Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 10

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-021-04988-9

COMMENTARY

Implications of Applying “Clinically Significant Impairment” to Autism


Assessment: Commentary on Six Problems Encountered in Clinical
Practice
Rachel Jellett1 · Joshua Muggleton2

Accepted: 22 March 2021


© Crown 2021

Abstract
The addition of ‘clinically significant impairment’ (American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and statistical manual
of mental disorders, Author, 2013) to the diagnostic criteria for autism in DSM-5 attempts to establish a threshold for the
condition. However, the increased prominence of the neurodiversity paradigm and social model of disability runs counter to
the idea that characteristics of autism are fundamentally impairing. Consequently, diagnostic criteria for autism are becom-
ing misaligned with the contemporary views of ‘disorder’ and ‘disability’. In this commentary, we outline six clinical issues
that arise from this misalignment during diagnostic assessment for autism, and the tension this creates in making diagnostic
decisions. We conclude by considering ways the ‘clinically significant impairment’ criterion could be changed, and the
implications this would have on clinical practice, and the concept of autism.

Keywords Autism Spectrum Disorders · Classification · Diagnostic Criteria · Diagnosis · Assessment · Neurodiversity

Introduction (APA, 2013). As a living document, new and salient ideas


are integrated over time, highlighting the nature of DSM
The conceptualisation of autism has shifted over time, diagnoses as “updatable working hypotheses” (Sonuga-
since Kanner’s (1943) and Asperger’s (1944) early clinical Barke, 2020, p. 1). Since the release of DSM-5, neurodiver-
reports. Once considered a rare childhood condition, autism sity and the social model of disability have gained promi-
has broadened as a diagnostic concept, and is increasingly nence and challenged traditional ideas about autism as a
recognised in individuals across the lifespan (see Happé & disorder (Baron-Cohen, 2017). Neurodiversity, originally
Frith, 2020). To reflect this change, the diagnostic criteria described by Singer as an “addition to the familiar political
for autism have been substantially revised in each iteration categories of class/gender/race” (1999, p. 64), reflects the
of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disor- natural variation between all human brains. Within this, dif-
ders (DSM; see Volkmar, 2016 for a review) since the term ferent neurotypes (groups of brains with similar patterns of
‘autism’ was first introduced as a diagnostic category in functioning) exist—including autism (a minority neurotype),
DSM-III (APA, 1980). and neurotypical (the majority neurotype). The Social Model
The DSM revisions process ideally creates alignment of Disability (Oliver, 1983) outlines the role of the interac-
between scientific understanding and best clinical practice tion between an individual’s traits and the society in which
they are situated in creating disability. The neurodiversity
movement, combined with the social model of disability,
Authors Rachel Jellett and Joshua Muggleton have contributed
equally to this work. highlight areas of incompatibility between a contemporary
view of autism and existing frameworks that define autism
* Joshua Muggleton as a disorder existing within the individual (Baron-Cohen,
joshua.muggleton@nhs.scot
2017; Kapp et al., 2013). In particular, although the social
1
Olga Tennison Autism Research Centre, La Trobe model recognises minority neurotypes can be disabling,
University, Melbourne, VIC, Australia the importance of the individual-environment interaction is
2
Clinical Psychologist for NHS Fife, Lynebank Hospital, highlighted as contributing to this disability.
Halbeath Road, Dunfermline, Fife KY11 4UW, Scotland

13
Vol.:(0123456789)
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

As a taxonomy of mental disorders, the DSM is intended conditions. Finally, while similar arguments could be applied
to provide accurate descriptions of symptom clusters to facil- to other neurodevelopmental disorders (such as Attention
itate communication and a shared understanding between Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder [ADHD]) and psychiatric
clinicians, service providers, and researchers. Such descrip- conditions, we argue that careful consideration of the issues
tions often necessitate a ‘cut off’ between a clinical and non- unique to autism is critical and timely given (a) the changing
clinical presentation (APA, 2013). The APA are forthright conceptual understanding of autism (Happe and Frith, 2020)
in acknowledging that diagnosis can be “particularly prob- and (b) a shift towards viewing autism with reference to the
lematic in clinical situations in which the patient’s symptom social model of disability.
presentation by itself […] is not inherently pathological” Future iterations of the DSM will, as ever, need to
(2013, p. 21). Variations of the phrase “symptoms cause account for how our understanding of autism has continued
clinically significant impairment in social, occupational, or to develop socially and scientifically. However, navigating
other important areas of current functioning” are used across between strict interpretation of the diagnostic criteria (of
DSM-5, including for Autism Spectrum Disorder, to mini- autism as a behaviourally defined disorder) and the changing
mise false positives and establish thresholds for disorder or culture in which these criteria operate (of autism as a minor-
disability (APA, 2013, p. 50; Lehman et al., 2002). However, ity neurotype) creates ethical and conceptual tensions which
“disorder boundaries are often unclear to even the most sea- directly impact clinical decision making today. In this paper
soned clinicians” (Kupfer et al., 2013, p. 1691). In prepara- we discuss the inherent difficulties in defining autism. We
tion for DSM-5, while the demarcation of separate disorders then outline six clinical issues encountered when applying
(Autistic Disorder, Asperger’s Disorder, Pervasive Devel- the “clinically significant impairment” criterion from the
opmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified) was the focus DSM-5 diagnostic criteria for Autism Spectrum Disorder,
of substantial research efforts (e.g., Frazier et al, 2012), less given the neurodiversity movement and social model of dis-
consideration has been given to the value and utility of the ability, and consider the practical and ethical dilemmas these
impairment criterion in establishing a clinical threshold. create in practice. Finally, we discuss ways in which a less
Further, the requirement introduced within DSM-5, that pathologising view of autism could be accommodated within
“symptoms cause clinically significant impairment” runs the diagnostic criteria to overcome these issues.
counter to the social model of disability, emphasised by the While our focus is on DSM-5, the criteria within ICD-
neurodiversity paradigm, where the role of the environment 11 also includes a requirement for clinically significant
in contributing to disability is acknowledged. This potential impairment, though allows for variability in functioning
misalignment requires careful consideration in clinical deci- due to context, meaning similar clinical issues arise (WHO,
sion making in the practice of diagnostic assessment. 2018). Equally, we must acknowledge that not all individuals
The “clinically significant impairment” criterion poses presenting for assessment will have difficulty meeting the
challenges in establishing diagnostic thresholds for a range “clinically significant impairment” criterion. For individuals
of behaviourally defined conditions (e.g., depression). How- where there is no doubt of the presence or degree of impair-
ever we consider that “clinically significant impairment” is ment as a result of their autism symptoms, the issues we
different for autism and neurodevelopmental disorders as raise with the necessity of applying this criterion will not
compared to psychiatric conditions. Being neurodevelop- be relevant as it is already met.
mental, autism is relatively stable over time, whereas mood
and anxiety disorders are often episodic (Thapar et al.,
2017) representing a deviation from the individual’s per- Difficulties Defining Autism
sonal ‘norm’. Further, these conditions by definition cause
distress, and are usually unwanted. In autism however, there [Nature] never draws a line without smudging it. (Win-
is no expectation, and often no desire (from those able to ston Churchill, 1908, p. 8)
express it), for autism to resolve, and, autistic1 behaviours
Since inception, it has been acknowledged that “the
are not necessarily distressing. Therefore, while we do not
boundaries [of autism] are not sharply differentiated from
deny that impairment can exist with autism, the nature of
other related behaviour patterns. The dividing line must
that impairment is conceptually distinct from psychiatric
therefore be fixed by arbitrary decision” (Wing et al., 1976,
p. 98). The DSM-5 incorporated dimensionality by adding
levels of support as indicators of severity, thereby avoid-
1
There are mixed opinions on identity-first/person-first language use ing the need to carve between categorical autism spectrum
(Bury et al., 2020), though identity first is increasingly being advo- diagnoses (Weitlauf et al., 2014). Yet, functioning levels are
cated for (Kenny et al., 2016). Throughout this manuscript we use transitory (APA, 2013), and not clearly defined (Mazurek
both and hope to respect broad preferences across the autism com- et al. 2019), making the division between these levels and
munity.

