Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 41

Exploring Consumers’ Purchase Intention toward Green Products in an

Emerging Market: The Role of Consumers’ Perceived Readiness

Author:

Denni Arli*
Department of Marketing
Griffith University, Australia,
Email: d.arli@griffith.edu.au

Lay Peng Tan


Department of Marketing and Management
Macquarie University, Australia,
Email: laypeng.tan@mq.edu.au

Fandy Tjiptono
Department of Marketing
Monash University, Malaysia,
Email: fandy.tjiptono@monash.edu

Lin Yang
Department of Marketing| Tasmanian School of Business & Economics
University of Tasmania
Email:l.yang@utas.edu.au

*Corresponding Author

This article has been accepted for publication and undergone full peer review but has not been
through the copyediting, typesetting, pagination and proofreading process which may lead to
differences between this version and the Version of Record. Please cite this article as an
‘Accepted Article’, doi: 10.1111/ijcs.12432
This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
Abstract

Consumers’ awareness of green products has increased in the last few years, but studies show
that the demand for green products has been stagnant. The purpose of this study is to explore
the roles of consumers’ perceived readiness to be green and subsequently, how readiness to
be green affects consumers’ purchase intention toward green products in an emerging market,
Indonesia. A total of 916 survey responses were collected in three universities, two major
shopping malls and several housing areas in Yogyakarta, Indonesia. The findings reveal that
consumers’ attitude (ATT), subjective norm, perceived behavioral control (PBC), pro-
environmental self-identity (PEI), ethical obligation and consumers’ readiness to be green are
the determinants of intention to purchase green products. Consumers’ readiness to be green
mediates the effects of ATT, PBC, PEI and perceived sense of responsibility on purchase
intention. The study provides further insights into the discrepancy between professed positive
attitudes toward the environment and the slow uptake of green behavior in an emerging
market.

Keywords: Consumer Green Perceptions, Readiness to Be Green, Green Marketing, Green


Attitude–Behavior, Theory of Planned Behavior

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


Introduction

Studies indicate that environmental beliefs have had a significant impact on consumer

behavior (D’Souza et al. 2007; Pereira and Chatzidakis 2012; Line and Hanks 2015; Vitell

and Muncy 2005). A report shows that ethical spending continues to grow, despite difficult

market conditions, with combined ethical money and ethical spending totals rising to US$111

billion in 2014 (Co-operative Group 2015). Consumers are reported to be more and more

willing to purchase green products (Chen 2010). As green products become increasingly

popular, companies are competing to produce products and services that satisfy customers’

environmental needs without damaging the environment (Chen and Chang 2012; McIntosh

1991; Whitson et al. 2014). Such marketing practices are suggested to be related to increased

firm performance and value as well as lower firm-idiosyncratic risk and higher cost-saving

advantages (Luo and Bhattacharya 2006; Menguc and Ozanne 2005; Porter and van der

Linde 1995).

However, an attitude–behavior gap phenomenon has been found consistently in many

studies on green/sustainable consumption (e.g., Fraj and Martinez 2007; Hines et al. 1986;

Kilbourne and Beckmann 2002; Lee and Holden 1999; Moisander 2007; Szmigin et al.

2009). Consumers’ positive attitudes about environmental issues do not necessarily translate

into actual green purchase behavior (Carrington et al. 2010). A survey by Pew Research

Center (2010), for instance, reveals that despite most respondents agreeing that the

environment should be protected, only one-third of the consumers are willing to pay higher

prices to address global climate change. Some of the reasons why consumers decided not to

buy greener products include price, brand image, green product availability, perceived low

performance, cynicism, confusion, trust, lack of environmental knowledge, situational factors

(e.g., economic constraints, lack of choice) and consumers’ internal obstacles (e.g., ethical

standards, sense of responsibility) (e.g., Albayrak et al. 2013; Bray et al. 2011; Chan et al.

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


2008; Gleim et al. 2013; Gupta and Ogden 2009; McDonald et al. 2012; Pagiaslis and

Krontalis 2014; Pickett-Baker and Ozaki 2008; Tanner and Kast 2003). Johnstone and Tan

(2015a) classified these barriers to green behavior into three categories: “it is too hard to be

green,” “the green stigma” (i.e. a mark of disgrace toward green consumers) and “green

reservations,” (i.e., consumers’ ambivalence or uncertainty that greener consumption

practices will make a difference to the environment). Nevertheless, it is important to note that

most of the existing research focused on developed-country contexts. Empirical studies on

the responsible consumption issue in emerging markets are rather limited, although there has

been a growing body of research in China and India in recent years (Chan et al. 2008; He et

al. 2015; Khare 2015; Nittala 2014). The context of this study is Indonesia, one of the largest

emerging markets, with a population of about 256 million people (Population Reference

Bureau 2015). Indonesia offers a unique perspective as it is currently being deforested faster

than any other country in the world (Oakford 2014). Indonesia is the fourth most populous

nation in the world with the Gross Domestic Per capita of US3,475.25 (Population Reference

Bureau 2015). Despite significant economic growth, due to lack of strong policy, Indonesia

suffers from environmental degradation (Measey 2010). Moreover, consumers’ attention and

support toward green product and services remains unclear. Thus, the current study aims to

focus on Indonesia as an emerging market, and explore how consumers’ attitude toward the

environment, perceived behavioral control, subjective norm, pro-environmental self-identity,

perceived sense of responsibility and ethical obligation influence their readiness to be green,

and subsequently how perceived readiness to be green affects consumers’ purchase intention

toward green products.

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


Literature Review

Theory of Planned Behavior

The present study uses the Theory of Planned Behavior (Ajzen 1985, 1991, 2005), which is

an extension of the Theory of Reasoned Action (TRA) developed by Fishbein and Ajzen

(1975). This well-known theory links attitude toward the behavior (i.e., the degree to which

an individual evaluates the particular behavior favorably or unfavorably), subjective norm

(i.e., perceived social pressure to perform the particular behavior) and perceived behavioral

control (i.e., perceived ease or difficulty of performing the particular behavior) to behavioral

intention.

Subsequently, research has supported the inclusion of further predictive variables to

the Theory of Planned Behavior, such as self-identity and ethical obligation (Shaw et al.

2000). In the last few decades, Theory of Planned Behavior has been used to predict various

behaviors, including physical activity (Armitage and Conner 2001; Conner and Sparks 2005;

Godin and Kok 1996), fat intake (de Bruijn et al. 2008; Paisley and Sparks, 1998); fruit and

vegetable consumption (Bogers et al. 2004), healthy eating behavior (Conner et al. 1991;

Povey et al. 2000), academic integrity behavior (Cronan et al. 2015) and weight control

(Netemeyer et al. 1991). Building on Theory of Planned Behavior, the current study attempts

to test new variables, namely consumers’ perceived sense of responsibility and consumers’

perceived readiness to be green as an effort to explore the gap between attitude and behavior

in the context of green marketing. We use the terms “environmentally friendly products” or

“green products” interchangeably throughout the paper. We define green products as

“products that consumers perceive to be environmentally friendly, whether it is due to the

types of materials used, the production process, packaging, promotion, and so on” (Johnstone

and Tan, 2015a, p.312). “To be green” is defined as engaging in environmentally friendly

activities, including purchasing/using green products (Polonsky 2011).

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


Green Attitude and Behavior

As widely posited in social psychology literatures, attitudes guide human behavior (Bredahl

2001). Multi-attribute models such as the Theory of Reasoned Action (Fishbein and Ajzen

1975) and Theory of Planned Behavior (Ajzen 1985, 1991, 2005) have theorized that

attitudes affect people’s intentions to perform behavior, and intentions subsequently impact

behavior (Petty et al. 1991). However, consumers’ uptake of green products has not kept pace

with their positive attitudes about the environment and their growing environmental

consciousness (e.g., Eurobarometer 2011; Finisterra do Paço et al. 2010; Pew Research

Center 2010). This attitude–behavior discrepancy is often referred to as “the green gap” in the

literature. Over the past few decades, numerous theoretical frameworks have been developed

to explain the green gap but no definitive explanation has yet been found (Kollmuss and

Agyeman 2002; He et al. 2015). In an exploratory study by Johnstone and Tan (2015a), the

authors identified several barriers to green consumption behavior. In particular, they suggest

that even though consumers may have pro-environmental attitudes, their perceptions toward

“being green” may influence their perceived readiness, and thus intention to engage in green

consumption behavior. Social norms, or “behaviors we perceive to be morally right or what

ought to be done”, can play a large role in influencing green consumption practices (Peattie

2010, p. 211). Evidently, “being green” is not yet perceived as a social norm in most

countries, in particular emerging markets such as Indonesia. When green social norms are not

strong enough, consumers will probably experience minimal to no dissonance if there is a

discrepancy between their attitudes and behavior. As such, consumers’ attitude toward the

environment might be inadequate to predict their behavior. The next section will discuss the

hypothesis development of this study.

