Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 20

Augmentative and Alternative Communication

ISSN: 0743-4618 (Print) 1477-3848 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/iaac20

Supporting individuals with challenging behavior


through functional communication training and
AAC: research review

Pat Mirenda

To cite this article: Pat Mirenda (1997) Supporting individuals with challenging behavior
through functional communication training and AAC: research review, Augmentative and
Alternative Communication, 13:4, 207-225, DOI: 10.1080/07434619712331278048

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/07434619712331278048

Published online: 12 Jul 2009.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 989

View related articles

Citing articles: 9 View citing articles

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=iaac20

Download by: [La Trobe University] Date: 25 February 2016, At: 09:19
0743-4618/97/1304-0207 $3.00/0; Volume 13, December 1997
AAC Augmentative and Alternative Communication
Copyright © 1997 by ISAAC

Supporting Individuals with Challenging Behavior


through Functional Communication Training and
AAC: Research Review
Pat Mirenda
Faculty of Education, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada

The term “functional communication training” (FCT) has been used over the past decade to
refer to a set of procedures designed to reduce challenging behavior by teaching functionally
equivalent communication skills. Functional communication training requires a thorough
Downloaded by [La Trobe University] at 09:19 25 February 2016

assessment to identify the function (or “message”) of the behavior of concern and systematic
instruction related to the new communicative behaviors. The growing body of empirical litera-
ture demonstrating the efficacy and mechanisms of this procedure has included a number of
examples in which augmentative and alternative communication (AAC) techniques were used
during intervention. The purpose of this review is to summarize the extant FCT/AAC research
in an accessible format and to identify areas for future research in this area.

KEY WORDS: behavior problems, functional communication, severe disabilities, training

Since 1985, numerous studies have examined var- the authors noted in their discussion of the results, “It
ious aspects of an intervention strategy used to sup- is likely that we will never be able to demonstrate def-
port individuals with challenging behavior (i.e., behav- initely that a particular child intended to ask for assis-
ior problems) that has become known as “functional tance or attention by means of his or her misbehav-
communication training” (FCT). FCT involves “both ior. Nonetheless, we may find it helpful to view
the assessment of the function of the problem behav- misbehavior as if it were a form of nonverbal commu-
ior and the teaching of a more appropriate form that nication, specifically, a request for certain behaviors
serves the same function…” (Durand, 1990, p. 23). For on the part of others” (p. 125).
example, Carr and Durand (1985) used this approach Many FCT studies have involved child or adult par-
with four children with developmental disabilities who ticipants for whom speech was the primary mode of
engaged in a variety of aggressive, self-destructive, communication (e.g., Durand & Carr, 1987, 1991,
and disruptive behaviors. The authors first conducted 1992). However, an increasing body of research has
systematic assessments to determine the functional applied FCT to individuals who use some form of aug-
motivation of the behaviors for each child. In three mentative or alternative communication (AAC). These
cases, challenging behaviors occurred primarily when studies were identified through a computer literature
the children were presented with difficult tasks, sug- search using the terms “functional communication
gesting that the behaviors were escape motivated. training” and “functional equivalence,” as well as
For one of these three children, as well as for the through examination of all issues from 1985–1996 of
fourth child, the behaviors occurred when the children the Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, The Journal
were provided with low levels of adult attention, sug- of the Association for Persons with Severe Handi-
gesting that, in these cases, the behaviors also served caps, and Augmentative and Alternative Communica-
an attention-getting function. The children were taught tion. Whenever a relevant article was found, its refer-
two verbal communicative phrases: (a) “Am I doing ence list was examined to identify new studies not
good work?,” a phrase designed to elicit adult atten- already included in the corpus. Studies that did not
tion, and (b) “I don’t understand,” a phrase designed specifically focus on behavioral support or that did
to elicit adult assistance during difficult tasks. For all not specifically identify “functional communication
four children, there was an immediate and substantial training” as an intervention strategy were not included.
reduction in the frequency of the target behaviors only A total of 21 studies that employed FCT/AAC with one
after they learned the communicative phrase that was or more participants were identified in this manner.
relevant to the specific function of their behavior. As The purpose of this review is to summarize the extant

207
208 Mirenda

FCT/AAC research in an accessible format and to use of one or more of the assessment procedures
identify areas for future research. from the studies reviewed for this article; these are
summarized in Table 2.
TARGET SUBJECTS AND BEHAVIORS The most common assessment strategy (used with
32 of the 52 participants) was some form of interview
The 21 studies reviewed included FCT/AAC inter- used in the initial phase of assessment to gather infor-
ventions for a total of 52 participants. A few of these mation from caregivers about the conditions in which
studies also included participants with whom verbal the problem behavior was most likely to occur. Sec-
FCT was used (e.g., Northup et al., 1991; Peck et al., ond in prevalence was a functional analysis (also
1996); however, those participants were not included referred to as an “analogue assessment”), which was
in this review. Table 1 summarizes the most relevant used with 29 of the participants. The third most com-
participant characteristics. mon strategy was the Motivation Assessment Scale
The 52 participants included 30 males and 22 (Durand and Crimmins, 1992), which was used to
females ranging in age from 1.3 to 36 years, with a assess 10 participants. Other assessment strategies
mean age of 13.8. All had one or more developmen- included observations, A-B-C charts, scatter plots,
tal disabilities. Forty-one participants (79%) were con- and descriptive analyses. These assessment strate-
sidered to have severe and/or profound mental hand- gies will be described in the sections that follow.
icaps. Eight participants had autism, six were
Downloaded by [La Trobe University] at 09:19 25 February 2016

moderately mentally handicapped, and three had Interview


Down syndrome. Sensory impairments (vision and/or
hearing) were evident in 11 participants, one or more The purpose of an assessment interview is to assist
physical disabilities (e.g., cerebral palsy, heart anom- in identification of the function(s) of the problem
alies) were present in 12, and seizure disorders were behavior. Such interviews can be conducted either
present in 5 participants. Of the 52 participants, half informally (e.g., Carr et al., 1994) or in a more struc-
had no speech and no formal communication system tured fashion (e.g., Iwata, Wong, Riordan, & Lau,
prior to the study in which they participated. Thirteen 1982; Willis, LaVigna, & Donnellan, 1989). The most
participants used limited gestures, manual signs, often-cited structured interview used in the studies
and/or simple graphic symbols to communicate. Four- reviewed was the Functional Analysis Interview
teen participants had some functional speech, but it (O’Neill, Horner, Albin, Storey, & Sprague, 1990),
was considered insufficient to meet their ongoing daily which is designed to gather information in three main
communication needs. areas: (a) a description of the behavior(s) of concern;
Many participants exhibited more than one type of (b) identification of physical and environmental factors
challenging behavior. Over half (56%) engaged in self- that predict the behavior(s); and (c) identification of
injurious behavior (SIB) of varying topographies. Much the potential functions of the behavior(s). In all cases,
of the SIB was considered to be dangerous or even life the interview was used as one component of a multi-
threatening, and a few participants spent hours each element assessment package. Although interviews
day in restraints to prevent injury (e.g., “Mike” in are widely used during assessment prior to FCT, no
Northup et al., 1994). Similarly, 42% of the participants reliability or validity data are available for their use.
engaged in aggression against other people (hitting,
scratching, etc.). Other behaviors of concern included Functional Analysis
tantrums, off-task or “silly” behavior, pulling on the lines
of feeding tubes or other life support equipment, non- This assessment procedure involves exposing the
compliance, screaming, yelling, property destruction, individual of concern to a series of situations (called
and self-stimulatory behavior (e.g., repetitive rocking). “analogue conditions”) that simulate naturally occur-
ring environmental events that might be related to the
ASSESSMENT challenging behavior (Iwata, Dorsey, Slifer, Bauman,
& Richman, 1982). The presence of the behavior is
Assessment Strategies taken as an indication that the associated condition is
relevant to the function of the behavior. In a typical
There is no doubt that a successful FCT/AAC inter- functional analysis, the individual of concern is
vention must begin with an accurate assessment of exposed to two or more assessment conditions over a
the function of the challenging behavior. Without this, period of several days. For example, in the “tangibles”
it is simply impossible to determine which message(s) condition, preferred items (e.g., toys, food) are placed
to teach as alternatives to the behavior. The result can out of reach but within sight of the individual. Each
be that, even though communication is increased time the target behavior occurs, the item is provided
through instruction of a new communicative form, the briefly (e.g., for 30–60 seconds) and then removed.
behavior problem is unchanged because of a “mis- The occurrence of the behavior during this condition is
match” between the new form and the old behavior. an indication that the behavior functions as a request
This unfortunate outcome can be avoided through the for the preferred item (i.e., positive reinforcement).
Functional Communication Training and AAC 209

TABLE 1: Participant Description

FCT Preintervention Target Behavior


Participant Expressive Treated with
Study Name Age Disability Communication Mode FCT

Bird et al., 1989 Greg 27 Autism, profound Approx. 15 one-word utterances, SIB (multiple topographies)
MR, and self-inflicted semi-intelligible, rarely
blindness from SIB spontaneous
Jim 36 Autism and profound MR Nonverbal; two manual signs, Aggression and SIB
rarely spontaneous (multiple topographies)
Campbell & Don 8 Autism No formal system Crying, screaming,
Lutzker, 1993 property destruction
Day, Rea, Schussler, Mary 15 Severe MR Few manual signs, one-word SIB
Larsen, & Johnson, verbal labels
1988
Day et al., 1994 Brandi 9 Autism Nonverbal; simple gestures SIB (biting, hitting, pinching)
Dawn 34 Severe MR, seizure Same as above SIB (head hitting)
disorder
Jamie 18 Severe MR, hyperactivity, Same as above Aggression (grabbing
Downloaded by [La Trobe University] at 09:19 25 February 2016

mild spastic quadriplegia and biting)