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

between natural human variation unclear. Recent findings stands that an autism neurotype can be present without caus-
suggest the threshold for an autism diagnosis is shifting, and ing clinically significant impairment at the time of assess-
quantifiable differences between those who do and do not ment. Indeed, Lai et al. (2020) highlight that two people who
meet the criteria are lessening (Rødgaard et al., 2019). This share similar autistic traits may receive different diagnostic
has occurred despite the DSM-5 criteria being “more strin- outcomes—one may be unimpaired in their current func-
gent” than its predecessors (Kulage et al., 2020, p. 2120), tioning and not receive a diagnosis, while the other may
and indicates that the interpretation of the diagnostic criteria receive a diagnosis due to their support needs—which is
in practice is changing. The gap between those with and conceptually complicated. Further, while autism can cause
without autism created by this dividing line is narrowing, difficulties for those with the condition, those within the
increasing the challenge of a reliable and meaningful diag- autism community often reject the notion of being disor-
nostic assessment. dered (Dyck & Russell, 2020) and highlight the strengths
To overcome the difficulties in defining who does and of their differences (Urbanowicz et al., 2019). As diagnosti-
does not have autism, the DSM-5 uses behavioural crite- cians, applying the distinction forged in the DSM-5 whilst
ria that define social deficits, and restrictive and repetitive trying to value and respect the cultural move to autism as
behaviours (Criteria A and B), alongside three additional a naturally-occurring human variation creates six problems
requirements. First, while symptoms must be present from when providing clinical assessment:
childhood, they “may not become fully manifest until social
demands exceed limited capacities, or may be masked by
learned strategies in later life” (Criterion C) and second, the Six Clinical Issues Encountered
symptoms need to cause “clinically significant impairment
in social, occupational, or other important areas of current Will Autism ‘Symptoms’ Cause Impairment,
functioning” (Criterion D; APA, 2013, p. 50). Criterion E and Must We Wait for it to Happen?
indicates the behaviours observed are not better explained by
another condition (e.g., intellectual disability). In this defini- As noted previously, the way ‘impairment’ is currently oper-
tion, the DSM-5 forges a distinction between core features of ationalised in clinical practice is unclear. In addition to the
autism described in parts A and B, and autism as a disorder core features of autism, a range of physical, genetic, devel-
that interacts with sociocultural factors potentially causing opmental and mental health comorbidities are associated
impairment in Criterion D. In clinical practice, this thresh- with the condition, as well as wider systemic disadvantage
old criterion is difficult to operationalise, not well captured (such as accessing education, employment, independence,
by diagnostic instruments (Evers et al., 2020), dependent managing money and maintaining relationships; Lord et al.,
on clinical judgement, and underpinned by socio-cultural 2018), all of which could factor into whether a person is
assumptions, making it difficult to apply (Wakefield, 2009). judged to be ‘impaired’. Further, impairment needs to be
It must be acknowledged that autistic people can (by their considered from a lifespan approach, and the trajectory can
autism, comorbidities, or environment) experience signifi- change within the same individual (Szatmari et al., 2015).
cant impairment, and clearly demonstrate meeting criterion As a result, at assessment, a person with an autistic neuro-
D. However, the social model of disability challenges the type may not yet or not currently show clinically significant
pathologising of autism, and highlights the contribution of impairment that meets criteria, potentially delaying their
sociocultural factors to disability (e.g., den Fenton & Krahn, diagnosis or contributing to missed or misdiagnosis.
2007; Houting, 2019). That is, while impairment may be Reliable diagnosis is possible by the time a child is
present, impairment is not a necessary marker of an autis- 2-years-old (Chawarska et al., 2007; Kleinman et al. 2008),
tic neurotype, and can result from environmental factors yet in these younger age groups, it can be difficult to dis-
such as a lack of accessibility, understanding, or inclusion, cern whether an individual is currently ‘impaired’ by autism
which can be changed. Considering factors such as subjec- ‘symptoms’, and more difficult still to anticipate their future
tive wellbeing and quality of life is particularly relevant trajectory. Concern that the “impairment” criterion might
in understanding functional impairment or the absence of be prohibitive for certain groups (e.g., infants/toddlers) has
impairment, and we argue this is the case whether the indi- been addressed empirically. Peters and Matson (2020) found
vidual has co-ocurring intellectual disability, strengths or it was more difficult to capture infants and toddlers under
challenges in conversational ability, or subtle or obvious DSM-5 (which introduced the ‘clinically significant impair-
characteristics of autism (see Kapp, 2018). ment’ criterion) as compared to DSM-IV-TR criteria. For
When providing a diagnostic assessment, a mismatch young children, meeting DSM-5 criteria (or both DSM-5 and
between how pronounced core characteristics of autism are DSM-IV-TR criteria) was associated with a higher symp-
and their impact on functioning is not uncommon (Constan- tom threshold compared to children who met only DSM-
tino & Charman, 2016; Szatmari et al., 2015). It therefore IV-TR criteria. Using a measure of adaptive functioning to

