Download as pptx, pdf, or txt
Download as pptx, pdf, or txt
You are on page 1of 47

Because learning changes everything.

Chapter 6

Psychosocial Development during the First Three Years

Experience Human Development


FOURTEENTH EDITION
Diane E. PAPALIA
Gabriela MARTORELL

© 2021 McGraw Hill. All rights reserved. Authorized only for instructor use in the classroom.
No reproduction or further distribution permitted without the prior written consent of McGraw Hill.
Learning Objectives

Discuss the development of emotions and personality in


infancy.

Describe infants’ social relationships with caregivers,


including attachment.

Discuss the emerging sense of self, autonomy, and moral


development in toddlerhood.

Explain how social contexts influence early development.

Explain child maltreatment and its effects.

© McGraw Hill 2
Foundations of Personality Development

Personality: the relatively consistent blend of emotions,


temperament, thought, and behavior that makes a person
unique.

From infancy on, personality development is intertwined with


social relationships—a combination called psychosocial
development.

© McGraw Hill 3
Emotions

Emotions: subjective responses to experience that are associated with


physiological and behavioral changes.
• Sadness, joy, fear.

People differ in:


• How often and how strongly they feel a particular emotion;
• The kinds of events that produce it;
• The physical manifestations they show; and
• How they act as a result.

Culture also influences the display of emotion.


• Some cultures that stress social harmony discourage open expressions
of anger.
© McGraw Hill 4
First Signs of Emotion: Crying

Newborns plainly show when they are unhappy; and human


brains are wired to respond.
Crying is the earliest and most powerful way infants can
communicate their needs.
• Basic hunger cry.
• Angry cry.
• Pain cry.
• Frustration cry.
By 5 months, babies have learned to monitor their caregivers’
expressions and if ignored, to cry harder.

© McGraw Hill 5
First Signs of Emotion: Smiling and Laughing

Involuntary smiles occur spontaneously soon after birth.


Social smiling: beginning in the 2nd month, newborn infants
gaze at their parents and smile at them, signaling positive
participation in the relationship.
• Laughter is a smile-linked vocalization.
By 12 to 15 months, infants intentionally communicate to
others about objects by smiling.
• Anticipatory smiling: infant smiles at an object and then
gazes at an adult while still smiling.
Positive affective processes are reciprocal.

© McGraw Hill 6
First Signs of Emotion: When Do Emotions Appear?

Complex emotions unfold from simpler ones.


Self-conscious emotions arise only after children have
developed self-awareness.
• Self-conscious emotions: emotions, such as
embarrassment, empathy, and envy, that depend on self-
awareness.
• Self-awareness: realization that one’s existence and
functioning is separate from those of other people and
things.
By about age 3, children are better able to evaluate
themselves against what is socially appropriate and can
demonstrate the self-evaluative emotions of pride, guilt, and
shame.
© McGraw Hill 7
First Signs of Emotions: Altruistic Helping and Empathy

Altruistic behavior: activity intended to help another person


with no expectation of reward.
Altruistic behavior comes naturally to infants.
• The tendency to share, to help, and to comfort seem to be
unrelated to each other; presumably they have different
developmental trajectories.
• Such behavior may collectively reflect empathy: the ability
to put oneself in another person’s place and feel what the
other person feels.
• Mirror neurons: neurons that fire when a person does
something or observes someone else doing the same
thing.
© McGraw Hill 8
Collaborative Activities and Cultural Transmission

The motivation to help and share plus the ability to understand


others’ intentions together contribute to collaboration with
caregivers in joint activities.
• This develops between 9 and 12 months of age and
increases during the 2nd year of life with improved
communication.
Collaborative activities are quintessentially human.
• For example, young children engage in over-imitation,
closely copying all actions they see an adult doing even if
some are clearly irrelevant or impractical.

© McGraw Hill 9
Temperament

Temperament: characteristic disposition or style of


approaching and reacting to situations.
• It is early-appearing and biologically based and results in
predictable ways of responding.
Temperament is closely linked to emotional responses to the
environment, and many responses are emotional in nature.
• Temperament is relatively consistent and enduring,
however.
Individual differences in temperament form the core of the
developing personality.

