Download as docx, pdf, or txt
Download as docx, pdf, or txt
You are on page 1of 8

Script veideo 17

Last time we talked about tests


for sufficient conditions and necessary conditions.
And those tests have worked fine in a lot
of cases, but they're not going to work in all cases.
because remember, we were looking at what caused death.
Reproduce el video desde ::16 y sigue la transcripción0:16
Death, well, you're either dead or not dead.
And we talk about whether the person had fish or not.
We didn't look at cases where people liked just a little bit of fish.
Either they ate fish or they didn't eat fish.
Reproduce el video desde ::28 y sigue la transcripción0:28
But there are a lot of causal relations that hold, not between
absolute or dichotomous properties. You're either dead or not dead, you ate
fish or you didn't eat fish. Instead, some casual
relations hold between properties
that come in degrees. For example,
carbon dioxide and global warming.
Causing heating in the world?
What we need to think about is the degree of carbon dioxide.
And sure enough, that's what the intergovernmental panel on climate
change, the IPCC used to reach its conclusion that increasing
levels of carbon dioxide were causing increasing global temperatures.
Well, we can't use the sufficient
condition test or the necessary condition test.
But we can still reach a conclusion about causation.
We can even know the mechanism for the causation.
Reproduce el video desde :1:36 y sigue la transcripción1:36
The carbon dioxide reflects the heat from the sun back to the Earth.
Traps it in, like a greenhouse effect, as it's called.
Reproduce el video desde :1:44 y sigue la transcripción1:44
So the question for
this lecture is, how are we going to use
arguments to justify those kinds of causal claims?
Reproduce el video desde :1:53 y sigue la transcripción1:53
So, let's look at another example.
Suppose a runner runs a mile, and it takes him ten minutes.
Why were they so slow? Well maybe because they were really heavy.
Reproduce el video desde :2:5 y sigue la transcripción2:05
Now, weight can't be sufficient for running
slow, because everybody's got a certain weight.
So it's not an on-and-off property. It's a property that comes in degrees.
And again, the heavier, the harder it's going to be to run.
We know the mechanism, because it takes more energy to move a heavy body.
So we've got a pretty good causal story
and a pretty good causal hypothesis, but you
can't use the necessary and sufficient condition test,
because we're dealing with properties that come in degrees.
A third example.
Suppose the
wages go down.
Well that's because the unemployment was high.
There's always some unemployment.
Wages are never as high as you'd like them to be.
So how do you get a causal claim?
Well, it's because the more unemployment there is, the lower the wages are.
because if a lot of people are unemployed, they're
willing to take jobs for a lot less wage.
Reproduce el video desde :2:58 y sigue la transcripción2:58
So we've got a causal story and we've got a causal claim.
They seem to justified,
but you can't use the necessary and sufficient condition test.
We need a different test.
Reproduce el video desde :3:8 y sigue la transcripción3:08
And the test you need has to deal with properties that come in
degrees, and the test that does that
is called the method of concomitant variation.
It was developed by John Stuart Mill in the 19th Century.
Brilliant philosopher.
He also developed tests that, basically what we've been
calling the necessary condition test and the sufficient condition test, parallel.
He gave them different names, but it's the same basic idea.
This lecture's on the method of concomitant or concomitant variation,
also developed by John Stuart Mill in the 19th century.
Reproduce el video desde :3:43 y sigue la transcripción3:43
And in order to apply the method
of concomitant or concomitant variation, the first thing
we've got to get straight is the idea
of what are concomitant or concomitant variation is.
Well to avoid that word, people these days call them correlations.
Reproduce el video desde :3:58 y sigue la transcripción3:58
The first kind of correlation we've got to understand is a positive correlation.
Reproduce el video desde :4:3 y sigue la transcripción4:03
X and Y are positively correlated when an increase in X is associated
with an increase in Y, and a decrease in X is associated with
a decrease in Y. That's what a positive correlation is.
In contrast, a negative correlation is when an increase
in X is associated with a decrease in Y, and a decrease
in X is associated with an increase in Y. That's why
they're negatively correlated, because increase is associated with decrease.
Whereas they're positively correlated when increase and increase
are associated and decrease and decrease are associated.
So let's look at some examples.
First of all, calorie intake is associated with weight.
More calories you take in, the more weight you're going to gain.
So they're positively correlated.
Reproduce el video desde :4:56 y sigue la transcripción4:56
Whereas weight and exercise are
negatively correlated.
The more exercise, the less weight, because it
burns off those calories you took in, so you
get a negative correlation between weight and exercise,
and a positive correlation between weight and calorie intake.
Another example. Height and age are positively correlated
before the age of 20, because people tend to get taller as they grow older.
