Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

The clock drawing test as a screening tool in

mild cognitive impairment and very mild


dementia: a new brief method of scoring
and normative data in the elderly

Monica Ricci, Martina Pigliautile,


Valeria D’Ambrosio, Sara Ercolani,
Cinzia Bianchini, Carmelinda Ruggiero,
Nicola Vanacore, et al.
Neurological Sciences
Official Journal of the Italian
Neurological Society

ISSN 1590-1874
Volume 37
Number 6

Neurol Sci (2016) 37:867-873


DOI 10.1007/s10072-016-2480-6

1 23
Your article is protected by copyright and
all rights are held exclusively by Springer-
Verlag Italia. This e-offprint is for personal
use only and shall not be self-archived
in electronic repositories. If you wish to
self-archive your article, please use the
accepted manuscript version for posting on
your own website. You may further deposit
the accepted manuscript version in any
repository, provided it is only made publicly
available 12 months after official publication
or later and provided acknowledgement is
given to the original source of publication
and a link is inserted to the published article
on Springer's website. The link must be
accompanied by the following text: "The final
publication is available at link.springer.com”.

1 23
Author's personal copy
Neurol Sci (2016) 37:867–873
DOI 10.1007/s10072-016-2480-6

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

The clock drawing test as a screening tool in mild cognitive


impairment and very mild dementia: a new brief method
of scoring and normative data in the elderly
Monica Ricci1 • Martina Pigliautile2 • Valeria D’Ambrosio2 • Sara Ercolani2 •
Cinzia Bianchini2 • Carmelinda Ruggiero2 • Nicola Vanacore3 • Patrizia Mecocci2

Received: 17 September 2015 / Accepted: 9 January 2016 / Published online: 10 February 2016
Ó Springer-Verlag Italia 2016

Abstract many studies sustained that the clock drawing items more predictive of cognitive decline: omission of
test (CDT) was not able to accurately detect people with numbers or hands, writing numbers or hands in a wrong
CDR = 0.5. Other researchers have promoted the use of position and writing numbers or hands in a different code.
scoring approaches with multiple scales that rate quantitative Our CDT’ scoring system is very short and easy method
and qualitative features of the production. Nevertheless, which can be used also by non-specialist.
these scoring systems are complex and time-consuming. We
propose a new brief CDT’ scoring system in order to find a Keywords mild Alzheimer’s disease  mild cognitive
good measure for mild cognitive decline which is at the same impairment  clock drawing test  scoring system
time easy to administer. we enrolled 719 subjects: n. 181 with
mild Alzheimer’s disease (AD); n. 200 with amnesic mild
cognitive impairment (MCI) and n. 338 healthy elderly Introduction
subjects (C). our CDT-three-cluster scoring system demon-
strated a good sensitivity and an excellent specificity to The clock drawing test (CDT) is a widely used test for
discriminate MCI subjects from normal elderly (76 and screening cognitive impairment and dementia [1–3]. It is a
84 %, respectively) and an excellent sensitivity and speci- multidimensional tool able to evaluate visuo-constructive
ficity to discriminate patients affected by mild Alzheimer and visuo-spatial skills, symbolic and conceptual repre-
disease (CDR: 1) from normal elderly (91 and 90 %, sentation, hemiattention, semantic memory, executive
respectively). We found that CDT’ score = 1.30 discrimi- function including organization, planning, and parallel
nate people with MCI, whereas a score = 4.38 discriminate processing [3–6].
AD patients. The three-cluster-scoring-system demonstrated Several methods have been proposed for CDT scoring
a good diagnostic accuracy, taking into account those error- [5, 7–13], but some are too complex and time consuming
and other too simple to get enough sensitivity and speci-
ficity for a screening test, ranging between 77–94 % and
65–96 %, respectively [1, 8, 14–19]. Moreover, recent
Electronic supplementary material The online version of this studies, found that normal elderly subjects commit a high
article (doi:10.1007/s10072-016-2480-6) contains supplementary
material, which is available to authorized users. number of errors [20–23], suggesting that CDT’s perfor-
mances might be influenced by confounding variables,
& Monica Ricci such as age and education. In particular, one study reported
monica.ricci@students.mq.edu.au
that normal elderly have difficulties with the layout of the
1
ARC Centre of Excellence for Cognition and its Disorders, clock figure and the placement of the hands [22]. Specifi-
Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia cally, 50.8 % of observed subjects had difficulties to place
2
Institute of Gerontology and Geriatrics, University of the numbers in cardinal positions and 33.1 % failed to
Perugia, Perugia, Italy differentiate correctly the hands. Almost half of these
3
National Centre of Epidemiology, National Institute of errors were made by those aging over 80 years and with the
Health, Rome, Italy lowest education level.