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

approximate clinically significant impairment, Zander and is noted in Criterion C of the DSM-5 (i.e., “…may not
Bölte (2015) found 12% of children (aged 20–47 months) become fully manifest until social demands exceed limited
who met DSM-IV-TR criteria for autism did not reach the capacities, or may be masked by learned strategies in later
threshold for even a ‘mild’ level of impairment (defined as 1 life”, APA, 2013), lack of awareness of these strategies by
standard deviation below the mean, based on the measure’s healthcare professionals is likely to contribute to missed
normative sample), suggesting these children did not clearly diagnosis, because characteristics of autism as outlined
meet the ‘clinically significant impairment’ criterion intro- in criteria A and B are disguised (Bargiela et al., 2016)
duced within DSM-5. The authors therefore suggested that and the individual is perceived as managing well when
attention should be paid when applying the impairment cri- this may not be the case (Camm-Crosbie et al., 2019).
terion to young children, and that strict adherence in clinical As such, individuals on the autism spectrum may only be
practice, or waiting for the impairment to occur, might cause identified once the environmental demands, or the strain of
delays in accessing services. This is concerning given that compensation has exceeded their capabilities—they must
the value of tailored early learning experiences is well docu- fail before their needs can be recognised.
mented (Howlin et al., 2009; McConachie & Diggle, 2007). Importantly, compensation does not negate the underly-
As having an autistic neurotype is part of the individual, ing neurotype. Although engaging in compensation strate-
clinicians may be unable to diagnose a child at first assess- gies can help autistic people participate in society, these
ment, as while they are confident in the characteristics strategies come at a cost. Social compensation strategies
described in criteria A and B, the child does not yet meet are associated with increased risk of burnout, distress,
criteria D as the demands on the child have yet to exceed mental health difficulties, and suicidality (Cassidy et al.,
their capabilities. If the requirement for significant impair- 2018; Livingston et al., 2019). Therefore, a person may be
ment delays diagnosis, it creates duplication of work and able to, through compensation, appear to most bystand-
frustration, doubt, and additional cost for parents (or the ers (and clinicians) as ‘functioning’ in employment,
state) seeking understanding of their child’s differences. As maintaining social relationships at work, and self care.
support services and accommodations are often accessed However, this may come at the longer-term and (initially)
based on an accurate diagnosis, additional avoidable delays hidden cost of their wellbeing. As such, those most diffi-
might cost children important early learning opportunities cult to identify and accurately assess may be among those
during a critical window of development (Dawson et al., at greatest risk, and in need of support and acceptance.
2012; Smith et al., 2015). Therefore, clinicians need to In contrast, it is those least able to compensate (or have
decide whether to (1) make the diagnosis despite difficulty features more difficult to compensate for, such as speech
finding evidence for impairment; (2) delay the diagnosis prosody) who are more likely to be captured by diagnostic
and ‘watch and wait’, re-evaluating the child at a later point criteria and therefore access support. While this should not
and accepting the possible delays in support, or (3) exer- minimise the needs of those who are less able to compen-
cise discretion in what characteristics they are willing to sate, the requirement for clinically significant impairment
consider impairing from a developmental perspective. In before support needs can be recognised means that support
conjunction, while aiming for diagnostic accuracy, clini- (when needed) may not be equitably distributed.
cians must weigh up the risks and benefits of a potential For diagnosticians, there is currently no validated,
false positive versus false negative diagnosis. Further, due objective way to measure compensation, nor factor in the
to the high rates of co-occurring conditions (e.g., anxiety, effects of compensation on other clinical concerns. While
depression) in those with autism (McPheeters et al., 2011; attempts are being made to better measure compensation
Simonoff et al., 2008) waiting for impairment to materialise in people with autism (Hull et al., 2019; Livingston et al.,
can be risky at any age (see issue 3). 2020), these are still in the early stages of development.
Therefore, as it stands, until diagnosticians have a better
‘Failure’ to Compensate as a Diagnostic Criterion understanding and method for assessing compensation and
identifying the underlying impact of the A and B criteria, a
Autistic people, including those not diagnosed, employ person with an autism neurotype may need to ‘fail to com-
strategies that prevent them from becoming (or appear- pensate’ before a diagnostician is able to formally recog-
ing to be) functionally impaired (Bargiela et al., 2016; nise their needs with a diagnosis. Measurement and iden-
Lai & Baron-Cohen, 2015; Livingston et al., 2020). When tification of clinically significant impairment within an
autistic behaviours are habitually suppressed (i.e., cam- individual who engages in camouflage and compensation
ouflaged) and neurotypical social skills are enacted (i.e., presents a unique challenge to clinicians and a potential
compensation) the individual might not be referred for barrier to diagnosis. In the practice of diagnostic assess-
assessment or provided with a diagnosis (Lai & Baron- ment, clinicians need to be able to see beyond the mask.
Cohen, 2015). Although the possibility of compensating