Reproduce el video desde :5:24 y sigue la transcripción5:24
But height and age are negatively correlated after the age of 60.
Because the space between people's vertebrae
tend to decrease after a certain age.
Reproduce el video desde :5:36 y sigue la transcripción5:36
So, this shows that two things can be positively
correlated in some circumstances, and negatively correlated in others.
Which correlation holds is going to depend on the circumstances.
That is, how old the person is.
And now the next
question, is how can we get from these correlations to causal relations?
Reproduce el video desde :5:55 y sigue la transcripción5:55
When two features are correlated, positively or negatively,
then there're four possible causal relations between them.
First, A might cause B.
Reproduce el video desde :6:7 y sigue la transcripción6:07
Sometimes, when A and B are correlated, that means
that A is causing some kind of change in B.
Reproduce el video desde :6:14 y sigue la transcripción6:14
Second
possibility. Is it B causes A?
because the correlation's symmetrical.
If A is correlated with B, B's correlated with A.
Reproduce el video desde :6:23 y sigue la transcripción6:23
And then we don't know whether A causes B, or B causes A.
But those are two different possibilities, and we'll have
to see how to distinguish them in a minute.
Reproduce el video desde :6:33 y sigue la transcripción6:33
But the third possibility is that some third thing, C, causes both A and B.
If
C causes B, and C also causes A, then A and B are going to be correlated.
There's never C's there, you get both of them, and when C's
not there, unless something else causes them, then they're not going to be there.
So A and B'll be correlated simply because
of the third thing, C is causing them both.
Reproduce el video desde :6:58 y sigue la transcripción6:58
And the fourth possibility is that the correlation is purely accidental.
A and B just happen to change together.
Reproduce el video desde :7:6 y sigue la transcripción7:06
Okay? So here are some examples.
First of all, let's do an example of A causing B, okay?
The speed of driving is positively correlated with
automobile accidents and deaths from automobile accidents, because
when you're going very quickly in your car,
and have an accident, you're more likely to die.
Okay, which causes which?
It's the
speed that causes the accidents and the deaths.
The death didn't cause you to drive faster before you died, that seems pretty clear.
Reproduce el video desde :7:40 y sigue la transcripción7:40
Okay?
And what about the second possibility, B causes A?
Well, it's just the reverse. You can use the same examples.
You can say, automobile accidents and deaths
are correlated with fast driving, with speed.
Reproduce el video desde :7:54 y sigue la transcripción7:54
And in that case, it's not
that the thing that we mentioned first causes the
thing we mentioned second, it's the other way around.
Because as we said before, it's the speed
that causes the automobile accidents and the deaths.
Not the deaths and the accidents that cause the speed.
Reproduce el video desde :8:12 y sigue la transcripción8:12
Okay?
Reproduce el video desde :8:14 y sigue la transcripción8:14
What about the third possibility? That some third thing causes them both.
Reproduce el video desde :8:20 y sigue la transcripción8:20
Well, here's an example.
Reproduce el video desde :8:22 y sigue la transcripción8:22
Having yellowed teeth is correlated with having lung cancer.
Reproduce el video desde :8:28 y sigue la transcripción8:28
But why is that?
Reproduce el video desde :8:30 y sigue la transcripción8:30
because there's some third thing that causes people's teeth to become yellow.
And also causes lung cancer.
Namely, smoking.
Reproduce el video desde :8:37 y sigue la transcripción8:37
So people who smoke tend to have yellower teeth,
and they also tend to have more lung cancer.
So those are correlated because smoking causes both of them,
okay? Another example.
In young children, shoe size is correlated with the quality of handwriting.
Reproduce el video desde :8:58 y sigue la transcripción8:58
As people's shoes get bigger, their handwriting gets better!
Well why is that? It's' because they're maturing.
And as their bodies mature, their feet
get bigger, and also, their handwriting gets better.
Reproduce el video desde :9:14 y sigue la transcripción9:14
And so now we have an example of each of
the first three types of cases that we talked about.
What about the fourth?
Correlation is accidental.
Well, here's an example.
The height of my son, and the height
of the tree outside the window, well, they're correlated.
The taller my son gets, the taller the tree gets.
And the taller the tree gets, the taller my son gets.
They're symmetrical. They're correlated together.
Reproduce el video desde :9:40 y sigue la transcripción9:40
But does that mean that something about the height
of my son causes the height of the tree?
No.
Or vice versa? No.
Well, you might say there's some third cause, namely maturation.
But unlike the case of handwriting and size of feet, where
it was the maturation of the same person, the same body.
Here, my son's body maturing and the tree's body maturing are just unrelated.
You might say that time is the third cause that
makes my son grow and also makes the tree grow.
But notice that time is an abstract thing that's always
there and always moves at the same rate, so it
can't really cause my son to grow at this particular
moment, or the tree to grow at this particular moment.
