is primarily responsible for the con-
scious perception of visceral pain from the abdomen (Fig. 3.3 and explanation 6).

Explanation 6 Perception of the inner world—the insular cortex

You feel the sunshine, a bird tweet, a pleasant smell of spring, the taste of tiramisu,
a tingling on your foot from a fly. Usually, these perceptions are related to the five
senses: sight, hearing, smell, taste and touch. In addition, we feel whether our body is
in balance and we recognize the position of our body or the position of our movable
parts of the body; in short, you perceive the outside world.
If we pay attention, we also perceive things from the inside of the body. So we feel
our heartbeat, the filling of the stomach, the stretching of the intestine and the tension
of the bladder wall. If the blood pressure rises, we notice a feeling of pressure in the
chest or neck area. For these individual sensations, we have specific receptors (recep-
tors on nerve fibers; Nobel Prize in Medicine 2021) in different body areas that send
information to the brain.
Therefore, there are regions in the brain that tell us something about things from
the outside world and the inside world. Things from the inside world are perceived
in a special region, the insular cortex (Fig. 3.3), and they can be very versatile. For
example, we feel the accumulation of metabolic products (lactate in the tissue), cell
contents (when cells are destroyed), parasites (via the hormone histamine), infectious
agents (e.g. bacterial components), cytokines (immune cell messengers) and hor-
mones. We usually don’t have clear concepts for this and rather vague feelings that
we can only connect with difficulty to a location in the body. While things from the
outside world can be named very well (postcentral gyrus in Fig. 3.3), they can only be
felt and described badly for the inside world. To give clear names to the consciously
perceived things of the inside world, we need quite a bit of training and experience.
A beautiful example of this is the start of a cold. The day before, we notice
strange restlessness that is suddenly replaced by a bad feeling. Even before the
mucous membranes in the nose and throat swell and hurt, we then have a feeling of
“being affected”, “being weak”, “lack of motivation” and sometimes “being numb”.
Instinctively, we switch from high activity to a low level. Friends of home remedies
now consciously use those small helpers such as tea, warm beer, herbs and others in a
prophylactic way. ◄

Intensified pain does not only result from a reduced inhibition of pain input, but is also
caused by expectations, which in turn can be based on an anxious or depressive mood.
3.2 The Brain Probably Suffers the Most 89

Expectations can, in fact, influence the way pain is experienced (Atlas and alʼAbsi
2018). Furthermore, attention to pain can be increased by childhood trauma (Lane et al.
2018). Finally, sleep is of great importance because sleep disorders increase pain sen-
sitivity (Mathur et al. 2018). Pain, in turn, leads to sleep disorders, and thus a vicious
circle can develop.

3.2.8 Summary

Many brain regions in Fig. 3.3 play an important role in the previous descriptions of cen-
tral nervous locations for addiction, anxiety, depression and sleep disorders (Fig. 3.1 and
3.2). Since these regions described earlier are closely related to early trauma, the connec-
tion between pain—especially pain sensitivity—and childhood adversity is explained.
The medial prefrontal cortex and the anterior cingulate gyrus (ACC) are inhibited
and/or reduced in size and there is a dopamine dysregulation in the nucleus accumbens
and elsewhere (Rauschecker et al. 2015), which severely impairs the descending inhibi-
tory pathway (Fig. 3.3). These regions influence the evaluation of pain, the memory of
pain, the expectation of pain, the emotions associated with pain, and the “Gain Control”,
which is what the doctor means by the control of peripheral pain inputs (Rauschecker
et al. 2015). The embedding of pain processing in the brain regions discussed is schemat-
ically shown in Fig. 3.4.
The elements of Fig. 3.4 are often connected to each other, that is, they influence each
other in inhibitory or stimulatory ways. You can see this, for example, in the magenta
connection lines between insular cortex, amygdala and locus coeruleus with the output
of the central sympathetic nervous system, the caudal and rostral ventrolateral medulla
(in the medulla oblongata, shortly C/RVLM in Fig. 3.4). These connections will be
picked up again later in the book.
Without much effort, I can imagine the normally functioning brain in a balanced situ-
ation: The weighing scale in the middle of Fig. 3.4 shows a horizontal position. Damages
either by too much influence on one side of the balance (too many synapses, too many
cells, over-activation, too strong connection between regions, enlarged brain regions)
or too little influence (loss of synapses, cell death, under-activation, weak connection
between regions, shrinking of brain areas) deflect the scale in the wrong direction, which
leads to follow-up problems like addiction, anxiety, depression, chronic pain, sleep dis-
orders and other neuro- and psychopathologies. The long-term programmed change after
childhood adversities deflects the scale—so to speak reprograms its basic setting—and
thus leads to neuro- and psychopathological follow-up problems, especially when further
events occur, which deflect the scale in the same direction. The memory is programmed
in a wrong way. These central nervous follow-up problems can cause plenty of difficul-
ties also in the body periphery.
90 3 Consequences of Early Traumatic Experiences

Emotional
processing Place of pain
Evaluation perception
Memory of pain
Consciousness

Insular cortex Post central


brain
Frontal brain:
Emotion
medial
social role Amygdala Fear, emotion
prefrontal
memory memory
cortex
ex. control Hippocampus Memory
ACC: anterior
cingulate Central
Locus coeruelus
gyrus noradrenaline
[reticular formation]
Reward Nucleus
accumbens balance NTS (N. Vagus)
Sympathetic
C/RVLM
Hypothalamus noradrenaline

Pituitary PAG
Ascending pathways
HPA axis Thalamus report pain from the
chest and abdominal
Cortisol Ascending Descending
area, among others
pathways pathways inhibit
report pain or increase
pain input

Fig. 3.4 Important brain regions for the consequences of early trauma. This figure schematically sum-
marizes the elements from Fig. 3.1, 3.2 and 3.3 and others. Due to the complexity of the situation, this
figure must be interpreted as greatly simplified. Descriptions are given in the text. Abbreviations: ACC;
anterior cingulate gyrus; C/RVLM; caudal (C) and rostral (R) ventrolateral medulla (in the medulla
oblongata); ex. control, executive control (self-control, emotion regulation, planning ability, working
memory, and conscious attention control); HPA axis, hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis; NTS, nucleus
tractus solitarii (inputs from the chest and abdomen); PAG, periaqueductal gray (periaqueductal gray)

3.3 The Body Periphery Suffers as Well

With regard to the body periphery, I would like to focus on body weight first, because
it is often the starting point for follow-up problems when it is in the range of a Body-
Mass-Index of 30 kg/m2 or more (explanation 7). Diabetes mellitus of the aging person is
then much more common. On the other hand, follow-up problems such as hypertension,
cardiovascular diseases, stroke, osteoarthritis of large joints (knee, hip, etc.) and tumors
occur more frequently. These people are more susceptible to viral diseases like COVID-
19 (Mahamat-Saleh et al. 2021). If obesity is combined with an addiction problem—for
example smoking—the negative effect is amplified and the risk for the consequences
goes up. We recognize the snowball effect again.
This is why I treat heart attacks and other cardiovascular events, lung diseases, gas-
trointestinal diseases, and age-related problems, and finally address the epidemiological
link between early trauma and activation of the immune system. The next main chapter
explains why chronic immune activation can occur.
3.3 The Body Periphery Suffers as Well 91

Explanation 7 Obesity—the Body Mass Index

The doctor speaks of obesity from a body mass index of 30 kg/m2 (body mass
index = weight in kg, divided by height in m squared). People are called normal
weight if the body mass index is between 18.5 and 25 kg/m2. There is overweight
between 25 and 30 kg/m2. The body mass index is only an approximate measure
because it does not take into account the body composition, that is, whether muscle
or fat tissue. It is favorable to have a lot of muscle and unfavorable to have a lot
of fat tissue. People with a high body mass index can be quite healthy if the body
weight in kilograms is essentially determined by the muscles. To better assess this
situation, the doctor would have to examine the body composition using bioimped-
ance analysis or magnetic resonance imaging. ◄

3.3.1 High Body Weight

The topic obesity has already occupied me in my two previous books, and the consider-
ations led to a comprehensive table, which I reproduce here and now add more points.
Elements or consequences of early childhood stress were already mentioned in this table.
These include food shortages for the child in the womb (Barker phenomenon, see 2.1),
the reward behavior with the possibility of developing an addiction (see Sect. 3.2.1), the
lack of self-control as a result of trauma (impairment of executive function, Table 2.4)
and the socioeconomic status, which—nowadays widely recognized—is an adverse
experience according to Urie Bronfenbrenner (2.1.1).
An example of an extensive study of children with low socioeconomic status and a
developmental course with high Body-Mass-Index can be found in Bae et al., who inter-
viewed over 11,000 people over a period of 13 years (Bae et al. 2014). This work is con-
firmed by other researchers (e.g. Wickrama et al. 2014). This table must now be extended
to include additional early traumatic experiences after further study.
The increased body weight is certainly not determined solely by the early traumatic
experiences, but is often based on a combination including the genetic disposition (the
25% from Table 3.2) (Beach et al. 2020). Early childhood adversities, on the other hand,
are not associated with the opposite—that is, underweight—as a study of over 105,000
students in the USA showed (Davis et al. 2019).
If mothers experience objective hardships when the children are about 5 years old,
this affects the children’s body weight at the age of 11 years. If the mothers were highly
stressed, the child’s obesity risk was 2.3 times higher (Hope et al. 2019). Here we see
the important influence of the mothers again, which extends even into the time of the
pregnancy.
For example, American-New Zealand scientists were able to identify in more than
5800 pregnant women with objective risk factors such as smoking, older age, status as
a single parent, low educational level, economic constraints, unemployment, living in a
92 3 Consequences of Early Traumatic Experiences

Table 3.2  Concepts for explaining obesity


Name Meaning
Calories* In relation to body size and body weight, too many
calories are consumed and too few are used.
Physical activity* Physical activity is too low. Too few calories are
burned.
Wrong calories* Wrong calories—namely carbohydrate calories—
instead of protein or fat calories induce obesity more,
because more insulin is released (the storage hormone
No. 1). This theory is now being questioned again
because newer comparative studies did not confirm the
concept.
Genetics* About 25% of obesity is due to genetic factors.
However, the 97 known genetic factors only explain
2.7% of all cases of obesity.
This means: The combination of genetic factors with
each other and the combination of genetic factors with
environmental factors are not yet understood.
Epigenetics Barker phenomenon* Food shortages for the child in the womb lead to faster
weight gain in childhood. The weight once acquired is
maintained.
Fat mother, A luxury situation in the womb leads to obese children
fat child* who are obese in adulthood.
Anatomical The number of fat cells is determined after birth and in
requirements* early childhood and remains the same throughout life.
The amount of brown, energy-consuming and
heat-generating fat tissue is adjustable. Most obese
people have little brown fat tissue.
Chemical environmental factors Factors such as polyfluorinated chemicals and
Bisphenol A have been suspected because they act
on body’s own hormone receptors. The matter is not
clarified, but important and exciting.
Smoking* Smoking reduces body weight and quitting smoking
increases body weight. Nicotine can slow down food
intake, and smoking shifts the microbiome to lower
food utilization.
Disruption of the gut-fat-brain axis* The success of bariatric surgery (obesity surgery)
points to important connections between the gut and
brain that depend on the storage state in the fat tissue.
Leptin is the prototype of the connecting hormones.
Here, the so-called microbiome can have an additional
influence, because an obesity-promoting microbiome
can break down energy carriers and increase the utili-
zation of nutrients. Smoking disrupts this.

(continued)
3.3 The Body Periphery Suffers as Well 93

Table 3.2 (continued)


Name Meaning
Pro-inflammatory situation in the fat Obesity is associated with more inflammation in the
tissue* fat tissue and this mild inflammation prevents the
adequate release of fatty acids from the fat tissue.
Pretty sure the inhibition of sympathetic influence
in the fat tissue is important. Since the sympathetic
nervous system promotes fat release, its inhibition is
obesity-promoting.
Behavior Reward* Food is considered a reward. The reward memory
plays an important role. Similar to addiction, this is
more pronounced in the obese.
Self-control* Lack of self-control and impulsiveness towards food
cues lead to increased food intake. This is associated
with increased sensitivity and emotionality towards
food cues. Change of executive functions (Table 2.4).
Overcompensation* In stressful life circumstances, too many calories are
consumed that exceed the expenses through the stress.
Here alcohol has a meaning (verified in men).
Cool types* In stress-inducing life circumstances, the stress axes
are only slightly activated in relaxed people. Somehow
they stay cool. This way, the stored energy reserves
cannot be accessed.
Ignorance* The calories consumed in excess are not recognized
and ignored. The lack of self-control leads to high
calorie intake. The corresponding episodic memory for
the relationship between food stimulus and “obesity
problem” does not work properly.
Socioeconomic status A lower socioeconomic status is associated with a
higher rate of obesity. Quite certainly, cheap, readily
available, heavily marketed, calorie-dense foods with
quickly digestible energetic substrates are important
(keywords: snacks, junk food, etc.).
Early traumatic experiences The events mentioned here are those in Table 2.1 and
2.2.
*Already mentioned in previous books. Table from (Straub 2020).

social housing, welfare recipient, and high population density in the apartment how their
children developed a higher body weight after 2 and especially after 5 years. These risk
factors determined 17% of the variation in the Body-Mass-Index (Explanation 7) or, in
other words, 17% of the causes of child obesity were explained by this stressful environ-
ment (Farewell et al. 2018). This is a very high value when you compare it with the 2.7%
that results from the 97 known genetic obesity factors (Table 3.2).
94 3 Consequences of Early Traumatic Experiences

Iowa is a US state that is regularly hit by devastating flood disasters, even though
the state is not located by the sea but in the middle of the continent (Iowa Flood Center
2021). The floods occur as a result of a unique river system between the Mississippi in
the east and the Missouri in the west in a densely populated country with wide, rela-
tively flat river valleys. The homepage of the Iowa Flood Centers impressively shows the
extent of flood disasters. In June 2008, there was a devastating flood, and the authors of
a study included 217 pregnant women who were affected by the floods. They recorded
objective measures of adversity and subjective measures of perceived stress, and they
noted the time during the pregnancy (i.e. first, middle, or last trimester).
If the mothers were burdened during the first trimester of pregnancy, the extent of the
burden predicted a higher increase in the body-mass-index of the child at 2.5 and 4 years
(Dancause et al. 2015). Regardless of the flood-related burden, the body-mass-index of
the mother or maternal nicotine abuse predicted the body weight of the children at both
assessment points. This sounds like a double hit, because pre-existing burden (maternal
factors) and prenatal trauma have to be present at the same time to have an effect. In a
follow-up study, the authors found cognitive problems and language difficulties in the
children if the mothers were burdened during pregnancy (Laplante et al. 2018).
Very similar results can be found in many studies: stress in the uterus or in early
childhood leads to later adiposity (Thomas et al. 2008; Entringer et al. 2010; Davis
et al. 2014; Tanenbaum et al. 2017; Wickrama et al. 2017; Robson et al. 2020), and the
early influence during pregnancy is more problematic than at later stages of pregnancy
(Li et al. 2010; Spencer 2013; Dancause et al. 2015). The children are thus set on life
courses that lead to a much higher weight level if we compare them with people without
previous adversity (Fig. 3.5). Furthermore, they develop the metabolic syndrome with
the four cardinal signs of obesity, hypertension, insulin resistance (insensitivity to insulin
in muscle, fat tissue and liver) and disturbed lipid metabolism (Delpierre et al. 2016).
These factors are the platform for the development of diabetes mellitus in susceptible
individuals (Campbell et al. 2018; Amemiya et al. 2019; Lown et al. 2019; Salas et al.
2019). When I write “susceptible”, this points to a second or even third factor that must
also be present (in the sense of the double hit). This could, for example, be a genetic
variant that promotes diabetes mellitus in old age. Similarly, a second traumatic experi-
ence in adulthood can stimulate this development (Farr et al. 2015). Women with previ-
ous childhood trauma are more likely to develop gestational diabetes (Schoenaker et al.
2019).
Early traumatic situations lead to similar problems in animals. In this respect, obesity
is well studied there—for example, as a result of separation from the mother (maternal
deprivation). The animals develop similar to humans a metabolic syndrome (Eller et al.
2020). Other researchers looked at prenatal influences and saw how stress in the mother
during pregnancy leads to increased body weight in the offspring (Mueller and Bale
2006). The offspring also showed altered eating behavior, which contributed to weight
gain (Lesage et al. 2004).
3.3 The Body Periphery Suffers as Well 95

34
32 The BMI development line
Obesity Line
passes through various
30 percentiles to always
Body mass index (kg/m2)

28 higher values.
Overweight line
26
24
22 Standard line
20
18
16

14

0 2 4 6 8 10 12 14 16 18 20
Age (years)

Fig. 3.5 Life courses of the Body-Mass-Index for boys (similar for girls). Three lines for normal, over-
weight and obese boys are shown. Furthermore, the red line shows an example of how the development
from an initially normal level over the years leads to an obese level (thick red line). The figure shows
a case with rapid weight development in an exemplary manner. The data for the normal line, the over-
weight line and the obesity line come from the “Contributions to Health Reporting of the Federal Repu-
blic of Germany” of the Robert Koch Institute from the year 2013, which are published there as a table
(Robert Koch Institute 2013)

When we ask about the causes of increased weight development, the items in
Table 3.2 come into consideration, and some factors stand out. Eating behavior changes
greatly (Table 3.2 under the heading “Behavior”). With regard to the genetic predispo-
sition, variants are involved that mainly have tasks in the central nervous system, which
also points to causes in the brain, such as eating behavior and similar. So I could have
dealt with obesity in the previous Sect. 3.2 because there are a large number of influenc-
ing brain factors.

3.3.1.1 Fat-Making Eating Behavior


People who have experienced previous traumatic events often have a fattening eating
behavior. In stressful life circumstances, too many calories are consumed (overcompen-
sation; Hitze et al. 2010), the wrong calories are consumed in the form of high-calorie
snacks (type chocolate bar) (Spencer 2013), the consumed calories are not perceived as
problematic and ignored, and in the absence of self-control, fattening foods are preferred
(summarized in: Straub 2018, 2020). Food can be considered a reward, and similar to
an addiction to alcohol, cigarettes or drugs, the eater rewards himself, and he switches
on similar brain areas that we have learned about in the other addictions (Spencer 2013)
(Fig. 3.1).
96 3 Consequences of Early Traumatic Experiences

A study found the phenomenon of “eating without hunger” in traumatized children,


which typically takes place after the regular meal—that is, in between two regular meals.
Likewise, the children show increased food intake when they are anxious, and anxiety
can be a consequence of child adversity (Sect. 3.2.3). The problem can increase with the
age of the affected children (Miller et al. 2018).
Other researchers demonstrated the susceptibility to so-called external stimuli such
as the smell and sight of food, as you experience them when you pass by a beautiful
display of a grocery store (van Dammen et al. 2019). The external stimuli are added to
the internal, bodily stimuli and increase the intake of highly palatable snacks outside of
regular meals. Some called this behavior “Junk Food Self Medication” (Hemmingsson
2018). All of these things are coupled with an inactive sedentary lifestyle (Wickrama
et al. 2017).
More recent studies show a positive effect of lifestyle change. In a longitudinal study,
the authors showed in almost 110 women with a Body Mass Index of 29 kg/m2 and more,
in comparison to a control group, how those who were exposed to early traumatic situ-
ations benefited particularly and had a 3.6 kg/m2 lower Body Mass Index after six years
(van Dammen et al. 2021).
Furthermore, bad childhood experiences are often associated with severe eating dis-
orders such as binge eating or addiction to eating (Schmidt et al. 1993; Vartanian et al.
2014; Su et al. 2016; Quilliot et al. 2019; Stojek et al. 2019), but not all researchers were
able to identify this relationship (Björkenstam et al. 2016). The eating disorders often
occur together with other addictions (Hodson et al. 2006).
Experiments with animals can help to characterise the affected areas in the brain
more closely, because animals under stress-filled conditions prefer junk food (Machado
et al. 2013). The neurotransmitters of the reward system such as dopamine and serotonin
appear more frequently in the considerations (de Souza et al. 2020; Tavares et al. 2020).
Finally, the HPA axis from Fig. 3.2 plays an important role (Spencer 2013). All of these
neurotransmitters and hormone systems target the regulation of eating in the hypothala-
mus, where we have a feeding center and a satiety center, both of which are involved in
food intake (Spencer 2013). The feeding center prevails over the satiety center. The scale
is deflected in the wrong direction. If the deflection is chronically fixed, that is, the basic
setting is in an unbalanced position, the correction will be difficult.

3.3.2 Heart Attack and Co.

The evacuation of children to rural areas in war is a problem because this separation and
the fear for the parents represent a severe trauma. This took place in several places dur-
ing the Second World War. I have mentioned the fate of Michael Rutter (Sect. 2.1.3) and
his sister (Michael Rutter, psychiatrist, 1933–2021). In Finland, as a result of the wars
with the Soviet Union during the Second World War, there was an evacuation of children
to other locations. After this, Finnish researchers included a very large number of chil-
dren born between 1934 and 1944 in their considerations (Eriksson et al. 2014).
3.3 The Body Periphery Suffers as Well 97

Of the people enclosed, 1781 people were evacuated to Sweden and Denmark. The
children were between 2.5 months and 10.6 years old at the time of separation, and the
duration of separation was between 18 days and 8 years. A comprehensive follow-up
study was carried out between 2001 and 2004. In this study, the typical mental disor-
ders such as depression and cognitive dysfunction were examined in addition to physi-
cal problems such as reduced athletic performance, hypertension and high cardiovascular
risk (Eriksson et al. 2014).
The rate of cardiovascular diseases and mortality was higher in evacuated children
than in children of the control group. If the children were young, they were more often
affected, and a long separation led to more problems. The risk of a cardiovascular
event was 2 times higher than in controls (Eriksson et al. 2014). In terms of a dose-re-
sponse effect, the risk of a heart attack depended on the number of childhood adversities
(Gilbert et al. 2015; Chou and Koenen 2019; Pierce et al. 2020). Interestingly, the prin-
ciple of double hit comes into play again here, as it is precisely the combined stress of
early traumatic experiences plus adverse life experiences in adulthood that increases the
risk of cardiovascular events (Halonen et al. 2015).
As early as the 2000s, New Zealand researchers pointed to another connection. They
showed the importance of socioeconomic status, which under unfavorable conditions
increases the risk of cardiovascular diseases (Poulton et al. 2002; O‘Rand and Hamil-
Luker 2005). Part of this may be due to higher blood pressure, which has been observed
again and again in these affected people and in other children/adolescents who have
been traumatized (Lehman et al. 2009; Stein et al. 2010; McIntyre et al. 2012; Su et al.
2015; Wickrama et al. 2015; Appleton et al. 2017; Murphy et al. 2017; Jakubowski et al.
2018). The increased blood pressure response occurs more often in men than in women
(Schreier et al. 2019). Early traumatic experiences are also more often associated with
kidney diseases up to kidney failure (Ozieh et al. 2020; Weldegiorgis et al. 2020). Poor
kidney function can promote hypertension.
Animals showed a higher blood pressure reaction to stress if they were exposed to
early traumatic experiences (Igosheva et al. 2007; Loria et al. 2010a, b, 2013; Alastalo
et al. 2013a). The animals showed an increased reaction of the adrenal gland with the
release of larger amounts of cortisol, the stress hormone, and the circadian rhythm of
heart rate was disturbed (Mastorci et al. 2009).
About the metabolic syndrome and diabetes mellitus as the cause of cardiovas-
cular problems, I have already spoken in the preceding chapter (e.g. Delpierre et al.
2016; Burgueno et al. 2020). The special activation of the sympathetic nervous system,
which can lead to high blood pressure through fluid retention and vasoconstriction, is
mentioned below (Sect. 3.3.5). A disturbed lipid metabolism, oxidative stress, physical
inactivity and a generally accelerated aging process are also discussed in the Sect. 3.3.5.
Furthermore, there is the increased general inflammatory state, which additionally stimu-
lates the inflammation-related arteriosclerosis of large and small vessels. I will address it
more specifically in the next main chapter.
98 3 Consequences of Early Traumatic Experiences

Finally, the American Heart Association has recognized the fact of “early traumatic
experiences leading to more heart attacks” and has begun to pay more attention to these
findings (Suglia et al. 2018).

3.3.3 Chronic Lung Diseases

We start with one of the largest studies on the subject, which was conducted in the USA.
Here, the researchers examined the relationships between early traumatic experiences on
the one hand and chronic obstructive lung disease or asthma on the other. The authors
included more than 100,000 people in their analysis and were able to determine how
smoking partly explained the aforementioned link to chronic obstructive lung disease
and asthma. In other words, the addiction, namely nicotine abuse, preceded or accom-
panied the lung problems. The more hardships were endured, the more often chronic
obstructive lung disease and asthma occurred (Waehrer et al. 2020) (Fig. 3.6). This large
study confirmed an earlier study from Sweden (Hjern et al. 1999).
But first let’s come to the stressful life situations during pregnancy. While in the
above study by Waehrer and colleagues smoking still contributed as a risk factor to
explain, it is different in the following situation. Japanese mothers with a stressful preg-
nancy had more children with asthma. In particular, maternal irritation and anger were
predictive factors for asthma. 10% of the women smoked, but it had no effect on the
results (Kawamoto et al. 2020). The development of asthma could be significantly atten-
uated by breastfeeding the infant if only one trauma was present. If the child had two or
more traumas, breastfeeding was no longer effective (Abarca et al. 2019).
If mothers were more heavily burdened during pregnancy and had experienced five
or more stress-inducing episodes, their children showed objectively measurable signs of
asthma at the age of seven, and boys were more affected than girls (Lee et al. 2017a).
The stronger burden on boys compared to girls was described by others (Bose et al.
2017; Rosa et al. 2018).
At the same time, a European working group carried out a meta-analysis and brought
together 30 studies on prenatal stress. The stressful burden of the mother and thus of the
child in the third trimester of pregnancy was more often associated with allergic rhinitis,
asthma and atopic dermatitis. Especially the previous loss of one’s own child or of the
partner were severe stress experiences that affected the lung problems of the later born
child (Flanigan et al. 2018), and these stressors were confirmed by others (Khashan et al.
2012; Liu et al. 2015).
And there we have it again, the double hit! If there was a traumatic burden on the
mother during pregnancy and at the same time a high nitrate load acted on mother and
child, the risk of asthma in the offspring was significantly higher, and it affected boys in
particular. The time window of the effect was in two ranges, between the 7th and 19th
week of pregnancy and the 33rd and 40th week (Bose et al. 2017). The combination of
traffic-related air pollution and stressful life situations during pregnancy and childhood
3.3 The Body Periphery Suffers as Well 99

25 Asthma 70 Smoking
Percentage of persons in relation to all COPD
60
20
persons included (%)

50

15
40

30
10

20

5
10

0 0
0 1 2–3 4 and 0 1 2–3 4 and
more more
Number of childhood traumatic experiences

Fig. 3.6 Dose-response relationship between stress level and lung disease. 109628 people were inclu-
ded, and they developed asthma and chronic obstructive lung disease (COPD) spontaneously or in
dependence on the number of hardships. The number of early traumas was closely associated with smo-
king. In all cases, a dose-response relationship is shown. The data come from the large epidemiological
study by Waehrer et al. and are given there in numerical form (Waehrer et al. 2020)

asthma was examined in meta-analysis and confirmed (Exley et al. 2015). So the envi-
ronment plays a role in programming that ultimately leads to asthma in the child.
Stress episodes are not only relevant during pregnancy, as they also act during child-
hood and adolescence (Waehrer et al. 2020 or Bhan et al. 2014). In a study from Norway,
the connection between early traumatic experiences and asthma or chronic obstructive
pulmonary disease was confirmed, whereby the influences of the parents’ smoking and
the parents’ asthma were taken into account and controlled or removed statistically
(Sheikh 2018).
Others recognized a connection between childhood adversities and the onset of
asthma in adulthood (Scott et al. 2008). If adolescents had not yet developed asthma
by the age of 16, previous stressful experiences promoted the development of asthma,
even though nicotine abuse was taken into account and controlled or removed statisti-
cally (Oren et al. 2017). Experiences of violence in adolescence also promote the devel-
opment of asthma, as described in a study of more than 6000 adolescents (McLaughlin
et al. 2016).
Animal models showed in the same way the connection between stress-inducing sit-
uations and later lung problems (Nogueira et al. 1999; Pincus-Knackstedt et al. 2006;
100 3 Consequences of Early Traumatic Experiences

Vig et al. 2010; Ouchi et al. 2018; Zazara et al. 2018). The increased inflammation in the
lungs was particularly in focus.
In humans, mild inflammation is relevant in the same way as in animal models with
regard to the connection between traumatic experiences and later asthma (Packard et al.
2011). This finally leads us to the possible causes of asthma and chronic obstructive
lung disease. In addition to inflammatory components, genetic factors, gender, smoking,
the shift of stress axes, a changed microbiome on the body surfaces and in the respira-
tory tract and chemical stressors from the environment are important (Rosa et al. 2018).
These things are particularly shown in the next main chapter.

3.3.4 The Stomach Aches and the Stool Consistency Bothers

Every field in medicine treats people with a higher pain sensitivity. While this is the case
for rheumatologists and orthopedic surgeons with patients with fibromyalgia, for urol-
ogists it is patients with chronic pain in the pelvic area or at the bladder/urinary tract
(Schrepf et al. 2018), for gastroenterologists it is patients with irritable bowel syndrome
and for dermatologists those with lipomatosis dolorosa (painful fatty tissue deposits on
the abdomen, knee, elbow and buttocks).
About 50% of patients visiting a gastroenterological specialist suffer from irritable
bowel syndrome. After excluding serious problems—such as bowel or stomach cancer,
chronic inflammatory bowel diseases, liver, biliary and pancreatic diseases, food aller-
gies, disorders of food absorption, bacterial overgrowth, motility disorders of the diges-
tive tract, autoimmune diseases and others—often only the diagnosis of irritable bowel
syndrome remains, which is largely harmless, but still unpleasant (Drossman 2016).
There is an irritable bowel syndrome if recurrent abdominal pain has existed for at
least once a week over the last three months and at the same time problems of defeca-
tion, stool frequency and stool consistency are present. These things can be accompanied
by flatulence. Detailed examinations and differentiations from other aspects of functional
gastrointestinal disorders are left to the specialists (Drossman 2016). At the last meet-
ing of the experts on functional gastrointestinal disorders in Rome in 2014, the biopsy-
chosocial model was extended and the importance of stress in early life was discussed
(Drossman 2016).
In any case, there is a clear connection between early traumatic experiences and irri-
table bowel syndrome, as I already mentioned in the introductory Chap. 1 (Berens et al.
2020). Others reported increased air swallowing in patients after previous stress exposure
in childhood and adolescence (Rajindrajith et al. 2018). These things affect the quality
of life of the patients (Biggs et al. 2004). The more adversity there is, the more intense
the irritable bowel problem (Park et al. 2016). Furthermore, the irritable bowel syndrome
is associated with depression and increased anxiety , because the affected people suf-
fer more frequently from both (Lee et al. 2017b). Women are more affected than men,
3.3 The Body Periphery Suffers as Well 101

and women have more pain, for which during the reproductive years estrogens can be
responsible (Straub 2007).
Higher pain sensitivity in irritable bowel syndrome has been studied in humans
and is closely related to depression and increased anxiety (Elsenbruch et al. 2010a).
Furthermore, these patients show a more extensive reaction in central areas of emotion
regulation, as determined by functional magnetic resonance imaging (Elsenbruch et al.
2010b). Furthermore, altered connections between various brain regions are present, and
this may be a central nervous cause of the high pain sensitivity (Icenhour et al. 2017).
An accompanying inflammation, wherever it may come from, exacerbates the higher
pain sensitivity and abdominal pain (Benson et al. 2012). It can, for example, be due to
a higher permeability of the intestinal wall to bacteria, as has been shown in irritable
bowel syndrome (Witt et al. 2019).
Animal experiments also make the increased visceral pain sensitivity very clear
(O’Mahony et al. 2009, 2017; Chaloner and Greenwood-Van Meerveld 2013; Pohl et al.
2015, 2017; Holschneider et al. 2016; Prusator and Greenwood-Van Meerveld 2017;
Wong et al. 2019). The amygdala is important because changes can be found there that
are associated with the increased sensitivity (Prusator and Greenwood-Van Meerveld
2017). Furthermore, early-stressed animals have increased inflammation when an addi-
tional inflammatory stimulus in the form of a double hit occurs (Veenema et al. 2008).
Another observation in animals after prenatal stress is the later change in the compo-
sition of the gut bacteria (Bailey et al. 2004; Golubeva et al. 2015; Gur et al. 2019). A
criterion for a poor bacterial composition is the reduced diversity of the various types of
bacteria (Moussaoui et al. 2017). Furthermore, there are more pathogenic and less regu-
latory gut bacteria, as a study of stressed mothers and children showed (Zijlmans et al.
2015) and animal experiments suggest (O‘Mahony et al. 2017). Fig. 3.7 summarizes the
situation.
Furthermore, the composition of the bacteria in the intestine affects the brain in
return. For example, the quality of sleep can be influenced by different bacterial compo-
sitions (Sen et al. 2021). At the same time, poor sleep affects the bacterial composition
of the intestine in return (Sen et al. 2021). The information between the gut lumen and
the brain is conveyed via afferent nerves of the vagus nerve, via inflammatory media-
tors from the gut wall via the bloodstream and via microbial metabolites that are recog-
nized by nerve fibers (Sen et al. 2021). A microbial-induced disturbance of the circadian
rhythm can be one of the causes of sleep disorders (Sen et al. 2021).

3.3.5 And When We Get Old?

Then other things are added or intensify. In my book about aging, the dramatic expe-
rience of the menopause of women and the andropause of men over the age of 50 is
reported. From this point on, the energy consumption changes impressively (Straub
2018). The energy consumption for physical activities decreases continuously and
102 3 Consequences of Early Traumatic Experiences

Genetics
Sex
Early life influences Psychosocial factors
Culture Life stress
Environment Personality
Trauma Psychology (anxiety, depression)
Infection Coping / Cognition / Resilience
Parental behaviour Physical activity
Social support

Physiology
Gastrointestinal movement
Genetics Pain sensitivity
Sex Modified bacteria
Nutrition
Inflammation
Intestinal wall permeability

Functional gastrointestinal
disorders
Genetics
e.g. irritable bowel syndrome etc.
Sex
Symptoms
Severity
Behavior

Fig. 3.7 Biopsychosocial model for functional gastrointestinal disorders. The model is based on the
summary by Drossman (Drossman 2016). The elements of the so-called bidirectional gut-brain axis are
schematically incorporated into this figure. Psychologists use the term coping to refer to the coping strat-
egy in dealing with stressful experiences

parallel to this, fat mass increases steadily (Speakman and Westerterp 2010). Under
these conditions, we should not be surprised that the Body-Mass-Index (explanation 7)
increases and follow-up problems develop.
Above I wrote: “In addition, they [the traumatized] develop the metabolic syndrome
with the four cardinal signs of obesity, hypertension, insulin resistance (insensitiv-
ity to insulin in muscle, fat tissue and liver) and disturbed lipid metabolism.” Animals
also develop a similar disease pattern after stressful life experiences (Eller et al. 2020).
Regardless of trauma, the metabolic syndrome is a problem of older age. It is accom-
panied by an increased activation of the sympathetic nervous system, while the activity
of the HPA axis remains roughly the same (Straub et al. 2001). However, the hormones
of the sympathetic nervous system (adrenaline, noradrenaline) and the HPA axis (corti-
sol) are relatively higher than many other hormones (for example, sex hormones). Higher
3.3 The Body Periphery Suffers as Well 103

sympathetic activity can contribute to hypertension, and high blood pressure has been
linked to early trauma (Alastalo et al. 2013b).
In addition, high blood levels of the hormones of the sympathetic nervous sys-
tem (adrenaline, noradrenaline) and the HPA axis (cortisol) promote insulin resistance,
in which insulin does not work sufficiently at the target cells of the liver, muscle and
fat tissue. During insulin resistance, the blood levels of glucose and free fatty acids are
increased because they are not taken up into the target tissue, and this contributes to fol-
low-up problems. For example, susceptible people can develop age-related diabetes. In
addition, the general inflammatory situation and oxidative stress increase slightly with
age, additional reasons for a deterioration of the situation.
As people age, adversities from their younger years worsen the situation. In addi-
tion to obesity, traumas also cause depression and anxiety disorders, sleep disorders and
increased pain, all of which can also increase with age. In my book on aging, I explained
how these things can rob energy as single, double, and triple hits, which can be espe-
cially problematic because this energy is missing for the desired activities of daily life
(Straub 2018). These activities include desired physical and mental work.
When these hits are combined with early trauma, the problems in old age are clearly
visible. For example, the Finnish study of children sent to other locations during the
war years showed that those individuals with hardships had poorer physical condition
and poorer functioning brains (von Bonsdorff et al. 2019). The researchers again see a
dose-response effect, because those with more hardships showed the physical and men-
tal deterioration more pronouncedly (Alastalo et al. 2013b). The affected persons had a
higher frailty (Haapanen et al. 2018) and their physical abilities were reduced (Anderson
et al. 2017). If symptoms of depression are added, the process of frailty is exacerbated
and aging is accelerated (Shrira and Litwin 2014).

3.3.5.1 Accelerated Aging
When we talk about accelerated aging, the length of telomeres is often used as a measure
of cell aging. What is that? The telomeres consist of repeating units of DNA at the end
of chromosomes (Fig. 3.8). If these repeating pieces become smaller over the course of
cell life, this is an indication of cell aging. Since almost all cells in all organisms age, the
shortened telomeres are a widely accepted phenomenon of cell aging (Epel et al. 2004).
The enzyme telomerase ensures the extension of telomeres and thus works against the
cellular aging process (Fig. 3.8). If the molecular biologist artificially introduces tel-
omerase into an otherwise telomerase-poor cell, the lifespan can be increased (Jaskelioff
et al. 2011).
But let’s get back to the topic of this book. The acceleration of the aging process
after early traumatic experiences was an important topic in the last two decades, and the
length of telomeres was taken into account for this (Blackburn and Epel 2012). If stress
experiences occur already during pregnancy, the telomeres are shortened in young adults
(Entringer et al. 2011). The more adversities occurred in early childhood, the shorter the
telomeres were in adolescence (Shalev et al. 2013; Puterman et al. 2016) and in later life
104 3 Consequences of Early Traumatic Experiences

Fig. 3.8 The telomere and the telomerase. On the left, the chromosome is shown in black, at the end
of which are the pink caps of the telomeres. Schematically, DNA (genetic material) is now shown in
the telomere area (the pink area). The resolution increases from left to right until the individual letters
(A, C, G, T) of the genetic code become visible on the right. The telomere consists of always the same
DNA sections, i.e. the sequence of the genetic code in the telomere repeats itself constantly. On the right
is the blue telomerase, which extends the end of the telomere. In this way, the total length of the telo-
mere remains as constant as possible. The telomerase attaches DNA pieces that correspond exactly to the
repeating code of the telomere

stages (Surtees et al. 2011; Savolainen et al. 2014; Mayer et al. 2019). These findings
were confirmed in animals (Schneper et al. 2016). The researchers typically looked at the
telomeres in the leukocytes present in the blood, and thus essentially take a look at the
aging of the immune cells.
In addition to the telomeres, the focus is on many other places where this acceler-
ated aging is recognized. Girls reach the menarche earlier and are thus more fertile ear-
lier (Belsky and Shalev 2016; Zhang et al. 2019). With functional magnetic resonance
imaging, an acceleration of brain maturation was observed (Herzberg and Gunnar 2020;
Miller et al. 2020). Electrical derivations of the brain on the skull surface (electroenceph-
alography) show the finding of accelerated aging in a technically different way (Hassan
et al. 2020). The consideration of epigenetic markers in blood cells is aimed at the same
phenomenon of faster aging (Fiorito et al. 2017; Austin et al. 2018; Lawn et al. 2018;
Pepper et al. 2018; Ridout et al. 2018; Sumner et al. 2019). Especially the DNA changes
were related to tumor diseases, and so the question arises of the higher frequency of can-
cer in old age in people after early traumas.

3.3.5.2 Cancer Disease
If I subsume cancer under the age-related diseases, it is due to the increased probability
of occurrence in old age. In addition, most tumors are based on acquired genetic cell
changes that are common in old age.
3.3 The Body Periphery Suffers as Well 105

Similar to work on depression and obesity, the groundbreaking work on cancer by


Vincent J. Felitti and a group of scientists from the Centers for Disease Control and
Prevention in Atlanta, USA, found that compared to controls, those with six or more
childhood adversities had three times the risk of lung cancer. When the tumor was diag-
nosed, the individuals with childhood adversities were, on average, 13 years younger
than those without adversities. So they experienced the cancer much earlier and had a
shorter life expectancy. Increased smoking among the traumatized individuals also con-
tributed to the stimulation of tumors (Brown et al. 2010).
According to a large British study that reported on a birth cohort from 1958 (Child
Development Study), those with two or more childhood adversities had twice the risk of
cancer before the age of 50. This result persisted after important known cancer risk fac-
tors such as smoking, alcohol, and obesity were included and controlled for in the analy-
sis (Kelly-Irving et al. 2013). Both studies from the USA and England suggest premature
aging because the tumors occur earlier.
In a comparative study between Finland and Japan, childhood adversities in Japan
and especially poverty were associated with an increased risk of cancer (Amemiya et al.
2019). When the researchers looked at typical cancer risk factors such as smoking, alco-
hol, cancer-causing agents from the environment, chronic inflammation, obesity, etc.,
these cancer-causing risk factors were significantly more common in people with previ-
ous trauma, especially for smoking, alcohol, and obesity (Ports et al. 2019).
In another large American study of 67,011 people, there was a relationship between
childhood adversities and cancer risk with a maximum increase in the risk factor of 1.35
(which is not so much). In this study, smoking, alcohol, and obesity had a measurable
impact on the aforementioned relationship (Waehrer et al. 2020).
When we now think of possible causes of the greater frequency of cancer, in addition
to smoking and alcohol, the immune system must be important. Why? In recent years,
we have recognized how important the immune system is for the body’s own and contin-
uous cancer fighting, which we do not experience consciously. This is shown to us by the
new therapies with the so-called checkpoint inhibitors. Immunological checkpoints are
central and important switches in immune cells that can inhibit the respective immune
cell in its activity (Fig. 3.9). The inhibition of this inhibition by means of checkpoint
drugs activates the tumor-fighting immune cells and represents an important cancer ther-
apy. For the discovery of the first checkpoints and the checkpoint inhibition, James P.
Allison, USA, and Tasuku Honjo, Japan, were awarded the Nobel Prize in Medicine for
2018.
Therapies that block checkpoint number 10 in Fig. 3.9 can fight tumors and trigger
autoimmune reactions at the same time. These works clearly show the special role of
the immune system in the development of cancer. If tumors now depend on the good
function of the immune system, a disorder of the immune system or a inhibition of the
same is more often associated with the development of tumors. Similar questions were
asked by the trauma researchers, and so they examined important immune cells that are
involved in tumor defense.
106 3 Consequences of Early Traumatic Experiences

Uptake of the tumor material in the tumor area

Nucleus

Phagocyte

tumor
fragments

Migration in the lymph nodes via lymphatic vessels

Presentation of tumor antigen and activation of T cells

T-cell
T-cell
receptor
Nucleus

Nucleus

antigen-
presenting cell

Clonal
expansion
CHECKPOINT

Nucleus Immigration into


the tumor tissue
Nucleus
3.3 The Body Periphery Suffers as Well 107


Fig. 3.9 Role of immunological checkpoints. Go through the figure from top to bottom using the num-
bers. 1) First, a phagocyte recognizes a piece of tumor with a green receptor while wandering in the
tumor tissue. 2) Then the green receptor and the tumor piece are taken up and stored in a vesicle. 3) Now
the tumor pieces are digested and 4) deposited on a red receptor. 5) This receptor with the small tumor
piece migrates to the surface of the cell and 6) is presented to the environment there. Now the prepared
cell migrates along the lymphatics into the lymph nodes. 7) There, the phagocyte—now called an anti-
gen-presenting cell—presents this tumor piece to an immunological T cell via the red receptor, which
randomly recognizes the tumor piece in the blue T cell receptor. 8) Additional receptors on the cell sur-
face of both cells are involved in this process (dotted red and blue line). These additional receptors are
called checkpoints because only if the two receptors match, the T cell is activated or inhibited. 9) If the T
cell is activated, it begins to multiply identically. It migrates back into the tumor tissue and meets tumor
cells that present the same tumor piece on their surface. 10) Then the checkpoints become important
again. If they are inhibitory checkpoints, the tumor can inhibit the activation of the T cells and the deadly
contents of the T cells cannot be released (11, red vesicles). If the experimenter blocks the checkpoint at
10) by means of suitable drugs, the T cell is activated and releases the contents that are deadly for tumor
cells

In addition to the T-cells and antigen-presenting cells mentioned in Fig. 3.9, other
important cells are so-called “natural killer cells”. If the investigator offers tumor mate-
rial to the killer cells in the experiment similar as in Fig. 3.9, they react approximately
like T-cells, in that they secrete deadly substances for tumor cells. However, killer cells
related to T-cells must first be activated. This activation is inhibited by stress factors such
as cortisol (HPA axis) and noradrenaline (sympathetic nervous system).
In patients with breast cancer, those women with previous childhood trauma more
often showed poor quality of life after tumor diagnosis, more fatigue and depressive
symptoms, and the activity of “natural killer cells” was reduced compared to a control
group without stressors (Witek Janusek et al. 2013). Such factors can be relevant for the
body’s own control of the tumor.
Another indication of a possible involvement of the immune system is the consider-
ation of antibodies against herpesviruses. The blood concentration of antibodies is high
when the body cannot control the herpesviruses and they are constantly present in the
body (Fagundes et al. 2013). This chronic reaction is recognizable by the serum concen-
tration of the antibodies, and this does not speak for the quality of the immunological
fight against the viruses by T-cells and their partners, the B-cells. Since the same T-cells
are important for cancer defense, a reduced function of these immune cells gives an indi-
rect indication of the lack of body’s own immunological cancer defense.
In a study of patients with breast cancer, a higher concentration of antibodies against
herpesviruses was now found in those women with childhood trauma compared to the
control group without adversity (Fagundes et al. 2013). The affected women had more
depressive symptoms and poorer sleep quality, both of which impair the HPA axis and/or
the sympathetic nervous system. These data speak for a reduced T-cell/B-cell function,
which can partly explain tumor development.
These relationships point to the involvement of the immune system, which can be
either suppressed or activated nonspecifically. The suppression refers more to the T-cells
108 3 Consequences of Early Traumatic Experiences

and killer cells, which play a protective role in viral infections and tumors. The unspe-
cific activation refers more to the B-cells and to the elements of the innate immune sys-
tem, which has important tasks in the acute bacterial, fungal and parasitic defense on the
surfaces of the body.

3.4 And Finally the Immune System is Activated

First indications of a connection between early childhood stress or socio-economic


adversity on the one hand and an increased inflammatory response on the other hand
were made in the CARDIA study (CARDIA = Coronary Artery Risk Development in
Young Adults) (Taylor et al. 2006). This study appeared eight years after the epidemio-
logical study by Vincent J. Felitti (Sect. 2.1.5), which showed the follow-up problems
after childhood stress (Felitti et al. 1998).
In this CARDIA study, people in the USA were systematically examined from the
mid-1980s to investigate the risk of coronary heart disease in young white and black peo-
ple. 3248 people aged 32 to 47 years were re-examined 15 years after the study began
and the C-reactive protein was determined in the serum. In addition, the researchers had
complete data sets of these people describing the socio-economic status in childhood and
childhood adversity (Taylor et al. 2006).
The factors of childhood stress had a direct effect on C-reactive protein and an indi-
rect effect on this inflammation marker via body weight. Children with traumatic experi-
ences showed disturbances in psychosocial coping, which was positively associated with
serum concentration of C-reactive protein. They were also overweight and this was also
associated with a higher C-reactive protein (Taylor et al. 2006). However, it should be
said right away: The C-reactive protein was within the normal range in almost all people
at less than 5.0 mg/l and the mean was 1.76 mg/l, because the investigators excluded
people with a serum value greater than 10 mg/l (it was assumed that an immunological/
infectious disease was present).
This epidemiological study was preceded by studies in animals that showed a rela-
tionship between prenatal stress and increased inflammation in later life (Klein and
Rager 1995; Vanbesien-Mailliot et al. 2007). Other experiments in animals showed
clear immunological changes in adulthood, which can be better described as inhibited or
altered immune response (Kay et al. 1998; Chisari et al. 2001; Coe et al. 2002; Llorente
et al. 2002; Tuchscherer et al. 2002; Fonseca et al. 2005).
Further work followed describing the connection between early traumatic events
or socio-economic status and higher inflammation in humans (Danese et al. 2007;
Carpenter et al. 2010; Slopen et al. 2010, 2015; Kiecolt-Glaser et al. 2011; Packard
et al. 2011; Cole et al. 2012; Pace et al. 2012; Hartwell et al. 2013; Witek Janusek
et al. 2013; Ehrlich et al. 2016; Elwenspoek et al. 2017; Janusek et al. 2017; Finy und
Christian 2018; Pinto Pereira et al. 2019; Rasmussen et al. 2019; Dieckmann et al. 2020;
Kuhlman et al. 2020; Kuzminskaite et al. 2020; Lacey et al. 2020). A later longitudinal
3.4 And Finally the Immune System is Activated 109

study showed a clear connection between early childhood trauma and later increased
levels of C-reactive protein (Slopen et al. 2013). Extensive meta-analyses summarized
the situation: There is a positive connection between childhood adversities and slightly
increased inflammation later in life. Factors such as body weight, concurrent depression,
smoking and others have an additional influence (Slopen et al. 2012; Tursich et al. 2014;
Baumeister et al. 2016; Muscatell et al. 2020).
Do people with early adversities have higher inflammation later in life because they
had more stress experiences in adulthood? One study looked at this question specifically
and came to the following conclusion. Childhood trauma and stress-inducing episodes
occurring shortly before the study slightly increased inflammation (Hostinar et al. 2015).
If both factors were present at the same time in the form of a double hit, inflammation
was higher than with just one factor alone. This theory of the double hit was confirmed
independently by other authors (Lin et al. 2016).
At this point, we ask how high the serum values of C-reactive protein or the often also
measured cytokine interleukin-6 actually rise. In fact, the increase in these two inflam-
mation values, as it results from the many studies mentioned, is not high. For C-reactive
protein, serum values up to a maximum of 10 mg/l and for interleukin-6 a maximum
of 10 pg/ml are achieved. Compared to a real autoimmune disease, these are relatively
low values (there for C-reactive protein: 100–200 mg/l and for IL-6: 100–1000 pg/ml),
and the lower serum values correspond approximately to a situation with a well-treated
chronic inflammatory disease. I take up this point of the possibly mild inflammation in
Chap. 5 in more detail.

3.4.1 Early Trauma and Autoimmunity

C-reactive protein and IL-6 discussed in the former subchapter are rather markers of the
innate immune system. With regard to the acquired immune system, the important question
arose as to whether autoimmune diseases also occur more frequently after childhood traumas.
First major publications date back to the 1970s, when a group of clinical scientists
in Rochester, New York, investigated links between early childhood stress and juvenile
idiopathic arthritis (the form of arthritis in children) (Henoch et al. 1978). Rochester
in New York began at that time under Bob Ader, Nicolas Cohen and David Felten to
become the first center for psychoneuroimmunological research (Ader 2007). Although
the modern classification of childhood arthritis and a questionnaire in the style of the
Felitti questionnaire (Table 2.1 or 2.2) were still missing at that time, the clinical picture
and the suffering of the children were well known, and so the researchers included 88
girls and boys with arthritis and almost 3000 control persons in their considerations.
A total of 28% of children with arthritis had a “broken family structure” due to
divorce, single parenting, or death of a parent, and in the comparison group only 11%
of children had a similar fate (Henoch et al. 1978). In addition, 9% of the children in the
arthritis group had been adopted before the onset of the disease, and in the control group
110 3 Consequences of Early Traumatic Experiences

it was only 3%. Both findings differed at a high statistical level (Henoch et al. 1978).
Further studies with similar statements followed afterwards (summarized until 2000 in:
Herrmann et al. 2000). In 2013, these findings were confirmed in almost 700 children
and about 1000 control persons: the risk of a childhood form of arthritis increased by
a factor of 2 to almost 7 (depending on the type of joint inflammation and the type of
childhood experience) if traumatic situations preceded (Neufeld et al. 2013).
The longitudinal epidemiological study on the adult form of arthritis only came onto
the market in 2004, which examined a connection between childhood or adolescent
trauma and arthritis in adulthood in more than 9000 Canadians (Kopec and Sayre 2004).
The authors relied on the Felitti questionnaire (Table 2.1), which must have been known
to them. People with arthritis more often had traumatic experiences in their younger
years, for example a longer hospital stay or episodes that were very anxiety-provoking
(Kopec and Sayre 2004). However, the authors did not explain which form of arthritis
was involved, and since there are very different types with more or less inflammation,
these data are incomplete.
For this question, similar epidemiological studies had to be carried out, since only
they could definitely uncover the connection. Here the Canadian work from the year
2004 took on a pioneer role. So a groundbreaking report appeared in 2009 by Shanta
Dube from the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention(CDC) in Atlanta, Georgia,
which I mentioned in Chap. 1. She used the data from Vincent J. Felitti and Robert Anda
from the late 1990s on more than 15,000 people (Dube et al. 2009), and she used the
Felitti-ACE questionnaire from Table 2.1. If two, three or more childhood traumas were
present, the risk of a chronic autoimmune disease was increased (Dube et al. 2009).
These findings were confirmed in a German study of patients with rheumatoid arthri-
tis, where the risk increased by a factor of 2.0 to 2.6 (Spitzer et al. 2013). Even for the
much less inflammatory osteoarthritis, researchers found a link between early adversities
and the onset of the disease (Fuller-Thomson et al. 2009).
Another autoimmune disease is systemic lupus erythematosus (SLE for short), which
is characterized by a colorful picture of disease manifestations, including kidney inflam-
mation, vasculitis, brain involvement, joint inflammation, blood cell depletion, and other
things. In the large American study of 67,516 nurses that began in 1989, 94 women had
SLE in 2015, and for those with physical and emotional abuse, the risk for SLE was 2.6
times higher (Feldman et al. 2019). The work on SLE patients was recently confirmed in
a group of black women in the USA (Cozier et al. 2020).
In the autoimmune disease of multiple sclerosis, which affects the central nerv-
ous system, this link between childhood stress experiences and disease could also be
observed (Spitzer et al. 2012). For psoriasis, confirmatory data are available in a small
group of patients (Crosta et al. 2018). In patients with autoimmune type-1 diabetes melli-
tus, in which the insulin-producing pancreatic cells are destroyed by autoimmune inflam-
mation, the link was confirmed by means of an epidemiological study in a very large
cohort (Bengtsson et al. 2021).
3.5 To the Point 111

These studies suggest a possible transition from an increased inflammatory state to


a chronic inflammatory disease that manifests as a chronic autoimmune disease. In the
next chapter, I try to explain which mechanisms in particular can contribute to this devel-
opment of chronic immune activation.