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

The Risks of Missed or Delayed Diagnosis 2007) which does not need to be cured (see Temple Gran-
din in Sacks, 1995). This might reflect a changed outlook
Whether due to waiting for impairment to occur, or for a regarding autism (e.g., greater self-acceptance) or changes
person’s compensation strategies to fail, there are risks in not in individual characteristics over time.
recognising an autism neurotype. For example, in autism, “Optimal outcomes” (Fein et al, 2013) describes those
co-occurring mental health concerns are the rule, rather than who were diagnosed with autism but no longer meet cri-
the exception, indicating “mental health assessment should teria when followed up; this has also been referred to as
be an integral aspect of clinical care [and] ongoing support “resolved ASD” (Shulman et al., 2019). Yet, as we learn
for autistic people” (Lai et al., 2019, p. 8). “Possibly autistic” how people with autism reach optimal outcomes, the ‘opti-
(p. 1508) adults have also been shown to be more likely to mal’ part is brought into question (Georgiadis & Kasari,
receive a mental health diagnosis than adults in the general 2018). Where optimal outcomes are a result of camouflag-
population, at similar rates to those with a diagnosis (Au- ing (Lai et al., 2017) or compensating, these become risk
Yeung et al., 2019) suggesting this effect is not limited to the factors for burnout, distress (Livingston et al., 2019) and
‘diagnosed’ autistic but may also be present in the autistic suicidality (Cassidy et al., 2018). Further, Leong, Hedley
neurotype. and Uljarević (2019) have questioned whether those with
Worryingly, people with autism are more likely to die pre- “resolved ASD” are actually resolved, or if the underlying
maturely, and those without a co-occuring learning disability neurotype remains, albeit with possibly reduced or refocused
are 9.4 times greater risk of dying due to suicide (Hivikoski support needs (such as greater support needs for anxiety, or
et al., 2016). Missed or misdiagnosis may mean risks are ADHD). These examples highlight the need for clearer guid-
not fully identified and managed, and that patients feel mis- ance on how clinically significant impairment is assessed.
understood, blamed, anxious, and have a reduced trust in It has been suggested (Parish-Morris, 2019) that subjec-
services (Punshon et al., 2009). Receiving a diagnosis and tive distress of the individual could be taken as evidence
the process of understanding one’s life history through the of impairment, thereby accounting for these side effects of
diagnosis can also have a significant impact on a person’s camouflage and compensation. This may also better capture
identity, and connection to community (Arnold et al., 2020; overall difficulties, avoiding autism appearing “resolved”
Bagatell, 2010; Jones et al., 2015; Punshon et al., 2009; Sar- when their needs are refocused. Careful consideration of
rett, 2016). whether such distress is due to autism, or due to co-occur-
This presents an ethical dilemma to clinicians. It is possi- ring mood, anxiety, ADHD, or external factors would be
ble, and not uncommon, to assess a client who meets criteria informative in clinical practice promoting individualised,
for the core characteristics of autism without clear evidence person-centered care and would reduce stigma, but is hard to
of impairment (e.g., Zander and Bölte, 2015). Whether operationalise or validate in diagnostic criteria. Therefore, if
or not a diagnosis is made has implications for access to we as clinicians judge impairment (in, according to DSM-5,
appropriate support services, and whether those services are social occupational, or other important areas of function-
suitably adapted to that person’s needs. Further, given the ing) by only what we observe, we may deduce that someone
increased risk of suicide and premature death, withholding who is able to compensate or camouflage is not impaired,
a diagnosis or providing an incorrect diagnosis may mean and therefore not autistic. However, this does not account
these additional risks are not recognised and managed appro- for either the effort and inconvenience of camouflaging, nor
priately by services for the person across the lifespan. the risk factors associated with it. Likewise, someone who
is well supported across environments, without compensa-
tion, ceases to be impaired and therefore no longer autistic,
Where Does the Autism Go? but seemingly ‘acquire’ autism when they transition to a
new environment, or support is withdrawn. This room for
“It is unfair to use successful living in such an envi- movement is misaligned with the view that autism is a rela-
ronment as a litmus test of who properly qualifies as tively stable neurotype and highlights the role fluctuations in
functional. (Fenton & Krahn, 2007, p. 2) person-environment fit have in how autism presents.
The DSM-5 allows for core symptom domains to be met
People with autism can, often through their own voli-
historically for those first presenting for assessment in adult-
tion, or with supports and strategies, cease to be considered
hood (criterion C). However, this does not bypass the need
as impaired (Kanner, 1972; Fein et al., 2013). This can be
for symptoms to cause clinically significant impairment in
through adapting to societal norms and expectations through
areas of current functioning. If autism is an inherent part of a
compensation and camouflage, comfortably finding a place
person’s neuropsychological development present from early
in society, or as we are beginning to understand, through liv-
years, described by a list of symptoms that are currently
ing and valuing an authentic autistic life (Fenton & Krahn,
causing impairment, yet a person can at different times be

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

and not be impaired, and therefore fulfill or not fulfill these an autistic person couldn’t do, rather than their strengths.
criteria, then where does the autism go? As Happé and Frith Therefore, by assigning a diagnostic label requiring
(2020) ask “Are we ready to consider autism as something impairment and called ‘disorder’, clinicians are at risk of
that comes and goes?”. stigmatising those they work with. Further, by recognising
only those with autism who are impaired, clinicians may
Perpetuation of Pathology and Stigma perpetuate a purely pathological understanding of autism
in society. This in turn may make it harder to recognise
Now that the voices of people on the spectrum are being and meet the needs of those who are meeting society’s
expressed and heard more, a novel and more balanced pic- expectations through camouflage and compensation at
ture of autism is taking form and increasingly accepted. considerable personal cost.
(Bölte and Richman, 2019, p. 1).
If people with the underlying autistic neurotype are
not recognised, or delayed in their recognition (Volkmar Unclear Communication
& Reichow, 2013), then we restrict our understanding of
autism to only those who are judged to meet the (somewhat The DSM-5 is “primarily a manual to be used by clini-
arbitrary) impairment threshold. This impairment may not cians” (APA, 2013, p. 7) and was designed as a commu-
be due to autism, but due to their support, coping skills, co- nication tool, aimed at “creating a common language for
occurring conditions, and environment. Thus, what is disor- communication between clinicians about the diagnosis
dered is not the person or their level of autistic symptomatol- of disorders” (APA, 2013, p. 10). Communication across
ogy, but the fit between the person and the environment (Lai research and clinical practice is essential for bidirectional
& Baron-Cohen, 2015). Therefore, research samples drawn knowledge translation, where clinical practice informs
from the diagnosed population will represent people who research and vice versa.
not only have autism, but also lack the strategies, supports, The need to communicate this descriptive, clinically
or environment they need. This could bias our understand- relevant information is prioritised over the construct valid-
ing and perpetuate the pathologising and medicalisation of ity of various diagnoses (Lehman et al., 2002; Hyman,
autism. For this reason, several studies now include par- 2010). If descriptive, clinically relevant information is
ticipants based on identification as autistic, rather than a prioritised, then it is arguably more relevant for services
formal diagnosis (Au-Yeung et al., 2019; Livingston et al., attempting to meet stakeholders needs to know whether
2020). While including those who identify with autism has someone has an autistic neurotype (and may benefit from
face validity, adopting this more widely may create further autism supports and strategies) as opposed to whether or
heterogeneity within the autism population, making further not their neurotype is causing clinically significant impair-
advances in our understanding more difficult. ment sufficient to meet the ‘clinically significant impair-
The resistance to treating or curing characteristics of ment’ criterion. Indeed, if clinicians assume that someone
autism has been long standing (“If I could snap my fingers with autism is necessarily impaired by their autism (as
and be non-autistic, I wouldn’t—it would not be me. Autism implied by their diagnosis), then this may increase the
is part of who I am”; Grandin, 1995, p. 278) and is another risk of diagnostic overshadowing (Au-Yeung et al., 2019;
lens through which to understand the mismatch between Lai & Baron-Cohen, 2015). For example, diagnosed and
this neurotype and autism the ‘disorder’. Those with lived self-identified adults have been shown to report similar
experience of autism advocate for service to improve well- rates of co-occurring mental health diagnoses (Au-Yeung
being while “preserving autistic ways of being” (den Hout- et al., 2019) suggesting clinically, their needs are similar.
ing, 2019, p. 272). This challenges a biomedical framework The result is another dilemma for diagnosticians. A per-
based on pathology and impairment and suggests the need son may not meet diagnostic criteria for autism due to lack
for a reconceptualisation of our understanding of autism. of impairment, despite autism being the best descriptor
Finally, those who identify with autism often want a for their presentation. Should a diagnosis be refused in
diagnosis to validate their sense of difference and strug- order to preserve the integrity of the diagnosis, preserve
gle in a world not designed for them (Jones et al., 2015). resources, and ensure research validity, as they do not
However, the diagnosis can also create stigma depend- technically meet criteria? Or, should a diagnosis be given
ing on the framework of understanding applied to autism on the basis of the similarities in needs between diagnosed
(Bölte & Richman, 2019). For example, Crane et al (2018) and undiagnosed but self-identifying as autistic, and that
found that a significant source of dissatisfaction for autistic this diagnosis would be readily understood by others need-
adults, parents, and professionals in the diagnostic pro- ing to respond to the person’s needs?
cess was professionals’ focus on the negatives and what