It's a background condition, rather that a cause in and of itself.
So now we've got four possibilities and we've got examples of each.
The question that we have to face is, fine, all four are possible.
How do you tell which of these possibilities
is the one that applies in a particular case?
That's going to be tricky.
So the problem is, how can we tell which
of the four possibilities applies in a particular case?
And one simple rule is that when A causes B, A has to come before B.
So if the first possibility is instantiated, then
you have to have A coming before B,
but if the second possibility is instantiated, then
you have to have B coming before A.
Reproduce el video desde :11:16 y sigue la transcripción11:16
Now in the last two, you can have them on any temporal relation you want.
But if A comes before B, then you know that B doesn't cause A,
and if B comes before A, then you know that A doesn't cause B.
So for example, exercise is correlated with weight loss.
Could it be that the weight loss causes the exercise?
Well, it's possible, I suppose. But if the exercise occurs before
the weight loss, then we know that it's the exercise that causes
the weight loss, because the exercise occurs before the weight loss occurs.
Reproduce el video desde :11:58 y sigue la transcripción11:58
We can use that temporal relation to decide what causes what.
Reproduce el video desde :12:2 y sigue la transcripción12:02
At least in some cases, because sometimes, when you're dealing with
a constant factor like CO2 variations or pollution variations over a long
period of time, and if pollution causes acid rain, you don't know exactly
which part of the pollution is causing which part of the acid rain.
And the parts are occuring over a long period
of time, so you can't tell which comes first.
But at least in cases where you can, like the exercise
and the weight loss, then you can say which causes which.
Now when they're not in temporal order like
that, you have to use a different method.
Reproduce el video desde :12:41 y sigue la transcripción12:41
The second method of determining what causes what, is manipulation.
This method is used in a lot of scientific experiments where
they manipulate one factor to see whether that factor causes something else.
When you do those types of experiments, you have to make a lot of assumptions,
or test a lot of background conditions
to make sure that there's no independent factor
that's causing the effect.
But in the right circumstances, it can work.
If you want more detail about which circumstances need to be met, then
there're books by Woodward and by Perell that spell this out in great detail.
But here, we're not going to go into any great detail.
Were just going to give you the basic idea.
And the basic idea is that you manipulate A and then look to see
whether B changes according to the change in A, and you manipulate B
and look to see whether A changes in accordance with the change in B.
And that's going to help you determine what causes what, because if when you
manipulate A, B changes, then that's an indication that A causes B.
Because if B caused A, then manipulating
A wouldn't have an effect on B. Think about it this way.
Reproduce el video desde :13:57 y sigue la transcripción13:57
You put the wood into the engine of a steam train.
Reproduce el video desde :14:2 y sigue la transcripción14:02
And the wood affects the motion of the
train, because it affects the way the engine works.
And then the train produces steam.
But the steam doesn't cause the train to move.
It's caused by the train. So if you manipulate the
steam, like with wind blowing the steam
around, that's not going to change the train.
Reproduce el video desde :14:23 y sigue la transcripción14:23
So, similarly, if you change A, and B changes, but you change B
and A doesn't change, then that's a pretty good indication that
A causes B, and that's going to rule out the second one, right?
That can't be true if A causes
B, the third one.
Reproduce el video desde :14:45 y sigue la transcripción14:45
That says that C causes them both, so it's going to rule that out.
And the fourth one, it's not just an accidental correlation if A cause B.
So if you do that manipulation, you can find
out that it really is the first case that holds.
At least if all the other conditions are met.
Now, you can also do it the other way around.
Reproduce el video desde :15:9 y sigue la transcripción15:09
If you manipulate B and A changes, but you manipulate A
and B doesn't change, then you know that B causes A.
Reproduce el video desde :15:22 y sigue la transcripción15:22
But, you also know, that A does not cause B, because if
it did, then when you manipulated A, B would change.
And you know that it's
not just some third cause.
And you know that it's not just accidental.
So now you've isolated the second condition as the one that holds if when
you manipulate B, A changes, but when you manipulate A, B doesn't change.
Again, this only holds when certain circumstances
are in place and I'm not going to
go into detail and spell out what those are, but this is the basic idea
behind a lot of experiments in science. Here's an example.
Reproduce el video desde :16:5 y sigue la transcripción16:05
Long time ago, they discovered that smoking correlated with lung cancer, and
that led a lot of people to say that smoking causes lung cancer.
Reproduce el video desde :16:16 y sigue la transcripción16:16
But just imagine that you're a manufacturer of cigarettes.