123
Author's personal copy
868 Neurol Sci (2016) 37:867–873

While there is a general agreement in considering the was built (see results), based on the extracted factors. Since
CDT a useful test to detect moderate and severe dementia the drawing of circle did not affect the results, we decided
[24], its utility in distinguishing patients with mild cogni- to perform the study in the phase II population with a pre-
tive impairment or mild dementia is still debated [19, 20, drawn clock with a contour of 11 cm in diameter and the
24, 25]. Recently, a modified version of the Rouleau’ instructions have been modified (see supplementary
scoring method has been proposed, in order to evaluate and material).
score other supplementary drawing inaccuracies for Phase II. We enrolled 145 free-living, cognitively
detecting patients with MCI [26]. However this scoring healthy subjects to clarify the influence of age and level of
system is complex and highly time consuming. education on CDT and calculate the correction factors. All
A simple and sensitive test is largely needed as the subjects had no history of psychiatric/neurological
screening tool in outpatient settings, but its scoring and disorders and they did not report memory complain.
interpretation should be easy to be handled also by non- Phase III. In order to evaluate the validity of the new
specialists. The purpose of this study was to build a new scoring method, in the Memory Clinic we then enrolled 310
brief, quantitative scoring method of the CDT to identify consecutive subjects with mild AD (N. 102), with MCI
subjects with MCI and mild Alzheimer’s disease in an (N.104) and 104 cognitively normal elderly. All CDTs
elderly population. achieved by the subjects were scored with the new scoring
system by two independent raters (MP, VDA). Differences
amongst groups, sensitivity and specificity of the scale and
Methods and subjects inter-rater reliability were analyzed. A sheet paper with a
pre-drawn circle was presented with the following instruc-
The study was developed in different phases with the aim tions ‘‘This is supposed to be a clock. Please put the numbers
to (1) define a new scoring method of the CDT; (2) define on the clock. Thereafter please set the time at 10 past 11’’.
the influence of gender, age and education and, accord- All 719 subjects of this study underwent the MMSE
ingly, a correction grid; (3) validate this scoring method in [30], and corrected scores were used for the analyses.
another population. Statistical analyses were carried out using SPSS program
Phase I. In the Memory Clinic of the Geriatric Depart- (version 22.0). Comparisons amongst groups were per-
ment, University of Perugia, we enrolled 264 consecutive formed with ANOVA and Tukey’s post hoc analysis.
subjects older than 65 years with mild (CDR: 1) [27] Multiple regression analysis was performed to evaluate the
Alzheimer Disease (AD) (N.79) based on NINCDS- influence of the demographic variables (age, gender and
ADRDA criteria [28], with Mild Cognitive Impairment education). The ROC (receive operated characteristics)
(MCI) (N. 96) [29] and 89 cognitively healthy elderly (C). curves were used to detect the optimal cut-off scores to
Participants were provided with a blank sheet of A4 discriminate the three clinical groups.
paper. The instructions were as follows: ‘‘Please draw a
clock face, and place all the numbers on it. Now set the
hands at 10 past 11. One rater (MR) analyzed and listed the Results
type of errors made by each subject and then scored its
severity according to a 5-point Likert scale (from 0 for a In Table 1, demographic and clinical characteristics and
perfect performance to 5 for the worst performance). Errors CDT’s errors type observed in the phase 1 study population
on CDT were categorized based on the layout of the circle are reported. Post-hoc analyses showed that AD patients
(C), of the numbers (N) and of the hands (H). The fol- obtained worse performances compared with the C group
lowing parameters were considered and analyzed: circle in N- and H-omission, N- and H-perseveration; in writing
diameter (C-diameter), circle shape (C-distortion), lack of the numbers in a reverse order (N-reversal) and with a
numbers (N-omission), repeated numbers (N-persevera- different code (N-code); to place numbers and hands in
tion), reverting the order of numbers on the clock face (N- wrong positions (H- and H-position). The MCI group
reversal), modifying number positions (N-position), draw- achieved worse performances compared with the C group
ing the number outside the circle (N-outside), or upside in N-position and H-omission, whereas the AD group
down (N-upturned), or with different coding system such obtained impaired scores compared with the MCI group,
as Roman or Arabic (N-code); position of short and long also, in N-reversal. No significant differences were found
hands (H-position), lack of one or both hands (H-omis- amongst the groups on distortion and size of the circle; on
sion), drawing of more than two hands (H-perseveration) upturned numbers or numbers posed outside the circle and
and writing the digital time (H-code). Principal component on hands or code perseveration.
analysis, with Varimax rotation and Kaiser normalization, Principal component analysis was performed to reduce
was performed. After this analysis, a three-cluster-scale the variables, deleting redundant items (Table 2). Six