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

Clinical Discussion and Conclusion conditions associated with autism, removing the diagnostic
criteria would potentially be a step backward in providing
The DSM-5 criteria for autism represent 33 years of iter- understanding and appropriate support for autism and co-
ative attempts by the APA to define a collection of com- occurring conditions. Further, the existing tension between
monly co-occurring behaviours with varied aetiologies, in a the neurotype and disorder of autism has led to tension
spirit of nonmaleficence (Swedo, et al., 2012). The need for within the autism community, with some ‘biocertified’ while
these behaviours to cause clinically significant impairment others self-diagnosed (Sarrett, 2016). This has the potential
has been used to identify whether symptoms are significant to give people a sense of understanding and belonging, but
enough to be classified as a disorder. However, as the social also risks self-misdiagnoses. Removing Autism Spectrum
model of disability highlights, autistic behaviours and char- Disorder as a diagnosis seems unlikely to stop the term being
acteristics may not inherently cause impairment (den Hout- used but may instead lead to it being used without diagnostic
ing, 2019). This creates a dichotomy between the formal rigor, creating confusion. Therefore, although a future pos-
criteria for autism the neurodevelopmental disorder and the sibility, removal of autism as a diagnostic category does not
profile of strengths and difficulties associated with an autism resolve the challenges highlighted herein.
neurotype. Difficulties arise in clinical practice where a neu- Alternatively, if the core (A and B) symptom criteria ade-
rological difference is defined and evaluated behaviourally quately capture the clinical presentation of autism, inform
(Leong et al., 2019; McPartland, 2019), and broadly reflects the next steps in providing support, and help individuals
the limitations of clinical opinion as the gold standard of understand themselves, the requirement that “symptoms
assessment (Constantino & Charman, 2016). cause clinically significant impairment” may be redundant
Doing nothing is not an option. Diagnostic decisions (Spitzer & Wakefield, 1999). Yet, removing this attempt to
have profound implications (both positive and negative) on define the diagnostic threshold could create new problems.
the person, which must be carefully balanced in diagnos- Given the heterogeneity of autism, applying a single term to
tic assessments. A diagnosis of autism affects the provision those with wide ranging support needs, from those who do
and potentially reimbursement of care and the support and not need support (and, indeed, may be able to use autism to
understanding a person receives. A diagnosis also helps their advantage in their life and work) to those who require
to anticipate and clarify possible co-occurring conditions, regular, potentially intensive support, creates difficulties.
which is imperative given the disproportionately poor mental The term autism may lose its utility when using diagnostic
health outcomes for those on the spectrum. Diagnoses also categories to help determine support needs and access to
have broader effects, as society’s perceptions of autism and services.
research participants are created by the exemplars diagnosed A middle ground may be to remove the requirement for
by clinicians. Finally, a diagnosis or lack thereof can have impairment and reduce deficit language in the way Autism is
significant effects on a person’s self-concept and self-iden- described. Instead of Autism Spectrum Disorder, an “autistic
tity (Jones et al., 2015, Arnold et al., 2020). Therefore, the neurotype” could be defined based on a pattern of character-
decisions clinicians make extend beyond applying a diag- istics already recognised as autistic, without the requirement
nostic yardstick to a person, and also involve an evaluation for this pattern to cause impairment.2 A diagnosis of an autism
of the ethical and risk implications for each person assessed, neurotype could highlight potential future risk and associ-
placing them in a bind. ated factors, allowing those around the person (e.g., services,
One radical option would be to remove Autism Spectrum schools, employers, family) to be aware that a person has a
Disorder from the DSM-5. This would reduce the pathologis- specific profile of strengths and difficulties, and may benefit
ing of autism, and recognise autism as part of natural human from awareness and understanding of this. For example, an
variation (as argued by the neurodiversity paradigm). It autistic person may be unimpaired by their autism, but be
could be argued that those currently judged to be ‘impaired’ suffering from PTSD, and benefit from recommendations for
by their autism are actually impaired by co-occurring condi- adapting therapy for people with autism (e.g., NICE, 2013).
tions, or (from a social model of disability) a lack of support This approach takes a depathologising and destigmatising
and inclusion, both of which could be used to support their stance, and moves away from a medical model, consistent with
continued access to services. However, removal of autism the preferences of many autistic adults (Bury et al., 2020).
as a diagnosis seems premature given the utility of the The term “Autism Spectrum Disorder” could then become
diagnostic category and the practice of diagnosis for clini-
cians and individuals. Removal also seems premature given
the ongoing unmet needs of those on the autism spectrum 2
We assume here that difficulties are internal: a descriptor which
within today’s socio-cultural context. Given our increasing marks how well a person can do a particular task. Impairment we
understanding of risks to mental health, epilepsy, and other define something created when the effects of difficulties reach a point
where they interfere with a person’s functioning.