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


Hypothesis Development

Attitude

Attitude refers to the degree to which an individual has a favorable or unfavorable evaluation

of the behavior in question (Ajzen 1985). In general, attitude is a significant factor

influencing consumer behaviors such as: adoption of internet banking (Lee et al. 2009), halal

food purchasing (Shar and Sayuti 2011) and exercise (Rhodes and Courneya 2003). In regard

to green products, studies show that consumers’ purchasing decisions are also being

influenced by their attitude (e.g., Ha and Janda 2012; Irland 1993; Schwepker and Cornwell

1991; Tanner and Kast 2003; Zhou et al. 2013). Hansla et al. (2008) found that consumers’

willingness to pay for green electricity increased with a positive attitude toward green

electricity. The same was found for environmentally-friendly wines (Barber et al. 2010). In

the green-hotel context, many studies determined that intention is positively influenced by

attitude (e.g., Chen and Tung 2014; Han and Yoon 2015). In a nutshell, the literature posits

that the more favorable the attitude toward a behavior, the stronger the individuals’ intention

to perform the behavior. Thus, it is hypothesized that:

H1a: Attitude toward green products has a direct, positive influence on consumers’
perceived readiness to be green.
H1b: Attitude toward green products has a direct, positive influence on purchase intention
toward green products.

Subjective Norm

Derived from the Theory of Reasoned Action (Ajzen and Fishbein 1980; Fishbein and Ajzen

1975), subjective norm refers to the belief about whether most people approve or disapprove

of the particular behavior (Ajzen 1991). Subjective norm recognizes the importance of

reference groups and it is also interested in finding out to what degree an individual will be

motivated to comply with these groups. Studies found that subjective norm significantly

influences various green-related consumer behavioural intention, such as intention to buy

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


environmentally friendly products (Kalafatis et al. 2006; Wahid et al. 2011) and buying

organic foods and organic personal care products (Smith and Paladino, 2010; Tarkiainen and

Sundqvist 2005; Yeon and Chung 2005). It has also been found that consumers’ choice of

green products is influenced by their reference group (Welsch and Kühling 2009). Subjective

norm will influence consumers’ perceived readiness to be green and their purchase intention

toward green products as peers or people of importance often influence an individual’s intent

to act in a certain way. People who perceive a higher social pressure from significant others

will perceive themselves to be more ready to be green, and in turn will have a greater

tendency to purchase green products. Hence, we hypothesize:

H2a: Subjective norm has a direct, positive influence on consumers’ perceived readiness to
be green.
H2b: Subjective norm has a direct, positive influence on purchase intention toward green
products.

Perceived Behavioral Control

Perceived behavioral control refers to the perceived difficulty or ease of performing the

behavior (Ajzen 1985). Perceived behavioral control has been shown to influence intention

in various contexts, such as digital piracy (Chang 1998; Chen et al. 2009; Cronan and Al-

Rafee 2008), sun-protective behaviors (Banon, 2013), taxpayer acceptance of E-tax payment

system (Keramati et al. 2012), problem drinking (Schelegel et al. 1992), leisure activities

(Ajzen and Driver 1992) etc. The effect of perceived behavioral control on pro-environmental

behavior has also been found significant in energy conservation (Albayrak et al. 2013) and

the choice of green electricity products (Bamberg 2003). Ajzen (1988) suggests that

perceived behavioral control should predict behavioral intention. In the context of green

product consumption, perceived behavioral control will have positive influence on

individuals’ readiness to be green and purchase intention toward green products. Thus, we

hypothesize that:

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


H3a: Perceived behavioral control has a direct, positive influence on consumers’ perceived
readiness to be green.
H3b: Perceived behavioral control has a direct, positive influence on purchase intention
toward green products.

Pro-Environmental Self-Identity

One of the key predictors of behavior is self-identity (Sparks and Shepherd 1992). This can

be described as the label used to describe oneself (Cook et al. 2002). Self-identity serves both

as a means to differentiate oneself from others and to conform to the values, beliefs and

behavior of the social groups to which one belongs (Christensen et al. 2004; Whitmarsh and

O’Neill 2010). It has long been established that the self-expressive nature of consumption is

important for consumers to construct and preserve their identities (e.g., see Belk, 2010;

Thorbjornsen et al., 2007). Some researchers go beyond and state that identity may even

override attitude in cases where our role identity dictates the way we behave, irrespective of

how we feel about that behavior (Charng, Piliavin & Callero, 1988). Environmental identity

significantly influences a person’s self-identity. It includes self-meanings that are tied to an

individual instead of being attached to a particular role (Stets 1995; Stets and Biga 2003).

Pro-environmental self-identity has been found to predict environmental behavior, such as

recycling (White & Hyde, 2012), carbon offsetting, waste reduction and energy conservation

(Whitmarsh & O’Neill, 2010). Hence, consumers who consider themselves to be pro-

environment are more likely to be ready and more likely to purchase green products. Thus,

we propose:

H4a: Pro-environmental self-identity has a direct, positive influence on consumers’


perceived readiness to be green.
H4b: Pro-environmental self-identity has a direct, positive influence on purchase intention
toward green products.

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


Perceived Sense of Responsibility

Consumers in different countries and cultures behave differently according to the acceptable

practices and rules in those cultures (Ji et al. 2000; Kalafatis et al. 1999; Kan et al. 2014).

When environmental issues are considered as a major concern in a country, consumers may

have a higher sense of responsibility toward the environment (Quazi et al. 2015; Quazi et al.

2016). A study conducted by Chan and Lau (2001), for instance, found that consumer

behavioral intention toward green products is higher among American consumers than

Chinese consumers. They suggested that Chinese consumers tend to have a weaker sense of

responsibility toward environmental sustainability than their western counterparts. In this

study, we argue that perceived sense of responsibility intertwines but differs from ethical

obligation in that the former addresses “what an individual perceives as their responsibility to

improve a situation”. It captures first an individual’s acknowledgement of “situation of

concern and/or consequences”, and then, their “sense of responsibility” of doing something

toward it. It is an attributed responsibility of righting the wrongs. On the other hand, ethical

obligations address an individual’s internalized ethical rules, which reflect their belief about

appropriate behavior (Kan et al. 2014; Shaw et al. 2000), i.e., “what is the right thing to do”.

Therefore, consumers who perceive stronger responsibility for environmental deterioration

may be more ready to adopt a green behavior and are more likely to purchase green products

(Johnstone and Tan 2015a). Hence, we propose the following hypotheses:

H5a: Perceived sense of responsibility has a direct, positive influence on consumers’


perceived readiness to be green.
H5b: Perceived sense of responsibility has a direct, positive influence on purchase intention
toward green products.

Ethical Obligation

In the context of consumer behavior, ethical obligation is an important driver, in part driven

by a sense of obligation with ethical issues (Cook et al. 2002; Shaw and Shui 2002). Studies

10

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


found strong support for the impact of ethical obligation on behavioral intention (e.g., Valle,

et al, 2005, Nordlund and Garvill, 2002). Ethical consumers do hold stronger ethical

obligation than others that influence their purchase choices (Shaw and Clarke 1999).

Consumers with higher ethical obligation tend to support environmentally friendly behavior

such as recycling and purchasing green products (Arli et al. 2016; Shaw et al. 2000).

Consequently, we hypothesize that ethical obligation will influence consumers’ perceived

readiness to be green and intention to purchase green products. Hence:

H6a: Ethical obligation has a direct, positive influence on consumers’ perceived readiness
to be green.
H6b: Ethical obligation has a direct, positive influence on purchase intention toward green
products.

Consumers’ Perceived Readiness to be Green

We conceptualize consumers’ perceived readiness to be green as a condition in which

consumers perceive themselves as “ready” to engage in green consumption behavior, such as

buying green products. As Haller and Handler (2008) posits, to change ones’ attitude and

behavior to be more compatible with the environment, “individuals must engage in some

personal sacrifices and even major changes in lifestyle” (p.282). The concept of “readiness”

is well established in the behavior-change literature, underpinned by cognitive-behavior

theories, including the Theory of Planned Behavior (Ajzen 1985, 1991, 2005) and Theory of

Triadic Influence (e.g., Snyder and Flay 2012). In particular, an individual’s readiness to

change is often posited as a proximal predictor of behavior change, for example to quit

smoking (Kleinjan et al. 2009), or the likelihood to adopt new technology, like self-service

technologies in hospitality settings (Kim et al. 2012). In this study, we argue that in countries

where “being green” is not yet perceived as a social norm, engaging in green consumption

behavior is parallel to “behavior change”. Beyond the business discipline and within the

context of nursing, readiness is a ubiquitous term associated with a patient’s willingness to

11

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


change or to comply with a medical regime (Dalton and Gottlieb 2003). As Dalton and

Gottlieb exert, “…before clients can create change, they need to become ready to change”.

The authors went on to propose that, “the process of readiness involves recognizing the need

to change, weighing the costs and benefits, and when benefits outweigh costs, planning for

change” (p.108). Readiness can also be conceptualized as “a state where a person is assessed

as ready or not ready” (Dalton and Gottlieb 2003). We postulate that this state of mind

facilitates and expedites the formation of tendency or intention to act. As such, we argue that

consumers’ perceived readiness to be green would be an immediate precursor to their

intention of “being green.” Hence, we propose that:

H7: Consumers’ perceived readiness to be green has a direct, positive influence on


purchase intention toward green products.