Durand, 1993 Michelle 5 Moderate MR and Nonverbal; inconsistent head nods Aggression (crying, hair
cerebral palsy and pointing pulling)
Peter 15 Severe MR and Nonverbal; no formal system SIB (head hitting, face
cerebral palsy slapping)
Joshua 3 Severe MR Nonverbal; no formal system Tantrums (crying,
screaming, aggression)
Durand & Kishi, 1987 Tina 20 Profound MR, dual Nonverbal; no formal system Aggression (scratching,
sensory impairments pinching)
John 20 Severe MR, dual Same as above Aggression (hitting,
sensory impairments throwing objects)
Lew 21 Profound MR, dual Same as above SIB (hand biting, face
sensory impairments slapping)
Jim 21 Severe MR, dual sensory Same as above SIB (head banging, hand
impairments biting)
Kim 21 Same as above Same as above SIB (slapping) and
aggression (head butting)
Fisher et al., 1993 Bob Child Profound MR No formal system; reach for SIB (multiple) and other
preferred objects destructive behaviors
Art Same Same as above No formal system Same as above
as above
Jan Same Same as above No formal system; reach for Same as above
as above preferred objects
Abe Same Same as above No formal system Same as above
as above
Gerra et al., 1995 Sara 8 Blindness and severe MR Approx. 30 words, mostly echolalic SIB (multiple topographies)
Horner and Budd, 1985 Male 11 Autism, moderate MR Limited expressive language Grabbing, yelling
and manual signs
Horner & Day (1991) Paul 12 Severe MR, severe hearing Approx. 20 manual signs SIB (head hitting),
impairment, hemiparesis, aggression (kicking, hitting,
cerebral dysgenesis scratching)
Peter 14 Profound MR with autistic Nonverbal; no formal system SIB (face hitting)
tendencies
Mary 27 Autism, severe MR Single spoken words SIB (head hitting)
Horner et al., 1990 David 14 Moderate MR, No formal system Aggression, property
seizure disorder destruction
Hunt, Alwell, & Paula 16 Severe MR Speech limited to single words Variety of “silly” behaviors
Goetz, 1988 and echolalic phrases directed toward peers
Peter 16 Same as above Same as above Same as above
Mary 14 Same as above Same as above Same as above

MR = mental retardation; SIB = self-injurious behavior; MLU = mean length of utterance; PKU = phenylketonuria.

(continued)
210 Mirenda

TABLE 1: (continued) Participant Description


FCT Preintervention Target Behavior
Participant Expressive Treated with
Study Name Age Disability Communication Mode FCT

Hunt et al., 1990 Matt 18 Down syndrome Limited speech with moderate to Variety of disruptive and
severe articulation deficit and aggressive behaviors
MLU = 4 directed toward peers
Katie 18 Same as above Limited speech with severe Same as above
articulation deficit and MLU = 2
Ty 17 Severe MR, Speech with moderate articulation Same as above
seizure disorder deficit and MLU = 6
Lalli et al., 1993 Al 10 Severe-profound MR and No speech; pointed to “yes” and SIB (head banging)
cerebral palsy “no” card in a picture book
Bob 10 Down syndrome No speech, signed for “toilet” Aggression
Mary 14 Severe-profound MR and No formal system SIB (hand scratching)
cerebral palsy
Lalli et al., 1995 Joe 10 Moderate MR Verbal utterances SIB (head and mouth
banging)
Downloaded by [La Trobe University] at 09:19 25 February 2016

Kim 13 Moderate MR, autism Speech 1 gestures Aggression


Northup et al., 1991 Curtis 24 Severe-profound MR, Nonverbal, no formal system Aggression (multiple
legal blindness topographies)
Heidi 21 Severe-profound MR Same as above SIB and aggression
(multiple topographies)
Northup et al., 1994 Mike 5 Severe-profound MR Nonverbal, no formal system SIB (head banging, head
and seizures hitting)
Rebecca 6 Severe-profound MR, Manual signs (unspecified) SIB (head banging, face
Goltz syndrome, vision slapping)
impairment, incomplete
palate and right leg
Heidi 11 Severe-profound MR Single-word utterances Aggression (hair pulling,
hitting, grabbing)
Jane 8 Severe-profound MR, Nonverbal, no formal system SIB (hand mouthing with
blind, nonambulatory tissue breakdown)
Peck et al., 1996 Alexander 22 mo Short-bowel syndrome, Crying, laughing, some babbling, Pulling and chewing on
severe developmental delay no formal system feeding tube and IV lines,
aggression
Cassandra 16 mo Multiple heart and lung Crying, no formal system Pulling on feeding tube and
anomalies, severe cardiac monitor lines, crying,
developmental delay SIB (hair pulling)
Maxwell 4 Severe MR Manual signs and speech for SIB (head hitting, head
“done” and “please” banging), aggression, other
disruptive behaviors
Kevin 2 Failure to thrive Vocalizations, some word Severe noncompliance and
approximations aggression
Sigafoos & Meikle, Dale 8 Autism, moderate- No speech; manual sign “eat,” few Aggression, SIB, property
1996 severe MR black-and-white line drawing destruction, screaming,
symbols crying
Steege et al., 1990 Ann 5 Profound MR, Nonverbal; no formal system SIB (biting)
nonambulatory
Dennis 6 Same as above Same as above Same as above
Wacker et al., 1990 Bobby 7 Autism, profound MR, Nonverbal; no formal system SIB (hand biting), tantrums,
seizure disorder noncompliance, self-
stimulatory behavior
Barb 30 Untreated PKU and Same as above Self-stimulatory behavior
profound MR (rocking and others)
Jim 9 Severe-profound MR Same as above Aggression (slapping, biting)

MR = mental retardation; SIB = self-injurious behavior; MLU = mean length of utterance; PKU = phenylketonuria.
Functional Communication Training and AAC 211

TABLE 2: Assessment Descriptions

Assessment Method Functional Motivation


Study Participant and Setting of Behavior

Bird et al., 1989 Greg Observation and MAS; day Escape from task demands
program classroom
Jim Same as above Escape from task, tangibles
Campbell & Lutzker, 1993 Don A-B-C chart; home and community Tangibles, escape from demands
Day et al., 1988 Mary Functional analysis; school Tangibles, escape
Day et al., 1994 Brandi Interview (FAI), functional analysis; Escape from difficult task and
home tangibles
Dawn Same as above Same as above
Jamie Same as above Same as above
Durand, 1993 Michelle MAS; staff training workshop Attention, escape from 1:1 tasks
Peter Same as above Escape from 1:1 tasks
Joshua Same as above Tangibles during meals
Durand & Kishi, 1987 Tina Interview, MAS; community day program Escape from task demands
Downloaded by [La Trobe University] at 09:19 25 February 2016

John Interview, MAS; institution school Same as above


Lew Same as above Tangibles (toys, food)
Jim Interview, MAS; group home Same as above
Kim Same as above Attention
Fisher et al., 1993 Bob Observation, functional analysis; Escape from nonpreferred tasks
in-patient hospital unit
Art Same as above Same as above
Jan Same as above Tangibles
Abe Same as above Same as above
Gerra et al., 1995 Sara Interview, observation; special Tangibles
education classroom
Horner & Budd, 1985 Male Observation of antecedents and Tangibles
consequences; special education
classroom
Horner & Day, 1991 Paul Interview (FAI), functional analysis; Escape from difficult tasks
group home
Peter Same as above Same as above
Mary Same as above Same as above
Horner et al., 1990 David Interview (FAI), functional analysis; Escape from difficult task
special education classroom
Hunt et al., 1988 Paula Interviews, observations, analysis of Attention/social interaction from peers
consequences; regular and special
education classrooms and
community sites
Peter Same as above Same as above
Mary Same as above Same as above
Hunt et al., 1990 Matt Observation of antecedents and Attention/social interaction from peers
consequences; regular and special
education settings in high school,
community sites
Katie Same as above Same as above
Ty Same as above Same as above
Lalli et al., 1993 Al Interview, scatter plot, observation of Escape from task demands
antecedents and consequences, (transition) and attention
descriptive analysis; special
education classrooms
Bob Same as above Attention
Mary Same as above Attention

MAS = Motivation Assessment Scale (Durand & Crimmins, 1992); FAI = Functional Analysis Interview (O’Neill et al., 1990).
(continued)
212 Mirenda

TABLE 2: (continued) Assessment Descriptions

Assessment Method Functional Motivation


Study Participant and Setting of Behavior

Lalli et al., 1995 Joe Interviews, observations, Escape from tasks


functional analysis; in-patient
hospital setting
Kim Same as above Same as above
Northup et al., 1991 Curtis Interviews, questionnaire, functional Escape from difficult tasks
analysis; out-patient hospital clinic
Heidi Same as above Tangibles
Northup et al., 1994 Mike Interview, observations, record Escape from difficult tasks
review, functional analysis;
special education classroom
Rebecca Same as above Escape from difficult tasks and attention
Heidi Same as above Escape from difficult tasks
Jane Same as above Attention
Downloaded by [La Trobe University] at 09:19 25 February 2016

Peck et al., 1996 Alexander Interview, antecedent assessment, Attention


functional analysis; in-patient
hospital setting
Cassandra Interview, antecedent assessment; Same as above
in-patient hospital setting
Maxwell Interview, antecedent assessment; Escape from task demands
integrated preschool
Kevin Interview, antecedent assessment, Same as above
functional analysis; in-patient
hospital setting
Sigafoos and Meikle, 1996 Dale Functional analysis; special Attention and tangibles
education classroom
Steege et al., 1990 Ann Interview, functional analysis; Escape from unpleasant tasks
in-patient hospital setting (grooming)
Dennis Same as above Same as above
Wacker et al., 1990 Bobby Functional analysis; Tangibles
in-patient hospital setting
Barb Same as above Sensory
Jim Same as above Escape from unpleasant task

MAS = Motivation Assessment Scale (Durand & Crimmins, 1992); FAI = Functional Analysis Interview (O’Neill et al., 1990).