What are you going to do? Well they were pretty inventive.
They said,
it's not the smoking that causes the lung
cancer, it's the lung cancer that causes the smoking!
Because when people have certain types of
incipient lung cancer, then this lung cancer
that's about to develop into full-blown lung cancer, it makes them want to smoke.
It creates a discomfort in their lungs that's relieved
by the smoking, and that explains why people who
smoke more, have more lung cancer.
Reproduce el video desde :16:53 y sigue la transcripción16:53
So, we think that A causes B, the smoking causes the lung cancer.
They're claiming that B causes A, the lung cancer causes the smoking.
How do you tell?
Reproduce el video desde :17:6 y sigue la transcripción17:06
Well you can't look at the temporal relation, because the smoking
is going on at the same time when the lung cancer's developing.
So you gotta do a manipulation.
Reproduce el video desde :17:16 y sigue la transcripción17:16
You set it up in a lab.
You take a bunch of animals.
In this case, it was monkeys, who don't have lung cancer,
you check them first, and then you force them to smoke.
You put a cigarette in their mouth so that when they're breathing, they're
smoking.
And sure enough, they developed a lot of lung cancer.
No surprise there, poor animals, and that showed that it was lung
cancer that was caused by smoking, instead of causing smoking.
Why?
Because we didn't manipulate the lung cancer, it didn't affect the smoking.
But we manipulated the smoking, and that affected the lung cancer.
And that shows that it was the
smoking that caused the lung cancer.
Reproduce el video desde :17:59 y sigue la transcripción17:59
Here's another example, which was kind of a mystery for awhile.
An economist friend of mine told me about this.
Turns out, back in the 1960's,there was a positive correlation
between having a television in the home and performance in school.
Students were
more successful in school if they had a television in their house.
Some people actually claimed that television is making them better students.
Reproduce el video desde :18:31 y sigue la transcripción18:31
But in the 90s, it was the other way around.
In the 90s, kids who had televisions in their home did not do
as well on average, as kids who had no televisions in their home.
Reproduce el video desde :18:42 y sigue la transcripción18:42
So there was a negative correlation.
So how could there be a positive correlation
between television and school success in the 1960s, but a negative
correlation between television and school success In the 1990s?
Reproduce el video desde :19:1 y sigue la transcripción19:01
In order to decide between those hypotheses, you can't manipulate it.
You can't go back and change the rate at which
they're watching television and change their school performance, and so on.
You need to have some background information about the societies
of the time and the kind of people who had televisions.
Reproduce el video desde :19:19 y sigue la transcripción19:19
Well back in the 1960s, it was largely the
more affluent, higher socio-economic status people who had television,
because they were quite expensive and not that common.
Reproduce el video desde :19:32 y sigue la transcripción19:32
And those people, typically, are
correlated with high school success anyway.
High socio-economic status and high school success have been correlated.
And so it's that correlation that's explaining why, in the
1960s, students who had televisions in their homes did better
in school.
Reproduce el video desde :19:54 y sigue la transcripción19:54
But in the 1990s, things had turned around.
Parents were worried about their children watching too much television.
And some parents actually kept televisions out of
the home, so that their kids would read more.
They had more books, and less television.
And those kids were doing better in school, no surprise there.
So in the 1990s,
there were a lot of people who kept the televisions out, that
produced greater school performance, and so
you got.different correlations in different times.
And the only way to tell what was causing what, was by looking at
the background circumstances and knowing something about
the societies in which these effects are occurring.

Explica este tema de forma sencilla


Coach
Claro, puedo explicar el tema de forma sencilla. El tema se llama "Cómo razonar
inductivamente". En este curso aprenderás a utilizar el razonamiento inductivo para tomar
decisiones y sacar conclusiones basadas en la evidencia que tienes. Aquí hay algunos
puntos clave para entender el tema:

 El razonamiento inductivo es un tipo de razonamiento que se basa en


observaciones y evidencia para llegar a una conclusión general.
 A diferencia del razonamiento deductivo, que se basa en reglas y premisas para
llegar a una conclusión específica, el razonamiento inductivo se basa en la
probabilidad y la inferencia.
 En el curso, aprenderás sobre diferentes tipos de argumentos inductivos, como el
razonamiento causal y el razonamiento por probabilidad.
 También aprenderás sobre la importancia de las correlaciones en el razonamiento
inductivo y cómo identificarlas.
 Al final del curso, tendrás una mejor comprensión de cómo utilizar el razonamiento
inductivo en tu vida diaria y en la toma de decisiones.

Espero que esta explicación te haya sido útil. Si tienes alguna pregunta específica sobre el
tema, no dudes en hacerla.
Si quieres seguir explorando este tema, prueba alguna de estas preguntas de seguimiento:

You might also like