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

a transitory term applied to those with an “autistic neuro- References


type” but where their symptoms are also currently causing
impairment. American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and statistical
We suggest that removing the requirement for impairment manual of mental disorders. (3rd ed.). Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical
could be achieved through the dimensional ‘functioning lev- manual of mental disorders. (5th ed.). Author.
els’ specifier already available within DSM-5, by adding the Arnold, S. R. C., Huang, Y., Hwang, Y. I., Richdale, A. L., Trollor,
option of ‘Level 0′ (signifying no current impairment based J. N., & Lawson, L. P. (2020). “The single most important thing
on autism characteristics), and allowing impairment levels that has happened to me in my life”: Development of the impact
of diagnosis scale - preliminary revision. Autism in Adulthood.
to be transitory over time as the environment, the demands https://​doi.​org/​10.​1089/​aut.​2019.​0059.
placed on the person, their supports, and their coping strategies Asperger, H. (1944). Die “autistischen Psychopathen” im kindesalter.
change over time. This middle ground would allow for impair- Archiv fur Psychiatrie und Nerverkrakheiten, 117, 76–136.
ment to be decoupled from the concept of autism. This would Au-Yeung, S. K., Bradley, L., Robertson, A. E., Shaw, R., Baron-
Cohen, S., & Cassidy, S. (2019). Experience of mental health
be a small but significant step, as removing the requirement diagnosis and perceived misdiagnosis in autistic, possibly autistic
of impairment, would create space for the consideration of and non-autistic adults. Autism, 23, 1508–1518. https://d​ oi.o​ rg/1​ 0.​
autistic strengths in future diagnostic criteria. This would not 1177/​13623​61318​818167.
only create greater alignment in the diagnostic criteria with Bagatell, N. (2010). From cure to community: transforming notion of
autism. Ethos, 38, 33–55. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1111/j.​1548-​1352.​
the neurodiversity movement, but also help the public under- 2009.​01080.x.
stand a more balanced and rounded view of autism. In turn, Bargiela, S., Steward, R., & Mandy, W. (2016). The experiences of
this may allow diagnosticians to move away from ‘diagnosis’ late-diagnosed women with autism spectrum conditions: An
and towards ‘recognition’ of, as Happé and Frith describe “a investigation of the female autism phenotype. Journal of Autism
and Developmental Disorders, 46, 3081–3094. https://​doi.​org/​10.​
cognitive style or personality type, perhaps?” (2020, p. 11). 1007/​s10803-​016-​2872-8.
As such, the decoupling of autism and impairment may be the Baron-Cohen, S. (2017). Editorial Perspective: Neurodiversity–a rev-
first step on “[possibly] the most dramatic reconceptualisation olutionary concept for autism and psychiatry. Journal of Child
of autism yet” (2020, p. 11). Psychology and Psychiatry, 58(6), 744–747. https://​doi.​org/​10.​
1111/​jcpp.​12703.
There is no simple solution to the nature and boundaries Bölte, S., & Richman, K. A. (2019). Hard talk: Does autism need phi-
of autism, which this paper has briefly highlighted through a losophy? Autism, 23, 3–7. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1177/​13623​61318​
clinical lens. As always, further research is needed to resolve 808181.
the dichotomy of applying ‘clinically significant impairment’ Bury, M., Jellett, R., Spoor, J. R., & Hedley, D. (2020). “It defines
who I am” or “It’s something I have”: What language do [Autis-
criterion to people with autism, and how we define autism in tic] Australian adults [on the autism spectrum] prefer? Journal
a society more inclusive of diverse and minority neurotypes. of Autism and Developmental Disorders. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​
Until this time, clinicians are faced with practical and ethical s10803-​020-​04425-3.
considerations whenever approaching an assessment. This is Camm-Crosbie, L., Bradley, L., Shaw, R., Baron-Cohen, S., & Cas-
sidy, S. (2019). ‘People like me don’t get support’: Autistic adults’
not to say that the clinically significant impairment criterion experiences of support and treatment for mental health difficulties,
should be ignored. For autism as a diagnostic label to retain self-injury and suicidality. Autism, 23(6), 1431–1441. https://​doi.​
integrity, it requires clinicians to use the label consistently, and org/​10.​1177/​13623​61318​816053.
inconsistent application of the clinically significant impair- Cassidy, S., Bradley, L., Shaw, R., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2018). Risk
markers for suicidality in autistic adults. Molecular Autism.
ment criterion would undermine this. However, careful con- https://​doi.​org/​10.​1186/​s13229-​018-​0226-4.
sideration of the implications of its use or disuse is required Chawarska, K., Klin, A., Paul, R., & Volkmar, F. (2007). Autism
at an international level, a service operation level, and daily spectrum disorder in the second year: Stability and change in
by practicing clinicians evaluating the unique aspects of each syndrome expression. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychia-
try, 48(2), 128–138. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1111/j/​1469-​7610.​2006.​
individual case. 01685.x.
Churchill, W. (1908). In M Soames (2001). Winston and Clementine:
Acknowledgments The authors would like to thank colleagues for their The personal letters of the Churchills. Houghton Mifflin Company.
comments on early drafts, and the reviewers for their helpful critiques. Constantino, J. N., & Charman, T. (2016). Diagnosis of autism spec-
trum disorder: Reconciling the syndrome, its diverse origins, and
Author Contributions Both authors contributed equally to all elements variation in expression. Lancet Neurology, 15, 279–291. https://​
of this work. doi.​org/​10.​1016/​S1474-​4422(15)​00151-9.
Crane, L., Batty, R., Adeyinka, H., Goddard, L., Henry, L. A., & Hill,
Funding This study received no funding. E. (2018). Autism diagnosis in the United Kingdom: Perspective
of autistic adults, parents and professionals. Journal of Autism
and Developmental Disorders, 48, 3761–3772. https://​doi.​org/​10.​
Declarations 1007/​s10803-​018-​3639-1.
Dawson, G., Jones, E. J., Merkle, K., Venema, K., Lowy, R., Faja, S.,
Conflict of interest The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare Kamara, D., Murias, M., Greenson, J., Winter, J., Smith, M., Rog-
that are relevant to the content of this article. ers, S. J., & Webb, S. J. (2012). Early behavioral intervention is