In the “attention” condition, the individual is given cific alternative hypotheses regarding the function of
access to a preferred activity (e.g., a toy) and is pro- the behavior. For example, Horner and Day (1991)
vided with attention each time the target behavior used a variation of the escape condition with a par-
occurs (e.g., the assessor might say, “Don’t do that”). ticipant named “Peter” (see Tables 1 and 2). They
Behaviors that occur during this condition have an presented Peter with a difficult task and provided
attention-getting function (i.e., positive reinforcement). assistance each time he engaged in SIB (the target
In the “escape” condition, the individual is given a dif- behavior). Because his behavior increased in this con-
ficult task to complete and, each time the target dition, it was assessed as being motivated by a
behavior occurs, both attention and the task are request for assistance.
removed. In this case, the occurrence of behavior Several authors have demonstrated that brief forms
suggests that the function is to escape from tasks or of functional analysis can be used effectively and
associated demands (i.e., negative reinforcement). accurately in out-patient clinic or classroom settings in
Finally, in the “alone” condition, no demands are which the time available for assessment may be lim-
made and the individual is provided with no toys and ited. For example, Northup et al. (1991) created ana-
no attention. Behaviors that occur during this condi- logue conditions that were 10 minutes in duration and
tion are likely to be sensory motivated, since no envi- rapidly alternated them to produce differing frequen-
ronmental consequences are provided. Variations of cies of the behavior. These researchers were able to
these four conditions might also be used to test spe- complete clinic out-patient assessments within 90
Functional Communication Training and AAC 213

minutes using this strategy and went on to design pared the MAS with both functional analysis and an A-
successful FCT/AAC interventions based on the out- B-C chart approach to assessment. In addition, evi-
comes. Similarly, Northup et al. (1994) combined dence for the validity of the factor structure (i.e., the
teacher interviews, classroom observations, and func- four motivational categories) of the MAS was provided
tional analyses to analyze the behaviors of four par- by Bihm, Kienlen, Ness, and Poindexter (1991) on a
ticipants with whom FCT/AAC was later employed in sample of 118 persons with severe or profound devel-
a school setting. These studies and others support the opmental disabilities. However, other studies have
use of an abbreviated functional analysis in applied questioned the inter-rater reliability (Sigafoos & Kerr,
settings in order to plan communication interventions 1994; Zarcone, Rodgers, Iwata, Rourke, & Dorsey,
for individuals with severe challenging behavior 1991) and factor structure of the instrument (Singh &
(Cooper & Harding, 1993). Donatelli, 1993). Given these discrepant results, the
MAS is best used as one component of a multiele-
Motivation Assessment Scale (MAS) ment assessment battery to assess the function(s) of
behavior across a variety of settings (Kearney, 1994).
The MAS is a 16-item questionnaire that can be used
to assess the functions or motivations of behavior prob- Observations and A-B-C Charting
lems. The 16 items are organized into the four func-
tional categories described previously for the functional Nine of the 20 studies reviewed reported the use of
Downloaded by [La Trobe University] at 09:19 25 February 2016

analysis, namely, tangibles, attention, escape, and systematic observations or A-B-C charting as one
sensory. The MAS asks questions about the likelihood component of behavioral assessment. In fact, obser-
of a problem behavior occurring in a variety of condi- vation is also implicit in the functional analysis when
tions. Each question is rated on a 0 to 6 scale, where the individual is exposed to the analogue conditions.
0 = never, 3 = half the time, and 6 = always. Examples Usually, observations are recorded via anecdotal
of questions related to each of the categories include: records that describe the antecedents (A), behaviors
(B), and/or consequences (C) that occurred during
• Tangible function: “Does the behavior occur when each behavioral episode. Alternatively, these three
you take away a favorite toy, food, or activity?” components can be recorded on a three-column form
• Attention function: “Does the behavior seem to (called an A-B-C chart), rather than narratively. From
occur in response to your talking to other persons these descriptive analyses, hypotheses are formed
in the room?” regarding the functional motivation of the behavior
• Escape function: “Does the behavior occur follow- (see Donnellan, Mirenda, Mesaros, & Fassbender,
ing a request to perform a difficult task?” 1984). For example, it might be observed that a stu-
• Sensory function: “Would the behavior occur dent tantrums (behavior) whenever she is asked to
repeatedly, in the same way, for very long periods stop a preferred activity (antecedent), and that the
of time if no one was around (e.g., rocking back usual response of the teacher is to allow continued
and forth for over an hour)?” access to the activity for a period of time (conse-
quence). If this pattern were observed repeatedly over
As should be evident from these examples, the several days, it would suggest that the behavior is tan-
MAS is, in effect, an interview form of the functional gibly motivated (i.e., the student engages in the behav-
analysis described previously. In the end, the scores ior to gain access to the preferred activity). In order for
for the four questions related to each category are observational assessment to be accurate and useful,
tallied and ranked in order of magnitude. The highest it should be conducted over several days in a variety
scoring category is considered to be the primary func- of contexts and situations. Because no reliable and
tion of the behavior. When two or more categories valid protocols for completing observational assess-
are given high scores, the behavior may have multi- ments or A-B-C charts have been developed to date,
ple motivations. For example, a behavior might be these assessment techniques are usually used in con-
used by an individual to both get attention and to junction with other assessment approaches.
escape from a difficult task.
A number of studies have investigated the reliabil- Other Assessment Strategies
ity and validity of the MAS, with somewhat mixed
results. The original study by Durand and Crimmins Additional assessment strategies reported in the 21
(1988) indicated excellent inter-rater reliability (i.e., studies reviewed were a written questionnaire, record
Pearson correlation coefficients ranged from .80–.95) review, and scatter plot analysis (Touchette, Mac-
and test–retest reliability (Pearson r = .89–.98.). This Donald, & Langer, 1985). The latter strategy requires
study also compared teachers’ ratings on the MAS an observer to record zero, low-, and high-frequency
with functional analysis outcomes for eight individuals occurrences of the problem behavior on a grid in
with SIB, with identical conclusions for all participants. which the columns represent successive days and the
Similar positive outcomes were reported by Crawford, rows represent time segments (e.g., hours, half hours,
Brockel, Schauss, and Miltenberger (1992), who com- etc.). Visual inspection is then used to identify the
214 Mirenda

time segments (and, by association, the activities that behavior, 13 (81%) were escape motivated, 8 (50%)
occurred during those segments) characterized by were attention motivated, and 5 (31%) were tangibly
high-frequency occurrences of the target behavior. motivated (percentages total >100% because some
Other assessment strategies that were not used in aggression was multiply motivated). Thus, it is clear
the studies reviewed but that might be useful include that no single behavior was used for a single function
the index card observation and recording format across individuals, nor vice versa. This provides fur-
described by Carr et al. (1994) and the assessment ther support for the use of comprehensive behavior
process described by Meyer and Evans (1989). None assessments that are individualized in nature.
of these techniques are supported with data regard-
ing their reliability or validity. INTERVENTION STRATEGIES

Assessment Settings AAC Techniques

For 67% of the participants, assessment was con- A variety of AAC techniques were employed across
ducted in one or more regular education, special edu- the 52 individuals included in the review sample. Table
cation, day program, group home, or family home set- 3 summarizes the AAC techniques used, the function-
tings. The remaining 17 assessments were conducted ally equivalent messages taught, the responses to the
in out-patient or in-patient hospital settings, so that the messages, and the outcomes of the intervention for
Downloaded by [La Trobe University] at 09:19 25 February 2016

experimenters could control extraneous variables that each of these individuals.


might have affected the outcomes of the research. Ten individuals (19%) were taught to use more than
The variety of natural settings in which FCT/AAC one AAC technique during intervention. The vast
assessments have occurred is encouraging, in that majority of the participants (92%) were taught to use
they represent a broad array of the types of environ- communication techniques that were either no- or low-
ments in which individuals with developmental dis- technology in nature. The most commonly used tech-
abilities typically live, work, learn, and play. This sug- nique was manual signing, which was used with 19
gests that the assessment strategies summarized (37%) of the individuals involved. Gestures or com-
here are potentially usable across a wide variety of munication books were each taught to eight partici-
environments. pants (15%). Microswitches with single-message
tapes, tangible symbols, and cards with printed words
Functional Motivation of Behavior were each used with six individuals (12%). Other AAC
techniques included voice output communication aids
The results of the assessment processes employed (three participants), line drawings or photographs (two
across the 52 participants are summarized in Table 2. participants), a simple attention-getting device (a ball
For 10 of the 52 participants (19%), two functions of with a bell in it, one participant), and a Canon Com-
behavior were identified. The most common function municator M™ (one participant). With few exceptions
of behavior was escape, which occurred for 29 par- (e.g., Durand, 1993), little or no information was pro-
ticipants (56%). The activities from which participants vided regarding how and why a particular AAC tech-
were motivated to escape included those that were nique was selected. It is likely that manual signing
apparently difficult, nonpreferred, or perceived as and gestures were the most commonly used tech-
unpleasant (e.g., grooming activities). For these 29 niques (one or both were used with 50% of the par-
participants, the “message” of their behavior was per- ticipants) because most of the participants were
ceived to be “I don’t want to ______.” Equal numbers ambulatory and these techniques are imminently
of participants engaged in behavior that was attention portable. Tangible symbols were used exclusively with
motivated and/or tangibly motivated (16 participants participants who were blind or dual sensory impaired,
or 31% each). These participants used their behavior although not all participants with these disabilities
to communicate “Pay attention to me” (attention) and were provided with tangible symbols. Communication
“I want ______” messages (tangibles), respectively. books were used with all six participants for whom
Finally, one of the FCT/AAC participants in the stud- social interaction with peers was a primary motivator
ies reviewed engaged in behavior that was primarily of behavior. In many of the remaining cases, it
sensory motivated (“Barb” in Wacker et al., 1990). appeared that AAC technique selection was quite
Examination of Tables 1 and 2 together suggest arbitrary, or was determined by equipment availabil-
that the behavior-function relationships across partic- ity rather than participant need. Durand (1990) and
ipants were quite varied. For example, consider the Carr et al. (1994) provided discussions of systematic
patterns of the two most frequently occurring behav- decision making in this regard.
iors, SIB and aggression. Of the 29 participants who
engaged in SIB, 17 (58%) used this behavior to Communication Messages and Responses
escape from tasks, 11 (38%) used it to gain access to
preferred tangibles, and 7 (24%) used it to get atten- In all of the studies reviewed, the messages taught
tion (percentages total >100% because some SIB was and the responses to those messages were directly
multiply motivated). Of the 16 instances of aggressive related to the assessed function of the challenging
Functional Communication Training and AAC 215