3.5 To the Point

• Early traumatic experiences primarily affect the brain. Nevertheless, they have an
effect on the periphery of the body and can cause various long-term problems there.
• The number of early experiences is directly linked to the severity of the problem.
• In the foreground are: alcohol and drug abuse, smoking, depression with/without
suicide, anxiety disorder, sleep disorder, increased pain, obesity, heart attack, lung
disease (e.g. asthma), gastrointestinal disorders (e.g. irritable bowel syndrome) and
chronic inflammation.
• With regard to the resulting problems of the brain, I spoke specifically about the
mechanisms of addiction, depression, anxiety, sleep disorder and increased pain sensi-
tivity, and I briefly touched on personality changes.
• In Table 3.1 further psycho- and neuropathological sequelae are reported. These
include schizophrenia, paranoid psychosis, bipolar disorder, autism spectrum disor-
der, dementia, fatigue, epilepsy and changed sexual orientation.
• Sleep problems and increased pain are treated separately; both are more common in
previous trauma. There is an increased sensitivity to pain. The central nervous system
components of pain processing are discussed.
• The interaction of the different brain regions is presented in an integrated approach,
and the image of the weighing scale is used to represent a horizontal pointer on the
scale as a normal situation. Childhood adversities can permanently change the pro-
gramming of the weighing scale. A trauma memory with a disturbed position of the
pointer of the weighing scale arises.
• The disturbed position of the weighing scale affects the function of the HPA axis,
the sympathetic nervous system, the parasympathetic nervous system, the descend-
ing inhibitory and facilitatory pain pathways (and thus the perception of pain), among
other things, in the periphery.
• With regard to the resulting problems in the periphery, I am dealing with a high body
weight (and disturbed eating behavior), heart attack, asthma and chronic obstructive
lung disease, irritable bowel syndrome, accelerated aging and the higher probability
of cancer.
• Last, the presence and extent of the slightly increased inflammation are illuminated.
Various autoimmune diseases are more common after early traumatic events. These
include the childhood form of arthritis (juvenile idiopathic arthritis), rheumatoid
arthritis of adults, systemic lupus erythematosus (SLE), multiple sclerosis, psoriasis
and autoimmune type-1-diabetes mellitus. Why this is so is explained in more detail
in the following text.
112 3 Consequences of Early Traumatic Experiences

References

Abarca NE, Garro AC, Pearlman DN (2019) Relationship between breastfeeding and asthma prev-
alence in young children exposed to adverse childhood experiences. J Asthma 56:142–151
Ader R (2007) Psychoneuroimmunology. Elsevier/Academic, San Diego
Afifi TO, Mather A, Boman J, Fleisher W, Enns MW, Macmillan H, Sareen J (2011) Childhood
adversity and personality disorders: results from a nationally representative population-based
study. J Psychiatr Res 45:814–822
Airagnes G, Lemogne C, Hoertel N, Goldberg M, Limosin F, Zins M (2016) Childhood adversity
and depressive symptoms following retirement in the Gazel cohort. J Psychiatr Res 82:80–90
Ajnakina O, Trotta A, Forti MD et al (2018) Different types of childhood adversity and 5-year out-
comes in a longitudinal cohort of first-episode psychosis patients. Psychiatry Res 269:199–206
Alastalo H, Räikkönen K, Pesonen AK, Osmond C, Barker DJ, Heinonen K, Kajantie E, Eriksson
JG (2013a) Early life stress and blood pressure levels in late adulthood. J Hum Hypertens
27:90–94
Alastalo H, von Bonsdorff MB, Räikkönen K, Pesonen AK, Osmond C, Barker DJ, Heinonen K,
Kajantie E, Eriksson JG (2013b) Early life stress and physical and psychosocial functioning in
late adulthood. PLoS One 8:e69011
Ali I, O’Brien P, Kumar G et al (2013) Enduring effects of early life stress on firing patterns of
hippocampal and thalamocortical neurons in rats: implications for limbic epilepsy. PLoS One
8:e66962
Alkon A, Boyce WT, Neilands TB, Eskenazi B (2017) Children’s autonomic nervous system reac-
tivity moderates the relations between family adversity and sleep problems in latino 5-year olds
in the CHAMACOS study. Front Public Health 5:155
Alvarez P, Green PG, Levine JD (2013) Stress in the adult rat exacerbates muscle pain induced by
early-life stress. Biol Psychiatry 74:688–695
Amemiya A, Fujiwara T, Shirai K, Kondo K, Oksanen T, Pentti J, Vahtera J (2019) Association
between adverse childhood experiences and adult diseases in older adults: a comparative
cross-sectional study in Japan and Finland. BMJ Open 9:e024609
Anderson EL, Heron J, Ben-Shlomo Y, Kuh D, Cooper R, Lawlor DA, Fraser A, Howe LD (2017)
Adversity in childhood and measures of aging in midlife: findings from a cohort of british
women. Psychol Aging 32:521–530
Angelakis I, Gillespie EL, Panagioti M (2019) Childhood maltreatment and adult suicidality: a
comprehensive systematic review with meta-analysis. Psychol Med 49:1057–1078
Appleton AA, Holdsworth E, Ryan M, Tracy M (2017) Measuring childhood adversity in life
course cardiovascular research: a systematic review. Psychosom Med 79:434–440
April-Sanders A, Duarte CS, Wang S, McGlinchey E, Alcántara C, Bird H, Canino G, Suglia SF
(2020) Childhood adversity and sleep disturbances: longitudinal results in Puerto Rican chil-
dren. Int J Behav Med 28:107–115
Atlas LY, al’Absi M (2018) The neuroscience of pain: biobehavioral, developmental, and psycho-
social mechanisms relevant to intervention targets. Psychosom Med 80:788–790
Austin MK, Chen E, Ross KM, McEwen LM, Maclsaac JL, Kobor MS, Miller GE (2018) Early-
life socioeconomic disadvantage, not current, predicts accelerated epigenetic aging of mono-
cytes. Psychoneuroendocrinology 97:131–134
Bae D, Wickrama KA, O’Neal CW (2014) Social consequences of early socioeconomic adversity
and youth BMI trajectories: gender and race/ethnicity differences. J Adolesc 37:883–892
Bailey MT, Lubach GR, Coe CL (2004) Prenatal stress alters bacterial colonization of the gut in
infant monkeys. J Pediatr Gastroenterol Nutr 38:414–421
References 113

Barrett B (2009) The impact of childhood sexual abuse and other forms of childhood adversity on
adulthood parenting. J Child Sex Abus 18:489–512
Bartley M, Kelly Y, Sacker A (2012) Early life financial adversity and respiratory function in
midlife: a prospective birth cohort study. Am J Epidemiol 175:33–42
Bath KG (2020) Synthesizing views to understand sex differences in response to early life adver-
sity. Trends Neurosci 43:300–310
Baumeister D, Akhtar R, Ciufolini S, Pariante CM, Mondelli V (2016) Childhood trauma and
adulthood inflammation: a meta-analysis of peripheral C-reactive protein, interleukin-6 and
tumour necrosis factor-α. Mol Psychiatry 21:642–649
Beach SRH, Ong ML, Lei MK et al (2020) Childhood adversity is linked to adult health among
African Americans via adolescent weight gain and effects are genetically moderated. Dev
Psychopathol 6:1–18
Beck JE, Shaw DS (2005) The influence of perinatal complications and environmental adversity on
boys’ antisocial behavior. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 46:35–46
Belsky J, Shalev I (2016) Contextual adversity, telomere erosion, pubertal development, and
health: two models of accelerated aging, or one? Dev Psychopathol 28:1367–1383
Bengtsson J, Rieckmann A, Carstensen B, Svensson J, Jørgensen ME, Rod NH (2021) Trajectories
of childhood adversity and type 1 diabetes: a nationwide study of one million children.
Diabetes Care 44:740–747
Benson S, Kattoor J, Wegner A et al (2012) Acute experimental endotoxemia induces visceral
hypersensitivity and altered pain evaluation in healthy humans. Pain 153:794–799
Berens S, Banzhaf P, Baumeister D, Gauss A, Eich W, Schaefert R, Tesarz J (2020) Relationship
between adverse childhood experiences and illness anxiety in irritable bowel syndrome – the
impact of gender. J Psychosom Res 128:109846
Berg KL, Shiu CS, Acharya K, Stolbach BC, Msall ME (2016) Disparities in adversity among
children with autism spectrum disorder: a population-based study. Dev Med Child Neurol
58:1124–1131
Berufsverbände und Fachgesellschaften für Psychiatrie; Kinder- und Jugendpsychiatrie;
Psychotherapie; Psychosomatik; Nervenheilkunde und Neurologie aus Deutschland und der
Schweiz. Persönlichkeitsstörungen. 2021. https://www.neurologen-und-psychiater-im-netz.
org/psychiatrie-psychosomatik-psychotherapie/erkrankungen/persoenlichkeitsstoerungen/
was-sind-persoenlichkeitsstoerungen/. Accessed 13 Jan 2021
Bhan N, Glymour MM, Kawachi I, Subramanian SV (2014) Childhood adversity and asthma prev-
alence: evidence from 10 US states (2009–2011). BMJ Open Respir Res 1:e000016
Biggs AM, Aziz Q, Tomenson B, Creed F (2004) Effect of childhood adversity on health related
quality of life in patients with upper abdominal or chest pain. Gut 53:180–186
Björkenstam C, Kosidou K, Björkenstam E (2017a) Childhood adversity and risk of suicide:
cohort study of 548 721 adolescents and young adults in Sweden. BMJ 357:j1334
Björkenstam E, Burström B, Vinnerljung B, Kosidou K (2016) Childhood adversity and psychiat-
ric disorder in young adulthood: an analysis of 107,704 Swedes. J Psychiatr Res 77:67–75
Björkenstam E, Ekselius L, Burström B, Kosidou K, Björkenstam C (2017b) Association between
childhood adversity and a diagnosis of personality disorder in young adulthood: a cohort study
of 107,287 individuals in Stockholm County. Eur J Epidemiol 32:721–731
Björkenstam E, Björkenstam C, Jablonska B, Kosidou K (2018) Cumulative exposure to childhood
adversity, and treated attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a cohort study of 543 650 adoles-
cents and young adults in Sweden. Psychol Med 48:498–507
Blackburn EH, Epel ES (2012) Telomeres and adversity: too toxic to ignore. Nature 490:169–171
114 3 Consequences of Early Traumatic Experiences

Bock J, Breuer S, Poeggel G, Braun K (2017) Early life stress induces attention-deficit hyperactiv-
ity disorder (ADHD)-like behavioral and brain metabolic dysfunctions: functional imaging of
methylphenidate treatment in a novel rodent model. Brain Struct Funct 222:765–780
Bose S, Chiu YM, Hsu HL et al (2017) Prenatal nitrate exposure and childhood asthma. Influence
of maternal prenatal stress and fetal sex. Am J Respir Crit Care Med 196:1396–1403
Bower JE, Crosswell AD, Slavich GM (2014) Childhood adversity and cumulative life stress: risk
factors for cancer-related fatigue. Clin Psychol Sci 2:108–115
Bowman RE, MacLusky NJ, Sarmiento Y, Frankfurt M, Gordon M, Luine VN (2004) Sexually
dimorphic effects of prenatal stress on cognition, hormonal responses, and central neurotrans-
mitters. Endocrinology 145:3778–3787
Brannigan R, Tanskanen A, Huttunen MO, Cannon M, Leacy FP, Clarke MC (2020) The role
of prenatal stress as a pathway to personality disorder: longitudinal birth cohort study. Br J
Psychiatry 216:85–89
Brenhouse HC, Lukkes JL, Andersen SL (2013) Early life adversity alters the developmental pro-
files of addiction-related prefrontal cortex circuitry. Brain Sci 3:143–158
Bromis K, Calem M, Reinders A, Williams SCR, Kempton MJ (2018) meta-analysis of 89 struc-
tural MRI studies in posttraumatic stress disorder and comparison with major depressive disor-
der. Am J Psychiatry 175:989–998
Brown DW, Anda RF, Felitti VJ, Edwards VJ, Malarcher AM, Croft JB, Giles WH (2010) Adverse
childhood experiences are associated with the risk of lung cancer: a prospective cohort study.
BMC Public Health 10:20
Brunson KL, Eghbal-Ahmadi M, Bender R, Chen Y, Baram TZ (2001) Long-term, progressive hip-
pocampal cell loss and dysfunction induced by early-life administration of corticotropin-releas-
ing hormone reproduce the effects of early-life stress. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 98:8856–8861
Brunson KL, Kramár E, Lin B, Chen Y, Colgin LL, Yanagihara TK, Lynch G, Baram TZ (2005)
Mechanisms of late-onset cognitive decline after early-life stress. J Neurosci 25:9328–9338
Buitelaar JK, Huizink AC, Mulder EJ, de Medina PG, Visser GH (2003) Prenatal stress and cogni-
tive development and temperament in infants. Neurobiol Aging 24:S53–S60
Burgueno AL, Juarez YR, Genaro AM, Tellechea ML (2020) Systematic review and meta-analysis
on the relationship between prenatal stress and metabolic syndrome intermediate phenotypes.
Int J Obes 44:1–12
Calem M, Bromis K, McGuire P, Morgan C, Kempton MJ (2017) meta-analysis of associations
between childhood adversity and hippocampus and amygdala volume in non-clinical and gen-
eral population samples. Neuroimage Clin 14:471–479
Campbell JA, Farmer GC, Nguyen-Rodriguez S, Walker R, Egede L (2018) Relationship between
individual categories of adverse childhood experience and diabetes in adulthood in a sample of
US adults: does it differ by gender? J Diabetes Complicat 32:139–143
Carballedo A, Lisiecka D, Fagan A, Saleh K, Ferguson Y, Connolly G, Meaney J, Frodl T (2012)
Early life adversity is associated with brain changes in subjects at family risk for depression.
World J Biol Psychiatry 13:569–578
Carpenter LL, Gawuga CE, Tyrka AR, Lee JK, Anderson GM, Price LH (2010) Association
between plasma IL-6 response to acute stress and early-life adversity in healthy adults.
Neuropsychopharmacology 35:2617–2623
Carr CP, Martins CM, Stingel AM, Lemgruber VB, Juruena MF (2013) The role of early life stress
in adult psychiatric disorders: a systematic review according to childhood trauma subtypes. J
Nerv Ment Dis 201:1007–1020
Chaloner A, Greenwood-Van Meerveld B (2013) Early life adversity as a risk factor for visceral
pain in later life: importance of sex differences. Front Neurosci 7:13
References 115

Chen A, Panter-Brick C, Hadfield K, Dajani R, Hamoudi A, Sheridan M (2019) Minds under siege:
cognitive signatures of poverty and trauma in refugee and non-refugee adolescents. Child Dev
90:1856–1865
Cheval B, Chabert C, Sieber S et al (2019) Association between adverse childhood experiences
and muscle strength in older age. Gerontology 65:474–484
Chisari AN, Giovambattista A, Perelló M, Gaillard RC, Spinedi ES (2001) Maternal under-
nutrition induces neuroendocrine immune dysfunction in male pups at weaning.
Neuroimmunomodulation 9:41–48
Cho HJ, Bower JE, Kiefe CI, Seeman TE, Irwin MR (2012) Early life stress and inflammatory
mechanisms of fatigue in the Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults (CARDIA)
study. Brain Behav Immun 26:859–865
Chou PH, Koenen KC (2019) Associations between childhood maltreatment and risk of myocar-
dial infarction in adulthood: results from the national epidemiologic survey on alcohol and
related conditions. J Psychiatr Res 116:172–177
Cisler JM, James GA, Tripathi S, Mletzko T, Heim C, Hu XP, Mayberg HS, Nemeroff CB, Kilts
CD (2013) Differential functional connectivity within an emotion regulation neural network
among individuals resilient and susceptible to the depressogenic effects of early life stress.
Psychol Med 43:507–518
Clark C, Caldwell T, Power C, Stansfeld SA (2010) Does the influence of childhood adversity on
psychopathology persist across the lifecourse? A 45-year prospective epidemiologic study. Ann
Epidemiol 20:385–394
Clarke-Walper K, Riviere LA, Wilk JE (2014) Alcohol misuse, alcohol-related risky behaviors,
and childhood adversity among soldiers who returned from Iraq or Afghanistan. Addict Behav
39:414–419
Coe CL, Kramer M, Kirschbaum C, Netter P, Fuchs E (2002) Prenatal stress diminishes the
cytokine response of leukocytes to endotoxin stimulation in juvenile rhesus monkeys. J Clin
Endocrinol Metab 87:675–681
Cole SW, Conti G, Arevalo JM, Ruggiero AM, Heckman JJ, Suomi SJ (2012) Transcriptional mod-
ulation of the developing immune system by early life social adversity. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S
A 109:20578–20583
Cozier YC, Barbhaiya M, Castro-Webb N, Conte C, Tedeschi S, Leatherwood C, Costenbader KH,
Rosenberg L (2020) Association of child abuse with systemic lupus erythematosus in black
women during adulthood. Arthritis Care Res 13:24188
Crawley E, Hughes R, Northstone K, Tilling K, Emond A, Sterne JA (2012) Chronic disabling
fatigue at age 13 and association with family adversity. Pediatrics 130:e71–e79
Crosta ML, De Simone C, Di Pietro S et al (2018) Childhood trauma and resilience in psoriatic
patients: a preliminary report. J Psychosom Res 106:25–28
Dahl SK, Larsen JT, Petersen L, Ubbesen MB, Mortensen PB, Munk-Olsen T, Musliner KL (2017)
Early adversity and risk for moderate to severe unipolar depressive disorder in adolescence and
adulthood: a register-based study of 978,647 individuals. J Affect Disord 214:122–129
Dancause KN, Laplante DP, Hart KJ, O’Hara MW, Elgbeili G, Brunet A, King S (2015) Prenatal
stress due to a natural disaster predicts adiposity in childhood: the Iowa Flood Study. J Obes
2015:570541
Danese A, Pariante CM, Caspi A, Taylor A, Poulton R (2007) Childhood maltreatment predicts
adult inflammation in a life-course study. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 104:1319–1324
Danielewicz J, Hess G (2014) Early life stress alters synaptic modification range in the rat lateral
amygdala. Behav Brain Res 265:32–37
116 3 Consequences of Early Traumatic Experiences

Davis CR, Dearing E, Usher N et al (2014) Detailed assessments of childhood adversity enhance
prediction of central obesity independent of gender, race, adult psychosocial risk and health
behaviors. Metabolism 63:199–206
Davis DA, Luecken LJ, Zautra AJ (2005) Are reports of childhood abuse related to the experience
of chronic pain in adulthood? A meta-analytic review of the literature. Clin J Pain 21:398–405
Davis EP, Pfaff D (2014) Sexually dimorphic responses to early adversity: implications for affec-
tive problems and autism spectrum disorder. Psychoneuroendocrinology 49:11–25
Davis L, Barnes AJ, Gross AC, Ryder JR, Shlafer RJ (2019) Adverse childhood experiences and
weight status among adolescents. J Pediatr 204:71–76.e71
Delpierre C, Fantin R, Barboza-Solis C, Lepage B, Darnaudéry M, Kelly-Irving M (2016) The
early life nutritional environment and early life stress as potential pathways towards the met-
abolic syndrome in mid-life? A lifecourse analysis using the 1958 British Birth cohort. BMC
Public Health 16:815
Derks NA, Krugers HJ, Hoogenraad CC, Joëls M, Sarabdjitsingh RA (2016) Effects of early life
stress on synaptic plasticity in the developing hippocampus of male and female rats. PLoS One
11:e0164551
Deutsche Gesellschaft für Suizidprävention. Bundesweite Beratungsangebote bei Krisen. 2021.
https://www.suizidprophylaxe.de/hilfsangebote/adressen/. Accessed 13 Jan 2021
DeWit DJ, Chandler-Coutts M, Offord DR, King G, McDougall J, Specht J, Stewart S (2005)
Gender differences in the effects of family adversity on the risk of onset of DSM-III-R social
phobia. J Anxiety Disord 19:479–502
Dieckmann L, Cole S, Kumsta R (2020) Stress genomics revisited: gene co-expression analysis
identifies molecular signatures associated with childhood adversity. Transl Psychiatry 10:34
Donley GAR, Lönnroos E, Tuomainen TP, Kauhanen J (2018) Association of childhood stress with
late-life dementia and Alzheimer’s disease: the KIHD study. Eur J Pub Health 28:1069–1073
Drossman DA (2016) Functional gastrointestinal disorders: history, pathophysiology, clinical fea-
tures and Rome IV. Gastroenterology 150:1262–1279
Dubé CM, Molet J, Singh-Taylor A, Ivy A, Maras PM, Baram TZ (2015) Hyper-excitability and
epilepsy generated by chronic early-life stress. Neurobiol Stress 2:10–19
Dube SR, Anda RF, Felitti VJ, Chapman DP, Williamson DF, Giles WH (2001) Childhood abuse,
household dysfunction, and the risk of attempted suicide throughout the life span: findings from
the Adverse Childhood Experiences Study. JAMA 286:3089–3096
Dube SR, Fairweather D, Pearson WS, Felitti VJ, Anda RF, Croft JB (2009) Cumulative childhood
stress and autoimmune diseases in adults. Psychosom Med 71:243–250
Edwards HE, Dortok D, Tam J, Won D, Burnham WM (2002) Prenatal stress alters seizure thresh-
olds and the development of kindled seizures in infant and adult rats. Horm Behav 42:437–447
Ehrlich KB, Ross KM, Chen E, Miller GE (2016) Testing the biological embedding hypothesis:
is early life adversity associated with a later proinflammatory phenotype? Dev Psychopathol
28:1273–1283
Eiland L, McEwen BS (2012) Early life stress followed by subsequent adult chronic stress potenti-
ates anxiety and blunts hippocampal structural remodeling. Hippocampus 22:82–91
Eller OC, Morris EM, Thyfault JP, Christianson JA (2020) Early life stress reduces voluntary
exercise and its prevention of diet-induced obesity and metabolic dysfunction in mice. Physiol
Behav 223:113000
Ellis L, Cole-Harding S (2001) The effects of prenatal stress, and of prenatal alcohol and nicotine
exposure, on human sexual orientation. Physiol Behav 74:213–226
Elsenbruch S, Rosenberger C, Bingel U, Forsting M, Schedlowski M, Gizewski ER (2010a)
Patients with irritable bowel syndrome have altered emotional modulation of neural responses
to visceral stimuli. Gastroenterology 139:1310–1319
References 117

Elsenbruch S, Rosenberger C, Enck P, Forsting M, Schedlowski M, Gizewski ER (2010b)


Affective disturbances modulate the neural processing of visceral pain stimuli in irritable bowel
syndrome: an fMRI study. Gut 59:489–495
El-Sheikh M, Keiley M, Bagley EJ, Chen E (2015) Socioeconomic adversity and women’s sleep:
stress and chaos as mediators. Behav Sleep Med 13:506–523
Elwenspoek MMC, Kuehn A, Muller CP, Turner JD (2017) The effects of early life adversity on
the immune system. Psychoneuroendocrinology 82:140–154
Entringer S, Buss C, Wadhwa PD (2010) Prenatal stress and developmental programming of
human health and disease risk: concepts and integration of empirical findings. Curr Opin
Endocrinol Diabetes Obes 17:507–516
Entringer S, Epel ES, Kumsta R, Lin J, Hellhammer DH, Blackburn EH, Wüst S, Wadhwa PD
(2011) Stress exposure in intrauterine life is associated with shorter telomere length in young
adulthood. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 108:E513–E518
Epel ES, Blackburn EH, Lin J, Dhabhar FS, Adler NE, Morrow JD, Cawthon RM (2004)
Accelerated telomere shortening in response to life stress. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A
101:17312–17315
Eriksson M, Räikkönen K, Eriksson JG (2014) Early life stress and later health outcomes – find-
ings from the Helsinki Birth Cohort Study. Am J Hum Biol 26:111–116
Evans EA, Grella CE, Upchurch DM (2017) Gender differences in the effects of childhood adver-
sity on alcohol, drug, and polysubstance-related disorders. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol
52:901–912
Exley D, Norman A, Hyland M (2015) Adverse childhood experience and asthma onset: a system-
atic review. Eur Respir Rev 24:299–305
Fagundes CP, Glaser R, Malarkey WB, Kiecolt-Glaser JK (2013) Childhood adversity and herpes-
virus latency in breast cancer survivors. Health Psychol 32:337–344
Farewell CV, Thayer ZM, Tracer DP, Morton S (2018) Prenatal stress exposure and early child-
hood BMI: exploring associations in a New Zealand context. Am J Hum Biol 30:e23116
Farr OM, Ko BJ, Joung KE et al (2015) Posttraumatic stress disorder, alone or additively with
early life adversity, is associated with obesity and cardiometabolic risk. Nutr Metab Cardiovasc
Dis 25:479–488
Favaro A, Tenconi E, Degortes D, Manara R, Santonastaso P (2015) Neural correlates of prenatal
stress in young women. Psychol Med 45:2533–2543
Feldman CH, Malspeis S, Leatherwood C, Kubzansky L, Costenbader KH, Roberts AL (2019)
Association of childhood abuse with incident systemic lupus erythematosus in adulthood in a
longitudinal cohort of women. J Rheumatol 46:1589–1596
Felitti VJ, Anda RF, Nordenberg D, Williamson DF, Spitz AM, Edwards V, Koss MP, Marks JS
(1998) Relationship of childhood abuse and household dysfunction to many of the leading
causes of death in adults. The Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACE) study. Am J Prev Med
14:245–258
Finy MS, Christian LM (2018) Pathways linking childhood abuse history and current socioeco-
nomic status to inflammation during pregnancy. Brain Behav Immun 74:231–240
Fiorito G, Polidoro S, Dugué PA et al (2017) Social adversity and epigenetic aging: a multi-cohort
study on socioeconomic differences in peripheral blood DNA methylation. Sci Rep 7:16266
Flanigan C, Sheikh A, DunnGalvin A, Brew BK, Almqvist C, Nwaru BI (2018) Prenatal mater-
nal psychosocial stress and offspring’s asthma and allergic disease: a systematic review and
meta-analysis. Clin Exp Allergy 48:403–414
Fonseca ES, Sakai M, Carvalho-Freitas MI, Palermo NJ (2005) Naloxone treatment prevents pre-
natal stress effects on peritoneal macrophage activity in mice offspring. Neuroendocrinology
81:322–328
118 3 Consequences of Early Traumatic Experiences

Ford E, Clark C, Stansfeld SA (2011) The influence of childhood adversity on social relations and
mental health at mid-life. J Affect Disord 133:320–327
Fride E, Dan Y, Feldon J, Halevy G, Weinstock M (1986) Effects of prenatal stress on vulnerability
to stress in prepubertal and adult rats. Physiol Behav 37:681–687
Frodl T, Carballedo A, Fagan AJ, Lisiecka D, Ferguson Y, Meaney JF (2012) Effects of ear-
ly-life adversity on white matter diffusivity changes in patients at risk for major depression. J
Psychiatry Neurosci 37:37–45
Frodl T, Skokauskas N, Frey EM, Morris D, Gill M, Carballedo A (2014) BDNF Val66Met geno-
type interacts with childhood adversity and influences the formation of hippocampal subfields.
Hum Brain Mapp 35:5776–5783
Fuller-Thomson E, Stefanyk M, Brennenstuhl S (2009) The robust association between childhood
physical abuse and osteoarthritis in adulthood: findings from a representative community sam-
ple. Arthritis Rheum 61:1554–1562
Gilbert LK, Breiding MJ, Merrick MT, Thompson WW, Ford DC, Dhingra SS, Parks SE (2015)
Childhood adversity and adult chronic disease: an update from ten states and the District of
Columbia, 2010. Am J Prev Med 48:345–349
Goff B, Gee DG, Telzer EH, Humphreys KL, Gabard-Durnam L, Flannery J, Tottenham N (2013)
Reduced nucleus accumbens reactivity and adolescent depression following early-life stress.
Neuroscience 249:129–138
Goldney RD (1981) Parental loss and reported childhood stress in young women who attempt sui-
cide. Acta Psychiatr Scand 64:34–47
Golubeva AV, Crampton S, Desbonnet L et al (2015) Prenatal stress-induced alterations in
major physiological systems correlate with gut microbiota composition in adulthood.
Psychoneuroendocrinology 60:58–74
Goodman JB, Freeman EE, Chalmers KA (2019) The relationship between early life stress and
working memory in adulthood: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Memory 27:868–880
Graham AM, Pfeifer JH, Fisher PA, Carpenter S, Fair DA (2015) Early life stress is associated
with default system integrity and emotionality during infancy. J Child Psychol Psychiatry
56:1212–1222
Gray JP, Müller VI, Eickhoff SB, Fox PT (2020) Multimodal abnormalities of brain structure
and function in major depressive disorder: a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Am J
Psychiatry 177:422–434
Green MK, Rani CS, Joshi A, Soto-Piña AE, Martinez PA, Frazer A, Strong R, Morilak DA (2011)
Prenatal stress induces long term stress vulnerability, compromising stress response sys-
tems in the brain and impairing extinction of conditioned fear after adult stress. Neuroscience
192:438–451
Grigoryan G, Segal M (2013) Prenatal stress alters noradrenergic modulation of LTP in hippocam-
pal slices. J Neurophysiol 110:279–285
Groenewald CB, Murray CB, Palermo TM (2020) Adverse childhood experiences and chronic pain
among children and adolescents in the United States. Pain Rep 5:e839
Gur TL, Palkar AV, Rajasekera T, Allen J, Niraula A, Godbout J, Bailey MT (2019) Prenatal stress
disrupts social behavior, cortical neurobiology and commensal microbes in adult male off-
spring. Behav Brain Res 359:886–894
Haapanen MJ, Perälä MM, Salonen MK et al (2018) Early life stress and frailty in old age: the
Helsinki birth cohort study. BMC Geriatr 18:179
Hackman DA, Farah MJ, Meaney MJ (2010) Socioeconomic status and the brain: mechanistic
insights from human and animal research. Nat Rev Neurosci 11:651–659
References 119

Halonen JI, Stenholm S, Pentti J, Kawachi I, Subramanian SV, Kivimäki M, Vahtera J (2015)
Childhood psychosocial adversity and adult neighborhood disadvantage as predictors of cardio-
vascular disease: a cohort study. Circulation 132:371–379
Harrington M, Robinson J, Bolton SL, Sareen J, Bolton J (2011) A longitudinal study of risk fac-
tors for incident drug use in adults: findings from a representative sample of the US population.
Can J Psychiatr 56:686–695
Hartwell KJ, Moran-Santa Maria MM, Twal WO, Shaftman S, DeSantis SM, McRae-Clark AL,
Brady KT (2013) Association of elevated cytokines with childhood adversity in a sample of
healthy adults. J Psychiatr Res 47:604–610
Hassan R, MacMillan HL, Saigal S, Schmidt LA (2020) Brain, interrupted: alpha/delta EEG ratio
in survivors of pre- and post-natal adversity. Int J Neurosci 4:1–7
Hausknecht K, Haj-Dahmane S, Shen RY (2013) Prenatal stress exposure increases the excitation
of dopamine neurons in the ventral tegmental area and alters their reponses to psychostimu-
lants. Neuropsychopharmacology 38:293–301
Hemmingsson E (2018) Early childhood obesity risk factors: socioeconomic adversity, family dys-
function, offspring distress, and junk food self-medication. Curr Obes Rep 7:204–209
Henoch MJ, Batson JW, Baum J (1978) Psychosocial factors in juvenile rheumatoid arthritis.
Arthritis Rheum 21:229–233
Herrmann M, Schölmerich J, Straub RH (2000) Stress and rheumatic diseases. Rheum Dis Clin N
Am 26:737–763
Herzberg MP, Gunnar MR (2020) Early life stress and brain function: activity and connectivity
associated with processing emotion and reward. NeuroImage 209:116493
Hines M, Johnston KJ, Golombok S, Rust J, Stevens M, Golding J (2002) Prenatal stress and gen-
der role behavior in girls and boys: a longitudinal, population study. Horm Behav 42:126–134
Hitze B, Hubold C, van Dyken R, Schlichting K, Lehnert H, Entringer S, Peters A (2010) How the
selfish brain organizes its supply and demand. Front Neuroenerg 2:7–17
Hjern A, Haglund B, Bremberg S, Ringbäck-Weitoft G (1999) Social adversity, migration and hos-
pital admissions for childhood asthma in Sweden. Acta Paediatr 88:1107–1112
Hodson C, Newcomb MD, Locke TF, Goodyear RK (2006) Childhood adversity, poly-substance
use, and disordered eating in adolescent Latinas: mediated and indirect paths in a community
sample. Child Abuse Negl 30:1017–1036
Holschneider DP, Guo Y, Mayer EA, Wang Z (2016) Early life stress elicits visceral hyperalgesia
and functional reorganization of pain circuits in adult rats. Neurobiol Stress 3:8–22
Hope S, Micali N, Deighton J, Law C (2019) Maternal mental health at 5 years and childhood
overweight or obesity at 11 years: evidence from the UK Millennium Cohort Study. Int J Obes
43:43–52
Hostinar CE, Stellern SA, Schaefer C, Carlson SM, Gunnar MR (2012) Associations between
early life adversity and executive function in children adopted internationally from orphanages.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 109(Suppl 2):17208–17212
Hostinar CE, Lachman ME, Mroczek DK, Seeman TE, Miller GE (2015) Additive contributions of
childhood adversity and recent stressors to inflammation at midlife: findings from the MIDUS
study. Dev Psychol 51:1630–1644
Hsu YT, Kawachi I (2019) Timing of family adversity during adolescence and its impact on alco-
hol and tobacco initiation: a longitudinal study among Taiwanese adolescents. Child Psychiatry
Hum Dev 50:257–267
Humphreys KL, King LS, Sacchet MD, Camacho MC, Colich NL, Ordaz SJ, Ho TC, Gotlib IH
(2019) Evidence for a sensitive period in the effects of early life stress on hippocampal volume.
Dev Sci 22:e12775
120 3 Consequences of Early Traumatic Experiences

Iakunchykova OP, Andreeva TI, Nordstrom DL, Shkiryak-Nizhnyk ZA, Antipkin YG, Hryhorczuk
DO, Zvinchuk AV, Chislovska NV (2015) The impact of early life stress on risk of tobacco
smoking initiation by adolescents. Addict Behav 50:222–228
Icenhour A, Witt ST, Elsenbruch S, Lowén M, Engström M, Tillisch K, Mayer EA, Walter S
(2017) Brain functional connectivity is associated with visceral sensitivity in women with
Irritable Bowel Syndrome. Neuroimage Clin 15:449–457
Igosheva N, Taylor PD, Poston L, Glover V (2007) Prenatal stress in the rat results in increased
blood pressure responsiveness to stress and enhanced arterial reactivity to neuropeptide Y in
adulthood. J Physiol 582:665–674
Jakubowski KP, Cundiff JM, Matthews KA (2018) Cumulative childhood adversity and adult car-
diometabolic disease: a meta-analysis. Health Psychol 37:701–715
Janusek LW, Tell D, Gaylord-Harden N, Mathews HL (2017) Relationship of childhood adver-
sity and neighborhood violence to a proinflammatory phenotype in emerging adult African
American men: an epigenetic link. Brain Behav Immun 60:126–135
Jaskelioff M, Muller FL, Paik JH et al (2011) Telomerase reactivation reverses tissue degeneration
in aged telomerase-deficient mice. Nature 469:102–106
Jensen SK, Dickie EW, Schwartz DH, Evans CJ, Dumontheil I, Paus T, Barker ED (2015) Effect of
early adversity and childhood internalizing symptoms on brain structure in young men. JAMA
Pediatr 169:938–946
John-Henderson NA, Williams SE, Brindle RC, Ginty AT (2018) Changes in sleep quality and lev-
els of psychological distress during the adaptation to university: the role of childhood adversity.
Br J Psychol 109:694–707
Jones GT (2016) Psychosocial vulnerability and early life adversity as risk factors for central sen-
sitivity syndromes. Curr Rheumatol Rev 12:140–153
Jones NC, O’Brien TJ, Carmant L (2014) Interaction between sex and early-life stress: influence
on epileptogenesis and epilepsy comorbidities. Neurobiol Dis 72:233–241
Kaiser RH, Clegg R, Goer F, Pechtel P, Beltzer M, Vitaliano G, Olson DP, Teicher MH, Pizzagalli
DA (2018) Childhood stress, grown-up brain networks: corticolimbic correlates of threat-re-
lated early life stress and adult stress response. Psychol Med 48:1157–1166
Kawamoto T, Miyake Y, Tanaka K, Nagano J, Sasaki S, Hirota Y (2020) Maternal prenatal stress
and infantile wheeze and asthma: the Osaka Maternal and Child Health Study. J Psychosom
Res 135:110143
Kay G, Tarcic N, Poltyrev T, Weinstock M (1998) Prenatal stress depresses immune function in
rats. Physiol Behav 63:397–402
Kelly-Irving M, Lepage B, Dedieu D et al (2013) Childhood adversity as a risk for cancer: findings
from the 1958 British birth cohort study. BMC Public Health 13:767
Kessler RC, Pecora PJ, Williams J et al (2008) Effects of enhanced foster care on the long-term
physical and mental health of foster care alumni. Arch Gen Psychiatry 65:625–633
Khashan AS, Wicks S, Dalman C, Henriksen TB, Li J, Mortensen PB, Kenny LC (2012) Prenatal
stress and risk of asthma hospitalization in the offspring: a Swedish population-based study.
Psychosom Med 74:635–641
Kiecolt-Glaser JK, Gouin JP, Weng NP, Malarkey WB, Beversdorf DQ, Glaser R (2011) Childhood
adversity and personality disorders: results from a nationally representative population-based
study. Psychosom Med 73:16–22
Kiff CJ, Cortes R, Lengua L, Kosterman R, Hawkins JD, Mason WA (2012) Effects of timing of
adversity on adolescent and young adult adjustment. J Res Adolesc 22:284–300
Kim MJ, Farber MJ, Knodt AR, Hariri AR (2019) Corticolimbic circuit structure moderates an
association between early life stress and later trait anxiety. Neuroimage Clin 24:102050
References 121

Kim ST, Hwang SS, Kim HW, Hwang EH, Cho J, Kang JI, Kim SJ (2018) Multidimensional
impulsivity as a mediator of early life stress and alcohol dependence. Sci Rep 8:4104
Kinney DK, Munir KM, Crowley DJ, Miller AM (2008) Prenatal stress and risk for autism.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev 32:1519–1532
Kircanski K, Sisk LM, Ho TC, Humphreys KL, King LS, Colich NL, Ordaz SJ, Gotlib IH (2019)
Early life stress, cortisol, frontolimbic connectivity, and depressive symptoms during puberty.
Dev Psychopathol 31:1011–1022
Klein SL, Rager DR (1995) Prenatal stress alters immune function in the offspring of rats. Dev
Psychobiol 28:321–336
Kleinhaus K, Harlap S, Perrin M, Manor O, Margalit-Calderon R, Opler M, Friedlander Y,
Malaspina D (2013) Prenatal stress and affective disorders in a population birth cohort. Bipolar
Disord 15:92–99
Kopec JA, Sayre EC (2004) Traumatic experiences in childhood and the risk of arthritis: a prospec-
tive cohort study. Can J Public Health 95:361–365
Kraaijenvanger EJ, Pollok TM, Monninger M, Kaiser A, Brandeis D, Banaschewski T, Holz
NE (2020) Impact of early life adversities on human brain functioning: a coordinate-based
meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 113:62–76
Kraszpulski M, Dickerson PA, Salm AK (2006) Prenatal stress affects the developmental trajectory
of the rat amygdala. Stress 9:85–95
Kuhlman KR, Horn SR, Chiang JJ, Bower JE (2020) Early life adversity exposure and circulating
markers of inflammation in children and adolescents: a systematic review and meta-analysis.
Brain Behav Immun 86:30–42
Kuhn M, Scharfenort R, Schümann D et al (2016) Mismatch or allostatic load? Timing of life
adversity differentially shapes gray matter volume and anxious temperament. Soc Cogn Affect
Neurosci 11:537–547
Kuzminskaite E, Vinkers CH, Elzinga BM, Wardenaar KJ, Giltay EJ, Penninx B (2020) Childhood
trauma and dysregulation of multiple biological stress systems in adulthood: results from
the Netherlands Study of Depression and Anxiety (NESDA). Psychoneuroendocrinology
121:104835
Lacey RE, Pinto Pereira SM, Li L, Danese A (2020) Adverse childhood experiences and adult
inflammation: single adversity, cumulative risk and latent class approaches. Brain Behav
Immun 87:820–830
Lajud N, Torner L (2015) Early life stress and hippocampal neurogenesis in the neonate: sexual
dimorphism, long term consequences and possible mediators. Front Mol Neurosci 8:3
Laloux C, Mairesse J, Van Camp G et al (2012) Anxiety-like behaviour and associated neu-
rochemical and endocrinological alterations in male pups exposed to prenatal stress.
Psychoneuroendocrinology 37:1646–1658
Lambert HK, Sheridan MA, Sambrook KA, Rosen ML, Askren MK, McLaughlin KA (2017)
Hippocampal contribution to context encoding across development is disrupted following ear-
ly-life adversity. J Neurosci 37:1925–1934
Lane RD, Anderson FS, Smith R (2018) Biased competition favoring physical over emotional
pain: a possible explanation for the link between early adversity and chronic pain. Psychosom
Med 80:880–890
Lange I, Goossens L, Bakker J et al (2019) Neurobehavioural mechanisms of threat generalization
moderate the link between childhood maltreatment and psychopathology in emerging adult-
hood. J Psychiatry Neurosci 44:185–194
Laplante DP, Brunet A, Schmitz N, Ciampi A, King S (2008) Project Ice Storm: prenatal maternal
stress affects cognitive and linguistic functioning in 5 1/2-year-old children. J Am Acad Child
Adolesc Psychiatry 47:1063–1072
122 3 Consequences of Early Traumatic Experiences

Laplante DP, Hart KJ, O’Hara MW, Brunet A, King S (2018) Prenatal maternal stress is associated
with toddler cognitive functioning: the Iowa Flood Study. Early Hum Dev 116:84–92
Lawn RB, Anderson EL, Suderman M et al (2018) Psychosocial adversity and socioeconomic
position during childhood and epigenetic age: analysis of two prospective cohort studies. Hum
Mol Genet 27:1301–1308
Lee AG, Chiu YM, Rosa MJ, Cohen S, Coull BA, Wright RO, Morgan WJ, Wright RJ (2017a)
Association of prenatal and early childhood stress with reduced lung function in 7-year-olds.
Ann Allergy Asthma Immunol 119:153–159
Lee C, Doo E, Choi JM, Jang SH, Ryu HS, Lee JY, Oh JH, Park JH, Kim YS (2017b) The
increased level of depression and anxiety in irritable bowel syndrome patients compared with
healthy controls: systematic review and meta-analysis. J Neurogastroenterol Motil 23:349–362
Lehman BJ, Taylor SE, Kiefe CI, Seeman TE (2009) Relationship of early life stress and psycho-
logical functioning to blood pressure in the CARDIA study. Health Psychol 28:338–346
Lemaire V, Koehl M, Le Moal M, Abrous DN (2000) Prenatal stress produces learning deficits
associated with an inhibition of neurogenesis in the hippocampus. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A
97:11032–11037
Lemche E (2018) Early life stress and epigenetics in late-onset Alzheimer’s dementia: a systematic
review. Curr Genomics 19:522–602
Lesage J, Del-Favero F, Leonhardt M, Louvart H, Maccari S, Vieau D, Darnaudery M (2004)
Prenatal stress induces intrauterine growth restriction and programmes glucose intolerance and
feeding behaviour disturbances in the aged rat. J Endocrinol 181:291–296
Lewis CR, Bastle RM, Manning TB et al (2016) Interactions between early life stress, nucleus
accumbens MeCP2 expression, and methamphetamine self-administration in male rats.
Neuropsychopharmacology 41:2851–2861
Li J, Olsen J, Vestergaard M, Obel C, Baker JL, Sørensen TI (2010) Prenatal stress exposure
related to maternal bereavement and risk of childhood overweight. PLoS One 5:e11896
Li N, Wang Y, Zhao X et al (2015) Long-term effect of early-life stress from earthquake exposure
on working memory in adulthood. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat 11:2959–2965
Lin JE, Neylan TC, Epel E, O’Donovan A (2016) Associations of childhood adversity and adult-
hood trauma with C-reactive protein: a cross-sectional population-based study. Brain Behav
Immun 53:105–112
Liu X, Olsen J, Agerbo E, Yuan W, Sigsgaard T, Li J (2015) Prenatal stress and childhood asthma
in the offspring: role of age at onset. Eur J Pub Health 25:1042–1046
Llorente E, Brito ML, Machado P, González MC (2002) Effect of prenatal stress on the hormo-
nal response to acute and chronic stress and on immune parameters in the offspring. J Physiol
Biochem 58:143–149
Longden E, Read J (2016) Social adversity in the etiology of psychosis: a review of the evidence.
Am J Psychother 70:5–33
Loria AS, D’Angelo G, Pollock DM, Pollock JS (2010a) Early life stress downregulates endothelin
receptor expression and enhances acute stress-mediated blood pressure responses in adult rats.
Am J Phys Regul Integr Comp Phys 299:R185–R191
Loria AS, Pollock DM, Pollock JS (2010b) Early life stress sensitizes rats to angiotensin
II-induced hypertension and vascular inflammation in adult life. Hypertension 55:494–499
Loria AS, Brands MW, Pollock DM, Pollock JS (2013) Early life stress sensitizes the renal and
systemic sympathetic system in rats. Am J Physiol Ren Physiol 305:F390–F395
Loudermilk E, Loudermilk K, Obenauer J, Quinn MA (2018) Impact of adverse childhood experi-
ences (ACEs) on adult alcohol consumption behaviors. Child Abuse Negl 86:368–374
Lovallo WR, Acheson A, Cohoon AJ, Sorocco KH, Vincent AS, Hodgkinson CA, Goldman
D (2019) Working memory reflects vulnerability to early life adversity as a risk factor for
References 123

substance use disorder in the FKBP5 cortisol cochaperone polymorphism, rs9296158. PLoS
One 14:e0218212
Low LA, Schweinhardt P (2012) Current research trends in early life stress and depression: review
of human studies on sensitive periods, gene-environment interactions, and epigenetics. Pain Res
Treat 2012:140832
Lown EA, Lui CK, Karriker-Jaffe K, Mulia N, Williams E, Ye Y, Li L, Greenfield TK, Kerr WC
(2019) Adverse childhood events and risk of diabetes onset in the 1979 National longitudinal
survey of youth cohort. BMC Public Health 19:1007
Luby JL, Tillman R, Barch DM (2019) Association of timing of adverse childhood experiences
and caregiver support with regionally specific brain development in adolescents. JAMA Netw
Open 2:e1911426
Machado TD, Dalle Molle R, Laureano DP, Portella AK, Werlang IC, Benetti Cda S, Noschang C,
Silveira PP (2013) Early life stress is associated with anxiety, increased stress responsivity and
preference for „comfort foods“ in adult female rats. Stress 16:549–556
MacKinnon N, Kingsbury M, Mahedy L, Evans J, Colman I (2018) The association between pre-
natal stress and externalizing symptoms in childhood: evidence from the avon longitudinal
study of parents and children. Biol Psychiatry 83:100–108
Mahamat-Saleh Y, Fiolet T, Rebeaud ME et al (2021) Diabetes, hypertension, body mass index,
smoking and COVID-19-related mortality: a systematic review and meta-analysis of observa-
tional studies. BMJ Open 11:e052777
Martínez-Téllez RI, Hernández-Torres E, Gamboa C, Flores G (2009) Prenatal stress alters spine
density and dendritic length of nucleus accumbens and hippocampus neurons in rat offspring.
Synapse 63:794–804
Mastorci F, Vicentini M, Viltart O et al (2009) Long-term effects of prenatal stress: changes in
adult cardiovascular regulation and sensitivity to stress. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 33:191–203
Matheson SL, Shepherd AM, Pinchbeck RM, Laurens KR, Carr VJ (2013) Childhood adversity in
schizophrenia: a systematic meta-analysis. Psychol Med 43:225–238
Mathur A, Graham-Engeland JE, Slavish DC, Smyth JM, Lipton RB, Katz MJ, Sliwinski MJ
(2018) Recalled early life adversity and pain: the role of mood, sleep, optimism, and control. J
Behav Med 41:504–515
Mayer SE, Prather AA, Puterman E, Lin J, Arenander J, Coccia M, Shields GS, Slavich GM, Epel
ES (2019) Cumulative lifetime stress exposure and leukocyte telomere length attrition: the
unique role of stressor duration and exposure timing. Psychoneuroendocrinology 104:210–218
McIntyre RS, Soczynska JK, Liauw SS et al (2012) The association between childhood adversity
and components of metabolic syndrome in adults with mood disorders: results from the interna-
tional mood disorders collaborative project. Int J Psychiatry Med 43:165–177
McLaughlin KA, Hatzenbuehler ML, Xuan Z, Conron KJ (2012) Disproportionate exposure to
early-life adversity and sexual orientation disparities in psychiatric morbidity. Child Abuse
Negl 36:645–655
McLaughlin KA, Basu A, Walsh K, Slopen N, Sumner JA, Koenen KC, Keyes KM (2016)
Childhood exposure to violence and chronic physical conditions in a national sample of US
adolescents. Psychosom Med 78:1072–1083
McWhorter KL, Parks CG, D’Aloisio AA, Rojo-Wissar DM, Sandler DP, Jackson CL (2019)
Traumatic childhood experiences and multiple dimensions of poor sleep among adult women.
Sleep 42:zsz108
Meek LR, Schulz KM, Keith CA (2006) Effects of prenatal stress on sexual partner preference in
mice. Physiol Behav 89:133–138
124 3 Consequences of Early Traumatic Experiences

Middlebrook N, Rushton AB, Abichandani D, Kuithan P, Heneghan NR, Falla D (2021) Measures
of central sensitization and their measurement properties in musculoskeletal trauma: a system-
atic review. Eur J Pain 25:71–87
Miller AL, Gearhardt AN, Retzloff L, Sturza J, Kaciroti N, Lumeng JC (2018) Early childhood
stress and child age predict longitudinal increases in obesogenic eating among low-income
children. Acad Pediatr 18:685–691
Miller JG, Ho TC, Humphreys KL, King LS, Foland-Ross LC, Colich NL, Ordaz SJ, Lin J, Gotlib
IH (2020) Early life stress, frontoamygdala connectivity, and biological aging in adolescence: a
longitudinal investigation. Cereb Cortex 30:4269–4280
Miranda A (2009) Early life stress and pain: an important link to functional bowel disorders.
Pediatr Ann 38:279–282
Mittal C, Griskevicius V, Simpson JA, Sung S, Young ES (2015) Cognitive adaptations to stressful
environments: when childhood adversity enhances adult executive function. J Pers Soc Psychol
109:604–621
Moloney RD, Sajjad J, Foley T, Felice VD, Dinan TG, Cryan JF, O’Mahony SM (2016) Estrous
cycle influences excitatory amino acid transport and visceral pain sensitivity in the rat: effects
of early-life stress. Biol Sex Differ 7:33
Morgan CP, Bale TL (2011) Early prenatal stress epigenetically programs dysmasculinization in
second-generation offspring via the paternal lineage. J Neurosci 31:11748–11755
Moussaoui N, Jacobs JP, Larauche M, Biraud M, Million M, Mayer E, Taché Y (2017) Chronic
early-life stress in rat pups alters basal corticosterone, intestinal permeability, and fecal micro-
biota at weaning: influence of sex. J Neurogastroenterol Motil 23:135–143
Mueller BR, Bale TL (2006) Impact of prenatal stress on long term body weight is dependent on
timing and maternal sensitivity. Physiol Behav 88:605–614
Murphy MO, Cohn DM, Loria AS (2017) Developmental origins of cardiovascular disease: impact
of early life stress in humans and rodents. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 74:453–465
Murthy S, Gould E (2020) How early life adversity influences defensive circuitry. Trends Neurosci
43:200–212
Muscatell KA, Brosso SN, Humphreys KL (2020) Socioeconomic status and inflammation: a
meta-analysis. Mol Psychiatry 25:2189–2199
Nanni V, Uher R, Danese A (2012) Childhood maltreatment predicts unfavorable course of illness
and treatment outcome in depression: a meta-analysis. Am J Psychiatry 169:141–151
Navratilova E, Atcherley CW, Porreca F (2015) Brain circuits encoding reward from pain relief.
Trends Neurosci 38:741–750
Nelson S, Simons LE, Logan D (2018) The incidence of adverse childhood experiences (ACEs)
and their association with pain-related and psychosocial impairment in youth with chronic pain.
Clin J Pain 34:402–408
Nettle D (2009) Personality – what makes you the way you are. Oxford University Press, Oxford
Neufeld KM, Karunanayake CP, Maenz LY, Rosenberg AM (2013) Stressful life events antedating
chronic childhood arthritis. J Rheumatol 40:1756–1765
Nielsen T, Carr M, Picard-Deland C, Marquis LP, Saint-Onge K, Blanchette-Carrière C, Paquette T
(2019) Early childhood adversity associations with nightmare severity and sleep spindles. Sleep
Med 56:57–65
Nogovitsyn N, Addington J, Souza R et al (2020) Childhood trauma and amygdala nuclei volumes
in youth at risk for mental illness. Psychol Med 17:1–8
Nogueira PJ, Ferreira HH, Antunes E, Teixeira NA (1999) Chronic mild prenatal stress exacerbates
the allergen-induced airway inflammation in rats. Mediat Inflamm 8:119–122
References 125

Norton MC, Smith KR, Østbye T et al (2011) Early parental death and remarriage of widowed
parents as risk factors for Alzheimer disease: the Cache County study. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry
19:814–824
O’Mahony SM, Marchesi JR, Scully P, Codling C, Ceolho AM, Quigley EM, Cryan JF, Dinan TG
(2009) Early life stress alters behavior, immunity, and microbiota in rats: implications for irrita-
ble bowel syndrome and psychiatric illnesses. Biol Psychiatry 65:263–267
O’Mahony SM, Clarke G, Dinan TG, Cryan JF (2017) Early-life adversity and brain development:
is the microbiome a missing piece of the puzzle? Neuroscience 342:37–54
O’Neill S, O’Connor RC (2020) Suicide in Northern Ireland: epidemiology, risk factors, and pre-
vention. Lancet Psychiatry 7:538–546
O’Rand AM, Hamil-Luker J (2005) Processes of cumulative adversity: childhood disadvantage
and increased risk of heart attack across the life course. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci
60:117–124
Opendak M, Gould E, Sullivan R (2017) Early life adversity during the infant sensitive period for
attachment: programming of behavioral neurobiology of threat processing and social behavior.
Dev Cogn Neurosci 25:145–159
Ordway MR, Sadler LS, Jeon S, O’Connell M, Banasiak N, Fenick AM, Crowley AA, Canapari
C, Redeker NS (2020) Sleep health in young children living with socioeconomic adversity. Res
Nurs Health 43:329–340
Oren E, Gerald L, Stern DA, Martinez FD, Wright AL (2017) Self-reported stressful life events
during adolescence and subsequent asthma: a longitudinal study. J Allergy Clin Immunol Pract
5:427–434
Østergaard SD, Larsen JT, Dalsgaard S, Wilens TE, Mortensen PB, Agerbo E, Mors O, Petersen
L (2016) Predicting ADHD by assessment of Rutter’s indicators of adversity in infancy. PLoS
One 11:e0157352
Oswald LM, Wand GS, Kuwabara H, Wong DF, Zhu S, Brasic JR (2014) History of childhood
adversity is positively associated with ventral striatal dopamine responses to amphetamine.
Psychopharmacology 231:2417–2433
Otten R, Mun CJ, Shaw DS, Wilson MN, Dishion TJ (2019) A developmental cascade model for
early adolescent-onset substance use: the role of early childhood stress. Addiction 114:326–334
Ouchi R, Kawano T, Yoshida H, Ishii M, Miyasaka T, Ohkawara Y, Takayanagi M, Takahashi
T, Ohno I (2018) Maternal separation as early-life stress causes enhanced allergic airway
responses by inhibiting respiratory tolerance in mice. Tohoku J Exp Med 246:155–165
Ozieh MN, Garacci E, Campbell JA, Walker RJ, Egede LE (2020) Adverse childhood experiences
and decreased renal function: impact on all-cause mortality in U.S. adults. Am J Prev Med
59:e49–e57
Pace TW, Wingenfeld K, Schmidt I, Meinlschmidt G, Hellhammer DH, Heim CM (2012)
Increased peripheral NF-κB pathway activity in women with childhood abuse-related posttrau-
matic stress disorder. Brain Behav Immun 26:13–17
Packard CJ, Bezlyak V, McLean JS et al (2011) Early life socioeconomic adversity is associ-
ated in adult life with chronic inflammation, carotid atherosclerosis, poorer lung function and
decreased cognitive performance: a cross-sectional, population-based study. BMC Public
Health 11:42
Palmier-Claus JE, Berry K, Bucci S, Mansell W, Varese F (2016) Relationship between childhood
adversity and bipolar affective disorder: systematic review and meta-analysis. Br J Psychiatry
209:454–459
Park SH, Videlock EJ, Shih W, Presson AP, Mayer EA, Chang L (2016) Adverse childhood expe-
riences are associated with irritable bowel syndrome and gastrointestinal symptom severity.
Neurogastroenterol Motil 28:1252–1260
126 3 Consequences of Early Traumatic Experiences

Pastor V, Antonelli MC, Pallarés ME (2017) Unravelling the link between prenatal stress, dopa-
mine and substance use disorder. Neurotox Res 31:169–186
Pastor V, Pallarés ME, Antonelli MC (2018) Prenatal stress increases adult vulnerability to cocaine
reward without affecting pubertal anxiety or novelty response. Behav Brain Res 339:186–194
Peh OH, Rapisarda A, Lee J (2019) Childhood adversities in people at ultra-high risk (UHR) for
psychosis: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol Med 49:1089–1101
Pepper GV, Bateson M, Nettle D (2018) Telomeres as integrative markers of exposure to stress and
adversity: a systematic review and meta-analysis. R Soc Open Sci 5:180744
Perry RE, Blair C, Sullivan RM (2017) Neurobiology of infant attachment: attachment despite
adversity and parental programming of emotionality. Curr Opin Psychol 17:1–6
Persson G, Skoog I (1996) A prospective population study of psychological risk factors for late
onset dementia. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 11:15–22
Pesonen AK, Eriksson JG, Heinonen K et al (2013) Cognitive ability and decline after early life
stress exposure. Neurobiol Aging 34:1674–1679
Peters AT, Burkhouse KL, Kinney KL, Phan KL (2019) The roles of early-life adversity and rumi-
nation in neural response to emotional faces amongst anxious and depressed adults. Psychol
Med 49:2267–2278
Petruccelli K, Davis J, Berman T (2019) Adverse childhood experiences and associated health out-
comes: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Child Abuse Negl 97:104127
Phillips NK, Hammen CL, Brennan PA, Najman JM, Bor W (2005) Early adversity and the pro-
spective prediction of depressive and anxiety disorders in adolescents. J Abnorm Child Psychol
33:13–24
Pierce JB, Kershaw KN, Kiefe CI, Jacobs DR Jr, Sidney S, Merkin SS, Feinglass J (2020)
Association of childhood psychosocial environment with 30-year cardiovascular disease inci-
dence and mortality in middle age. J Am Heart Assoc 9:e015326
Pincus-Knackstedt MK, Joachim RA, Blois SM, Douglas AJ, Orsal AS, Klapp BF, Wahn U,
Hamelmann E, Arck PC (2006) Prenatal stress enhances susceptibility of murine adult off-
spring toward airway inflammation. J Immunol 177:8484–8492
Pinto Pereira SM, Stein Merkin S, Seeman T, Power C (2019) Understanding associations of ear-
ly-life adversities with mid-life inflammatory profiles: evidence from the UK and USA. Brain
Behav Immun 78:143–152
Pohl CS, Medland JE, Moeser AJ (2015) Early-life stress origins of gastrointestinal disease:
animal models, intestinal pathophysiology, and translational implications. Am J Physiol
Gastrointest Liver Physiol 309:G927–G941
Pohl CS, Medland JE, Mackey E, Edwards LL, Bagley KD, DeWilde MP, Williams KJ, Moeser AJ
(2017) Early weaning stress induces chronic functional diarrhea, intestinal barrier defects, and
increased mast cell activity in a porcine model of early life adversity. Neurogastroenterol Motil
29:e13118
Popova NK, Morozova MV, Amstislavskaya TG (2011) Prenatal stress and ethanol exposure pro-
duces inversion of sexual partner preference in mice. Neurosci Lett 489:48–52
Ports KA, Holman DM, Guinn AS, Pampati S, Dyer KE, Merrick MT, Lunsford NB, Metzler M
(2019) Adverse childhood experiences and the presence of cancer risk factors in adulthood: a
scoping review of the literature from 2005 to 2015. J Pediatr Nurs 44:81–96
Post RM, Altshuler LL, Kupka R et al (2016) Age of onset of bipolar disorder: combined effect of
childhood adversity and familial loading of psychiatric disorders. J Psychiatr Res 81:63–70
Poulton R, Caspi A, Milne BJ, Thomson WM, Taylor A, Sears MR, Moffitt TE (2002) Association
between children’s experience of socioeconomic disadvantage and adult health: a life-course
study. Lancet 360:1640–1645
References 127