Furthermore, consumers’ readiness to be green will mediate the relationship between

the antecedents and consumers’ intention to purchase green products (Johnstone and Tan

2015a). Therefore, we hypothesize that:

H8: Consumers’ readiness to be green will mediate the relationship between: (a) Attitude;
(b) Subjective Norm; (c) Perceived Behavioral Control; (d) Pro-environmental Self-
identity ; (e) Ethical Obligation; (f) Perceived Sense of Responsibility and purchase
intention toward green products.

Methodology

Data Collection

We collected data in Indonesia, the world’s 17th-largest economy and the sixth largest among

developing countries (Fletcher 2012). Data were derived using a convenience sampling of

student and nonstudent populations. Student samples were approached in classrooms and

public spaces of three universities (one public and two major private universities) in

Yogyakarta, Indonesia. Nonstudent samples were interviewed in two shopping malls and

several housing areas in the city. A total of 1,000 questionnaires were distributed, 970

12

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


returned and only 916 were usable, thereby offering an overall response rate of 92%. About

56% of the samples were students, while 44% of them were non-students. Male and female

respondents were 48.3% and 51.7%, respectively. A majority of the respondents was between

the ages of 18 and 20 years (38.3%) followed by 21–23 years (17.6%), 24–26 years (13.5%)

and aged above 26 years old (30.6%). In terms of income, most respondents earned less than

20 million rupiah annually (±US$1,493, where US$ 1 = Rp. 13,394 as of 10 March 2017).

This income distribution reflects the condition of Indonesia as a low income country (Credit

Suisse, 2016). Despite the Statistics Indonesia (BPS) does not provide the Indonesian

population income distribution data (Pulungan, 2017), it was estimated that about 82% of the

Indonesia’s population had income of less than US$ 4 per day (or US$120 per month) (World

Bank, cited in Sharpe, 2014) and almost 100 million (± 39%) Indonesians earned less than

US$2 per day (The Economist, 2011). Finally, most of the respondents were Muslim

(47.4%), followed by Christians (25.1%), Catholics (20.9%) and others (6.6%). About 74.2%

of the respondents reported that they had previously purchased household products that were

promoted as environmentally friendly. In the questionnaire, we provided four household

products, i.e., soaps, toilet paper rolls, laundry detergents and dishwashing liquids. These

products are commonly available to consumers and easy to understand, unlike for example

biofuel or green cars. Additionally, the four products are rather homogenous (cleaning). This

would avoid potential, unnecessary additional source of variation within the dataset as

compared with studies examining diverse pro-environmental behaviours in one study (e.g.,

recycling, water reduction, eco-shopping, energy conservation, eco-driving, etc.) (Whitmarsh

& O’Neill, 2010). The respondents were instructed to choose the product category that they

were most familiar with because product knowledge may influence consumers’ product

evaluations (Blair & Innis, 1996) and perceptions (Laroche, Bergeron, & Goutaland, 2003).

13

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


Soaps and laundry detergents were mentioned as the most purchased green products during

the last six months. Table 1 summarizes the respondent profiles.

Insert Table 1 about Here

Measures

Ajzen’s (1991) measures were used for “attitude” (single item), “subjective norm” (three

items) and “perceived behavioral control” (single item). We adopted the scales by Cook et al.

(2002) to measure “pro-environmental self-identity” (three items) and “ethical obligation”

(four items). Consumers’ perceived sense of responsibility (two items–reverse coded), and

consumers’ perceived readiness to be green (three items–reverse coded) were measured using

consumers’ green perception scales developed by Johnstone et al. (2014). These factors

display good internal consistency with alpha greater than .70 (Hair et al. 2010; Nunnally

1978). The scale items used in this study are presented in Table 2. Structural equation

modeling was used to address the research objectives and Likert scales were employed

(1=strongly disagree; 5= strongly agree).

Analysis

Using structural equation modeling (SEM), the analysis followed Andersen and Gerbing’s

(1988) two-stage procedure. First, the goodness of the measurement instruments was

analyzed using confirmatory factor analysis and second, the structural relations among the

theoretically proposed latent variables were analyzed through SEM. Both the measurement

model and the causal-relations model were estimated using maximum likelihood methods

(Satorra and Bentler 1994) for the model fit. The goodness-of-fit indicators for the

measurement instrument (Bentler–Bonett Nonnormed Fit Index (BBNFI)) = .941,

comparative fit index (CFI) = .958, incremental fit index (IFI) = .959, root mean square error

14

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


of approximation (RMSEA) = .049, easily exceed the commonly accepted critical values

(Andersen and Gerbing 1988). Table 2 shows that the measurement instrument reliability can

be confirmed as Cronbach’s alphas exceed the critical value of .7 (Cronbach 1951) and

composite reliability and average variance extracted indexes also exceed the critical values of

.7 and .5, respectively (Fornell and Larcker 1981). In addition to the goodness-of-fit

indicators, two criteria were used to verify convergent validity. The results show that the

observed variables were significant and the average loads were above .7 (Hair et al. 2010).

Insert Table 2 about Here

Finally, the measurement model was checked to ensure discriminant validity. First, it

was found that interfactor correlations were significantly below 1. Second, for each pair of

factors, it was verified that the difference of 2 between the proposed measurement model

and a restricted model where the correlation between said factors was set at 1 (Fornell and

Larcker 1981) was significant. The variance extracted test was carried out, which showed that

Average Variance Extracted for each factor was higher than the square of the correlation

coefficient with each of the other factors (see Table 3). Thus, in Table 3, the test confirms the

measurement model’s discriminant validity (Fornell and Larcker 1981).

Insert Table 3 about Here

Results

Direct Effect

Having established the reliability and validity of the model, in the next step we estimate the

hypothesized relationships between the constructs of the present study. The analysis was

conducted to examine the impact of attitude, subjective norm, perceived behavioral control,

pro-environmental self-identity, perceived sense of responsibility, and ethical obligation on

consumers’ perceived readiness to be green and purchase intention toward green products.

15

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


Subsequently, we examine the impact of consumers’ readiness to be green on purchase

intention. The findings presented in Table 4 suggest that the hypothesized structural model

(BBNFI = .948; Tucker–Lewis Index (TLI) = .952; CFI = .965; IFI = .965; RMSEA = .046;

2/df = 361.941/124) fits the data well.

The results show that attitude toward green products significantly influences

consumers’ readiness to be green (β = .090, p < .05) and purchase intention (β = .190, p <

.01). Hence, H1a and H1b are supported. Moreover, subjective norm significantly influences

purchase intention toward green products (β = .228, p < .01) but not readiness to be green.

Thus H2b is supported while H2a is not. Perceived behavioral control positively influences

both consumers’ readiness to be green (β = .076, p < .05) and purchase intention (β = .138, p

< .01). Therefore, H3a and H3b are supported. Pro-environmental self-identity positively

influences both readiness to be green (β = .329, p < .01) and purchase intention (β = .260, p <

.01). Hence, H4a and H4b are supported. Additionally, perceived sense of responsibility

positively affects consumers’ readiness to be green (β = .340, p < .01). However, the effect of

perceived sense of responsibility on purchase intention was negative (β = –.082, p < .05). As

a result, H5a is supported while H5b is not. In regard to ethical obligation, the results indicated

that ethical obligation positively influences purchase intention (β = .090, p < .05) but not

consumers’ readiness to be green. Consequently, H6b is supported while H6a is not. Finally,

consumers’ readiness to be green positively influences consumers’ intention to purchase

green products (β = .160, p < .01). Consumers who are ready to be green are more likely to

purchase green products. Hence, H7 is supported.

Insert Table 4 about Here

Mediating Effects

To test for mediation effects, two separate bias-corrected bootstrap analyses with the

mediator (perceived readiness to be green) were conducted using a series of multiple

16

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


regression analyses (Baron and Kenny 1986; Preacher 2008; Zhao et al. 2010). Meaningful

mediating effects can occur without a significant direct relationship between independent and

dependent variables (Zhao et al. 2010). Thus, only direct effects between the mediator and

independent variables and between the mediator and dependent variables were established

prior to assessment of mediation. A mediating effect is significant (p < .05) if the 95%

confidence intervals do not include zero. This test establishes whether statistically significant

results exist for mediation by perceived readiness to be green of the relationship between

attitude, subjective norm, perceived behavioral control, pro-environmental self-identity,

perceived sense of responsibility, ethical obligation and intention to purchase green products.

Finally, following Podsakoff et al. (2003) we adopted statistical methods to assess common-

method bias. We applied a marker variable (MV) to assess whether our measure suffered

from common-method bias. We found that common-method bias was not an issue in this

study.