TABLE 3: Intervention Strategies

AAC Technique Response to


Study Participant Used Message Taught Message Outcome

Bird et al., 1989 Greg Tangible symbol “Break” message assigned Brief break from task, ISR to near-zero;
to token access to self-restraint 20-wk FU
and preferred reinforcers
Jim Manual sign “Break,” “work,” “eat,” Brief break from task or ISR to <5/wk; 1-yr FU
“music,” “bathroom” access to requested
item/activity
Campbell & Don Manual sign 1 gesture “Please” 1 point to item; Access to requested item ISR to near-0; no FU
Lutzker, 1993 “Please eat;” “Please drink”
Day et al., 1988 Mary Gesture (hand clap) + Tangibles: “Pay attention Tangibles: access to ISR from a mean SIB
speech to me” (message assigned requested item; escape: rate of 53/day to a mean
to hand clap) 1 manual sign brief break from task of 6/day over a 2-mo FU
or speech for desired item;
escape: “no” (verbal)
Day et al., 1994 Brandi Manual sign 1 speech Tangibles: “want” sign 1 Tangibles: praise, access ISR once behavior
point; escape: “Go” (speech) to preferred items; escape: related to the function
Downloaded by [La Trobe University] at 09:19 25 February 2016

brief break from task was taught; 1-yr


anecdotal FU
Dawn Manual sign and card Tangibles: “want” sign; Same as above Same as above
with printed word escape: card with “help”
on it
Jamie Same as above Tangibles: “want” sign; Same as above Same as above,
escape: card with “break” even after task
on it demands increased
Durand, 1993 Michelle Wolf™ and head “I want to be with the group” End of 1:1 task and ISR to 89% of
pointer access to group activity baseline
Peter Introtalker™ “I want a break” Brief break from task ISR to 78% of baseline
Joshua Wolf™ “I want more” More food, drink items ISR to 95% of baseline
Durand & Tina Tangible symbol “I want a break” written Brief break from ISR to 8% of baseline
Kishi, 1987 on token task at 1-mo FU
John Same as above Same as above Same as above ISR to zero at 1-mo FU
Lew Same as above Request function assigned Access to favorite NR; staff did not
to symbol tangibles implement program
Jim Gesture (raised hand) Request function assigned Access to favorite ISR to near-zero at
to raised hand tangibles 9-mo FU
Kim Same as above Initial meaning assigned to Attention from staff NR at 1-mo FU; GR to
gesture: “Come spend some 10% of baseline at 9-mo
time with me,” later: FU after second message
“Can I help you?” assigned to gesture
Fisher et al., 1993 Bob Manual signs “Go” Brief break from task NR, even with FCT 1
punishment
Art Same as above “Go” Same as above ISR only when FCT 1
punishment was used
Jan Same as above “More” Access to preferred toys Same as above, but
poor generalization
across settings
Abe Same as above “More” Same as above NR, even with FCT 1
punishment
Gerra et al., 1995 Sara Communication board Choices: lie on the mat, get Access to requested activity GR of behavior over 114
with 7 tangible symbols a hug, eat peanut butter days; anecdotal FU
(objects, textured with a cracker, drink juice,
materials) for preferred eat cereal, listen to music,
activities and bounce on a large
rubber ball
Horner & Male Manual signs “juice,” “timer,” “choose,” Access to requested item ISR once instruction
Budd, 1985 “bottle,” “folder” occurred in natural
setting (classroom)

ISR = immediate substantial reduction; FU = follow-up; NR = no reduction; GR = gradual reduction.

(continued)
216 Mirenda

TABLE 3: (continued) Intervention Strategies


AAC Technique Response to
Study Participant Used Message Taught Message Outcome

Horner & Paul Manual signs Sentence (“I want to go, Brief break from task ISR to near-zero only
Day, 1991 please”) and single word after single word was
(“break”) taught
Peter Same as above “Help” Task assistance provided ISR to near-zero
on an FR3 and FR1 only during FR1
schedule schedule
Mary Card with printed word “BREAK” Brief break from task ISR only when break was
immediately or after immediate
20-sec delay
Horner David Canon “Help, please” (letter-by- Assistance with task from NR when letter-by-letter
et al., 1990 Communicator M™ letter spelling vs. single- teacher (reduced task spelling required; ISR to
with alphabet letter message key) difficulty) near-zero with single
keys and single- message key
message key
Hunt et al., 1988 Paula Conversation book with Books used to establish Social interaction during ISR to near-zero after
Downloaded by [La Trobe University] at 09:19 25 February 2016

colored photographs and maintain topics during conversational interactions conversation training with
and line drawings conversation with peers book
Peter Same as above 1 Same as above Same as above Same as above
written phrases
and labels
Mary Same as Paula above Same as above Same as above ISR to zero after
conversation training
Hunt et al., 1990 Matt Conversation book Books used to establish and Social interaction during ISR to near-zero in
with colored maintain topics during conversational interactions untrained settings
photographs, line conversation with peers
drawings, written
words, and phrases
Katie Same as above Same as above ISR to zero in untrained
settings
Ty Same as above Same as above ISR to near-zero in
untrained settings
Lalli et al., 1993 Al Photographs in a Request/choice re: photo Praise and access to ISR to zero; 4-wk FU
picture booklet touched chosen object/activity
Bob Gesture (hand/arm “Hello, pay attention to Brief 1:1 attention Same as above
wave) and tangible me” (meaning assigned from teacher
symbol (toy) to gesture 1 toy)
Mary Gesture (tap on Same as above Same as above ISR to near-zero;
teacher’s arm) 4-wk FU
Lalli et al., 1995 Joe Card with printed word “BREAK” Brief removal of task GR to zero even after
task demands increased
Kim Gesture (head shake) “No” Same as above ISR to near-zero even
after task demands
increased
Northup et al., 1991 Curtis Manual sign “Please” Brief removal of task ISR to near-zero even
after task demands
increased
Heidi Same as above “Please” Access to preferred items ISR to zero
Northup Mike Microswitch positioned “I’d like a break now, Brief break from task, ISR To near zero;
et al., 1994 on shirt, attached to a please” (taped message) access to pillow and music 17-mo FU
tape recorder with faded later to “Please”
single-message tape, (manual sign)
and manual sign
Rebecca Manual signs “No,” “help,” “more” “No” = brief break from task; GR to near-zero after 9
“help” or “more” = assistance mo; 17-mo FU
or continued attention
Heidi Same as above “Please” Brief break from task ISR but not to acceptable
levels for teacher;
intervention terminated

ISR = immediate substantial reduction; FU = follow-up; NR = no reduction; GR = gradual reduction.


(continued)
Functional Communication Training and AAC 217

TABLE 3: (continued) Intervention Strategies


AAC Technique Response to
Study Participant Used Message Taught Message Outcome

Jane Microswitch and tape “Please come here” Brief teacher attention ISR to near-zero,
recorder with single- 17-mo FU
message tape
Peck et al., 1996 Alex Microswitch with “Somebody come here, Brief attention from adult ISR to near-zero,
pretaped message please” 6-mo FU
Cass Ball with bell “Come here” (meaning Brief attention from adult ISR to zero; 2-mo FU
assigned to sound of bell)
Maxwell Manual sign and “Done” Brief break from task ISR to near-zero; no FU
card with printed word
Kevin Speech or card with “Play” Brief break from task GR to low levels over
printed word 6-mo FU
Sigafoos & Dale Gesture (tap adult’s “Pay attention to me” Gesture = brief 1:1 ISR to near-zero;
Meikle, 1996 hand) and three black- (meaning assigned to attention from teacher; 3-mo FU
and-white line drawings gesture); “food,” “drink,” point to line drawing =
“toy” (line drawings) provision of requested item
Downloaded by [La Trobe University] at 09:19 25 February 2016

Steege Ann Microswitch device “Stop!” Brief break from undesired ISR to near-zero;
et al., 1990 with single prerecorded activity behavior returned at
message 1-yr FU because
intervention not
implemented at home
Dennis Same as above Same as above Same as above ISR to zero; rare SIB at
6-mo FU
Wacker Bobby Gesture (touch to Request function Access to favorite yellow ISR to zero with
et al., 1990 chin with finger) assigned to gesture bowl FCT 1 time-out package
Barb Microswitch and tape “I’m tired of rocking; Access to preferred ISR to near-zero
recorder with single somebody give me activities (rocking chair
taped message something to do” and exercise bike)
Jim Manual sign “Please,” “eat” “Please” = brief break ISR to near-zero with
from task; “eat” = FCT 1 graduated
brief break 1 cracker guidance

ISR = immediate substantial reduction; FU = follow-up; NR = no reduction; GR = gradual reduction.

behavior. As noted previously, this is perhaps the participants displayed escape-motivated behavior,
most critical component for an FCT/AAC intervention and nine different basic messages were taught as
to be successful and has been referred to as communicative alternatives: “break,” “go,” “no,”
“response match” (Durand, Berotti, & Weiner, 1993). “done,” “stop,” “please,” “help,” “play,” and choice. In
The new communicative behavior must serve the some cases, the polite (e.g., “Help, please”) and/or
same function as the challenging behavior in order for sentence forms (e.g., “I want a break”) of these mes-
the latter to be reduced. Day, Horner, and O’Neill sages were used. The first five of these messages are
(1994) demonstrated this with three individuals clearly related to the escape function of behavior, in
(“Brandi,” “Dawn,” and “Jamie”) who used a variety of that they all signal the need for a pause during a task.
behaviors for two functions: to escape from tasks and Subjects who produced these five messages were all
to obtain desired tangibles. After each individual was provided with a brief (usually, 30–90 seconds) break
taught a functionally equivalent communication mode from the task at hand. The “help” message was used
for one function, levels of problem behavior in situations where the assessment indicated the
decreased for that function but not for the other. Only behavior to be a request for assistance more than a
after separate communication forms were taught for request for a break per se, and assistance was pro-
both functions was problem behavior reduced to vided in response. Finally, the “please,” “play,” and
acceptable levels. This study provided the first empir- choice messages for escape-motivated behaviors
ical evidence of the importance of response matching were used in situations where the assessment indi-
for FCT/AAC applications. cated a need to “kill two birds with one stone” because
It is interesting to note that, in the studies reviewed, of multiply motivated behaviors. For example, the
there was considerable variation regarding the spe- choice message was taught to “Al,” a 10-year-old boy
cific messages taught within functional categories, with severe handicaps who engaged in SIB during
depending on the specific contexts in which the chal- transitions between classes at school (Lalli, Browder,
lenging behaviors occurred. For example, 29 of the Mace, & Brown, 1993). Assessment indicated that the
218 Mirenda