123
Author's personal copy
Neurol Sci (2016) 37:867–873 869

Table 1 Demographic and


AD N. 79 MCI N. 96 C N. 89 * p \ 0.05
clinical characteristics of phase
** p \ 0.01
I population (n. 264)
*** p \ 0.001

Age 76.5 (5.5) 75.2 (5.8) 73.3 (6.9) AD vs C *


Education 5.2 (3.5) 7.4 (4.7) 7.2 (4.2) AD vs C **
AD vs MCI**
MMSE 22.2 (3.2) 26.6 (2.2) 28.4 (1.4) AD vs C**
AD vs MCI**
MCI vs C*
CDT errors
C-diameter 4.79 (2.13) 5.24 (2.94) 5.49 (2.50) n.s.
C-distortion 0.05 (0.36) 0 (0.0) 0.5 (0.43) n.s.
N-omission 1.27 (1.66) 0.53 (1.41) 0.24 (0.89) AD vs C***
N-perseveration 0.22 (0.77) 0.13 (0.58) 0.01 (0.11) AD vs C*
N-reversal 0.51 (1.49) 0.12 (0.74) 0.06 (0.54) AD vs C**
AD vs MCI*
N-position 2.42 (1.97) 1.13 (1.6) 0.53 (1.07) AD vs C***
AD vs MCI***
MCI vs C*
N-outside 0.08 (0.58) 0.25 (1.05) 0.18 (0.92) n.s.
N-upturned 0.17 (0.65) 0.13 (0.66) 0.07 (0.37) n.s.
N-code 0.90 (1.72) 0.54 (1.39) 0.11 (0.56) AD vs C***
H-position 0.87 (1.50) 0.47 (1.03) 0.38 (0.88) AD vs C*
H-omission 2.56 (2.4) 1.05 (1.90) 0.34 (1.20) AD vs C***
AD vs MCI***
MCI vs C*
H-perseveration 0.06 (0.57) 0.03 (0.23) 0.02 (0.21) n.s.
H-code 0.21 (0.97) 0.21 (0.95) 0.03 (0.24) n.s.
C circle, N numbers, H hands

factors (namely N-omission and H-omission; N-code; represented by the second and third extracted components
H-code; H-position; N-reversal and N-position) globally (code of numbers and hands, respectively); the third cluster
accounted for the 72.8 % of total variance. (named Position) is represented by the fourth, fifth and
Taking into consideration the extracted factors, we sixth extracted components (wrong position of the hands,
defined a new brief scoring method of the CDT, with three numbers placed in a reverse order and wrong position of
clusters of errors. The first cluster (named Omission) is the numbers, respectively). In each cluster, the severity of
represented by the first extracted components (omission of errors was scored by a 5-point-likert-scale (see supple-
numbers and hands); the second cluster (named Code) is mental material).

Table 2 Principal component analysis (rotation method: Varimax with Kaiser normalization)
1 2 3 4 5 6 Eigenvalues % of variance % of cumulative variance

N-omission 0.83 1.99 18.116 18.106


H-omission 0.77
N-code 0.84 1.53 13.955 32.061
H-code 0.80 1.19 10.831 42.893
H-position 0.87 1.18 10.730 53.622
N-reversal 0.94 1.08 9.840 63.462
N-position 0.83 1.02 9.302 72.765