The results show that readiness to be green mediated the relationship between attitude

and purchase intention; perceived behavioral control and purchase intention; pro-

environmental self-identity and purchase intention; and perceived sense of responsibility and

purchase intention. Thus, H8a, H8c, H8d and H8f are supported. However, readiness to be green

did not mediate the relationship between subjective norms and purchase intention and the

relationship between ethical obligation and purchase intention. Therefore, H8b and H8e are not

supported. Table 5 summarizes the hypothesis-testing results of the mediating effects.

Insert Table 5 about Here

Discussions and Implications

From a theoretical perspective, the results of the present study provide further empirical

support for the usefulness of the Theory of Planned Behavior as a guiding framework to

17

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


understand how to influence green products purchase intention in the context of an emerging

market. In addition, the present study highlights the role of consumers’ perceived readiness to

be green as an important determinant of green product purchase intention. When consumers

think that they are ready to be green (e.g., have the ability in terms of knowledge and time),

they are more likely to purchase green products (Johnstone et al., 2014). This finding is

consistent with behavior-change literature in that “readiness” is a proximal predictor for

behavior change. Readiness to be green also mediates the impacts of attitude, perceived

behavioral control, pro-environmental self-identity, and perceived sense of responsibility on

purchase intention. Our findings suggest that positive attitudes toward buying a green product

may not translate into green purchase intention if consumers do not think that they are ready

to be green. This may in part help to explain the subsequent discrepancy between attitude and

behavior. These initial findings highlight the potential of consumers’ “green readiness” to

better understand the green gap.

The current study reveals that green products purchase intention is being influenced

by multiple factors. First, we will discuss antecedents of consumers’ readiness to be green. It

shows that people who have stronger attitude, perceived behavioral control, self-identity

toward the environment and perceived sense of responsibility are more likely to be ready to

be green. Meanwhile, subjective norm and ethical obligation were found to be insignificant

predictors of consumer readiness to be green. The findings suggest that perceived readiness to

be green comes from the “internal” commitment of those consumers (e.g., identity, sense of

responsibility) and not from other external influences (i.e., families and obligation). That is,

to increase consumers’ readiness to be green and subsequently green product purchase

intention, marketers/policy-makers should focus on those “internal drivers.” For example,

heightening the sense of responsibility toward the environment, and reinforcing pro-

environmental self-identity through education and training. However, marketers/policy-

18

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


makers should be cautioned that consumers’ pro-environmental behaviors do not necessarily

increase when they are provided with very detailed technical information (Kollmuss and

Agyeman 2002). In fact, as Chan (2001) found, intuitive and emotional factors can exert a

greater influence on attitudes toward green purchases. In the Indonesian context, a more

integrative and comprehensive program will be needed. This could include using joint

industry, government and nongovernmental organizations’ action to drive home the message

that the environment is a major concern in the country, which requires immediate actions.

Building on the findings of this study, the focus on these joint actions could also be on

reinforcing the belief that “individuals” can and have the responsibility to make a difference,

and that all individuals have the responsibility to protect the environment for future

generations. Furthermore, it is necessary to encourage consumers that their small acts toward

sustainability do matter to the environment. Recently, the Indonesian Retailers Association

(Aprindo) decided to support the Indonesian government’s policy to reduce plastic waste.

Since 21 February 2016, customers have had to pay for plastic bags when they shop at

modern markets, such as supermarkets, department stores, minimarkets and other retail

outlets throughout 23 major cities in Indonesia (Jong, 2016).

The findings also provided empirical support for the positive impacts of attitude,

subjective norm, perceived behavioral control, pro-environmental self-identity and ethical

obligation on purchase intention of environmentally friendly products. Among the significant

predictors, pro-environmental self-identity and subjective norm are the strongest. The

findings again highlight the importance of building/reinforcing pro-environmental self-

identity amongst consumers. In addition, although subjective norm was not found to affect

consumers’ green readiness significantly, it was a significant predictor for consumers’ green

product purchase intention. These results suggest that consumers would be likely to purchase

green products when they believe that such behavior will gain the approval of people that are

19

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


important to them, regardless of whether they are ready to be green. This illustrates the

importance of using “social approval” to drive greater green uptake. On the other hand, an

interesting research question could be, “Will the behavior of these consumers who purchase

green products to ‘gain social approval’ persist, when the external influence is absent?”

(Johnstone and Tan 2015b).

Surprisingly, perceived sense of responsibility was found to influence consumers’

purchase intention toward green products negatively. When consumers felt responsible for

environmental deterioration, they were less likely to buy green products. This finding is

counterintuitive. However, readiness to be green mediates the effect of perceived sense of

responsibility on purchase intention. This may further indicate the importance for a highly

responsible consumer to be ready before he/she is willing to buy a green product. More

empirical studies would be needed to delineate further the roles of “consumers’ readiness” in

pursuing green/sustainable consumption behavior.

Limitation and Future Research

Our study has several limitations that also offer opportunities for future research. First, our

respondents are mainly younger consumers and earning lower income compared to the

general population. Future research should explore consumers from various age cohorts to

increase the generalizability of the study. Secondly, the current study found that readiness to

be green and purchase intention is not significantly different across religions. Further study

may explore the role of religiosity instead of religion in examining green attitude and

behavioral intentions (Martin and Bateman 2014). Several previous studies have indicated

that religiousness influences consumer attitude, which subsequently affects purchasing

behavior (Essoo and Dibb 2004) including consumer ethical behavior (Arli and Tjiptono 2014;

Vitell et al. 2005). Moreover, although some interesting findings are drawn from a

20

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


combination of student and nonstudent samples, it is important to note some caveats in

interpreting the results. The research context (one city in one country) may limit its

generalizability. Future study may focus on more cities and/or cross-country comparisons.

This study did not look at the impact of green products’ price and quality on consumers’

readiness and purchase intention. Through a series of experiments, future studies may

examine the impact of price and quality on consumers’ purchase intention between high and

low environmentally conscious consumers.

Another limitation of this study is the construct perceived behavioral control used in

this study. This construct may represent two dimensions (i.e. controllability and self-efficacy)

(Ajzen 2002).1 Hence, the results of this study may be interpreted in light of this issue. This

result, however, is consistent with Bamberg’s study which also investigated specific

environmentally related behaviour, with unsatisfactory factor loading of the item capturing

controllability (λ=0.43) (Bamberg, 2003). Future research in the similar context may be

required to examine and validate the dimensions of this construct. Some consumers may

have more control and self-efficacy than other consumers which subsequently affect their

attitude and purchase intention.

In this study, we adopted the suggestions by several scholars to include ethical

obligation to the Theory of Planned Behaviour (e.g., Cook et al., 2002; Shaw and Shui, 2002

and Chatzidakis, Hibbert, and Smith, 2007) and further distinguished the difference between

ethical obligation and perceived sense of responsibility. We acknowledge that the constructs

of ethical obligations, perceived sense of responsibility to some extent overlap with the

prevailing “moral norm” used in many other studies2. Future studies could attempt to

disentangle the definition of moral norms and further delineate the relationships / differences

amongst these constructs.

1
We thank an anonymous reviewer for this feedback.
2
We thank an anonymous reviewer for this feedback.

21

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


The result of this study may only be applicable to a particular product, for example

household products. Future research should test various products such as organic food,

durable products and appliances3. Finally, this study did not examine consumers’ revealed or

actual purchase behavior as such data is scarce to the academic community. Future research

may include consumers’ purchase behavior which can be matched clearly with their intention

to purchase green products.

3
We thank an anonymous reviewer for this feedback.

22

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


References

Agarwal, B. (2000). Conceptualizing environmental collective action: Why gender matters.


Cambridge Journal of Economics, 24(3), 283-310.

Albayrak, T., Aksoy, S., and Caber, M. (2013). The effect of environmental concern and
scepticism on green purchase behaviour. Marketing Intelligence and Planning, 21(1), 27-39.

Ajzen, I. (1985). From intentions to actions: A theory of planned behavior. In J. Kuhl and J.
Beckman (eds.), Action Control: From Cognitions to Behavior, (pp. 11–39). Berlin: Springer-
Verlag.

Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human
Decision Processes, 50(2), 179-211.

Ajzen, I. (2002) Perceived behavioral control, self-efficacy, locus of control and the theory of
planned behavior. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 32(4), 665-683.)

Ajzen, I. (2005). Attitudes, Personality, and Behavior, 2nd ed. New York: Open University
Press.

Ajzen, I. and Fishbein, M. (1980). Understanding Attitude and Predicting Social Behavior.
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Ajzen, I. (1988). Attitudes, Personality and Behaviour, Open University Press, Milton
Keynes

Ajzen, I., & Driver, B. L. (1992). Application of the theory of planned behavior to leisure
choice. Journal of Leisure Research, 24(3), 207.

Arli, D. and Tjiptono, F. (2014). The end of religion? Examining the role of religiousness,
materialism, and long-term orientation on consumer ethics in Indonesia. Journal of Business
Ethics, 123(3), 385-400.

Arli,D., Leo, C. and Tjiptono, F. (2016). Investigating the impact of guilt and shame
proneness on consumer ethics: A cross national study. International Journal of Consumer
Studies, 40(1), 2-13.