behavior was Al’s attempt to escape from the transi- they provide a valuable and broad-based empirical
tion, apparently because he was unsure about the foundation for the use of this approach. Other studies
activity to follow, while also getting attention from the focused on specific aspects of the technique and
teacher. He was taught to choose his next scheduled sought to explore these within an experimental para-
activity by pointing to a photograph in a communica- digm. From these studies, we can understand the
tion book at the beginning of the transition period and principles of response mastery that are critical to suc-
was then praised and provided with this activity if he cess. Durand et al. (1993) described response mas-
completed the transition without engaging in SIB. tery as “the ability of the trained communicative
Thus, the choice message alleviated his uncertainty response or responses to successfully and efficiently
about the next activity and, presumably, reduced his produce the desired outcome” (p. 324). Durand et al.
motivation to escape while also providing him with (1993) described four main components of response
attention for appropriate behavior. Teaching a request mastery: response success, response efficiency,
or choice-making alternative for escape-motivated response acceptability, and response recognizability.
behavior may have the added advantage of increased In the sections that follow, these four principles and
social acceptability for caregivers, in that the individ- selected examples to illustrate them from the studies
ual is making a “request to” rather than a “request reviewed will be presented in summary form.
from” a task (O’Neill & Reichle, 1993).
Sixteen participants engaged in attention-motivated Response Success
Downloaded by [La Trobe University] at 09:19 25 February 2016

behavior and were taught a variety of alternatives


designed to elicit attention. These included the basic, This principle states that an alternative communi-
polite, or sentence forms of “pay attention to me,” cation behavior must be successful in obtaining the
“come here,” and “more,” in addition to “I want to be desired response in order for a reduction in challeng-
with the group” and the use of communication books ing behavior to occur. If the individual uses his or her
during conversational interactions with peers. Either new communication behavior in place of challenging
brief or extended attention/social interaction was pro- behavior and no one “listens,” the challenging behav-
vided in all cases, depending on the context. Again, ior will not be reduced. This principle was illustrated
the specific message appeared to depend on the con- in many of the studies in which FCT/AAC had no long-
text in which the behavior occurred. An additional 16 term effect for one or more participants (e.g., Durand
participants were taught FCT/AAC alternatives to tan- & Kishi, 1987; Northup et al., 1994; Steege et al.,
gible-motivated behavior. In some of these cases, a 1990). In each of these cases, caregivers were unable
generic message (e.g., “want,” “please,” “more,” and “I or unwilling to respond to the new communication
want something to do”) was taught, and caregivers behavior in accordance with its function. The result
were told which preferred tangible item to provide in was that the FCT/AAC intervention had either no
response (e.g., “Heidi” in Northup et al., 1991). In other impact on the challenging behavior or had an imme-
cases, the response to the generic message was diate impact but had returned by follow-up. This may
access to a variety of preferred items or activities from be directly related to the degree of control or choice
which the individual was allowed to make a selection that is generally available to the individuals involved
(see Day et al., 1994). Still other studies involved (see Carr et al. [1994] and Durand et al. [1993] for
teaching the use of specific vocabulary items corre- extensive discussions of this concern). The fact is that
sponding to preferred items (Horner & Budd, 1985) or FCT/AAC interventions shift control from caregivers to
of multiple-message communication choice displays the individuals they support as the latter learn to ask
(e.g., Gerra, Dorfman, Plaue, Schlackman, & Work- for preferred items, attention, breaks from work, and
man, 1995; Sigafoos & Meikle, 1996). In all cases, the so forth. Although this issue requires further empirical
response was access to one or more preferred items attention, it would appear that, in settings in which
or activities. Finally, one participant who engaged in caregivers are reluctant to share control or in which
sensory-motivated behavior (self-stimulatory rocking) choice making is discouraged, FCT/AAC interventions
was taught to ask for preferred activities using a sin- are likely to be less successful (Durand et al., 1993).
gle-message tape and a microswitch. This successful
intervention was apparently designed to reduce the Response Efficiency
sensory “boredom” that caused her to engage in self-
stimulatory behavior during “down time,” although this This issue has also been deemed the “principle of
was not stated explicitly (Wacker et al., 1990). response effectiveness” (Beukelman & Mirenda,
1992; Mirenda, 1995, 1996). People communicate in
Principles of Response Mastery the most efficient and effective manner available to
them at any given point in time. This means that an
Many of the published FCT/AAC studies were alternative communication behavior must be at least
designed to replicate the efficacy of the original Carr as easy to produce and at least as effective as is the
and Durand (1985) study with individuals who had behavior it is meant to replace. Several studies have
specific disabilities or who required AAC. As such, explored one or more dimensions of this complex
Functional Communication Training and AAC 219

issue. The first component of efficiency is physical lenging behavior. For example, if a child receives an
effort. In a study by Horner, Sprague, O’Brien, and entire cookie each time he or she tantrums, but only
Heathfield (1990), “David,” an adolescent whose receives a piece of cookie when he asks for it using
behaviors included escape-motivated aggression, the new FCT/AAC behavior, it is unlikely that the new
property destruction, and yelling, was taught two behavior will replace the old over the long term.
methods for producing a message to elicit assistance, A final consideration related to response efficiency
using a Canon Communicator M.™ The first method is the delay between the FCT/AAC behavior and the
involved having him type the message “Help, please” response to it. Horner and Day (1991) provided evi-
using letter-by-letter spelling. The second required dence of the impact of delay in a study involving
him to simply push a single key on the Canon™ to “Mary,” an adult with autism who engaged in escape-
produce the preprogrammed message “Help, please.” motivated aggression. She was taught to hand an
In both cases, the response was to provide him with index card with the word “BREAK” on it to a teacher
assistance for a difficult task. David continued to during difficult tasks. In one condition, she received a
engage in challenging behaviors even after he was brief break after a 1-second delay, while in the con-
taught to type the message in a letter-by-letter fash- trasting condition the teacher gave her a break after a
ion, but the behaviors were reduced to zero once he 20-second delay. As predicted, Mary asked for breaks
was provided with the preprogrammed alternative. frequently and her aggression decreased dramatically
This effect was replicated in a second study (Horner during the 1-second delay condition, but she reverted
Downloaded by [La Trobe University] at 09:19 25 February 2016

and Day, 1991) with “Paul,” a child who was taught to to aggression and stopped asking for breaks when the
produce a manually signed sentence (“I want to go, 20-second delay condition was initiated. In this case,
please”) versus a single word (“break”) for escape- the physical effort required of Mary was identical
motivated behavior. These examples illustrate the regardless of the delay. Nevertheless, the 20-second
importance of selecting FCT/AAC behaviors that delay caused her challenging behavior to be more effi-
require less physical effort than do the challenging cient than the FCT/AAC behavior, with the logical out-
behaviors they are meant to replace. For Paul and come. In combination, the schedule of reinforcement
David, the single-message and single-sign options and the delay examples provide evidence that, at least
worked to reduce their behaviors because they were during the initial learning phase of intervention, the
easier to produce than the alternatives. response requested through FCT/AAC be provided
A second dimension of response efficiency involves each time and immediately in order for the intervention
the density or schedule of reinforcement. Horner and to be successful.
Day (1991) illustrated this through “Peter,” an adoles- It is important to note that the issue of delay is one
cent who engaged in escape-motivated SIB. Like that can and must be dealt with over time, especially
David in the above example, he was taught to sign for learners whose behavior is escape motivated. Oth-
“help” during difficult tasks to receive assistance. erwise, once the FCT/AAC escape behavior that
Sometimes, Peter received assistance only after he results in a break from the task has been learned, the
signed “help” three times (FR3 schedule of reinforce- individual may use it continuously, thus avoiding all
ment). At other times, he received assistance each work. If this problem occurs, increasing delays or task
time he signed “help” (FR1 schedule). Not surprisingly, demands can be systematically inserted between the
a reduction in challenging behavior occurred only dur- communication behavior and the response. For exam-
ing the FR1 condition. During the other condition, ple, Lalli, Casey, and Kates (1995) taught “Joe” and
Peter initially signed “help” quite frequently, and there “Kim” to complete increasing numbers of steps in a
was an initial reduction in his behavior. However, he 16-step task prior to getting the break they requested.
seemed to realize quite quickly that signing three times The frequency of their SIB remained at near-zero as
actually required more effort than did SIB, and he the task demands were gradually increased. Other
stopped signing after a few attempts. researchers have also taught participants with escape-
Related to the schedule of reinforcement is the third motivated behavior to tolerate increased task demands
aspect of efficiency, duration of reinforcement . A over time (e.g., Bird, Dores, Moniz, & Robinson, 1989;
recent study by Peck et al. (1996) demonstrated that Day et al., 1994; Fisher et al., 1993; Northup et al.,
one way of maximizing the likelihood that a learner will 1991). The key to success in this regard appears to be
use a new FCT/AAC behavior instead of an old chal- to increase task demands systematically only after the
lenging behavior is to make the former more “worth- new FCT/AAC behavior has been well learned.
while” than the latter, at least during initial instruction.
In the Peck et al. study, the quality and/or amount of Response Acceptability
reinforcement was manipulated to demonstrate that a
learner is likely to engage in whichever behavior Another aspect of response mastery involves the
(FCT/AAC or challenging) will result in more or better acceptability of the FCT/AAC behavior to others, since
results. Thus, it is important to ensure that the amount this is likely to affect whether or not they respond to it.
and quality of reinforcement must be at least as great Clearly, if the new behavior does not receive the
for the FCT/AAC behavior as it is/was for the chal- desired response, the old, challenging behavior that
220 Mirenda

does receive the response will persist. One research his behavior over a 2-year period. However, Hal’s
study has provided evidence of this aspect to date. behavior returned during the third year because his
Durand and Kishi (1987) described an intervention for new teacher did not understand what he was saying
“Kim,” a woman with dual sensory impairments and and thus did not provide assistance when he asked for
severe handicaps whose behaviors included SIB and it. A brief intervention to improve his articulation skills
aggression, both of which were attention motivated. again resulted in a dramatic reduction in the chal-
They taught her to “sign for adult attention” (p. 6) but lenging behavior, which was maintained 1 year later.
their report provided no details about either the specific Although this FCT study did not involve AAC, it is one
sign or the message. In a later article, Durand et al. of the few published reports that illustrates the impor-
(1993) provided additional details, noting that Kim had tance of response recognizability.
been taught to raise her hand for attention and that Several of the studies reviewed appeared to incor-
staff had been told that her raised hand meant “Come porate this principle into the selection of an FCT/AAC
spend some time with me.” Despite the fact that Kim technique, symbol, and/or message, while others did
learned to raise her hand, the intervention was unsuc- not. One report included the use of voice output com-
cessful because, staff argued, they “were very busy munication devices for three participants. Most of the
and did not have time to respond each time she raised studies produced by researchers from the University
her hand” (Durand et al., 1993, p. 327). Subsequently, of Iowa incorporated single-message tapes activated
the message assigned to the raised hand was by microswitches as AAC techniques for one or more
Downloaded by [La Trobe University] at 09:19 25 February 2016