123
Author's personal copy
870 Neurol Sci (2016) 37:867–873

In order to clarify the influence of the age, level of Table 4 Demographic distribution of the normative study sample
education and gender on the CDT we enrolled 145-healthy- Education Age
subjects (descriptive data and demographic distribution of
our sample are summarized in the Table 3 and 4). 60-69 70-79 80-90 Total
Multiple regression analysis considering age, education 5 18 39 9 66
and gender as independent variables reveal a trend towards 8 12 29 6 47
significance of these variables on the CDT [F(3) = 2.50 13 8 10 3 21
p = 0.06; age: T = 1.70 p = 0.09; education: -1.90 17 2 8 1 11
p = 0.06; gender: T = 0.45 p = 0.31]. Even though we Total 40 86 19 145
observed just a slight tendency to the significance of age
and education, we performed the correction factors for
these variables (Table 5).
Table 5 Correction factors to the CDT raw scores
The demographic characteristics of the phase III popu-
lation are summarized in Table 6. As expected statistical Education Age
differences amongst groups were observed on the MMSE. 60 65 70 75 80 85 90
Significant differences were also observed on age (AD and
MCI groups being older than the C group) and on educa- 5 0.17 0.05 -0.07 -0.19 -0.31 -0.43 -0.55
tion level (AD being less educated than MCI and C). A 8 0.29 0.18 0.06 -0.06 -0.18 -0.30 -0.042
multivariate analysis, with age and education as covariates, 13 0.51 0.39 0.27 0.15 0.03 -0.09 -0.21
showed a significant interaction between groups and CDT 17 0.68 0.56 0.44 0.32 0.20 0.08 -0.04
measures [F(4,610) = 11,4; p \ 0.001]. Post-hoc analyses
revealed that AD patients achieved worse performances
compared with MCI and C groups (p’s \ 0.001), and the groups (Fig. 2a), a cut-off of 1.87 on the CDT corrected
MCI group obtained worse performances compared with total score had a sensitivity of 91 % and a specificity of
the C group (p \ 0.001). In order to break down the 90 %; while in distinguishing the MCI group from the C
interaction, we computed differences between CDT mea- group (Fig. 2b), the CDT’s total score best cut-off was 1.30
sures (i.e., position minus omission, position minus code, with a sensitivity of 76 % and specificity of 84 %. When
omission minus code). One-way ANOVA revealed group the CDT was used to discriminate between the AD group
differences for all the new computed measures [position- and the MCI group (Fig. 2c), the best cut-off was 4.38 with
minus-omission score, F(2,307) = 8.5 p \ 0.001; position- a sensitivity of 65 % and specificity of 72 %. The inter-
minus-code score, F(2,307) = 82.3 p \ 0.001; omission- rater reliability was excellent for all CDT’ scores: 0.98 for
minus-code score, F(2,307) = 11.5 p \ 0.001]. Post-hoc total score, 0.98 for code score, 0.94 for position score and
analyses showed that on position-minus-omission score the 0.97 for omission score.
MCI group achieved the larger difference compared with
the AD and C group (p \ 0.01 and p \ 0.001, respec-
tively), no other group differences were evident; on posi- Discussion
tion-minus-code score all groups differ significantly from
each other (p’s \ 0.001); on omission-minus-code score Accordingly with other studies [31, 32], our results showed
the AD group obtained a larger difference compared with that the omission of numbers or hands, the wrong position
both the MCI and C group (p’s \ 0.001) and no other of numbers or hands and writing numbers in a different
group differences were noted (see Fig. 1). code are more related to cognitive decline than other
In order to find a cut-off with the best sensitivity and mistakes. Taking into consideration the extracted factors,
specificity, ROC analyses were performed using the CDT we built a new brief scoring method of the CDT, involving
total score corrected for age and education accordingly three clusters of errors. The first cluster (named Omission)
with the data reported in Table 5. Between AD and C is represented by the first components extracted (i.e.,

Table 3 Descriptive data of


Lowest score Highest score Mean Standard deviation
145-healthy control subjects
(phase II population) Age 60 90 71.99 6.50
Education 1 18 7.92 4.25
MMSE 24.7 30 28.44 1.75
CDT 0 7 0.66 1.10

123
Author's personal copy
Neurol Sci (2016) 37:867–873 871

Table 6 Demographic and


AD MCI C *p \ 0.05
clinical characteristics of phase
N. 104 N. 102 N. 104 ** p \ 0.01
III population (n. 310)
M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) *** p \ 0.001