Provençal N, Booij L, Tremblay RE (2015) The developmental origins of chronic physical aggres-
sion: biological pathways triggered by early life adversity. J Exp Biol 218:123–133
Prusator DK, Greenwood-Van Meerveld B (2016) Sex-related differences in pain behaviors fol-
lowing three early life stress paradigms. Biol Sex Differ 7:29
Prusator DK, Greenwood-Van Meerveld B (2017) Amygdala-mediated mechanisms regulate vis-
ceral hypersensitivity in adult females following early life stress: importance of the glucocorti-
coid receptor and corticotropin-releasing factor. Pain 158:296–305
Puterman E, Gemmill A, Karasek D, Weir D, Adler NE, Prather AA, Epel ES (2016) Lifespan
adversity and later adulthood telomere length in the nationally representative US Health and
Retirement Study. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 113:E6335–E6342
Quilliot D, Brunaud L, Mathieu J, Quenot C, Sirveaux MA, Kahn JP, Ziegler O, Witkowski P
(2019) Links between traumatic experiences in childhood or early adulthood and lifetime binge
eating disorder. Psychiatry Res 276:134–141
Rajindrajith S, Hettige S, Gulegoda I, Jayawickrama N, De Silva SC, Samarakoon HK, de Silva
RL, Abeyagunawardena S, Devanarayana NM (2018) Aerophagia in adolescents is associated
with exposure to adverse life events and psychological maladjustment. Neurogastroenterol
Motil 30:e13224
Rao U, Chen LA, Bidesi AS, Shad MU, Thomas MA, Hammen CL (2010) Hippocampal changes
associated with early-life adversity and vulnerability to depression. Biol Psychiatry 67:357–364
Rasmussen LJH, Moffitt TE, Eugen-Olsen J et al (2019) Cumulative childhood risk is associ-
ated with a new measure of chronic inflammation in adulthood. J Child Psychol Psychiatry
60:199–208
Rau AR, Chappell AM, Butler TR, Ariwodola OJ, Weiner JL (2015) Increased basolateral amyg-
dala pyramidal cell excitability may contribute to the anxiogenic phenotype induced by chronic
early-life stress. J Neurosci 35:9730–9740
Rauschecker JP, May ES, Maudoux A, Ploner M (2015) Frontostriatal gating of tinnitus and
chronic pain. Trends Cogn Sci 19:567–578
Ravona-Springer R, Beeri MS, Goldbourt U (2012) Younger age at crisis following parental
death in male children and adolescents is associated with higher risk for dementia at old age.
Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 26:68–73
Richards M, Wadsworth ME (2004) Long term effects of early adversity on cognitive function.
Arch Dis Child 89:922–927
Ridout KK, Levandowski M, Ridout SJ, Gantz L, Goonan K, Palermo D, Price LH, Tyrka AR
(2018) Early life adversity and telomere length: a meta-analysis. Mol Psychiatry 23:858–871
Robert Koch-Institut (2013) Beiträge zur Gesundheitsberichterstattung des Bundes—
Referenzperzentile für anthropometrische Maßzahlen und Blutdruck aus der Studie
zur Gesundheit von Kindern und Jugendlichen in Deutschland (KiGGS). Berlin,
RKI-Hausdruckerei
Robson E, Norris T, Wulaningsih W, Hamer M, Hardy R, Johnson W (2020) The relationship
of early-life adversity with adulthood weight and cardiometabolic health status in the 1946
national survey of health and development. Psychosom Med 82:82–89
Rosa MJ, Lee AG, Wright RJ (2018) Evidence establishing a link between prenatal and early-life
stress and asthma development. Curr Opin Allergy Clin Immunol 18:148–158
Rosenman S, Rodgers B (2006) Childhood adversity and adult personality. Aust N Z J Psychiatry
40:482–490
Said N, Lakehayli S, El Khachibi M, El Ouahli M, Nadifi S, Hakkou F, Tazi A (2015) Prenatal
stress induces vulnerability to nicotine addiction and alters D2 receptors’ expression in the
nucleus accumbens in adult rats. Neuroscience 304:279–285
128 3 Consequences of Early Traumatic Experiences

Salas J, van den Berk-Clark C, Skiöld-Hanlin S, Schneider FD, Scherrer JF (2019) Adverse child-
hood experiences, depression, and cardiometabolic disease in a nationally representative sam-
ple. J Psychosom Res 127:109842
Santavirta T, Santavirta N, Gilman SE (2018) Association of the world war II finnish evacuation
of children with psychiatric hospitalization in the next generation. JAMA Psychiatry 75:21–27
Savolainen K, Eriksson JG, Kananen L, Kajantie E, Pesonen AK, Heinonen K, Räikkönen K
(2014) Associations between early life stress, self-reported traumatic experiences across the
lifespan and leukocyte telomere length in elderly adults. Biol Psychol 97:35–42
Scheinost D, Sinha R, Cross SN, Kwon SH, Sze G, Constable RT, Ment LR (2017) Does prenatal
stress alter the developing connectome? Pediatr Res 81:214–226
Schmidt U, Slone G, Tiller J, Treasure J (1993) Childhood adversity and adult defence style in eat-
ing disorder patients – a controlled study. Br J Med Psychol 66:353–362
Schneider ML, Roughton EC, Koehler AJ, Lubach GR (1999) Growth and development following
prenatal stress exposure in primates: an examination of ontogenetic vulnerability. Child Dev
70:263–274
Schneper LM, Brooks-Gunn J, Notterman DA, Suomi SJ (2016) Early-life experiences and tel-
omere length in adult rhesus monkeys: an exploratory study. Psychosom Med 78:1066–1071
Schoenaker D, Callaway LK, Mishra GD (2019) the role of childhood adversity in the develop-
ment of gestational diabetes. Am J Prev Med 57:302–310
Schreier HMC, Jones EJ, Nayman S, Smyth JM (2019) Associations between adverse child-
hood family environments and blood pressure differ between men and women. PLoS One
14:e0225544
Schrepf A, Naliboff B, Williams DA et al (2018) Adverse childhood experiences and symptoms
of urologic chronic pelvic pain syndrome: a multidisciplinary approach to the study of chronic
pelvic pain research network study. Ann Behav Med 52:865–877
Schroeder K, Langeland W, Fisher HL, Huber CG, Schäfer I (2016) Dissociation in patients with
schizophrenia spectrum disorders: what is the role of different types of childhood adversity?
Compr Psychiatry 68:201–208
Schwabe L, Bohbot VD, Wolf OT (2012) Prenatal stress changes learning strategies in adulthood.
Hippocampus 22:2136–2143
Scott KM, Von Korff M, Alonso J et al (2008) Childhood adversity, early-onset depressive/anxiety
disorders, and adult-onset asthma. Psychosom Med 70:1035–1043
Sen P, Molinero-Perez A, O’Riordan KJ, McCafferty CP, O’Halloran KD, Cryan JF (2021)
Microbiota and sleep: awakening the gut feeling. Trends Mol Med 27:935–945
Shalev I, Moffitt TE, Sugden K, Williams B, Houts RM, Danese A, Mill J, Arseneault L, Caspi A
(2013) Exposure to violence during childhood is associated with telomere erosion from 5 to 10
years of age: a longitudinal study. Mol Psychiatry 18:576–581
Shaw DS, Vondra JI (1993) Chronic family adversity and infant attachment security. J Child
Psychol Psychiatry 34:1205–1215
Shaw DS, Vondra JI, Hommerding KD, Keenan K, Dunn M (1994) Chronic family adversity and
early child behavior problems: a longitudinal study of low income families. J Child Psychol
Psychiatry 35:1109–1122
Sheehan CM, Li L, Friedman EM (2020) Quantity, timing, and type of childhood adversity and
sleep quality in adulthood. Sleep Health 6:246–252
Sheikh MA (2018) Retrospectively reported childhood adversity is associated with asthma and
chronic bronchitis, independent of mental health. J Psychosom Res 114:50–57
Sheridan MA, Peverill M, Finn AS, McLaughlin KA (2017) Dimensions of childhood adver-
sity have distinct associations with neural systems underlying executive functioning. Dev
Psychopathol 29:1777–1794
References 129

Shrira A, Litwin H (2014) The effect of lifetime cumulative adversity and depressive symptoms on
functional status. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 69:953–965
Sideli L, Murray RM, Schimmenti A, Corso M, La Barbera D, Trotta A, Fisher HL (2020)
Childhood adversity and psychosis: a systematic review of bio-psycho-social mediators and
moderators. Psychol Med 50:1761–1782
Slopen N, Lewis TT, Gruenewald TL, Mujahid MS, Ryff CD, Albert MA, Williams DR (2010)
Early life adversity and inflammation in African Americans and whites in the midlife in the
United States survey. Psychosom Med 72:694–701
Slopen N, Koenen KC, Kubzansky LD (2012) Childhood adversity and immune and inflamma-
tory biomarkers associated with cardiovascular risk in youth: a systematic review. Brain Behav
Immun 26:239–250
Slopen N, Kubzansky LD, McLaughlin KA, Koenen KC (2013) Childhood adversity and inflam-
matory processes in youth: a prospective study. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38:188–200
Slopen N, Loucks EB, Appleton AA, Kawachi I, Kubzansky LD, Non AL, Buka S, Gilman SE
(2015) Early origins of inflammation: an examination of prenatal and childhood social adver-
sity in a prospective cohort study. Psychoneuroendocrinology 51:403–413
Smith PH, Oberleitner LM, Smith KM, McKee SA (2016) Childhood adversity interacts with adult
stressful events to predict reduced likelihood of smoking cessation among women but not men.
Clin Psychol Sci 4:183–193
Sorocco KH, Carnes NC, Cohoon AJ, Vincent AS, Lovallo WR (2015) Risk factors for alcoholism
in the Oklahoma Family Health Patterns project: impact of early life adversity and family his-
tory on affect regulation and personality. Drug Alcohol Depend 150:38–45
de Souza JA, da Silva MC, Costa FCO et al (2020) Early life stress induced by maternal separation
during lactation alters the eating behavior and serotonin system in middle-aged rat female off-
spring. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 192:172908
Speakman JR, Westerterp KR (2010) Associations between energy demands, physical activity, and
body composition in adult humans between 18 and 96 y of age. Am J Clin Nutr 92:826–834
Spencer SJ (2013) Perinatal programming of neuroendocrine mechanisms connecting feeding
behavior and stress. Front Neurosci 7:109
Spitzer C, Bouchain M, Winkler LY, Wingenfeld K, Gold SM, Grabe HJ, Barnow S, Otte C,
Heesen C (2012) Childhood trauma in multiple sclerosis: a case-control study. Psychosom Med
74:312–318
Spitzer C, Wegert S, Wollenhaupt J, Wingenfeld K, Barnow S, Grabe HJ (2013) Gender-specific
association between childhood trauma and rheumatoid arthritis: a case-control study. J
Psychosom Res 74:296–300
Stein DJ, Scott K, Haro Abad JM et al (2010) Early childhood adversity and later hypertension:
data from the World Mental Health Survey. Ann Clin Psychiatry 22:19–28
Stojek MM, Maples-Keller JL, Dixon HD, Umpierrez GE, Gillespie CF, Michopoulos V (2019)
Associations of childhood trauma with food addiction and insulin resistance in African-
American women with diabetes mellitus. Appetite 141:104317
Straub RH (2007) The complex role of estrogens in inflammation. Endocr Rev 28:521–574
Straub RH (2018) Altern, Müdigkeit und Entzündungen verstehen – Wenn Immunsystem und
Gehirn um die Energie im Körper ringen. Springer, Berlin/Heidelberg
Straub RH (2020) Drei Gedächtnisse für den Körper. Springer Nature, Berlin
Straub RH, Cutolo M, Zietz B, Schölmerich J (2001) The process of aging changes the interplay of
the immune, endocrine and nervous systems. Mech Ageing Dev 122:1591–1611
Su S, Wang X, Pollock JS, Treiber FA, Xu X, Snieder H, McCall WV, Stefanek M, Harshfield GA
(2015) Adverse childhood experiences and blood pressure trajectories from childhood to young
adulthood: the Georgia stress and Heart study. Circulation 131:1674–1681
130 3 Consequences of Early Traumatic Experiences

Su X, Liang H, Yuan W, Olsen J, Cnattingius S, Li J (2016) Prenatal and early life stress and
risk of eating disorders in adolescent girls and young women. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry
25:1245–1253
Suglia SF, Koenen KC, Boynton-Jarrett R et al (2018) Childhood and adolescent adversity and
cardiometabolic outcomes: a scientific statement from the American Heart Association.
Circulation 137:e15–e28
Suglia SF, Chen C, Wang S et al (2020) Childhood adversity and pubertal development among
Puerto Rican boys and girls. Psychosom Med 82:487–494
Sullivan K, Rochani H, Huang LT, Donley DK, Zhang J (2019) Adverse childhood experiences
affect sleep duration for up to 50 years later. Sleep 42:zsz087
Sumner JA, Colich NL, Uddin M, Armstrong D, McLaughlin KA (2019) Early experiences of
threat, but not deprivation, are associated with accelerated biological aging in children and ado-
lescents. Biol Psychiatry 85:268–278
Surtees PG, Wainwright NW, Pooley KA, Luben RN, Khaw KT, Easton DF, Dunning AM
(2011) Life stress, emotional health, and mean telomere length in the European Prospective
Investigation into Cancer (EPIC)-Norfolk population study. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci
66:1152–1162
Suzuki H, Luby JL, Botteron KN, Dietrich R, McAvoy MP, Barch DM (2014) Early life stress and
trauma and enhanced limbic activation to emotionally valenced faces in depressed and healthy
children. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 53:800–813.e810
System IFI (2021) Iowa Flood Center. https://iowafloodcenter.org/
Tanenbaum HC, Li Y, Felicitas-Perkins JQ, Zhang M, Palmer P, Johnson CA, Xie B (2017) A lon-
gitudinal analysis of the impact of childhood stress on weight status among Chinese youth. Int J
Obes 41:820–823
Tang W, Xu D, Xu J (2020) Impact of earthquake exposure, family adversity and peer problems
on anxiety-related emotional disorders in adolescent survivors three years after the Ya’an earth-
quake. J Affect Disord 273:215–222
Tani Y, Fujiwara T, Kondo K (2020) Association between adverse childhood experiences and
dementia in older Japanese adults. JAMA Netw Open 3:e1920740
Tavares GA, Torres A, de Souza JA (2020) Early life stress and the onset of obesity: proof of
microRNAs’ involvement through modulation of serotonin and dopamine systems’ homeosta-
sis. Front Physiol 11:925
Taylor SE, Lehman BJ, Kiefe CI, Seeman TE (2006) Relationship of early life stress and psycho-
logical functioning to adult C-reactive protein in the coronary artery risk development in young
adults study. Biol Psychiatry 60:819–824
Tesarz J, Eich W, Treede RD, Gerhardt A (2016) Altered pressure pain thresholds and increased
wind-up in adult patients with chronic back pain with a history of childhood maltreatment: a
quantitative sensory testing study. Pain 157:1799–1809
Thapar A, Pine DS, Leckman JF, Scott S, Snowling MJ, Taylor E (2015) Rutter’s child and adoles-
cent psychiatry, 6. Aufl. Wiley, Chichester/West Sussex
Thomas C, Hyppönen E, Power C (2008) Obesity and type 2 diabetes risk in midadult life: the role
of childhood adversity. Pediatrics 121:e1240–e1249
Tuchscherer M, Kanitz E, Otten W, Tuchscherer A (2002) Effects of prenatal stress on cellular and
humoral immune responses in neonatal pigs. Vet Immunol Immunopathol 86:195–203
Tursich M, Neufeld RW, Frewen PA, Harricharan S, Kibler JL, Rhind SG, Lanius RA (2014)
Association of trauma exposure with proinflammatory activity: a transdiagnostic meta-analysis.
Transl Psychiatry 4:e413
van Bodegom M, Homberg JR, Henckens M (2017) Modulation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-ad-
renal axis by early life stress exposure. Front Cell Neurosci 11:87
References 131

van Campen JS, Jansen FE, de Graan PN, Braun KP, Joels M (2014) Early life stress in epilepsy: a
seizure precipitant and risk factor for epileptogenesis. Epilepsy Behav 38:160–171
van Dammen L, Bush NR, de Rooij SR, Mol BWJ, Groen H, Hoek A, Roseboom TJ (2019)
Childhood adversity and women’s cardiometabolic health in adulthood: associations with
health behaviors, psychological distress, mood symptoms, and personality. BMC Womens
Health 19:102
van Dammen L, Bush NR, de Rooij S, Mol BW, Mutsaerts M, van Oers A, Groen H, Hoek A,
Roseboom T (2021) A lifestyle intervention randomized controlled trial in obese women with
infertility improved body composition among those who experienced childhood adversity.
Stress Health 37:93–102
van Dijken S (1998) John Bowlby: his early life: a biographical journey into the roots of attach-
ment theory. Free Association Books, London/New York
Van Someren EJW (2021) Brain mechanisms of insomnia: new perspectives on causes and conse-
quences. Physiol Rev 101:995–1046
Vanbesien-Mailliot CC, Wolowczuk I, Mairesse J, Viltart O, Delacre M, Khalife J, Chartier-Harlin
MC, Maccari S (2007) Prenatal stress has pro-inflammatory consequences on the immune sys-
tem in adult rats. Psychoneuroendocrinology 32:114–124
Vartanian LR, Smyth JM, Zawadzki MJ, Heron KE, Coleman SR (2014) Early adversity, personal
resources, body dissatisfaction, and disordered eating. Int J Eat Disord 47:620–629
Veenema AH, Reber SO, Selch S, Obermeier F, Neumann ID (2008) Early life stress enhances
the vulnerability to chronic psychosocial stress and experimental colitis in adult mice.
Endocrinology 149:2727–2736
Venta A (2020) Attachment facilitates acculturative learning and adversity moderates: validating
the theory of epistemic trust in a natural experiment. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev 51:471–477
Vig R, Gordon JR, Thébaud B, Befus AD, Vliagoftis H (2010) The effect of early-life stress on air-
way inflammation in adult mice. Neuroimmunomodulation 17:229–239
von Bonsdorff MB, Kokko K, Salonen M, von Bonsdorff ME, Poranen-Clark T, Alastalo H,
Kajantie E, Osmond C, Eriksson JG (2019) Association of childhood adversities and home
atmosphere with functioning in old age: the Helsinki birth cohort study. Age Ageing 48:80–86
Waehrer GM, Miller TR, Silverio Marques SC, Oh DL, Burke HN (2020) Disease burden of
adverse childhood experiences across 14 states. PLoS One 15:e0226134
Wainwright NW, Surtees PG (2002) Childhood adversity, gender and depression over the life-
course. J Affect Disord 72:33–44
Wajid A, van Zanten SV, Mughal MK, Biringer A, Austin MP, Vermeyden L, Kingston D (2020)
Adversity in childhood and depression in pregnancy. Arch Womens Ment Health 23:169–180
Walker RA, Andreansky C, Ray MH, McDannald MA (2018) Early adolescent adversity inflates
threat estimation in females and promotes alcohol use initiation in both sexes. Behav Neurosci
132:171–182
Wan Y, Chen R, Ma S, McFeeters D, Sun Y, Hao J, Tao F (2019) Associations of adverse childhood
experiences and social support with self-injurious behaviour and suicidality in adolescents. Br J
Psychiatry 214:146–152
Wang XD, Labermaier C, Holsboer F, Wurst W, Deussing JM, Müller MB, Schmidt MV (2012)
Early-life stress-induced anxiety-related behavior in adult mice partially requires forebrain cor-
ticotropin-releasing hormone receptor 1. Eur J Neurosci 36:2360–2367
Wang Y, Raffeld MR, Slopen N, Hale L, Dunn EC (2016) Childhood adversity and insomnia in
adolescence. Sleep Med 21:12–18
Wanner B, Vitaro F, Tremblay RE, Turecki G (2012) Childhood trajectories of anxiousness and
disruptiveness explain the association between early-life adversity and attempted suicide.
Psychol Med 42:2373–2382
132 3 Consequences of Early Traumatic Experiences

Ward IL (1972) Prenatal stress feminizes and demasculinizes the behavior of males. Science
175:82–84
Ward IL (1984) The prenatal stress syndrome: current status. Psychoneuroendocrinology 9:3–11
Weldegiorgis M, Smith M, Herrington WG, Bankhead C, Woodward M (2020) Socioeconomic dis-
advantage and the risk of advanced chronic kidney disease: results from a cohort study with 1.4
million participants. Nephrol Dial Transplant 35:1562–1570
Wickrama KA, Kwon JA, Oshri A, Lee TK (2014) Early socioeconomic adversity and young
adult physical illness: the role of body mass index and depressive symptoms. J Adolesc Health
55:556–563
Wickrama KAS, Bae D, O’Neal CW (2017) Explaining the association between early adversity
and young adults’ diabetes outcomes: physiological, psychological, and behavioral mecha-
nisms. J Youth Adolesc 46:2407–2420
Wickrama KK, O‘Neal CW, Lee TK, Wickrama T (2015) Early socioeconomic adversity, youth
positive development, and young adults’ cardio-metabolic disease risk. Health Psychol
34:905–914
Wicks S, Hjern A, Gunnell D, Lewis G, Dalman C (2005) Social adversity in childhood and the
risk of developing psychosis: a national cohort study. Am J Psychiatry 162:1652–1657
Williams LM, Gatt JM, Schofield PR, Olivieri G, Peduto A, Gordon E (2009) ‚Negativity bias‘ in
risk for depression and anxiety: brain-body fear circuitry correlates, 5-HTT-LPR and early life
stress. NeuroImage 47:804–814
Wilson CA, Vazdarjanova A, Terry AV Jr (2013) Exposure to variable prenatal stress in rats: effects
on anxiety-related behaviors, innate and contextual fear, and fear extinction. Behav Brain Res
238:279–288
Winsper C, Zanarini M, Wolke D (2012) Prospective study of family adversity and maladaptive
parenting in childhood and borderline personality disorder symptoms in a non-clinical popula-
tion at 11 years. Psychol Med 42:2405–2420
Witek Janusek L, Tell D, Albuquerque K, Mathews HL (2013) Childhood adversity increases vul-
nerability for behavioral symptoms and immune dysregulation in women with breast cancer.
Brain Behav Immun 30(Suppl):S149–S162
Witt ST, Bednarska O, Keita ÅV et al (2019) Interactions between gut permeability and brain
structure and function in health and irritable bowel syndrome. Neuroimage Clin 21:101602
Wong HLX, Qin HY, Tsang SW et al (2019) Early life stress disrupts intestinal homeostasis via
NGF-TrkA signaling. Nat Commun 10:1745
Yang J, Han H, Cao J, Li L, Xu L (2006) Prenatal stress modifies hippocampal synaptic plasticity
and spatial learning in young rat offspring. Hippocampus 16:431–436
Yeh CM, Huang CC, Hsu KS (2012) Prenatal stress alters hippocampal synaptic plasticity in
young rat offspring through preventing the proteolytic conversion of pro-brain-derived neuro-
trophic factor (BDNF) to mature BDNF. J Physiol 590:991–1010
You DS, Meagher MW (2016) Childhood adversity and pain sensitization. Psychosom Med
78:1084–1093
You DS, Meagher MW (2018) Childhood adversity and pain facilitation. Psychosom Med
80:869–879
Zazara DE, Perani CV, Solano ME, Arck PC (2018) Prenatal stress challenge impairs fetal lung
development and asthma severity sex-specifically in mice. J Reprod Immunol 125:100–105
Zhang L, Zhang D, Sun Y (2019) Adverse childhood experiences and early pubertal timing among
girls: a meta-analysis. Int J Environ Res Public Health 16:2887
References 133

Zhu J, Lowen SB, Anderson CM, Ohashi K, Khan A, Teicher MH (2019) Association of prepuber-
tal and postpubertal exposure to childhood maltreatment with adult amygdala function. JAMA
Psychiatry 76:843–853
Zhu P, Hao JH, Tao RX, Huang K, Jiang XM, Zhu YD, Tao FB (2015) Sex-specific and time-de-
pendent effects of prenatal stress on the early behavioral symptoms of ADHD: a longitudinal
study in China. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 24:1139–1147
Zijlmans MA, Korpela K, Riksen-Walraven JM, de Vos WM, de Weerth C (2015) Maternal pre-
natal stress is associated with the infant intestinal microbiota. Psychoneuroendocrinology
53:233–224
Chronic Immune System Activation
4

4.1 Egoistic Brain and Egoistic Immune System are Fourfold


Interconnected

Brain and immune system are basically egoistic and each reacts according to the respec-
tive stimulus without consulting the other in order to eliminate the problem or the dis-
turbing stimulus. A brain-specific stimulus is, for example, noise or psychological stress
(e.g. an attack situation). An immune system-specific stimulus is an infectious agent or
an allergen. I have reported extensively on the egoism of the two organ systems, which is
expressed above all in the egoistically controlled self-allocation of energy (Straub 2018).
The egoism refers to the, hierarchically considered, dominant position vis-à-vis other
body systems in the egoistic regulation of all downstream organs and the enforcement of
the energy self-allocation (Straub 2018). Nevertheless, there is a certain mutual influence
of brain and immune system, which becomes apparent through the tone or “ambiance” of
the other system preceding a stress response (more on this later in Sect. 4.2.1 and in Sect.
4.2.3.2 etc.).
After early traumatic experiences, the disturbing psychological stimulus primarily
affects the brain, and depending on the time window (Sect. 2.3) and type of stimulus, dif-
ferent brain regions are more or less severely affected, which is summarized in Fig. 3.4.
Time windows are very important because different areas of the brain develop at differ-
ent speeds (Fareri and Tottenham 2016).
Adversities lead to a changed maturation of the brain and to structural and functional
changes in different brain regions (McEwen et al. 2016), and these changes act back into
the periphery in order to influence the immune system there. But how do these things
now reach the periphery towards the immune system? For this purpose, so-called “con-
nectors” are needed, as I once called them for simplicity when assessing a Collaborative
Research Center of the German Research Foundation in Lübeck.

© The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer-Verlag GmbH, DE, part of Springer 135
Nature 2023
R. H. Straub, Early Trauma as the Origin of Chronic Inflammation,
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-662-66751-4_4
136 4 Chronic Immune System Activation

There are at least four different connectors. Direct connectors (No. 1) with the direc-
tion “brain → immune system” are typically hormonal systems such as the HPA axis or
the sex hormone axis, neurotransmitters of the nervous system extending from the brain
to the periphery (they are called efferent: sympathetic nervous system, parasympathetic
nervous system and the pain-regulating system emanating from the brain). In addition,
direct connectors with the direction “immune system → brain” should be mentioned,
such as circulating cytokines, circulating activated immune cells, hormones and neu-
rotransmitters from immune cells and nerves extending from the periphery to the brain
(they are called afferent: sensory nervous system and other afferent nerve tracts, espe-
cially in the vagus nerve).
Furthermore, we know indirect connectors stimulated by the brain or immune system
via other organ systems (No. 2), which release factors that can influence either the brain
or the immune system (explanation 8).

Explanation 8 Indirect connectors via other systems that influence brain and immune
system
Although brain and immune system have their own connectors that have a direct
influence on the other selfish organ system, there are indirect factors induced by the
brain and the immune system in other organs that play the role of a connector of the
two selfish organs. An example are the fatty acids, the basic components of all fats.
Fatty acids are stored in the fat tissue, but can be released from the fat tissue under
the influence of the brain or the immune system. Neither brain nor immune system
can release fatty acids as a messenger substance from their own stocks in a quanti-
tatively comparable way, but both—brain and immune system—can release the fatty
acids from the fat tissue by messenger substances. These fatty acids can influence the
brain and the immune system. In this way, fatty acids are indirect connectors via other
systems (here the fat tissue system) that influence brain and immune system (Straub
2020a, b). ◄

Furthermore, there are connectors that are located in the environment of the individual
and can be called extra-corporeal connectors of brain and immune system (No. 3) (expla-
nation 9).

Explanation 9 Extracorporeal connectors that influence the brain and immune system
These are factors that are changed by the brain or the immune system in the environ-
ment and that have an impact on the other selfish system.
For example, the brain decides whether you want to smoke cigarettes, that is,
whether you need cigarettes and smoking as “reward factors”. Smoking has an effect
on the immune system via the respiratory tract, mostly in an activating and unfavora-
ble way (for example in autoimmune diseases). Smoking is such an extracorporeal
connector between brain and immune system.
4.1 Egoistic Brain and Egoistic Immune System are Fourfold Linked 137

On the other hand, the immune system can influence the brain through extracor-
poreal connectors. The immune systems of different people can deal with a patho-
gen in different ways. As an example, the leprosy pathogen is mentioned here, which
is quickly and effectively fought by one immune system, but poorly by the other. In
the case of insufficient attack by the immune system, leprosy is not well controlled
and it leads to long-term illness and death. This has, among other things, chronic
socio-economic and family follow-up reactions during the often long-term illness of
the affected person, which in turn have an impact on the brain. All of this is spared to
the person with the effective immune system because he does not get leprosy. ◄

Then there are connectors from the field of genetics and epigenetics. A platform for this
connection are genetic or epigenetic predispositions that, for example, lead to an unfa-
vorable and disease-causing brain change after an early trauma. You will remember
genetic and epigenetic elements in Sect. 2.5, where such conditions were mentioned.
Let’s assume there is a defined genetic or epigenetic factor that directly worsens the fol-
low-up problems after early adversities in the brain at nerve cell assemblies, and let’s
assume that the same factor directly favors chronic immune system activation, then
there is a connection between trauma sequelae and chronic immune system activation.
Here we can speak of a pleiotropic connector (No. 4). I use the word “pleiotropic” here
because it is used for this purpose in genetics. What is it?
A gene with the fictitious name X that causes different things in two separate places in the
body is called pleiotropic by geneticists (gr. pleíōn, engl. more; gr. tropein, engl. change). In
other words, through a variant of gene X, more than one thing is changed in different places
in the body. If the variant of gene X causes psychiatric consequences after trauma and at the
same time stimulates chronic activation of the immune system, psychiatric problems and
chronic inflammatory diseases can occur at the same time, even though the brain and immune
system do not influence each other. I call such links “pleiotropic connectors” (No. 4).
In the case of earlier trauma experiences, these four connectors create the link from
the damaged brain to the immune system and vice versa. These four connectors must
form the platform for chronic immune system activation (Fig. 4.1 gives a first overview).
Basically, I am discussing the direction “brain → immune system” because this is
how the problem of chronic immune system activation can be explained. Before we deal
in detail with the four connectors and the associated examples of immune activation, we
must briefly clarify how the brain stimulates the direct and indirect connectors.

4.1.1 Areas in the Brain that Stimulate Direct and Indirect


Connectors

Remember the strong connection between the amygdala and the output of the central
sympathetic nervous system (the magenta colored lines in Fig. 3.4). These connections
are shown in more detail in Fig. 4.2. However, this representation only scratches the
138 4 Chronic Immune System Activation

Abb. 4.1 The four connectors. 1. Genetics


Direct connectors are messenger Epigenetics
substances that are released by pleiotropic No.4
the brain or immune system connectors
and influence the other selfish
system. 2. Indirect connectors selfish brain
come from subsystems (not
brain or immune system) and are
either released by the influence D
of the brain or immune system I
to manipulate the other selfish R
system (brain or immune system). E
3. Extracorporeal connectors C Fatty tissue
are located in the environment T Muscle tissue
Environment Intestine
of the individual. The brain or
immune system can control the No.1 C Skin
O Lung No.2
exposure to these connectors, and No.3 extra-
N Mouth
so the other system is influenced corporeal
N amongst others
because of the consequences connectors
E indirect
of the exposure. 4. Pleiotropic
C connectors
connectors are of genetic or
epigenetic nature, for example, T
promoting unfavorable brain O
changes and causing unfavorable R
changes in the immune system S
at the same time. The changes in
the brain and immune system do selfish immune system
not have to be mediated by direct
or indirect links between the two pleiotropic
organs. The parallel changes lead connectors No.4
to unfavorable consequences in
Genetics
one and the other system
Epigenetics

surface of the true complexity of the relationships, but it shows manifold possibilities
to transfer central nervous derangement from Fig. 3.4 into the periphery via three main
pathways. In the example of Fig. 4.2 this can lead to changes in the blood concentration
of cortisol, noradrenaline and adrenaline as well as the local tissue concentration of sub-
stance P (which are shown at the bottom of Fig. 4.2). In addition to the main pathways,
there are also bystander pathways, for example the sex hormone axis, which I will also
address.
However, the feedback of hormones and neurotransmitters to the brain is not given in
Fig. 4.2, such as how cortisol directly influences various brain regions in order to gener-
ally counteract too high cortisol blood concentration. There are similar negative feedback
possibilities for the sympathetic nervous system, and this function is mainly performed
by neuronal links. Such feedbacks can be defective or no longer function properly under
chronic illness.
Cortisol, noradrenaline, adrenaline and substance P can now directly influence
immune processes (more on this in the next chapters), and so we can identify them as
4.1 Egoistic Brain and Egoistic Immune System are Fourfold Linked 139

Insular
Post central
cortex
gyrus
Frontal brain:
medial
prefrontal Amygdala
cortex
Hippocampus
ACC. Anterior
cingulate gyrus
Locus coeruleus
(reticular formation)
Nucleus
accumbens NTS (N. Vagus)

C/RVLM
Hypothalamus

Thalamus PAG
Ascending
pathways report
pain from the
chest and
abdominal area,
among others
Spinal cord
segment
Ascending Descending Inter
Hypothalamus
pathways pathways inhibt mediolateral
(PVN) pain input
report pain nuclei in the
spinal cord

CRH

Dorsal root Sympathetic


Pituitary
ganglion chain
ganglia

ACTH Pain - nerve Sympathetic


fiber nerve fiber

Adrenal Adrenal
cortex medulla

Cortisol Substance P Noradrenaline Adrenaline


140 4 Chronic Immune System Activation


Fig. 4.2 The brain activates pathways into the periphery. The red lines show connections between diffe-
rent brain regions and the top center of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis (HPA axis). These can be
inhibitory or activating pathways. In any case, it becomes clear how present the connections are between
critical regions of Fig. 3.4 and the HPA axis. Similarly, there are manifold connections between different
brain regions and the top centers of the sympathetic nervous system (brown lines: locus coeruleus and
C/RVLM, hypothalamus and C/RVLM). There are inhibitory and facilitatory influences. The blue lines
show important connections between pain centers, amygdala, and peripheral pain fibers. Thus, it beco-
mes clear how a disturbed balance of the brain regions mentioned in Fig. 3.4 can easily be transferred to
hormonal and neuronal pathways in the periphery. This is only a small but nonetheless important selec-
tion of possibilities. Abbreviations: ACC; anterior cingulate gyrus; ACTH, corticotropin (other name:
adrenocorticotropic hormone); C/RVLM; caudal (C) and rostral (R) ventrolateral medulla (in the medulla
oblongata); CRH; corticotropin-releasing hormone; NTS, nucleus tractus solitarii (inputs from the tho-
racic and abdominal cavities via, for example, the vagus nerve); PAG, periaqueductal gray; PVN; nuc-
leus in the hypothalamus (periventricular nucleus: top center for the HPA axis and sympathetic nervous
system). The figure was created using information from the literature (Veinante et al. 2013; Bulloch and
Daly 2014; Barman and Yates 2017; Contreras et al. 2017; Mendiguren et al. 2018; Herman et al. 2020)

direct connectors in the sense of Fig. 4.1. These direct connectors must be at least par-
tially responsible for chronic immune system activation as a result of early adversity.
Cortisol, noradrenaline, adrenaline and substance P influence other systems, for example
adipose tissue, by promoting the release of fatty acids. In this way, they acquire a func-
tion in the release of indirect connectors such as fatty acids (explanation 8). These indi-
rect connectors can then influence the immune system.
The roles of individual connectors in the problem of chronic immune system activa-
tion will now be presented in the following text. Chronic immune system activation is
the core issue of this book for me as a clinical immunologist and rheumatologist, which
is why the reverse link from the immune system to the brain is not discussed here. This
reverse connection can induce further problems that can exacerbate the consequences of
trauma. A vicious circle can develop. The order of presentation is arbitrary, and I start
with the direct connectors, followed by the indirect and extracorporeal connectors, and
then the genetic pleiotropic connectors (“pleiotropic” see Fig. 4.1).

4.2 Direct Connectors Chronically Activate the Immune


System (No. 1)

The brain and immune system influence each other, which is ensured by the tone of the
other system. The brain largely determines the tone of the sympathetic and parasympa-
thetic nervous system, the HPA axis, the efferent pain system and the sex hormone axis
in the sense of Fig. 4.2. This tuning is usually expressed in people with previous trauma
experiences in a higher concentration of noradrenaline/adrenaline, cortisol and a hyper-
sensitivity of the pain system (substance P), and a suppression of sex hormones and the
parasympathetic nervous system (vagus nerve).
This is due to the long-term adjustments of many centers in the brain, for example,
the more active regions of the amygdala, the locus coeruleus, the caudal (C) and rostral
4.2 Direct Connectors Chronically Activate the Immune System (No. 1) 141

(R) ventrolateral medulla in the medulla oblongata (C/RVLM) and other regions shown
in Fig. 4.2. Other regions are less active (hypofunction), such as the medial prefrontal
cortex in the frontal lobe, the anterior cingulate gyrus and the hippocampus, including
the connecting tracts to the aforementioned more active areas. In other words, since the
latter inhibit the former, the former become overactive.
Eus van Someren, a scientist at the Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience in
Amsterdam, has compared this brain situation after early trauma with the state of
increased and constant arousal in a recently published review article on sleep disorders
(Van Someren 2021). I wrote earlier: “He [the person affected by the trauma] is more
sensitive to negative information and he becomes ‘hyperaroused’. (…) Finally, repeated
negative events can stabilize this misalignment in the long term.”
A “memory of the unfavorable inclination” of the weighing scale shown in Fig. 3.4 is
formed. The deflection of the balance remains in the long term because the highly plastic
brain can be changed so easily in fetal years, childhood and adolescence. What we feel
as a benefit in these young years, when memory can be easily shaped, is a negative fac-
tor after trauma experiences because a chronic trauma memory is created. What do the
changes in central nervous tone mean for the immunological periphery?

4.2.1 The Sympathetic Nervous System

I’m talking about a very extensive nerve fiber system that reaches into almost all organs
and tissues, and also the adrenal gland (more precisely, the inner part of the adrenal
gland, called the adrenal medulla). With regard to the nerve fibers, the main transmitter is
noradrenaline, followed by so-called co-transmitters such as neuropeptide Y. At low con-
centrations, noradrenaline primarily binds to the α-adrenergic receptor (2 types: α1, α2)
and at high concentrations to the β-adrenergic receptor (3 types: β1, β2, β3). Here we see
a different affinity between the neurotransmitter and the two different receptors: look at
Fig. 4.3. Adrenaline from the adrenal gland behaves in the opposite way, because it has a
higher affinity for the β-adrenergic receptor.
The concentration of noradrenaline in tissue is dependent on the presence of corre-
sponding sympathetic nerve fibers. If the number of sympathetic nerve fibers is low, the
concentration is correspondingly low. In addition to noradrenaline, other neurotrans-
mitters are present in the nerve ending. The best known of these is neuropeptide Y. In
contrast to noradrenaline, it is produced in the cell body of the nerve cell and it is trans-
ported along the fiber to the sympathetic nerve ending (Fried et al. 1985; Lundberg et al.
1989). This cell body can be very far from the actual site of action, and it can take a
long time for neuropeptide Y to arrive at the scene. In the sense of a co-transmitter, neu-
ropeptide Y supports the effect of noradrenaline essentially via the β-adrenergic recep-
tor, because it is not secreted at low activity of the nerve fiber (low firing rate, when
alpha-adrenergic signaling prevails) (Rudehill et al. 1987; Lundberg et al. 1989).
142 4 Chronic Immune System Activation

Concentration
of
noradrenaline

adrenergic adrenergic

pro-inflammatory often anti-inflammatory


(exception: antibody
formation, B-cell,
allergic reaction,
leukocyte migration,
leukocyte production
in bone marrow,
energy supply
for active immune cells)

Fig. 4.3 The affinity of noradrenaline for its receptors and the consequences for inflammation. Nor-
adrenaline has a higher affinity for alpha-adrenergic receptors than for beta-adrenergic receptors. So
you need a small concentration of noradrenaline to achieve binding to alpha-adrenergic receptors. For
binding of noradrenaline to the beta-adrenergic receptor, you need large amounts of the neurotransmit-
ter. In the synaptic cleft around the sympathetic nerve ending, there is usually a basal concentration of
about 10−7 mol/l (in the blood 10−9 mol/l). With strong stimulation, the concentration rises to about 10−6
mol/l, with weak stimulation it decreases to 10−8 mol/l. With alpha-adrenergic action, the observer often
sees a pro-inflammatory constellation (Bierhaus et al. 2003; Miksa et al. 2009; Grisanti et al. 2011; Lu
et al. 2014; Liu et al. 2020; Straub et al. 2020). This is the opposite with regard to the beta-adrenergic
side (important exceptions are mentioned in the figure)

At normal firing rates on the nerve axon of 3–5 Hz, noradrenaline is mainly released,
which acts via α-adrenergic receptors. At higher firing rates of 5–10 Hz and more, neuro-
peptide Y is additionally secreted. Noradrenaline thus acts on β-adrenergic receptors and
neuropeptide Y supports it via Y receptors. The nerve ending can become impoverished
in neuropeptide Y if the firing rate was high for some time, because the supply of neu-
ropeptide Y can no longer keep up. With β-adrenergic effects, an inhibition of immune
reactions is often observed (exceptions see Fig. 4.3), with α-adrenergic signaling almost
always a pro-inflammatory situation appears.
When we talk about the consequences of earlier traumatic experiences, there are sev-
eral possible sympathetic responses that can be observed as a consequence. The basal or
resting tone of the sympathetic nervous system can be increased, normal, or reduced. In
the same way, the response to an actual stressor can be increased, normal, or reduced.
In a large overview article from 2020, Holochwost and colleagues show how, after early
trauma, an increased resting tone and a reduced sympathetic response to a stimulus are
usually observed (Holochwost et al. 2021). However, this is not always the case, as,
especially in the situation of a physically related, stress-inducing stimulus, excessive
reactions have been observed (for example, in Rinnewitz et al. 2018). I will first present
the importance of sympathetic tone, as it has a clear influence on inflammatory situations
and inflammatory diseases.
4.2 Direct Connectors Chronically Activate the Immune System (No. 1) 143

4.2.1.1 Hans Selye and the Pro-inflammatory Response of the


Sympathetic Nervous System
Hans Selye, who was born in Vienna at the turn of the century (1907), grew up in a
Hungarian family of surgeons and began studying medicine at the age of 17 at Charles
University in Prague. Selye made his doctorate in medicine there, then a doctorate in
organic chemistry, then he moved to the USA to Johns Hopkins University, only to work
long-term in biochemistry at McGill University in Montreal (Tan and Yip 2018). Selye is
the “father of modern stress research”.
He discovered why different forms of stress—toxins—caused similar physical symp-
toms such as reduced body temperature, loss of appetite, weight loss, feeling of illness,
enlargement of the adrenal gland, stomach ulcers, shrinking of the thymus and lymph
organs and other things. He called this disorder of the internal milieu (after Claude
Bernard, 1813–1878) or homeostasis (after Walter Cannon, 1871–1945) the “general
adaptation syndrome”, which has three phases: 1. Alarm reaction, 2. Resistance and 3.
Exhaustion (up to death). This reaction is accompanied and promoted by a strong activa-
tion of the sympathetic nervous system and the HPA axis.
The activation of the sympathetic nervous system was mostly understood here as a
supporter or activator of the immune system. In the spirit of the fight-or-flight reactions,
the sympathetic nervous system stimulates, in addition to the heart, circulation, lungs
and muscles, the immune system. Hans Selye recognized early on how the B-cell-driven
immune response is promoted by the sympathetic nervous system and, thus, leads to the
formation of antibodies, which he called the “serological response” (Selye 1946). By the
way, the immunologists at that time had no idea of the multitude of different immune
cells or immune cell messenger substances (cytokines, etc.). From these first considera-
tions to the early 1980s, the sympathetic nervous system was therefore largely regarded
as a promoter of the immune system.
With the advent of immune cell cultures, the T-cellular immune response in addition
to the B-cellular antibody response, the consideration of individual adrenergic receptors
and their stimulators and inhibitors, as well as the ability to easily measure immuno-
logical messenger substances such as cytokines, the picture changed, however. From the
mid-1980s to around 2000, the sympathetic nervous system was considered an inhibitor
of the immune response. Today we know the complexity of the sympathetic influence
on the immune system quite well and have an insight into the possibilities of the sympa-
thetic nervous system to increase and inhibit (Fig. 4.4).

4.2.1.2 The Tone of the Sympathetic Nervous System Promotes


Inflammation
First, we consider the influence of the entire sympathetic nervous system without going
into individual fractions of this nervous system. Because if we turn off this nervous sys-
tem by chemical-pharmacological methods, we do not exactly know where we have put
our hand. We therefore do not see whether this or that subfraction of the sympathetic
nerve fibers is responsible for the observed effect on the bone marrow, lymph node,
144 4 Chronic Immune System Activation

pro-inflammatory
chronic
acute inflammation and acute chronic
inflammation inflammation
Selye

Today

View on T cells & subtypes


anti-inflammatory View of "serological" B cells and subtypes
B cell responses Macrophages
Allergy and Dendritic cells
anaphylactic reactions Natural killer cells
Neutrophils
Cytokines, antibodies,
etc. etc.

Fig. 4.4 The effect of the sympathetic nervous system on inflammation. In the past (between 1945 and
1985), the sympathetic nervous system was considered pro-inflammatory, then for a while until around
2000 quite anti-inflammatory. As always, the truth lies somewhere in between. Today it is pro-inflamm-
atory in some immune reactions and anti-inflammatory in others. With anaphylactic, the doctor means
an allergic immediate reaction within seconds to minutes, for which the patient, for example, must be
observed by the doctor for a few minutes after a COVID-19 vaccination

spleen, intestine, heart, lung, etc. This approach seems valuable to me because the gen-
erally high tone of the sympathetic nervous system after early traumatic situations is
more general than organ-specific in nature. However, we do not know exactly, since an
organ-specific measurement of the activity of sympathetic nerve fibers in humans and
animals is lacking in a comparative approach.
Scientists are sometimes forced to carry out animal experiments when new drugs are
being tested, as the new preparations have to be tried out on animals first. Studies of this
kind are carried out, for example, on mice and rats with chronic arthritis, which closely
resembles the human arthritis in young people and adults. Experimentally, the researcher
can trigger the autoimmune disease of chronic arthritis in mice and rats with a strong
immune stimulus and the simultaneous administration of an autoantigen localized in the
joint (Trentham et al. 1977). After immunization, the autoantigen is recognized by the
patrol of immune cells in the joint area and attacked permanently; a chronic arthritis, an
experimental autoimmune disease in the joint, develops.
In this form of chronic autoimmune disease, the researcher can very well see the
importance of the tone of the sympathetic nervous system. If, for example, he blocks
the sympathetic nervous system before immunization/vaccination with the autoantigen,
that is, if he drastically reduces the tone of the sympathetic nervous system, a much
milder form of arthritis develops or it does not develop at all (Härle et al. 2005). Similar
experiments by other groups confirmed this fact, and it is irrelevant how the arthritis was
4.2 Direct Connectors Chronically Activate the Immune System (No. 1) 145

Abb. 4.5 The blockade of the


sympathetic nervous system with intact sympathetic
inhibits the development of nervous system
arthritis. Arthritis is triggered at

Joint swelling
time zero. A few days earlier, the
sympathetic nervous system was
blocked. If there is a blockade with blockade of
of the sympathetic nervous the sympathetic
system, the joint inflammation nervous system
is significantly less than in the
presence of an intact sympathetic
nervous system. Figure
schematically according to data
from Ebbinghaus et al. (2012) Observation time

Triggering
arthritis

triggered: Reducing the sympathetic tone before triggering arthritis significantly reduces
the disease (Fig. 4.5) (Levine et al. 1986a; Basbaum and Levine 1991; Aloe et al. 1992;
Lorton et al. 1999; Härle et al. 2008; Ebbinghaus et al. 2012, 2017; Schaible und Straub
2014; Klatt et al. 2016). This can be an interesting drug approach.
In this example, we recognize the importance of the preceding tone of the sympa-
thetic nervous system in triggering chronic joint inflammation. Several mechanisms are
relevant that scientists gradually recognized (Schaible and Straub 2014). A key mecha-
nism is the migration of leukocytes, which is supported by the sympathetic nervous sys-
tem (Schedlowski et al. 1996).
The migration of leukocytes, such as T lymphocytes or B lymphocytes, is very impor-
tant in arthritis because only in this way can the antigen be recognized and attacked in
the joint area or in the lymph node. So the sympathetic nervous system supports the exit
of leukocytes from the involved and activated lymph nodes and from the spleen, and
blockade of the sympathetic nervous system reduces the exit of these leukocytes and thus
reduces joint inflammation (Klatt et al. 2016).
Other researchers confirm the supportive effect of the sympathetic nervous system
on the exit of leukocytes from lymphoid organs (Katayama et al. 2006; Wohleb et al.
2014; McKim et al. 2016; Ao et al. 2020). Interestingly, the leukocytes that have entered
the bloodstream can go to the brain and trigger anxiety reactions (Wohleb et al. 2014;
McKim et al. 2016). Currently, we already have a rough idea of the ​​ scene of the action,
the subfraction of the sympathetic nervous system in lymph nodes, spleen and bone
marrow.
Others found β-adrenergic increased production of monocytes and granulocytes in
the bone marrow. This led to an increased transition of activated cells from the bone
marrow into the blood, and these cells show a particular conserved inflammatory char-
acter (Powell et al. 2013). The increased production of bone marrow precursor cells of
146 4 Chronic Immune System Activation

monocytes and granulocytes was confirmed by others (Heidt et al. 2014). Migration and
production of leukocyte precursor cells are thus supported by the sympathetic nervous
system.
At this point we can also ask whether the sympathetic tone has any significance in
other inflammatory diseases. If the experimenter removes the sympathetic influence in
a parasitic infection, the infection worsens and the individuals die sooner. This finding
speaks for the immunosupportive effect of the sympathetic nervous system and indirectly
shows how the higher sympathetic tone elicits a stronger immune response (Roggero
et al. 2016). A similar finding is seen by the researcher after the reduction of sympathetic
tone in acute staphylococcal infection of the abdomen, because there the existing low
sympathetic tone leads to a reduced immune response and a greater spread of the bacte-
ria (Straub et al. 2005). The sympathetic nervous system helps in some infection situa-
tions, but not in all.
In a model of acute liver inflammation, the sympathetic nervous system supports a
higher inflammatory state in the liver and systemically in the blood (Lin et al. 2015). In
the case of chemically induced acute colitis, an increased sympathetic tone is unfavora-
ble because it promotes inflammation (Straub et al. 2008).
A group of researchers found out how the local removal of sympathetic influence
on the heart reduced inflammation in the heart tissue after a heart attack (Ziegler et al.
2018). This finding was associated with better heart function (Zanoni et al. 2017). Even
on the gingival tissue the sympathetic influence is mainly proinflammatory in nature
(Breivik et al. 2005; Haug and Heyeraas 2006). If the doctor removes the sympathetic
influence on the kidney in humans by destroying the sympathetic renal nerves, the gen-
eral inflammatory state in the blood is reduced after 3 and 6 months (Zaldivia et al.
2017).
If the examiner stops the sympathetic influence on the head by surgical interruption
of the sympathetic nerve on both sides, the fever response to the peripheral administra-
tion of bacterial components is significantly reduced (Romeo et al. 2009). The increased
sympathetic tone therefore contributes to a fever reaction. Furthermore, sympathetic tone
promotes the inflammatory immediate reaction triggered by traumatic blood loss (Xu
et al. 2015).
The sympathetic tone also stimulates pain caused by different stimuli (Levine et al.
1986b; Kinnman and Levine 1995; Chen et al. 2010; Xie et al. 2016). This also applies
to abdominal pain, such as bloating (Kalmari et al. 2001). In this way, sympathetic tone
stimulates the pro-inflammatory effects mediated by the pain system (α-adrenergic, see
Fig. 4.3).
If the experimenter reduces the tone of the sympathetic nervous system in chronic
sleep disorders, the inflammation caused by the sleep disorder is attenuated (Mishra et al.
2020). On the one hand, we can see here the increase in sympathetic activity caused
by sleep disorders and, on the other hand, the downstream sympathetic-dependent
inflammation.
4.2 Direct Connectors Chronically Activate the Immune System (No. 1) 147

These global pro-inflammatory reactions make it clear how sympathetic tone directly
affects inflammation. The sympathetic nervous system is a direct connector in the sense
of chronic immune system activation. But now I have to pour cold water on the matter
because the sympathetic nervous system sometimes has anti-inflammatory properties.
This picture, which contradicts the concept of chronic immune system activation, will be
discussed in the next subchapter.

4.2.1.3 The Tone of the Sympathetic Nervous System Inhibits


Inflammation
Sometimes the observer can see an anti-inflammatory effect instead of a pro-inflamma-
tory effect of the sympathetic nervous system in the same disease. That sounds pretty
paradoxical, but can sometimes be resolved quite well. We have conducted studies with
chronic inflammatory diseases: The sympathetic tone supported the acute inflammation
in arthritis and colitis, but the chronic inflammation was inhibited (Härle et al. 2005;
Straub et al. 2008). This may be due to different disease mechanisms that are switched
on at different stages of arthritis or colitis. The effect of the sympathetic nervous system
depends on several factors, which are briefly mentioned in Table 4.1.
Some studies have supported the idea of the immunosuppressive effect of sympathetic
tone (De Luigi et al. 1998; Lorton et al. 1999; Rice et al. 2002; Straub et al. 2005, 2008;
Pongratz et al. 2012; Willemze et al. 2018). The attentive observer begins to wonder why
there can be such differences, why the sympathetic nervous system can thus cause such
dual mechanisms. These considerations could fill an entire book, but here I can only give
a short explanation in order not to exceed the scope of this work.
A detailed discussion of some important factors mentioned in Table 4.1 is given in
the further text of the subchapter. Important are various adaptation reactions that
strongly change the effect of the anti-inflammatory β-adrenergic receptor.

4.2.1.4 The Sympathetic Nervous System Promotes Inflammation


Through Various Adaptation Reactions
Some rearrangement reactions that can result from an early traumatic experience and are
proinflammatory in nature are already shown in Table 4.1. Therefore, it is worth discuss-
ing a few of them in more detail, and I will start with the co-transmitter neuropeptide Y.
Increased prolonged stimulation of the sympathetic nerve fiber can lead to depletion
of neuropeptide Y at the sympathetic nerve ending. Since neuropeptide Y normally sup-
ports the β-adrenergic side as a co-transmitter at high firing rates of the nerves, the loss
of neuropeptide Y is associated with a lower effect through the β-adrenergic receptor.
The proinflammatory α-adrenergic effect prevails (Fig. 4.6).
For example, if there is a high resting tone after early traumatic experiences, the nerve
ending will increasingly become depleted of neuropeptide Y. This has not been investi-
gated so far, but this scenario is plausible. In the brain, however, neuropeptide Y is a neu-
rotransmitter that has been associated with the development of resilience (2.5.1) (Nahvi
and Sabban 2020).
148 4 Chronic Immune System Activation

Table 4.1  Factors that determine the pro- or anti-inflammatory effect of the sympathetic nervous
system (Pongratz and Straub 2013; Schaible and Straub 2014)
Factors
Immune stimulus and associated immune response to a particular disease
After all, not all types of diseases are always associated with the same immune reaction under the
same disease name. For example, there are at least two types of rheumatoid arthritis, one started
by B cells and the other by T cells (T helper type 1 or T helper type 17). The end result is always
joint inflammation, which the doctor does not see the hidden immune reactions behind. Since
sympathetic neurotransmitters have different effects on these different cell types, it can some-
times lead to stimulation and sometimes to inhibition. Nothing is fixed, because the relevant cell
type can change over the course of a disease. This is not only the case with rheumatoid arthritis,
but also with other autoimmune diseases
The importance of migration of immune cells promoted by the sympathetic nervous system. The
sympathetic nervous system promotes migration
The meaning of energy provided that can be important for immune reactions. The sympathetic
nervous system provides energy-rich substrates such as glucose and free fatty acids
The additional cell types involved in addition to the immune cells are important because they
react differently to sympathetic neurotransmitters (e.g. vascular endothelial or smooth muscle
cell versus epithelial cell, etc.)
The on and off of the sympathetic nervous system in relation to the triggering of the chronic
immune reaction (vaccination/immunization). In the acute phase, the sympathetic nervous system
is proinflammatory, in the chronic phase it is anti-inflammatory
The possible function of other neurotransmitters of the sympathetic nerve ending, such as neuro-
peptide Y, which has its own effect on immune cells and other cells. With long-term stimulation
of the sympathetic nerve fiber, the nerve ending becomes depleted of neuropeptide Y, and then
this influence is lost. This can be the case, for example, with a strong sympathetic response. This
reduces the effect via β-adrenergic receptors (see Fig. 4.3)
The concentration and type of sympathetic neurotransmitter (low concentrations act via α-adren-
ergic receptors that is pro-inflammatory, and high concentrations act via β-adrenergic receptors
anti-inflammatory, Fig. 4.3)
The variability of the presence of sympathetic nerve fibers in the tissue (low density, effect via
proinflammatory α-adrenergic receptors; high density, effect via β-adrenergic receptors)
The variability of the presence of adrenergic receptors on the surface of involved cells, especially
immune cells (that is, whether there are many or few α- or β-adrenergic receptors)
The variability of signal transduction from the adrenergic receptor into the respective cell. This
can vary greatly

Furthermore, the sympathetic stress response to an acute stimulus has the following
significance. After early trauma experiences, the sympathetic stress response was often
described as being reduced (in the case of psychosocial stress, Holochwost et al. 2021),
but in the case of physical stimuli, there are excessive sympathetic reactions (Rinnewitz
et al. 2018). Unfortunately, these physical stimuli have been little studied in patients with
early trauma, but it would be worth undertaking.
4.2 Direct Connectors Chronically Activate the Immune System (No. 1) 149

Noradrenaline Neuropeptide Y

Bacteria
wall
component

permanently
normal tone one time low tone high tone
high tone underreaction overshooting

NPY supports NPY Support


-action of -activity is
missing

anti-inflammatory pro-inflammatory pro-inflammatory

Fig. 4.6 Proinflammatory reactions in case of excessive sympathetic activity. Normally, at low sym-
pathetic activity, there is a balanced ratio of α- and β-adrenergic effects on stimulated TNF secretion
(leftmost field, bottom). With a onetime higher tone, the β-adrenergic side prevails, which is supported
by neuropeptide Y (NPY), and there is a TNF inhibition (2nd field from left, bottom). With low tone or
low sympathetic reaction, the α-adrenergic side prevails, and there is an increase in TNF (3rd field from
left, bottom). If there is a high sympathetic tone at rest or regular excessive sympathetic reactions, the
nerve terminal may become depleted of NPY, and NPY can no longer support the β-adrenergic side. So
the α-adrenergic side prevails with an increase in TNF. The α- and β-adrenergic modulation of TNF was
investigated by Spengler and colleagues (Spengler et al. 1990, 1994)
150 4 Chronic Immune System Activation

If the patients suffer from anxiety disorders, depression, weight gain, sleep disor-
ders, metabolic syndrome, alcohol abuse and nicotine abuse, they often have the status
of the physically untrained, and here the doctor typically expects excessive sympathetic
responses after physical stress exposure (Carter and Ray 2015; an example of an anxi-
ety disorder in Coupland et al. 2003). Untrained individuals have a continuously higher
sympathetic tone, which could be eliminated through training (Carter and Ray 2015). In
the case of excessive stress responses, the firing rates at the sympathetic nerves are sig-
nificantly increased, and so the parallel influence of neuropeptide Y is lost (Fig. 4.6). The
α-adrenergic proinflammatory side may predominate because the neuropeptide Y-related
help for the β-adrenergic receptor is lost (Fig. 4.6).
If additional pro-inflammatory factors are added to those affected by early trau-
matic adversity—as we discuss in this book—the balance of anti- and pro-inflammatory
can further shift in a pro-inflammatory direction. There is a very good explanation for
this based on the β-adrenergic receptor. Interestingly, in inflammatory conditions, the
β-adrenergic receptor on immune cells is subject to a switch with a different outcome.
Contrary to the inhibitory effects on the secretion of the highly pro-inflammatory TNF
shown in Fig. 4.6, the β-adrenergic receptor is switched so that it has an α-adrenergic
effect that increases TNF secretion Fig. 4.7.
The α-adrenergic effect supports the inflammatory side because signal transduction
into the cell occurs differently here. The switching processes inside the cell are partly
understood (Daaka et al. 1997; Baillie et al. 2003; Lorton et al. 2013; Jenei-Lanzl et al.
2015b; Zhang et al. 2018). A psychological stress reaction can, via this switch, induce an
important pro-inflammatory intracellular signalling factor—NFkappaB (Bierhaus et al.
2003).
In addition, inflammation can lead to a depletion of tetrahydrobiopterin, an important
cofactor in the biosynthesis of noradrenaline (Werner et al. 1993, 2011). An inflammato-
ry-induced vicious circle can stabilize and aggravate the condition. Another variant that
causes a β- to α-adrenergic proinflammatory switch is a loss of sympathetic nerve fibers
in an inflammatory area. Two possibilities can cause nerve fiber loss.
If there is inflammation in a tissue, activated macrophages and other cells can produce
substances that cause an active repulsion of sympathetic nerve fibers from the inflamma-
tory area (so-called semaphorins) (Miller et al. 2004; Straub et al. 2008; Fassold et al.
2009; Koeck et al. 2009; Stangl et al. 2015). In the second case, a tissue loses its sympa-
thetic nerve fibers when it grows and enlarges and when the adequate ingrowth of sym-
pathetic nerve fibers fails, as we can observe in intestinal polyps (Graf et al. 2012).
In both cases—with active repulsion and with staying away in growing tissue—
the area becomes impoverished in sympathetic nerve fibers (Straub et al. 2008; Koeck
et al. 2009; Graf et al. 2012; Pongratz and Straub 2013; Schaible and Straub 2014).
This causes the concentrations of noradrenaline to slip into the low to very low range,
and there are α-adrenergic proinflammatory effects of noradrenaline. At the same
time, pain fibers grow more strongly into the inflamed area and create an imbalance of
4.2 Direct Connectors Chronically Activate the Immune System (No. 1) 151

Noradrenaline

Bacteria
wall SWITCH
component

TNF
permanently
one time high low tone high tone
normal tone
tone underreaction overshooting

anti-inflammatory pro-inflammatory pro-inflammatory

because of SWITCH

Fig. 4.7 Pro-inflammatory reactions with altered signal transduction. Normally, in the absence of
inflammation and at rest there is a balanced ratio of α- and β-adrenergic effects (i.e. no influence on
TNF, far left, bottom). With a onetime increase in tone, the anti-inflammatory side predominates (2nd
from left, bottom). With inflammatory stimulation of the cell, there is a switch (SWITCH) in which the
β-adrenergic receptor becomes pro-inflammatory via α-adrenergic signal pathways. With low tone or
weak sympathetic response, the α-adrenergic side predominates, resulting in an increase in TNF (3rd
from left, bottom). With a permanently increased tone or with excessive sympathetic response and exis-
ting SWITCH, the α-adrenergic side predominates with a significant increase in TNF (far right, bottom)

proinflammatory substance P to anti-inflammatory noradrenaline (Pongratz and Straub


2013, 2014). The situation is illustrated in Fig. 4.8.
In a constellation with predominantly α-adrenergic action, noradrenaline supports
pain processing in the local area (Levine et al. 1986b; Kinnman and Levine 1995; Aley
et al. 1996; Chen et al. 2010; Xie et al. 2016), which promotes the release of inflamma-
tory substance P. Thus, the proinflammatory effect depends on the number of pain nerve
fibers present in the tissue. This number is variable and typically increases significantly
with inflammation (Straub et al. 2008; Pongratz and Straub 2013).
Another important phenomenon directly at the inflamed site is the production of
sympathetic neurotransmitters by immune cells. This allows them to intervene in local
processes in an unprecedented way (Mousa et al. 2004; Capellino et al. 2010, 2012;
152 4 Chronic Immune System Activation

healthy situation
ratio 1:1
sympathetic pain
nerve fibres nerve fibres
(noradrenaline, (substance P)
neuropeptide Y)

Tissue

alteration due to
inflammation

inflammatory situation
ratio 1:9
sympathetic pain
nerve fibres nerve fibres
(noradrenaline, (substance P)
neuropeptide Y)

Tissue

Fig. 4.8 Proinflammatory reactions after reduced sympathetic innervation. There is tissue without
(above) and with inflammation (below) inflammation. Without inflammation, there is a balance of sym-
pathetic and pain nerve fibers (ratio 1:1). In this situation, the concentration of the neurotransmitter nor-
adrenaline can be high and there can be an anti-inflammatory β-adrenergic effect. With inflammation,
sympathetic nerve fibers decrease (repulsion), and proinflammatory pain fibers with substance P predo-
minate (ratio 1:9). Now the noradrenaline concentration is low, and only a proinflammatory α-adrenergic
effect can occur

Jenei-Lanzl et al. 2015a). Here we still know little, but it looks as if the concentration of
the neurotransmitters released from these immune cells is not enough to inhibit inflam-
mation. Nevertheless, the β-adrenergic effect of the released noradrenaline is very small.
Prominent examples from Table 4.1 were presented in more detail above. Finally, we ask
the general question: Is the sympathetic nervous system now pro- or anti-inflammatory?
According to Fig. 4.4 I was a convinced supporter of the immunosuppressive effect
of the sympathetic nervous system from around 1990 to 2005. The verdict resulted from
studies of isolated cells or tissue with β-adrenergic stimulants, where the cells came from
a healthy human or animal. We took the immunological target cells for the observation
from a healthy environment and stimulated them only briefly with an infectious stimulus
(bacterial components). In these “healthy situations” onetime given noradrenaline acted
via β-adrenergic receptors to inhibit proinflammatory cytokines such as TNF (for exam-
ple Kees et al. 2003). This situation changes very much in stress and disease situations
4.2 Direct Connectors Chronically Activate the Immune System (No. 1) 153

(Dhabhar 2014; Pongratz and Straub 2014), because here there are long-term adaptation
reactions in the tissue and in the target cells (Figs. 4.6, 4.7 and 4.8). In addition, we rec-
ognized the proinflammatory importance of the α-adrenergic effect on various immune
cells (Bierhaus et al. 2003; Miksa et al. 2009; Grisanti et al. 2011; Lu et al. 2014; Liu
et al. 2020; Straub et al. 2020).
By now, the pro-inflammatory effect of the sympathetic nervous system prevails
for me for the aforementioned reasons (Figs. 4.6, 4.7 and 4.8 and Table 4.1). The most
important aspects—are above all—the strongly altered signal conduction through the
receptor (SWITCH in Fig. 4.7) and the nerve fiber loss in inflammation and prolifera-
tive tissue growth (Fig. 4.8). Therefore, you should not be surprised why a change in
sympathetic activity contributes to a generally increased inflammatory condition, regard-
less of whether there is an over- or underreaction. After all, sympathetic nerve fibers are
everywhere (exception: placenta, where they are prevented from growing in by the afore-
mentioned repulsion factors, as my good acquaintance Monika Brauer and her team in
Montevideo, Uruguay, were able to show [Marzioni et al. 2004; Brauer 2008; Richeri
et al. 2011]). This makes the role of the sympathetic nervous system as a direct connec-
tor obvious in order to promote chronic immune system activation in various places.