changed to “Can I help you?” and resulted in a rather of the participants. All of these examples ensured
dramatic decrease in Kim’s challenging behavior (see response recognizability through the use of specific
Durand & Kishi, 1987). The second message was spoken messages. Other studies used printed output
more acceptable to staff in that they could now pay from a Canon Communicator M,™ printed captions
attention to her while still engaging in the chores with under photographs, or printed words on cards or
which they were occupied. This is a familiar issue to tokens to ensure recognizability. Finally, a few stud-
many in the AAC community who have had the expe- ies used photographic or line drawing symbols to rep-
rience of providing a child with a negative message on resent messages. Presumably, these picture symbols
a communication display (e.g., “Leave me alone” or were either quite transparent or were accompanied by
“#$&*!@”), only to have the child ignored or even pun- written labels to ensure intelligibility, although this was
ished for using the message because of its content! not always stated explicitly.
Similarly, Kim’s example illustrates the need for care- Other studies used unaided techniques such as man-
ful selection of the FCT message in terms of its accept- ual signs, tangible symbols, gestures, or attention-get-
ability to others in the prevailing environment in order ting devices, all of which ran the risk of being misun-
for the intervention to be successful. derstood by unfamiliar respondents. Perhaps most
problematic with regard to iconicity were those inter-
Response Recognizability ventions in which gestures or manual signs were
assigned idiosyncratic meanings, depending on the
Basically, this principle says that if the new com- function of the behavior to which they were related.
municative behavior is not easily recognized by those These included arbitrary tangible symbols or gestures
who should respond to it, the desired response will not that meant “I want,” a raised hand that meant “Come
occur and the challenging behavior will not be spend some time with me,” opaque tangible symbols for
reduced. In AAC terms, this principle is related to the specific preferred activities, various gestures (e.g., a
degree of iconicity of a symbol or message. At one hand clap) that meant “Pay attention to me,” a ball with
end of the iconicity continuum are transparent sym- a bell in it that was meant to signal “Come here,” and
bols or messages, the meanings of which are highly the manual sign “please” to ask for a break from a task.
“guessable.” At the other end are opaque symbols or All of these are quite low in terms of response recog-
messages whose relationships to their referents are nizability, especially to unfamiliar caregivers, and their
not obvious and may be quite arbitrary. In the middle effectiveness might be expected to erode over time.
of the iconicity continuum are translucent symbols or Unfortunately, no studies have investigated the issue of
messages, which are not readily guessable without response recognizability specifically for FCT/AAC inter-
additional information (Beukelman & Mirenda, 1992; ventions, so empirical support for the use of specific
Reichle, York, & Sigafoos, 1991). A study by Durand communication techniques in this regard is not avail-
and Carr (1991) illustrated the importance of mes- able. It stands to reason, though, that the more trans-
sage iconicity through a child with autism named parent the symbol or message, the more successful a
“Hal,” who exhibited violent tantrums that were FCT/AAC intervention will be over the long term.
escape motivated. Because he could speak, he was
taught to say “I don’t understand” in order to elicit INSTRUCTIONAL TECHNIQUES
assistance from the teacher during difficult tasks at
school, thereby removing his need to escape. The The term “instructional techniques” is used here to
intervention was successful in dramatically reducing refer to the (a) strategies used to teach new commu-
Functional Communication Training and AAC 221

nicative behaviors, (b) consequences applied to chal- redirection or response blocking (especially of SIB
lenging behaviors if they occur, and (c) teaching and aggression), as recommended by Durand (1990).
schedules and settings used for instruction. A wide Other consequences included guided compliance
variety of instructional techniques were used in the (i.e., hand-over-hand assistance to complete a task),
FCT/AAC studies reviewed. These were largely brief time-out (e.g., 30–60 seconds), withholding
dependent on the focus of a particular study. For desired reinforcers, and mild verbal reprimands. Gen-
example, some studies were designed to compare erally, it is recommended that the least intrusive and
the relative efficacy of two types of consequences most neutral response that ensures the safety of the
(Fisher et al., 1993; Wacker et al., 1990) or of con- individual and those around him or her should be used
ducting intervention in two types of settings (Horner & in response to challenging behaviors that may occur
Budd, 1985). Others were designed to examine FCT during and after intervention (Durand et al., 1993).
as one component of a multielement treatment pack- However, the results of a few studies suggest that
age (Peck et al., 1996; Sigafoos & Meikle, 1996). Still some individuals may require mild forms of negative
others were less formal case studies (Campbell & consequences, at least initially (e.g., Fisher et al.,
Lutzker, 1993; Gerra et al., 1995). These wide varia- 1993; Wacker et al., 1990).
tions in purpose made it difficult to analyze the instruc-
tional techniques used since the techniques them- Settings and Schedule
selves were often experimental in nature. However, a
Downloaded by [La Trobe University] at 09:19 25 February 2016

general summary will be provided in the sections that Early in the development of FCT as a behavioral
follow. intervention, Horner and Budd (1985) established the
importance of teaching the new communicative
Teaching Strategies behavior in the natural settings in which it will be used.
All of the studies reviewed incorporated this principle
In all cases, systematic teaching strategies such into at least part of the FCT/AAC instruction that was
as prompting and fading were used to teach new provided. In most cases, intervention was conducted
communicative behaviors. Prompts were most often in the individual’s home, classroom, day activity cen-
presented in a “least-to-most” intrusive order, with ter, and so forth. When initial instruction occurred in a
verbal prompts used initially to generate the desired controlled setting such as a hospital clinic, later
behavior, followed by more intrusive prompts as instruction almost always occurred in natural envi-
needed (e.g., gestural, modeling, partial physical, ronments during the generalization and/or follow-up
and/or hand-over-hand prompts; see Snell, 1987). In phases. It is unlikely that generalization will occur
a few cases, an errorless learning or “most-to-least” unless natural setting instruction is included at some
prompt hierarchy was used, starting with full physical point in the training process.
prompts and decreasing the amount of assistance With regard to the schedule of instruction, instruc-
over time as the learner began to produce the desired tion should be implemented throughout the day,
communicative response (e.g., Bird et al., 1989; whenever naturally occurring, relevant communica-
Fisher et al., 1993; Sigafoos & Meikle, 1996). Prompts tion opportunities present themselves (Carr et al.,
were usually faded by gradually decreasing the 1994; Durand, 1990). This “distributed practice”
amount of assistance provided or, in a few cases, by approach has been found to result in the best overall
the use of a time delay procedure (e.g., Bird et al., generalization across settings and people (Snell,
1989; Horner et al., 1990). Time delay involves paus- 1987). On the other hand, distributed practice may
ing for gradually increasing increments of time before result in a slower rate of initial acquisition if naturally
prompting to give the learner opportunities to produce occurring opportunities for instruction occur sporadi-
the behavior without assistance (Snell, 1987). Read- cally, and may be logistically complicated. The alter-
ers are referred to Durand (1990) for a more complete native used in almost all of the research studies to
discussion of the prompt fading steps typically used date was a “massed trial” approach (Snell, 1987). This
in FCT. involves a 10- to 30-minute teaching session during
which the individual is exposed to one or more of the
Consequences situations that were shown during assessment to elicit
the behavior (e.g., difficult tasks or no-attention situ-
As noted previously, the consequence for the ations). Then, prior to the occurrence of any chal-
desired communicative behavior in FCT is always the lenging behavior, the selected instructional tech-
provision of the requested item or activity (i.e., tangi- niques are used to teach the new communicative
ble item, attention, or brief break, assistance, etc.). In behavior, and the desired response is provided. A
addition, the studies reviewed used a variety of con- series of trials can be set up in this manner within a
sequences in response to challenging behaviors that short period of time to provide concentrated instruc-
occurred during instruction. The most common con- tion. Of course, instructional trials will have to be inter-
sequences were extinction (i.e., ignoring the behavior spersed throughout the day, once initial learning has
and continuing with the task at hand) and physical occurred. However, the “massed trial” approach may
222 Mirenda

have the advantages of more rapid acquisition and in Northup et al. [1994] and “Ann” in Steege et al.
fewer logistical demands in the early phases of [1990]), follow-up data were positive in that the new
instruction. FCT/AAC behaviors continued to be used and the fre-
quencies of the challenging behaviors remained low.
OUTCOMES Implicit in these successful follow-up data are positive
generalization data as well, since the follow-up data all
Short-term Impact reflect the participants’ use of FCT/AAC behaviors in
untrained settings. In addition, one participant (“Kim”
As summarized in Table 3, the FCT/AAC interven- from Durand & Kishi [1987]) had acquired a new
tion resulted in an immediate and substantial reduc- FCT/AAC behavior at follow-up, despite the fact that
tion in the frequency of the target behavior for 44 of she was one of the short-term intervention “failures.”
the 52 participants (85%). For four participants, there This occurred because the message assigned to her
was a gradual reduction in challenging behavior and, FCT/AAC behavior was changed over time to make it
for another four participants, the intervention had no more acceptable to staff (see discussion under “Prin-
immediate impact. Two of the latter four participants ciples of Response Mastery”).
were “Lew” and “Kim” from the report by Durand and Overall, the research reviewed suggests that both
Kishi (1987); their stories provide clear examples of generalization and maintenance of new communica-
the negative impact of ineffective implementation by tive behaviors can be enhanced by use of the follow-
Downloaded by [La Trobe University] at 09:19 25 February 2016