AGE 77.1 (5.7) 75.3 (5.7) 71.9 (6.8) AD vs C***


MCI vs C***
EDUCATION 5.7 (4.0) 7.3 (4.7) 8.1 (4.4) AD vs C***
AD vs MCI***
MMSE 21.7 (3.6) 26.2 (2.9) 28.7 (1.6) AD vs C***
AD vs MCI*** MCI vs C***
CDT scores Omission 2.21 (1.76) 0.82 (1.46) 0.05 (0.40) AD vs C***
AD vs MCI*** MCI vs C***
Position 2.86 (1.22) 2.16 (1.20) 0.57 (0.86) AD vs C***
AD vs MCI***
MCI vs C***
Code 1.17 (1.94) 0.68 (1.58) 0.07 (0.42) AD vs C***
AD vs MCI***
MCI vs C***
Total 6.09 (3.31) 3.57 (2.94) 0.67 (1.06) AD vs C***
AD vs MCI***
MCI vs C**

Recently, an interesting study on the modified Rouleau’


system showed an excellent specificity of that method in
discriminating C versus MCI and MCI versus AD (88 and
100 %, respectively) [26]. Nevertheless, we have few
concerns on these results. The first is the low sensitivity
values in discriminating the groups, being around 39 %
between C and MCI and 58 % between MCI and AD. The
second concern is about the severity of illness amongst AD
patients. In fact, around 30 % (10 on 33) of AD patients
scored 2 on the CDR, revealing moderate severity levels of
dementia. It is our opinion, that this heterogeneity in a
small sample could have exacerbated the specificity value
between MCI and AD. Lastly, that scoring system is
complex and highly time consuming and we are in accord
Fig. 1 CDT sub-scores. Omission, Position and Code sub-scores
obtained by the groups. Bars represent the standard error with other authors in emphasizing the need for brief,
quantitative scoring approaches that clinicians can easily
omissions of numbers and hands); the second cluster use and interpret [3, 33–35].
(named Code) is represented by the second and third There is a common consensus that age and level of
components extracted (i.e., code of numbers and code of education have an impact on the CDT [e.g., 10, 15, 36],
hands, respectively); the third cluster (named Position) is however, our findings revealed that age and education had
represented by the fourth, fifth and sixth component just a slight influence on the CDT’s performance. In the
extracted (i.e., wrong position of the hands, numbers posed normative phase, our sample was not evenly distributed
in a reverse order and wrong position of the numbers, across all the cells of demographic characteristics (as
respectively). In each cluster, the errors’ severity was showed in Table 4), with fewer subjects enrolled in the age
scored based on a 5-point-likert-scale (see supplemental range 80–90 or in cells with higher education (i.e.,
material). All groups showed significant differences on the 17 years). It is our opinion that it might have obscured age
CDT using our brief method of scoring but most impor- and education effects on CDT in our sample.
tantly, using 1.30 as cut-off, this measure can well distin- In spite of other studies that considered the integrity of
guish between MCI and C whereas a cut-off of 4.38 the clock face in the scoring system [23, 37], we suggested
discriminate AD patients. a pre-drawn circle version, because no significant

123
Author's personal copy
872 Neurol Sci (2016) 37:867–873

Fig. 2 ROC curve analysis between AD and C (a); MCI and C (b); AD and MCI (c), using CDT corrected total scores