Armitage, C. J. and Conner, M. (2001). Efficacy of the theory of planned behaviour: A meta‐
analytic review. British Journal of Social Psychology, 40(4), 471-499.

Bandura, A. (1991). Social cognitive theory of self-regulation. Organizational Behavior and


Human Decision Processes, 50(2), 248-287.

Bannon, K. A. (2013). Exploring Attitudes, Subjective Norms, and Perceived Behavioral


Control of Sun-Protective Behaviors in Intercollegiate Student Athletes (Doctoral
dissertation, Emory University).

Barber, N. Taylor, D.C., and Deale, C.S. (2010). Wine tourism, environmental concerns, and
purchase intention. Journal of Travel and Tourism Marketing, 27(2), 146-165.

23

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


Baron, R.M., and Kenny, D.A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social
psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 51(6), 1173-82.

Bogers, R. P., Van Assema, P., Kester, A. D., Westerterp, K. R., and Dagnelie, P. C. (2004).
Reproducibility, validity, and responsiveness to change of a short questionnaire for
measuring fruit and vegetable intake. American Journal of Epidemiology, 159(9), 900-909.

Bonini, S. and Oppenheim, J. (2008). Cultivating the green consumer. Stanford Social
Innovation Review, 6 (4)56–61.

Borin, N., Lindsey-Mullikin, J., and Krishnan, R. (2013). An analysis of consumer reactions
to green strategies. Journal of Product and Brand Management, 22(2), 118-128.

Bray, J., Johns, N., and Kilburn, D. (2011). An exploratory study in the factors impeding
ethical consumption. Journal of Business Ethics, 98, 597-608.

Bredahl, L. (2001). Determinants of consumer attitudes and purchase intentions with regard
to genetically modified food—Results of a cross-national survey. Journal of Consumer
Policy, 24(1), 23–61.

Bridges, E. (1993). Service attributes: Expectation and judgments. Psychology and


Marketing, 10(3), 185-197.

Carrington, M., Neville, B., and Whitwell, G. (2010). Why ethical consumers don’t walk
their talk: Towards a framework for understanding the gap between the ethical purchase
intentions and actual buying behaviour of ethically minded consumers. Journal of Business
Ethics, 97(1),139-158.

Chan, R. Y. K. (2001). Determinants of Chinese consumers' green purchase behavior.


Psychology & Marketing, 18(4), 389-413.

Chan, R., Wong, Y., and Leung, T. (2008). Applying ethical concepts to the study of ‘green’
consumer behavior: An analysis of Chinese consumers’ intentions to bring their own
shopping bags. Journal of Business Ethics, 79(4), 469-481.

Chan, R. Y., and Lau, L. B. (2002). Explaining green purchasing behavior: A cross-cultural
study on American and Chinese consumers. Journal of International Consumer
Marketing, 14(2-3), 9-40.

Chang, M.K. (1998). Predicting Unethical Behavior: A Comparison of the Theory of


Reasoned Action and the Theory of Planned Behavior. Journal of Business Ethics, 17(16),
1825-1834.

Chatzidakis, A., Hibbert, S., & Smith, A. P. (2007). Why people don’t take their concerns
about fair trade to the supermarket: The role of neutralisation. Journal of Business Ethics,
74(1), 89-100.

24

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


Chen, Y.-S. (2010). The drivers of green brand equity: green brand image, green satisfaction,
and green trust. Journal of Business Ethics, 93(2), 307-19.

Chen, Y.S. and Chang, C.H. (2012). Enhance green purchase intentions: The roles of
green perceived value, green perceived risk, and green trust. Management Decision,
50(3), 502-520.

Chen, M.-F. and Tung, P.-J. (2014). Developing an extended theory of planned behaviour
model to predict consumers’ intention to visit green hotels. International Journal of
Hospitality Management, 36(1), 221-230.
Christensen, N., Rothberger, H., Wood, W., and Matz, D. (2004). Social norms and identity
relevance: a motivational approach to normative behaviour. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 30(10), 1295-1309.
Conner, M., Gaston, G., Sheeran, P., and Germain, M. (2013). Some feelings are more
important: Cognitive attitudes, affective attitudes, anticipated affect, and blood donation.
Health Psychology, 32, 264–272.

Cook, A. J., Kerr, G. N., and Moore, K. (2002). Attitudes and intentions towards purchasing
GM food. Journal of Economic Psychology, 23(5), 557-572.
Cooperative Group (2015). Ethical consumer markets report 2015. Accessed 20 February
2016. Available at http://www.ethicalconsumer.org/researchhub/ukethicalmarket.aspx.

Cornwell, B., Chi Cui, C., Mitchell, V., Schlegelmilch, B., Dzulkiflee, A., & Chan, J. (2005).
A cross-cultural study of the role of religion in consumers' ethical positions. International
Marketing Review, 22(5), 531-546.

Credit Suisse (2016). Global wealth databook 2016. Accessed 10 March 2017. Available at
http://publications.credit-suisse.com/tasks/render/file/index.cfm?fileid=AD6F2B43-B17B-
345E-E20A1A254A3E24A5

Cronan, T.P. and Al-Rafee, S. (2008). Factors that influence the intention to pirate software
and media. Journal of Business Ethics, 78(4), 527–545.

Cronan, T.P., Mullins, J.K., and Douglas, D.E.(2015). Further understanding factors that
explain freshman business students’ academic integrity intention and behavior: Plagiarism
and sharing homework. Journal of Business Ethics. (In press), DOI:10.1007/s10551-015-
2988-3.

Cronbach, L.J. (1951). Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of tests. Psychometrika,
16(3), 297-334.

Cui, J., Jo. H. and Velasquez, M.G. (2014). The influence of Christian religiosity on
managerial decisions concerning the environment. Journal of Business Ethics. 132(1), 203-
231.

Dalton, C. C., & Gottlieb, L. N. (2003). The concept of readiness to change. Journal of
Advanced Nursing, 42(2), 108-117.

25

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


d’Astous, A. and Montpetit, D. (2005). Music piracy on the web: How effective are anti-
piracy arguments? Evidence from the theory of planned behaviour. Journal of Consumer
Policy, 28(3), 289–310.

de Bruijn, G. J., Kroeze, W., Oenema, A., and Brug, J. (2008). Saturated fat consumption and
the theory of planned behaviour: Exploring additive and interactive effects of habit
strength. Appetite, 51(2), 318-323.

Diamantopoulos, A., Schlegelmilch, B.B., Sinkovics, R.R., and Bohlen, G.M. (2003). Can
socio-demographics still play a role in profiling green consumers? A review of the evidence
and an empirical investigation. Journal of Business Research, 56(6), 465-480.

D'Souza, C., Taghian, M., and Khosla, R. (2007). Examination of environmental beliefs and
its impact on the influence of price, quality and demographic characteristics with respect to
green purchase intention. Journal of Targeting, Measurement and Analysis for
Marketing, 15(2), 69-78.

Eggert, A. (2006). Intangibility and perceived risk in online environments. Journal of


Marketing Management, 22(5/6), 553-572.

Essoo, N., & Dibb, S. (2004). Religious influences on shopping behaviour: An exploratory
study. Journal of Marketing Management, 20(7-8), 683-712.

Eurobarometer. (2011). Attitudes of European citizens towards the environment. Accessed


23 January 2016. Available at http://ec.europa.eu/environment/pdf/ebs_365_en.pdf

Finisterra do Paço, A. M., & Raposo, M. L. B. (2010). Green consumer market segmentation:
empirical findings from Portugal. International Journal of Consumer Studies, 34(4), 429-436.

Fishbein, M. and Ajzen, I. (1975), Belief, Attitude, Intention and Behavior. Reading, MA:
Addison Wesley.

Fletcher, R. (2012). Creating a sustainable society in fast-growing Indonesia. Accessed 2


February 2016. Available at http://www.thejakartapost.com/news/2012/01/12/creating-a-
sustainable-society-fast-growing-indonesia.html.

Fornell, C. and Larcker, D.F. (1981). Evaluating structural equation models with
unobservable variables and measurement error. Journal of Marketing Research, 18(1), 39-50.

Fraj, E. and Martinez, E., 2007. Ecological consumer behaviour: An empirical analysis.
International Journal of Consumer Studies, 31(1), 26-33.

Gilligan, C. (1982). In a Different Voice: Psychological Theory and Women’s Development.


Cambridge, M.A: Harvard University Press.

Gleim, M.R., Smith, J.S., Andrews, D., and Cronin, J.J. (2013). Against the green: A multi-
method examination of the barriers to green consumption. Journal of Retailing, 89(1), 44-61.

Godin, G. and Kok, G. (1996). The theory of planned behavior: A review of its applications
to health-related behaviors. American Journal of Health Promotion, 11(2), 87-98.

26

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


Goles, T., Jayatilaka, B., George, B., Parsons, L., Chambers, V., Taylor, D., and Brune, R.
(2008). Softlifting: Exploring determinants of attitude. Journal of Business Ethics, 77(4),
481–499.