staff. The other two “failure” participants were “Bob” ing practices: (a) natural settings for instruction, (b)
and “Abe” in the Fisher et al. (1993) report. These distributed practice trials, and (c) selection of
authors were unable to explain the intervention failure FCT/AAC behaviors that are efficient, acceptable, and
but offered several hypotheses as to why this might recognizable by others. On the other hand, FCT/AAC
have occurred. Despite these exceptions, it is clear interventions are less likely to be successful over time
that FCT/AAC is a promising practice for the reduction in environments that (a) are homogeneous (e.g.,
of severe behavior problems, at least on a short-term classrooms specifically for students with challenging
basis. behaviors), (b) de-emphasize the importance of
choice and control in the lives of the people sup-
Generalization ported, and (c) do not work within a collaborative (i.e.,
team) model of intervention (Carr et al., 1994; Durand,
Once a new FCT/AAC behavior has been taught, it 1990). Intervention in homogeneous settings presents
is important to assess its impact and, if needed, imple- formidable barriers in terms of staff intervention time,
ment additional intervention procedures to ensure that the demands made of staff, and the quality of appro-
the learner can use it across novel (i.e., untrained) priate communication and behavior models available.
settings, people, and situations. Unfortunately, such Lack of an atmosphere in which genuine and regular
generalization measures were evident for fewer than opportunities for control and choice making occur
25% of the participants in the studies reviewed. In a often means that, once FCT/AAC behaviors are
few studies, systematic data were provided with taught, staff are unwilling to respond to them because
regard to generalization across instructors (Bird et al., they are inconvenient or require a “giving up” of staff
1989), communication partners (Hunt, Alwell, Goetz, control. And, without proper supports and teamwork,
& Sailor, 1990), and settings (Horner & Budd, 1985). FCT/AAC interventions will probably fail to generalize
In the remaining studies that explored this issue, across either personnel or environments.
anecdotal information about successful generaliza-
tion was evident (Campbell & Lutzker, 1993; Gerra et FUTURE RESEARCH
al., 1995; Hunt et al., 1988; Steege et al., 1990). The
absence of specific generalization data in most of the This final section will offer suggestions for future
research reviewed probably reflects the fact that the research on FCT/AAC in a variety of areas, based on
purpose of many of these studies was to explore a the research review.
specific aspect of the FCT intervention in a scientifi-
cally rigorous fashion, rather than to provide accounts Subjects and Behaviors
of successful long-term interventions.
The combination of FCT and AAC has been applied
Follow-up and Maintenance exclusively to individuals with developmental disabil-
ities to date. However, it seems logical to assume that
Either anecdotal or empirical follow-up information this strategy might also be useful with other individu-
about the long-term impact of FCT/AAC interventions als who have both limited speech and either chronic
was provided for 24 of the 52 participants (46%) or acute behavior problems. Such individuals might
across 10 of the 21 studies reviewed (48%). Follow-up include adults with degenerative illnesses such as
periods ranged from 1 to 17 months, with an average Alzheimer’s and Huntington’s disease. They might
of 8 months. For all but two of the participants (“Heidi” also include individuals with traumatic brain injury,
Functional Communication Training and AAC 223

stroke, or spinal cord injury during the early stages of perspective? How can training in FCT/AAC be deliv-
recovery. In addition, there is no reason to assume ered most effectively to staff who may have little or no
that the challenging behaviors with which FCT can be background in AAC or behavior analysis? Answers to
used are limited to those in the studies reviewed. In these and other intervention questions are of critical
particular, extension of this work to sensory-motivated importance if FCT/AAC interventions are to gain wide-
behaviors would appear to merit additional attention, spread acceptance and use.
since intervention with the one participant who
demonstrated this type of behavior was successful, at Outcomes
least in the short term.
Clearly, longitudinal studies that document general-
Assessment ization and long-term maintenance would provide
additional information regarding the impact of
Future research in this area should aim to further FCT/AAC. In addition, interventions in which parents,
refine existing assessment procedures toward teachers, and other supporters are trained to provide
increasing efficiency and simplicity in a variety of set- intervention during both the initial and follow-up stages
tings (e.g., rehabilitation facilities, general education are needed in order to refine existing procedures so
classrooms). In addition, there is no doubt that an that they are maximally usable in natural settings.
increasing array of professionals are being asked to Finally, as noted by Meyer and Evans (1993), it is crit-
Downloaded by [La Trobe University] at 09:19 25 February 2016

provide FCT instruction both to individuals who can ical to develop outcome measures that go beyond the
speak and to those who use AAC. Research is traditional “did the behavior decrease after interven-
needed to identify how to best provide instruction tion?” measures. Such measures might include, for
regarding behavioral assessment to speech-language example, those related to (a) positive collateral effects
pathologists, teachers, parents, group home staff, and (e.g., increased peer interactions or access to pre-
others who might be involved in this process (see ferred activities; see Malette et al., 1992), (b) less
Berg and Sasso, 1993). restrictive placements and greater participation in inte-
grated activities (e.g., Campbell & Lutzker, 1993), (c)
Intervention participant quality-of-life measures (e.g., positive affect;
see Durand, 1993), and (d) perceptions of improve-
A number of questions remain in the area of ment by family members, classmates, and so forth.
FCT/AAC instruction. Among them are those related
to response recognizability, which, as noted previ- SUMMARY
ously, is directly related to the issue of symbol and
message iconicity. Research regarding systematic This research review has summarized and analyzed
procedures for selecting FCT/AAC techniques that the extant literature on FCT/AAC interventions across
are maximally transparent is sorely needed, as is a variety of dimensions. It is interesting to note that, at
research investigating the relative impact of teaching the present time, this research is driven by the field of
generic (e.g., “want”) messages versus specific (e.g., applied behavior analysis, as reflected in the fact that
“cookie,” “juice”) messages for FCT. What are the half of the studies reviewed were published in the pre-
cost/benefit considerations of generic versus specific mier journal in that field (Journal of Applied Behavior
messages with regard to rapidity of acquisition, care- Analysis). The remaining studies appeared in education
giver responsiveness, generalization across settings, or disability-specific journals, and only one (Durand,
and long-term maintenance? In addition, future 1993) was published in a communication journal (Aug-
research is needed regarding identification of the opti- mentative and Alternative Communication). This prob-
mal techniques for instruction and for responding to ably reflects both the novelty of FCT relative to other
challenging behavior that occurs during instruction. types of communication interventions and the fact that
Perhaps most important of all, investigations regard- information about the relationship between challenging
ing strategies for changing staff attitudes and practices behavior and communication has only recently been
to promote successful, functional communication in available in written forms that communication special-
school and community settings is of paramount impor- ists are likely to access (e.g., Beukelman & Mirenda,
tance. Work has begun in this area, and readers are 1992; Durand & Berotti, 1991; Reichle & Wacker, 1993;
referred to the discussion by Anderson, Albin, Mesaros, Reichle et al., 1991). Nonetheless, it is clear from this
Dunlap, and Morelli-Robbins (1993) in this regard. In review that FCT/AAC interventions should be among
addition, research is needed to address questions such the first interventions considered for many individuals
as the following: What are the variables that prevent who engage in severe behavior problems. Speech-lan-
some staff from creating more appropriate and mean- guage pathologists, teachers, and other communica-
ingful communication opportunities for the people they tion specialists must familiarize themselves with the
support? What types of interventions are most effective assessment and intervention components of this tech-
in assisting them to share control and approach chal- nique that are most likely to result in positive outcomes.
lenging behavior from a proactive rather than a reactive FCT/AAC interventions represent the actualization of
224 Mirenda

the belief that “communication and behavior are insep- Durand, V. M., & Carr, E. (1987). Social influences of “self-stimu-
arable” and, as such, they deserve the careful attention latory” behavior: Analysis and treatment application. Journal of
Applied Behavior Analysis, 20, 119–132.
of both AAC researchers and practitioners.
Durand, V. M., & Carr, E. (1991). Functional communication training
Address reprint requests to: Pat Mirenda, Faculty to reduce challenging behavior: Maintenance and application in
of Education, University of British Columbia, Vancou- new settings. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 24, 251–264.
ver, British Columbia V6T 1Z4, Canada. Durand, V. M., & Carr, E. (1992). An analysis of maintenance fol-
lowing functional communication training. Journal of Applied
REFERENCES Behavior Analysis, 25, 777–794.
Durand, V. M., & Crimmins, D. (1988). Identifying the variables
Anderson, J., Albin, R., Mesaros, R., Dunlap, G., & Morelli-Rob- maintaining self-injurious behavior. Journal of Autism and
bins, M. (1993). Issues in providing training to achieve compre- Developmental Disorders, 18, 99–117.
hensive behavior support. In J. Reichle & D. Wacker (Eds.), Durand, V. M., & Crimmins, D. (1992). The Motivation Assess-
Communicative alternatives to challenging behavior (pp. ment Scale (MAS) administration guide. Topeka, KS: Monaco &
363–396). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes. Associates.
Berg, W., & Sasso, G. (1993). Transferring implementation of func- Durand, V. M., & Kishi, G. (1987). Reducing severe behavior prob-
tional assessment procedures from the clinic to natural settings. lems among persons with dual sensory impairments: An evalu-
In J. Reichle & D. Wacker (Eds.), Communicative alternatives to ation of a technical assistance model. Journal of the Associa-
challenging behavior (pp. 343–362). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes. tion for Persons with Severe Handicaps, 12, 2–10.
Beukelman, D., & Mirenda, P. (1992). Augmentative and alterna- Fisher, W., Piazza, C., Cataldo, M., Harrell, R., Jefferson, G., &
Downloaded by [La Trobe University] at 09:19 25 February 2016