4.2.2 The Parasympathetic nervous system

When I speak of the parasympathetic nervous system, I am essentially referring to the


vagus nerve, which has been studied primarily. The sacral autonomic nerves that were
previously assigned to the parasympathetic nervous system are not parasympathetic, but
sympathetic nerve fibers (Espinosa-Medina et al. 2016).
In cases with a high sympathetic tone, we expect a low parasympathetic tone because
the two systems antagonize each other, and this fact has been common knowledge in
medicine for decades (e.g. Genovely and Pfeifer 1988). If an early traumatic experience
leads to a increased sympathetic tone or to excessive sympathetic responses, we expect
a low parasympathetic tone or an excessive inhibition of parasympathetic responses as a
consequence.
Using a respiratory sinus arrhythmia test (i.e. sinusoidal fluctuations in heart rate in
relation to deep breathing), the examiner can approximately determine the parasympa-
thetic tone. If the respiratory fluctuation is strong, the parasympathetic tone is high
(Genovely and Pfeifer 1988). Conversely, the respiratory fluctuation becomes very small
when only the sympathetic nervous system has an influence (Genovely and Pfeifer 1988).
When considering various adverse experiences in young years, researchers found,
with the help of this method, a low parasympathetic tone or a reduced reaction to stress
(Miskovic et al. 2009; Oosterman et al. 2010; Shenk et al. 2010; Skowron et al. 2011;
Gray et al. 2017; Lunkenheimer et al. 2018). Significantly fewer publications show no
reaction or an increased tone (summarized in Holochwost et al. 2021). This altered vagal
situation can have some effects on the immune system.
154 4 Chronic Immune System Activation

Kevin Tracey is a descendant of a Sicilian father and an Irish mother who studied
medicine in Boston. He was trained as a neurosurgeon in New York and soon moved to
the prestigious Feinstein Institute in New York. After a few years of scientific work, he
became president and CEO there, after publishing important publications (Borovikova
et al. 2000; Wang et al. 2003). The important observation of the vagal inhibition of
inflammation is due to Kevin Tracey and his group. Although he always emphasizes in
his work how mainly the vagus nerve plays the anti-inflammatory role in the inflamma-
tory reflex, he neglects the anti-inflammatory effects of the sympathetic nervous system
when it acts under acute “healthy conditions” (see above). Nevertheless, I recognize how
Kevin Tracey gave the vagus nerve this new and important anti-inflammatory meaning.
His group examined the acute effect of electrical vagus nerve stimulation on the
release of TNF induced by bacterial components in live animals (Borovikova et al.
2000). This acute effect of vagus nerve stimulation was only given if the spleen was
present in the body. If the researchers cut the vagus nerve in animals, they had a higher
TNF level in the blood, which spoke for the inhibitory effect of the parasympathetic
tone. Many groups confirmed the anti-inflammatory effects of the vagus nerve in model
diseases such as arthritis (Levine et al. 2014), chronic inflammatory bowel disease
(Meregnani et al. 2011), postoperative intestinal ileus (de Jonge et al. 2005), acute renal
artery occlusion and reperfusion (Inoue et al. 2016) etc. The essential effects were medi-
ated by the important neurotransmitter of the vagus nerve, the acetylcholine, and a spe-
cial acetylcholine receptor (name: α7nAChR) (Wang et al. 2003).
The work around the vagus nerve caused some criticism in professional circles
because anti-inflammatory effects were observed throughout. The attentive observer does
not expect such a large number of acetylcholine receptors (at least 20 different) to have
uniform anti- or pro-inflammatory effects, but a mixed picture, as we saw with the adr-
energic receptor and noradrenaline. I don’t want to go into this here, because an anti-in-
flammatory role of the vagus nerve is pretty clear.
With this information we can now briefly summarize: Since the parasympathetic tone
and the parasympathetic reaction to early trauma and later stressful second reactions are
often reduced in the face of a high sympathetic tone, there is a clear pro-inflammatory
constellation. We recognize another important direct connector in acetylcholine and in
the vagus nerve.

4.2.3 The HPA Axis

Before we steer towards inflammation, I would like to briefly introduce the HPA axis
here first, even though this partly already took place in Fig. 4.2. The corresponding feed-
back regulations are shown in Fig. 4.9. Cortisol production in the adrenal cortex is influ-
enced by the brain and also by peripheral factors. Figure 4.9 likewise shows the effect
of cortisol inside the cell and the intracellular negative feedback via a protein called
FKBP51.
4.2 Direct Connectors Chronically Activate the Immune System (No. 1) 155

other brain regions


ce Hippocampus
see Fig. 4.2 en
flu

n
yi
tor
Cortisol Cortisol

inhibi
Hypothalamus Cortisol
(PVN)

CRH

Pituitary Cortisol

ACTH

Adrenal
cortex

intravascular intravascular
Cortisol

Cortisol
Glucocorticoid
Cortisol
receptor FKBP51 blocks
FKBP51 the glucocorticoid
receptor
Inhibition or
Cort
stimulation of
isol
gene reading
Immune cell FKBP51

or any other for example


cell in the stimulation of
FKBP51
body Nucleus

Fig. 4.9 The HPA axis with feedback loops. The stimulus for the activation of the hypothalamus comes
from various brain regions, as schematically illustrated in Fig. 4.2 and as shown in the upper left of
the image. More specifically, this is the paraventricular nucleus (PVN). There, corticotropin releasing
hormone (CRH) is produced, and this leads in the pituitary to the production of adrenocorticotropic hor-
mone (ACTH). ACTH stimulates the adrenal cortex to produce cortisol, which has a negative (with bars
at the end of the line) or positive (with an arrow at the end of the line) effect. The effect on the memory
center of the hippocampus is positive, and the hippocampus in turn inhibits the hypothalamus. Cortisol
also affects other brain regions. Cortisol enters cells quite unhindered and exerts its effect there when it
binds to the intracellular glucocorticoid receptor (brown square U-shape) and enters the cell nucleus with
it to reach the DNA. The complex binds to the DNA and causes the stimulation of factors (e.g. FKBP51)
or the inhibition of factors (e.g. TNF, not shown). FKBP51 blocks the glucocorticoid receptor and can
thus reduce the effect of cortisol in the cell (local intracellular, negative feedback)
156 4 Chronic Immune System Activation

The effect of cortisol in the cell can inhibit and increase gene reading of various fac-
tors. For example, cortisol inhibits the new formation of TNF and other proinflammatory
cytokines. In contrast, it increases the generation of other factors, such as the anti-in-
flammatory cytokine IL-10 or FKBP51, which is shown in Fig. 4.9. FKBP51 blocks the
glucocorticoid receptor in the cell, so that this cannot bind free cortisol. FKBP51 thus
inhibits the glucocorticoid effect in the sense of a negative feedback within the cell.

4.2.3.1 What is the Tone and Stress Reactivity in Previous Early


Traumas
In the systematic review by Holochwost et al. from 2021, the effects of earlier adverse
experiences on the later investigated tone of the HPA axis and the later acute stress reac-
tion are summarized from many studies (Holochwost et al. 2021). Of a total of 37 studies
published by different authors, 19 describe a tonic overactivity of the HPA axis, 5 an
underactivity, and 7 publications find no effects. In other words: Most observers find a
tonic overactivity of the HPA axis, and that fits very well with findings in depression, a
common consequence of earlier traumatic experiences (Rothe et al. 2020).
There the situation is clear, because depression is associated with a higher tone of
the HPA axis and with glucocorticoid resistance (newer overview in Rothe et al. 2020).
Glucocorticoid resistance means nothing other than the lack of effect of cortisol via its
glucocorticoid receptor (Fig. 4.9), because we see no hormone deficiency, but a reduc-
tion in efficacy at high concentrations of cortisol in the blood (Rohleder et al. 2003a).
In terms of reactivity to stress, we experience the opposite of increased tone, as there
is a decrease in HPA axis activity after a typical psychosocial stressor, similar to what we
see with the sympathetic nervous system. Holochwost found 11 studies in total that doc-
umented the decrease in activity, and 6 out of 23 studies showed overactivity. As with the
sympathetic nervous system, physical stress such as the cold water stress test can trigger
overactivity (Rinnewitz et al. 2018). Only one out of 23 studies showed no reaction of
the HPA axis at all (Holochwost et al. 2021). In summary, I find that most studies report
an increased baseline tone and a reduced acute stress reaction of the HPA axis.
However, tonic overactivity is not always present, and we do not know the reasons for
this with certainty. Let me remind you again of Thomas Boyce. In one study, he found an
inverse U-shaped relationship between early stress-inducing experiences and later cor-
tisol response. For people with high or low levels of stress, the cortisol stress response
was high. For people with a moderate level of stress in childhood, the later cortisol stress
response was smaller (Shakiba et al. 2020). Such considerations can explain the discrep-
ancy in the cortisol stress response.
Further explanations for sometimes high and sometimes low stress responses are pro-
vided by studies that examine the timing of the trauma in relation to the time of puberty.
These studies show that people with a trauma before or at the beginning of puberty
have a high tone and a reduced stress response—and those after puberty have a lower
stress tone and a high stress response (King et al. 2017; DePasquale et al. 2019; Gunnar
et al. 2019; Khoury et al. 2019). Here, sex hormones can play an important role, as
4.2 Direct Connectors Chronically Activate the Immune System (No. 1) 157

their influence on the HPA axis is well known (Kirschbaum et al. 1996; Rohleder et al.
2003b). The sex hormones are discussed in the Sect. 4.2.6 The tone of the sex hormone
axis.
Further investigations to clarify the differences in HPA responses point to the type of
trauma, whether physical abuse, sexual abuse, emotional abuse, physical or emotional
neglect, etc. Depending on the situation, there is a high or low tone or a low or high
stress reactivity of the HPA axis (Essex et al. 2011; Kuhlman et al. 2015; Laceulle et al.
2017; Khoury et al. 2019). In Chap. 5 I make a further suggestion based on energy con-
sumption how this discrepancy of the high and low response patterns of the HPA axis
can be explained.
In summary, after traumas, usually a higher tone of the HPA axis and a reduced stress
response are found. In this respect, the findings are very similar to those in the studies of
the sympathetic nervous system.

4.2.3.2 The Increased Tone of the HPA Axis Increases Inflammation


First indications of an immunostimulatory effect of preceding increased cortisol levels
were found in humans in a simple-looking experiment (Barber et al. 1993). The research-
ers gave volunteers a defined amount of bacterial components intravenously over 5 min
to simulate an inflammatory situation. By the way, this is a good experimental idea
because after stress bacteria can cross from the intestinal lumen into the body (see Sect.
4.3.5 The permeability of the gut is pro-inflammatory below), where bacterial compo-
nents occur more frequently.
Components of bacteria trigger an immediate inflammation reaction, which the
researcher detects by measuring proinflammatory cytokines such as TNF and IL-6 in
the blood (Barber et al. 1993). Surprisingly, this inflammation reaction was significantly
increased in subjects to whom the investigators had infused a larger amount of cortisol
intravenously 12 hours or 6 days earlier. We can call this pre-infusion of cortisol the
“cortisol-priming”. If subjects received cortisol at the same time as components of bacte-
ria, the inflammation reaction was strongly suppressed. Figure 4.10 summarizes the find-
ings for TNF.
The data for C-reactive protein, serum IL-6, and soluble TNF receptor in serum look
very similar to those in Fig. 4.10, and the working group confirmed these findings in fur-
ther studies two years later (Barber et al. 1995). In the discussion of the publications, the
authors recognize the danger of the preceding high cortisol levels as a possible cause of
overshooting inflammatory responses. Even if this constellation does not exactly corre-
spond to what we call a high tone of the HPA axis, these studies provide first indications
of a pro-inflammatory effect of hypercortisolemia.
These work from the mid-1990s were later confirmed by other researchers in an inde-
pendent manner (Yeager et al. 2009), and they confirm earlier work on rats (Renz et al.
1992). In a more recent study in humans, the scientists confirm the cortisol-priming on
the acute inflammatory reaction subsequently triggered by bacterial components. They
show an enhancing effect on pro-inflammatory factors after a 12-hour preceding cortisol
158 4 Chronic Immune System Activation

LPS + Cortisol 6 days ago

Serum TNF (pg/ml)


LPS + cortisol 12 hours ago

LPS

LPS + cortisol simultaneously

Time (min)

Fig. 4.10 Cortisol tone and increased inflammation. If the researcher gives bacterial components (LPS,
lipopolysaccharides) intravenously, he can trigger an inflammation reaction in humans (red curve). If he
gives the LPS at the same time as the cortisol (black line), the TNF production is completely inhibited
(34 pg/ml is the detection limit of the measurement procedure). If the researcher had infused cortisol eit-
her 12 hours (light violet) or 6 days (dark violet) before the LPS, the inflammation reaction was strongly
increased. Thus, hypercortisolism, in other words an increased HPA-axis tone, can contribute to an incre-
ased inflammation reaction. The experiments were carried out by Barber et al. The figure summarizes
results from the study group in graphical form using tabular data of the original publication (Barber et al.
1993)

infusion that produced cortisol levels comparable to a strong stress response (Kamisoglu
et al. 2014).
In this work, they demonstrated the increased migration of leukocytes—especially
neutrophilic granulocytes (short: neutrophils)—already before and especially after
administration of the bacterial components (Kamisoglu et al. 2014). These findings
in humans confirm a supportive effect of glucocorticoids on leukocyte migration, as
observed in animals.
The migration of leukocytes such as granulocytes and monocytes stimulated by corti-
sol in humans leads to an increased presence of these cells in inflammatory regions. This
is due to an increase in cytokines that are important for the directed movement into the
inflammatory area (called: chemokines) (Yeager et al. 2016). A preceding cortisol-prim-
ing thus promotes increased migration and presence in the inflammatory area, which can
fuel the inflammation.
If the researcher looks at immune cells in a culture dish, the three-day treatment with
low-dose cortisol compared to a situation without cortisol leads to a significantly higher
4.2 Direct Connectors Chronically Activate the Immune System (No. 1) 159

release of TNF, IL-6 and other pro-inflammatory factors when now stimulated with bac-
terial components after the cortisol-priming. Ergo, this cortisol-priming exists in the cul-
ture dish to make the cells more pro-inflammatory (Chae 2021).
If the experimenter gives rats corticosteroids in the drinking water (increased tone)
for 14 days and now obtains their macrophages from the lungs, the macrophages of the
animals respond with cortisol priming to additional administration of bacterial compo-
nents with a significantly stronger TNF response, which is about 8- to 10-times stronger
than in animals without corticosteroids (Renz et al. 1992). Further studies on immune
cells in culture also show stimulating effects of cortisol on pro-inflammatory factors
from B cells, T cells, macrophages or mixed blood leukocytes (Cooper et al. 1981;
Wiegers et al. 1995; Broug-Holub and Kraal 1996; Wiegers et al. 2000; Lim et al. 2007).
Corticosteroids induce a special form of T cell (T helper type 2 cell), which can play a
role in allergies and autoimmune diseases (Ramírez et al. 1996; DeKruyff et al. 1998;
Franchimont et al. 2002; de Castro Kroner et al. 2018; Shimba and Ikuta 2020; Taves
and Ashwell 2021).
At this point, the experienced reader begins to wonder why corticosteroids like cor-
tisol can be pro-inflammatory instead of anti-inflammatory. Obviously, the anti-in-
flammatory effect at high concentrations of corticosteroids is a clear matter, and this is
supported by the success of acute therapy with corticosteroids (Buttgereit et al. 2011). So
what makes cortisol pro-inflammatory? On the one hand, the pro-inflammatory effect of
corticosteroids has something to do with the time of action, because with cortisol prim-
ing the tone of the corticosteroids must be increased before the immunostimulus is given.
An increase in cortisol that arrives at the same time as the bacterial components—that
is, cortisol simultaneity—inhibits inflammation. With cortisol priming, it is as if the
immune cells are prepared for the immunostimulus that arrives later.
On the other hand, the lack of effect of glucocorticoids may be due to the concentra-
tion of this hormone. For the anti-inflammatory effect, high concentrations of about and
greater than 10−7 mol/l are required. With regard to the effect on macrophages, a low
concentration of glucocorticoids in the dose range of 10−10 to 10−8 mol/l is even stim-
ulating (in the blood the doctor normally observes 10−7 mol/l) (Giles et al. 2001; Lim
et al. 2007; Zhou et al. 2010). And finally we can ask ourselves the following question:
What happens if the receptor for the anti-inflammatory cortisol is not produced in suf-
ficient quantities or if the movement of cortisol together with its receptor is prevented
from entering the cell nucleus (cf. Fig. 4.9)? Does this happen after early traumatic
experiences?

4.2.3.3 Glucocorticoid Receptor Resistance Increases Inflammation


General high glucocorticoid levels—regardless of their nature—lead to reduced sensi-
tivity of their own receptor: a high HPA-axis tone, with multiple high stress reactions
or after administration of glucocorticoids leads to glucocorticoid receptor resistance.
This is textbook knowledge. The reduced sensitivity is due to a real deficiency of the
receptor molecule and an increase in the receptor blocker FKBP51 (cf. Fig. 4.9).
160 4 Chronic Immune System Activation

If FKBP51 is increased, the binding of cortisol to the glucocorticoid receptor is inhib-


ited and the movement of the glucocorticoid receptor into the cell nucleus is prevented.
Glucocorticoids such as cortisol can then not exert their effects. But what about the glu-
cocorticoid receptor after early traumatic experiences?
In systematic studies of the facts in leukocytes, researchers found the following. After
early traumatic experiences, the amount of the glucocorticoid receptor is significantly
reduced, which is mediated by epigenetic processes (Explanation 1). This finding could
be confirmed in 89% of all studies conducted on human material (Turecki and Meaney
2016). Another systematic review confirms the facts (Holmes Jr. et al. 2019). The glu-
cocorticoid receptor mechanism is disrupted in white blood cells, and this means that at
higher glucocorticoid levels in the blood or tissue, the effect is reduced. So the range of
action of the glucocorticoids is shifted to higher concentrations (red line in Fig. 4.11).
In the Sect. 2.5.5 I wrote how the researchers often use leukocytes to detect the evi-
dence of methylation of important gene sections. I doubted this approach because the
things that were changed should be in the center of attention, namely the brain. For
the consideration of immunological processes, however, the analysis of leukocytes is
extremely valuable because these cells trigger inflammation.
Another problem of glucocorticoid resistance is the insensitivity to cortisol in various
places in the brain, because the same resistance is relevant there. Although it has not
been epigenetically examined in the brain, the clinical findings speak in favor of a gluco-
corticoid resistance in the hypothalamus and in the pituitary gland. Therefore, the nega-
tive feedback of Fig. 4.9 is weakened and the production of cortisol remains high (Rothe
et al. 2020). However, not all regions in the brain are resistant to cortisol. Some regions
Immune response

Immunostimulation

Immunosuppression

therapeutical range

Cortisol concentration in blood (mol/l)

Fig. 4.11 Anti-inflammatory and pro-inflammatory range of action of cortisol. The usual range of
action of cortisol is adequately represented by the black line. If we observe pro-inflammatory reactions
of cortisol, the concentration is 10−8 mol/l or less. By glucocorticoid resistance, the dose-response curve
is shifted to the right to higher values, as schematically represented by the red curve. Now the researcher
suddenly observes at concentrations of 10−7 to 10−8 mol/l pro-inflammatory immunostimulatory effects
of cortisol. Any form of desensitization of the receptor leads to a higher inflammatory effect of cortisol
4.2 Direct Connectors Chronically Activate the Immune System (No. 1) 161

are sensitive and therefore suffer particularly from high concentrations of this hormone
(Rothe et al. 2020). This situation exacerbates the central nervous system problems such
as depression and anxiety disorders (Sect. in 3.2), and the inclination of the pointer on
the weighing scale from Fig. 3.4 is stabilized and amplified. What are the other reasons
for the lack of effect of cortisol in addition to glucocorticoid resistance?
After early traumatic situations, an intracellular increase of the FKBP51 protein was
found on the basis of epigenetic influences (significance: see Fig. 4.9). An increase in
FKBP51 activity has an pro-inflammatory constellation as a result (Zannas et al. 2019),
and here direct effects of FKBP51 on inflammatory factors and additionally a gluco-
corticoid resistance are relevant. Furthermore, genetic effects can come into play with
FKBP51, because certain genetic changes of the FKBP51 gene are associated with dis-
eases such as depression (Wang et al. 2018).
For the sympathetic nervous system, I wrote: “If additional inflammatory factors
are added to those affected by early traumatic adversity (…), the balance of anti- and
pro-inflammatory can further shift in the pro-inflammatory direction.” We can use the
same sentence when considering the HPA axis, because additional inflammatory activa-
tion increases glucocorticoid resistance (Pace et al. 2007; Quax et al. 2013). The black
curve in Fig. 4.11 moves to the right due to inflammation. Here, a vicious circle can eas-
ily develop if an increased inflammatory state has once begun, promotes glucocorticoid
resistance and further increases the inflammatory state, which in turn promotes glucocor-
ticoid resistance, and so on.

4.2.3.4 An Increased Breakdown of Cortisol Increases Inflammation


Another way to reduce the effect of cortisol in tissue is an increased breakdown of cor-
tisol by a cortisol-degrading enzyme (it is called 11β-Hydroxysteroid Dehydrogenase
Type 2). Researchers have shown the possibility of why this enzyme is increased in pla-
cental tissue of prenatally traumatized newborns. They observed a reduced methylation
of the gene for this enzyme (means increased production, see Explanation 1), the cor-
tisol-degrading enzyme was more present and local cortisol levels were low (Appleton
et al. 2013). In the animal experiment with prenatal stress, there was also an increased
concentration of the enzyme in the placenta (Lucassen et al. 2009). On the other hand,
the loss or reduced function of this enzyme during pregnancy in the child leads to higher
cortisol levels and various consequences such as low birth weight, lifelong increased
cortisol levels, high blood pressure and cardiovascular consequences (Seckl and Holmes
2007; Yehuda et al. 2009). Since immune cells also have this enzyme and promote cor-
tisol breakdown there, it can easily lead to an increased inflammatory state due to the
lower cortisol levels (Schmidt and Straub 2015).

4.2.3.5 Cortisol Mobilizes Important Elements of Inflammation


The stress hormone cortisol is an important factor for the migration of leukocytes.
People who are stressed and come to the emergency room for some reason often have
very high leukocyte counts in their blood. The treating physician then often thinks of an
162 4 Chronic Immune System Activation

infection, as in this case typically many leukocytes swim in the blood. This does not nec-
essarily have to be the case, as the stress reaction alone with high cortisol values ​​already
causes an increase in leukocyte counts. We had already discussed the matter along with
the sympathetic nervous system, as noradrenaline has the same effect. Here we see again
how cortisol and noradrenaline do the same thing.
Furthermore, cortisol like noradrenaline is important for the provision of energy-rich
substrates from energy stores, for example free fatty acids or glucose (Straub 2018).
Without these energy-rich substrates, the immune system cannot work because immune
reactions are very energy-consuming. We see the importance of cortisol for energy pro-
vision when the pituitary is defective for reasons of illness. If the doctor injects bacte-
ria components into such persons with weak pituitary function, the provision reaction
of free fatty acids is clearly lower than in healthy control persons (Bach et al. 2016). In
contrast, people with increased HPA axis activity—that is, the opposite of weak pitui-
tary function—permanently make more energy-rich substrates such as glucose, free fatty
acids and triglycerides available in the blood, and this situation can drive the inflamma-
tory reaction.

4.2.3.6 Summary of the HPA Axis and Inflammation


All of the mentioned aspects can, with increased tone of the HPA axis instead of the
usually expected anti-inflammatory effect of cortisol, cause a pro-inflammatory situ-
ation. Let’s also imagine how cortisol repeatedly drops in phases that trigger stress—
this has been described as reduced stress response or stress reaction. Repeated drops in
bood cortisol would further exacerbate the existing problem of the lack of cortisol-in-
duced immunosuppression if stressful life circumstances occur. This is especially true
for psychosocial stressors (Holochwost et al. 2021), whereas with physical stressors—or
with sports—high glucocorticoid levels are to be expected and therefore an inhibition of
inflammation can be expected.
So far, we have only talked about the constellation of permanently increased HPA-
axis tone and reduced stress reactivity. But what about the low HPA-axis tone and
increased stress reactivity that have been observed in a few studies? Does this constel-
lation have an inflammation-promoting effect? The logic of the matter is shown in Fig.
4.12. Basically, this latter constellation has not been studied often, which is why corre-
sponding data is lacking. I can imagine how a low HPA-axis tone and intermittent high
stress reactions can neither properly inhibit inflammation nor properly increase it (Fig.
4.12). Here, a stalemate exists, which can quickly be diverted in the wrong direction
towards higher glucocorticoid resistance in the presence of additional inflammation, and
that means reduced glucocorticoid effects and increased inflammation.
Thus, the HPA axis behaves similarly to the sympathetic nervous system with cor-
tisol, an anti-inflammatory hormone, because stress reactions interfere with the anti-in-
flammatory effects of the two systems. The altered function of the receptors of cortisol
and noradrenaline is decisive. In this constellation, there is the danger of the vicious
circle illustrated in Fig. 4.13. The early traumatic episode triggers the stress reaction
leading to an altered glucocorticoid receptor, an altered adrenergic receptor system and
4.2 Direct Connectors Chronically Activate the Immune System (No. 1) 163

Glucocorticoid
receptor FKBP51 blocks
the glucocorticoid
receptor
Inhibition or
stimulation of
gene reading
Immune cell
or any other for example
cell in the stimulation of
body Nucleus FKBP51

with early traumatic experiences

normal tone high tone low tone


normal stress low stress high stress
response reaction response

Glucocorticoid receptor

Cortisol effect Cortisol effect Cortisol effect


good very weak weak-good

Inflammation Inflammation Inflammation

Fig. 4.12 Summary of the HPA axis and inflammation. In this figure, the normal situation is shown
in the lower left, in which the cortisol concentration in the blood, the number of glucocorticoid recep-
tors and the natural cortisol blocker FKBP51 are normal. In the presence of inflammation and with ade-
quate cortisol levels, inflammation is well suppressed.—In the middle, the constellation with increased
HPA-axis tone and low stress reaction is shown. The glucocorticoid receptor is reduced by the increa-
sed tone and the previous trauma, and at the same time FKBP51 is intracellularly increased due to the
trauma (epigentic reasons): too little receptor and too much receptor blocker. Here, the cortisol effect is
weak, which contributes to the increased inflammation.—The opposite reaction of the HPA axis, which
has been documented in a few studies, is shown in the lower right. There, although the HPA-axis tone
is low—and so the glucocorticoid receptor may indeed be higher than in the middle—, the number of
molecules of the glucocorticoid receptor is not as high as in the normal situation (left) due to the previ-
ous trauma. At the same time, the number of FKBP51 molecules is higher than in the normal situation
due to the previous trauma. The inflammation is probably not properly suppressed, but otherwise not
increased (arrow up and down, the stalemate)
164 4 Chronic Immune System Activation

late disease
5. aggravated glucocorticoid
problem
receptor resistance,
aggravated switching of
the adrenergic receptor
towards α

3. enhanced α-adrenergic
effects of noradrenaline
5. increase in inflammation

2. increase in inflammation
4. increase in inflammation

1. methylations: Glucocorticoid
receptor low, FKBP51 increased,
cortisol effect on immune system early trauma
reduced

Fig. 4.13 Vicious circle of stress reactions and inflammation

consequently to inflammation, which further exacerbates the unfavorable situation. This


makes the importance of cortisol and the HPA axis as a direct connector apparent in
order to promote chronic immune system activation at various sites.

4.2.4 Disruption of the Circadian Rhythm

You know in healthy people the ups and downs of hormone levels in the blood during the
day. So the body’s own cortisol in the blood at 7 am is maximally high and at midnight
very low. But at midnight, melatonin and prolactin are maximally high and in the morn-
ing both hormones are minimal. We recognize different hormones that behave totally
contrary to each other like cortisol and prolactin and totally in phase like melatonin and
prolactin. This is not a random whim of nature! It is a deliberate desynchronization and
synchronization, because in this way the hormones can act separately or at the same
time. Why does man need something like this?
In synchronization, two hormones can act synergistically or additively, following the
motto “Together we are stronger”. With desynchronization, hormones often have oppo-
site effects and can act separately. Synchronization applies to melatonin and prolactin
because they both have a pro-inflammatory component that they develop together in the
early hours of the night to promote a special form of nocturnal immune response (Lange
et al. 2006; Lange et al. 2011). Cortisol would prevent this, which is why this hormone
is minimal at midnight. From these different rhythms, diseases such as morning stiffness
4.2 Direct Connectors Chronically Activate the Immune System (No. 1) 165

of the hands and feet in certain rheumatic diseases (Straub and Cutolo 2007; Buttgereit
et al. 2008) can now be explained.
Synchronized partners in this sense are cortisol and noradrenaline, because both reach
their maximum in the early morning hours. The partnership of the two hormones is often
evidenced by a mutual reinforcement of effects. The two signal paths help each other in
the cell by prolonging and amplifying each other’s effects (Oikarinen et al. 1984; Gruol
et al. 1986; Nakada et al. 1987; Dong et al. 1989; DiBattista et al. 1991; Korn et al. 1998;
Eickelberg et al. 1999; Schmidt et al. 2001). Throughout the body, they promote each
other by cortisol stimulating the production of noradrenaline (Brion et al. 1978; Schubert
et al. 1980) and vice versa noradrenaline stimulating the production of cortisol (Ehrhart-
Bornstein et al. 1998).
The synchronization of cortisol and noradrenaline is crucial for the daily morning
to afternoon immunosuppression (Straub and Cutolo 2007). So the morning symptoms
in patients with rheumatic and other inflammatory diseases get better throughout the
morning and afternoon because these two hormones inhibit inflammation (Straub and
Cutolo 2007; Buttgereit et al. 2008). Only in the night hours up to the early morning do
the inflammatory symptoms increase again due to the lack of these two hormones while
nocturnal secretion of pro-inflammatory hormones such as melatonin and prolactin takes
place. These considerations have already led to the development of new anti-inflamma-
tory glucocorticoids that are specifically released at 2 a.m. to inhibit the morning rise in
inflammation (Buttgereit et al. 2008; Buttgereit et al. 2013). If the circadian rhythm of
these two hormones is disturbed, a reduced anti-inflammatory effect is expected.

4.2.4.1 Circadian Rhythm After Early Traumatic Experiences


First considerations in the direction of disturbed circadian rhythm in children with pre-
vious trauma came up in the early 1990s when observers noticed disturbed sleep-wake
rhythms (Glod 1992). In rats after prenatal stress, other investigators observed phase
shifts in glucocorticoid secretion and higher levels throughout the day in the late 1990s
(Koehl et al. 1997, 1999; Mastorci et al. 2009). First work on circadian rhythm disorders
in adults and children with early traumatic experiences appeared around 2010 (Gonzalez
et al. 2009; Saridjan et al. 2010).
By looking at several measurements from waking up to falling asleep, which are typ-
ically captured in saliva samples, the doctor can recognize increases, decreases, and flat
curves—that is, disturbances of the normal secretion—of the HPA axis hormone corti-
sol. The situation with early traumatic experiences was summarized by Holochwost and
colleagues in 2021 (Holochwost et al. 2021). Of 10 studies, 8 reported an increase in
rhythmic cortisol levels, which was accompanied by a visibly flatter circadian rhythm.
“The system doesn’t swing right anymore. Keyword: flat and high!” Only two of the 10
studies showed a decrease in the cortisol day curve (Holochwost et al. 2021).
Unfortunately, there are no studies in which cortisol and noradrenaline or another
measure of the sympathetic nervous system such as neuropeptide Y (Fig. 4.6) were
measured together. Such findings could provide information about synchronization or
166 4 Chronic Immune System Activation

phase shifts of the two hormones. Disruptions of synchronization disturb a common


anti-inflammatory effect (Straub et al. 2002; Meyer-Hermann et al. 2009).
So it is only logical that we describe the observed changes in the sympathetic nervous
system and HPA axis—especially the flat circadian rhythm—in the presence of disturbed
receptor mechanisms as a proinflammatory signal that is stimulated by the shift of the
adrenergic receptor system (Figs. 4.6, 4.7 and 4.8) and the glucocorticoid receptor sys-
tem (Fig. 4.12). This again shows the role of the sympathetic nervous system and the
HPA axis as direct connectors and how they promote chronic immune system activation
by a disturbance of the circadian rhythm.

4.2.5 Tone of the Pain Pathways

Painful stimuli reach the dorsal root ganglion in the periphery via sensory nociceptive nerve
fibers and are received in the dorsal horn of the spinal cord, once switched to the opposite
side and travel up the spinal cord to regions in the brainstem and midbrain, from where
they are forwarded to the cerebral cortex (Fig. 4.14). Painful stimuli can be heat, cold,
mechanical stimuli, acidic pH, immunological messenger substances such as cytokines, the
chili extract capsaicin, bacterial components, etc. Figure 4.14 shows a selection of the pos-
sible pain stimuli that can act on the nerve ending. Typically, with pain there is also sensiti-
zation of the pain pathway at different levels. Sensitization means that, in the long term, the
readiness to forward pain stimuli from the periphery to the brain is increased.
The sensory nerve fiber is often of afferent nature (i.e. periphery → brain), but it
also has a strong efferent aspect (brain → periphery, Fig. 4.14, pink pathway). With this
efferent side, the sensory nerve fiber exerts a constant tone by ensuring a constant release
of neurotransmitters into the area of the nerve ending. These neurotransmitters can con-
tribute significantly to the local inflammatory situation. They are the key molecules of
“neurogenic inflammation” (Basbaum et al. 2009). The central neurotransmitter is sub-
stance P, a peptide with 11 amino acids, a neuropeptide.
This constant tone is influenced on the one hand by pain stimuli themselves, that is,
the afferent side, and on the other hand by descending tracts from the brain (blue ele-
ments in Fig. 4.14) (e.g. (Willis Jr. 1988; Sandkühler 1996; Heinricher et al. 2009;
Bannister and Dickenson 2017; Lockwood and Dickenson 2020)). We had already
addressed these aspects of the blue descending tracts in Fig. 3.3. With the help of the
blue nerve tract from Fig. 4.14, the brain takes a significant influence on things in the
periphery through its effect at the level of the spinal cord cross-section. Do not imagine
this to be simple, because this regulation depends on many factors and cells in the spinal
cord (Xanthos and Sandkühler 2014). In this respect, the sensory nerve fiber is an affer-
ent or efferent element for pain experience or inflammation regulation.
The first clear indications of the pro-inflammatory importance of these pain fib-
ers in a chronic inflammatory disease (arthritis) came from Kathleen A. Sluka from the
University of Iowa (the work originates from the time at the University of Galveston,
4.2 Direct Connectors Chronically Activate the Immune System (No. 1) 167

Cortex

Frontal brain:
medial
prefrontal
cortex ACC

NAc
DOPAMINE
PAG thalamic
nuclei
Raphe
LC SEROTONIN
NORADRENALINE Pain nerve
pathway in the
descending spinal pathways spinal cord

Dorsal root ganglion

Spinal cord
cross section

Outputs
Histamine Muscle and
sympathetic ganglia
Prosta- sensitive Noradrenaline
glandine nerve α-adrenergic
ending

Bacterial wall cytokine


components
Substance P, CGRP
Heat, cold Glutamate, Galanin
mechanical stimuli
Growth
factors
Capsaicin acidic pH
(chili) Inflammation
Substance P, CGRP
Glutamate, Galanin
168 4 Chronic Immune System Activation


Fig. 4.14 Pain pathways and inflammation. Red pathways are afferent and come from the nerve ending
via the dorsal root ganglion to the spinal cord cross-section, to the pain pathways and so to the thala-
mus and the cerebral cortex. The red factors around the nerve ending can stimulate the pain fiber and
trigger a pain stimulus that becomes conscious in the cerebral cortex. The same pain fiber can be acti-
vated in an efferent way (pink), and the nerve ending releases various neurotransmitters in the tissue
(pink). These neurotransmitters can stimulate inflammation (pink). The red and pink pain system can be
directly inhibited and excited by the descending blue system. The relevant neurotransmitters for the blue
pathways are shown in green and capitals. Compare Fig. 3.3. Abbreviations: ACC, anterior cingulate
gyrus; CGRP; calcitonin gene-regulated peptide; LC, locus coeruleus; NAc, nucleus accumbens (reward
center); PAG, periaqueductal gray; raphe; raphe nuclei (serotonin)

Texas). She recognized the efferent path to the inflamed joint because she was able to sup-
press the inflammation in the periphery of the joint directly in the spinal cord cross-sec-
tion from Fig. 4.14 by means of certain inhibitors of a selected neurotransmitter (Sluka
and Westlund 1993). The manipulation of the nerve cells at spinal cord level changes the
inflammation in the periphery. This work has been confirmed by many authors in a similar
way with a view to different aspects of inflammation (for example Lin et al. 1999, 2007;
Dong et al. 2002; Pertovaara and Koivisto 2011; Riol-Blanco et al. 2014).
Another proof of the importance of efferent pain fibers was published by two groups
of workers at approximately the same time. In the human chronic inflammatory disease
of rheumatoid arthritis, the rheumatologist successfully uses so-called TNF inhibitors,
which inhibit the pro-inflammatory TNF and thus reduce joint inflammation. Since TNF
has an inflammation-promoting function in the spinal cord section of Fig. 4.14, the inhi-
bition of TNF at spinal cord level may influence peripheral inflammation in the joint.
This is also what the working group of Hans-Georg Schaible at the University of Jena
and the working group of Gary Firestein at the University of San Diego thought. Both
observed how inhibition of TNF at spinal cord level reduced peripheral joint inflam-
mation (Boyle et al. 2006; Boettger et al. 2010). In order to create a clear effect, the
researchers needed much less of the inhibitor when injecting into the spinal cord than
when administered subcutaneously or intravenously (Boettger et al. 2010).
Hans-Georg Schaible and his working group were also able to work out the impor-
tance of peripheral cytokines, where above all TNF, IL-1β, IL-6 and IL-17 strongly stim-
ulate the nerve ending, which can intensify the inflammatory process (Schaible 2014).
If now the brain can vary the tone of the blue descending nerve tract, it can influence
peripheral inflammation via the sensory nerve ending of Fig. 4.14 in a very direct way.
The brain thus defines the amount of peripherally released pro-inflammatory neurotrans-
mitters, for example substance P. Substance P thus activates almost every type of innate
and adaptive immune response (Suvas 2017). Substance P dilates the blood vessels, the
blood flow is slowed down and leukocytes can more easily leave the bloodstream and
enter the tissue. Substance P is a chemoattractant for leukocytes of all kinds and thus
helps with the migration and accumulation of leukocytes in the tissue. This list is very
long and there is no doubt about the pro-inflammatory importance of this neurotransmit-
ter (Straub 2000).
4.2 Direct Connectors Chronically Activate the Immune System (No. 1) 169

With the known information from the Sect. 3.2.7 we recognize the increased pain sen-
sitivity in people after early traumas (Low and Schweinhardt 2012; Jones 2016; Tesarz
et al. 2016; You and Meagher 2016; Burke et al. 2017; Atlas and alʼAbsi 2018; You and
Meagher 2018), and animal models with maternal deprivation confirm this (Alvarez
et al. 2013; Prusator and Greenwood-Van Meerveld 2016a). There, early traumatic expe-
riences are coupled with increased pain sensitivity, which can be explained by altered
function of the blue descending nerve pathway from Fig. 4.14. These changes lead to
an increased tone of the pain pathways and to an enhanced release of substance P and
other neurotransmitters into the tissue. Thus, the pain system becomes a direct connector
between brain and immune system in the periphery.

4.2.6 Tone of the Sex Hormone Axis

The sex hormones estrogen (female), progesterone (female) and testosterone (male) have a
strong influence on the immune system and inflammation. Table 4.2 gives an overview of
the most important aspects of this influence. Normally, this is little considered in the world

Table 4.2  Sex hormones influence the immune system and inflammation


Hormone Influence
17β-estradiol in ovula- Inhibit T helper 1 and T helper 17 T cell immunity
tion concentration and Inhibit macrophages, dendritic cells, neutrophilic granulocytes, micro-
pregnancy glia, fibroblasts via the proinflammatory factor NFkappaB
Support regulatory T cells and T helper type 2 T cell immunity (B cell
help)
Support B cells and antibody production
17β-estradiol in meno- Support T-cell immunity and macrophages as well as B-cells and
pausal concentration antibody production
Progesterone Inhibits macrophages and neutrophilic granulocytes
Inhibits natural killer cells, T helper 1 and T helper 17 T-cell immunity
and interferons
Supports T helper 2 T-cell immunity (B-cell help), B cells and antibody
formation
Testosterone Support the formation of neutrophilic granulocytes in the bone marrow
and regulatory T-cells
Inhibits monocytes and macrophages and their proinflammatory
cytokines TNF, IL-1β and IL-6
Inhibits mast cells and IL-33
Inhibits B cells and antibody production, as well as T helper 1 T cell
immunity, interferon γ and IL-12
Information from review articles (Straub 2007; Cutolo and Straub 2020)
170 4 Chronic Immune System Activation

of childhood adversity research, although we recognize great differences in the suscepti-


bility of women compared to men with regard to the occurrence of autoimmune diseases
and painful diseases such as fibromyalgia etc. So the often mentioned rheumatoid arthritis
is 3 times more common in women than in men, as is multiple sclerosis, and systemic
lupus erythematosus is 9 times more common in women than in men (Whitacre 2001).
On the other hand, if we see on the intensive care unit four people with sepsis, then,
three are men and one is a woman, which speaks for the better immune system of the
woman. Women are generally and especially during pregnancy and afterwards during
breastfeeding more susceptible to infection. The stronger immune response in women
compared to men was positively selected during evolution for these reasons. During
pregnancy, B cells with their antibody production are primarily responsible for the
immune response to infectious agents. T cells and killer cells are particularly inhibited in
the placenta.
If we look more closely at Table 4.2, we can see the inhibitory effect of all sex hor-
mones on so-called T helper type 1 and T helper type 17 T cell immunity, which are
relevant to many chronic inflammatory diseases. In addition, all sex hormones inhibit
monocytes and macrophages, which are crucial to innate immunity. Estrogens lose
their inhibitory effect at low concentrations, as they prevail after menopause. Then they
can even stimulate chronic inflammatory diseases on the basis of T helper type 1 and
T helper type 17 T cell immunity and support chronic inflammatory diseases after meno-
pause (Straub 2007).
At one point, estrogens/progesterone differ from testosterone in that they promote
B-cell and antibody production. Here, estrogens/progesterone support the B-cells, and
testosterone inhibits the B-cells. This is a fundamental contrast between female and male
sex hormones. This is particularly important in those chronic inflammatory diseases that
are primarily triggered by B-cells and antibody production (Straub 2007). These chronic
inflammatory diseases primarily occur in the reproductive phase of women. But how are
early traumatic experiences linked to these sex hormones?
To discuss this, we need to address two questions:

• How does trauma in early life affect the world of sex hormones?
• How does the increased HPA-axis with cortisol affect sex hormones?

4.2.6.1 Influence of Early Trauma on the World of Sex Hormones


In general, women and men may react differently to early trauma because women and
men go through different brain development, have different time windows for the influ-
ence of trauma, receive different attention and education, and on the basis of the different
sex chromosomes—genetically independent of sex hormones—produce different func-
tions in the brain, etc. (Bath 2020). These factors may intervene at important points in
the brain and therefore have consequences for the chronic inflammation situation (Bath
2020). In this book, I am concerned with the peripheral role of sex hormones, whose
importance for the immune system is quite clear.
4.2 Direct Connectors Chronically Activate the Immune System (No. 1) 171

Many studies confirm an earlier maturity (menarche) in girls after previous traumas
(Kim and Smith 1998; Mendle et al. 2011; Bleil et al. 2013; Amir et al. 2016; Magnus
et al. 2018; Zhang et al. 2019; Suglia et al. 2020). Girls with previous adversities have
earlier first sex and are often pregnant at a younger age (Anderson 2015). Consequently,
they already have high estrogen levels at an early age and experience menopause at a
limited ovarian reserve of oocytes at an earlier age—before the 40th birthday or around
the age of 40 (Vélez et al. 2010; Magnus et al. 2018). This speaks for an increased effect
of estrogens and progesterone in young years and parallel for an early decline of this
hormone activity after an early menopause.
Similar observations have been made in female guinea pigs and there the progester-
one levels are higher in stressed animals compared to control animals in the young years
(Schöpper et al. 2012). After maturity, however, the estrogen levels are lower in stressed
animals (at higher cortisol serum values) (Kapoor and Matthews 2008; Ordyan et al.
2013; Del Cerro et al. 2015).
Women who were traumatized at an early age and are now 40+ years old show
methylations in the regulatory DNA section (promoter) of the estrogen receptor gene
(Explanation 1), and thus the function of the estrogen receptor and the effect of estrogens
are reduced in leukocytes. The extent of the methylations in this section correlates with
the number of adversities in the young years (Fiacco et al. 2019). At the same time, the
ratio of saliva estrogen to saliva cortisol is significantly lower in those with adversities
compared to those without (Fiacco et al. 2019). This may be due to an earlier menopause
in people with 40+ years, to generally increased glucocorticoids or to both.
In men, however, there is rather a delayed maturity (Suglia et al. 2020), which can
also be observed in animals after early trauma (Anderson et al. 1985; Kerchner et al.
1995; Kaiser et al. 2003; Morgan and Bale 2011; Pérez-Laso et al. 2013; Ashworth et al.
2016; Hernández-Arteaga et al. 2016; Davis et al. 2020). In animal experiments, this
delayed development can be reversed by administration of testosterone (Pereira et al.
2006; Kapoor and Matthews 2011). In stressed male animals, androgen levels are low-
ered and estrogen levels increased (Kapoor and Matthews 2011; Eck et al. 2020).
This gives us the following picture for women and men: Women experience sexual
maturity earlier and have higher levels of estrogen and progesterone at that time, which
fall sharply over the course of life, especially after early menopause, and the estrogen/
progesterone effect is lost soon. Men, on the other hand, have a generally lower sup-
ply of the strongly anti-inflammatory testosterone. With this interim result, we ask our-
selves another question and summarize afterwards. If the tone of the HPA axis is often
increased after early trauma (Sect. 4.2.3), does this heightened HPA axis activity takes
direct influence on the sex hormones?

4.2.6.2 HPA Axis and Sex Hormones


Remember the increased tone of the HPA Axis after early traumas! The HPA axis has
an inhibitory effect on the sex hormone axis (Kalantaridou et al. 2010; Tsigos et al.
2021). For example, the hormone with the highest hierarchy in the HPA axis, called
172 4 Chronic Immune System Activation

corticotropin-releasing hormone (CRH), inhibits the neighboring gonadotropin-releasing


hormone (GnRH), which is the hormone with the highest hierarchy in the sex hormone
axis. Cortisol from the adrenal gland can inhibit at all levels of the sex hormone axis
(Tsigos et al. 2021). The GnRH is secreted in pulsatile waves, which is very important
for reproductive function, and cortisol can suppress these waves (Tsigos et al. 2021).
Under various chronic stress conditions, the function of the sex hormone axis in
women and men is severely impaired, and this can lead to amenorrhea in women and
lack of libido and reduced fertility in men (Kyrou and Tsigos 2008; Valsamakis et al.
2019). Cortisol and other hormones of the HPA axis are directly involved in this phenom-
enon. If inflammation is added and, for example, IL-6 is produced to a greater extent, this
IL-6 can suppress the production of sex hormones within hours (Tsigos et al. 1999).
In summary, cortisol has an inhibitory effect on the gonads and sex hormones and
can therefore turn off the anti-inflammatory effects of these hormones (Table 4.2). Since
the activity of the HPA Axis is often increased after early traumatic experiences, a high
cortisol tone is more likely in a large proportion of those affected (Sect. 4.2.3). If cortisol
has a lower or no immunosuppressive effect (Sect. 4.2.3), the inflammatory situation is
additionally aggravated by the shutdown of the sex hormones. This can open doors for
those chronic inflammatory diseases that occur after menopause or andropause.

4.2.6.3 Differential Effects of Sex Hormones on Inflammation


The influence of sex hormones on various elements of the immune system is shown in
Table 4.2. The influence is not uniformly inhibiting or stimulating, but quite differenti-
ated. This results in a different susceptibility of men and women to autoimmune diseases
and other chronic inflammations, which are controlled by different components of the
immune system.
With the development of so-called biologics, which can specifically shut down indi-
vidual components of the immune response (therapies with neutralizing inhibitors), we
gradually recognize different subtypes of autoimmune diseases. So the same disease may
appear the same from the outside—for example, rheumatoid arthritis in the fingers and
toes—but a different immune response may be found behind it.
For example, the doctor can very specifically inhibit T cells by an anti-T cell therapy,
but this can only help a smaller part of the affected people very well. The rate of com-
plete suppression of the immune response in rheumatoid arthritis, depending on other
circumstances such as concomitant medication, etc., is between 10 and 40% (Lutt 2009).
So 10–40% of people with rheumatoid arthritis benefit very well from this therapy. So
10–40% of patients have a T cell-dependent chronic inflammation, and in 60–90% of
patients the disease is based on another immunological process. Now let’s look at the
targeted therapy to shut down the B cells, the picture looks similar. This allows us to
recognize different disease subtypes that, from the outside—phenotypically—produce a
very similar disease picture with comparable symptoms.
These different subtypes show why female sex hormones can act differently and
therefore why the appearance or disappearance of hormones can promote or inhibit
4.2 Direct Connectors Chronically Activate the Immune System (No. 1) 173

different subtypes of the same disease at different times in life. With this knowledge, we
now look at Fig. 4.15.
An inflammatory influence is added in women because estrogens stimulate many
different aspects of pain pathways (review article by Straub 2007). The result is an
increased sensitization, which leads to increased input signals into the spinal cord
(2007). In addition, central nervous aspects of pain processing in the brain are stimu-
lated by estrogens (2007). Heightened pain pocessing leads to an increased release of the
proinflammatory substance P at the nerve ending in peripheral tissue (Sect. 4.2.5 Tone of
the Pain Pathways).
Looking on the effects of estrogens, we are not surprised that typical diseases of early
traumatized people such as fibromyalgia, migraine, pelvic pain, irritable bowel syndrome
and urethral and bladder pain are more common in women than in men (e.g. Chaloner
and Greenwood-Van Meerveld 2013). The estrogens sensitize nociceptive pathways.

Women
early B-cell driven immune early T-cell driven immune
response increases response increases

high estrogens low estrogens


Traumas high progesterone low progesterone
additional inhibition by HPA axis

childhood reproductive years postmenopausal


early early
menarche menopause

Men
various immune responses increased

Traumas low testosterone very low testosterone


additional inhibition by HPA axis

childhood reproductive years posttestosterone


late
puberty andropause

Fig. 4.15 Trauma, sex hormones and chronic immune system activation. Women with early high est-
rogen/progesterone levels as a result of early childhood adversity would stimulate the B-cell-dependent
diseases in the reproductive age. If there is a drop in estrogen/progesterone after early menopause, the
T-cell-dependent chronic immune diseases are intensified. The latter mechanism is aggravated by high
activity of the HPA axis with high cortisol, because cortisol inhibits the sex hormone axis. In men after
early trauma, the generally lower tone of testosterone would mean stimulation of chronic inflammation
at all stages of life, and the problem increases especially after andropause. Parallel increased cortisol
levels further aggravate the situation when cortisol inhibits the sex hormone axis and when they are no
longer anti-inflammatory in usual concentrations (Sect. 4.2.3)
174 4 Chronic Immune System Activation

This makes the function of sex hormones as direct connectors obvious, because both
the increased appearance (with estrogens/progesterone promote B-cell immunity and
pain) and the shutdown (with estrogens/progesterone after premature menopause and tes-
tosterone) can promote chronic inflammation by different immune reactions at different
times of life.
In this Sect. 4.2 “Direct connectors chronically activate the immune system (No. 1)”
we learned about the most important connectors that can promote an inflammatory situ-
ation in a direct, brain-influenced way. Part of the anti-inflammatory system has become
pro-inflammatory by receptor switching processes (sympathetic nervous system, HPA
axis). Other direct connectors could be mentioned here, such as growth hormone and
especially thyroid hormones, but I do not want to exceed the scope of the book.