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

associated with normalized brain activity in young children with Kapp, S. K., Gillespie-Lynch, K., Sherman, L. E., & Hutman, T.
autism. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent (2013). Deficit, difference, or both? Autism and neurodiversity.
Psychiatry, 51(11), 1150–1159. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1016/j.​jaac.​ Developmental Psychology, 49(1), 59. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1037/​
2012.​08.​018. a0028​353.
den Houting, J. (2019). Neurodiversity: An insider’s perspective. Kenny, L., Hattersley, C., Molins, B., Buckley, C., Povey, C., & Pel-
Autism, 23(2), 271–273. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1177/​13623​61318​ licano, E. (2016). Which terms should be used to describe autism?
820762. Perspectives from the UK autism community. Autism, 20(4), 442–
Dyck, E., & Russell, G. (2020). Challenging psychiatric classifica- 462. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1177/​13623​61315​588200.
tion: Healthy autistic diversity the neurodiversity movement. In Kleinman, J. M., Ventola, P. E., Pandey, J., Verbalis, A. D., Barton,
S. J. Taylor & A. Brumby (Eds.), Healthy minds in the twentieth M., Hodgson, S., Green, J., Dumont-Mathieu, T., Robins, D. L.,
century. (pp. 167–187). Palgrave Macmillan. & Fein, D. (2008). Diagnostic stability in very young children
Evers, K., Maljaars, J., Carrington, S. J., Carter, A. S., Happé, F., Stey- with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Devel-
aert, J., Leekham, S. R., & Noens, I. (2020). How well are DSM-5 opmental Disorders, 38, 606–615. https://​d oi.​o rg/​1 0.​1 007/​
diagnostic criteria for ASD represented in standardized diagnostic s10803-​007-​0427-8.
instruments? European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. https://​ Kulage, K. M., Goldberg, J., Usseglio, J., Romero, D., Bain, J. M., &
doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s00787-​020-​01481-z. Smaldone, A. M. (2020). How has DSM-5 affected autism diag-
Fein, D., Barton, M., Eigsti, I. M., Kelley, E., Naigles, L., Schultz, R. nosis? A 5-year follow-up systematic literature review and meta-
T., Stevens, M., Helt, M., Orinstein, A., Rosenthal, M., Toryb, analysis. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 50,
E., & Tyson, K. (2013). Optimal outcome in individuals with a 2102–2127. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s10803-​019-​03967-5.
history of autism. Journal of Child Psychology and psychiatry, Kupfer, D. J., Kuhl, E. A., & Regier, D. A. (2013). DSM-5 – The
54, 195–205. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1111/​jcpp.​12037. future arrived. Journal of the American Medical Association, 309,
Fenton, A., & Krahn, T. (2007). Autism, neurodiversity, and equality 1691–1692. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1001/​jama.​2013.​2298.
beyond the “normal.” Journal of Ethics in Mental Health, 2, 1–6. Lai, M. C., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2015). Identifying the lost generation
Frazier, T. W., Youngstrom, E. A., Speer, L., Embacher, R., Law, P., of adults with autism spectrum conditions. Lancet Psychiatry,
Constantino, J., Findling, R. L., Hardan, A. Y., & Eng, C. (2012). 11, 1013–1027. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1016/​S2215-​0366(15)​00277-1.
Validation of proposed DSM-5 criteria for autism spectrum dis- Lai, M. C., Lombardo, M. V., Ruigrok, A. N. V., Chakrabarti, B., Auye-
order. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent ung, B., Szatmari, P., Happé, F., Baron-Cohen, S., & MRC AIMS
Psychiatry, 51, 28–40. https://d​ oi.o​ rg/1​ 0.1​ 016/j.j​ aac.2​ 011.0​ 9.0​ 21. Consortium. (2017). Quantifying and exploring camouflaging in
Georgiades, S., & Kasari, C. (2018). Reframing optimal outcomes in men and women with autism. Autism, 21, 690–702. https://​doi.​
autism. JAMA Pediatrics, 172, 716–717. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1001/​ org/​10.​1177/​13623​61316​671012.
jamap​ediat​rics.​2018.​1016. Lai, M. C., Kassee, C., Besney, R., Bonato, S., Hull, L., Mandy, W.,
Grandin, T. (1995) in O. Sacks An anthropologist on Mars: Seven para- Szatmari, P., & Ameis, S. H. (2019). Prevalance of co-occoring
doxical tales. Knopf. mental health diagnoses in the autism population: A systematic
Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2020). Looking back to look forward – changes review and meta-analysis. The Lancet Psychiatry, 6, 819–829.
in the concept of autism and implications for future research. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1016/​S2215-​0366(19)​30289-5.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 61, 218–232. https://​ Lai, M. C., Anagnostou, E., Wiznitzer, M., Allison, C., & Baron-
doi.​org/​10.​1111/​jcpp.​13176. Cohen, S. (2020). Evidence-based support for autistic people
Hivikoski, T., Mittendorfer-Rutz, E., Boman, M., Larsson, H., Lichten- across the lifespan: Maximising potential, minimising barriers,
stein, P., & Bölte, S. (2016). Premature mortality in autism spec- and optimising the person-environment fit. The Lancet Neurology,
trum disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry, 208, 232–238. 19, 434–451. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1016/​S1474-​4422(20)​30034-x.
https://​doi.​org/​10.​1192/​bjp.​bp.​114.​160192. Lehman, A. F., Alexopoulos, G. S., Goldman, H., Jeste, D., & Üstün,
Howlin, P., Magiati, I., & Charman, T. (2009). Systematic review of B. (2002). Mental disorders and disability: Time to reevaluate the
early intensive behavioral interventions for children with autism. relationship? In D.J. Kupfer, M.B. First, & D.A. Regier (Ed.) A
American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, Research Agenda for DSM-V. (pp. 201–218). American Psychi-
114(1), 23–41. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1352/​2009.​114:​23;​nd41. atric Association.
Hull, L., Mandy, W., Lai, M.-C., Baron-Cohen, S., Allison, C., Smith, Leong, D., Hedley, D., & Uljarević, M. (2019). Poh-tay-toe, Po-tah-toe:
P., & Petrides, K. V. (2019). Development and validation of the Autism diagnosis and conceptualzation. Journal of Child Neurol-
camouflaging autistic traits questionnaire (CAT-Q). Journal of ogy, 35, 247–248. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1177/​08830​73819​887587.
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 49, 819–833. https://​doi.​ Livingston, L. A., Shah, P., & Happe, F. (2019). Compensatory stratag-
org/​10.​1007/​s10803-​018-​3792-6. ies below the behavioural surface in autism: A qualitative study.
Hyman, S. E. (2010). The diagnosis of mental disorders: The problem The Lancet Psychiatry, 6, 766–777. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1016/​
of reification. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 6, 155–179. S2215-​0366(19302​24-x.
https://​doi.​org/​10.​1146/​annur​ev.​clinp​sy.3.​022806.​091532. Livingston, L. A., Shah, P., Milner, V., & Happé, F. (2020). Quan-
Jones, J. L., Gallus, K. L., Viering, K. L., & Oseland, L. M. (2015). tifying compensatory strategies in adults with and without
‘Are you by chance on the spectrum?’ Adolescents with autism diagnosed autism. Molecular Autism. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1186/​
spectrum disorder making sense of their diagnoses. Disability & s13229-​019-​0308-y.
Society, 30, 1490–1504. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1080/​09687​599.​2015.​ Lord, C., Elsabbagh, M., Baird, G., & Veenstra-Vanderweele, J. (2018).
11089​02. Autism spectrum disorder. The Lancet, 392(10146), 508–520.
Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Mazurek, M. O., Lu, F., Macklin, E. A., & Handen, B. L. (2019). Fac-
child, 2, 217–253. tors Associated with DSM-5 severity level ratings for autism
Kanner, L. (1972). How far can autistic children go in matters of social spectrum disorder. Autism, 23, 468–476. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1177/​
adaptation? Journal of autism and childhood schizophrenia, 2, 13623​61318​75531​18.
9–33. McConachie, H., & Diggle, T. (2007). Parent implemented early inter-
Kapp, S. K. (2018). Social support, well-being, and quality of life vention for young children with autism spectrum disorder: A sys-
among individuals on the autism spectrum. Pediatrics, 141(Sup- tematic review. Journal of evaluation in clinical practice, 13(1),
plement 4), S362–S368. 120–129. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1111/j.​1365-​2753.​2006.​00674.x.