differences were found amongst our groups on the diameter Test predict cognitive decline in older persons independent of the
and distortion of the circle. To sum up, it is our opinion that Mini-Mental State Examination? The FINE Study Group. Fin-
land, Italy, The Netherlands Elderly. J Am Geriatr Soc
our CDT’ scoring method could be easily used also by non- 44:1326–1331
specialists in detecting those patients at risk of dementia. 3. Shulman KI (2000) Clock-drawing: is it the ideal cognitive
screening test? Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 15:548–561
4. Libon DJ, Malamut BL, Swenson R, Sands LP, Cloud BS (1996)
Further analyses of clock drawings among demented and non-
Future research demented older subjects. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 11:193–205
5. Mendez MF, Ala T, Underwood KL (1992) Development of
The proposed CDT scoring method demonstrated to be a scoring criteria for the clock drawing task in Alzheimer’s disease.
reliable tool for revealing initial cognitive decline. The J Am Geriatr Soc 40:1095–1099
6. Rouleau I, Salmon DP, Butters N, Kennedy C, McGuire K (1992)
Italian normative data are also available for the Freedman Quantitative and qualitative analyses of clock drawings in Alz-
version of the CDT [38]. This version is brief and is heimer’s and Huntington’s disease. Brain Cogn 18:70–87
administered in three different conditions: (1) free-drawn 7. Sunderland T, Hill JL, Mellow AM, Lawlor BA, Gundersheimer
clock condition, in which the subjects were provided with a J, Newhouse PA, Grafman JH (1989) Clock drawing in Alzhei-
mer’s disease. A novel measure of dementia severity. J Am
blank sheet and they were asked to draw all aspects of the Geriatr Soc 37:725–729
clock; (2) pre-drawn clock condition, in which the a pre- 8. Tuokko H, Hadjistavropoulos T, Miller JA, Beattie BL (1992)
drawn circle is on a A4 sheet; (3) and the examiner pre- The Clock Test: a sensitive measure to differentiate normal
drawn clock condition, in which the subjects are provided elderly from those with Alzheimer disease. J Am Geriatr Soc
40:579–584
with a pre-drawn clock and only the hands are missing; 9. Cahn DA, Salmon DP, Monsch AU, Butters N, Wiederholt WC,
they are asked to set the hands at different times (i.e., 8:20, Corey-Bloom J, Barrett-Connor E (1996) Screening for dementia
11:10, 3 o’clock). Further investigations should consider of the alzheimer type in the community: the utility of the Clock
both our scoring system and the one proposed by Freedman Drawing Test. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 11:529–539
10. Borson S, Scanlan J, Brush M, Vitaliano P, Dokmak A (2000)
et al. [39] with its Italian normative scores in order to The mini-cog: a cognitive ‘vital signs’ measure for dementia
compare their accuracy to detect mild cognitive decline. screening in multi-lingual elderly. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry
15:1021–1027
Compliance with ethical standards 11. Scanlan JM, Brush M, Quijano C, Borson S (2002) Comparing
clock tests for dementia screening: naive judgments vs formal
Conflict of interest The authors report no conflict of interests. systems–what is optimal? Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 17:14–21
12. Freund B, Gravenstein S, Ferris R, Burke BL, Shaheen E
(2005) Drawing clocks and driving cars. J Gen Intern Med
20:240–244
13. Shulman KI, Herrmann N, Brodaty H, Chiu H, Lawlor B, Ritchie
References K, Scanlan JM (2006) IPA survey of brief cognitive screening
instruments. Int Psychogeriatr 18:281–294
1. Shulman KI, Pushkar Gold D, Cohen CA, Zucchero CA (1993) 14. Ben-Yehuda A, Bentur N, Friedman G (1995) The clock drawing
Clock-drawing and dementia in the community: A longitudinal test as a cognitive screening tool for elderly patients in an acute-
study. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 8:487–496 care hospital. Aging (Milano) 7:188–190
2. Ferrucci L, Cecchi F, Guralnik JM, Giampaoli S, Lo Noce C, 15. Brodaty H, Moore CM (1997) The clock drawing test for
Salani B, Bandinelli S, Baroni A 1996 Does the Clock Drawing dementia of the Alzheimer’s type: a comparison of three scoring

