Applied essays

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

Behaviorism:

Behaviorist theory, a cornerstone of psychology in the early 20th century, focuses on the idea
that all behaviors are acquired through conditioning. This theory was significantly shaped by
several key figures, including B.F. Skinner, Ivan Pavlov, John B. Watson, and Edward
Thorndike. Skinner is renowned for his work on operant conditioning, which involves
learning through rewards and punishments. Pavlov’s experiments with dogs laid the
foundation for classical conditioning, demonstrating how a neutral stimulus, when paired with
an unconditioned stimulus, can elicit a conditioned response. Watson, often considered the
father of behaviorism, famously applied these principles to human behavior, as seen in his
Little Albert experiment. Thorndike introduced the Law of Effect, which states that behaviors
followed by satisfying consequences are more likely to recur, while those followed by
unpleasant consequences are less likely to be repeated.

Operant conditioning, as developed by Skinner, is a method of learning that employs rewards


and punishments for behavior. Through this process, an association is made between a
behavior and a consequence for that behavior. For instance, Skinner's experiments with rats
and pigeons showed that behaviors could be shaped by systematically reinforcing desired
behaviors and ignoring or punishing undesired ones. This type of conditioning has been
widely used in various fields, from education to animal training, emphasizing the role of
reinforcement and punishment in shaping behavior.

Classical conditioning, introduced by Pavlov, involves learning through association. Pavlov


discovered that dogs could be trained to salivate at the sound of a bell if the bell was
consistently rung just before they were fed. This demonstrated that a neutral stimulus (the
bell) could come to elicit a conditioned response (salivation) when paired with an
unconditioned stimulus (food). This form of learning highlights how involuntary responses
can be conditioned, providing a basis for understanding human and animal behavior in
various contexts.

Despite its contributions, behaviorism has several limitations. One significant criticism is its
neglect of internal mental processes. Behaviorists argue that only observable behaviors should
be studied, which excludes the understanding of thoughts, emotions, and other internal states.
The famous Little Albert experiment by Watson, which involved conditioning a young child
to fear a white rat, raised ethical concerns due to its potential psychological harm.
Additionally, behaviorism often fails to account for innate biological factors and the
complexity of human learning and behavior, which involves more than just responses to
stimuli and reinforcement. These limitations have led to the development of other theories,
such as cognitive and social learning theories, which incorporate a broader range of factors
influencing behavior and learning.
Cognitivism:

Cognitivism emerged as a significant reaction to the limitations of behaviorist theory,


emphasizing the importance of internal mental processes in understanding how people learn.
Jean Piaget, a pivotal figure in this movement, introduced a detailed framework on how
cognitive development occurs in children. Piaget’s theory of cognitive development outlines
four stages: sensorimotor, preoperational, concrete operational, and formal operational. Each
stage represents a different level of thinking complexity, illustrating how children's cognitive
abilities evolve as they grow. This shift towards examining internal processes provided a
more comprehensive understanding of learning, going beyond the stimulus-response patterns
emphasized by behaviorism.

Key ideas of cognitivism include the focus on understanding how information is received,
processed, stored, and retrieved by the mind. Cognitivism posits that the mind works like a
computer, where learning involves encoding, processing, and storing information. This
perspective highlights the active role of learners in constructing knowledge, rather than
passively receiving information from the environment. Cognitive processes such as
perception, memory, and problem-solving are central to understanding how learning occurs.
This theory brought attention to the importance of prior knowledge, the mental organization
of information, and the strategies individuals use to learn and solve problems.

Piaget introduced the concept of schemas, which are cognitive structures that help individuals
organize and interpret information. Schemas are mental models or frameworks that represent
different aspects of the world and help in understanding and responding to new information.
Piaget suggested that learning occurs through the processes of assimilation and
accommodation. Assimilation involves integrating new information into existing schemas,
while accommodation requires altering existing schemas or creating new ones in response to
new information. These processes enable individuals to adapt their understanding and
continue learning throughout their lives.

Despite its significant contributions, cognitivism has some limitations. One criticism is that it
can be overly focused on the internal processes of the mind, sometimes neglecting the social
and emotional aspects of learning. While cognitivism emphasizes mental processes, it may
not fully account for the role of cultural and contextual factors in shaping how individuals
learn. Additionally, the comparison of the mind to a computer can be seen as an
oversimplification, as human thinking involves emotions, creativity, and other complex
elements that are not easily reducible to computational models. Furthermore, Piaget's stages
of cognitive development have been critiqued for underestimating children's abilities and the
variability in development across different individuals and cultures. Despite these limitations,
cognitivism remains a fundamental theory in understanding the complexities of human
learning.
Innateness:

The Innateness theory, particularly associated with Noam Chomsky, posits that the ability to
acquire language is innate to humans and not solely dependent on environmental factors.
Chomsky’s critique of behaviorism, especially B.F. Skinner’s explanation of language
learning through operant conditioning, marked a significant shift in the field of linguistics and
cognitive science. Chomsky argued that behaviorism could not adequately explain the
complexity and creativity of human language, which involves understanding and producing
an infinite number of sentences never encountered before.