Gunelius, S. (2008, November 10). Generation Y weighs in on green brands. Retrieved


March 5, 2010, from Corporate Eye, Corporate-eye.com

Gupta, S. and Ogden, D.T. (2009). To buy or not to buy? A social dilemma perspective on
green buying. Journal of Consumer Marketing, 26(6), 376-391.

Ha, H.-Y and Janda, S. (2012). Predicting consumer intentions to purchase energy-efficient
products. Journal of Consumer Marketing, 29(7), 461-469.

Hair, J.F., Black, W.C., Babin, B.J., and Anderson, R.E. (2010). Multivariate Data
Analysis, 7th ed. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Education.

Haller, M., & Hadler, M. (2008). Dispositions to act in favor of the environment: Fatalism
and readiness to make sacrifices in a cross‐national perspective. Sociological Forum, 23(2),
281-311.

Han, H., Hsu, L. T. J., and Sheu, C. (2010). Application of the theory of planned behavior to
green hotel choice: Testing the effect of environmental friendly activities. Tourism
Management, 31(3), 325-334.

Han, H. and Yoon, H.J. (2015). Hotel customers’ environmentally responsible behavioural
intention: Impact of key constructs on decision in green consumerism. International Journal
of Hospitality Management. 45(2), 22-33.

Hansla, A., Gamble, A., Juliusson, A., & Gärling, T. (2008). Psychological determinants of
attitude towards and willingness to pay for green electricity. Energy Policy, 36(2), 768-774.

He, AZ., Cai, T., Deng, T.X., and Li, X. (2015). Factors affecting non-green consumer
behaviour: An exploratory study among Chinese consumers. International Journal of
Consumer Studies, (In press). DOI: 10.1111/ijcs.12261

Hines, J.M., Hungerford, H.R., and Tomera, A.N. (1986). Analysis and synthesis of research
on responsible environmental behavior: A meta-analysis. Journal of Environmental
Education, 18(2), 1-8.

Hunt, S.D. and Vitell, S. (1986). A general theory of marketing ethics. Journal of
Macromarketing, 6(1), 5-16.

Irland, L. C. (1993). Wood producers face green marketing era: Environmentally sound
products. Wood Technology, 120(2) 34.

Ji, L. J., Peng, K., and Nisbett, R. E. (2000). Culture, control, and perception of relationships
in the environment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(5), 943-955.

Johnstone, M-L. and Tan, L.P. (2015a). Exploring the gap between consumers' green

27

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


rhetoric and purchasing behaviour. Journal of Business Ethics, 132(2), 311-328.

Johnstone, M-L. and Tan, L.P. (2015b). An exploration of environmentally-conscious


consumers and the reasons why they do not buy green products. Marketing Intelligence &
Planning, 33(5), 1-22.

Johnstone, M-L., Yang, L. and Tan, L.P (2014). The attitude-behavior gap: The development
of a consumers’ green perception scale. Australian and New Zealand Marketing Academy
Conference, Brisbane.

Jong, H.N. (2016). Retailers to crack down on plastic bags. Accessed on 8 February 2016.
Available at: http://www.thejakartapost.com/news/2016/02/05/retailers-crack-down-plastic-
bags.html

Kalafatis, S. P., Pollard, M., East, R., and Tsogas, M. H. (1999). Green marketing and Ajzen's
theory of planned behaviour: A cross-market examination. Journal of Consumer
Marketing, 16(5), 441-460.

Kan, G., Cliquet, G., and Puelles Gallo, M. (2014). The effect of country image on
hypermarket patronage intention: A cross-cultural study in China and Spain. International
Journal of Retail & Distribution Management, 42(2), 106-130.

Keramati, A., Sharif, H. J., Azad, N., & Soofifard, R. (2012). Role of subjective norms and
perceived behavioral control of tax payers in acceptance of e-tax payment
system. International Journal of E-Adoption (IJEA), 4(3), 1-14.

Khare, A. (2015). Antecedents of green buying behaviour: A study on consumers in an


emerging economy. Marketing Intelligence & Planning, 33(3), 309-329.

Kilbourne, W.E. and Beckmann, S.C. (2002). The role of the dominant social paradigm in
environmental attitudes: A multinational examination. Journal of Business Research, 55(3),
193- 204.

Kim, J. S., Christodoulidou, N., & Brewer, P. (2012). Impact of individual differences and
consumers’ readiness on likelihood of using self-service technologies at hospitality settings.
Journal of Hospitality & Tourism Research, 36(1), 85-114.

Kleinjan, M., Engels, R. C., van Leeuwe, J., Brug, J., van Zundert, R. M., & van den Eijnden,
R. J. (2009). Mechanisms of adolescent smoking cessation: Roles of readiness to quit,
nicotine dependence, and smoking of parents and peers. Drug and alcohol dependence, 99(1),
204-214.

Kollmuss, A., and Agyeman, J. (2002). Mind the gap: Why do people act environmentally
and what are the barriers to pro-environmental behavior? Environmental Education Research,
8(3), 239-260.

Lee, M. C. (2009). Factors influencing the adoption of internet banking: An integration of


TAM and Theory of Planned Behavior with perceived risk and perceived benefit. Electronic
Commerce Research and Applications, 8(3), 130-141.

28

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


Lee, J.A. and Holden, S.J. (1999). Understanding determinants of environmentally conscious
behaviour. Psychology and Marketing, 16(5), 373-92.

Lee, K. (2009). Gender differences in Hong Kong adolescent consumers' green purchasing
behavior. Journal of Consumer Marketing, 26(2), 87-96.

Limayem, M., Khalifa, M. and Chin, W.W. (2004). Factors motivating software piracy: A
longitudinal study’, IEEE Transactions on Engineering Management, 51(4), 414–425.

Line, N. D. and Hanks, L. (2015). The effects of environmental and luxury beliefs on
intention to patronize green hotels: the moderating effect of destination image. Journal of
Sustainable Tourism (in press), DOI: 10.1080/09669582.2015.1091467

Lowe, B., and Alpert, F. (2010). Pricing strategy and the formation and evolution of reference
price perceptions in new product categories. Psychology and Marketing, 27(9), 846-873.

Lowe, B., and Barnes, B. R. (2012). Consumer perceptions of monetary and non-monetary
introductory promotions for new products. Journal of Marketing Management, 28(5/6), 629-
651.

Luo, X. and Bhattacharya, C.B. (2006). Corporate social responsibility, customer satisfaction
and market value. Journal of Marketing, 70(4), 1-18.

Martin, W.C. and Bateman, C.R. (2014). Consumer religious commitment’s influence on
ecocentric attitudes and behavior. Journal of Business Research 67(2), 5-11.

Martinsons, M.G., So, S.K.K., Tin, C., and Wong, D. (1997). Hong Kong and China:
Emerging markets for environmental products and technologies. Long Range Planning,
30(2), 277-290.

McDonald, S., Oates, C.J., Alevizou, P.J., Young, C.W., and Hwang, K. (2012). Individual
strategies for sustainable consumption. Journal of Marketing Management, 28(3-4), 445-468.

McIntosh, A. (1991). The impact of environmental issues on marketing and politics in the
1990s. Journal of the Market Research Society, 33(3), 205-217.

Measey, M. (2010). Indonesia: a vulnerable country in the face of climate change. Global
Majority E-Journal, 1(1), 31-45.

Menguc, B. and Ozanne, L.K. (2005). Challenges of the ‘green imperative’: A natural
resource-based approach to the environmental orientation—business performance
relationship. Journal of Business Research, 58(4), 430-438.

Moisander, J. (2007). Motivational complexity of green consumerism. International Journal


of Consumer Studies, 31(4), 404-409.

Mostafa, M.M. (2007). Gender differences in Egyptian consumers’ green purchase


behaviour: The effects of environmental knowledge, concern and attitude. International
Journal of Consumer Studies, 31(3), 220-229.

29

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


Netemeyer, R. G., Burton, S., & Johnston, M. (1991). A comparison of two models for the
prediction of volitional and goal-directed behaviors: A confirmatory analysis approach.
Social Psychology Quarterly, 54(2), 87-100.

Nittala, R. (2014). Green consumer behaviour of the educated segment in India. Journal of
International Consumer Marketing, 26(2), 138-152.

Nunnally, J.C. (1978). Psychometric Theory, 2nd Ed. New York: McGraw-Hill, Inc.

Ottman, J.A., Stafford, E.R., and Hartman, C.L. (2006). Avoiding green marketing myopia:
Ways to improve consumer appeal for environmentally preferable products. Environment,
48(5), 22-36.

Oakford, S, (20134). Indonesia is killing the planet for palm oil. Assessed on 25 March 2016.
https://news.vice.com/article/indonesia-is-killing-the-planet-for-palm-oil.

Pagiaslis, A. and Krontalis, A.K. (2014). Green consumption behaviour antecedents:


Environmental concern, knowledge, and beliefs. Psychology and Marketing, 31(5), 335-348.

Paisley, C. M., and Sparks, P. (1998). Expectations of reducing fat intake: The role of
perceived need within the theory of planned behaviour. Psychology and Health, 13(2), 341-
353.