123
Author's personal copy
Neurol Sci (2016) 37:867–873 873

methods in a memory disorders clinic. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 28. McKhann G, Drachman D, Folstein M, Katzman R, Price D,
12:619–627 Stadlan EM (1984) Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease:
16. Lam LCW, Chiu HFK, Ng KO, Chan C, Chan WF, Li SW, Wong report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices
M (1998) Clock-face drawing, reading and setting tests in the of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on
screening of dementia in chinese elderly adults. J Gerontol Series Alzheimer’s Disease. Neurology 34:939–944
B Psychol Sci Social Sci 53B:P353–P357 29. Petersen RC, Doody R, Kurz A, Mohs RC, Morris JC, Rabins PV,
17. Watson YI, Arfken CL, Birge SJ (1993) Clock completion: an Ritchie K, Rossor M, Thal L, Winblad B (2001) Current concepts
objective screening test for dementia. J Am Geriatr Soc in mild cognitive impairment. Arch Neurol 58:1985–1992
41:1235–1240 30. Magni E, Binetti G, Bianchetti A, Rozzini R, Trabucchi M (1996)
18. Wolf-Klein GP, Silverstone FA, Levy AP, Brod MS (1989) Mini-mental State Examination: a normative study in italian
Screening for Alzheimer’s disease by clock drawing. J Am elderly population. Eur J Neurol 3:198–202
Geriatr Soc 37:730–734 31. Lee AY, Kim JS, Choi BH, Sohn EH (2009) Characteristics of
19. Chiu YC, Li CL, Lin KN, Chiu YF, Liu HC (2008) Sensitivity clock drawing test (CDT) errors by the dementia type: quantita-
and specificity of the Clock Drawing Test, incorporating Rouleau tive and qualitative analyses. Arch Gerontol Geriatr 48:58–60
scoring system, as a screening instrument for questionable and 32. Lessig MC, Scanlan JM, Nazemi H, Borson S (2008) Time that
mild dementia: scale development. Int J Nurs Stud 45:75–84 tells: critical clock-drawing errors for dementia screening. Int
20. Seigerschmidt E, Mosch E, Siemen M, Forstl H, Bickel H (2002) Psychogeriatr 20:459–470
The clock drawing test and questionable dementia: reliability and 33. Juby A, Tench S, Baker V (2002) The value of clock drawing in
validity. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 17:1048–1054 identifying executive cognitive dysfunction in people with a
21. Paganini-Hill A, Clark LJ (2007) Preliminary assessment of normal Mini-Mental State Examination score. CMAJ
cognitive function in older adults by clock drawing, box copying 167:859–864
and narrative writing. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 23:74–81 34. van der Burg M, Bouwen A, Stessens J, Ylieff M, Fontaine O, de
22. von Gunten A, Ostos-Wiechetek M, Brull J, Vaudaux-Pisquem I, Lepeleire J, Buntinx F (2004) Scoring clock tests for dementia
Cattin S, Duc R (2008) Clock-drawing test performance in the screening: a comparison of two scoring methods. Int J Geriatr
normal elderly and its dependence on age and education. Eur Psychiatry 19:685–689
Neurol 60:73–78 35. Samton JB, Ferrando SJ, Sanelli P, Karimi S, Raiteri V, Barnhill
23. Hubbard EJ, Santini V, Blankevoort CG, Volkers KM, Barrup JW (2005) The clock drawing test: diagnostic, functional, and
MS, Byerly L, Chaisson C, Jefferson AL, Kaplan E, Green RC, neuroimaging correlates in older medically ill adults. J Neu-
Stern RA (2008) Clock drawing performance in cognitively ropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 17:533–540
normal elderly. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 23:295–327 36. Caffarra P, Gardini S, Zonato F, Concari L, Dieci F, Copelli S,
24. Pinto E, Peters R (2009) Literature review of the clock drawing Freedman M, Stracciari A, Venneri A (2011) Italian norms for the
test as a tool for cognitive screening. Dement Geriatr Cogn Freedman version of the Clock Drawing Test. J Clin Exp Neu-
Disord 27:201–213 ropsychol 33:982–988
25. Ehreke L, Luck T, Luppa M, Konig HH, Villringer A, Riedel- 37. Babins L, Slater ME, Whitehead V, Chertkow H (2008) Can an
Heller SG (2011) Clock drawing test—screening utility for mild 18-point clock-drawing scoring system predict dementia in
cognitive impairment according to different scoring systems: elderly individuals with mild cognitive impairment? J Clin Exp
results of the Leipzig Longitudinal Study of the Aged (LEILA Neuropsychol 30:173–186
75 ?). Int Psychogeriatr 23:1592–1601 38. Caffarra P, Gardini S, Zonato F, Concari L, Dieci F, Copelli S,
26. Parsey CM, Schmitter-Edgecombe M (2011) Quantitative and Freedman M, Stracciari A, Venneri A (2011) Italian norms for the
qualitative analyses of the clock drawing test in mild cognitive Freedman version of the Clock Drawing Test. J Clin Exp Neu-
impairment and Alzheimer disease: evaluation of a modified ropsychol 33:982–988
scoring system. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 24:108–118 39. Freedman A, Leach A, Kaplan E, Winocur G, Shulman KJ, Delis
27. Morris JC (1993) The Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR): current DC (1994) Clock drawing. Oxford University Press, A neu-
version and scoring rules. Neurology 43:2412–2414 ropsychological analysis. New York

123

You might also like