Chomsky introduced several key concepts central to the Innateness theory. One of the most
influential is the idea of the “universal grammar,” a set of grammatical principles and
structures shared by all human languages. According to Chomsky, children are born with an
inherent understanding of this universal grammar, which enables them to rapidly and
effortlessly acquire the specific language to which they are exposed. This innate linguistic
capability explains why children can learn complex grammatical rules and generate novel
sentences without explicit instruction, something behaviorism struggles to account for.

Another key concept is the "poverty of the stimulus" argument, which posits that the linguistic
input available to children is insufficient to explain their ability to understand and produce
complex sentences. The language children hear is often fragmented and grammatically
imperfect, yet they manage to develop a robust understanding of their native language's rules
and structures. Chomsky argued that this ability must be due to an inborn linguistic capacity
rather than learned behavior reinforced by external stimuli, as behaviorism suggests.

Despite its influential impact, the Innateness theory has its limitations and has been subject to
criticism. One limitation is that it can be overly deterministic, suggesting that language
development is predominantly a result of biological endowment and underestimating the role
of social interaction and environmental factors. Critics argue that language acquisition is also
significantly shaped by the communicative environment and the interactions children have
with caregivers and peers. Additionally, the concept of universal grammar has been
challenged by linguistic diversity and the variations in grammatical structures across different
languages, leading some to question the extent to which universal principles govern all
languages. Furthermore, the theory has been critiqued for being difficult to empirically test
and verify, given the abstract nature of innate grammatical knowledge. Despite these
criticisms, Chomsky's Innateness theory remains a foundational framework in understanding
the biological basis of language acquisition
Krashen:

Stephen Krashen, a prominent figure in the field of second language acquisition, developed a
comprehensive theory comprising five key hypotheses: the Learning-Acquisition Hypothesis,
the Natural Order Hypothesis, the Monitor Hypothesis, the Input Hypothesis, and the
Affective Filter Hypothesis. These hypotheses collectively aim to explain how individuals
acquire a second language and the factors influencing this process.

The Learning-Acquisition Hypothesis differentiates between 'learning' and 'acquisition.'


According to Krashen, 'learning' refers to the conscious process of gaining knowledge about a
language's rules, often occurring in formal educational settings. In contrast, 'acquisition' is a
subconscious process similar to how children naturally learn their first language. Krashen
argues that acquisition is the primary route to developing language proficiency, while learning
plays a secondary, supportive role.

The Natural Order Hypothesis posits that language learners acquire grammatical structures in
a predictable sequence, regardless of their native language or the language being learned. This
sequence is consistent across learners and is not necessarily influenced by direct instruction.
For example, learners might acquire basic grammatical structures like plural forms before
more complex ones like subjunctive mood, suggesting an inherent order to language
acquisition.

The Monitor Hypothesis explains the relationship between acquisition and learning, proposing
that the learned system acts as a 'monitor' to correct or modify language output. This monitor
function relies on explicit knowledge of rules and is used to edit speech or writing after the
initial, spontaneous output from the acquired system. However, Krashen notes that over-
reliance on the monitor can hinder fluency, as excessive self-correction can disrupt the natural
flow of communication.

The Input Hypothesis emphasizes the importance of comprehensible input in language


acquisition. Krashen argues that learners acquire language best when they are exposed to
input slightly beyond their current proficiency level, often referred to as "i+1." This means
that learners should be able to understand the essence of the input while being challenged by
new linguistic elements, facilitating gradual language development.

The Affective Filter Hypothesis suggests that emotional factors such as motivation, anxiety,
and self-confidence significantly impact language acquisition. A positive emotional state
lowers the affective filter, allowing more input to be processed and acquired. Conversely,
negative emotions can raise the affective filter, blocking input and hindering language
acquisition.

Despite its widespread influence, Krashen's theory has faced criticisms. Some argue that the
distinction between learning and acquisition is too rigid and oversimplified, as real-world
language learning often involves a blend of conscious and subconscious processes. The
Natural Order Hypothesis has been questioned for its lack of specificity and empirical
support, with critics suggesting that the order of acquisition may vary more significantly
among individuals and contexts than Krashen proposed. The Monitor Hypothesis has been
criticized for underestimating the role of explicit learning and conscious practice in achieving
language proficiency. The Input Hypothesis has sparked debate over the definition and
measurement of "comprehensible input," and whether it alone is sufficient for language
acquisition. Lastly, the Affective Filter Hypothesis, while highlighting important emotional
factors, has been critiqued for not providing clear guidance on how to address these factors
effectively in different learning environments.

Overall, Krashen's hypotheses have significantly shaped the understanding of second


language acquisition, emphasizing the need for meaningful, comprehensible input and the
importance of emotional well-being in the learning process. However, ongoing research
continues to refine and challenge these ideas, contributing to a more nuanced view of
language learning.