Peattie, K. (2010). Green consumption: Behavior and norms. Annual Review of Environment
and Resources, 35(8), 195-228.

Pereira Heath, M. T., & Chatzidakis, A. (2012). ‘Blame it on marketing’: Consumers' views
on unsustainable consumption. International Journal of Consumer Studies, 36(6), 656-667.

Petty, R. E., Unnava, R. H., and Strathman, A. J. (1991). Theories of attitude change. In
Roberston, T. S. and Kassarjian, H. H. (Eds), Handbook of Consumer Behavior. Englewood
Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice Hall, 241-280.

Pew Research Center (2010). Obama More Popular Abroad than at Home, Global Image of
U.S. Continues to Benefit. Washington, D.C.: Pew Research Center.

Pickett-Baker, J. and Ozaki, R. (2008). Pro-environmental products: Marketing influence on


consumer purchase decision. Journal of Consumer Marketing, 25(5), 281-293.

Podsakoff, P.M., MacKenzie, S.B., Lee, J.Y., and Podsakoff, N.P. (2003). Common method
biases in behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and recommended remedies.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 88(5), 879-903.

Polonsky, M. J. (2011). Transformative green marketing: Impediments and opportunities.


Journal of Business Research, 64(12), 1311-1319.

Population Reference Bureau (2015). World Population Data Sheet. Washington, DC: PRB.

Porter. M. and van der Linde, C. (1995). Toward a new conception of the environment-
competitiveness relationship. The Journal of Economic Perspectives, 9(4)97-118.

30

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


Povey, R., Conner, M., Sparks, P., James, R., and Shepherd, R. (2000). The theory of planned
behaviour and healthy eating: Examining additive and moderating effects of social influence
variables. Psychology & Health, 14(6), 991-1006.

Preacher, K. J. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing
indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40(3), 879-891.

Pulungan, A.M. (2017). “Lingering problems behind surging inequality”. Accessed on 12


March 2017. Available at http://www.thejakartapost.com/academia/2017/03/10/lingering-
problems-behind-surging-inequality.html

Quazi, A., Nejati, M. & Amran, A. (2015). The CSR journey: Looking through the
evolutionary lens. In Corporate Social Responsibility and Sustainability: Contemporary
Perspectives (ed. by M. Nejati, A. Quazi, & A. Amran), Pearson, Kuala Lumpur.

Quazi, A., Amran, A., & Nejati, M. (2016). Conceptualizing and measuring consumer social
responsibility: A neglected aspect of consumer research. International Journal of Consumer
Studies, 40(1), 48-56.

Raats, M. M., Shepherd, R., and Sparks, P. (1995). Including moral dimensions of choice
within the structure of the theory of planned behavior. Journal of Applied Social
Psychology, 25(6), 484-494.

Rettie, R., Burchell, K. and Riley, D. (2012). Normalising green behaviours: A new approach
to sustainability marketing. Journal of Marketing Management, 28(3-4), 420-444.

Rex, E. and Baumann, H. (2007). Beyond ecolabels: What green marketing can learn from
conventional marketing. Journal of Cleaner Production, 15(6), 567-576.

Rhodes, R. E., & Courneya, K. S. (2003). Investigating multiple components of attitude,


subjective norm, and perceived control: An examination of the theory of planned behaviour
in the exercise domain. British Journal of Social Psychology, 42(1), 129-146.

Robin, D. and Babin, L. (1997). Making sense of the research on gender and ethics in
business: A critical analysis and extension. Business Ethics Quarterly, 7(4), 61-90.

Satorra, A.B. and Bentler, P.M. (1994). Corrections to test statistics and standard errors in
covariance structure analysis. In von Eye, A. and Clogg, C.C. (eds.). Latent Variables
Analysis: Applications for Developmental Research (pp. 399-419). SAGE Publications, Inc.,
Thousand Oaks.

Schlegel, R. P., d’Averna, J. R., Zanna, M. P., DeCourville, N. H. and Manske, S. R. (1992).
‘Problem drinking: A problem for the theory of reasoned action?’ Journal of Applied Social
Psychology, 22(5),358-385.

Schwepker, C.H. and Cornwell, T.B. (1991). An examination of ecologically concerned


consumers and their intention to purchase ecologically packaged products. Journal of Public
Policy and Marketing, 10(2), 77-101

31

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


Shah Alam, S., & Mohamed Sayuti, N. (2011). Applying the Theory of Planned Behavior in
halal food purchasing. International Journal of Commerce and Management, 21(1), 8-20.

Sharpe, J. (2014). “Meet Indonesia’s middle class”. Accessed on 12 March 2017. Available at
https://www.lowyinstitute.org/the-interpreter/meet-indonesias-middle-class

Shaw, D., Shiu, E., and Clarke, I. (2000). The contribution of ethical obligation and self-
identity to the theory of planned behaviour: An exploration of ethical consumers. Journal of
Marketing Management, 16(8), 879-894.

Shaw, D. and Shiu, E. (2002). An assessment of ethical obligation and self‐identity in ethical
consumer decision‐making: A structural equation modelling approach. International Journal
of Consumer Studies, 26(4), 286-293.

Smith, K. T. (2010). An examination of marketing techniques that influence Millennials'


perceptions of whether a product is environmentally friendly. Journal of Strategic
Marketing, 18(6), 437-450.

Shrum, L. J., McCarty, J. A., and Lowrey, T. M. (1995). Buyer characteristics of the green
consumer and their implications for advertising strategy. Journal of Advertising, 24(2), 71-
82.

Smith, S. and Paladino, A. (2010). Eating clean and green? Investigating consumer
motivations towards the purchase of organic food. Australasian Marketing Journal
(AMJ), 18(2), 93-104.

Sparks, P. and Guthrie, C.A. (1998). Self-identity and the theory of planned behavior: A
useful addition or an unhelpful artifice? Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 28(15), 1394-
1410.

Sparks, P. and Shepherd, R. (1992). Self-identity and the theory of planned behaviour:
Assessing the role of identification with green consumerism. Social Psychology Quarterly,
55(4), 388-399.

Stets, J.E. and Biga, C.F. (2003). Bringing identity theory into environmental
sociology. Sociological Theory, 21(4), 398-423.

Stets, J.E. (1995). Role identities and person identities, mastery identity, and controlling
one’s partner. Sociological Perspective, 38(2), 129-150.

Swimberghe, K., Sharma, D., and Flurry, L. (2009). An exploratory investigation of the
consumer religious commitment and its influence on store loyalty and consumer
complaint intentions. Journal of Consumer Marketing, 26(5), 340–347.

Snyder, F.J. and Flay, B.R. (2012). Brief Introduction to the Theory of Triadic Influence.
Unpublished document, Oregon State University. Accessed online on 17-Feb-2016.
http://people.oregonstate.edu/~flayb/MY%20COURSES/H571%20Principles%20of%20Heal
th%20Behavior%20Fall%202014/Readings/Snyder&Flay12%20Brief%20Introduction%20to
%20the%20Theory%20of%20Triadic%20Influence.pdf.

32

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


Szmigin, I., Carrigan, M., and McEachern, M.G. (2009). The conscious consumer: Taking a
flexible approach to ethical behaviour. International Journal of Consumer Studies, 33(2),
224-231.

Tanner, C. and Kast, W.S. (2003). Promoting sustainable consumption: Determinants of


green purchases by Swiss consumers. Psychology & Marketing, 20(10), 883-902.

Tarkiainen, A. and Sundqvist, S. (2005). Subjective norms, attitudes and intentions of Finnish
consumers in buying organic food. British Food Journal, 107(11), 808-822.

The Economist (2011). Indonesia’s poverty line: To make a million people unpoor. Accessed
on 10 March 2017. Available at
http://www.economist.com/blogs/banyan/2011/08/indonesias-poverty-line

Tikka, P., Kuitunen, M., and Tynys, S. (2000). Effects of educational background on
students’ attitudes, activity levels, and knowledge concerning the environment. Journal of
Environmental Education, 31(1), 12-19.

Tully, S. and Schonfeld, E. (1994, May 16). Teens: The most global market of all. Fortune,
129, 90-97.

Vitell, S. J., & Muncy, J. (2005). The Muncy–Vitell consumer ethics scale: A modification
and application. Journal of Business Ethics, 62(3), 267-275.

Vitell, S.J., Paolillo, J.G.P., and Singh, J.J. (2005). Religiosity and consumer ethics. Journal
of Business Ethics, 57(2), 175-181.

Wahid, N. A., Rahbar, E., and Shyan, T. S. (2011). Factors influencing the green purchase
behavior of Penang environmental volunteers. International Business Management, 5(1), 38-
49

Whitmarsh, L. (2008a). Are flood victims more concerned about climate change than other
people? The role of direct experience in risk perception and behavioural response. Journal of
Risk Research, 11(3), 351 - 374.

Yeon K.H. and Chung, J.E. (2011). Consumer purchase intention for organic personal care
products. Journal of Consumer Marketing, 28(1), 40-47.

Welsch, H. and Kühling, W. (2009). Determinants of pro-environmental consumption: the


role of reference groups and routine behaviour. Ecological Economics, 69(1), 166-176.