tive communication: Management of severe communication dis- Conner, R. (1993). Functional communication training with and
orders in children and adults. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes. without extinction and punishment. Journal of Applied Behavior
Bihm, E., Kienlen, T., Ness, M., & Poindexter, A. (1991). Factor Analysis, 26, 23–36.
structure of the Motivation Assessment Scale for persons with Gerra, L., Dorfman, S., Plaue, E., Schlackman, S., & Workman, D.
mental retardation. Psychological Reports, 68, 1235–1238. (1995). Functional communication as a means of decreasing
Bird, F., Dores, P., Moniz, D., & Robinson, J. (1989). Reducing self-injurious behavior: A case study. Journal of Visual Impair-
severe aggressive and self-injurious behaviors with functional ment & Blindness, 89, 343–347.
communication training. American Journal of Mental Retarda- Horner, R., & Budd, C. (1985). Acquisition of manual sign use: Col-
tion, 94, 37–48. lateral reduction of maladaptive behavior and factors limiting
Campbell, R., & Lutzker, J. (1993). Using functional equivalence generalization. Education and Training of the Mentally
training to reduce severe challenging behavior: A case study. Retarded, 20, 39–47.
Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 5, 203–216. Horner, R., & Day, H. M. (1991). The effects of response efficiency
Carr, E. G., & Durand, V. M. (1985). Reducing behavior problems on functionally equivalent competing behaviors. Journal of
through functional communication training. Journal of Applied Applied Behavior Analysis, 24, 719–732.
Behavior Analysis, 18, 111–126. Horner, R., Sprague, J., O’Brien, M., & Heathfield, L. (1990). The
Carr, E., Levin, L., McConnachie, G., Carlson, J., Kemp, D., & role of response efficiency in the reduction of problem behaviors
Smith, C. (1994). Communication-based intervention for prob- through functional equivalence training: A case study. Journal of
lem behavior. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes. the Association for Persons with Severe Handicaps, 15, 91–97.
Cooper, L., & Harding, J. (1993). Extending functional analysis Hunt, P., Alwell, M., & Goetz, L. (1988). Acquisition of conversa-
procedures to outpatient and classroom settings for children tion skills and the reduction of inappropriate social interaction
with mild disabilities. In J. Reichle & D. Wacker (Eds.), Com- behaviors. Journal of the Association for Persons with Severe
municative alternatives to challenging behavior (pp. 41–62). Handicaps, 13, 20–27.
Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes. Hunt, P., Alwell, M., Goetz, L., & Sailor, W. (1990). Generalized
Crawford, J., Brockel, B., Schauss, S., & Miltenberger, R. (1992). A effects of conversation skill training. Journal of the Association
comparison of methods for the functional assessment of stereo- for Persons with Severe Handicaps, 15, 250–260.
typed behavior in persons with mental retardation. Journal of the Iwata, B., Dorsey, M., Slifer, K., Bauman, K., & Richman, G.
Association for Persons with Severe Handicaps, 17, 77–86. (1982). Toward a functional analysis of self-injury. Analysis and
Day, H. M., Horner, R., & O’Neill, R. (1994). Multiple functions of Intervention in Developmental Disabilities, 2, 3–20.
problem behaviors: Assessment and intervention. Journal of Iwata, B., Wong, S., Riordan, M., & Lau, M. (1982). Assessment and
Applied Behavior Analysis, 27, 279–290. training of clinical interviewing skills: Analogue analysis and field
Day, R., Rea, J., Schussler, N., Larsen, S., & Johnson, W. (1988). replication. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 15, 191–204.
A functionally based approach to the treatment of self-injurious Kearney, C. (1994). Interrater reliability of the Motivation Assess-
behavior. Behavior Modification, 12, 565–589. ment Scale: Another, closer look. Journal of the Association for
Donnellan, A. M., Mirenda, P. L., Mesaros, R. A., & Fassbender, Persons with Severe Handicaps, 19, 139–142.
L. L. (1984). Analyzing the communicative functions of aberrant Lalli, J., Browder, D., Mace, C., & Brown, D. (1993). Teacher use
behavior. Journal of the Association for Persons with Severe of descriptive analysis data to implement interventions to
Handicaps, 9, 201–212. decrease students’ problem behaviors. Journal of Applied
Durand, V. M. (1990). Severe behavior problems: A functional Behavior Analysis, 25, 227–238.
communication training approach. New York: Guilford Press. Lalli, J., Casey, S., & Kates, K. (1995). Reducing escape behav-
Durand, V. M. (1993). Functional communication training using ior and increasing task completion with functional communica-
assistive devices: Effects on challenging behavior. Augmenta- tion training, extinction, and response chaining. Journal of
tive and Alternative Communication, 9, 168–176. Applied Behavior Analysis, 28, 261–268.
Durand, V. M., & Berotti, D. (1991). Treating behavior problems Malette, P., Mirenda, P., Kandborg, T., Jones, P., Bunz, T., &
with communication. Asha, 33, 37–39. Rogow, S. (1992). Application of a lifestyle development
Durand, V. M., Berotti, D., & Weiner, J. (1993). Functional com- process for persons with severe intellectual disabilities: A case
munication training: Factors affecting effectiveness, general- study report. Journal of the Association for Persons with Severe
ization, and maintenance. In J. Reichle & D. Wacker (Eds.), Handicaps, 17, 179–191.
Communicative alternatives to challenging behavior (pp. Meyer, L., & Evans, I. (1989). Nonaversive intervention for behav-
317–340). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes. ior problems. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.
Functional Communication Training and AAC 225

Meyer, L., & Evans, I. (1993). Meaningful outcomes in behavioral Reichle, J., York, J., & Sigafoos, J. (1991). Implementing aug-
intervention. In J. Reichle & D. Wacker (Eds.), Communicative mentative and alternative communication: Strategies for learn-
alternatives to challenging behavior (pp. 407–428). Baltimore: ers with severe disabilities. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.
Paul H. Brookes. Sigafoos, J., & Kerr, M. (1994). Interrater reliability of the Motiva-
Mirenda, P. (1995, November). “What’s next?” Schedule and tion Assessment Scale: Failure to replicate with aggressive
choicemaking interventions for students with challenging behav- behavior. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 15, 333–342.
iour. Paper presented at the Annual Conference of The Associ- Sigafoos, J., & Meikle, B. (1996). Functional communication train-
ation for Persons with Severe Handicaps, San Francisco. ing for the treatment of multiply determined challenging behav-
Mirenda, P. (1996, August). Supporting people with challenging ior in two boys with autism. Behavior Modification, 20, 60–84.
behaviour through AAC. Paper presented at the 7th Biennial Singh, N., & Donatelli, L. S. (1993). Factor structure of the Moti-
Conference of the International Society for Augmentative and vation Assessment Scale . Journal of Intellectual Disability
Alternative Communication, Vancouver, BC. Research, 37, 65–74.
Northup, J., Wacker, D., Berg, W., Kelly, L., Sasso, G., & DeRaad, Snell, M. (1987). Systematic instruction of persons with severe
A. (1994). The treatment of severe behavior problems in school handicaps. Columbus, OH: Merrill.
settings using a technical assistance model. Journal of Applied Steege, M., Wacker, D., Cigrand, K., Berg, W., Novak, C.,
Behavior Analysis, 27, 33–48. Reimers, T., Sasso, G., & DeRaad, A. (1990). Use of negative
Northup, J., Wacker, D., Sasso, G., Steege, M., Cigrand, K., Cook, reinforcement in the treatment of self-injurious behavior. Jour-
J., & DeRaad, A. (1991). A brief functional analysis of aggres- nal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 23, 459–468.
sive and alternative behavior in an outclinic setting. Journal of Touchette, P., MacDonald, R., & Langer, S. (1985). A scatter plot
Applied Behavior Analysis, 24, 509–522. for identifying stimulus control of problem behavior. Journal of
Downloaded by [La Trobe University] at 09:19 25 February 2016

O’Neill, R., Horner, R., Albin, R., Storey, K., & Sprague, J. (1990). Applied Behavior Analysis, 18, 343–351.
Functional analysis of problem behavior: A practical assess- Wacker, D., Steege, M., Northup, J., Sasso, G., Berg, W., Reimers,
ment guide. Sycamore, IL: Sycamore Publishing Co. T., Cooper, L., Cigrand, K., & Donn, L. (1990). A component
O’Neill, R., & Reichle, J. (1993). Addressing socially motivated chal- analysis of functional communication training across three
lenging behaviors by establishing communicative alternatives. In topographies of severe behavior problems. Journal of Applied
J. Reichle & D. Wacker (Eds.), Communicative alternatives to Behavior Analysis, 23, 417–429.
challenging behavior (pp. 205–235). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes. Willis, T., LaVigna, G., & Donnellan, A. (1989). Behavior assessment
Peck, S., Wacker, D., Berg, W., Cooper, L., Brown, K., Richman, guide. Los Angeles: Institute for Applied Behavior Analysis.
D., McComas, J., Frischmeyer, P., & Millard, T. (1996). Choice- Zarcone, J., Rodgers, T., Iwata, B., Rourke, D., & Dorsey, M. (1991).
making treatment of young children’s severe behavior prob- Reliability analysis of the Motivation Assessment Scale: A failure
lems. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 29, 263–290. to replicate. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 12, 349–360.
Reichle, J., & Wacker, D. (1993). Communicative alternatives to
challenging behavior. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.

OUTSTANDING AAC ARTICLES FOR 1996


The outstanding articles for 1996 have been selected by a vote of the associate editors. The student research award was
won by Kate Franklin, Pat Mirenda, and Gail Phillips for their article “Comparisons of Five Symbol Assessment Protocols
with Nondisabled Preschoolers with Severe Intellectual Disabilities.” This research was completed when Kate Franklin and
Gail Phillips were students of Dr. Mirenda. The recipients of the professional research award were Heidi Horstmann Koester
and Simon P. Levine for their article “Effect of a Word Prediction Feature on User Performance.” On behalf of ISAAC, I
wish to thank and congratulate the recipients. They will be honored at the ISAAC conference in Dublin, Ireland; however,
their awards have already been sent to them.

WORDS+/ISAAC OUTSTANDING CONSUMER LECTURE AWARD


The Words+/ISAAC Outstanding Consumer Lecture Award is presented at each International Conference. The award is
$2,000 (US), which is intended to defray expenses for the selected consumer to attend the conference and deliver the lec-
ture using their AAC device at an unopposed, feature presentation. ISAAC membership is required. The lecture can be about
any subject that the applicant would like to present to an international audience of consumers, clinicians, educators, and
researchers in AAC (but preferably not “How my life is better because of AAC”). Applications must include (1) a 500- to 1000-
word detailed description of the proposed lecture, (2) pertinent background information, (3) no more than two letters of rec-
ommendation, and (4) an agreement to attend the conference and present the lecture in person using the applicant’s AAC
device. Send applications to Words+/ISAAC Outstanding Consumer Lecture Award c/o ISAAC Secretariat. Applications must
be postmarked no later than January 31, 1998.

WORDS+/ISAAC CONSUMER SCHOLARSHIP AWARD


The Words+/ISAAC Consumer Scholarship Award is presented at each International Conference. The award is $2,000
(US) per year for 2 years ($4,000 total), which must be used for postsecondary education or training. Applicants must be
AAC device users who are ISAAC members. Applications must include (1) a 300- to 500-word essay focusing on the appli-
cant’s educational plans and a two-year time line, (2) the name of the institution that the applicant will attend, (3) the cost
of attending the institution, and (4) no more than two letters of recommendation. Send applications to Words+/ISAAC Con-
sumer Scholarship Award c/o ISAAC Secretariat. Applications must be postmarked no later than January 31, 1998.

You might also like