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

McPheeters, M. L., Davis, A., Navarre, J. R., & Scott, T. A. (2011). Swedo, S. E., Baird, G., Cook, E. H., Happé, F. G., Harris, J. C., Kauf-
Family report of ASD concomitant with depression or anxiety mann, W. E., King, B. H., Lord, C. E., Piven, J., Rogers, S. J.,
among US children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor- Spence, S. J., Wetherby, A., & Wright, H. H. (2012). Commentary
ders, 41(5), 646–653. https://d​ oi.o​ rg/1​ 0.1​ 007/s​ 10803-0​ 10-1​ 085-9. from the DSM-5 workgroup on neurodevelopmental disorders.
McPartland, J. C. (2019). Autism’s existential crisis: A reflection on Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psy-
Livingston et al (2018). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychia- chiatry, 51, 347–349. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1016/j.​jaac.​2012.​02.​013.
try, 60, 111–113. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1111/​jcpp.​12989. Szatmari, P., Georgiades, S., Duku, E., Bennett, T. A., Bryson, S.,
National Institute for Health and Care Excellence. (2013). Autism Fombonne, E., Mirenda, P., Roberts, W., Smith, I. M., Vaillan-
Spectrum Disorder in under 19s: support and management. NICE court, T., Volden, J., Waddell, C., Zwaigenbaum, L., Elsabbagh,
Clinical Guideline 170. https://​www.​nice.​org.​uk/​guida​nce/​cg170 L., & Thompson, A. (2015). Developmental trajectories of symp-
Oliver, M. (1983). Social work with disabled people. . Macmillan. tom severity and adaptive functioning in an inception cohort of
Parish-Morris, J. (2019). Seeing the unseen realities of autism. The preschool children with autism spectrum disorder. JAMA Psy-
Lancet Psychiatry, 6, 718–719. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1016/​s2215-​ chiatry, 72(3), 276–283. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1001/​jamap​sychi​atry.​
0366(19)​30295-0. 2014.​2463.
Peters, W. J., & Matson, J. L. (2020). Comparing rates of diagnosis Thapar, A., Cooper, M., & Rutter, M. (2017). Neurodevelopmental
using DSM-IV-TR versus DSM-5 criteria for autism spectrum disorders. The Lancet Psychiatry, 4(4), 339–346.
disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 50, Urbanowicz, A., Nicolaidis, C., Houting, J. D., Shore, S. M., Gaudion,
1898–1906. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s10803-​019-​03941-1. K., Girdler, S., & Savarese, R. J. (2019). An expert discussion on
Punshon, C., Skirrow, P., & Murphy, G. (2009). The ‘not guilty verdict’ strengths-based approaches in autism. Autism in Adulthood, 1(2),
psychological reactions to a diagnosis of Asperger syndrome in 82–89. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1089/​aut.​2019.​29002.​aju.
adulthood. Autism, 13(3), 265–283. https://d​ oi.o​ rg/1​ 0.1​ 177/1​ 3623​ Volkmar, F. R., & Reichow, B. (2013). Autism in DSM-5: Progress
61309​103795. and challenges. Molecular Autism. https://​d oi.​o rg/​1 0.​1 186/​
Rødgaard, E. M., Jensen, K., Vergnes, J. N., Soulières, I., & Mottron, 2040-​2392-4-​13.
L. (2019). Temporal changes in effect sizes of studies compar- Volkmar, F. R. (2016). From infantile autism to autism spectrum. Evo-
ing individuals with and without autism: A meta-analysis. JAMA lution of the Diagnostic Concept. Christopher McDougle (Eds)
Psychiatry, 76, 1124–1132. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1001/​jamap​sychi​ Autism spectrum disorder, 3–18. Oxford: Oxford University Press
atry.​2019.​1956. Wakefield, J. C. (2009). Disability and diagnosis: should role impair-
Sarrett, J. C. (2016). Biocertification and neurodiversity: The role and ment be eliminated from DSM/ICD diagnostic criteria? World
implications of self-diagnosis in autistic communities. Neuroeth- Psychiatry, 8(2), 87. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1002/j.​2051-​5545.​2009.​
ics, 9, 23–36. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s12152-​016-​9247-x. tb002​21.x.
Shulman, L., D’Agostino, E., Lee, S., Valicenti-McDermott, M., Seijo, Weitlauf, A. S., Gotham, K. O., Vehorn, A. C., & Warren, Z. E. (2014).
R., Tulloch, E., Meringolo, D., & Tarshis, N. (2019). When an Brief report: DSM-5 “levels of support:” A comment on discrep-
early diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder resolves, what ant conceptualizations of severity in ASD. Journal of Autism and
remains? Journal of Child Neurology, 34, 382–386. https://​doi.​ Developmental Disorders, 44(2), 471–476. https://​doi.​org/​10.​
org/​10.​1177/​08830​73819​834428. 1007/​s10803-​013-​1882-z.
Simonoff, E., Pickles, A., Charman, T., Chandler, S., Loucas, T., & Wing, L., Yeates, S. R., Brierley, L. M., & Gould, J. (1976). The
Baird, G. (2008). Psychiatric disorders in children with autism prevalence of early childhood autism: Comparison of administra-
spectrum disorders: Prevalence, comorbidity, and associated fac- tive and epidemiological studies. Psychological Medicine, 6(1),
tors in a population-derived sample. Journal of the American 89–100. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1017/​s0033​29170​00075​22.
Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 47(8), 921–929. World Health Organization. (2018). International statistical classifica-
https://​doi.​org/​10.​1097/​CHI.​0b013​e3181​79964f. tion of diseases and related health problems (11th Rev). https://​
Singer, J. (1999). Why can’t you be normal for once in your life from icd.​who.​int/​en. Accessed 2 Mar 2020.
a “problem with no name” to the emergence of a new category Zander, E., & Bölte, S. (2015). The new DSM-5 impairment criterion:
of difference. In D. Discourse (Ed.), Sally French. (pp. 57–67). A challenge to early autism spectrum disorder diagnosis? Jour-
Open University Press. nal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(11), 3634–3643.
Smith, I. C., Reichow, B., & Volkmar, F. R. (2015). The effects of https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s10803-​015-​2512-8.
DSM-5 criteria on number of individuals diagnosed with autism
spectrum disorder: A systematic review. Journal of Autism and Publisher’s Note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to
Developmental Disorders, 45(8), 2541–2552. https://​doi.​org/​10.​ jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
1007/​s10803-​015-​2423-8.
Sonuga-Barke, J. S. (2020). people get ready’: Are mental disorder
diagnostics ripe for a kuhnian revolution? Journal of Child Psy-
chology and Psychiatry, 61, 1–3. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1111/​jcpp.​
13181.
Spitzer, R. L., & Wakefield, J. C. (1999). DSM-IV diagnostic crite-
rion for clinical significance: Does it help solve the false positives
problem? American Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 1856–1864.

13

You might also like