Whitmarsh, L. and O'Neill, S. (2010). Green identity, green living? The role of pro-
environmental self-identity in determining consistency across diverse pro-environmental
behaviours. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 30(3), 305-314.

Whitson, D., Ozkaya, H. E., & Roxas, J. (2014). Changes in consumer segments and
preferences to green labelling. International Journal of Consumer Studies, 38(5), 458-466.

33

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


Zabkar, V. and Hosta, M. (2013). Willingness to act and environmentally conscious
consumer behaviour: Can prosocial status perceptions help overcome the gap? International
Journal of Consumer Studies, 37(3), 257-264.

Zhao, X., Lynch, J.G. and Chen, Q. (2010). Reconsidering Baron and Kenny: Myths and
truths about mediation analysis. Journal of Consumer Research, 37 (2), 197-206.

Zhou, Y., Thogersen, J., Ruan, Y., and Huang, G. (2013). The moderating role of human
values in planned behaviour: The case of Chinese consumers’ intention to buy organic food.
Journal of Consumer Marketing, 30(4), 335-344.

Zinkhan, G. M. and Carlson, L. (1995). Green advertising and the reluctant consumer.
Journal of Advertising, 24(2), 1-6.

34

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


Table 1. Profiles of Respondents

n = 916 Frequency Percentage


Age
18-20 years old 351 38.3%
21-23 years old 161 17.6%
24-26 years old 124 13.5%
> 26 years old 280 30.6%
Gender
Male 442 48.3%
Female 474 51.7%
Annual Income
< Rp 20 million 681 74.3%
Rp 21 million - Rp 40 million 111 12.1%
Rp 41 million - Rp 60 million 61 6.7%
> Rp 60 million 63 6.9%
Religion
Islam 434 47.4%
Christian (Protestant) 230 25.1%
Catholic 191 20.9%
Others 61 6.6%
Ever purchased household
products that were promoted as
environmentally-friendly
No 236 25.8
Yes (can be more than 1 answers): 680 74.2
Laundry detergents 334 42.1
Dishwashing liquids 256 32.2
Toilet paper rolls 214 27
Soaps 383 48.2
Others 130 16.4

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


Table 2. Confirmatory Factor Analysis

Factor Item Convergent Validity Reliability


Factor loading Loading average Cronbach’s α CR AVE
Purchase Intention (PI)
In the next six weeks, how likely are you to purchase PI01 0.85 0.81 0.79 0.65
environmentally-friendly household products?
I intend to buy environmentally-friendly household PI02 0.77
products during the next six weeks.
Readiness to Be Green (RI) (Reversed Coded)
I do not have sufficient knowledge about environmental RI01 0.63 0.74 0.78 0.83 0.56
issues to make decisions about these types of
products.
I do not have sufficient time to learn about RI02 0.80
environmentally-friendly household products.
I have too many other responsibilities at the moment to RI03 0.80
think about environmentally-friendly household
products.
Attitude (ATT)
In general, my attitude towards purchasing an ATT N/A
environmentally-friendly products is (favorable –
unfavorable).
Subjective Norm (SN)
Most people who are important to me buy SN01 0.80 0.82 0.86 0.86 0.55
environmentally-friendly household products.
Most people who are important to me are concerned SN02 0.80
about issues related to the environment.
Most people who are important to me think it is SN03 0.86
important to buy environmentally-friendly
household products.
Perceived Behavioral Control (PBC)
If I wanted, it would be easy for me to buy PBC N/A
environmentally-friendly household product

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


Table 2 (Cont). Confirmatory Factor Analysis

Factor Item Convergent Validity Reliability


Factor loading Loading average Cronbach’s α CR AVE
Pro-Environmental Self-Identity (PEI)
I think of myself as someone who is very concerned PI01 0.71 0.74 0.77 0.88 0.55
about environmental issues.
I think of myself as an environmentally-friendly PI02 0.85
consumer.
Purchasing environmentally-friendly products reflects PI03 0.66
who I am.
Ethical Obligation (EO)
The environment is one of the most important issues EO01 0.71 0.74 0.83 0.86 0.50
facing society today.
Unless each of us recognizes the need to protect the EO02 0.76
environment, future generations will suffer the
consequences.
The benefits of protecting the environment do not EO03 0.70
justify the expense involved.
The importance of the environment is not being EO04 0.79
exaggerated.
Perceived Sense of Responsibility (PSR)
(Reversed Coded)
I do not need to do anything at the moment because the PS01 0.92 0.92 0.83 0.84 0.84
environment is not a major concern in Indonesia.
I do not feel responsible for environmental PS02 0.91
deterioration.

Goodness-of-fit measures
BBNFI TLI CFI IFI RMSEA
X2 (130df) = 272.370 (p = 0.00) 0.941 0.945 0.958 0.959 0.049
Notes: CR Composite Reliability, AVE Average Variance Extracted

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


Table 3. Correlation and Discriminant Validity

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
1. Attitude N/A 0.132 0.030 0.158 0.152 0.059 0.073 0.179
2. Subjective Norm 0.364** 0.55 0.016 0.212 0.005 0.002 0.020 0.182
3. Perceived Behavioral Control 0.174** 0.129** N/A 0.015 0.031 0.000 0.017 0.063
4. Pro-environmental Self-Identity 0.398* 0.461** 0.123** 0.55 0.116 0.015 0.075 0.210
5. Ethical Obligation 0.390** 0.227** 0.177** 0.341** 0.50 0.126 0.072 0.093
6. Perceived Sense of Responsibility 0.244** 0.041 0.017 0.123** 0.356** 0.84 0.113 0.008
7. Perceived Readiness to be Green 0.271** 0.142** 0.129** 0.275** 0.268** 0.337** 0.56 0.080
8. Purchase Intention 0.424** 0.427** 0.251** 0.459** 0.305** 0.091** 0.284** 0.65
Mean 3.88 3.36 3.33 3.36 4.15 3.83 3.23 3.19
Std Dev 0.66 0.70 0.85 0.61 0.57 0.74 0.74 0.73
Notes: Values below the diagonal are bivariate correlations between the constructs, bold diagonal elements represents the Average Variance Extracted (AVEs) for the
relevant construct; Values above the diagonal represent squared correlations.
** significant at  = 1%; * significant at  = 5%.

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


Table 4. Structural Equation Model Results

Hypothesis Structural relationship Standardised coefficient Result


H1a Attitude  Perceived Readiness to Be Green 0.090* Supported
H1b Attitude  Purchase Intention 0.190** Supported
H2a Subjective Norm  Perceived Readiness to Be Green -0.039 Not supported
H2b Subjective Norm  Purchase Intention 0.228** Supported
H3a Perceived Behavioral Control  Perceived Readiness to Be Green 0.076* Supported
H3b Perceived Behavioral Control  Purchase Intention 0.138** Supported
H4a Pro-Environmental Self-Identity  Perceived Readiness to Be Green 0.206** Supported
H4b Pro-Environmental Self-Identity  Purchase Intention 0.294** Supported
H5a Perceived Sense of Responsibility  Perceived Readiness to Be Green 0.057** Supported
H5b Perceived Sense of Responsibility  Purchase Intention -0.115** Not Supported
H6a Ethical Obligation  Perceived Readiness to Be Green 0.057 Not Supported
H6b Ethical Obligation  Purchase Intention 0.090* Supported
H7 Perceived Readiness to Be Green  Purchase Intention 0.160** Supported
Goodness-of-fit measures
BBNFI TLI CFI IFI RMSEA
2
X (124 df) = 361.941 (p = 0.000) 0.948 0.952 0.965 0.965 0.046

Notes: ** significant at  = 1%; * significant at  = 5%.

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


Table 5. Mediating Model Results

Hypothesis Structural Relationship Mediator: Readiness to be Green Results


Standardized S.E Lower Upper
indirect effects
H8a Attitude  Purchase Intention 0.014 0.037 0.003 0.037 Supported
H8b Subjective Norm Pro-Environmental Self- -0.006 0.040 -0.024 0.007 Not Supported
Identity  Purchase Intention
H8c Perceived Behavioral Control  Purchase 0.012 0.024 0.000 0.029 Supported
Intention
H8d Pro-environmental Self-Identity  Purchase 0.033 0.024 0.014 0.066 Supported
Intention
H8e Ethical Obligation  Purchase Intention 0.009 0.054 -0.008 0.027 Not supported
H8f Perceived Sense of Responsibility  0.053 0.041 0.023 0.095 Supported
Purchase Intention

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.


Figure 1. Conceptual Framework
ATTITUDE

(+)
SUBJECTIVE
NORM (+)

(+)
(+)
PERCEIVED
BEHAVIORAL
CONTROL (+)

(+) PURCHASE
PRO-ENVIRONMENTAL (+) INTENTION
SELF-IDENTITY

(+) (+)

ETHICAL OBLIGATION
(+) (+)
(+)
PERCEIVED
PERCEIVED SENSE OF READINESS TO BE
RESPONSIBILITY GREEN
(+)

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.

You might also like