4.3 Indirect Connectors Activate the Immune System


Chronically (No. 2)

4.3.1 Adipose Tissue is Pro-inflammatory

I had already mentioned the topic of obesity under Sect. 3.3.1 in the context of trauma
sequelae and cited the epidemiological studies. The connection between early traumatic
episodes and later higher body weight is undisputed (e.g. Thomas et al. 2008; Entringer
et al. 2010; Bae et al. 2014; Davis et al. 2014; Wickrama et al. 2014; Tanenbaum et al.
2017; Wickrama et al. 2017; Farewell et al. 2018; Robson et al. 2020). The starting point
is clear. But what does this have to do with higher inflammation and with the indirect
connector from the chapter heading?
The brain is significantly involved in weight gain. In Table 3.2 and in Chap. “Fat-
making Eating Behavior”, I spoke about the brain-related causes of high body weight.
I focused on fat-making eating behavior in the sense of reward, just like with alcohol,
nicotine and drugs (overview in Spencer 2013).
The HPA axis plays an important role in overeating behavior. After childhood adver-
sities, the HPA axis is activated. If a stress reaction is triggered, the top hormone of the
HPA axis, corticotropin-releasing hormone (CRH), rises and cortisol from the adrenal
gland increases gradually over 15-30 minutes, which stimulates appetite (Spencer 2013).
A chronic increase in cortisol chronically stimulates appetite; the relationships are well
explained in a review article (Spencer 2013). Perhaps you can already imagine how the
brain uses the adipose tissue to chronically stimulate inflammation.
The famous publication from 1993 by Gökhan Hotamisligil in Spiegelman’s labora-
tory at Harvard Medical School in Boston brought attention to inflammatory factors in
adipose tissue (Hotamisligil et al. 1993), more specifically: to TNF. Other authors were
the first to investigate the influence of weight loss on TNF levels in adipose tissue, as
they observed significantly lower TNF levels in adipose tissue after a decrease in body
weight (Kern et al. 1995). In the early 1990s, an increasing awareness developed for a
4.3 Indirect Connectors Activate the Immune System Chronically (No. 2) 175

possible increased inflammatory state in adipose tissue. Later, other cytokines were
added, for example IL-6 (Mohamed-Ali et al. 1997).
These investigations were further stimulated because researchers in the field of car-
diovascular research discovered inflammatory aspects in the atherosclerotic vessel wall
(Kern et al. 1995). Since arteriosclerosis was in turn associated with a higher body
weight, a connection was made between vessel wall and adipose tissue inflammation.
Since IL-6 was recognized as the trigger of the acute phase inflammatory reaction in
the liver (Andus et al. 1988), the focus was on the universally measurable acute phase
C-reactive protein. As always, various scientific groups came from different directions
over time to take C-reactive protein as an indicator of the connection between adipose
tissue and inflammation (Deehan et al. 1994; Wolfe 1997; Maugeri et al. 1998; Hak et al.
1999; Visser et al. 1999; Yudkin et al. 1999). In 2000, the matter was clear, but the causa-
tive cell in adipose tissue was unknown.
Although macrophages in adipose tissue were already detected in rats after a fish oil
diet at the end of the 1970s (Danse and Verschuren 1978), this finding ended up in the
veterinary pathology drawer for years. In the early 1980s, others discovered the abil-
ity of adipose tissue cells to produce colony-stimulating factor (CSF) from bone mar-
row, which is relevant for the survival of monocytes and macrophages in adipose tissue
(Lanotte et al. 1982). The detection of specific immune cells took a long time because
first the corresponding methods for the detection of different types of immune cells had
to be discovered.
Macrophages have been associated with spontaneous or experimentally induced
necrosis in adipose tissue (Friedman and Winkelmann 1989), but were not recognized
as spontaneous settlers in adipose tissue. Although factors from macrophages influenced
lipolysis and insulin resistance in adipose tissue (e.g. Ogawa et al. 1989), the colonization
of adipose tissue by macrophages was unknown for a long time after the initial descrip-
tion by Danse and Verschuren (Danse and Verschuren 1978). The Spiegelman laboratory
in Harvard discovered additional immune factors in adipose tissue that spoke for the pres-
ence of macrophages without finding them in these endeavors (White et al. 1992).
Finally, various groups discovered lymphoid tissue in abdominal fat—so-called milk
spots—and there they were, the macrophages and lymphocytes, who felt comfortable in
the adipose tissue (Dullens et al. 1993; Pond and Mattacks 1995). However, these studies
were limited to the special case of lymphoid tissue in abdominal adipose tissue. Then
migration factors were found in fat cells that favor the accumulation of macrophages in
this tissue (Hirokawa et al. 1997). More and more researchers recognized many inflam-
matory factors from adipose tissue. Finally, the real breakthrough came in 2003 when
two groups described the accumulation of macrophages in adipose tissue in the same
prestigious journal (Weisberg et al. 2003; Xu et al. 2003). Unfortunately, neither of them
mentioned the 25-year-old original work from veterinary pathology from 1978, where
the situation was already visible (Danse and Verschuren 1978).
I summarize (Fig. 4.16): After early traumatic situations, the brain contributes signifi-
cantly to the increase in fat tissue (obesity), and the enlargement of fat tissue is associated
176 4 Chronic Immune System Activation

with an inflammatory activity in fat tissue (Schäffler and Schölmerich 2010; Choi et al.
2013). The main players in fat tissue are macrophages and fat cells themselves. The inflam-
mation in fat tissue is transferred to the rest of the body because inflammatory factors in the
blood are increased (C-reactive protein). Many authors see this as a way in which obesity
can act back on the brain to be involved in depression (Ambrósio et al. 2018).
Finally, the brain can still act on fat tissue in other ways, for example by sending hor-
mones from the HPA axis (cortisol) and the sympathetic nervous system (noradrenaline).
Both systems can activate lipolysis—the breakdown of stored fatty acids—and insulin
resistance (see next section), or they can prevent the uptake of fatty acids and glucose
from the bloodstream into the fat cell (summarized in Straub 2020b). If the brain releases
fatty acids in this way, they can have a pro-inflammatory effect, especially in the case of
saturated fatty acids (summarized in Straub 2020b).
Here is the dilemma. People affected by early trauma take in too many bad foods
high in saturated fatty acids over a long period of time. In Chap. “Fat-Making Eating
Behavior”, I called it Junk Food Self Medication. These fatty acids are typically stored
in fat tissue, and the release of these fatty acids stimulated by the brain leads to a proin-
flammatory constellation (Straub 2020b). We have a kind of memory for bad fatty acids
in fat tissue when we have previously taken them in (Straub 2020b). Furthermore, people
who have experienced trauma and obesity have lower blood levels of good HDL choles-
terol and higher blood levels of bad LDL cholesterol and triglycerides, which we recog-
nize as additional factors in a proinflammatory constellation (Reid et al. 2018; Cubbin
et al. 2019; Péterfalvi et al. 2019).
This makes obvious the function of the brain and its downstream assistants—the HPA
axis and the sympathetic nervous system—as well as the fat tissue indirect connectors,
because they promote inflammation in fat tissue and induce fat tissue factors such as
TNF, IL-6, free fatty acids and others to promote chronic immune system activation in
various places (Fig. 4.16). This also includes sympathetic overactivity and resistance to
insulin, that is, the lack of effect of insulin in fat tissue and elsewhere.

4.3.2 The Insulin from the Pancreas Promotes Inflammation

The constellation of high inflammation in fat tissue and simultaneous activation of the
HPA axis and the sympathetic nervous system is a strong causal platform for the devel-
opment of insulin resistance, that is, the reduced effectiveness of insulin due to changes
in signal transduction through the insulin receptor into the cell. Each individual factor
can cause insulin resistance, and in combination they are particularly unpleasant. So var-
ious disorders of the central nervous system (e.g. stress, pain, depression, schizophre-
nia) and many inflammatory diseases are associated with insulin resistance (compiled in
Straub 2014).
Therefore, it should not surprise us if people with previous trauma in young years
develop insulin resistance with increased body weight, increased sympathetic activity
4.3 Indirect Connectors Activate the Immune System Chronically (No. 2) 177

Fig. 4.16 The importance of fat


tissue as an indirect connector. selfish brain
Factors from the brain use
fat tissue to stimulate chronic
inflammation. Lipolysis serves fattening diet
to release fatty acids. Insulin smoking
resistance (next section) serves excessive alcohol
lipolysis
to prevent the uptake of fatty high cortisol
insulin
acids/glucose into the fat cell. high nor-/adrenaline
resistance
Both circulate more in the low physical activity
bloodstream. Both contribute
to the increase in inflammation much
in addition to the local fat
macrophages and lymphocytes. tissue
Typical mediators are cytokines indirect
such as TNF, IL-6 and free fatty connector
acids themselves
fat cells themselves
macrophages in adipose tissue
mediators
lymphocytes in adipose tissue
TNF, IL-6, free fatty acids, etc.

chronic inflammation

and increased HPA axis activity (Vargas et al. 2016; Suarez et al. 2017; Campbell et al.
2018; Reid et al. 2018; Fuller-Rowell et al. 2019; Robakis et al. 2019; Stojek et al.
2019). Similar findings of insulin resistance can be seen in animal experiments with
early trauma (Lesage et al. 2004; Kaufman et al. 2007; Brunton et al. 2013; Ruiz et al.
2018; Eller et al. 2020; van Niekerk et al. 2020; Zardooz et al. 2021). The ineffective-
ness of insulin is caused by the hormones, neurotransmitters and cytokines directly at the
insulin receptor (compiled in Straub 2014, 2018).
You remember the selfishness of the brain and the immune system (Sect. 4.1). Both
use insulin resistance in different ways—brain: hormones/neurotransmitters, immune
system: cytokines—because insulin resistance means reduced uptake of fatty acids
and glucose in fat cells, muscle cells and liver cells. Thus, higher levels of glucose and
fatty acids circulate in the blood, and in crisis situations the brain and immune system
can access these circulating energy-rich substrates. So what’s the problem with insulin
resistance?
Higher blood levels of glucose and fatty acids stimulate the pancreas to further pro-
duce insulin. Inevitably, insulin resistance leads to hyperinsulinemia; that is, more insu-
lin circulates in the flowing blood. Here we have the indirect influence of the brain,
because the brain indirectly increases insulin resistance and insulin levels in the blood
through the HPA axis (cortisol) and the sympathetic nervous system (noradrenaline,
adrenaline) as well as through increased inflammation in fat tissue (Fig. 4.17). The
direct influence of the brain on the parasympathetic nervous system of the vagus nerve
178 4 Chronic Immune System Activation

physical
higher inactivity
inflammatory
activity in the
abdominal immobility overeating
adipose tissue
increase in increase in
fat tissue fat tissue

genetic insulin resistance


microbiome
aspects insulin
glucose
free fatty acids

malnutrition with
activation of the
high-calorie snacks
HPA axis and the
with spikes in
sympathetic nervous
circulating glucose
system (cortisol,
and fatty acids
nor-/adrenaline)

Fig. 4.17 Development of insulin resistance and hyperinsulinemia. The described factors influence
insulin resistance and hyperinsulinemia. Those that are determined by the brain are marked in red. Insu-
lin, glucose and free fatty acids are increased in the blood and contribute to inflammation. We will dis-
cuss the microbiome, the sum of all microorganisms that live on the skin or mucous membranes of an
individual (nose, mouth, gastrointestinal tract, urogenital, respiratory tract, etc.), below

increases insulin secretion, and this influence is lost when the parasympathetic nervous
system is inhibited.
Insulin, glucose and free fatty acids can be proinflammatory factors at high con-
centrations (Frauwirth and Thompson 2004; Calder et al. 2007; Maciver et al. 2008;
Ieronymaki et al. 2019; van Niekerk et al. 2021). Insulin is an important element for
immune cell survival and growth (van Niekerk et al. 2021). Therefore, insulin is impor-
tant for macrophage function (Ieronymaki et al. 2019). Insulin can have anti-inflamma-
tory effects to some extent, but especially in combination with higher blood values of
glucose and free fatty acids and parallel inflammation processes, the picture can change.
The dual role of insulin can be reversed into a proinflammatory direction by intracellular
switching reactions (van Niekerk et al. 2020).
Furthermore, insulin can contribute to improved conduction and sensitization of pain
nerve fibers (Lázár et al. 2020). Here, those sensory nerve fibers are particularly favored by
4.3 Indirect Connectors Activate the Immune System Chronically (No. 2) 179

the presence of insulin, which possess the chili receptor (capsaicin, TRPV1) (Lázár et al.
2020). The increased activation of pain nerve fibers can in turn set off an inflammatory
reaction because substance P is released locally (see Sect. 4.2.5 Tone of the Pain Pathways).
Thus, the role of the brain, the HPA axis, and the sympathetic nervous system
becomes visible, because they indirectly promote high blood levels of insulin from the
pancreas, glucose from the liver, and free fatty acids from the adipose tissue (indirect
connectors), and all contribute to chronic immune system activation.

4.3.3 Reduced Physical Activity—Anti-inflammatory Factors are


Missing

Early trauma has a significant impact on physical activity, muscle strength, and phys-
ical dexterity (Cooper et al. 2010; Birnie et al. 2011; Murray et al. 2013; Anderson
et al. 2017; Cosco et al. 2018; Allen et al. 2019; Cheval et al. 2019; Hwang et al. 2019;
Maunder et al. 2019), all of which an be reduced in those affected. We also see such
relationships in animal experiments (Eller et al. 2020). In a comprehensive review of the
literature, the authors found increased muscle mass in people with favorable socio-eco-
nomic conditions, which was particularly relevant in countries with high income levels.
The opposite picture emerged in middle-income countries (Bridger Staatz et al. 2021).
In monkeys with trauma in young years, important functional parameters of the perfor-
mance of the heart are lower than in controls (Coplan et al. 2018).
In traumatized people, little muscle mass and muscle strength go hand in hand with
increased adipose tissue. This constellation was called the “sarcopenic obesity”, in which
the muscle mass is low (that is, sarcopenic) and at the same time a high body mass index
is present (fat instead of muscle) (Stenholm et al. 2008). What does this sarcopenic obe-
sity mean for the inflammatory situation? The role of adiposity has already been reported
(Fig. 4.16). We have to talk about the muscle here, because if muscular work is anti-in-
flammatory, the loss of muscular work is pro-inflammatory.
I am referring here mainly to work by Bente Pedersen from the Center of Physical
Activity Research at the University of Copenhagen. In the 1990s, the scientists postulated
a “Work Factor”, which is released into the blood during muscular work and affects
many other organs. At the beginning of the 2000s, Pedersen and her group found the first
Work Factor in IL-6 (Steensberg et al. 2000). IL-6 was considered mixed pro- or anti-in-
flammatory at that time, no one knew for sure.
A little later, rheumatologists from Japan recognized the pro-inflammatory importance
of high tissue concentrations of IL-6 in the treatment of rheumatic patients, as the neu-
tralizing therapy with antibodies against IL-6 had very good anti-inflammatory effects
(Mihara et al. 2005). Bente Pedersen was not deterred by these new findings and prop-
agated IL-6 as an anti-inflammatory factor (Pedersen and Fischer 2007). Finally, various
investigators recognized other factors that are released from the muscle into the blood,
and the endocrine function of the muscle took shape (Pedersen 2013). The scientists
180 4 Chronic Immune System Activation

called these new factors “myokines”, similar to the new factors of white adipose tissue
“adipokines” and those of brown adipose tissue “batokines” (brown adipose tissue).
Of these myokines, IL-6 was anti-inflammatory in that it could inhibit TNF from
macrophages and also stimulate the anti-inflammatory cytokine IL-10 and the neutral-
izer of the pro-inflammatory IL-1 (IL-1 receptor antagonist). Its anti-inflammatory role
in the body depends on the duration of its presence and the level, as short IL-6 peaks are
favorable but prolonged increased IL-6 concentrations are unfavorable (Severinsen and
Pedersen 2020). In rheumatic diseases, the long-term presence of IL-6 has a pro-inflam-
matory meaning.
In addition, the anti-inflammatory role of physical exercise was gradually recognized
(Benatti and Pedersen 2015), as well as the protective effect of sport with respect to the
natural aging of the immune system (Duggal et al. 2019). These anti-inflammatory and
protective effects of physical exercise are multifaceted and involve many organs of the
body (for example, by inhibiting white and promoting brown adipose tissue). In addition,
a direct anti-inflammatory component of muscle factors is recognized (Severinsen and
Pedersen 2020). In this way, the brain is responsible for the reduced physical activity
after early trauma, and this lack of muscular work indirectly induces a higher level of
inflammation. We recognize in the muscle and its myokines another indirect connector.

4.3.4 Microbiome and Inflammation

Nobel laureate Joshua Lederberg (1925–2008) was a molecular biologist and geneticist
and participated in the Human Genome Project of the 1990s. He coined the term micro-
biome because he recognized the involvement of the multitude of bacteria in human
metabolism and suspected that this might influence the body. Lederberg saw in the coex-
istence of humans and microbiome a symbiosis that can be disturbed in case of disease.
Lederberg’s statements were the starting point for the more extensive microbiome stud-
ies with the motto: “What else do we have to genetically investigate?”
The microbiome is the sum of all microorganisms that live on the skin or mucous
membranes of an individual (nose, mouth, gastrointestinal tract, urogenital, respiratory
tract, etc.). This includes bacteria, fungi, parasites, viruses and bacteriophages, and one
thing is quite certain: Most people live peacefully with their microbiome. In the intestine
and other places, bacteria perform many helpful tasks, so we cannot do without them
(e.g. metabolic tasks). In this sense, it is a symbiosis.
Since scientists have been refining the so-called high-throughput sequencing of DNA
since 2000, many researchers have begun to include the microbiome in their genetic con-
siderations. They take samples of the microbiome from different body sites and analyze
the genetic material of the microbes. From the found sequences, the researchers can infer
the presence and amount of different bacterial strains. The vast majority of the micro-
biome is made up of the bacterial species Bacteroides (Bacteroides and Prevotella) and
Firmicutes (Ruminococcus, Lactobacillus and Clostridium).
4.3 Indirect Connectors Activate the Immune System Chronically (No. 2) 181

In the last 10 years, this microbiome has increasingly been associated with chronic
inflammatory diseases (van den Munckhof et al. 2018; Salem et al. 2019; Dalmády et al.
2020; Aldars-García et al. 2021). While causal relationships are clearly visible in animal
experiments on mice and rats, because experimenters can transfer very defined bacterial
strains to an otherwise germ-free animal (called monoassociation), the situation is not
yet clear in humans. The proponents of a bidirectional relationship between the microbi-
ome in the intestine and changes in the brain rely on animal experiments on mice.
So far, we have only found a few causal links between microbiomes and diseases. The
shining exception is antibiotic-induced pseudomembranous colitis, a severe acute dis-
ease caused by infection with the bacterium Clostridium difficile, because in this case the
transfer of gut bacteria from healthy donors really helps. In this particular case, the dys-
biosis caused by antibiotics is cured in 80–90% of cases by microbiome therapy, because
the diversity of bacteria increases sharply (Brandt 2015). There are only small effects
in the therapy of colitis ulcerosa with probiotics or synbiotics (probiotics + bacteri-
um-promoting additive), because here the diversity of bacteria in the intestine is directly
supported (Kaur et al. 2020; Zhang et al. 2021). But what about all the non-gut-related
diseases?
At the moment it is open whether a certain microbiome causes or worsens a disease
(causality) or whether, on the other hand, a special microbiome is a consequence of a
certain disease situation (correlation). One thing is relatively certain: A high diversity
of bacterial strains is healthy (e.g. Brandt 2015; Shi et al. 2016; Gavriilaki et al. 2020).
However, findings in stressed animals, in which the microbiome was examined before
and after stress, indicate that the change of the microbiome can be a consenquence rather
than a cause.
Such studies have been carried out on rodents. For example, Mike Bailey from
Columbus, Ohio, observed how repeated aggression stress (social disruption) reduced
this bacterial diversity. Furthermore, there were differences in groups of bacte-
ria between stressed and non-stressed animals (Bailey et al. 2011). Other research-
ers support these findings in rodents (Tannock and Savage 1974; Golubeva et al. 2015;
Bharwani et al. 2016; El Aidy et al. 2017; Moussaoui et al. 2017; Gur et al. 2019).
Prenatal stress can already change the composition of bacteria in the intestine in mon-
keys after birth (Bailey et al. 2004). Thus, early trauma has an effect on the composition
of gut bacteria.
In animal experiments, connections from the microbiome to the brain have been
established, which contributed to the designation microbiome-gut-brain axis. Such con-
tacts can contribute to pathological changes in the body. For example, John Cryan’s
research group from Cork, Ireland, was able to show a stronger stress response of the
HPA axis in germ-free mice compared to control animals (Clarke et al. 2013). This work
confirmed earlier work by a Japanese research group led by Sudo and colleagues (Sudo
et al. 2004). Here, the authors saw how the administration of certain bacterial strains nor-
malized the stress response of the HPA axis.
182 4 Chronic Immune System Activation

Similarly, the activity of the sympathetic nervous system is related to the colonization
of germs in the gut. Germ-free mice have increased activity of sympathetic nerve fibers
(Muller et al. 2020). This increased sympathetic activity can be reduced by colonization
with germs. In this respect, the sympathetic nervous system behaves similarly to the HPA
axis, which often show synchronous behavior (see Sect. 4.2.4). In these experiments, a
connection was found between the short-chain fatty acids produced by bacteria and the
activation of sympathetic nerve fibers (Muller et al. 2020).
In addition, anxiety reactions in animals with separation experiences are stimulated
by the presence of germs in the gut. Animals without germs show a significantly reduced
anxiety reaction (De Palma et al. 2015). These experiments show the influence of the
microbiome on different levels of the brain. However, we only recognize this in mice
because the germ influence can be controlled very well experimentally (monoassocia-
tion). There is a huge difference between germ-free and normal germ diversity, which
makes a significant difference in animal experiments compared to the situation in
humans. When we look at humans, we can only speak of slightly different microbiomes,
which never comes close to the drastic contrast between germ-free and normal germ
diversity.
However, there can be indirect influencers—that is, via the microbiome—on the
immune system. This is suggested by studies on a large number of newborns with a high
diabetes risk. In these children, the early administration of probiotics already in the first
month of life led to a lower risk of developing an autoimmune reaction against the insu-
lin-producing cells in the pancreas nine years after birth (Uusitalo et al. 2016). Wow! A
recent small randomized, unblinded study in healthy people over 10 weeks showed the
slightly anti-inflammatory importance of a diet with fermented foods (e.g. yogurt, cot-
tage cheese, kimchi vegetables, sauerkraut, kombucha tea, kefir, buttermilk, kvass, vege-
table drinks, etc.) (Wastyk et al. 2021). Here we see a direct influence of therapeutically
given bacteria on the immune system. However, the number of studies is still small.
Finally, there are the first small studies in humans, which compared to controls
observe a different microbiome in those with early traumatic experiences (Hantsoo
et al. 2019; Callaghan et al. 2020; Flannery et al. 2020; Hantsoo and Zemel 2021; Reid
et al. 2021). Now we can formulate the following hypotheses: 1) After early trauma, the
microbiome changes through the sustainable influence of the HPA axis and the sym-
pathetic nervous system, 2) the change in the microbiome affects the immune system
in return. 3) This influence creates chronic immune system activation. At this point, I
warn all readers against premature conclusions. Let’s wait for the well-designed study in
humans.

4.3.5 The Permeability of the Gut is Pro-inflammatory

Stressed individuals have a “leaky” intestinal wall, which allows bacteria to enter the
body and thus stimulate the intestinal immune system. The penetration of particularly
4.3 Indirect Connectors Activate the Immune System Chronically (No. 2) 183

infectious intestinal bacteria across the barrier of the intestinal mucosa has been known
in animals and humans for a long time. Doctors have examined this in the context of
typhus infection by Salmonella species, as these bacteria enter the body (Orskov and
Moltke 1928; Sprinz et al. 1966).
Now, stressful conditions like shock situations or pain reactions (heat, cold) can pro-
mote translocation of harmless bacteria from the intestine into the tissue (Deitch and
Bridges 1987; Berg 1999). Furthermore, psychological stress situations increase the per-
meability for intestinal bacteria (Bailey et al. 2006; Reber et al. 2011). High glucocor-
ticoids of the HPA axis are important for this (Meddings and Swain 2000; Reber et al.
2011). In addition, the sympathetic nervous system is relevant, because its deactivation
reduces bacterial translocation through the leaky intestinal wall, but the effect may be
different for different bacterial strains (Straub et al. 2005; Worlicek et al. 2010). Thus,
both main stress axes favor the permeability of the intestinal wall.
This bacterial translocation occurs in humans under stressful life situations, such
as after major surgery (Woodcock et al. 2000; Ono et al. 2005; Nishigaki et al. 2014;
Schietroma et al. 2015). Extreme sport experiences like an ultra-marathon reduce the
imperviousness of the intestinal wall (Gill et al. 2015).
In this way, the brain can increase the permeability of the intestinal mucosa, bacteria
can enter the interior of the body and activate the immune system in lymph nodes or
elsewhere. It is not difficult to imagine how the permanent deflection of the weighing
scale from Fig. 3.4 activates the HPA axis and the sympathetic nervous system, which
influence the intestinal imperviousness and finally a continuous immune activation. In
this case, the intestine, bacteria and the permeability of the intestinal mucosa are the
indirect connectors.

4.3.6 The Skin Itches Chronically and is Inflamed

The neurodermatitis or atopic dermatitis is the most common chronic inflammatory skin
disease worldwide. The prevalence is between 15 and 20%. It is characterized by intense
pruritus, a relapsing-remitting course with free intervals and systemic inflammation. The
pruritus leads to sleep problems, concentration problems, social stigmatization, reduced
quality of life of those affected, work absenteeism, reduced productivity at school, at uni-
versity or in working life (summarized in Thijs et al. 2015; Weidinger et al. 2018).
The systemic inflammation becomes apparent when considering biomarkers of atopic
dermatitis, because there are increased numbers of eosinophilic granulocytes, raised
immunoglobulins of the IgE type, and some increased cytokines and chemokines in the
blood (Thijs et al. 2015; Sinikumpu et al. 2018; Weidinger et al. 2018; Daniluk et al.
2019). Nevertheless, atopic dermatitis typically does not belong to the highly inflamma-
tory skin diseases, and I therefore rather speak of low inflammation. However, in rare
cases, the extent of the disease can affect up to 90% of the skin, and then the systemic
signs of inflammation are clearly increased.
184 4 Chronic Immune System Activation

The development of atopic dermatitis is based on genetic factors with a relatively


high heritability, on dysfunction of the epidermal barrier, on a high susceptibility to skin
germs (especially Staphylococcus aureus), on intradermal inflammation with a T helper
type 2 T cell immunity (IL-4, IL-13, IL-31, IL-33; in addition, IL-17 and IL-22) and an
increased irritation of the skin showing up as pruritus, which is conveyed to the brain via
special pruritus-conducting sensory nerve fibers (Weidinger et al. 2018).
With regard to pruritus, the doctors recognized the neurotransmitter histamine in addi-
tion to other factors (IL-4, IL-13, IL-31, endothelin-1 and TSLP [thymic stromal lym-
phopoetin]), which can bind to the pruritus-conducting sensory nerve fibers via specific
receptors (Weidinger et al. 2018). At the same time, there is an increased ingrowth of
these nerve fibers into the affected skin regions (Haas et al. 2010). This increased inner-
vation is another important platform for an inflammation that is mediated by the neuro-
transmitters of these sensory nerve fibers (substance P and others) (Lönndahl et al. 2019).
Stress and anxiety exacerbate atopic dermatitis and itching, which has been shown
many times (overview in Sanders and Akiyama 2018). In addition, in recent years, the
connection between early traumatic life situations and atopic dermatitis has been con-
firmed in population-based studies (Andersson et al. 2016; Chang et al. 2016; McKenzie
and Silverberg 2020; van der Leek et al. 2020). We thus recognize the influence of the
brain on local skin inflammation and itching, and itching exacerbates the problem in
a vicious circle. All findings point to the skin as a platform for an indirect connector
between the brain and a chronically activated immune system.

4.4 Environmental Factors Chronically Activate the Immune


System (No. 3)

We are talking here about extra-corporeal connectors, in which early trauma stimulates
exposure to an environmental factor more often, an environmental factor that in turn trig-
gers chronic immune system activation.

4.4.1 Where There’s Smoke, There’s Fire

In Sect. 3.2.1 under alcohol, nicotine and drugs, I presented the common follow-up prob-
lem of smoking. Early adversity leads to a significant increase in smoking in young and
old age (Iakunchykova et al. 2015; Smith et al. 2016; Hsu and Kawachi 2019). Smoking
is experienced as a reward (text around Fig. 3.1) and smoking changes important centers
in the brain, similar as a psychopharmaceutical can do (Jacobsen et al. 2004; Newhouse
et al. 2011). Not only people with early trauma smoke, but also patients with psychiatric
illnesses, especially schizophrenia. They treat themselves in a special way with smoking
(psychopharmaceutical).
4.4 Environmental Factors Chronically Activate the Immune System (No. 3) 185

Table 4.3  Smoking and chronic inflammatory disease


Chronic inflammatory disease is promoted Literature
Rheumatoid arthritis (Jiang und Alfredsson 2020; Prisco et al. 2020;
Qian et al. 2020; Jansen et al. 2021; Safy-Khan
et al. 2021)
Psoriasis vulgaris (psoriasis) (Zhou et al. 2020)
Psoriasis-Arthritis (psoriasis with arthritic (Dalmády et al. 2020)
joints)
Multiple Sclerosis (autoimmunity in the (Nishanth et al. 2020)
brain or spinal cord)
Periodontitis (Dalmády et al. 2020)
Systemic Lupus Erythematosus (multiple (Costenbader et al. 2004)
autoimmunity)
Thyroid Autoimmunity (Morbus Basedow) (Köhling et al. 2017)
Primary biliary cirrhosis (gallbladder (Howel et al. 2000)
­autoimmune disease)
Morbus Crohn (not in colitis ulcerosa!) (Berkowitz et al. 2018)

Smoking is thus stimulated by the brain and has a peripheral importance inde-
pendently of direct or indirect connectors, because it acts especially in the mouth, nose,
throat, lungs and gastrointestinal tract. Smoking is clearly associated with chronic inflam-
matory diseases (Table 4.3). How can smoking have this influence on these diseases?
Smoking can, for example, disturb the bacterial composition on inner and outer body
surfaces, the microbiome (Gui et al. 2021). The change in microbiome is associated
with chronic inflammatory diseases (van den Munckhof et al. 2018; Salem et al. 2019;
Dalmády et al. 2020; Aldars-García et al. 2021). Smoking, among other things, stimu-
lates a certain mouth bacterium called Porphyromonas gingivalis (short P. gingivalis),
which is significantly involved in gingivitis. This mouth bacterium has an interesting
enzyme called PAD (Peptidylarginine Deaminase), which converts proteins with the
amino acid arginine into proteins with the amino acid citrulline (Fig. 4.18).
While arginine is a typical amino acid of the human proteome, citrulline is not a typ-
ical amino acid. This can be dangerous because citrulline proteins can be recognized by
the immune system as foreign. The disturbance of the immune system, which leads to
an incorrect recognition of the citrulline proteins, is actually a cause of the most com-
mon chronic inflammatory rheumatic disease, rheumatoid arthritis (Hochberg et al.
2015; Manoil et al. 2021) and the arthritis associated with psoriasis (psoriatic arthritis)
(Dalmády et al. 2020). The one with the wrong arginine, namely citrulline, is also dis-
cussed as a possible cause in other chronic inflammatory diseases such as juvenile auto-
immune diabetes mellitus (Yang et al. 2021), multiple sclerosis (Gudmann et al. 2015)
and ankylosing spondylitis (Gudmann et al. 2015).
186 4 Chronic Immune System Activation

Fig. 4.18 Smoking stimulates Smoking


a bacterium that causes
inflammation. Smoking
stimulates Porphyromonas
gingivalis and this bacterium
can convert arginine proteins
into citrulline proteins because it
has peptidylarginine deaminase arginine
(PAD). This creates a foreign
bacterium:
protein (the blue part is
P. gingivalis
foreign). This process is called
citrullination. This foreign part is
attacked by the immune system
and corresponding antibodies are
peptidylarginine
produced (the pink Y-structure), deaminase
which recognize and bind
the antigen. Once an immune
memory is established, there autoimmune
is a permanent production of antibodies
corresponding autoimmune
antibodies that can lead to an
inflammatory reaction such changes are
recognized as foreign citrulline
by the immune system

Inflammation

In addition, smoking stimulates a type of T cell that is often involved in chronic


inflammatory diseases, namely the T helper type 17 T cell (Donate et al. 2021). Smoking
is associated with activation of lymphocytes and thrombocytes as well as increased
inflammation values in the blood (IL-6, C-reactive protein, fibrinogen) (Huan et al.
2016). Smoking can lead to epigenetic changes in leukocytes that persist after smoking
cessation for years (Joehanes et al. 2016).
Smoking induces oxidative stress, which in medicine is meant to be an increase in
dangerous and pro-inflammatory free radicals compared to anti-inflammatory radical
scavengers (Jansen et al. 2021). Oxidative stress arises as a result of previous adversities
(Horn et al. 2019), and so additive or synergistic effects can occur here. Smoking is pri-
marily responsible for structural lung changes, such as chronic obstructive lung disease,
which in turn, similar to atherosclerosis, entails a pro-inflammatory situation. A nice
overview of the topic of smoking and chronic inflammation is provided by a work from
the teaching hospital of Harvard University (Costenbader and Karlson 2006).
I can therefore summarize: Where there is smoke, there is fire; in other words: it
ignites. Inflammation and lung cancer are more common after long-term smoking. Since
4.4 Environmental Factors Chronically Activate the Immune System (No. 3) 187

unfortunate people with a difficult childhood or youth smoke much more often, we expe-
rience here with smoking the typical example of an extra-corporeal connector between
brain and immune system.

4.4.2 Where Fine Particles are Flying, It Gets Uncomfortable

Particles floating in the air are called dusts, and depending on the particle size, the occu-
pational physician speaks of coarse and fine dust. Dusts can contain pollutants that
adversely affect human health if no protective masks are worn. During the Corona pan-
demic, many people learned to wear protective masks, and that was important. I myself
now put on the mask more easily when I am exposed to an increased amount of dust
(household and gardening work). Nothing is so stupid (Corona pandemic) not to be good
for anything (mask use).
In the quarry, in construction, in mining, in dealing with sand (sandblasting, etc.), and
everywhere where stones are mechanically processed, such fine rock dusts can arise, to
give an example of particles floating in the air. Fine dusts are particularly relevant for
chronic inflammatory diseases because they can penetrate deep into the lungs via the tra-
chea and bronchi. The classic examples in medicine are chronic bronchitis and the dust
lung or silicosis, both of which are associated with the development of lung cancer and
have pro-inflammatory significance (Poinen-Rughooputh et al. 2016). In the foreground
are the quartz fine particles, which the chemist calls crystalline silicon dioxide (SiO2)
(silica). These silica dusts pack a punch.
People with silica exposure are more likely to suffer from the chronic inflammatory
joint disease of rheumatoid arthritis, as studies of a very large number of patients showed
(Prisco et al. 2020). These people are more likely to have other chronic inflammatory
autoimmune diseases, such as systemic sclerosis, systemic lupus erythematosus and vas-
culitis (ANCA-positive vasculitis and glomerulonephritis) (Miller et al. 2012).
The intruded fine particles can locally stimulate the innate inflammatory system in
the lung. The activation of, for example, macrophages takes place via surface receptors
that can normally detect infectious dangers (toll-like receptors and others) (Fig. 4.19)
(Anaya et al. 2016; Anaya et al. 2018). This permanent stimulation causes a cascade-like
immune activation in susceptible individuals, which can lead to an autoimmune disease.
Decisive is the loss of the actually existing immunological tolerance towards own pro-
teins (Anaya et al. 2016, 2018). Partly, the transformation of proteins is of importance,
as we have discussed them in Fig. 4.18. In this respect, smoking at the same time is
unfavorable.
What does it have to do with childhood adversities and where are the connectors?
Early experiences of stress often lead to a lower level of education and less upward
mobility (Pesonen et al. 2011; Heinonen et al. 2013). This connection was very clear in
the large Finnish study of children separated from their parents during World War II. If
the fathers were employed in manual occupations, children with separation experiences
188 4 Chronic Immune System Activation

lung
Inflammasome phagocytes

5
IL-1β 6 4
TNF
IFN-γ
IL-17 Nucleus
3
Phagolysosome

2
Phagosome 1
acute: inflammation
Silica
7 (300–1000 nm)

late: fibrosis

Fig. 4.19 Silica crystals cause inflammatory reaction and scarring fibrosis. 1) At the beginning is the
uptake of a silica crystal in a phagocyte of the lung, which is mediated by receptors on the cell surface.
2) The crystals are stored in an intermediate stage in a phagosome. 3) Phagosome and lysosome fuse to
the phagolysosome. 4) The presence of the silica crystals make the phagolysosome unstable, and parts
can escape into the cell interior. 5) The released elements now stimulate the inflammasome, and there
is activation of interleukin-1β (IL-1β). 6) More cytokines are stimulated (TNF: tumor necrosis factor,
IFN-γ: interferon gamma and IL-17: interleukin-17). 7) There is an acute inflammation, which manifests
itself as bronchitis and can later be converted into scarring fibrosis

showed no upward mobility, but rather a downward trend (Pesonen et al. 2011). Social
position is clearly linked to early traumatic experiences in a bidirectional way.
There is a connection between socio-economic disadvantages in childhood and ado-
lescence and school education and training, occupational choice and annual income
(Romito et al. 2003; Anand et al. 2019). Those occupations in the quarry, construction,
mining or handling of sand with direct exposure to silica dust are heavy manual occu-
pations that are not taken up by everyone and are found in lower positions on a com-
parative salary list (Federal Employment Agency 2020). Socio-economic disadvantages
are passed on, which is especially true if early experiences of stress are also present
(Pesonen et al. 2011). So the start of working life can be accompanied by an imbalance
in important health-impairing circumstances (e.g. more or less dust).
In this example—and I can list other airborne substances such as solvents, pesticides,
smoke from welding, agricultural dusts, fertilizers, cleaning solution aerosols, coal dusts,
soot, irritating gases (CO, NO, NO2, NOx, O3) etc. (Miller et al. 2012; Prisco et al. 2020;
Adami et al. 2021)—it becomes clear how an extra-corporeal connector can arise here
between early traumatic experiences, social position, occupational work and exposure to
4.4 Environmental Factors Chronically Activate the Immune System (No. 3) 189

particulate matter and chronic immune system activation. The matter is aggravated by
smoking cigarettes at the same time. The problem can only be prevented by consistent
protection—for example, a functional mask—and giving up smoking. Of course, the
employers and employees involved must play their part responsibly.
The above-mentioned connectors also make it clear why school and vocational train-
ing and the achievements resulting therefrom can be closely linked to the occurrence of
chronic inflammatory diseases, as a large genetic-epidemiological study recently showed
for rheumatoid arthritis (Jiang and Alfredsson 2020). There it becomes visible how a
favorable effect (absence of autoimmune disease) is less linked to training than to favora-
ble risk behavior (avoiding risks, e.g. dust, smoking and drugs). Avoiding risks is an edu-
cational or training issue.

4.4.3 Asthma

Asthma belongs in this subchapter of the extra-corporeal connectors, if the exposure to


environmental factors is increased by early traumas and these environmental factors trig-
ger asthma, which in turn leads to increased immune activation. When we talk about the
environment, we are thinking here of asthma with allergens (nearly 90% of all asthma
forms).
Asthma also fits into Sect. 4.3 Indirect connectors activate the immune system chron-
ically (No. 2), when factors from the brain trigger non-allergic asthma and the down-
stream lung involvement leads to chronic immune system activation. So there would be
an indirect connections through the lungs, leading to a chronic inflammatory situation. I
will deal with both forms here.

4.4.3.1 Environmental Factors (Extra-corporeal Connectors, 90% of All


Forms)
Typical stimuli for the development of asthma are allergens of all kinds of pollen and
grain dust, house dust mites, mold spores, animal hair, bird feathers and droppings, cer-
tain foods (e.g. nuts), chemicals (such as chlorine, varnishes or disinfectants) and others.
The question arises as to whether someone with early traumas comes into contact with
these allergens more often.
An intensive search in common literature databases yielded only 3 hits for the com-
bination of “various early traumatic experiences” plus “allergen” out of a total of more
than 40,000 hits for “various early traumatic experiences”. However, the 3 hits did not
contribute to understanding the discussed connection.
So there must be other environmental factors for allergic asthma that need to be con-
sidered. Table 4.4 shows the most common points for triggering asthma attacks. The
elements of Table 4.4 are in part closely related to early traumatic experiences—for
example smoking, fine dust, psychological stress and possibly painkillers and respira-
tory infections. If we look only at smoking and psychological stress, we do not need to
190 4 Chronic Immune System Activation

Table 4.4  The following Physical or sports activity (triggers exercise-induced asthma)


factors can trigger an asthma
attack independently of the Respiratory infections
type of asthma Certain medications (for example, some painkillers)
Cigarette smoke and exhaust fumes
Fine dust
Strong smells (salmiac)
Cold ambient air
Fog and wind
Psychological stress
Stress, social position, economic situation
From: (Novartis Pharma GmbH 2021)

research whether these things occur more frequently in people with early adversities. We
can say: These environmental factors for the triggering of asthma occur more often.
Regardless of the basis of asthma, there are environmental factors that are more com-
mon in people with early severe stress experiences and promote asthma and can trigger
an attack. In addition, prenatal stress situations, which mainly come from the mother, are
particularly asthmogenic when additional environmental factors such as gases and dusts
are also present (Bose et al. 2017; Rancière et al. 2017; Lee et al. 2018).
Finally, socio-economic background and school education play a role, as asthma is
more common in children/adolescents with a low social position and with less education
(Sternthal et al. 2011; Altman et al. 2021; Schyllert et al. 2021). Now, we have to clarify
whether asthma causes chronic immune system activation (in the subchapter after the
next one).

4.4.3.2 The Brain Promotes Non-allergic Asthma (Indirect Connector,


10% of All Forms)
Among some possibilities of central nervous system influence on the lungs, I would like
to highlight the direct influence through sensory nerve fibers that run with the vagus
nerve into the lungs, the best-studied way. These nerve fibers have proinflammatory neu-
ropeptides such as substance P and calcitonin gene-regulated peptide that can create an
inflammatory environment locally (Camp et al. 2021). Patients with asthma are hyper-
sensitive to substance P, substance P induces bronchoconstriction, substance P increases
local mucus production, facilitates cholinergic innervation of bronchi (narrowing) and
promotes plasma leakage from vessels (swelling) (Camp et al. 2021). Substance P can
activate mast cells and the local immune system (Sect. 4.2.5 Tone of Pain Pathways).
In addition, early trauma often results in increased pain sensitivity (see Sect. 3.2.7
More Pain after Childhood Adversities). This hypersensitivity is partly the product
of poorer inhibition of descending pain pathways from the brain (Figs. 3.3, 4.14, and
Sect. 4.2.5 Tone of Pain Pathways). Diseases such as fibromyalgia, chronic pelvic pain
4.4 Environmental Factors Chronically Activate the Immune System (No. 3) 191

syndrome or irritable bowel syndrome indicate this. Is this hypersensitivity regulated


by the brain involved in non-allergic asthma? Allergy researchers are asking the same
question for the non-allergic problems in the nose (Tai and Baraniuk 2002; Baraniuk and
Zheng 2010; Singh and Bernstein 2014; López-Requena et al. 2017).
This question of hypersensitivity in non-allergic asthma has been raised several times
and doctors have developed a test for this purpose (Millqvist et al. 1998; Pullerits et al.
2014). Today, many people recognize the phenomenon of hypersensitivity. Patients with
positive test results in the lungs also report more pain from mechanical stimuli on the
skin and have lower pain thresholds (Heba et al. 2020). The same people show conspic-
uous changes in brain networks that are responsive and altered after early trauma (Heba
et al. 2020).
Furthermore, people with asthma and positive test results have a worse course of envi-
ronmental allergic asthma (Kanemitsu et al. 2020). The brain can control the readiness to
cough and asthma, and the brain is involved in the triggering of non-allergic asthma. At
least it can worsen the situation in allergic asthma, as the mentioned study with positive
hypersensitivity test shows (Kanemitsu et al. 2020). The brain can negatively influence
the situation in asthma. Does asthma actually cause inflammation?

4.4.3.3 Asthma Makes Chronic Inflammation


Whether asthma is a chronic inflammatory disease is beyond doubt when looking at lung
tissue. However, the inflammation remains largely local and does not lead to large sys-
temic inflammatory reactions.
On the allergic side, processes take place in which so-called T helper cells type 2 (a
certain subtype of T cells) play a major role—similar to atopic dermatitis (Sect. 4.3.6).
These T helper cells type 2 create a favorable starting point for the production of anti-
bodies directed against the allergens that are produced by B cells/plasma cells. These
antibodies of type IgE continue the inflammatory process when allergens arrive in the
lungs by stimulating other cells, such as mast cells, eosinophilic granulocytes and bron-
chial muscle cells. A local cytokine cocktail of IL-4, IL-5, IL-13, etc. rounds off the
inflammatory process. All reactions lead to an acute inflammation of the lung mucosa,
which can lead to a change in the lung framework structure over years and to a chronic
obstructive lung disease (Fanta 2009; Rosa et al. 2018; Chau-Etchepare et al. 2019;
Hammad und Lambrecht 2021).
In non-allergic asthma, however, macrophages and so-called ILC type 2 (innate lym-
poid cells type 2) are involved in the inflammatory reaction, which activate eosinophilic
granulocytes and bronchial muscle cells. On this non-allergic side, there is a colorful
cytokine cocktail of IL-5, IL-9, IL-13, IL-15, IL-33, etc., which, in addition to prosta-
glandins, stimulates the inflammatory process (Chau-Etchepare et al. 2019; Hammad and
Lambrecht 2021). Similarly to allergic asthma, chronic activation leads to a remodeling
of the lung. Although asthma causes a relatively low systemic inflammation, the treating
physician notices a very slight inflammatory reaction of the whole organism on the basis
of the C-reactive protein in serum (Oh et al. 2020; Abdo et al. 2021; Zhu et al. 2021).
192 4 Chronic Immune System Activation

At this point, we therefore recognize how asthma is associated with a mild inflamma-
tory process, to be more precise: with a mild chronic immune system activation. Now we
ask whether asthma has a relationship to real autoimmune diseases, or whether asthma
is even a risk factor for an autoimmune disease. In fact, patients with asthma more often
have rheumatoid arthritis, as a meta-analysis showed (Charoenngam et al. 2020). How
asthma is causally related to autoimmune diseases is unclear.
I summarize: After early traumatic experiences, the brain can favor the triggering
of asthma either through extra-corporeal connectors (higher exposure to triggers) or
through indirect linking (pain nerve fibers, hypersensitivity, substance P) and thus con-
tribute to chronic immune system activation. In this respect, the asthma-triggering envi-
ronmental factors are extra-corporeal connectors between the brain and the immune
system.

4.4.4 Infections are Extra-corporeal Connectors

We have already discussed the links between smoking and periodontal disease. You will
remember the bacterium Porphyromonas gingivalis, which is associated with the devel-
opment of autoimmune diseases (Fig. 4.18). Smoking was the extra-corporeal connec-
tor. The relationship between early traumatic experiences and poor oral health has been
described several times (Poulton et al. 2002; Bright et al. 2015; Crouch et al. 2019; Ford
et al. 2020). The lack of dental care and the missed preventive examination were of great
importance (Crouch et al. 2019), so that periodontal infections can spread.
In the group of New Zealand children (Explanation 3), which has been followed up
over a long period of time since the early 1970s, a clear connection was found between
socio-economic status and periodontal diseases such as periodontitis and tooth decay
(Poulton et al. 2002). In the foreword of the editor, a connection to infection is men-
tioned (Power 2002). This study was confirmed more recently: The lower the socio-eco-
nomic position, the more pronounced the periodontal diseases were (Schuch et al. 2019).
So the socio-economic position of the parents/the affected persons is the trauma and at
the same time the extra-corporeal connector, which determines the bacterial composition
and thus promotes infection and chronic inflammation.
Furthermore, Roswith Roth from the Helmholtz center in Munich and many interna-
tional colleagues from the TEDDY study group were able to observe a clear connection
between negative life situations in childhood and a higher rate of infection episodes of
the respiratory tract (Roth et al. 2019). Since in this phase of life immunological toler-
ance and immunological aggression are learned, there is a danger of educational errors
of the immune system (Rook 2012; Murdaca et al. 2021; von Mutius 2021). So negative
effects in youth and adulthood can become important and lead to a kind of “misalign-
ment” of the immune system.
There is a special relationship to infection by sexually transmitted diseases. Why is
this relevant in early trauma? In people after early traumatic experiences, conspicuous
4.4 Environmental Factors Chronically Activate the Immune System (No. 3) 193

sexual behavior can be observed (Dillard et al. 2019), girls enter puberty earlier (Belsky
and Shalev 2016; Zhang et al. 2019; Suglia et al. 2020), they are younger and less expe-
rienced at first intercourse (Sansone et al. 2009), human trafficking for the purpose of
sexual exploitation is more common (Naramore et al. 2017; Reid et al. 2019b), sexual
behavior is risky (Naramore et al. 2017; Kidman et al. 2018; Alley et al. 2021), and
drug use with injections is more common. These risk factors increase the number of
infections.
Although the connection between infectious diseases on the one hand and the occur-
rence of chronic inflammatory autoimmune diseases on the other is not often causally
proven, there is a clear causal relationship for one or the other pathogen. For example,
the chain of infection with the Epstein-Barr virus (EBV) and multiple sclerosis is quite
obvious (Ascherio and Munger 2010; Jakhmola et al. 2021; Yuan et al. 2021). Since the
EBV infection or infectious mononucleosis is called kissing disease, the reference to the
awakening sexuality in adolescents is illustrated. If this happens more often and earlier,
as can happen after early trauma, the risk of EBV infection is higher.
The EBV infection can remain in the body without symptoms for a longer period of
time, and then those affected constantly produce antibodies against the virus. The con-
stantly increased antibody level in the blood speaks for a continuous unresolved infection
(Fagundes et al. 2013). These antibody levels are increased in people after early traumas,
and this speaks for the difficulty of finally defeating the pathogen (Fagundes et al. 2013;
Slopen et al. 2013; Yazawa et al. 2019). More often there is a latent virus infection that
is reactivated during stress situations (Schmeer et al. 2019). A study showed the connec-
tion between socio-economic situation and the occurrence of high EBV antibody levels.
People with a lower socio-economic status had higher EBV antibody concentrations in
their blood (Slopen et al. 2013).
Furthermore, the cytomegalovirus is more active in people with previous traumatic
experiences in childhood and adolescence, and increased antibody concentrations can be
seen in the blood (Fagundes et al. 2013; Elwenspoek et al. 2017; Reid et al. 2019a). The
continuous infection with cytomegalovirus has been linked to the aging of the immune
system and misguided immune activation (Fülöp et al. 2013; Weltevrede et al. 2016).
Early traumas are associated with a stronger reactivation of herpesviruses—cold sores—
(Shirtcliff et al. 2009).
Another human pathogenic virus is HTLV-1 (Human T-cell Lymphotropic Virus type
1), which is transmitted through sexual contact. HTLV-1 is associated with all sorts of
diseases and often an immune activation is visible (Schierhout et al. 2020). Apparently
it is causally related to chronic inflammatory diseases, for example Sjögren’s syndrome,
chronic bronchitis, asthma, rheumatoid arthritis and other forms of arthritis (Schierhout
et al. 2020).
To be up to date, we can look at the coronavirus pandemic of recent years and see a
connection here between trauma and chronic inflammation. According to a study from
Switzerland, people in lower socio-economic positions test themselves less for COVID-
19, but they are more often COVID-19-positive (Riou et al. 2021). A similar study from
194 4 Chronic Immune System Activation

Norway showed that, among immigrants who are socio-economically disadvantaged


(close living quarters, low income, little education), there are more frequent COVID-19
illnesses. Furthermore, the mortality rate from COVID-19 is higher among migrants than
among non-migrants (Vanthomme et al. 2021). And being a migrant is, per se, a trau-
matic experience for children and adolescents (Fazel et al. 2012; Kosidou et al. 2012;
Das-Munshi et al. 2014; Vossoughi et al. 2018; Chen et al. 2019).
If early childhood/adolescent trauma preceded, the trust in the official COVID-19
recommendations is significantly reduced. These people wear less mouth and nose pro-
tection and do not comply with the distance recommendations. They more often than
non-affected people refuse the proposed vaccinations. The key to understanding the con-
spicuous situation is the lack of trust of early traumatized people in the official advisers
(Bellis et al. 2022).
But how is it related to chronic inflammation? Children with COVID-19 more often
get a so-called multisystemic inflammatory reaction, which affects the skin, the mucous
membranes (mouth, stomach-intestine, urogenital, lung), the coronary vessels, the
heart muscle, the joints and the brain (Chen et al. 2021). Typical are fever and lymph
node swelling. This inflammatory disease has a lot in common with a disease that has
been known since 1967 as Kawasaki-Syndrom after the Japanese Tomisaku Kawasaki.
Ultimately, it is an acute-chronic inflammation of medium-sized vessels, and problems
can persist in the long term if the coronary vessels are affected (Hochberg et al. 2015). In
addition, many of the above-mentioned axes are shifted in the Long-COVID-Syndrome,
so that chronic immune system activation can result from this.
Liver inflammation caused by the Hepatitis C Virus, which is also transmitted sex-
ually or via injections, is another example of a chronic infection. If more risky sexual
behavior leads to a chronic hepatitis C infection, this can result in a state of permanent
immunoactivation (Younossi et al. 2016). Hepatitis C complications such as cryoglobu-
linemia (antibodies that form large complexes at low temperatures and induce inflamma-
tion), chronic kidney disease, type 2 diabetes mellitus, lymphoma, Sjögren’s syndrome,
and arthritis (similar to rheumatoid arthritis) are accompanied by permanent immunoac-
tivation. Patients often suffer from depression and chronic fatigue (Younossi et al. 2016).
Even prenatal stress for the child or stress events for the expectant mother influence
the later occurrence of infections in newborns (Nielsen et al. 2011).
At this point, the doctor would have mentioned sexually transmitted diseases such as
syphilis, gonorrhea, and other bacterial diseases without hesitation 100 years ago. These
diseases are no longer considered chronic inflammatory diseases in at least Western
countries because they are well treatable and therefore only of short duration. The situa-
tion for infections as extra-corporeal connectors is summarized in Fig. 4.20.
In all these examples, we can see how preceding adversities in childhood and adoles-
cence—for example, socio-economic position and other trauma experiences—increase
the risk of dangerous, often sexually transmitted infections and favor the persistence
of infection. In this way, risk factors from the environment are more easily accepted in
those affected by traumatic situations in young life stages, and there can be an increased
4.4 Environmental Factors Chronically Activate the Immune System (No. 3) 195

Traumas

Brain

No.3

Smoking - Porphyromonas gingivalis - Gum infection - Chronic


inflammation and immune activation
Early and frequent sexual experiences - Chronic viral diseases
(EBV, CMV, hepatitis viruses, HTLV-1, coronavirus, etc.) - Chronic
inflammation and immune activation
Lack of trust in medical advisors - more frequent infections
(e.g. COVID-19) - chronic inflammation and immune activation
Drugs - chronic viral diseases (HIV, hepatitis viruses, HTLV-1, etc.) -
chronic inflammation and immune activation

Immune system

Fig. 4.20 Traumata induce chronic infections. Abbreviations: CMV, cytomegalovirus; COVID-19,
coronavirus disease of 2019; EBV, Epstein-Barr virus; HIV, human immunodeficiency virus; HTLV-1,
human T-cell lymphotropic virus type 1

chronic immune system activation. The combination of several latent virus infections is
more inflammatory than the presence of a single virus. Here, the acquired infection acts
as an extra-corporeal connector between the brain and the immune system.

4.4.5 Risk Behavior

We have already learned about risk behavior in the context of infections. I am reminded
of sexually transmitted diseases. Risk behavior is also evident in increased alcohol con-
sumption, nicotine abuse, and drug use, as Vincent Felitti showed in his famous first
epidemiological study (Felitti et al. 1998). Risk behavior is also evident in overeating,
resulting in higher body weight and reduced physical activity as triggers of chronic
immune system activation.
We recognize risk behavior as externalizing behavior, for example as attention defi-
cit hyperactivity disorder and changes in social behavior. Symptoms such as attention
196 4 Chronic Immune System Activation

problems, increased activity, impulsivity, oppositional-defiant (disobedience) and aggres-


sive behavior are typical. In the long term, there is an increased risk of dropping out of
school prematurely, early parenthood, job problems, marital problems, violence, delin-
quency, etc. (Thapar et al. 2015). The risk of self-injury and other types of injuries is
increased (Jokela et al. 2009; Meza et al. 2021).
Risk behavior is also evident in an increased rate of injuries, such as head and brain
injuries and others (Rowe et al. 2004; Guinn et al. 2019). When more serious injuries
occur, such as with non-suicidal self-injury (Nock et al. 2006), we recognize the dan-
ger of chronic immune system activation. When there are severe injuries such as burns
(Piazza-Waggoner et al. 2005; Fritz and Butz 2007), there is long-term activation of the
immune system (Rendon and Choudhry 2012).
In this behavior defined by the brain and the injury situation with repercussions on
the body, we recognize extra-corporeal connectors between the brain and the immune
system.

4.5 Gene Mutations as Pleiotropic Connectors (No. 4)

In this chapter, I present a few genetic factors that have separate meanings in the brain
and in the immune system. In the brain, on the one hand, they promote unfavorable pro-
cesses that can lead to behavioral problems or psychiatric diseases. On the other hand,
they promote chronic immune system activation in the periphery. I call them pleiotropic
connectors, because they do not have direct material effects on each other because of the
separate place of action. Figure 4.21 illustrates the importance of pleiotropic connectors.
Today, the scientist can retrieve the pleiotropic connectors from databases on the
Internet, and to do it two things must be present at the same time (Tenesa et al. 2019;
Watanabe 2019; Watanabe et al. 2019; National Center for Biotechnology Information
2021). The database must first be fed with studies on genetic variants that are relevant to
various chronic inflammatory diseases or autoimmune diseases. I had already discussed
these studies under Phase 4—Human genome-wide association studies in Chap. 2.
Secondly, the database must be linked to genetic variants (also called single nucleo-
tide polymorphisms, short SNPs) that are relevant to a particular behavior or psychiatric
illness. The researchers call them phenome-wide association studies because they focus
on the visible phenotype—the character traits. With information from both databases,
one can bring together chronic inflammatory disease and behavior/psychiatric illness by
simultaneously focussing on a genetic variant. This type of study was carried out for the
pleiotropic connectors mentioned in this subchapter. I will discuss the search in more
detail in a moment.
This chapter could overwhelm some readers due to the complexity of the descrip-
tions of the found genetic variants, because it is about tiny details in the brain and in the
immune system. The author does not intend to overwhelm, which is why I will not pres-
ent the genetic variants found in the databases in detail. You must accept the mentioned
4.5 Gene Mutations as Pleiotropic Connectors (No. 4) 197

Early Change in
trauma Gen X

selfish brain

Smoking
Alcohol abuse
Drugs
Depression
Fear
Schizophrenia
Neurocism
amongst others
no material connecon, but pleiotropic connecon

Change in
Gen X

chronic inflammation

Chronic
Immune Acvaon
Chronic
Inflammatory disease

Fig. 4.21 Pleiotropic gene variants. Upper part of the figure: A gene with the fictitious name X leads
to the production of a factor that performs a certain function in the brain. A variant of the gene X is
associated with psychiatric problems. If this variant of the gene meets with early traumatic experiences,
the manifestation of the psychiatric problem can worsen.—Lower part of the figure: The same variant of
the fictitious gene X, which favors central nervous trauma sequelae in the brain, has a stimulating func-
tion in the immune system, causing chronic immune system activation. The gene has two different tasks
at the two different locations—in the brain and in the immune system—it is pleiotropic (Greek pleíōn,
Engl. more; Greek tropein, Engl. change), because more than one thing is changed by the gene. If the
variant of the gene causes chronic immune system activation, psychiatric problems and chronic inflam-
mation can occur at the same time, although the brain does not directly influence the immune system.—
Here, no material connection is needed to bring both things together at the same time. The pleiotropic
connection is sufficient to stimulate both at the same time
198 4 Chronic Immune System Activation

genetic variants without prejudice. This is relatively easy to do because even the geneti-
cists in the human genome-wide studies accept the found genetic variants that are related
to a disease or behavior in the same way without prejudice.
In Chap. 2 I wrote: “The genetic variants found in this way [without prejudice] were
often provided with a biological function by other working groups only in the course of
further research work, and biological plausibility post hoc shows up. This is painstaking
work that can take many years (e.g. Straub et al. 2018, 2021).”
The methods of human genome-wide and phenomenon-wide search for gene variants
associated with disease or behavior are free of hypotheses and prejudices. If the statis-
tics are correct, the researchers find gene variants that act as pleiotropic connectors. The
following search was carried out using known databases (Tenesa et al. 2019; Watanabe
2019; Watanabe et al. 2019; National Center for Biotechnology Information 2021).