Contrastive Analysis:

Contrastive Analysis is a linguistic approach that involves comparing two or more languages
to identify their structural differences and similarities. This method gained prominence in the
mid-20th century, particularly within the field of second language acquisition (SLA) and
language teaching. The primary objective of contrastive analysis is to predict and explain
difficulties that language learners might encounter based on the differences between their
native language (L1) and the target language (L2).

Contrastive Analysis Hypothesis (CAH) posits that similarities between L1 and L2 facilitate
learning, while differences create challenges and potential areas of interference. For instance,
if a grammatical structure or phonetic element exists in both languages and is used similarly,
learners will likely acquire it with ease. Conversely, if a feature in L2 is absent or used
differently in L1, learners may struggle with it, leading to errors influenced by their native
language patterns.

The field of contrastive analysis was notably advanced by Robert Lado, an influential linguist
who extensively explored the implications of cross-linguistic differences for language
teaching. In his seminal work "Linguistics Across Cultures" (1957), Lado systematically
outlined how linguistic contrasts could inform language instruction and help teachers
anticipate areas where students might face difficulties. His work emphasized the practical
application of contrastive analysis in developing teaching materials and curricula tailored to
the specific needs of learners based on their linguistic backgrounds.

Despite its contributions, contrastive analysis has faced several criticisms and limitations. One
major critique is that it often oversimplifies the complexities of language learning by focusing
primarily on surface-level structural differences and ignoring deeper, more abstract cognitive
processes involved in acquiring a new language. Critics also argue that not all errors can be
traced back to L1 interference; some may arise from developmental stages that all language
learners experience, regardless of their native language. Additionally, the predictive power of
contrastive analysis has been questioned, as not all predicted difficulties materialize, and
some errors occur in areas where L1 and L2 are similar.

Moreover, the approach has been criticized for its deterministic view, implying that negative
transfer from L1 to L2 is inevitable. However, research has shown that learners can often
overcome these predicted difficulties through exposure, practice, and effective teaching
strategies. Modern SLA research tends to favor more comprehensive approaches that integrate
cognitive, social, and contextual factors influencing language learning, moving beyond the
narrow scope of contrastive analysis.

In summary, contrastive analysis and the work of Robert Lado have significantly contributed
to understanding the role of cross-linguistic differences in language learning. While the
approach has its limitations, it has laid the groundwork for further research and has been
instrumental in developing more nuanced theories and practical methodologies in the field of
second language acquisition

Interlanguage:

Larry Selinker, a prominent figure in the field of second language acquisition (SLA),
introduced the concept of interlanguage in the early 1970s. Interlanguage is a theoretical
construct that describes the evolving linguistic system that second language learners create on
their way to achieving proficiency in the target language. Selinker's theory marked a
significant shift in understanding how learners develop language skills, moving away from
viewing errors merely as failures to a more dynamic view of language learning.

Interlanguage represents a transitional linguistic system that incorporates elements from the
learner's native language (L1), the target language (L2), and unique elements that are neither
found in L1 nor L2. This system is constantly evolving as learners receive more input and
refine their understanding and usage of the target language. Selinker identified several
processes that contribute to the development of interlanguage, including transfer from L1,
overgeneralization of L2 rules, and the application of learning strategies.

A key component of interlanguage theory is the concept of fossilization, which Selinker used
to describe the phenomenon where learners' language development stalls, and certain
incorrect forms become permanently ingrained. Fossilization highlights the idea that not all
learners achieve native-like proficiency in their second language, and it helps explain why
certain errors persist even after extensive exposure and practice. This concept has been
particularly influential in understanding the limits and challenges in adult language learning.

Selinker's interlanguage theory also emphasizes the active role of learners in the language
acquisition process. Unlike earlier theories that viewed language learning as a passive
reception of stimuli and responses, interlanguage posits that learners actively construct and
reconstruct their linguistic knowledge based on their experiences, hypotheses, and feedback.
This view aligns with cognitive theories of learning that stress the importance of mental
processes and strategies in acquiring new skills.

Despite its significant contributions, the theory of interlanguage has faced some criticisms.
One limitation is its focus on individual cognitive processes, which can overlook the social
and interactive dimensions of language learning. Critics argue that language acquisition is not
solely an internal process but is also heavily influenced by social interactions, cultural
context, and communicative needs. Additionally, the concept of fossilization has been
debated, with some researchers questioning its permanence and suggesting that with
appropriate intervention and motivation, learners can overcome fossilized errors.

Furthermore, interlanguage theory primarily addresses the formal aspects of language


learning, such as grammar and pronunciation, and may not fully account for pragmatic and
sociolinguistic competence. The theory also tends to emphasize the learner's errors and
deviations from native-like norms, which can sometimes overshadow the progress and
achievements that learners make in their language development.

In conclusion, Larry Selinker's interlanguage theory has profoundly impacted the


understanding of second language acquisition by highlighting the dynamic, evolving nature of
the learner's linguistic system. It underscores the active role of learners in constructing their
language knowledge and provides valuable insights into the challenges and processes
involved in becoming proficient in a second language. While it has its limitations,
interlanguage theory remains a foundational concept in SLA research and pedagogy

You might also like