4.5.1 The Search in the Gene Databases Leads to Pleiotropic


Connectors

I focused on known chronic inflammatory diseases, as listed in the first column of Table
4.5. For each of the diseases mentioned, a few to many gene variants were found in the
human genome-wide studies that are statistically highly significantly associated with
the disease. These gene variants, which the geneticists call SNPs (Single Nucleotide
Polymorphism, pronounced: snips), were given abbreviations that begin with the letters
“rs” for “reference SNP” (see column SNPs in Table 4.5). For different diseases, there
are different amounts of SNPs that act as genetic risk factors.
In the case of the chronic inflammatory joint disease of rheumatoid arthritis, genet-
icists have found more than 100 relevant SNPs (Okada et al. 2014). In the human
genome, therefore, more than 100 risk-associated gene variants are known that are
directly related to this particular chronic joint inflammation. However, the presence of a
SNP does not necessarily lead to the development of the disease, as a single risk-associ-
ated gene variant cannot trigger the disease on its own. If a person has several risk-asso-
ciated SNPs in the genome, the likelihood of developing the disease increases.
Now the expert can enter the name of a found SNP, which starts with “rs” and ends
with a number (see column SNP in Table 4.5), into a second database that contains infor-
mation about psychiatric diseases, behavioral aspects and many other characteristics.
This second database came about through human genome-wide and phenomenon-wide
studies. After entering the SNP, those psychiatric diseases, behavioral aspects and other
characteristics that are also significantly associated with the entered SNP are now men-
tioned (column “Meaning in the Brain” in Table 4.5). Of the more than 100 relevant
SNPs for rheumatoid arthritis, few are associated with psychiatric diseases, behavioral
aspects and other characteristics. Mostly it is a futile search for pleiotropic connectors,
but occasionally the lucky one hits a bull’s eye.
4.5 Gene Mutations as Pleiotropic Connectors (No. 4) 199

Table 4.5  Pleiotropic connectors in pleiotropic genes. Abbreviations: SNP, single nucleotide poly­
morphism (gene variant)
Chronic Name of the Gene Meaning in the Meaning in the Literature
inflam- SNP periphery brain
matory
situation
Rheumatoid rs8032939 RASGRP1 Proinflammatory Depression, *(Molineros

arthritis factor Schizophrenia et al. 2019)


rs4239702 CD40 Proinflammatory Depression, *

factor, B-cell Neuroticism


rs34695944 REL Many proinflam- Alcohol abuse *

matory functions
in immune cells
(partner of the proin-
flammatory factor
NFkappaB)
rs2736337 BLK B-cell function, proin- Neuroticism *

flammatory factor
Psoriasis rs702873 REL Many proinflam- Alcohol abuse *(Strange

matory functions et al. 2010)


in immune cells
(partner of the proin-
flammatory factor
NFkappaB)
rs240993 TRAF3IP2 Proinflammatory Smoking, *(Strange

factor, activates cannabis et al. 2010)


NFkappaB
Crohn’s rs6062496 TNFRSF6B Immunomodulatory Smoking *(Hsiehund
disease factor Lin 2017)
Thyroiditis rs2856698 HLA-DQB1 Proinflammatory Schizophrenia *

factor; antigen
presentation
rs8043085 RASGRP1 Proinflammatory Depression, *(Molineros

factor Schizophrenia et al. 2019)


Systemic rs7097397 WDFY4 T-cell supporting Depression *(Liet al.
lupus ery- factor, immunomodu- 2021a, b)
thematosus latory factor
rs2736337 BLK B-cell function, proin- Neuroticism *

flammatory factor
Asthma rs5743618 TLR1 Proinflammatory Alcohol abuse *

factor
rs773125 SUOX Immunomodulatory Smoking, high *(Luck et al.
factor body weight 2020)

(continued)
200 4 Chronic Immune System Activation

Table 4.5 (continued)


Chronic Name of the Gene Meaning in the Meaning in the Literature
inflam- SNP periphery brain
matory
situation
Chronic rs7733088 HTR4 Immunomodulatory Neuroticism *

obstructive factor, histamine


lung disease receptor 4
*(Tenesa et al. 2019; Watanabe 2019; Watanabe et al. 2019; National Center for Biotechnology
Information 2021). For neuroticism see Explanation 5

In a further step, the researcher asks himself in which gene the found SNP is present.
For this he needs another database, which is provided by the National Institute of Health
in Bethesda, USA (National Center for Biotechnology Information 2021). There he finds
the corresponding gene, which is of course responsible for a protein in the cell—he says:
encodes for a protein (see column “Gene” in Table 4.5). Now he can check in another
database of the National Institute of Health to what extent the protein is a key protein
for inflammation, and thus on the one hand he receives the significance in the context of
chronic immune system activation (see column “Meaning in the Periphery” in Table 4.5).
On the other hand, the corresponding SNP is closely associated with a psychiatric dis-
ease or unfavorable behavior (column “Meaning in the Brain” in Table 4.5). In further
studies, the researcher can work out why this SNP and the corresponding gene in the
brain have such a risk-related significance. I did not do this in Table 4.5 because I was
mainly interested in the chronic inflammatory situation and less in the function in the
brain. From Table 4.5 you can see a whole range of gene variants that are significantly
associated with the chronic inflammatory situation on the one hand and with the psychi-
atric problem on the other. In most cases, I can assess the significance for the immune
system as an immunologist and judge the chronic stimulation situation. Why these fac-
tors have a risk-related significance in the brain at the same time is not well researched,
but nevertheless highly relevant because it shows the pleiotropic connection.
So if I have a gene variant—a SNP—from Table 4.5 that is relevant to chronic acti-
vation of the immune system and even plays a role in triggering a chronic inflammatory
disease, and at the same time is associated with a psychiatric problem, this pleiotropic
connector can exacerbate the psychiatric problem in people after previous early traumas
on the one hand and stimulate the immune system on the other. This connection between
brain and immune system is possible without direct, indirect or extra-corporeal material
connectors.
In recent years, however, more and more statistical studies have been appear-
ing that, like in Table 4.5, bring together chronic inflammatory diseases and psychiat-
ric problems or features of brain function (for the interested: they are called Mendelian
Randomisation Studies). For example, one study linked the gene variants for rheumatoid
4.6 Summary 201

Table 4.6  Pleiotropic connectors from other genetic studies


Causative factor (closely linked Inflammatory follow-up Literature
to early trauma experiences) problems
Lower intelligence Rheumatoid arthritis (Jones et al. 2019; Huang et al.
2021)
Smoking Rheumatoid arthritis (Qian et al. 2020)
Depression Chronic inflammatory bowel (Luo et al. 2021)
diseases such as Crohn’s dis-
ease and ulcerative colitis
High body mass index Psoriasis (Budu-Aggrey et al. 2019)
Smoking Asthma (Skaaby et al. 2017)
Studies with the technique of Mendelian Randomisation

arthritis with the gene variants for intelligence. Here the researchers found a highly sig-
nificant link between the risk variants for rheumatoid arthritis and the risk variants for
lower intelligence (Huang et al. 2021).
Attention, in this analysis the researchers did not look at the actual outbreak of rheu-
matoid arthritis or at the actual intelligence, but only at the genetic risk of the two things
coming together. Other researchers confirmed the connection (Jones et al. 2019). Such
studies speak in favor of a pleiotropic connection in the sense of this chapter, since
reduced intelligence is a risk factor for subsequent problems of early traumatic experi-
ences and at the same time is closely related to the chronic inflammatory disease.
These studies with the designation Mendelian Randomisation have a causal charac-
ter that necessarily results from the epidemiological statistical approach. However, these
studies do not make any statements about the involved gene variants, as I was able to do
in Table 4.5 after days of searching. The approaches are different, but nevertheless prove
the existence of pleiotropic connectors. Table 4.6 summarises the studies on Mendelian
Randomisation.

4.6 Summary

In people with early adversity, it became apparent that, as with all four connectors, there
are several good reasons for stimulating and maintaining chronic immune system activa-
tion. With the direct connectors, often significant adjustment reactions occur after early
trauma. I bring to mind the normally anti-inflammatory but after switching the pro-in-
flammatory signal transduction through adrenergic receptors (noradrenaline, adrenaline)
and through the glucocorticoid receptor (cortisol). The circadian desynchronization of
the sympathetic system and the HPA axis, the increased tone of the pain pathways (sub-
stance P) and the change in the sex hormone axis exacerbate the process.
202 4 Chronic Immune System Activation

The inflammation is accompanied and additionally stimulated by indirect connectors


such as weight gain, insulin resistance, hyperinsulinemia, lack of physical activity and
stress-related increased intestinal permeability with changes in the microbiome. Extra-
corporeal connectors such as smoking, fine particles and increased risk-taking with
infection exacerbate the situation. If certain genetic variants are present that favor trauma
sequelae in the brain and inflammation by the immune system, there are further reasons
for escalation.
From this list, the observer can easily see the possibility of the formation of a vicious
circle, as it was already shown in Fig. 4.13, since the increase in inflammation further
promotes the switching reactions that negatively feedback on the brain, the pain sys-
tem and on psychological-psychiatric aspects. Important are weight gain and the lack
of physical activity (sarcopenic obesity), since both increasingly stimulate unfavorable
sequelae. We do not get a high body weight from one day to the next. For this we need
some years, as I know from my own experience.
In addition, accelerated cellular aging, as discussed in Sect. 3.3.5 (Entringer et al.
2012; Belsky and Shalev 2016), exacerbates the situation. Faster cell aging increases oxi-
dative stress, reduces the regenerative capacity of various tissues and increases the risk of
malignant transformation.
Special importance is attached to other stress situations occurring during life, since
accumulation is unfavorable, as Bruce McEwen mentioned in the theory of allostatic
overload (McEwen 1998). However, we cannot attribute the resulting problems of over-
load solely to events in the brain, since during accumulation the periphery increasingly
changes, as I wanted to emphasize in this book. Inflammation and pain act back on the
brain, overload is promoted by unfavorable peripheral sources, and so vicious circles can
arise (see Dantzer et al. 2014; Nusslock and Miller 2016; Danese and Lewis 2017).
In any case, the arrival of more than one severe stress experience during life is, so to
speak, an important platform for stabilizing the imbalance acquired in young years (the
out-of-balance weighing scale in Fig. 3.4) and thus of brain changes on the one hand and
inflammation on the other (Lupien et al. 2009; McLaughlin et al. 2010; Gustafsson et al.
2012; Daskalakis et al. 2013; Woodward et al. 2013; Loria et al. 2014; Oldehinkel et al.
2014; Dich et al. 2015; Farr et al. 2015; Halonen et al. 2015; Bandoli et al. 2017; Loeb
et al. 2018; Mayer et al. 2019). Similar cumulative findings have been demonstrated in
animals with early traumas and stress situations later in life (Stewart et al. 2004; Pinheiro
et al. 2012; Diz-Chaves et al. 2013; Prusator and Greenwood-Van Meerveld 2016b;
Zardooz et al. 2021).
We have now reached a point in this book where I can put down the pen or turn off
the computer because the most important psychoneuroimmunological points on the way
to chronic immune system activation have been discussed. I don’t want to make it that
easy for myself, because in the two previous books of this Springer trilogy I discussed
important aspects of energy regulation (Straub 2018, 2020a), and so energy will be the
concluding topic of this book. Here you will be surprised.
4.7 To the Point 203

4.7 To the Point

• Four connectors link the selfish brain with the selfish immune system. These connec-
tors must be responsible for chronic immune system activation.
• Connectors No. 1 are direct linking factors such as hormones and neurotransmitters of
the brain-initiated stress pathways that have a direct effect on the immune system.
• The sympathetic nervous system promotes inflammation. Important shift reactions
that change the effect of sympathetic neurotransmitters are of great importance.
• The parasympathetic nervous system is underactive and therefore cannot exercise the
normal anti-inflammatory function. The brain causes the lower activity of this efferent
nervous system.
• The HPA axis can stimulate inflammation, with the preceding tone before an immune
stimulus contributing to this. Important shift reactions prevent the anti-inflammatory
cortisol effect.
• The disturbance of the circadian rhythm is a consequence of earlier traumatic expe-
riences. The desynchronization of cortisol and noradrenaline is the pro-inflammatory
consequence.
• An increased tone of the pain pathways promotes inflammation by the increased
release of substance P and other neurotransmitters from the peripheral nerve ending.
The increased sensitization of pain pathways is mainly involved in this.
• The sex hormone axis is suppressed after trauma, and the lack of sex hormones has a
pro-inflammatory meaning for many immune reactions in men and women. Estrogens
have a dual meaning, as they inhibit T cells and macrophages, but promote B cell-
driven immunity, and the latter can stimulate an early B cell-mediated autoimmune
disease with an early menarche.
• Connectors No. 2 are indirect linking elements that are stimulated by the brain and
exert an effect in another organ system to indirectly activate the immune system.
• The brain is responsible for a high body weight and, thus, causes an inflammatory
reaction in the adipose tissue. At the same time, the brain stimulates the release of
free fatty acids, which can have a pro-inflammatory effect (especially if the eater has
consumed pro-inflammatory, saturated fatty acids days, weeks, and months before;
Junk Food Self Medication).
• The brain stimulates insulin resistance, which prevents the uptake of free fatty acids
and glucose in the adipose tissue, in the liver and in the muscle, which has a pro-in-
flammatory effect. The simultaneously increased blood levels of insulin can be proin-
flammatory. Insulin stimulates pain sensitization and promotes the proliferation of
leukocytes.
• The brain determines the extent of physical/athletic activity, which is often low after
early trauma. Since anti-inflammatory factors are produced in the muscle, the low
muscle mass and the low activity are coupled with a proinflammatory constellation.
204 4 Chronic Immune System Activation

• The brain can provoke a “leakage” of the intestinal wall via an increased activity
of the HPA axis and the sympathetic nervous system, which has a proinflammatory
effect because bacterial components activate the immune system inside the body. It is
called bacterial translocation.
• The brain exacerbates itching and pain in atopic dermatitis and can thus act on the
immune system in a proinflammatory way.
• Connectors No. 3 are extra-corporeal factors from the environment, which are stimu-
lated by the brain and act back on the immune system in a stimulatory way.
• Early traumatic experiences increase the willingness to smoke and exposure to dusts,
both of which have a proinflammatory effect.
• The brain promotes asthma and asthma stimulates inflammation. In this respect, there
is an extra-corporeal (higher exposure to extra-corporeal triggers) and an indirect con-
nector (without allergens: pain nerve fibers, hypersensitivity, substance P) between
the brain and the immune system.
• The brain is responsible for a higher level of infection (e.g. higher sexual risk behav-
ior), which contributes to chronic infections and chronic immune system activation.
• Connectors No. 4 are pleiotropic genes, whose gene variants (SNPs, snips) on the one
hand have an unfavorable effect on the brain in terms of worsening trauma sequelae
and on the other hand promote immune stimulation independently of this. These can
also be epigenetic changes to pleiotropic genes that were not discussed in the book.

References

Abdo M, Trinkmann F, Kirsten AM et al (2021) Small airway dysfunction links asthma severity
with physical activity and symptom control. J Allergy Clin Immunol Pract 9:3359–3368
Adami G, Viapiana O, Rossini M, Orsolini G, Bertoldo E, Giollo A, Gatti D, Fassio A (2021)
Association between environmental air pollution and rheumatoid arthritis flares. Rheumatology
(Oxford) 60:4591–4597
Aldars-García L, Chaparro M, Gisbert JP (2021) Systematic review: the gut microbiome and its
potential clinical application in inflammatory bowel disease. Microorganisms 9:977
Aley KO, Kinnman E, Levine JD (1996) Delayed sympathectomy after a prolonged hyperalgesia
results in a subsequent enhanced acute hyperalgesic response. Neuroscience 71:1083–1090
Allen H, Wright BJ, Vartanian K, Dulacki K, Li HF (2019) Examining the prevalence of adverse
childhood experiences and associated cardiovascular disease risk factors among low-income
uninsured adults. Circ Cardiovasc Qual Outcomes 12:e004391
Alley J, Owen RY, Wawrzynski SE, Lasrich L, Ahmmad Z, Utz R, Adkins DE (2021) Illness,
social disadvantage, and sexual risk behavior in adolescence and the transition to adulthood.
Arch Sex Behav 50:205–217
Aloe L, Tuveri MA, Levi-Montalcini R (1992) Studies on carrageenan-induced arthritis in adult
rats: presence of nerve growth factor and role of sympathetic innervation. Rheumatol Int
12:213–216
Altman MC, Calatroni A, Ramratnam S et al (2021) Endotype of allergic asthma with airway
obstruction in urban children. J Allergy Clin Immunol 148:1198–1209
References 205

Alvarez P, Green PG, Levine JD (2013) Stress in the adult rat exacerbates muscle pain induced by
early-life stress. Biol Psychiatry 74:688–695
Ambrósio G, Kaufmann FN, Manosso L, Platt N, Ghisleni G, Rodrigues ALS, Rieger DK,
Kaster MP (2018) Depression and peripheral inflammatory profile of patients with obesity.
Psychoneuroendocrinology 91:132–141
Amir D, Jordan MR, Bribiescas RG (2016) A longitudinal assessment of associations between
adolescent environment, adversity perception, and economic status on fertility and age of
menarche. PLoS One 11:e0155883
Anand KJS, Rigdon J, Rovnaghi CR et al (2019) Measuring socioeconomic adversity in early life.
Acta Paediatr 108:1267–1277
Anaya JM, Ramirez-Santana C, Alzate MA, Molano-Gonzalez N, Rojas-Villarraga A (2016) The
autoimmune ecology. Front Immunol 7:139
Anaya JM, Restrepo-Jiménez P, Ramírez-Santana C (2018) The autoimmune ecology: an update.
Curr Opin Rheumatol 30:350–360
Anderson DK, Rhees RW, Fleming DE (1985) Effects of prenatal stress on differentiation of
the sexually dimorphic nucleus of the preoptic area (SDN-POA) of the rat brain. Brain Res
332:113–118
Anderson EL, Heron J, Ben-Shlomo Y, Kuh D, Cooper R, Lawlor DA, Fraser A, Howe LD (2017)
Adversity in childhood and measures of aging in midlife: findings from a cohort of British
women. Psychol Aging 32:521–530
Anderson KG (2015) Father absence, childhood stress, and reproductive maturation in South
Africa. Hum Nat 26:401–425
Andersson NW, Hansen MV, Larsen AD, Hougaard KS, Kolstad HA, Schlünssen V (2016)
Prenatal maternal stress and atopic diseases in the child: a systematic review of observational
human studies. Allergy 71:15–26
Andus T, Geiger T, Hirano T, Kishimoto T, Heinrich PC (1988) Action of recombinant human
interleukin 6, interleukin 1 beta and tumor necrosis factor alpha on the mRNA induction of
acute-phase proteins. Eur J Immunol 18:739–746
Ao T, Kikuta J, Sudo T, Uchida Y, Kobayashi K, Ishii M (2020) Local sympathetic neurons pro-
mote neutrophil egress from the bone marrow at the onset of acute inflammation. Int Immunol
32:727–736
Appleton AA, Armstrong DA, Lesseur C, Lee J, Padbury JF, Lester BM, Marsit CJ (2013)
Patterning in placental 11-B hydroxysteroid dehydrogenase methylation according to prenatal
socioeconomic adversity. PLoS One 8:e74691
Ascherio A, Munger KL (2010) 99th Dahlem conference on infection, inflammation and chronic
inflammatory disorders: Epstein-Barr virus and multiple sclerosis: epidemiological evidence.
Clin Exp Immunol 160:120–124
Ashworth CJ, George SO, Hogg CO, Lai YT, Brunton PJ (2016) Sex-specific prenatal stress effects
on the rat reproductive axis and adrenal gland structure. Reproduction 151:709–717
Atlas LY, alʼAbsi M (2018) The neuroscience of pain: biobehavioral, developmental, and psycho-
social mechanisms relevant to intervention targets. Psychosom Med 80:788–790
Bach E, Møller AB, Jørgensen JO, Vendelbo MH, Jessen N, Pedersen SB, Nielsen TS, Møller N
(2016) Stress hormone release is a key component of the metabolic response to lipopolysaccha-
ride: studies in hypopituitary and healthy subjects. Eur J Endocrinol 175:455–465
Bae D, Wickrama KA, O’Neal CW (2014) Social consequences of early socioeconomic adversity
and youth BMI trajectories: gender and race/ethnicity differences. J Adolesc 37:883–892
Bailey MT, Lubach GR, Coe CL (2004) Prenatal stress alters bacterial colonization of the gut in
infant monkeys. J Pediatr Gastroenterol Nutr 38:414–421
206 4 Chronic Immune System Activation

Bailey MT, Engler H, Sheridan JF (2006) Stress induces the translocation of cutaneous and gas-
trointestinal microflora to secondary lymphoid organs of C57BL/6 mice. J Neuroimmunol
171:29–37
Bailey MT, Dowd SE, Galley JD, Hufnagle AR, Allen RG, Lyte M (2011) Exposure to a social
stressor alters the structure of the intestinal microbiota: implications for stressor-induced immu-
nomodulation. Brain Behav Immun 25:397–407
Baillie GS, Sood A, McPhee I, Gall I, Perry SJ, Lefkowitz RJ, Houslay MD (2003) beta-Arres-
tin-mediated PDE4 cAMP phosphodiesterase recruitment regulates beta-adrenoceptor switch-
ing from Gs to Gi. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 100:940–945
Bandoli G, Campbell-Sills L, Kessler RC et al (2017) Childhood adversity, adult stress, and the
risk of major depression or generalized anxiety disorder in US soldiers: a test of the stress sen-
sitization hypothesis. Psychol Med 47:2379–2392
Bannister K, Dickenson AH (2017) The plasticity of descending controls in pain: translational
probing. J Physiol 595:4159–4166
Baraniuk JN, Zheng Y (2010) Relationships among rhinitis, fibromyalgia, and chronic fatigue.
Allergy Asthma Proc 31:169–178
Barber AE, Coyle SM, Marano MA, Fischer E, Calvano SE, Fong Y, Moldawer LL, Lowry SF
(1993) Glucocorticoid therapy alters hormonal and cytokine responses to endotoxin in man. J
Immunol 150:1999–2006
Barber AE, Coyle SM, Fischer E, Smith C, van der Poll T, Shires GT, Lowry SF (1995) Influence
of hypercortisolemia on soluble tumor necrosis factor receptor II and interleukin-1 receptor
antagonist responses to endotoxin in human beings. Surgery 118:406–410
Barman SM, Yates BJ (2017) Deciphering the neural control of sympathetic nerve activity: status
report and directions for future research. Front Neurosci 11:730
Basbaum AI, Levine JD (1991) The contribution of the nervous system to inflammation and
inflammatory disease. Can J Physiol Pharmacol 69:647–651
Basbaum AI, Bautista DM, Scherrer G, Julius D (2009) Cellular and molecular mechanisms of
pain. Cell 139:267–284
Bath KG (2020) Synthesizing views to understand sex differences in response to early life adver-
sity. Trends Neurosci 43:300–310
Bellis MA, Hughes K, Ford K, Madden HCE, Glendinning F, Wood S (2022) Associations
between adverse childhood experiences, attitudes towards COVID-19 restrictions and vaccine
hesitancy: a cross-sectional study. BMJ Open 12:e053915
Belsky J, Shalev I (2016) Contextual adversity, telomere erosion, pubertal development, and
health: Two models of accelerated aging, or one? Dev Psychopathol 28:1367–1383
Benatti FB, Pedersen BK (2015) Exercise as an anti-inflammatory therapy for rheumatic diseas-
es-myokine regulation. Nat Rev Rheumatol 11:86–97
Berg RD (1999) Bacterial translocation from the gastrointestinal tract. Adv Exp Med Biol
473:11–30
Berkowitz L, Schultz BM, Salazar GA, Pardo-Roa C, Sebastián VP, Álvarez-Lobos MM, Bueno
SM (2018) Impact of cigarette smoking on the gastrointestinal tract inflammation: opposing
effects in Crohn’s disease and ulcerative colitis. Front Immunol 9:74
Bharwani A, Mian MF, Foster JA, Surette MG, Bienenstock J, Forsythe P (2016) Structural
& functional consequences of chronic psychosocial stress on the microbiome & host.
Psychoneuroendocrinology 63:217–227
Bierhaus A, Wolf J, Andrassy M et al (2003) A mechanism converting psychosocial stress into
mononuclear cell activation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 100:1920–1925
References 207

Birnie K, Cooper R, Martin RM et al (2011) Childhood socioeconomic position and objectively


measured physical capability levels in adulthood: a systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS
One 6:e15564
Bleil ME, Adler NE, Appelhans BM, Gregorich SE, Sternfeld B, Cedars MI (2013) Childhood
adversity and pubertal timing: understanding the origins of adulthood cardiovascular risk. Biol
Psychol 93:213–219
Boettger MK, Weber K, Grossmann D et al (2010) Spinal tumor necrosis factor alpha neutraliza-
tion reduces peripheral inflammation and hyperalgesia and suppresses autonomic responses in
experimental arthritis: a role for spinal tumor necrosis factor alpha during induction and main-
tenance of peripheral inflammation. Arthritis Rheum 62:1308–1318
Borovikova LV, Ivanova S, Zhang M et al (2000) Vagus nerve stimulation attenuates the systemic
inflammatory response to endotoxin. Nature 405:458–462
Bose S, Chiu YM, Hsu HL et al (2017) Prenatal nitrate exposure and childhood asthma. Influence
of maternal prenatal stress and fetal sex. Am J Respir Crit Care Med 196:1396–1403
Boyle DL, Jones TL, Hammaker D, Svensson CI, Rosengren S, Albani S, Sorkin L, Firestein GS
(2006) Regulation of peripheral inflammation by spinal p38 MAP kinase in rats. PLoS Med
3:e338
Brandt LJ (2015) Fecal microbiota transplant: respice, adspice, prospice. J Clin Gastroenterol
49:S65–S68
Brauer MM (2008) Cellular and molecular mechanisms underlying plasticity in uterine sympa-
thetic nerves. Auton Neurosci 140:1–16
Breivik T, Gundersen Y, Opstad PK, Fonnum F (2005) Chemical sympathectomy inhibits perio-
dontal disease in Fischer 344 rats. J Periodontal Res 40:325–330
Bridger Staatz C, Kelly Y, Lacey RE, Blodgett JM, George A, Arnot M, Walker E, Hardy R (2021)
Life course socioeconomic position and body composition in adulthood: a systematic review
and narrative synthesis. Int J Obes 45:2300–2315
Bright MA, Alford SM, Hinojosa MS, Knapp C, Fernandez-Baca DE (2015) Adverse childhood
experiences and dental health in children and adolescents. Community Dent Oral Epidemiol
43:193–199
Brion F, Parvez H, Parvez S, Marnay-Gulat C, Raoul Y (1978) Effects of glucocorticoids upon
adrenal and urinary epinephrine and norepinephrine and the activity of enzyme phenylethan-
olamine-N-methyltransferase in rats made partially deficient in vitamin D: role of vitamin D
supplementation. Horm Metab Res 10:556–560
Broug-Holub E, Kraal G (1996) Dose- and time-dependent activation of rat alveolar macrophages
by glucocorticoids. Clin Exp Immunol 104:332–336
Brunton PJ, Sullivan KM, Kerrigan D, Russell JA, Seckl JR, Drake AJ (2013) Sex-specific effects
of prenatal stress on glucose homoeostasis and peripheral metabolism in rats. J Endocrinol
217:161–173
Budu-Aggrey A, Brumpton B, Tyrrell J et al (2019) Evidence of a causal relationship between
body mass index and psoriasis: a Mendelian randomization study. PLoS Med 16:e1002739
Bulloch JM, Daly CJ (2014) Autonomic nerves and perivascular fat: interactive mechanisms.
Pharmacol Ther 143:61–73
Bundesagentur für Arbeit (2020) Entgeldatlas. https://con.arbeitsagentur.de/prod/entgeltatlas/.
Accessed 27 July 2021
Burke NN, Finn DP, McGuire BE, Roche M (2017) Psychological stress in early life as a predis-
posing factor for the development of chronic pain: Clinical and preclinical evidence and neuro-
biological mechanisms. J Neurosci Res 95:1257–1270
208 4 Chronic Immune System Activation

Buttgereit F, Doering G, Schaeffler A et al (2008) Efficacy of modified-release versus stand-


ard prednisone to reduce duration of morning stiffness of the joints in rheumatoid arthritis
(CAPRA-1): a double-blind, randomised controlled trial. Lancet 371:205–214
Buttgereit F, Burmester GR, Straub RH, Seibel MJ, Zhou H (2011) Exogenous and endogenous
glucocorticoids in rheumatic diseases. Arthritis Rheum 63:1–9
Buttgereit F, Mehta D, Kirwan J et al (2013) Low-dose prednisone chronotherapy for rheumatoid
arthritis: a randomised clinical trial (CAPRA-2). Ann Rheum Dis 72:204–210
Calder PC, Dimitriadis G, Newsholme P (2007) Glucose metabolism in lymphoid and inflamma-
tory cells and tissues. Curr Opin Clin Nutr Metab Care 10:531–540
Callaghan BL, Fields A, Gee DG et al (2020) Mind and gut: associations between mood and gas-
trointestinal distress in children exposed to adversity. Dev Psychopathol 32:309–328
Camp B, Stegemann-Koniszewski S, Schreiber J (2021) Infection-associated mechanisms of neu-
ro-inflammation and neuro-immune crosstalk in chronic respiratory diseases. Int J Mol Sci
22:5699
Campbell JA, Mendez CE, Garacci E, Walker RJ, Wagner N, Egede LE (2018) The differential
impact of adverse childhood experiences in the development of pre-diabetes in a longitudinal
cohort of US adults. J Diabetes Complicat 32:1018–1024
Capellino S, Cosentino M, Wolff C, Schmidt M, Grifka J, Straub RH (2010) Catecholamine-
producing cells in the synovial tissue during arthritis: modulation of sympathetic neurotrans-
mitters as new therapeutic target. Ann Rheum Dis 69:1853–1860
Capellino S, Weber K, Gelder M, Härle P, Straub RH (2012) First appearance and location of cat-
echolaminergic cells during experimental arthritis and elimination by chemical sympathectomy.
Arthritis Rheum 64:1110–1118
Carter JR, Ray CA (2015) Sympathetic neural adaptations to exercise training in humans. Auton
Neurosci 188:36–43
Chae BS (2021) Effect of low-dose corticosterone pretreatment on the production of inflammatory
mediators in super-low-dose LPS-primed immune cells. Toxicol Res 37:47–57
Chaloner A, Greenwood-Van Meerveld B (2013) Early life adversity as a risk factor for visceral
pain in later life: importance of sex differences. Front Neurosci 7:13
Chang HY, Suh DI, Yang SI et al (2016) Prenatal maternal distress affects atopic dermatitis in off-
spring mediated by oxidative stress. J Allergy Clin Immunol 138:468–475
Charoenngam N, Ponvilawan B, Rittiphairoj T, Tornsatitkul S, Wattanachayakul P, Rujirachun P,
Ungprasert P (2020) Patients with asthma have a higher risk of rheumatoid arthritis: a system-
atic review and meta-analysis. Semin Arthritis Rheum 50:968–976
Chau-Etchepare F, Hoerger JL, Kuhn BT, Zeki AA, Haczku A, Louie S, Kenyon NJ, Davis CE,
Schivo M (2019) Viruses and non-allergen environmental triggers in asthma. J Investig Med
67:1029–1041
Chen A, Panter-Brick C, Hadfield K, Dajani R, Hamoudi A, Sheridan M (2019) Minds under siege:
cognitive signatures of poverty and trauma in refugee and non-refugee adolescents. Child Dev
90:1856–1865
Chen HS, Qu F, He X, Wang Y, Wen WW (2010) Chemical or surgical sympathectomy prevents
mechanical hyperalgesia induced by intraplantar injection of bee venom in rats. Brain Res
1353:86–93
Chen MR, Kuo HC, Lee YJ, Chi H, Li SC, Lee HC, Yang KD (2021) Phenotype, susceptibility,
autoimmunity, and immunotherapy between Kawasaki disease and coronavirus disease-19
associated multisystem inflammatory syndrome in children. Front Immunol 12:632890
Cheval B, Chabert C, Sieber S et al (2019) Association between adverse childhood experiences
and muscle strength in older age. Gerontology 65:474–484
References 209

Choi J, Joseph L, Pilote L (2013) Obesity and C-reactive protein in various populations: a system-
atic review and meta-analysis. Obes Rev 14:232–244
Clarke G, Grenham S, Scully P, Fitzgerald P, Moloney RD, Shanahan F, Dinan TG, Cryan JF
(2013) The microbiome-gut-brain axis during early life regulates the hippocampal serotonergic
system in a sex-dependent manner. Mol Psychiatry 18:666–673
Contreras C, Nogueiras R, Diéguez C, Rahmouni K, López M (2017) Traveling from the hypothal-
amus to the adipose tissue: The thermogenic pathway. Redox Biol 12:854–863
Cooper DA, Duckett M, Hansen P, Petts V, Penny R (1981) Glucocorticosteroid enhancement of
immunoglobulin synthesis by pokeweed mitogen-stimulated human lymphocytes. Clin Exp
Immunol 44:129–136
Cooper R, Kuh D, Hardy R (2010) Objectively measured physical capability levels and mortality:
systematic review and meta-analysis. BMJ 341:c4467
Coplan JD, Rozenboym AV, Fulton SL et al (2018) Reduced left ventricular dimension and func-
tion following early life stress: a thrifty phenotype hypothesis engendering risk for mood and
anxiety disorders. Neurobiol Stress 8:202–210
Cosco TD, Hardy R, Howe LD, Richards M (2018) Early-life adversity, later-life mental
health, and resilience resources: a longitudinal population-based birth cohort analysis. Int
Psychogeriatr 23:1–10
Costenbader KH, Karlson EW (2006) Cigarette smoking and autoimmune disease: what can we
learn from epidemiology? Lupus 15:737–745
Costenbader KH, Kim DJ, Peerzada J, Lockman S, Nobles-Knight D, Petri M, Karlson EW (2004)
Cigarette smoking and the risk of systemic lupus erythematosus: a meta-analysis. Arthritis
Rheum 50:849–857
Coupland NJ, Wilson SJ, Potokar JP, Bell C, Nutt DJ (2003) Increased sympathetic response to
standing in panic disorder. Psychiatry Res 118:69–79
Crouch E, Nelson J, Radcliff E, Martin A (2019) Exploring associations between adverse child-
hood experiences and oral health among children and adolescents. J Public Health Dent
79:352–360
Cubbin C, Kim Y, Panisch LS (2019) Familial childhood adversity is associated with chronic dis-
ease among women: data from the geographic research on wellbeing (GROW) study. Matern
Child Health J 23:1117–1129
Cutolo M, Straub RH (2020) Sex steroids and autoimmune rheumatic diseases: state of the art. Nat
Rev Rheumatol 16:628–644
Daaka Y, Luttrell LM, Lefkowitz RJ (1997) Switching of the coupling of the beta2-adrenergic
receptor to different G proteins by protein kinase A. Nature 390:88–91
Dalmády S, Kemény L, Antal M, Gyulai R (2020) Periodontitis: a newly identified comorbidity in
psoriasis and psoriatic arthritis. Expert Rev Clin Immunol 16:101–108
Danese A, Lewis SJ (2017) Psychoneuroimmunology of early-life stress: the hidden wounds of
childhood trauma? Neuropsychopharmacology 42:99–114
Daniluk U, Filimoniuk A, Kowalczuk-Krystoń M, Alifier M, Karpińska J, Kaczmarski MG,
Lebensztejn DM (2019) Association of antioxidants and vitamin D level with inflammation in
children with atopic dermatitis. Int J Dermatol 58:1056–1061
Danse LH, Verschuren PM (1978) Fish oil-induced yellow fat disease in rats. I. Histological
changes. Vet Pathol 15:114–124
Dantzer R, Heijnen CJ, Kavelaars A, Laye S, Capuron L (2014) The neuroimmune basis of fatigue.
Trends Neurosci 37:39–46
Daskalakis NP, Bagot RC, Parker KJ, Vinkers CH, de Kloet ER (2013) The three-hit concept of
vulnerability and resilience: toward understanding adaptation to early-life adversity outcome.
Psychoneuroendocrinology 38:1858–1873
210 4 Chronic Immune System Activation

Das-Munshi J, Clark C, Dewey ME, Leavey G, Stansfeld SA, Prince MJ (2014) Born into adver-
sity: psychological distress in two birth cohorts of second-generation Irish children growing up
in Britain. J Public Health (Oxf) 36:92–103
Davis CR, Dearing E, Usher N et al (2014) Detailed assessments of childhood adversity enhance
prediction of central obesity independent of gender, race, adult psychosocial risk and health
behaviors. Metabolism 63:199–206
Davis LK, Bolton JL, Hanson H, Guarraci FA (2020) Modified limited bedding and nesting is a
model of early-life stress that affects reproductive physiology and behavior in female and male
Long-Evans rats. Physiol Behav 224:113037
de Castro Kroner J, Knoke K, Kofler DM, Steiger J, Fabri M (2018) Glucocorticoids promote
intrinsic human T(H)17 differentiation. J Allergy Clin Immunol 142:1669–1673.e1611
de Jonge WJ, van der Zanden EP, The FO et al (2005) Stimulation of the vagus nerve attenu-
ates macrophage activation by activating the Jak2-STAT3 signaling pathway. Nat Immunol
6:844–851
De Luigi A, Terreni L, Sironi M, De Simoni MG (1998) The sympathetic nervous system tonically
inhibits peripheral interleukin-1beta and interleukin-6 induction by central lipopolysaccharide.
Neuroscience 83:1245–1250
De Palma G, Blennerhassett P, Lu J et al (2015) Microbiota and host determinants of behavioural
phenotype in maternally separated mice. Nat Commun 6:7735
Deehan DJ, Heys SD, Simpson W, Herriot R, Broom J, Eremin O (1994) Correlation of serum
cytokine and acute phase reactant levels with alterations in weight and serum albumin in
patients receiving immunotherapy with recombinant IL-2. Clin Exp Immunol 95:366–372
Deitch EA, Bridges RM (1987) Effect of stress and trauma on bacterial translocation from the gut.
J Surg Res 42:536–542
DeKruyff RH, Fang Y, Umetsu DT (1998) Corticosteroids enhance the capacity of macrophages to
induce Th2 cytokine synthesis in CD4 PLUS_SPI lymphocytes by inhibiting IL-12 production.
J Immunol 160:2231–2237
Del Cerro MC, Ortega E, Gómez F, Segovia S, Pérez-Laso C (2015) Environmental prenatal stress
eliminates brain and maternal behavioral sex differences and alters hormone levels in female
rats. Horm Behav 73:142–147
DePasquale CE, Donzella B, Gunnar MR (2019) Pubertal recalibration of cortisol reactivity fol-
lowing early life stress: a cross-sectional analysis. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 60:566–575
Dhabhar FS (2014) Effects of stress on immune function: the good, the bad, and the beautiful.
Immunol Res 58:193–210
DiBattista JA, Martel-Pelletier J, Cloutier JM, Pelletier JP (1991) Modulation of glucocorticoid
receptor expression in human articular chondrocytes by cAMP and prostaglandins. J Rheumatol
Suppl 27:102–105
Dich N, Hansen ÅM, Avlund K, Lund R, Mortensen EL, Bruunsgaard H, Rod NH (2015) Early
life adversity potentiates the effects of later life stress on cumulative physiological dysregula-
tion. Anxiety Stress Coping 28:372–390
Dillard R, Maguire-Jack K, Showalter K, Wolf KG, Letson MM (2019) Abuse disclosures of youth
with problem sexualized behaviors and trauma symptomology. Child Abuse Negl 88:201–211
Diz-Chaves Y, Astiz M, Bellini MJ, Garcia-Segura LM (2013) Prenatal stress increases the expres-
sion of proinflammatory cytokines and exacerbates the inflammatory response to LPS in the
hippocampal formation of adult male mice. Brain Behav Immun 28:196–206
Donate PB, Alves de Lima K, Peres RS et al (2021) Cigarette smoke induces miR-132 in Th17
cells that enhance osteoclastogenesis in inflammatory arthritis. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A
118:e2017120118
References 211

Dong XW, Williams PA, Jia YP, Priestley T (2002) Activation of spinal ORL-1 receptors prevents
acute cutaneous neurogenic inflammation: role of nociceptin-induced suppression of primary
afferent depolarization. Pain 96:309–318
Dong Y, Aronsson M, Gustafsson JA, Okret S (1989) The mechanism of cAMP-induced gluco-
corticoid receptor expression. Correlation to cellular glucocorticoid response. J Biol Chem
264:13679–13683
Duggal NA, Niemiro G, Harridge SDR, Simpson RJ, Lord JM (2019) Can physical activity ame-
liorate immunosenescence and thereby reduce age-related multi-morbidity? Nat Rev Immunol
19:563–572
Dullens HF, Rademakers LH, Doffemont M, Van Veen PT, Bulder R, Den Otter W (1993)
Involvement of the omental lymphoid organ in the induction of peritoneal immunity against
tumor cells. Invasion Metastasis 13:267–276
Ebbinghaus M, Gajda M, Boettger MK, Schaible HG, Bräuer R (2012) The anti-inflammatory
effects of sympathectomy in murine antigen-induced arthritis are associated with a reduction of
Th1 and Th17 responses. Ann Rheum Dis 71:253–261
Ebbinghaus M, Natura G, Segond von Banchet G et al (2017) Interleukin-17A is involved in
mechanical hyperalgesia but not in the severity of murine antigen-induced arthritis. Sci Rep
7:10334
Eck SR, Ardekani CS, Salvatore M et al (2020) The effects of early life adversity on growth, matu-
ration, and steroid hormones in male and female rats. Eur J Neurosci 52:2664–2680
Ehrhart-Bornstein M, Hinson JP, Bornstein SR, Scherbaum WA, Vinson GP (1998) Intraadrenal
interactions in the regulation of adrenocortical steroidogenesis. Endocr Rev 19:101–143
Eickelberg O, Roth M, Lorx R, Bruce V, Rudiger J, Johnson M, Block LH (1999) Ligand-
independent activation of the glucocorticoid receptor by beta2-adrenergic receptor agonists in
primary human lung fibroblasts and vascular smooth muscle cells. J Biol Chem 274:1005–1010
El Aidy S, Ramsteijn AS, Dini-Andreote F, van Eijk R, Houwing DJ, Salles JF, Olivier JDA (2017)
Serotonin transporter genotype modulates the gut microbiota composition in young rats, an
effect augmented by early life stress. Front Cell Neurosci 11:222
Eller OC, Morris EM, Thyfault JP, Christianson JA (2020) Early life stress reduces voluntary
exercise and its prevention of diet-induced obesity and metabolic dysfunction in mice. Physiol
Behav 223:113000
Elwenspoek MMC, Sias K, Hengesch X et al (2017) T cell immunosenescence after early life
adversity: association with cytomegalovirus infection. Front Immunol 8:1263
Entringer S, Buss C, Wadhwa PD (2010) Prenatal stress and developmental programming of
human health and disease risk: concepts and integration of empirical findings. Curr Opin
Endocrinol Diabetes Obes 17:507–516
Entringer S, Buss C, Wadhwa PD (2012) Prenatal stress, telomere biology, and fetal programming
of health and disease risk. Sci Signal 5:pt12
Espinosa-Medina I, Saha O, Boismoreau F, Chettouh Z, Rossi F, Richardson WD, Brunet JF
(2016) The sacral autonomic outflow is sympathetic. Science 354:893–897
Essex MJ, Shirtcliff EA, Burk LR, Ruttle PL, Klein MH, Slattery MJ, Kalin NH, Armstrong JM
(2011) Influence of early life stress on later hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis functioning
and its covariation with mental health symptoms: a study of the allostatic process from child-
hood into adolescence. Dev Psychopathol 23:1039–1058
Fagundes CP, Glaser R, Malarkey WB, Kiecolt-Glaser JK (2013) Childhood adversity and herpes-
virus latency in breast cancer survivors. Health Psychol 32:337–344
Fanta CH (2009) Asthma. N Engl J Med 360:1002–1014
Fareri DS, Tottenham N (2016) Effects of early life stress on amygdala and striatal development.
Dev Cogn Neurosci 19:233–247
212 4 Chronic Immune System Activation

Farewell CV, Thayer ZM, Tracer DP, Morton S (2018) Prenatal stress exposure and early child-
hood BMI: exploring associations in a New Zealand context. Am J Hum Biol 30:e23116
Farr OM, Ko BJ, Joung KE et al (2015) Posttraumatic stress disorder, alone or additively with
early life adversity, is associated with obesity and cardiometabolic risk. Nutr Metab Cardiovasc
Dis 25:479–488
Fassold A, Falk W, Anders S, Hirsch T, Mirsky VM, Straub RH (2009) Soluble neuropilin-2, a
nerve repellent receptor, is increased in rheumatoid arthritis synovium and aggravates sympa-
thetic fiber repulsion and arthritis. Arthritis Rheum 60:2892–2901
Fazel M, Reed RV, Panter-Brick C, Stein A (2012) Mental health of displaced and refugee children
resettled in high-income countries: risk and protective factors. Lancet 379:266–282
Felitti VJ, Anda RF, Nordenberg D, Williamson DF, Spitz AM, Edwards V, Koss MP, Marks JS
(1998) Relationship of childhood abuse and household dysfunction to many of the leading
causes of death in adults. The Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACE) Study. Am J Prev Med
14:245–258
Fiacco S, Gardini ES, Mernone L, Schick L, Ehlert U (2019) DNA methylation in healthy older
adults with a history of childhood adversity-findings from the women 40+ healthy aging study.
Front Psychiatry 10:777
Flannery JE, Stagaman K, Burns AR, Hickey RJ, Roos LE, Giuliano RJ, Fisher PA, Sharpton TJ
(2020) Gut feelings begin in childhood: the gut metagenome correlates with early environment,
caregiving, and behavior. MBio 11:e02780–e02719
Ford K, Brocklehurst P, Hughes K, Sharp CA, Bellis MA (2020) Understanding the association
between self-reported poor oral health and exposure to adverse childhood experiences: a retro-
spective study. BMC Oral Health 20:51
Franchimont D, Galon J, Vacchio MS et al (2002) Positive effects of glucocorticoids on T cell
function by up-regulation of IL-7 receptor alpha. J Immunol 168:2212–2218
Frauwirth KA, Thompson CB (2004) Regulation of T lymphocyte metabolism. J Immunol
172:4661–4665
Fried G, Lundberg JM, Theodorsson-Norheim E (1985) Subcellular storage and axonal trans-
port of neuropeptide Y (NPY) in relation to catecholamines in the cat. Acta Physiol Scand
125:145–154
Friedman SJ, Winkelmann RK (1989) Subcutaneous fat necrosis of the newborn: light, ultrastruc-
tural and histochemical microscopic studies. J Cutan Pathol 16:99–105
Fritz KM, Butz C (2007) Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and pediatric burn injury: impor-
tant considerations regarding premorbid risk. Curr Opin Pediatr 19:565–569
Fuller-Rowell TE, Homandberg LK, Curtis DS, Tsenkova VK, Williams DR, Ryff CD (2019)
Disparities in insulin resistance between black and white adults in the United States: the role of
lifespan stress exposure. Psychoneuroendocrinology 107:1–8
Fülöp T, Larbi A, Pawelec G (2013) Human T cell aging and the impact of persistent viral infec-
tions. Front Immunol 4:271
Gavriilaki M, Sakellari I, Anagnostopoulos A, Gavriilaki E (2020) The impact of antibiotic-me-
diated modification of the intestinal microbiome on outcomes of allogeneic hematopoietic
cell transplantation: systematic review and meta-analysis. Biol Blood Marrow Transplant
26:1738–1746
Genovely H, Pfeifer MA (1988) RR-variation: the autonomic test of choice in diabetes. Diabetes
Metab Rev 4:255–271
Giles KM, Ross K, Rossi AG, Hotchin NA, Haslett C, Dransfield I (2001) Glucocorticoid augmen-
tation of macrophage capacity for phagocytosis of apoptotic cells is associated with reduced
p130Cas expression, loss of paxillin/pyk2 phosphorylation, and high levels of active Rac. J
Immunol 167:976–986
References 213

Gill SK, Teixeira A, Rama L et al (2015) Circulatory endotoxin concentration and cytokine pro-
file in response to exertional-heat stress during a multi-stage ultra-marathon competition. Exerc
Immunol Rev 21:114–128
Glod CA (1992) Circadian dysregulation in abused individuals: a proposed theoretical model for
practice and research. Arch Psychiatr Nurs 6:347–355
Golubeva AV, Crampton S, Desbonnet L et al (2015) Prenatal stress-induced alterations in
major physiological systems correlate with gut microbiota composition in adulthood.
Psychoneuroendocrinology 60:58–74
Gonzalez A, Jenkins JM, Steiner M, Fleming AS (2009) The relation between early life adver-
sity, cortisol awakening response and diurnal salivary cortisol levels in postpartum women.
Psychoneuroendocrinology 34:76–86
Graf N, McLean M, Capellino S, Schölmerich J, Murray GI, El-Omar EM, Straub RH (2012) Loss
of sensory and noradrenergic innervation in benign colorectal adenomatous polyps – a putative
role of semaphorins 3F and 3A. Neurogastroenterol Motil 24:120–128
Gray SAO, Jones CW, Theall KP, Glackin E, Drury SS (2017) Thinking across generations: unique
contributions of maternal early life and prenatal stress to infant physiology. J Am Acad Child
Adolesc Psychiatry 56:922–929
Grisanti LA, Woster AP, Dahlman J, Sauter ER, Combs CK, Porter JE (2011) α1-adrenergic recep-
tors positively regulate Toll-like receptor cytokine production from human monocytes and mac-
rophages. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 338:648–657
Gruol DJ, Campbell NF, Bourgeois S (1986) Cyclic AMP-dependent protein kinase promotes glu-
cocorticoid receptor function. J Biol Chem 261:4909–4914
Gudmann NS, Hansen NU, Jensen AC, Karsdal MA, Siebuhr AS (2015) Biological relevance of
citrullinations: diagnostic, prognostic and therapeutic options. Autoimmunity 48:73–79
Gui X, Yang Z, Li MD (2021) Effect of cigarette smoke on gut microbiota: state of knowledge.
Front Physiol 12:673341
Guinn AS, Ports KA, Ford DC, Breiding M, Merrick MT (2019) Associations between adverse
childhood experiences and acquired brain injury, including traumatic brain injuries, among
adults: 2014 BRFSS North Carolina. Inj Prev 25:514–520
Gunnar MR, DePasquale CE, Reid BM, Donzella B, Miller BS (2019) Pubertal stress recalibration
reverses the effects of early life stress in postinstitutionalized children. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S
A 116:23984–23988
Gur TL, Palkar AV, Rajasekera T, Allen J, Niraula A, Godbout J, Bailey MT (2019) Prenatal stress
disrupts social behavior, cortical neurobiology and commensal microbes in adult male off-
spring. Behav Brain Res 359:886–894
Gustafsson PE, Janlert U, Theorell T, Westerlund H, Hammarström A (2012) Social and material
adversity from adolescence to adulthood and allostatic load in middle-aged women and men:
results from the Northern Swedish Cohort. Ann Behav Med 43:117–128
Haas S, Capellino S, Phan NQ, Bohm M, Luger TA, Straub RH, Stander S (2010) Low density of
sympathetic nerve fibers relative to substance P-positive nerve fibers in lesional skin of chronic
pruritus and prurigo nodularis. J Dermatol Sci 58:193–197
Hak AE, Stehouwer CD, Bots ML, Polderman KH, Schalkwijk CG, Westendorp IC, Hofman A,
Witteman JC (1999) Associations of C-reactive protein with measures of obesity, insulin resist-
ance, and subclinical atherosclerosis in healthy, middle-aged women. Arterioscler Thromb Vasc
Biol 19:1986–1991
Halonen JI, Stenholm S, Pentti J, Kawachi I, Subramanian SV, Kivimäki M, Vahtera J (2015)
Childhood psychosocial adversity and adult neighborhood disadvantage as predictors of cardio-
vascular disease: a cohort study. Circulation 132:371–379
Hammad H, Lambrecht BN (2021) The basic immunology of asthma. Cell 184:1469–1485
214 4 Chronic Immune System Activation

Hantsoo L, Zemel BS (2021) Stress gets into the belly: early life stress and the gut microbiome.
Behav Brain Res 414:113474
Hantsoo L, Jašarević E, Criniti S et al (2019) Childhood adversity impact on gut microbiota and
inflammatory response to stress during pregnancy. Brain Behav Immun 75:240–250
Härle P, Möbius D, Carr DJ, Schölmerich J, Straub RH (2005) An opposing time-depend-
ent immune-modulating effect of the sympathetic nervous system conferred by altering the
cytokine profile in the local lymph nodes and spleen of mice with type II collagen-induced
arthritis. Arthritis Rheum 52:1305–1313
Härle P, Pongratz G, Albrecht J, Tarner IH, Straub RH (2008) An early sympathetic nervous sys-
tem influence exacerbates collagen-induced arthritis via CD4 PLUS_SPI CD25 PLUS_SPI
cells. Arthritis Rheum 58:2347–2355
Haug SR, Heyeraas KJ (2006) Modulation of dental inflammation by the sympathetic nervous sys-
tem. J Dent Res 85:488–495
Heba S, Sczesny-Kaiser M, Sucker K, Bünger J, Brüning T, Tegenthoff M, Schmidt-Wilcke T
(2020) Pain perception, brain connectivity, and neurochemistry in healthy, capsaicin-sensitive
subjects. Neural Plast 2020:9125913
Heidt T, Sager HB, Courties G et al (2014) Chronic variable stress activates hematopoietic stem
cells. Nat Med 20:754–758
Heinonen K, Eriksson JG, Kajantie E, Pesonen AK, Barker DJ, Osmond C, Raikkonen K (2013)
Late-preterm birth and lifetime socioeconomic attainments: the Helsinki birth cohort study.
Pediatrics 132:647–655
Heinricher MM, Tavares I, Leith JL, Lumb BM (2009) Descending control of nociception: speci-
ficity, recruitment and plasticity. Brain Res Rev 60:214–225
Herman JP, Nawreen N, Smail MA, Cotella EM (2020) Brain mechanisms of HPA axis regulation:
neurocircuitry and feedback in context - Richard Kvetnansky lecture. Stress 23:617–632
Hernández-Arteaga E, Hernández-González M, Rentería MLR, Almanza-Sepúlveda ML, Guevara
MA, Silva MA, Jaime HB (2016) Prenatal stress alters the developmental pattern of behavioral
indices of sexual maturation and copulation in male rats. Physiol Behav 163:251–257
Hirokawa J, Sakaue S, Tagami S, Kawakami Y, Sakai M, Nishi S, Nishihira J (1997) Identification
of macrophage migration inhibitory factor in adipose tissue and its induction by tumor necrosis
factor-alpha. Biochem Biophys Res Commun 235:94–98
Hochberg MC, Silman AJ, Smolen JS, Weinblatt ME, Weisman MH (2015) Rheumatology.
Elsevier Mosby, Philadelphia
Holmes L Jr, Shutman E, Chinaka C, Deepika K, Pelaez L, Dabney KW (2019) Aberrant epig-
enomic modulation of glucocorticoid receptor gene (NR3C1) in early life stress and major
depressive disorder correlation: systematic review and quantitative evidence synthesis. Int J
Environ Res Public Health 16:4280
Holochwost SJ, Wang G, Kolacz J, Mills-Koonce WR, Klika JB, Jaffee SR (2021) The neurophysi-
ological embedding of child maltreatment. Dev Psychopathol 33:1107–1137
Horn SR, Leve LD, Levitt P, Fisher PA (2019) Childhood adversity, mental health, and oxidative
stress: a pilot study. PLoS One 14:e0215085
Hotamisligil GS, Shargill NS, Spiegelman BM (1993) Adipose expression of tumor necrosis fac-
tor-alpha: direct role in obesity-linked insulin resistance. Science 259:87–91
Howel D, Fischbacher CM, Bhopal RS, Gray J, Metcalf JV, James OF (2000) An exploratory pop-
ulation-based case-control study of primary biliary cirrhosis. Hepatology 31:1055–1060
Hsieh SL, Lin WW (2017) Decoy receptor 3: an endogenous immunomodulator in cancer growth
and inflammatory reactions. J Biomed Sci 24:39
References 215

Hsu YT, Kawachi I (2019) Timing of family adversity during adolescence and its impact on alco-
hol and tobacco initiation: a longitudinal study among Taiwanese adolescents. Child Psychiatry
Hum Dev 50:257–267
Huan T, Joehanes R, Schurmann C et al (2016) A whole-blood transcriptome meta-analysis identi-
fies gene expression signatures of cigarette smoking. Hum Mol Genet 25:4611–4623
Huang G, Cai J, Li W, Zhong Y, Liao W, Wu P (2021) Causal relationship between educational
attainment and the risk of rheumatoid arthritis: a Mendelian randomization study. BMC
Rheumatol 5:47
Hwang PW, Dos Santos GC, Auais M, Braun KL, Guralnik JM, Pirkle CM (2019) Economic
adversity transitions from childhood to older adulthood are differentially associated with lat-
er-life physical performance measures in men and women in middle and high-income sites. J
Aging Health 31:509–527
Iakunchykova OP, Andreeva TI, Nordstrom DL, Shkiryak-Nizhnyk ZA, Antipkin YG, Hryhorczuk
DO, Zvinchuk AV, Chislovska NV (2015) The impact of early life stress on risk of tobacco
smoking initiation by adolescents. Addict Behav 50:222–228
Ieronymaki E, Daskalaki MG, Lyroni K, Tsatsanis C (2019) Insulin signaling and insulin resist-
ance facilitate trained immunity in macrophages through metabolic and epigenetic changes.
Front Immunol 10:1330
Inoue T, Abe C, Sung SS et al (2016) Vagus nerve stimulation mediates protection from kid-
ney ischemia-reperfusion injury through α7nAChR PLUS_SPI splenocytes. J Clin Invest
126:1939–1952
Jacobsen LK, D’Souza DC, Mencl WE, Pugh KR, Skudlarski P, Krystal JH (2004) Nicotine effects
on brain function and functional connectivity in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 55:850–858
Jakhmola S, Upadhyay A, Jain K, Mishra A, Jha HC (2021) Herpesviruses and the hidden links to
Multiple Sclerosis neuropathology. J Neuroimmunol 358:577636
Jansen G, de Rotte M, de Jonge R (2021) Smoking and methotrexate inefficacy in rheumatoid
arthritis: what about underlying molecular mechanisms? J Rheumatol 48:1495–1497
Jenei-Lanzl Z, Capellino S, Kees F, Fleck M, Lowin T, Straub RH (2015a) Anti-inflammatory
effects of cell-based therapy with tyrosine hydroxylase-positive catecholaminergic cells in
experimental arthritis. Ann Rheum Dis 74:444–451
Jenei-Lanzl Z, Zwingenberg J, Lowin T, Anders S, Straub RH (2015b) Proinflammatory recep-
tor switch from Galphas to Galphai signaling by beta-arrestin-mediated PDE4 recruitment in
mixed RA synovial cells. Brain Behav Immun 50:266–274
Jiang X, Alfredsson L (2020) Modifiable environmental exposure and risk of rheumatoid arthri-
tis-current evidence from genetic studies. Arthritis Res Ther 22:154
Joehanes R, Just AC, Marioni RE et al (2016) Epigenetic signatures of cigarette smoking. Circ
Cardiovasc Genet 9:436–447
Jokela M, Power C, Kivimäki M (2009) Childhood problem behaviors and injury risk over the life
course. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 50:1541–1549
Jones GT (2016) Psychosocial vulnerability and early life adversity as risk factors for central sen-
sitivity syndromes. Curr Rheumatol Rev 12:140–153
Jones HJ, Hubbard L, Mitchell RE, Jones SA, Williams NM, Zammit S, Hall J (2019) Association
of genetic risk for rheumatoid arthritis with cognitive and psychiatric phenotypes across child-
hood and adolescence. JAMA Netw Open 2:e196118
Kaiser S, Kruijver FP, Straub RH, Sachser N, Swaab DF (2003) Early social stress in male
Guinea-pigs changes social behaviour, and autonomic and neuroendocrine functions. J
Neuroendocrinol 15:761–769
216 4 Chronic Immune System Activation

Kalantaridou SN, Zoumakis E, Makrigiannakis A, Lavasidis LG, Vrekoussis T, Chrousos GP


(2010) Corticotropin-releasing hormone, stress and human reproduction: an update. J Reprod
Immunol 85:33–39
Kalmari J, Niissalo S, Konttinen YT, Pertovaara A (2001) Modulation of visceral nociceptive
responses of rat spinal dorsal horn neurons by sympathectomy. Neuroreport 12:797–801
Kamisoglu K, Sleight K, Nguyen TT, Calvano SE, Coyle SM, Corbett SA, Androulakis IP (2014)
Effects of coupled dose and rhythm manipulation of plasma cortisol levels on leukocyte tran-
scriptional response to endotoxin challenge in humans. Innate Immun 20:774–784
Kanemitsu Y, Fukumitsu K, Kurokawa R et al (2020) Increased capsaicin sensitivity in patients
with severe asthma is associated with worse clinical outcome. Am J Respir Crit Care Med
201:1068–1077
Kapoor A, Matthews SG (2008) Prenatal stress modifies behavior and hypothalamic-pituitary-ad-
renal function in female guinea pig offspring: effects of timing of prenatal stress and stage of
reproductive cycle. Endocrinology 149:6406–6415
Kapoor A, Matthews SG (2011) Testosterone is involved in mediating the effects of prenatal stress
in male guinea pig offspring. J Physiol 589:755–766
Katayama Y, Battista M, Kao WM, Hidalgo A, Peired AJ, Thomas SA, Frenette PS (2006) Signals
from the sympathetic nervous system regulate hematopoietic stem cell egress from bone mar-
row. Cell 124:407–421
Kaufman D, Banerji MA, Shorman I, Smith EL, Coplan JD, Rosenblum LA, Kral JG (2007)
Early-life stress and the development of obesity and insulin resistance in juvenile bonnet
macaques. Diabetes 56:1382–1386
Kaur L, Gordon M, Baines PA, Iheozor-Ejiofor Z, Sinopoulou V, Akobeng AK (2020) Probiotics
for induction of remission in ulcerative colitis. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 3:CD005573
Kees MG, Pongratz G, Kees F, Schölmerich J, Straub RH (2003) Via beta-adrenoceptors, stimula-
tion of extrasplenic sympathetic nerve fibers inhibits lipopolysaccharide-induced TNF secretion
in perfused rat spleen. J Neuroimmunol 145:77–85
Kerchner M, Malsbury CW, Ward OB, Ward IL (1995) Sexually dimorphic areas in the rat medial
amygdala: resistance to the demasculinizing effect of prenatal stress. Brain Res 672:251–260
Kern PA, Saghizadeh M, Ong JM, Bosch RJ, Deem R, Simsolo RB (1995) The expression of
tumor necrosis factor in human adipose tissue. Regulation by obesity, weight loss, and relation-
ship to lipoprotein lipase. J Clin Invest 95:2111–2119
Khoury JE, Bosquet Enlow M, Plamondon A, Lyons-Ruth K (2019) The association between
adversity and hair cortisol levels in humans: a meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology
103:104–117
Kidman R, Nachman S, Dietrich J, Liberty A, Violari A (2018) Childhood adversity increases the
risk of onward transmission from perinatal HIV-infected adolescents and youth in South Africa.
Child Abuse Negl 79:98–106
Kim K, Smith PK (1998) Childhood stress, behavioural symptoms and mother-daughter pubertal
development. J Adolesc 21:231–240
King LS, Colich NL, LeMoult J, Humphreys KL, Ordaz SJ, Price AN, Gotlib IH (2017)
The impact of the severity of early life stress on diurnal cortisol: the role of puberty.
Psychoneuroendocrinology 77:68–74
Kinnman E, Levine JD (1995) Involvement of the sympathetic postganglionic neuron in capsai-
cin-induced secondary hyperalgesia in the rat. Neuroscience 65:283–291
Kirschbaum C, Schommer N, Federenko I et al (1996) Short-term estradiol treatment enhances
pituitary-adrenal axis and sympathetic responses to psychosocial stress in healthy young men. J
Clin Endocrinol Metab 81:3639–3643
References 217

Klatt S, Stangl H, Kunath J, Lowin T, Pongratz G, Straub RH (2016) Peripheral elimination of the
sympathetic nervous system stimulates immunocyte retention in lymph nodes and ameliorates
collagen type II arthritis. Brain Behav Immun 54:201–210
Koeck FX, Bobrik V, Fassold A, Grifka J, Kessler S, Straub RH (2009) Marked loss of sympathetic
nerve fibers in chronic Charcot foot of diabetic origin compared to ankle joint osteoarthritis. J
Orthop Res 27:736–741
Koehl M, Barbazanges A, Le Moal M, Maccari S (1997) Prenatal stress induces a phase advance
of circadian corticosterone rhythm in adult rats which is prevented by postnatal stress. Brain
Res 759:317–320
Koehl M, Darnaudéry M, Dulluc J, Van Reeth O, Le Moal M, Maccari S (1999) Prenatal stress
alters circadian activity of hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis and hippocampal corticosteroid
receptors in adult rats of both gender. J Neurobiol 40:302–315
Köhling HL, Plummer SF, Marchesi JR, Davidge KS, Ludgate M (2017) The microbiota and auto-
immunity: their role in thyroid autoimmune diseases. Clin Immunol 183:63–74
Korn SH, Wouters EF, Wesseling G, Arends JW, Thunnissen FB (1998) Interaction between glu-
cocorticoids and beta2-agonists: alpha and beta glucocorticoid-receptor mRNA expression in
human bronchial epithelial cells. Biochem Pharmacol 56:1561–1569
Kosidou K, Hellner-Gumpert C, Fredlund P, Dalman C, Hallqvist J, Isacsson G, Magnusson C
(2012) Immigration, transition into adult life and social adversity in relation to psychological
distress and suicide attempts among young adults. PLoS One 7:e46284
Kuhlman KR, Geiss EG, Vargas I, Lopez-Duran NL (2015) Differential associations between
childhood trauma subtypes and adolescent HPA-axis functioning. Psychoneuroendocrinology
54:103–114
Kyrou I, Tsigos C (2008) Chronic stress, visceral obesity and gonadal dysfunction. Hormones
(Athens) 7:287–293
Laceulle OM, Nederhof E, van Aken MAG, Ormel J (2017) Adversity-driven changes in
hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis functioning during adolescence. The trails study.
Psychoneuroendocrinology 85:49–55
Lange T, Dimitrov S, Fehm HL, Westermann J, Born J (2006) Shift of monocyte function toward
cellular immunity during sleep. Arch Intern Med 166:1695–1700
Lange T, Dimitrov S, Bollinger T, Diekelmann S, Born J (2011) Sleep after vaccination boosts
immunological memory. J Immunol 187:283–290
Lanotte M, Metcalf D, Dexter TM (1982) Production of monocyte/macrophage colony-stim-
ulating factor by preadipocyte cell lines derived from murine marrow stroma. J Cell Physiol
112:123–127
Lázár BA, Jancsó G, Sántha P (2020) Modulation of sensory nerve function by insulin: possible
relevance to pain, inflammation and axon growth. Int J Mol Sci 21:2507
Lee A, Leon Hsu HH, Mathilda Chiu YH et al (2018) Prenatal fine particulate exposure and
early childhood asthma: effect of maternal stress and fetal sex. J Allergy Clin Immunol
141:1880–1886
Lesage J, Del-Favero F, Leonhardt M, Louvart H, Maccari S, Vieau D, Darnaudery M (2004)
Prenatal stress induces intrauterine growth restriction and programmes glucose intolerance and
feeding behaviour disturbances in the aged rat. J Endocrinol 181:291–296
Levine JD, Dardick SJ, Roizen MF, Helms C, Basbaum AI (1986a) Contribution of sensory affer-
ents and sympathetic efferents to joint injury in experimental arthritis. J Neurosci 6:3423–3429
Levine JD, Taiwo YO, Collins SD, Tam JK (1986b) Noradrenaline hyperalgesia is mediated
through interaction with sympathetic postganglionic neurone terminals rather than activation of
primary afferent nociceptors. Nature 323:158–160
218 4 Chronic Immune System Activation

Levine YA, Koopman FA, Faltys M, Caravaca A, Bendele A, Zitnik R, Vervoordeldonk MJ, Tak PP
(2014) Neurostimulation of the cholinergic anti-inflammatory pathway ameliorates disease in
rat collagen-induced arthritis. PLoS One 9:e104530
Li Y, Li J, Yuan Q et al (2021a) Deficiency in WDFY4 reduces the number of CD8(PLUS_SPI) T
cells via reactive oxygen species-induced apoptosis. Mol Immunol 139:131–138
Li Y, Wang A, Long F et al (2021b) Lack of WDFY4 aggravates ovalbumin-induced asthma via
enhanced Th2 cell differentiation. Int Arch Allergy Immunol 182:1089–1096
Lim HY, Muller N, Herold MJ, van den Brandt J, Reichardt HM (2007) Glucocorticoids exert
opposing effects on macrophage function dependent on their concentration. Immunology
122:47–53
Lin JC, Peng YJ, Wang SY, Young TH, Salter DM, Lee HS (2015) Role of the sympathetic nervous
system in carbon tetrachloride-induced hepatotoxicity and systemic inflammation. PLoS One
10:e0121365
Lin Q, Wu J, Willis WD (1999) Dorsal root reflexes and cutaneous neurogenic inflammation after
intradermal injection of capsaicin in rats. J Neurophysiol 82:2602–2611
Lin Q, Li D, Xu X, Zou X, Fang L (2007) Roles of TRPV1 and neuropeptidergic receptors in dor-
sal root reflex-mediated neurogenic inflammation induced by intradermal injection of capsaicin.
Mol Pain 3:30
Liu J, Huang S, Li F et al (2020) Sympathetic nerves positively regulate eosinophil-driven allergic
conjunctivitis via α1-adrenergic receptor signaling. Am J Pathol 190:1298–1308
Lockwood S, Dickenson AH (2020) What goes up must come down: insights from studies on
descending controls acting on spinal pain processing. J Neural Transm (Vienna) 127:541–549
Loeb TB, Joseph NT, Wyatt GE, Zhang M, Chin D, Thames A, Aswad Y (2018) Predictors of
somatic symptom severity: the role of cumulative history of trauma and adversity in a diverse
community sample. Psychol Trauma 10:491–498
Lönndahl L, Rasul A, Lonne-Rahm SB, Holst M, Johansson B, El-Nour H, Radu Djurfeldt
D, Nordlind K (2019) Tachykinin upregulation in atopic dermatitis. Immunopharmacol
Immunotoxicol 41:117–122
López-Requena A, Boonen B, Van Gerven L, Hellings PW, Alpizar YA, Talavera K (2017) In: Emir
TLR (eds) Roles of neuronal TRP channels in neuroimmune interactions. Neurobiology of TRP
channels. CRC Press/Taylor & Francis, Boca Raton
Loria AS, Ho DH, Pollock JS (2014) A mechanistic look at the effects of adversity early in life on
cardiovascular disease risk during adulthood. Acta Physiol (Oxford) 210:277–287
Lorton D, Lubahn C, Klein N, Schaller J, Bellinger DL (1999) Dual role for noradrenergic inner-
vation of lymphoid tissue and arthritic joints in adjuvant-induced arthritis. Brain Behav Immun
13:315–334
Lorton D, Bellinger DL, Schaller JA, Shewmaker E, Osredkar T, Lubahn C (2013) Altered sympa-
thetic-to-immune cell signaling via beta(2)-adrenergic receptors in adjuvant arthritis. Clin Dev
Immunol ID 764395:1–17
Low LA, Schweinhardt P (2012) Current research trends in early life stress and depression: review
of human studies on sensitive periods, gene-environment interactions, and epigenetics. Pain Res
Treat 2012:140832
Lu H, Xu M, Wang F, Liu S, Gu J, Lin S (2014) Chronic stress enhances progression of periodonti-
tis via α1-adrenergic signaling: a potential target for periodontal disease therapy. Exp Mol Med
46:e118
Lucassen PJ, Bosch OJ, Jousma E, Krömer SA, Andrew R, Seckl JR, Neumann ID (2009) Prenatal
stress reduces postnatal neurogenesis in rats selectively bred for high, but not low, anxiety:
possible key role of placental 11beta-hydroxysteroid dehydrogenase type 2. Eur J Neurosci
29:97–103
References 219

Luck K, Kim DK, Lambourne L et al (2020) A reference map of the human binary protein interac-
tome. Nature 580:402–408
Lundberg JM, Rudehill A, Sollevi A, Fried G, Wallin G (1989) Co-release of neuropeptide Y
and noradrenaline from pig spleen in vivo: importance of subcellular storage, nerve impulse
frequency and pattern, feedback regulation and resupply by axonal transport. Neuroscience
28:475–486
Lunkenheimer E, Busuito A, Brown KM, Skowron EA (2018) Mother-child coregulation of par-
asympathetic processes differs by child maltreatment severity and subtype. Child Maltreat
23:211–220
Luo J, Xu Z, Noordam R, van Heemst D, Li-Gao R (2021) Depression and inflammatory bowel
disease: a bidirectional two-sample Mendelian randomization study. J Crohns Colitis 5:jjab191
Lupien SJ, McEwen BS, Gunnar MR, Heim C (2009) Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on
the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci 10:434–445
Lutt JR (2009) Efficacy, safety, and tolerability of abatacept in the management of rheumatoid
arthritis. Open Access Rheumatol 1:17–35
Maciver NJ, Jacobs SR, Wieman HL, Wofford JA, Coloff JL, Rathmell JC (2008) Glucose metabo-
lism in lymphocytes is a regulated process with significant effects on immune cell function and
survival. J Leukoc Biol 84:949–957
Magnus MC, Anderson EL, Howe LD, Joinson CJ, Penton-Voak IS, Fraser A (2018) Childhood
psychosocial adversity and female reproductive timing: a cohort study of the ALSPAC mothers.
J Epidemiol Community Health 72:34–40
Manoil D, Courvoisier DS, Gilbert B, Möller B, Walker UA, Muehlenen IV, Rubbert-Roth A,
Finckh A, Bostanci N (2021) Associations between serum antibodies to periodontal pathogens
and preclinical phases of rheumatoid arthritis. Rheumatology (Oxford) 60:4755–4764
Marzioni D, Tamagnone L, Capparuccia L et al (2004) Restricted innervation of uterus and pla-
centa during pregnancy: evidence for a role of the repelling signal Semaphorin 3A. Dev Dyn
231:839–848
Mastorci F, Vicentini M, Viltart O et al (2009) Long-term effects of prenatal stress: changes in
adult cardiovascular regulation and sensitivity to stress. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 33:191–203
Maugeri D, Russo MS, Franzé C et al (1998) Correlations between C-reactive protein, interleu-
kin-6, tumor necrosis factor-alpha and body mass index during senile osteoporosis. Arch
Gerontol Geriatr 27:159–163
Maunder RG, Tannenbaum DW, Permaul JA, Nutik M, Haber C, Mitri M, Costantini D, Hunter JJ
(2019) The prevalence and clinical correlates of adverse childhood experiences in a cross-sec-
tional study of primary care patients with cardiometabolic disease or risk factors. BMC
Cardiovasc Disord 19:304
Mayer SE, Prather AA, Puterman E, Lin J, Arenander J, Coccia M, Shields GS, Slavich GM, Epel
ES (2019) Cumulative lifetime stress exposure and leukocyte telomere length attrition: the
unique role of stressor duration and exposure timing. Psychoneuroendocrinology 104:210–218
McEwen BS (1998) Protective and damaging effects of stress mediators. N Engl J Med
338:171–179
McEwen BS, Nasca C, Gray JD (2016) Stress effects on neuronal structure: hippocampus, amyg-
dala, and prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychopharmacology 41:3–23
McKenzie C, Silverberg JI (2020) Association of adverse childhood experiences with childhood
atopic dermatitis in the United States. Dermatitis 31:147–152
McKim DB, Patterson JM, Wohleb ES, Jarrett BL, Reader BF, Godbout JP, Sheridan JF (2016)
Sympathetic release of splenic monocytes promotes recurring anxiety following repeated social
defeat. Biol Psychiatry 79:803–813
220 4 Chronic Immune System Activation

McLaughlin KA, Conron KJ, Koenen KC, Gilman SE (2010) Childhood adversity, adult stressful
life events, and risk of past-year psychiatric disorder: a test of the stress sensitization hypothe-
sis in a population-based sample of adults. Psychol Med 40:1647–1658
Meddings JB, Swain MG (2000) Environmental stress-induced gastrointestinal permeability is
mediated by endogenous glucocorticoids in the rat. Gastroenterology 119:1019–1028
Mendiguren A, Aostri E, Pineda J (2018) Regulation of noradrenergic and serotonergic systems by
cannabinoids: relevance to cannabinoid-induced effects. Life Sci 192:115–127
Mendle J, Leve LD, Van Ryzin M, Natsuaki MN, Ge X (2011) Associations between early life
stress, child maltreatment, and pubertal development among girls in foster care. J Res Adolesc
21:871–880
Meregnani J, Clarençon D, Vivier M et al (2011) Anti-inflammatory effect of vagus nerve stimula-
tion in a rat model of inflammatory bowel disease. Auton Neurosci 160:82–89
Meyer-Hermann M, Figge MT, Straub RH (2009) Mathematical modeling of the circadian rhythm
of key neuroendocrine-immune system players in rheumatoid arthritis: a systems biology
approach. Arthritis Rheum 60:2585–2594
Meza JI, Owens EB, Hinshaw SP (2021) Childhood predictors and moderators of lifetime risk of
self-harm in girls with and without attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Dev Psychopathol
33:1351–1367
Mihara M, Nishimoto N, Ohsugi Y (2005) The therapy of autoimmune diseases by anti-interleu-
kin-6 receptor antibody. Expert Opin Biol Ther 5:683–690
Miksa M, Das P, Zhou M, Wu R, Dong W, Ji Y, Goyert SM, Ravikumar TS, Wang P (2009) Pivotal
role of the alpha(2A)-adrenoceptor in producing inflammation and organ injury in a rat model
of sepsis. PLoS One 4:e5504
Miller FW, Alfredsson L, Costenbader KH, Kamen DL, Nelson LM, Norris JM, De Roos AJ
(2012) Epidemiology of environmental exposures and human autoimmune diseases: find-
ings from a National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences Expert Panel Workshop. J
Autoimmun 39:259–271
Miller LE, Weidler C, Falk W, Angele P, Schaumburger J, Schölmerich J, Straub RH (2004)
Increased prevalence of semaphorin 3C, a repellent of sympathetic nerve fibers, in the synovial
tissue of patients with rheumatoid arthritis. Arthritis Rheum 50:1156–1163
Millqvist E, Bende M, Löwhagen O (1998) Sensory hyperreactivity – a possible mechanism
underlying cough and asthma-like symptoms. Allergy 53:1208–1212
Mishra I, Pullum KB, Thayer DC, Plummer ER, Conkright BW, Morris AJ, O’Hara BF, Demas
GE, Ashley NT (2020) Chemical sympathectomy reduces peripheral inflammatory responses
to acute and chronic sleep fragmentation. Am J Phys Regul Integr Comp Phys 318:R781–R789
Miskovic V, Schmidt LA, Georgiades K, Boyle M, MacMillan HL (2009) Stability of resting
frontal electroencephalogram (EEG) asymmetry and cardiac vagal tone in adolescent females
exposed to child maltreatment. Dev Psychobiol 51:474–487
Mohamed-Ali V, Goodrick S, Rawesh A, Katz DR, Miles JM, Yudkin JS, Klein S, Coppack SW
(1997) Subcutaneous adipose tissue releases interleukin-6, but not tumor necrosis factor-alpha,
in vivo. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 82:4196–4200
Molineros JE, Singh B, Terao C et al (2019) Mechanistic characterization of RASGRP1 variants
identifies an hnRNP-K-regulated transcriptional enhancer contributing to SLE susceptibility.
Front Immunol 10:1066
Morgan CP, Bale TL (2011) Early prenatal stress epigenetically programs dysmasculinization in
second-generation offspring via the paternal lineage. J Neurosci 31:11748–11755
Mousa SA, Shakibaei M, Sitte N, Schäfer M, Stein C (2004) Subcellular pathways of beta-endor-
phin synthesis, processing, and release from immunocytes in inflammatory pain. Endocrinology
145:1331–1341
References 221

Moussaoui N, Jacobs JP, Larauche M, Biraud M, Million M, Mayer E, Taché Y (2017) Chronic
early-life stress in rat pups alters basal corticosterone, intestinal permeability, and fecal micro-
biota at weaning: influence of sex. J Neurogastroenterol Motil 23:135–143
Muller PA, Schneeberger M, Matheis F et al (2020) Microbiota modulate sympathetic neurons via
a gut-brain circuit. Nature 583:441–446
Murdaca G, Greco M, Borro M, Gangemi S (2021) Hygiene hypothesis and autoimmune diseases:
a narrative review of clinical evidences and mechanisms. Autoimmun Rev 20:102845
Murray ET, Ben-Shlomo Y, Tilling K, Southall H, Aucott P, Kuh D, Hardy R (2013) Area depri-
vation across the life course and physical capability in midlife: findings from the 1946 British
Birth cohort. Am J Epidemiol 178:441–450
Nahvi RJ, Sabban EL (2020) Sex differences in the neuropeptide Y system and implications for
stress related disorders. Biomolecules 10:1248
Nakada MT, Stadel JM, Poksay KS, Crooke ST (1987) Glucocorticoid regulation of beta-adrener-
gic receptors in 3T3-L1 preadipocytes. Mol Pharmacol 31:377–384
Naramore R, Bright MA, Epps N, Hardt NS (2017) Youth arrested for trading sex have the highest
rates of childhood adversity: a statewide study of juvenile offenders. Sex Abus 29:396–410
National Center for Biotechnology Information (2021). Gene Database. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.
gov/home/genes/. Accessed 11 Jan 2021
Newhouse PA, Potter AS, Dumas JA, Thiel CM (2011) Functional brain imaging of nicotinic
effects on higher cognitive processes. Biochem Pharmacol 82:943–951
Nielsen NM, Hansen AV, Simonsen J, Hviid A (2011) Prenatal stress and risk of infectious dis-
eases in offspring. Am J Epidemiol 173:990–997
Nishanth K, Tariq E, Nzvere FP, Miqdad M, Cancarevic I (2020) Role of smoking in the pathogen-
esis of multiple sclerosis: a review article. Cureus 12:e9564
Nishigaki E, Abe T, Yokoyama Y, Fukaya M, Asahara T, Nomoto K, Nagino M (2014) The detec-
tion of intraoperative bacterial translocation in the mesenteric lymph nodes is useful in predict-
ing patients at high risk for postoperative infectious complications after esophagectomy. Ann
Surg 259:477–484
Nock MK, Joiner TE Jr, Gordon KH, Lloyd-Richardson E, Prinstein MJ (2006) Non-suicidal
self-injury among adolescents: diagnostic correlates and relation to suicide attempts. Psychiatry
Res 144:65–72
Novartis Pharma GmbH. Asthma und seine Ursachen. 2021 von https://www.asthma.de/asthma/
ursachen. Accessed 30 July 2021
Nusslock R, Miller GE (2016) Early-life adversity and physical and emotional health across the
lifespan: a neuroimmune network hypothesis. Biol Psychiatry 80:23–32
Ogawa H, Nielsen S, Kawakami M (1989) Cachectin/tumor necrosis factor and interleukin-1 show
different modes of combined effect on lipoprotein lipase activity and intracellular lipolysis in
3T3-L1 cells. Biochim Biophys Acta 1003:131–135
Oh JH, Ahn KM, Chung SJ, Shim JS, Park HW (2020) Usefulness of routine blood test-driven
clusters for predicting acute exacerbation in patients with asthma. Respir Med 170:106042
Oikarinen J, Hamalainen L, Oikarinen A (1984) Modulation of glucocorticoid receptor activity by
cyclic nucleotides and its implications on the regulation of human skin fibroblast growth and
protein synthesis. Biochim Biophys Acta 799:158–165
Okada Y, Wu D, Trynka G et al (2014) Genetics of rheumatoid arthritis contributes to biology and
drug discovery. Nature 506:376–381
Oldehinkel AJ, Ormel J, Verhulst FC, Nederhof E (2014) Childhood adversities and adolescent
depression: a matter of both risk and resilience. Dev Psychopathol 26:1067–1075
222 4 Chronic Immune System Activation

Ono S, Tsujimoto H, Yamauchi A, Hiraki S, Takayama E, Mochizuki H (2005) Detection of micro-


bial DNA in the blood of surgical patients for diagnosing bacterial translocation. World J Surg
29:535–539
Oosterman M, De Schipper JC, Fisher P, Dozier M, Schuengel C (2010) Autonomic reactivity in
relation to attachment and early adversity among foster children. Dev Psychopathol 22:109–118
Ordyan NE, Fedotova YO, Pivina SG (2013) Effects of prenatal stress on the activity of the pitui-
tary-ovarian system in female rats. Bull Exp Biol Med 155:433–435
Orskov J, Moltke O (1928) Studien über den Infektions-Mechanismus bei verschiedenen
Paratyphus-Infektionen aus weissen Mäusen. Z Immunitaetsforsch Exp Ther 59:357
Pace TW, Hu F, Miller AH (2007) Cytokine-effects on glucocorticoid receptor function: relevance
to glucocorticoid resistance and the pathophysiology and treatment of major depression. Brain
Behav Immun 21:9–19
Pedersen BK (2013) Muscle as a secretory organ. Compr Physiol 3:1337–1362
Pedersen BK, Fischer CP (2007) Physiological roles of muscle-derived interleukin-6 in response to
exercise. Curr Opin Clin Nutr Metab Care 10:265–271
Pereira OC, Bernardi MM, Gerardin DC (2006) Could neonatal testosterone replacement prevent
alterations induced by prenatal stress in male rats? Life Sci 78:2767–2771
Pérez-Laso C, Ortega E, Martín JL, Pérez-Izquierdo MA, Gómez F, Segovia S, Del Cerro MC
(2013) Maternal care interacts with prenatal stress in altering sexual dimorphism in male rats.
Horm Behav 64:624–633
Pertovaara A, Koivisto A (2011) TRPA1 ion channel in the spinal dorsal horn as a therapeutic tar-
get in central pain hypersensitivity and cutaneous neurogenic inflammation. Eur J Pharmacol
666:1–4
Pesonen AK, Räikkönen K, Kajantie E, Heinonen K, Osmond C, Barker DJ, Forsén T, Eriksson JG
(2011) Inter-generational social mobility following early life stress. Ann Med 43:320–328
Péterfalvi Á, Németh N, Herczeg R, Tényi T, Miseta A, Czéh B, Simon M (2019) Examining the
influence of early life stress on serum lipid profiles and cognitive functioning in depressed
patients. Front Psychol 10:1798
Piazza-Waggoner C, Dotson C, Adams CD, Joseph K, Goldfarb IW, Slater H (2005) Preinjury
behavioral and emotional problems among pediatric burn patients. J Burn Care Rehabil
26:371–378
Pinheiro RM, de Lima MN, Fries GR et al (2012) Early life stress exacerbates cognitive dys-
function induced by d-amphetamine: amelioration by valproic acid. J Neural Transm (Vienna)
119:627–637
Poinen-Rughooputh S, Rughooputh MS, Guo Y, Rong Y, Chen W (2016) Occupational exposure to
silica dust and risk of lung cancer: an updated meta-analysis of epidemiological studies. BMC
Public Health 16:1137
Pond CM, Mattacks CA (1995) Interactions between adipose tissue around lymph nodes and lym-
phoid cells in vitro. J Lipid Res 36:2219–2231
Pongratz G, Straub RH (2013) Role of peripheral nerve fibres in acute and chronic inflammation in
arthritis. Nat Rev Rheumatol 9:117–126
Pongratz G, Straub RH (2014) The sympathetic nervous response in inflammation. Arthritis Res
Ther 16:504
Pongratz G, Melzer M, Straub RH (2012) The sympathetic nervous system stimulates anti-inflam-
matory B cells in collagen-type II-induced arthritis. Ann Rheum Dis 71:432–439
Poulton R, Caspi A, Milne BJ, Thomson WM, Taylor A, Sears MR, Moffitt TE (2002) Association
between children’s experience of socioeconomic disadvantage and adult health: a life-course
study. Lancet 360:1640–1645
References 223

Powell ND, Sloan EK, Bailey MT et al (2013) Social stress up-regulates inflammatory gene
expression in the leukocyte transcriptome via β-adrenergic induction of myelopoiesis. Proc Natl
Acad Sci U S A 110:16574–16579
Power C (2002) Childhood adversity still matters for adult health outcomes. Lancet
360:1619–1620
Prisco LC, Martin LW, Sparks JA (2020) Inhalants other than personal cigarette smoking and risk
for developing rheumatoid arthritis. Curr Opin Rheumatol 32:279–288
Prusator DK, Greenwood-Van Meerveld B (2016a) Sex-related differences in pain behaviors fol-
lowing three early life stress paradigms. Biol Sex Differ 7:29
Prusator DK, Greenwood-Van Meerveld B (2016b) Sex differences in stress-induced visceral
hypersensitivity following early life adversity: a two hit model. Neurogastroenterol Motil
28:1876–1889
Pullerits T, Ternesten-Hasséus E, Johansson EL, Millqvist E (2014) Capsaicin cough threshold test
in diagnostics. Respir Med 108:1371–1376
Qian Y, Zhang L, Wu DJH, Xie Z, Wen C, Mao Y (2020) Genetic predisposition to smoking is
associated with risk of rheumatoid arthritis: a Mendelian randomization study. Arthritis Res
Ther 22:44
Quax RA, Manenschijn L, Koper JW, Hazes JM, Lamberts SW, van Rossum EF, Feelders RA
(2013) Glucocorticoid sensitivity in health and disease. Nat Rev Endocrinol 9:670–686
Ramírez F, Fowell DJ, Puklavec M, Simmonds S, Mason D (1996) Glucocorticoids promote a TH2
cytokine response by CD4 PLUS_SPI T cells in vitro. J Immunol 156:2406–2412
Rancière F, Bougas N, Viola M, Momas I (2017) Early exposure to traffic-related air pollution,
respiratory symptoms at 4 years of age, and potential effect modification by parental allergy,
stressful family events, and sex: a prospective follow-up study of the PARIS Birth Cohort.
Environ Health Perspect 125:737–745
Reber SO, Peters S, Slattery DA, Hofmann C, Schölmerich J, Neumann ID, Obermeier F (2011)
Mucosal immunosuppression and epithelial barrier defects are key events in murine psychoso-
cial stress-induced colitis. Brain Behav Immun 25:1153–1161
Reid BM, Harbin MM, Arend JL, Kelly AS, Dengel DR, Gunnar MR (2018) Early life adversity
with height stunting is associated with cardiometabolic risk in adolescents independent of body
mass index. J Pediatr 202:143–149
Reid BM, Coe CL, Doyle CM, Sheerar D, Slukvina A, Donzella B, Gunnar MR (2019a) Persistent
skewing of the T-cell profile in adolescents adopted internationally from institutional care.
Brain Behav Immun 77:168–177
Reid JA, Baglivio MT, Piquero AR, Greenwald MA, Epps N (2019b) No youth left behind to
human trafficking: exploring profiles of risk. Am J Orthopsychiatry 89:704–715
Reid BM, Horne R, Donzella B, Szamosi JC, Coe CL, Foster JA, Gunnar MR (2021) Microbiota-
immune alterations in adolescents following early life adversity: a proof of concept study. Dev
Psychobiol 63:851–863
Rendon JL, Choudhry MA (2012) Th17 cells: critical mediators of host responses to burn injury
and sepsis. J Leukoc Biol 92:529–538
Renz H, Henke A, Hofmann P, Wolff LJ, Schmidt A, Ruschoff J, Gemsa D (1992) Sensitization of
rat alveolar macrophages to enhanced TNF-alpha release by in vivo treatment with dexametha-
sone. Cell Immunol 144:249–257
Rice PA, Boehm GW, Moynihan JA, Bellinger DL, Stevens SY (2002) Chemical sympathectomy
increases numbers of inflammatory cells in the peritoneum early in murine listeriosis. Brain
Behav Immun 16:654–662
224 4 Chronic Immune System Activation

Richeri A, Chalar C, Martinez G, Greif G, Bianchimano P, Brauer MM (2011) Estrogen up-reg-


ulation of semaphorin 3F correlates with sympathetic denervation of the rat uterus. Auton
Neurosci 164:43–50
Rinnewitz L, Koenig J, Parzer P, Brunner R, Resch F, Kaess M (2018) Childhood adversity and
psychophysiological reactivity to pain in adolescent nonsuicidal self-injury. Psychopathology
51:346–352
Riol-Blanco L, Ordovas-Montanes J, Perro M, Naval E, Thiriot A, Alvarez D, Paust S, Wood JN,
von Andrian UH (2014) Nociceptive sensory neurons drive interleukin-23-mediated psoriasi-
form skin inflammation. Nature 510:157–161
Riou J, Panczak R, Althaus CL, Junker C, Perisa D, Schneider K, Criscuolo NG, Low N, Egger
M (2021) Socioeconomic position and the COVID-19 care cascade from testing to mortality in
Switzerland: a population-based analysis. Lancet Public Health 9:e683–e691
Robakis TK, Watson-Lin K, Wroolie TE, Myoraku A, Nasca C, Bigio B, McEwen B, Rasgon NL
(2019) Early life adversity blunts responses to pioglitazone in depressed, overweight adults.
Eur Psychiatry 55:4–9
Robson E, Norris T, Wulaningsih W, Hamer M, Hardy R, Johnson W (2020) The relationship
of early-life adversity with adulthood weight and cardiometabolic health status in the 1946
National Survey of Health and Development. Psychosom Med 82:82–89
Roggero E, Pérez AR, Pollachini N, Villar SR, Wildmann J, Besedovsky H, Del Rey A (2016) The
sympathetic nervous system affects the susceptibility and course of Trypanosoma cruzi infec-
tion. Brain Behav Immun 58:228–236
Rohleder N, Wolf JM, Kirschbaum C (2003a) Glucocorticoid sensitivity in humans-interindividual
differences and acute stress effects. Stress 6:207–222
Rohleder N, Wolf JM, Piel M, Kirschbaum C (2003b) Impact of oral contraceptive use on glu-
cocorticoid sensitivity of pro-inflammatory cytokine production after psychosocial stress.
Psychoneuroendocrinology 28:261–273
Romeo HE, Tio DL, Taylor AN (2009) Effects of superior cervical ganglionectomy on body
temperature and on the lipopolysaccharide-induced febrile response in rats. J Neuroimmunol
209:81–86
Romito P, Crisma M, Saurel-Cubizolles MJ (2003) Adult outcomes in women who experienced
parental violence during childhood. Child Abuse Negl 27:1127–1144
Rook GA (2012) Hygiene hypothesis and autoimmune diseases. Clin Rev Allergy Immunol
42:5–15
Rosa MJ, Lee AG, Wright RJ (2018) Evidence establishing a link between prenatal and early-life
stress and asthma development. Curr Opin Allergy Clin Immunol 18:148–158
Roth R, Lynch K, Hyöty H, Lönnrot M, Driscoll KA, Bennett JS (2019) The association between
stressful life events and respiratory infections during the first 4 years of life: the environmental
determinants of diabetes in the young study. Stress Health 35:289–303
Rothe N, Steffen J, Penz M, Kirschbaum C, Walther A (2020) Examination of peripheral basal and
reactive cortisol levels in major depressive disorder and the burnout syndrome: a systematic
review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 114:232–270
Rowe R, Maughan B, Goodman R (2004) Childhood psychiatric disorder and unintentional injury:
findings from a national cohort study. J Pediatr Psychol 29:119–130
Rudehill A, Olcén M, Sollevi A, Hamberger B, Lundberg JM (1987) Release of neuropeptide Y
upon haemorrhagic hypovolaemia in relation to vasoconstrictor effects in the pig. Acta Physiol
Scand 131:517–523
Ruiz R, Roque A, Pineda E, Licona-Limón P, José Valdéz-Alarcón J, Lajud N (2018) Early
life stress accelerates age-induced effects on neurogenesis, depression, and metabolic risk.
Psychoneuroendocrinology 96:203–211
References 225

Safy-Khan M, de Hair MJH, Welsing PMJ, van Laar JM, Jacobs JWG (2021) Current smoking
negatively affects the response to methotrexate in rheumatoid arthritis in a dose-responsive
way, independently of concomitant prednisone use. J Rheumatol 48:1504–1507
Salem F, Kindt N, Marchesi JR et al (2019) Gut microbiome in chronic rheumatic and inflamma-
tory bowel diseases: similarities and differences. United European Gastroenterol J 7:1008–1032
Sanders KM, Akiyama T (2018) The vicious cycle of itch and anxiety. Neurosci Biobehav Rev
87:17–26
Sandkühler J (1996) The organization and function of endogenous antinociceptive systems. Prog
Neurobiol 50:49–81
Sansone RA, Muennich E, Barnes J, Wiederman MW (2009) Childhood trauma and sexual behav-
ior in adulthood among internal medicine outpatients. Int J Psychiatry Clin Pract 13:341–344
Saridjan NS, Huizink AC, Koetsier JA, Jaddoe VW, Mackenbach JP, Hofman A, Kirschbaum C,
Verhulst FC, Tiemeier H (2010) Do social disadvantage and early family adversity affect the
diurnal cortisol rhythm in infants? The Generation R Study. Horm Behav 57:247–254
Schäffler A, Schölmerich J (2010) Innate immunity and adipose tissue biology. Trends Immunol
31:228–235
Schaible HG (2014) Nociceptive neurons detect cytokines in arthritis. Arthritis Res Ther 16:470
Schaible HG, Straub RH (2014) Function of the sympathetic supply in acute and chronic experi-
mental joint inflammation. Auton Neurosci 182:55–64
Schedlowski M, Hosch W, Oberbeck R, Benschop RJ, Jacobs R, Raab HR, Schmidt RE (1996)
Catecholamines modulate human NK cell circulation and function via spleen-independent beta
2-adrenergic mechanisms. J Immunol 156:93–99
Schierhout G, McGregor S, Gessain A, Einsiedel L, Martinello M, Kaldor J (2020) Association
between HTLV-1 infection and adverse health outcomes: a systematic review and meta-analysis
of epidemiological studies. Lancet Infect Dis 20:133–143
Schietroma M, Pessia B, Carlei F, Cecilia EM, De Santis G, Amicucci G (2015) Laparoscopic ver-
sus open colorectal surgery for colon cancer: the effect of surgical trauma on the bacterial trans-
location. A prospective randomized study. Am J Surg 210:263–269
Schmeer KK, Ford JL, Browning CR (2019) Early childhood family instability and immune sys-
tem dysregulation in adolescence. Psychoneuroendocrinology 102:189–195
Schmidt M, Straub RH (2015) 11beta-hydroxysteroid dehydrogenase enzymes modulate effects of
glucocorticoids in rheumatoid arthritis synovial cells. Neuroimmunomodulation 22:40–45
Schmidt P, Holsboer F, Spengler D (2001) beta(2)-adrenergic receptors potentiate glucocorticoid
receptor transactivation via G protein betagamma-Subunits and the phosphoinositide 3-kinase
pathway. Mol Endocrinol 15:553–564
Schöpper H, Klaus T, Palme R, Ruf T, Huber S (2012) Sex-specific impact of prenatal stress on
growth and reproductive parameters of guinea pigs. J Comp Physiol B 182:1117–1127
Schubert D, LaCorbiere M, Klier FG, Steinbach JH (1980) The modulation of neurotransmitter
synthesis by steroid hormones and insulin. Brain Res 190:67–79
Schuch HS, Nascimento GG, Peres KG et al (2019) The controlled direct effect of early-life socio-
economic position on periodontitis in a birth cohort. Am J Epidemiol 188:1101–1108
Schyllert C, Andersson M, Backman H, Lindberg A, Rönmark E, Hedman L (2021) Childhood
onset asthma is associated with lower educational level in young adults – a prospective cohort
study. Respir Med 186:106514
Seckl JR, Holmes MC (2007) Mechanisms of disease: glucocorticoids, their placental metabolism
and fetal ‚programming‘ of adult pathophysiology. Nat Clin Pract Endocrinol Metab 3:479–488
Selye H (1946) The general adaptation syndrome and the diseases of adaptation. J Clin Endocrinol
Metab 6:117–230
226 4 Chronic Immune System Activation

Severinsen MCK, Pedersen BK (2020) Muscle-organ crosstalk: the emerging roles of myokines.
Endocr Rev 41:594–609
Shakiba N, Ellis BJ, Bush NR, Boyce WT (2020) Biological sensitivity to context: a test of
the hypothesized U-shaped relation between early adversity and stress responsivity. Dev
Psychopathol 32:641–660
Shenk CE, Noll JG, Putnam FW, Trickett PK (2010) A prospective examination of the role of
childhood sexual abuse and physiological asymmetry in the development of psychopathology.
Child Abuse Negl 34:752–761
Shi Y, Dong Y, Huang W, Zhu D, Mao H, Su P (2016) Fecal microbiota transplantation for ulcera-
tive colitis: a systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS One 11:e0157259
Shimba A, Ikuta K (2020) Control of immunity by glucocorticoids in health and disease. Semin
Immunopathol 42:669–680
Shirtcliff EA, Coe CL, Pollak SD (2009) Early childhood stress is associated with elevated anti-
body levels to herpes simplex virus type 1. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106:2963–2967
Singh U, Bernstein JA (2014) Intranasal capsaicin in management of nonallergic (vasomotor) rhi-
nitis. Prog Drug Res 68:147–170
Sinikumpu SP, Huilaja L, Auvinen J, Jokelainen J, Puukka K, Ruokonen A, Timonen M, Tasanen
K (2018) The association between low grade systemic inflammation and skin diseases: a
cross-sectional survey in the Northern Finland birth cohort 1966. Acta Derm Venereol 98:65–69
Skaaby T, Taylor AE, Jacobsen RK et al (2017) Investigating the causal effect of smoking on hay
fever and asthma: a Mendelian randomization meta-analysis in the CARTA consortium. Sci
Rep 7:2224
Skowron EA, Loken E, Gatzke-Kopp LM, Cipriano-Essel EA, Woehrle PL, Van Epps JJ, Gowda
A, Ammerman RT (2011) Mapping cardiac physiology and parenting processes in maltreating
mother-child dyads. J Fam Psychol 25:663–674
Slopen N, McLaughlin KA, Dunn EC, Koenen KC (2013) Childhood adversity and cell-mediated
immunity in young adulthood: does type and timing matter? Brain Behav Immun 28:63–71
Sluka KA, Westlund KN (1993) Centrally administered non-NMDA but not NMDA receptor
antagonists block peripheral knee joint inflammation. Pain 55:217–225
Smith PH, Oberleitner LM, Smith KM, McKee SA (2016) Childhood adversity interacts with adult
stressful events to predict reduced likelihood of smoking cessation among women but not men.
Clin Psychol Sci 4:183–193
Spencer SJ (2013) Perinatal programming of neuroendocrine mechanisms connecting feeding
behavior and stress. Front Neurosci 7:109
Spengler RN, Allen RM, Remick DG, Strieter RM, Kunkel SL (1990) Stimulation of alpha-adr-
energic receptor augments the production of macrophage-derived tumor necrosis factor. J
Immunol 145:1430–4
Spengler RN, Chensue SW, Giacherio DA, Blenk N, Kunkel SL (1994) Endogenous norepineph-
rine regulates tumor necrosis factor-alpha production from macrophages in vitro. J Immunol
152:3024–31
Sprinz H, Gangarosa EJ, Williams M, Hornick RB, Woodward TE (1966) Histopathology of the
upper small intestines in typhoid fever. Biopsy study of experimental disease in man. Am J Dig
Dis 11:615–624
Stangl H, Springorum HR, Muschter D, Grässel S, Straub RH (2015) Catecholaminergic-to-
cholinergic transition of sympathetic nerve fibers is stimulated under healthy but not under
inflammatory arthritic conditions. Brain Behav Immun 46:180–191
Steensberg A, van Hall G, Osada T, Sacchetti M, Saltin B, Pedersen BK (2000) Production of
interleukin-6 in contracting human skeletal muscles can account for the exercise-induced
increase in plasma interleukin-6. J Physiol 529:237–242
References 227

Stenholm S, Harris TB, Rantanen T, Visser M, Kritchevsky SB, Ferrucci L (2008) Sarcopenic obe-
sity: definition, cause and consequences. Curr Opin Clin Nutr Metab Care 11:693–700
Sternthal MJ, Coull BA, Chiu YH, Cohen S, Wright RJ (2011) Associations among maternal child-
hood socioeconomic status, cord blood IgE levels, and repeated wheeze in urban children. J
Allergy Clin Immunol 128:337–345.e331
Stewart CA, Petrie RX, Balfour DJ, Matthews K, Reid IC (2004) Enhanced evoked responses
after early adversity and repeated platform exposure: the neurobiology of vulnerability? Biol
Psychiatry 55:868–870
Stojek MM, Maples-Keller JL, Dixon HD, Umpierrez GE, Gillespie CF, Michopoulos V (2019)
Associations of childhood trauma with food addiction and insulin resistance in African-
American women with diabetes mellitus. Appetite 141:104317
Strange A, Capon F, Spencer CC et al (2010) A genome-wide association study identifies new pso-
riasis susceptibility loci and an interaction between HLA-C and ERAP1. Nat Genet 42:985–990
Straub IR, Janer A, Weraarpachai W, Zinman L, Robertson J, Rogaeva E, Shoubridge EA (2018)
Loss of CHCHD10-CHCHD2 complexes required for respiration underlies the pathogenicity of
a CHCHD10 mutation in ALS. Hum Mol Genet 27:178–189
Straub IR, Weraarpachai W, Shoubridge EA (2021) Multi-OMICS study of a CHCHD10 variant
causing ALS demonstrates metabolic rewiring and activation of endoplasmic reticulum and
mitochondrial unfolded protein responses. Hum Mol Genet 30:687–705
Straub RH (2000) Tables of molecular and functional neuroendocrine immune interactions. Biozol,
Eching
Straub RH (2007) The complex role of estrogens in inflammation. Endocr Rev 28:521–574
Straub RH (2014) Insulin resistance, selfish brain, and selfish immune system: an evolutionarily
positively selected program used in chronic inflammatory diseases. Arthritis Res Ther 16(Suppl
2):S4
Straub RH (2018) Altern, Müdigkeit und Entzündungen verstehen – Wenn Immunsystem und
Gehirn um die Energie im Körper ringen. Springer, Berlin/Heidelberg
Straub RH (2020a) Drei Gedächtnisse für den Körper. Springer Nature, Berlin
Straub RH (2020b) The memory of the fatty acid system. Prog Lipid Res 79:101049
Straub RH, Cutolo M (2007) Circadian rhythms in rheumatoid arthritis: implications for patho-
physiology and therapeutic management. Arthritis Rheum 56:399–408
Straub RH, Günzler C, Miller LE, Cutolo M, Schölmerich J, Schill S (2002) Anti-inflammatory
cooperativity of corticosteroids and norepinephrine in rheumatoid arthritis synovial tissue
in vivo and in vitro. FASEB J 16:993–1000
Straub RH, Pongratz G, Weidler C, Linde HJ, Kirschning CJ, Glück T, Schölmerich J, Falk W
(2005) Ablation of the sympathetic nervous system decreases gram-negative and increases
gram-positive bacterial dissemination: key roles for tumor necrosis factor/phagocytes and inter-
leukin-4/lymphocytes. J Infect Dis 192:560–572
Straub RH, Grum F, Strauch U, Capellino S, Bataille F, Bleich A, Falk W, Schölmerich J,
Obermeier F (2008) Anti-inflammatory role of sympathetic nerves in chronic intestinal inflam-
mation. Gut 57:911–921
Straub RH, Dufner B, Rauch L (2020) Proinflammatory α-adrenergic neuronal regu-
lation of splenic IFN-γ, IL-6, and TGF-β of mice from day 15 onwards in arthritis.
Neuroimmunomodulation 27:58–68
Suarez A, Lahti J, Kajantie E, Eriksson JG, Räikkönen K (2017) Early life stress, FKBP5 polymor-
phisms, and quantitative glycemic traits. Psychosom Med 79:524–532
Sudo N, Chida Y, Aiba Y, Sonoda J, Oyama N, Yu XN, Kubo C, Koga Y (2004) Postnatal microbial
colonization programs the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal system for stress response in mice. J
Physiol 558:263–275
228 4 Chronic Immune System Activation

Suglia SF, Chen C, Wang S et al (2020) Childhood adversity and pubertal development among
Puerto Rican boys and girls. Psychosom Med 82:487–494
Suvas S (2017) Role of substance P neuropeptide in inflammation, wound healing, and tissue
homeostasis. J Immunol 199:1543–1552
Tai CF, Baraniuk JN (2002) Upper airway neurogenic mechanisms. Curr Opin Allergy Clin
Immunol 2:11–19
Tan SY, Yip A (2018) Hans Selye (1907–1982): Founder of the stress theory. Singap Med J
59:170–171
Tanenbaum HC, Li Y, Felicitas-Perkins JQ, Zhang M, Palmer P, Johnson CA, Xie B (2017) A lon-
gitudinal analysis of the impact of childhood stress on weight status among Chinese youth. Int J
Obes 41:820–823
Tannock GW, Savage DC (1974) Influences of dietary and environmental stress on microbial popu-
lations in the murine gastrointestinal tract. Infect Immun 9:591–598
Taves MD, Ashwell JD (2021) Glucocorticoids in T cell development, differentiation and function.
Nat Rev Immunol 21:233–243
Tenesa A, Canela-Xandri O, Rawlik K (2019). Gene ATLAS. http://geneatlas.roslin.ed.ac.uk/
Tesarz J, Eich W, Treede RD, Gerhardt A (2016) Altered pressure pain thresholds and increased
wind-up in adult patients with chronic back pain with a history of childhood maltreatment: a
quantitative sensory testing study. Pain 157:1799–1809
Thapar A, Pine DS, Leckman JF, Scott S, Snowling MJ, Taylor E (2015) Rutter’s child and adoles-
cent psychiatry, 6th edn. Wiley, Chichester
Thijs J, Krastev T, Weidinger S, Buckens CF, de Bruin-Weller M, Bruijnzeel-Koomen C, Flohr C,
Hijnen D (2015) Biomarkers for atopic dermatitis: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Curr
Opin Allergy Clin Immunol 15:453–460
Thomas C, Hyppönen E, Power C (2008) Obesity and type 2 diabetes risk in midadult life: the role
of childhood adversity. Pediatrics 121:e1240–e1249
Trentham DE, Townes AS, Kang AH (1977) Autoimmunity to type II collagen an experimental
model of arthritis. J Exp Med 146:857–868
Tsigos C, Papanicolaou DA, Kyrou I, Raptis SA, Chrousos GP (1999) Dose-dependent effects
of recombinant human interleukin-6 on the pituitary-testicular axis. J Interf Cytokine Res
19:1271–1276
Tsigos C, Kyrou I, Kassi E, Chrousos GP. Stress: endocrine physiology and pathophysiology. 2021
von https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK278995/. Accessed 1 Nov 2021
Turecki G, Meaney MJ (2016) Effects of the social environment and stress on glucocorticoid
receptor gene methylation: a systematic review. Biol Psychiatry 79:87–96
Uusitalo U, Liu X, Yang J et al (2016) Association of early exposure of probiotics and islet autoim-
munity in the TEDDY study. JAMA Pediatr 170:20–28
Valsamakis G, Chrousos G, Mastorakos G (2019) Stress, female reproduction and pregnancy.
Psychoneuroendocrinology 100:48–57
van den Munckhof ICL, Kurilshikov A, Ter Horst R et al (2018) Role of gut microbiota in chronic
low-grade inflammation as potential driver for atherosclerotic cardiovascular disease: a system-
atic review of human studies. Obes Rev 19:1719–1734
van der Leek AP, Bahreinian S, Chartier M, Dahl ME, Azad MB, Brownell MD, Kozyrskyj AL
(2020) Maternal distress during pregnancy and recurrence in early childhood predicts atopic
dermatitis and asthma in childhood. Chest 158:57–67
van Niekerk G, Christowitz C, Conradie D, Engelbrecht AM (2020) Insulin as an immunomodula-
tory hormone. Cytokine Growth Factor Rev 52:34–44
van Niekerk G, Christowitz C, Engelbrecht AM (2021) Insulin-mediated immune dysfunction in
the development of preeclampsia. J Mol Med (Berl) 99:889–897
References 229

Van Someren EJW (2021) Brain mechanisms of insomnia: new perspectives on causes and conse-
quences. Physiol Rev 101:995–1046
Vanthomme K, Gadeyne S, Lusyne P, Vandenheede H (2021) A population-based study on mortal-
ity among Belgian immigrants during the first COVID-19 wave in Belgium. Can demographic
and socioeconomic indicators explain differential mortality? SSM Popul Health 14:100797
Vargas J, Junco M, Gomez C, Lajud N (2016) Early life stress increases metabolic risk, HPA axis
reactivity, and depressive-like behavior when combined with postweaning social isolation in
rats. PLoS One 11:e0162665
Vélez MP, Alvarado B, Lord C, Zunzunegui MV (2010) Life course socioeconomic adversity
and age at natural menopause in women from Latin America and the Caribbean. Menopause
17:552–559
Veinante P, Yalcin I, Barrot M (2013) The amygdala between sensation and affect: a role in pain. J
Mol Psychiatry 1:9
Visser M, Bouter LM, McQuillan GM, Wener MH, Harris TB (1999) Elevated C-reactive protein
levels in overweight and obese adults. JAMA 282:2131–2135
von Mutius E (2021) The „hygiene hypothesis“ and the lessons learnt from farm studies. Front
Immunol 12:635522
Vossoughi N, Jackson Y, Gusler S, Stone K (2018) Mental health outcomes for youth living in ref-
ugee camps: a review. Trauma Violence Abuse 19:528–542
Wang H, Yu M, Ochani M et al (2003) Nicotinic acetylcholine receptor alpha7 subunit is an essen-
tial regulator of inflammation. Nature 421:384–388
Wang Q, Shelton RC, Dwivedi Y (2018) Interaction between early-life stress and FKBP5 gene var-
iants in major depressive disorder and post-traumatic stress disorder: a systematic review and
meta-analysis. J Affect Disord 225:422–428
Wastyk HC, Fragiadakis GK, Perelman D et al (2021) Gut-microbiota-targeted diets modulate
human immune status. Cell 184:4137–4153
Watanabe K (2019). GWAS ATLAS. https://atlas.ctglab.nl/
Watanabe K, Stringer S, Frei O et al (2019) A global overview of pleiotropy and genetic architec-
ture in complex traits. Nat Genet 51:1339–1348
Weidinger S, Beck LA, Bieber T, Kabashima K, Irvine AD (2018) Atopic dermatitis. Nat Rev Dis
Primers 4:1
Weisberg SP, McCann D, Desai M, Rosenbaum M, Leibel RL, Ferrante AW Jr (2003) Obesity is
associated with macrophage accumulation in adipose tissue. J Clin Invest 112:1796–1808
Weltevrede M, Eilers R, de Melker HE, van Baarle D (2016) Cytomegalovirus persistence and
T-cell immunosenescence in people aged fifty and older: A systematic review. Exp Gerontol
77:87–95
Werner ER, Werner-Felmayer G, Wachter H (1993) Tetrahydrobiopterin and cytokines. Proc Soc
Exp Biol Med 203:1–12
Werner ER, Blau N, Thöny B (2011) Tetrahydrobiopterin: biochemistry and pathophysiology.
Biochem J 438:397–414
Whitacre CC (2001) Sex differences in autoimmune disease. Nat Immunol 2:777–780
White RT, Damm D, Hancock N, Rosen BS, Lowell BB, Usher P, Flier JS, Spiegelman BM (1992)
Human adipsin is identical to complement factor D and is expressed at high levels in adipose
tissue. J Biol Chem 267:9210–9213
Wickrama KA, Kwon JA, Oshri A, Lee TK (2014) Early socioeconomic adversity and young
adult physical illness: the role of body mass index and depressive symptoms. J Adolesc Health
55:556–563
230 4 Chronic Immune System Activation

Wickrama KAS, Bae D, O’Neal CW (2017) Explaining the association between early adversity
and young adults’ diabetes outcomes: physiological, psychological, and behavioral mecha-
nisms. J Youth Adolesc 46:2407–2420
Wiegers GJ, Labeur MS, Stec IE, Klinkert WE, Holsboer F, Reul JM (1995) Glucocorticoids accel-
erate anti-T cell receptor-induced T cell growth. J Immunol 155:1893–1902
Wiegers GJ, Stec IE, Klinkert WE, Reul JM (2000) Glucocorticoids regulate TCR-induced eleva-
tion of CD4: functional implications. J Immunol 164:6213–6220
Willemze RA, Welting O, van Hamersveld HP et al (2018) Neuronal control of experimental coli-
tis occurs via sympathetic intestinal innervation. Neurogastroenterol Motil 30:e13163
Willis WD Jr (1988) Anatomy and physiology of descending control of nociceptive responses of
dorsal horn neurons: comprehensive review. Prog Brain Res 77:1–29
Wohleb ES, McKim DB, Shea DT, Powell ND, Tarr AJ, Sheridan JF, Godbout JP (2014)
Re-establishment of anxiety in stress-sensitized mice is caused by monocyte trafficking from
the spleen to the brain. Biol Psychiatry 75:970–981
Wolfe F (1997) The C-reactive protein but not erythrocyte sedimentation rate is associated with
clinical severity in patients with osteoarthritis of the knee or hip. J Rheumatol 24:1486–1488
Woodcock NP, Sudheer V, El-Barghouti N, Perry EP, MacFie J (2000) Bacterial translocation in
patients undergoing abdominal aortic aneurysm repair. Br J Surg 87:439–442
Woodward SH, Kuo JR, Schaer M, Kaloupek DG, Eliez S (2013) Early adversity and combat
exposure interact to influence anterior cingulate cortex volume in combat veterans. Neuroimage
Clin 2:670–674
Worlicek M, Knebel K, Linde HJ, Moleda L, Schölmerich J, Straub RH, Wiest R (2010)
Splanchnic sympathectomy prevents translocation and spreading of E coli but not S aureus in
liver cirrhosis. Gut 59:1127–1134
Xanthos DN, Sandkühler J (2014) Neurogenic neuroinflammation: inflammatory CNS reactions in
response to neuronal activity. Nat Rev Neurosci 15:43–53
Xie W, Chen S, Strong JA, Li AL, Lewkowich IP, Zhang JM (2016) Localized sympathectomy
reduces mechanical hypersensitivity by restoring normal immune homeostasis in rat models of
inflammatory pain. J Neurosci 36:8712–8725
Xu H, Barnes GT, Yang Q et al (2003) Chronic inflammation in fat plays a crucial role in the
development of obesity-related insulin resistance. J Clin Invest 112:1821–1830
Xu L, Yu WK, Lin ZL, Tan SJ, Bai XW, Ding K, Li N (2015) Chemical sympathectomy attenuates
inflammation, glycocalyx shedding and coagulation disorders in rats with acute traumatic coag-
ulopathy. Blood Coagul Fibrinolysis 26:152–160
Yang ML, Sodré FMC, Mamula MJ, Overbergh L (2021) Citrullination and PAD enzyme biology
in type 1 diabetes – regulators of inflammation, autoimmunity, and pathology. Front Immunol
12:678953
Yazawa A, Inoue Y, Cai G, Tu R, Huang M, He F, Chen J, Yamamoto T, Watanabe C (2019)
Association between early parental deprivation and cellular immune function among adults in
rural Fujian, China. Dev Psychobiol 61:1094–1099
Yeager MP, Rassias AJ, Pioli PA, Beach ML, Wardwell K, Collins JE, Lee HK, Guyre PM (2009)
Pretreatment with stress cortisol enhances the human systemic inflammatory response to bacte-
rial endotoxin. Crit Care Med 37:2727–2732
Yeager MP, Pioli PA, Collins J, Barr F, Metzler S, Sites BD, Guyre PM (2016) Glucocorticoids
enhance the in vivo migratory response of human monocytes. Brain Behav Immun 54:86–94
Yehuda R, Bierer LM, Andrew R, Schmeidler J, Seckl JR (2009) Enduring effects of severe
developmental adversity, including nutritional deprivation, on cortisol metabolism in aging
Holocaust survivors. J Psychiatr Res 43:877–883
References 231

You DS, Meagher MW (2016) Childhood adversity and pain sensitization. Psychosom Med
78:1084–1093
You DS, Meagher MW (2018) Childhood adversity and pain facilitation. Psychosom Med
80:869–879
Younossi Z, Park H, Henry L, Adeyemi A, Stepanova M (2016) Extrahepatic manifesta-
tions of hepatitis C: a meta-analysis of prevalence, quality of life, and economic burden.
Gastroenterology 150:1599–1608
Yuan S, Xiong Y, Larsson SC (2021) An atlas on risk factors for multiple sclerosis: a Mendelian
randomization study. J Neurol 268:114–124
Yudkin JS, Stehouwer CD, Emeis JJ, Coppack SW (1999) C-reactive protein in healthy subjects:
associations with obesity, insulin resistance, and endothelial dysfunction: a potential role for
cytokines originating from adipose tissue? Arterioscler Thromb Vasc Biol 19:972–978
Zaldivia MT, Rivera J, Hering D et al (2017) Renal denervation reduces monocyte activation and
monocyte-platelet aggregate formation: an anti-inflammatory effect relevant for cardiovascular
risk. Hypertension 69:323–331
Zannas AS, Jia M, Hafner K et al (2019) Epigenetic upregulation of FKBP5 by aging and stress
contributes to NF-κB-driven inflammation and cardiovascular risk. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A
116:11370–11379
Zanoni FL, Simas R, da Silva RG, Breithaupt-Faloppa AC, Coutinho ESRD, Jatene FB, Moreira
LF (2017) Bilateral sympathectomy improves postinfarction left ventricular remodeling and
function. J Thorac Cardiovasc Surg 153:855–863.e851
Zardooz H, Sadeghimahalli F, Khodagholi F (2021) Early postnatal stress impairs insulin secretion
in response to psychological stress in adult rats. J Endocrinol Investig 44:277–286
Zhang L, Zhang D, Sun Y (2019) Adverse childhood experiences and early pubertal timing among
girls: a meta-analysis. Int J Environ Res Public Health 16:2887
Zhang QH, Hao JW, Li GL, Ji XJ, Yao XD, Dong N, Yao YM (2018) Proinflammatory switch from
Galphas to Galphai signaling by Glucagon-like peptide-1 receptor in murine splenic monocyte
following burn injury. Inflamm Res 67:157–168
Zhang XF, Guan XX, Tang YJ, Sun JF, Wang XK, Wang WD, Fan JM (2021) Clinical effects and
gut microbiota changes of using probiotics, prebiotics or synbiotics in inflammatory bowel dis-
ease: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Eur J Nutr 60:2855–2875
Zhou H, Wu R, Kong Y, Zhao M, Su Y (2020) Impact of smoking on psoriasis risk and treatment
efficacy: a meta-analysis. J Int Med Res 48:300060520964024
Zhou JY, Zhong HJ, Yang C, Yan J, Wang HY, Jiang JX (2010) Corticosterone exerts immunostim-
ulatory effects on macrophages via endoplasmic reticulum stress. Br J Surg 97:281–293
Zhu X, Zhou L, Li Q, Pan R, Zhang J, Cui Y (2021) Combined score of C-reactive protein level
and neutrophil-to-lymphocyte ratio: a novel marker in distinguishing children with exacerbated
asthma. Int J Immunopathol Pharmacol 35:20587384211040641
Ziegler KA, Ahles A, Wille T, Kerler J, Ramanujam D, Engelhardt S (2018) Local sympathetic
denervation attenuates myocardial inflammation and improves cardiac function after myocar-
dial infarction in mice. Cardiovasc Res 114:291–299
Energy, Early Traumatic Experiences
and Chronic Immune System Activation 5

This chapter contains hypothetical aspects that the reader must already know before
reading in order to be able to critically engage with them. But what is a book without
testable hypotheses at the end? Such considerations lead into the future, to the next
experiments, to observations and to the description of true complexity. Therefore, con-
sider the conclusion from this perspective. You have been warned. Your valuable energy
will not be wasted.
The importance of energy questions was already clear in several places in the previ-
ous chapters. So we know: As a result of earlier traumatic experiences, there is often a
high body weight (stored energy in the form of fatty acids; Barker et al. 1989), disturbed
fattening eating behavior (Table 3.2, e.g. too many calories consumed, eating without
hunger, Junk Food Self Medication), insulin resistance (lack of storage of energy-rich
substrates in muscles, fat tissue and liver), cortisol changes (usually increased, releases
energy-rich substrates into the bloodstream), changes in the sympathetic nervous system
(usually increased, releases energy-rich substrates into the bloodstream), lowered sex
hormones (muscle growth blocked, insulin resistance promoted), etc.
All of the above points are signs of altered energy regulation in the body after early
adversities. We see the same things during aging and in chronic inflammatory diseases,
even if they are well treated (Straub 2018). This parallel occurrence of similar phenom-
ena in different diseases, pathological conditions, as a result of trauma and during aging
speaks for a general principle, and this prompts us to take a look at the energy regulation
from a bird’s eye perspective (Straub 2018).
Psychologists and psychiatrists have long recognized the importance of energy ques-
tions in stress-inducing situations (e.g. in allostatic overload: McEwen 1998; Danese and
McEwen 2012), but there is no true experimental application of this theoretical knowledge
in early traumatic experiences—I have found nothing in the literature. The knowledge,
however, is important in order to better explain different reactions of the stress axes—for

© The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer-Verlag GmbH, DE, part of Springer 233
Nature 2023
R. H. Straub, Early Trauma as the Origin of Chronic Inflammation,
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-662-66751-4_5
234 5 Energy, Early Traumatic Experiences and Chronic Immunization

example, high versus low cortisol or high versus low sympathetic activity. Since these dis-
crepant patterns – of too much and too little hormone activity – influence immunological
reactions, we are interested in clarification. I want to make a proposal that includes energy
questions. Furthermore, I describe the connection between energy and chronic immunoac-
tivation after early traumatic experiences, which results in an astonishing finding.

5.1 Brain and Immune System Define Energy Expenditure

In earlier texts, I made the selfish role of brain and immune system clear with regard to
energy allocation in the body (Straub 2017, 2018). There, the two selfish organs were
described in detail, and their function was briefly mentioned in this book in Sect. 4.1.
What makes them so special? These two organ systems can regulate energy distribution
in the body with their respective means and, in addition, shut down the other system (the
brain shuts down the immune system; the immune system shuts down the brain). These
means of energy self-allocation by brain and immune system are summarized in Table 5.1.
The respective factors of energy self-allocation were positively selected during evo-
lution because these two systems are the hierarchically highest authorities to sense
immediate dangers. In other words: The brain and the immune system quickly recog-
nize the dangers, they have the corresponding sensors, they have the methods of ade-
quate response and they set the energy allocation for the rest of the body (Table 5.2).
Most of the available energy regulated above the basal metabolic rate goes to the brain
and immune system depending on the type of dangers. The brain and muscles must be
considered together in terms of energy consumption because they form a psychomotor
unit. These topics were presented in detail in the previous book and are not repeated here
(Straub 2018).
In energy allocation, both organ systems stimulate a higher amount of circulating
energy-rich substrates in the bloodstream, as they both dictate and dominate the release
of stored energy in the form of fatty acids from fat tissue, of glucose from muscle and
from liver/kidney, and of amino acids from muscle (some amino acids go into the for-
mation of glucose in the liver and kidneys = gluconeogenesis). If increased amounts of
energy-rich substrates circulate in the bloodstream, they are available to the two egoists
and other organs when they do not need insulin for the uptake of energetic substrates
(because of parallel insulin resistance: fat tissue, muscles and liver cannot take up energy
with insulin resistance) (Straub 2014a).
As a rule, always one egoistic system dominates. If several dangers come together,
which require the brain and immune system to act at the same time, the danger of
overload is much greater. The energy shortages then increase dramatically, which can
develop into a life-threatening overall situation (e.g. intensive care unit/stress PLUS
infection).
I see another important point in energy regulation in how the egoistic organs can stim-
ulate each other in a certain way. I called this stimulation “mutual immediate help” in my
5.1 Brain and Immune System Define Energy Expenditure 235

Table 5.1  Means of energy self-allocation by brain and immune system


The means of the brain The means of the immune system
The sympathetic nervous system with adrenaline Cytokines such as TNF, IL-1β and IL-6 induce
and noradrenaline stimulates glycogenolysis, insulin resistance, gluconeogenesis (IL-6) and
gluconeogenesis and lipolysis and thus increases lipolysis and thus increase the free energy
the free energy substrates in the blood, at the substrates in the blood
same time it induces insulin resistance.
The HPA axis with cortisol stimulates glycog- Danger Signals = Toll-like receptor agonists
enolysis, gluconeogenesis, lipolysis and muscle induce insulin resistance, gluconeogenesis
breakdown (amino acids for gluconeogenesis) and lipolysis and thus increase the free energy
and thus increases the free energy substrates substrates in the blood.
in the blood, at the same time cortisol induces
insulin resistance.
Growth hormone from the pituitary gland Cytokines increase the production of cortisol in
stimulates glycogenolysis, gluconeogenesis the liver and thus contribute to glycogenolysis,
and lipolysis and thus increases the free energy gluconeogenesis, lipolysis and muscle break-
substrates in the blood, at the same time growth down (amino acids for gluconeogenesis). The
hormone promotes insulin resistance. free energy substrates in the blood increase, at
the same time it makes insulin resistance.
The thyroid hormone axis supports the sym- Immune cells can produce cortisol and thus
pathetic nervous system, promotes muscle contribute to glycogenolysis, gluconeogenesis,
breakdown and at the same time induces insulin lipolysis and muscle breakdown (amino acids
resistance. for gluconeogenesis). The free energy sub-
strates in the blood increase, at the same time it
makes insulin resistance.
The sympathetic renin-angiotensin-aldosterone Immune cells produce adrenaline and noradren-
system makes glycogenolysis and glucone- aline, and these hormones make glycogen-
ogenesis and thus increases the free energy olysis, gluconeogenesis and lipolysis and
substrates in the blood, while at the same time thus increase the free energy substrates in the
inducing insulin resistance. blood, while at the same time inducing insulin
resistance.
Inhibition of the parasympathetic nervous Immune cells convert inactive thyroid hor-
system and of digestive functions that would be mones into active ones. This supports the
relevant for the storage of energy substrates in sympathetic nervous system, promotes muscle
the body. breakdown and at the same time insulin resist-
ance. This increases the free energy substrates
in the blood.
Inhibition of the sex hormone axis: no muscle Immune cells produce growth hormone, which
growth (energy storage in muscle blocked), makes glycogenolysis, gluconeogenesis and
no insulin help (the hormones promote insulin lipolysis and thus increases the free energy
action) substrates in the blood, while at the same time
inducing insulin resistance.
From: Straub (2014a, b)
236 5 Energy, Early Traumatic Experiences and Chronic Immunization

Table 5.2  Dangers and sensory functions of the brain and immune system
Dangers Immediate response Response system
Predator Fight or flight Brain, SNS, HPA axis
Wounding and Blood clotting and Blood clotting system
bleeding Blood pressure stabilization Brain with SNS, HPA axis and the
SNS-dependent renin-angioten-
sin-aldosterone system
Later: Replacement of the cellular SNS stimulates leukocyte produc-
elements of the blood system tion in the bone marrow
Hunger Foraging behavior Brain, SNS, HPA axis
Thirst Water search Brain, SNS, HPA axis
Cold Warming up and searching for warm Brain, SNS, HPA axis
places
Heat Sweating and searching for cooler Brain, SNS
places
Infection Immune reaction, inflammatory Immune system
reaction
Injury and infection Immune reaction, inflammatory Immune system
reaction
Abbreviations: SNS, sympathetic nervous system; HPA axis, hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis

previous book (Straub 2018). This immediate help manifests itself in two different ways,
which are shown in Fig. 5.1.
On the one hand, the brain can induce a typical factor of the immune system—in the
example of Fig. 5.1 it's IL-6—and thus provide a kind of immediate help for the immune
system. On the other hand, cytokines of the immune system can activate the stress axes
of the brain, more precisely: the hypothalamus, the pituitary gland and the upper sym-
pathetic centers, and thus provide immediate help for the brain. In terms of energy reg-
ulation, the goal is ultimately to provide energy-rich substrates such as glucose and free
fatty acids, which the respective activated selfish organs—the brain or immune system—
remove from the bloodstream. This mutual immediate help is only active in the first
hours up to a maximum of 1–2 days after the recognition of the respective trigger, before
the organ-specific mechanisms of Table 5.1 increasingly take over. This takeover serves
to enforce the respective own goals in the context of energy self-allocation. Brain and
immune system are selfish!
Since the current energy consumption and the expected future energy consumption
were/are fundamental parameters in our evolutionary history—because every single reac-
tion only works in the presence of energy (e.g. in the form of ATP; Fig. 5.2)—the availa-
bility of energy dictates almost every reaction in the body. During the long evolutionary
process, the presence of energy and its conservation were/are a powerful selection factor.
5.1 Brain and Immune System Define Energy Expenditure 237

acvated brain acvated immune system

various
cytokines
e.g. IL-6

sympathec nervous system

brown adrenal
lymph fat gland
nodes

IL-6 corsol
free fay adrenaline
IL-6
acids

liver
corsol
IL-6 adrenaline

gluconeogenesis
insulin resistance

energy-rich substrates
(glucose, fay acids)

Fig. 5.1 Mutual immediate help of brain and immune system. The blue or blue-red paths show the
immediate help of the brain for the immune system. At the end of the blue path, for example, IL-6
(demonstrates immunoactivation) intervenes in energy regulation. The red and blue-red paths show the
immediate help of the activated immune system for the brain (demonstrates stress axis activation, a brain
function). At the end of the red path are cortisol and adrenaline, which intervene in energy regulation.
IL-6, cortisol and adrenaline are just examples (there are other factors). The activation of IL-6 from
brown fat tissue was described by Qing et al. (Qing et al. 2020). At the end, energy-rich substrates (at
the very bottom) can circulate in the bloodstream and are then taken up by the active organ—brain or
immune system

If there is no energy, reactions cannot take place. Reactions also include our behavior,
which was developed in the context of energy issues during evolution. With this prem-
ise, I say: The energy consumption and regulation are of central importance in traumatic
experiences in young years.
Unfortunately, the scientists who studied people with early traumatic experiences
have not yet carried out any specific energy investigations. At least I couldn’t find any-
thing tangible in the known databases, even though we can exactly determine the basal
238 5 Energy, Early Traumatic Experiences and Chronic Immunization

166 amino acids


for interferon-γ
mkytsyilaf
qlcivlgslg
cycqdpyvke 5 ATP molecules for an
aenlkkyfna
ghsdvadngt amino acid bond or 825
lflgilknwk ATP molecules for one
eesdrkimqs molecule of INTERFERON-
qivsfyfklf
knfkddqsiq
ksvetikedm
nvkffnsnkk
krddfekltn
ysvtdlnvqr
kaiheliqvm
aelspaaktg
krkrsqmlfr
grrasq
Interferon-γ

Fig. 5.2 The tiniest reaction in our body requires a lot of energy. The cytokine interferon-γ consists
of 166 amino acids (National Center for Biotechnology Information 2021), and the cell needs 5 ATP
molecules to produce an amino acid bond (Princiotta et al. 2003). ATP is the energy currency in the cell
(Straub 2018). For the production of one molecule of interferon-γ, the cell needs about 825 ATP mole-
cules

metabolic rate (basic requirement in absolute rest) and the total energy requirements of
the human being with two very good methods. For the interested readers, these methods
will be described briefly.
The basal metabolic rate is determined by indirect calorimetry, and with the doubly
labeled water technique the total energy consumption is derived (hydrogen and oxygen
in water are labeled with different isotopes). With the help of activity and fitness track-
ers, the proportion of energy consumption for physical activity is recorded. All three
methods provide exact statements about different forms of energy consumption. With the
doubly labeled water technique the researchers can even make a statement about the total
consumption over several weeks, without disturbing the test person in their everyday life.
The latter method is particularly suitable as a safe and non-invasive method, which is
why it is often used in human and veterinary medicine.

5.2 The Trauma Reaction can Use a Lot and a Little Energy

Thinking now in the context of this book of early traumatic experiences, we must inter-
pret those severe traumatic life events of Table 2.1 and 2.2 as dangers that are recog-
nized by the brain. Thus, we can easily insert an additional line into Table 5.2 which
contains the following points. In the first column we put under “Dangers” the trauma,
the “immediate response” is the trauma reaction and the “response system” is the brain
with its downstream helpers such as the sympathetic nervous system, the HPA axis, the
pain pathways, etc. With the immune system this initial trauma reaction has little to do,
5.3 Is there a Trauma and Energy Provision Memory? 239

although every stress reaction triggers a small inflammatory reaction in the sense of
‘mutual immediate help’ (Fig. 5.1). The thing is dominated by the brain, since it has the
essential sensory function in trauma situations.
In terms of energy, the trauma reaction is either energy-consuming, energy-saving or
without effect on energy consumption. Here you wonder when I mention “energy-saving
or without effect on energy consumption” at all. Do you not automatically think of high
energy consumption when these dangers act? Not always!
In addition to flight and fight, which really consume a lot of energy, reactions to dan-
gers can be opposite reactions that we know in the animal kingdom as freezing. This
“freezing of activity” is an essential survival program for reptiles, since they, unlike
mammals or birds, do not have energy stores and cannot maintain their body temperature
constant. They rely on immobility and excellent mimicry to save energy and not be dis-
covered in nature.
In humans, we know both reactions after traumas, the classical type of the traumatic
reaction with high energy expenditure (fight/flight) and the alternative type with freezing,
an inability to act and a distance to one’s own body (van der Kolk 2014; Straub 2020).
The first reaction is an energy-consuming reaction, the second reaction is not (however,
we do not know for sure because it was not measured). From this I hypothesize: Trauma
reactions can require a lot or a little energy.

5.3 Is there a Trauma and Energy Provision Memory?

Here I say clearly YES. If we measure the energy consumption and the reaction of the
stress axes in a child, adolescent or young adult during the impact of the trauma, we
can assess the provisioning reaction of the energy-rich substrates. So one affected person
may respond with high energy expenditure and another with energy protection. I foresee
how the type of trauma exerts a significant influence on the specific energy consump-
tion. Personal characteristics may also be relevant because they are also related to energy
issues. We do not know this at the moment because it has not been measured.
If a memory of the trauma is formed that permanently deflects the pointer of the
weighing scale in Fig. 3.4 (memory of the trauma), we expect, in view of the abso-
lutely central importance of energy expenditure, a combined trauma and energy provi-
sion memory. The hypothesis is: This memory and thus the energy provision reaction
can cause long-term changes in tone over a long period of time, or this memory of acute
recation can be recalled in the event of further traumas in the sense of double or multiple
hits.
The long-term incorrect deflection (tone: too much/too little) and the acute recall
(acute reaction: too much/too little) each influence, according to the original long-term
programming in young years, the subsequent reactions of energy provision and energy
consumption (too much/too little) in a later stress episode. The hypothesis is: It is
programmed whether a lot of energy or little energy is provided, and accordingly the
240 5 Energy, Early Traumatic Experiences and Chronic Immunization

responses of the systems involved in Table 5.2 and the means for this from Table 5.1
are stored. The reactions of the stress axes with cortisol or noradrenaline/adrenaline
can therefore be very different, since strong reactions are associated with high energy
expenditure and weak reactions with low energy consumption. This approach can help
us to clarify why there may be discrepant response patterns with too much or too little of
these hormones. Studies in this direction are worthwhile.

5.4 Energy and Chronic Immune System Activation

Now we return to the core messages in this book to link chronic immune activation and
energy together. Chap. 4 has shown how early traumatic experiences can influence the
inflammatory situation in the long term. We are no longer surprised when those affected
have increased inflammation values stimulated by the four connectors. What is deci-
sive for energy regulation in the body are other questions that are: How high are these
inflammation values really? Do people with early adversities need a special energy pro-
vision memory to provide the necessary energy for the activated immune system? Is the
immune system therefore the superordinate system that dominates the energy provision
reaction? Does the brain then play no role in energy provision?

5.4.1 How High is Inflammation in People with Early Traumatic


Experiences Really?

One of the first studies in people with early traumatic experiences and the popular
C-reactive protein was carried out by Andrea Danese from King’s College in London
together with Avshalom Caspi from the Explanation 3 (Danese et al. 2007). Those
affected with early trauma experiences had about 2.5 times as much C-reactive protein as
people without trauma. Sounds like a clear case, right?
However, the average values of the C-reactive protein in the serum were in the rela-
tively low range of 1.5 mg/l in the control group and 4.0 mg/l in the traumatized. People
with values over 10 mg/l were excluded because the researchers here suspected a “proper
inflammation” (Danese et al. 2007). Since the normal range of C-reactive protein is up
to about 5.0 mg/l (Thomas 2020), there is obviously no proper inflammation here as a
result of early trauma. Is the same true for IL-6, which induces C-reactive protein in the
liver (Andus et al. 1988)? With the help of a meta-analysis, which processed the relevant
literature up to 2016 (Baumeister et al. 2016), Table 5.3 was compiled. For this purpose,
those studies with a sample size of over 100 people were used from the meta-analysis,
which determined the important inflammatory parameter IL-6.
The use of serum IL-6 is valuable because it leads to a few more logical conclusions
about the level of inflammation and energy consumption. Unfortunately, serum IL-6
5.4 Energy and Chronic Immune System Activation 241

Table 5.3  Serum IL-6 in controls and people with early adversities (n in parentheses gives the
number of all investigated people in the respective study)
Controls People with trauma Literature
1.11 pg/ml (n=702) 1.34 pg/ml (Bertone-Johnson et al. 2012)
< 2.09 pg/ml (n=756) ≥ 2.09 pg/ml (Carroll et al. 2013)
2.40 pg/ml (n=130) 3.80 pg/ml (Gouin et al. 2012)
2.50 pg/ml (n=132) 3.02 pg/ml (Kiecolt-Glaser et al. 2011)
1.80 pg/ml (n=482) 2.40 pg/ml (Rooks et al. 2012)
Mean: 1.98 pg/ml Mean: 2.53 pg/ml Difference statistically significant,
but biologically very questionable
For comparison, healthy normal people
1.00 (in 20-year-olds) to 2.50 pg/ml (in 70-year-olds) (Straub et al. 1998)

v­ alues were not given in pg/ml in all studies, which I believe is not good because it pre-
vents the entire assessment of the severity of the inflammation. The serum IL-6 values
do go into the statistical analysis here and there, but no one knows if they are outside the
normal range (only the authors know it).
In the compilation of Table 5.3 the serum value of IL-6 is hardly increased in the early
traumatized people and is only minimally above the serum value of old people from
our own study with carefully selected normal controls (Straub et al. 1998). The level of
inflammation is therefore very low or non-existent.
Let us now compare this situation from Table 5.3 with other states and diseases, in
which the investigators used the same methods of IL-6 determination as in the studies
of Table 5.3. Accordingly, the traumatized persons of Table 5.3 are slightly above the
healthy normal persons and near the stressed old persons who had to move from their
home environment to a nursing home in later life (Lutgendorf et al. 1999). Persons with
obesity show slightly higher values than the resettled persons in old age (Fig. 5.3).
Even if we take the measurement value for patients with obesity as the target varia-
ble because obesity is a common complication in traumatized persons (Sect. 3.3.1), this
serum concentration of IL-6 is surprisingly low, because it only causes a small change in
energy expenditure. This relationship is shown in the next subchapter.

5.4.2 How High is Energy Expenditure at Different Levels


of Inflammation?

Here I repeat a few aspects of the previous book (Straub 2018), because they are central
to the following consideration. At that time I wrote:
242 5 Energy, Early Traumatic Experiences and Chronic Immunization

Sepsis, maximum
Serum level of interleukin-6 (pg/ml)

Sepsis, baseline
(logarithmic scale)

Sepsis, 2nd week after therapy


Sepsis, 3rd week after therapy

rheumatoid arthritis, 2nd study before therapy

rheumatoid arthritis, 1st study before therapy

rheumatoid arthritis, well treated


Obesity / care for Alzheimer patients
aged people moved to a nursing home
70 years old, healthy

20 years young, healthy

Fig. 5.3 Serum levels of interleukin-6 (IL-6) in different states. A logarithmic scale was used for the
Y-axis, which is often used in exponential growth. The green color indicates the normal situation. With
higher inflammation, the color changes to red, indicating a more severe inflammation situation. For rheu-
matoid arthritis, two examples of the serum IL-6 value before therapy are given, because this disease can
start more or less severely (some patients have serum values up to 200 pg/ml). The maximum in sepsis
occurred in one patient. The other data on sepsis are mean values of very many patients. Obesity is dia-
gnosed by a body mass index of 30 kg/m2 (BMI = weight in kg, divided by height in m squared). (Mod.
after Straub 2018)

In a much-cited study of healthy people, a team at the American National Institutes of


Health in Washington was able to show (Tsigos et al. 1997), how the administration of
IL-6 by means of injection under the skin increased the levels of the same IL-6 in the blood
and how this led to an increased expenditure of energy of the whole body (Fig. 5.4). If the
experimenter increased the levels of IL-6 from the usual 1 pg/ml to 7 pg/ml, the expendi-
ture of energy increased by 250 kJ (60 kcal) per day (Fig. 5.4). If a patient with rheumatic
disease is well treated, the level of IL-6 is 10 pg/ml. In the experiment with healthy people,
the increase from the usual 1 pg/ml to 10 pg/ml led to an increase in energy expenditure of
300 kJ (72 kcal) per day. (…) Quite different is the case with the high levels of IL-6 from
1000–3000 pg/ml in Fig. 5.4, which lead to an increase in energy expenditure of 2500 kJ
(600 kcal) per day.

A simulated inflammation situation with IL-6 thus results in higher energy expenditure.
If we look at our low IL-6 values from Table 5.3 and compare them with the data in
Fig. 5.4, we would not expect higher energy consumption in traumatized people. In other
words: An energy provision memory with activated stress axes is not necessary to
provide energy for the immune system with this low inflammation.
5.4 Energy and Chronic Immune System Activation 243

Increase in energy consumption per day in kJ (kcal)

Serum level of interleukin-6 (pg/ml)


(logarithmic scale)

Fig. 5.4 Energy expenditure per day. In the subjects studied, the cytokine interleukin-6 was injected
with a syringe under the skin. After a short time, the blood level of interleukin-6 increased (black dots).
At the same time, the increase in energy expenditure over the normal basal metabolic rate was measu-
red. For example, 500 kJ on the Y-axis means an increase in energy expenditure of 500 kJ, which is due
to the injection of interleukin-6 and the associated increase in inflammation. Numbers in brackets are
given in kcal. The blue curve and the black measurement points show the relationship between serum
levels of IL-6 and increase in energy expenditure. With increasing levels of interleukin-6 on the X-axis,
the energy expenditure on the Y-axis increases. With an increase in the blood level to 3.5 pg/ml (green
lines), the energy expenditure increases by 170 kJ (40 kcal) per day. With an increase in the level to 7 pg/
ml (red lines), the energy expenditure increases accordingly by 250 kJ per day. With an increase in the
level to 100 pg/ml (black dashed lines), the energy expenditure increases by about 1200 kJ per day. Data
according to Tsigos et al. (1997)

5.4.3 The Quintessence

For me, therefore, the mild inflammation is a pure accompanying phenomenon of ear-
lier traumatic experiences and not responsible for many of the so-called sequelae. Mild
inflammation is an accompanying phenomenon in the sense of “mutual immediate help”,
which ultimately only serves the stressed brain to provide a higher amount of energy-rich
substrates for the psychomotor unit (brain-muscles). The problem of the so-called sys-
temic inflammation, which we can easily measure in the blood, is, in my opinion, clearly
overestimated by many scientists, since this so-called inflammation hardly changes the
244 5 Energy, Early Traumatic Experiences and Chronic Immunization

energy expenditure. If one thing hardly changes energy expenditure, it plays hardly any
role in the whole system, because energy is a central control variable. Nevertheless, there
is an energy provision memory after trauma, in which the reactions of the stress axes are
stored accordingly in order to provide the right amount of energy for the brain’s response
reaction in case of need. However, the provision of energy does not serve the largely
calm immune system but only the activated brain.
In my previous book and in a publication I have already pointed out the following
(Straub 2017, 2018): In addition to the possibly mild chronic immune activation, there
are manifold other elements that trigger increased energy expenditure. The problems
mentioned in Table 5.4 and other trauma-related problems of the brain or the psycho-
motor unit (brain-muscles) described in this book have a much greater impact on energy
consumption than the mild chronic immune system activation.
Thus, I give the mild chronic immune system activation a low importance when dis-
cussing energy aspects. The central nervous system problems after trauma are much
more important for the question of energy consumption, for the energy provision mem-
ory and for the reaction of the stress axes than the mild inflammation. The latter hardly
needs extra energy above the basic requirement. Since the increased energy consump-
tion for the unwanted side effects mentioned in Table 5.4 limits the energy availability
for desired physical and mental activities, these side reactions become a decisive factor
for premature aging, early illness, frailty and early death (Straub 2017). The non-inflam-
matory side effects decisively influence these unwanted energy expenditures of Table
5.4. Most of it takes place in the brain, the immune system is the spectator—at most the
small first-aid kit. The immune system is not the pervasive system that dominates the
energy provision reaction. This role is played by the brain.
Allow me a final remark from the clinical immunologist on the increasingly occurring
autoimmune diseases after early traumatic experiences (see Sect. 3.4). If the immune

Table 5.4  Percentage of additional energy expenditure stimulated by the brain


Situation* Additional energy expenditure
Acute pain (electric shocks to the abdominal wall) up to 69% more
Chronic pain up to 15% more
Psychological stress (activates psychomotor unit) up to 30% more
Anxiety/fear up to 10% more
Sleep disorders up to 30% more
Chronic smoldering infections (stimulated by risky behavior) up to 10% more
Chronic smoking (more than 6 cigarettes/day) up to 15% more
*The individual situations cannot be considered independently of each other, since pain can be
associated with sleep disorders or anxiety with psychological stress, for example. Therefore, you
cannot add up the percentage figures to calculate the total increase in additional energy expendi-
ture. Data from Straub (2017)
5.5 To the Point 245

system hardly deviates from the normal situation, why are there more autoimmune dis-
eases? In addition to the known genetic factors, other elements must be significant here:
for example smoking, drugs, chemicals (occupation!), floating fine particles such as sil-
ica dusts, the wrong microbiome, pathogens (especially viruses), junk food and the false
free fatty acids (not the hardly measurable cytokines from the adipose tissue), lack of
physical and mental activity, etc. These create the platform for the development of auto-
immune diseases next to the genetic predispositions (e.g. for rheumatoid arthritis; Okada
et al. 2014). Chronic mild immune system activation is not a platform that bears all the
weight for the development of autoimmune diseases.
I am sometimes asked why I do not give therapy recommendations in the books. The
pathophysiological relationships are more important to me, especially the importance of
the connectors, whose function I highlighted. From here, the interested reader can derive
the therapy recommendations. Is it not a greater Aha experience when the reader comes
to the right conclusions on the basis of his on reading of the book? At the end of the pre-
vious paragraph, it was clear what we have to avoid. If you have read the book carefully,
you know what to avoid and what to stimulate.

5.5 To the Point

• The availability of energy is central to the evolutionary process for chemical, bio-
chemical and biological reactions. The availability of energy is a powerful force in
natural selection.
• If there is no energy, reactions do not take place, not even behavioral reactions, as
behavior developed on the basis of the presence of energy during evolution.
• The brain and immune system selfishly define the distribution of energy and use their
own practical means to act independently of each other.
• A trauma reaction can consume a lot (fight/flight) or a little (freezing) energy. In addi-
tion to the trauma memory, an energy provision memory is created.
• This memory can cause long-term changes in the tone of the stress axes or, in the
event of re-occurring trauma, it can define the acute reaction of the stress axes during
double or multiple hits.
• This approach can help us to clarify why there are discrepant patterns of response of
the stress axes with too much or too little of hormones and neurotransmitters.
• Surprisingly, early traumatic experiences only slightly increase the inflammatory sit-
uation (C-reactive protein, IL-6) later in life. This minimal increase in inflammation
only marginally changes energy expenditure.
• Therefore, the increased energy expenditure must be explained by mechanisms out-
side the immune system. Only the selfish brain can be responsible for increased
energy expenditure after early adversities.
246 5 Energy, Early Traumatic Experiences and Chronic Immunization

• Increased energy expenditure is caused by pain, psychological stress, anxiety, sleep


problems (remember itchiness, etc.), possibly smoldering unnoticed infections (cyto-
megalovirus in immunocompromised people after risky behavior), chronic smoking,
drugs, and other things.
• These energy expenditures are not desired by the affected person because they rob the
body of the necessary energy for physical and mental activity. The lack of physical
and mental activity is the decisive risk factor for premature aging, early illness, frailty
and early death. Most of this takes place in the brain, the immune system is the spec-
tator and at most an “immediate helper”.

References

Andus T, Geiger T, Hirano T, Kishimoto T, Heinrich PC (1988) Action of recombinant human


interleukin 6, interleukin 1 beta and tumor necrosis factor alpha on the mRNA induction of
acute-phase proteins. Eur J Immunol 18:739–746
Barker DJ, Winter PD, Osmond C, Margetts B, Simmonds SJ (1989) Weight in infancy and death
from ischaemic heart disease. Lancet 2:577–580
Baumeister D, Akhtar R, Ciufolini S, Pariante CM, Mondelli V (2016) Childhood trauma and
adulthood inflammation: a meta-analysis of peripheral C-reactive protein, interleukin-6 and
tumour necrosis factor-α. Mol Psychiatry 21:642–649
Bertone-Johnson ER, Whitcomb BW, Missmer SA, Karlson EW, Rich-Edwards JW (2012)
Inflammation and early-life abuse in women. Am J Prev Med 43:611–620
Carroll JE, Gruenewald TL, Taylor SE, Janicki-Deverts D, Matthews KA, Seeman TE (2013)
Childhood abuse, parental warmth, and adult multisystem biological risk in the Coronary
Artery Risk Development in Young Adults study. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 110:17149–17153
Danese A, McEwen BS (2012) Adverse childhood experiences, allostasis, allostatic load, and
age-related disease. Physiol Behav 106:29–39
Danese A, Pariante CM, Caspi A, Taylor A, Poulton R (2007) Childhood maltreatment predicts
adult inflammation in a life-course study. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 104:1319–1324
Gouin JP, Glaser R, Malarkey WB, Beversdorf D, Kiecolt-Glaser JK (2012) Childhood abuse and
inflammatory responses to daily stressors. Ann Behav Med 44:287–292
Kiecolt-Glaser JK, Gouin JP, Weng NP, Malarkey WB, Beversdorf DQ, Glaser R (2011) Childhood
adversity heightens the impact of later-life caregiving stress on telomere length and inflamma-
tion. Psychosom Med 73:16–22
Lutgendorf SK, Garand L, Buckwalter KC, Reimer TT, Hong SY, Lubaroff DM (1999) Life stress,
mood disturbance, and elevated interleukin-6 in healthy older women. J Gerontol A Biol Sci
Med Sci 54:M434–M439
McEwen BS (1998) Protective and damaging effects of stress mediators. N Engl J Med
338:171–179
National Center for Biotechnology Information. Domains & Structures Database. from https://
www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/guide/domains-structures/. Accessed 11 Jan 2021
Okada Y, Wu D, Trynka G et al (2014) Genetics of rheumatoid arthritis contributes to biology and
drug discovery. Nature 506:376–381
Princiotta MF, Finzi D, Qian SB, Gibbs J, Schuchmann S, Buttgereit F, Bennink JR, Yewdell
JW (2003) Quantitating protein synthesis, degradation, and endogenous antigen processing.
Immunity 18:343–54
References 247

Qing H, Desrouleaux R, Israni-Winger K, Mineur YS, Fogelman N, Zhang C, Rashed S, Palm NW,
Sinha R, Picciotto MR, Perry RJ, Wang A (2020) Origin and Function of Stress-Induced IL-6 in
Murine Models. Cell 182:372–387
Rooks C, Veledar E, Goldberg J, Bremner JD, Vaccarino V (2012) Early trauma and inflammation:
role of familial factors in a study of twins. Psychosom Med 74:146–152
Straub RH (2014a) Insulin resistance, selfish brain, and selfish immune system: an evolutionarily
positively selected program used in chronic inflammatory diseases. Arthritis Res Ther 16(Suppl
2):S4
Straub RH (2014b) Interaction of the endocrine system with inflammation: a function of energy
and volume regulation. Arthritis Res Ther 16:203
Straub RH (2017) The brain and immune system prompt energy shortage in chronic inflammation
and ageing. Nat Rev Rheumatol 13:743–751
Straub RH (2018) Altern, Müdigkeit und Entzündungen verstehen – Wenn Immunsystem und
Gehirn um die Energie im Körper ringen. Springer, Berlin/Heidelberg
Straub RH (2020) Drei Gedächtnisse für den Körper. Springer Nature, Berlin
Straub RH, Konecna L, Hrach S, Rothe G, Kreutz M, Schölmerich J, Falk W, Lang B (1998)
Serum dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA) and DHEA sulfate are negatively correlated with
serum interleukin-6 (IL-6), and DHEA inhibits IL-6 secretion from mononuclear cells in man
in vitro: possible link between endocrinosenescence and immunosenescence. J Clin Endocrinol
Metab 83:2012–2017
Thomas L. Labor und Diagnose 2020 – Internet version. at 27.11.2021 from https://www.
labor-und-diagnose-2020.de/
Tsigos C, Papanicolaou DA, Defensor R, Mitsiadis CS, Kyrou I, Chrousos GP (1997) Dose effects
of recombinant human interleukin-6 on pituitary hormone secretion and energy expenditure.
Neuroendocrinology 66:54–62
van der Kolk B (2014) The body keeps the score – mind, brain and body in transformation of
trauma. Penguin Random House, New York
Glossary

Adaptive When a trait is positively selected during evolutionary history due to its good
adaption to the environment, the trait is then said to be adaptive.
Adipokines Factors produced by fat tissue that have hormone-like or cytokine-like
effects in other parts of the body and in fat tissue.
Adolescence Adolescence is the period from late childhood (11th year of life) through
puberty to full adulthood (21st year of life), Fig. 1.1.
Adrenal gland, cortex and medulla The adrenal glands are glands that sit on both
sides of the kidneys like caps. They each have the size of an apricot, and they pro-
duce various hormones that are released into the bloodstream. Anatomists distinguish
between the adrenal cortex, from which cortisol and androgens originate. In the adre-
nal cortex, a important hormone for blood pressure regulation is also produced. In
addition, the anatomist distinguishes the adrenal medulla, which is surrounded by
the cortex (similar to the apricot kernel by the apricot flesh). Adrenaline is produced
in the adrenal medulla. Adrenaline is the stress hormone number 1. The brain is the
supreme master of the adrenal gland, which activates the adrenal cortex hormonally
via the pituitary gland and the adrenal medulla via the sympathetic nervous system.
Adrenal cortex and adrenal medulla belong to the stress system.
Age-related diabetes See diabetes mellitus type 2.
ACC anterior cingulate gyrus, responsible for emotional control, evaluation, emotions,
social role, memory and executive function.
Amygdala The amygdala is a core area that is connected to many different areas of the
brain. There, remembered emotional aspects are processed. In particular, the amyg-
dala is responsible for fear and valuation of fear. The amygdala is the starting point
of immediate reactions to danger with an increase in the activity of the HPA axis,
the sympathetic nervous system and respiration. After early traumatic experiences, the
amygdala experiences inadequate increased activity and responsiveness.
Andropause Andropause describes the menopause of man (gr. andrós, man). It
occurs a little later than the menopause of the woman (around the 50th year of life).
Andropause is associated with a increasingly lower production of male sex hormones.

© The Editor(s) (if applicable) and The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer- 249
Verlag GmbH, DE, part of Springer Nature 2023
R. H. Straub, Early Trauma as the Origin of Chronic Inflammation,
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-662-66751-4
250 Glossary

Although the man’s fertility remains basically until old age, the probability of late
fatherhood decreases significantly.
Antigens, Autoantigens Antigens are recognized by our immune system as foreign and
are attacked. Antigens are often surface-located structures of bacteria or viruses (e.g.
tetanus toxin). The attack is carried out with exactly matching antibodies and immune
cells with exactly matching receptors. The immunologist speaks of autoantigens when
the antigens consist of body-own material (just auto), against which an immune reac-
tion and an inflammatory reaction are erroneously initiated.
Antibodies Antibodies are produced by immune cells (more precisely: by B cell-­
derived plasma cells). Antibodies always target antigens (for example, virus compo-
nents, in COVID-19 it is the spike protein). In many cases, the antigen is neutralized
and the antigen is taken up and destroyed by phagocytes.
Atherosclerosis Sometimes called “artery calcification”. It is a disease of the vessel
walls of the arteries with increasing hardening (increase in connective tissue), inflam-
mation and deposition of blood fats and calcium. It is the platform for the formation
of sudden blood clots that can break off (embolism) or completely block the vessel
(heart attack, stroke, vascular occlusion).
Arthritis Joint inflammation. As rheumatoid arthritis, it is a chronic joint inflammation,
an autoimmune disease.
ATP Adenosine triphosphate is the universally accepted currency of energy that is pro-
duced in mitochondria. A molecule of ATP consists of 10 carbon atoms, 16 hydrogen
atoms, 5 nitrogen atoms, 13 oxygen atoms, and 3 (hence triphosphate) phosphorus
atoms. ATP is needed by the cell in many metabolic steps and in cell growth. The
production of a single protein molecule of medium size requires approximately 2000
ATP molecules.
Acetylcholine The important neurotransmitter of the parasympathetic nervous system,
or the vagus nerve.
Autoimmune disease An immunological disease in which the target structure is body
tissue. The cause is an incorrect immune response to self. The attack is against body
protein structures, called autoantigens. The body tissue deteriorates as part of an
inflammatory reaction. An example is rheumatoid arthritis.
B cell The B cell belongs to the lymphocytes and is responsible for the production
of antibodies. It is called a B cell because it was first discovered in a lymph organ
of birds - the Bursa fabricii (bursa, Latin for bag; fabricii from Italian anatomist
Girolamo Fabrizio, 1619). It belongs to the adaptive immune system. B-memory cells
can maintain B-cell memory for decades. The descendants of B cells with memory
and antibody production are called plasma cells, and they live long-term in the bone
marrow.
Bariatric surgery Operation in the area of the stomach and/or small intestine, which is
carried out in case of severe obesity if conservative measures fail. These operations
are very successful with regard to weight loss.
Glossary 251

Basal metabolic rate Basic requirement of energy. This amount of energy is required
in absolute rest in the morning in a warm bed. It serves the basic supply of all organs
and organ systems. The basal metabolic energy cannot be negotiated between differ-
ent organs.
Body-Mass-Index Body measure for the assessment of obesity (weight by body height
squared). See explanation 7.
Chronic obstructive lung disease Here, the smallest airways are increasingly narrowed
(obstruction) by remodeling processes within the lungs. Consequences are chronic
wall thickening of the smallest airways and increased mucus production. Asthma or
smoking is often the starting point for chronic obstructive lung disease (COPD).
Citrullination of antigens Antigens consist of amino acids. The amino acid arginine
may be present in the antigen. Citrulline can be formed from arginine, which is called
citrullination. Since citrulline is not a typical amino acid, it hardly occurs during the
maturation process of the immune system. Later formed citrulline within a self-pro-
tein can be recognized and attacked as a foreign substance. In rheumatoid arthritis,
this process is important.
Cortisol Cortisol is the active hormone of the adrenal cortex (people say cortisone, but
that is the inactive degradation product). Cortisol is a stress hormone of the selfish
brain. Cortisol leads to the release of energy rich substrates from stores such as fat
tissue and liver. This can increase levels of fatty acids and sugar in the blood. At the
same time it is anti-inflammatory. See also glucocorticoids.
CRH (Corticotropin Releasing Hormon) Hierarchically considered, the highest hor-
mone in the hypothalamus for controlling the HPA axis (Fig. 4.9).
Cytokines Cytokines are the messenger substances of immune cells and other cells that
act in the immediate vicinity of the cells. We can call cytokines messenger substances
for the immediate neighborhood or “neighbor’s messenger substances”. See messen-
ger substances. Sometimes cytokines can have an effect at a distance, like IL-6.
Diabetes mellitus Sugar disease, i.e. the sugar levels in the blood are too high. In type
1 diabetes mellitus, there is an autoimmune disease in which the immune system rec-
ognizes proteins of the insulin-producing pancreas as “foreign”. Through the inflam-
matory destruction of insulin production, these patients need insulin as a permanent
therapy. These patients are usually younger. In type 2 diabetes mellitus (age-related
diabetes), on the other hand, insulin production is intact for a long time until it runs
out after years of overproduction (exhaustion of the pancreas). The latter disease is
not an autoimmune disease.
Epigenetics Influence of genes and thus the production of proteins by environmental
factors that inhibit or promote the reading of genes. The composition of the genes (the
sequence of nucleotides) is not changed. The Barker phenomenon was an example.
See explanation 1.
252 Glossary

Epithelial cell These are the cells of the inner and outer surfaces of the skin or mouth,
nose, intestine, lung, urinary tract and others. In addition, the endocrine organs are
made up of epithelial cells, as they develop from the surface tissues.
Executive functions These include self-control, emotion regulation, planning, working
memory and conscious attention control. In addition, self-motivation, will formation
and action initiative belong to this block of executive functions. The better the execu-
tive functions are, the higher the competence and resilience.
Fatty acids Basic unit of stored fat.
Genome, genome-wide Genetic material, totality of all genes of a cell. Geneticists
understand this to mean the totality of the inheritable information of an individual. A
genome-wide study includes all the genes of the individual.
Glucocorticoids These hormones come from the adrenal cortex and are called that
because they are involved in the release of glucose from stores into the bloodstream.
Cortisol is the major stress hormone in humans and it is the key hormone of the HPA
axis. Cortisone is the biologically inactive breakdown product of cortisol.
Glucocorticoid resistance Means lack of cortisol effect through its glucocorticoid
receptor. This is an important platform why the anti-inflammatory cortisol does not
properly inhibit inflammation.
Glucose Glucose is an important basic unit of carbohydrates. Glucose is an important
energy source that is used especially under oxygen-poor conditions to generate ATP
(energy). Glucose can be stored in the form of starch.
HPA axis Hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis (Fig. 4.9).
Hippocampus Hippocampus is a region in the brain, more precisely: in the inner mid-
dle temporal lobe. It is closely associated with the formation of memory. Especially
the memory for places is stored there. The hippocampus is closely linked to the limbic
system.
Human genome-wide A human genome-wide study includes all genes of the
individual.
Hypothalamus A brain area in which the upper centers of the hormones and the auto-
nomic nervous system are integrated. The control of the brain goes from there to
hormone pathways or autonomic nervous pathways. There is the eating and satiety
center, the center for body temperature, etc.
Insulin It is the storage hormone par excellence from the pancreas, because it promotes
the uptake of glucose and fatty acids into the storage organs (fat tissue, muscle, liver).
Furthermore, it is a growth factor for many tissues, including the immune system.
Diabetics with high blood glucose levels receive insulin therapeutically so that the
blood glucose level decreases and is transported to the storage organs.
Interleukin, IL-1, IL-6 For example, interleukin-1 or interleukin-6. These are immuno-
logical hormones or cytokines that act in the immediate vicinity (local action). Some
cytokines like interleukin-6 have long-range effects by being transported to distant
locations via circulation. See cytokines and TNF.
Glossary 253

Leptin The “thin hormone” leptin (gr. leptos, thin) is produced in fat tissue, acts in
the brain to suppress appetite, and activates the sympathetic nervous system and fat
breakdown. Leptin is part of the feedback loop to maintain fat mass. At the same time,
it affects many immunological processes in a stimulating way.
Leukocytes White blood cells. This includes the antigen-presenting cells discussed in
the book, macrophages, lymphocytes (T-cells and B-cells).
Limbic system It is a functional unit as a delimited region between the cerebral cor-
tex and the brainstem. It is closely linked to emotions, smell, fear, drives, mood and
memory. There are close connections to many other regions of the brain, including the
top hormone centers and centers of the autonomic nervous system.
Metabolic syndrome Metabolic syndrome is characterized by the following symptoms:
abdominal obesity (large energy-storing memory), high blood pressure (sign of high
sympathetic activity), altered blood lipid values (high triglyceride levels), insulin
resistance (the main storage hormone no longer works, and energy-rich substrates like
glucose and fatty acids are no longer stored properly).
Meta-analysis In a meta-analysis, the data from many individual studies are com-
bined. The evaluation can be done with a larger number of people. The conduct of a
meta-analysis is strictly standardized.
Mitochondria Mitochondria are the energy producers of our cells. They produce ATP
(adenosine triphosphate), which is needed everywhere. ATP is the energy coin that is
paid for almost everywhere. ATP production is continuous and everywhere.
Multimorbidity When a patient suffers from several diseases, the doctor speaks of
multimorbidity.
Myokines Factors of the muscle that have a hormone-like or cytokine-like effect in
other parts of the body and in the muscle.
Noradrenaline Noradrenaline is primarily a messenger of the sympathetic nerve fiber.
When noradrenaline is considered in flowing blood, it is sometimes called a hormone.
In addition, immune cells can locally produce noradrenaline. See sympathetic nervous
system.
Nucleus accumbens Important core area in the reward system (Fig. 3.1).
Obesity The term obesity is used by doctors when a person’s body mass index is 30
kg/m2 or higher (body mass index = weight in kg divided by height in m squared).
People are considered to be of normal weight if their body mass index is between 18.5
and 25 kg/m2. Between 25 and 30 kg/m2 is considered overweight.
Parasympathetic nervous system The opponent of the sympathetic nervous system.
Where the sympathetic nervous system is responsible for attack and flight, the par-
asympathetic nervous system is responsible for digestion and uptake of energy-rich
substrates (glucose, fatty acids, amino acids). Parasympathetic and sympathetic nerv-
ous system are opponents: If one system is more active, the other is quite inactive.
Personality traits See explanation 5.
254 Glossary

Phenome The totality of all externally visible characteristics of an individual, accessible


to the senses, including, for example, height, weight, blue eyes, thick lips, protruding
chin, intelligence, behavior, etc. A phenome-wide study includes all externally visible
characteristics of the individual.
Plasma cell It is the descendant of a naive B cell that has seen antigen and has devel-
oped into an antibody-producing cell. It lives long-term in the bone marrow.
Pleiotropic gene The same variant of a gene called X that causes different things at
two separate locations in the body is called pleiotropic by the geneticist (from Greek
pleíōn, more; Greek tropein, change). In other words, through a variant of the gene X,
more than one thing is changed at different locations in the body.
Positively selected see selection.
Psoriasis vulgaris Psoriasis is a chronic inflammatory autoimmune skin disease that is
associated with an increased keratinisation reaction and the formation of scales. The
areas of the skin affected by psoriasis do not usually heal spontaneously.
Psychomotor system The functional unit of the brain and skeletal muscle. Skeletal
muscle cannot decide how much energy it wants to spend per day. The brain dictates
the daily energy expenditure of skeletal muscle by deciding on the physical activities.
Therefore, it is a functional unit for the sake of energy consumption.
Renin-angiotensin-aldosterone The RAA hormones Renin, Angiotensin, and
Aldosterone have a main role in blood pressure stabilisation. During stressful events,
blood pressure must rise. The sympathetic nervous system with its messenger sub-
stances noradrenaline and adrenaline stimulates the RAA hormone system. The
RAA hormone system and the sympathetic nervous system increase blood pressure
by reducing water excretion in the kidney and constricting blood vessels. So the
increased water remains in the constricted vascular system, and this increases blood
pressure.
Receptor A protein structure that can bind a molecule (a chemical stimulus) and thus be
activated, and that transmits this activation to the interior of the cell (signal transduc-
tion). The receptor can be on the cell membrane and inside the cell. Comes from the
Latin recipere = to receive.
Rheumatoid arthritis Joint inflammation at multiple joints, usually the hands and
feet, less often the large joints and spine. The disease can also affect tissue outside
the joints. It is an autoimmune disease in which the immune system mistakenly rec-
ognizes the body’s own tissue as foreign and attacks it. Since the immune system
is active, these patients need more energy for this immune system. The above-men-
tioned citrullination of antigens plays an important pathophysiological role in this
disease.
Selection, positively selected The individuals better adapted to the respective envi-
ronment have the greater chance of surviving in the competition and reproducing
more easily. We say of a species that still exists today that it has been positively
selected or that it has experienced positive selection. This means that the species or a
Glossary 255

characteristic of this species (e.g. red comb in the cock) experienced positive selection
after many generations, so the species or the characteristic is still there. In contrast, all
species that no longer exist and are extinct experienced negative selection, they were
negatively selected.
Stress axes The stress axes are switched on in acute stress. They serve the acute redis-
tribution of energy-rich substrates from the stores (fat tissue, liver) to the consumers
(brain, muscles, heart muscle, immune system). In essence, stress axes are the hypo-
thalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis (end product: cortisol) and the brain-sympathetic
nervous system axis (end products: adrenaline from the adrenal gland and noradren-
aline from the sympathetic nerve fibers). In addition, stress also activates the thyroid
and the RAA hormones (see there).
Somatic pain Pain that can be precisely localized in the area of ​​muscles, bones, liga-
ments, tendons and skin.
Sympathetic nervous system The central stress system of the human body with the two
messenger substances adrenaline and noradrenaline. Adrenaline comes from the adre-
nal gland. The nerve fibers of the sympathetic nervous system are called sympathetic
nerve fibers, which are virtually everywhere in the body. Noradrenaline is located in
the terminal buttons of the sympathetic nerve fibers.
Synapse Connection point between two nerve cells, through which information can be
forwarded.
Systemic lupus erythematosus Systemic lupus erythematosus (SLE for short) is char-
acterized by a colorful picture of disease manifestations, with a skin involement, kid-
ney inflammation, a vascular inflammation, a brain involvement, a joint inflammation,
a depletion of blood cells and other things. It is an autoimmune disease.
T cell The T stands for thymus. T cells are part of the lymphocytes and have two main
functions: 1) They can kill other cells (using an enzyme machinery). They like to do
this if the cells are virus-infected. 2) They can help other cells of the immune sys-
tem (preferably B cells) by activating or inhibiting their function. Various T helper
cell types are distinguished by their production of key cytokines (type 1: interferon-γ;
type 2: IL-4, IL-5, IL-13; type 3: TGF-β; type 9: IL-9; type 17: IL-17A; type 22:
IL-22).
Telomere Telomeres consist of repeating units of DNA at the end of chromosomes
(Fig. 3.8). If these repeating pieces become smaller over the course of a cell’s life, this
is an indication of cell aging.
TNF TNF stands for tumor necrosis factor, because this cytokine can destroy tumors.
When scientists first described it, this tumor-destroying property was primary. Today,
TNF is considered one of the most important pro-inflammatory factors of the acti-
vated immune system. TNF is relevant to infections and autoimmune diseases, among
other things. There are now therapeutic inhibitors of TNF.
Vagus The vagus nerve is the main nerve of the parasympathetic nervous system, which
controls the digestive system. It arises in the medulla oblongata within the skull and
256 Glossary

is responsible for the continuous movement of the stomach and intestine. It also pro-
motes the release of digestive enzymes and insulin from the pancreas. It supplies the
intestine up to the middle of the large intestine. After that, the large intestine is sub-
ject to the sympathetic nervous system alone – especially with regard to the release
of stool. The vagus nerve contains many sensory nerve fibers that send information
about stimuli from the gastrointestinal tract back to the brain.
Visceral pain These pains come from pain-sensing nerve fibers from internal organs
like the gut, the lungs, heart, stomach, intestine, bladder, etc.

You might also like