© McGraw Hill 10
Studying Temperamental Patterns: The New York
Longitudinal Study

Researchers followed 133 infants into adulthood and placed


most into one of three categories:
• “Easy” children: children (about 40% in the study) with a
generally happy temperament, regular biological rhythms,
and a readiness to accept new experiences.
• “Difficult” children: children (about 10%) with irritable
temperament, irregular biological rhythms, and intense
emotional responses.
• “Slow-to-warm-up” children: children (about 15%)
whose temperament is generally mild but who are hesitant
about accepting new experiences.
• Some children do not fit neatly into any one category.
© McGraw Hill 11
Stability of Temperament

Stability of temperament from 2 to 13 months of age is quite


high.
A variety of studies have found links between infant
temperament and later personality and stability in certain
traits, from toddlerhood to early childhood and later.
Culturally influenced practices may influence temperament.
• Proposed influences include the cultural dimensions of
collectivism and individualism, as well as those of power
distance, long-term orientation, masculinity, and
uncertainty avoidance.

© McGraw Hill 12
Temperament and Goodness of Fit

The key to healthy adjustment may be goodness of fit—


the match between a child’s temperament and the
environmental demands and constraints the child must deal
with.

Children also differ in their susceptibility to environmental


influences.

© McGraw Hill 13
Temperament: Behavioral Inhibition

In longitudinal research, Kagan and colleagues studied


behavioral inhibition—how boldly or cautiously a child
approaches unfamiliar objects and situation.
• An underlying difference in physiology—in the excitability
of the amygdala—is theorized to be involved.

Experience can moderate or accentuate early tendencies;


and children may themselves develop buffering strategies.

© McGraw Hill 14
Early Social Experiences: The Family 1

The mother’s role:


• In a series of pioneering experiments, Harlow and
colleagues studied rhesus monkeys separated from their
mothers shortly after birth.
• Two kinds of surrogate “mothers” were presented—one
that was warm and cuddly and one that was wire mesh.
• The experiments showed that mothering includes the
comfort of close bodily contact.
• Human infants need a mother who responds warmly and
promptly.

© McGraw Hill 15
Early Social Experiences: The Family 2

The father’s role:


• The fathering role is in many ways a social construction,
with different meanings in different cultures.
• In the United States, fathers’ involvement in caregiving has
greatly increased.
• A father’s frequent and positive involvement with his child,
from infancy on, is directly related to the child’s well-being
and physical, cognitive, and social development.

© McGraw Hill 16
Gender

Gender: significance of being male or female.

Measurable differences between baby girls and baby boys


are few, at least in U.S. samples.

There is some evidence for differences in social behavior, but


the differences are relatively small.

Behavioral differences are more robust and consistently


identified, including in preferences for toys, play activities,
and the sex of playmates.
• Some of these appear to be innate, while others result
from gender socialization processes.

© McGraw Hill 17
Parental Influences on Gender Differences

Generally, parents use broadly similar parenting styles with


their boys and girls.
However, parents in the United States do tend to stereotype
baby boys and girls.
Parents often unconsciously impart their knowledge of
gendered behaviors through parenting practices.
• Children also watch their parents’ behaviors carefully.

Fathers, especially, promote gender-typing: the


socialization process by which children, at an early age, learn
appropriate gender roles.

© McGraw Hill 18
Developmental Issues: Developing Trust

In Erikson’s stage-based theory of psychosocial


development, babies are faced with the challenge of forming
a basic sense of trust versus mistrust.
• If successful, they develop a sense of the reliability of
people and objects.
• The stage continues until about 18 months.

• Ideally babies develop a balance between trust and


mistrust.

© McGraw Hill 19
Developmental Issues: Developing Attachments 1

Attachment: a reciprocal and enduring tie between two


people—especially between infant and caregiver—each of
whom contributes to the quality of the relationship.

Strange Situation: a classic, laboratory-based technique


used to study infant attachment.
• Episodes are designed to trigger the emergence of
attachment-related behaviors.

© McGraw Hill 20
Developmental Issues: Developing Attachments 2

Ainsworth and colleagues observed 1-year-olds in the


Strange Situation and at home and found three main patterns
of attachment:
• Secure attachment: infant is quickly and effectively able to
obtain comfort from an attachment figure in the face of
distress.
• Avoidant attachment: infant rarely cries when separated
from the primary caregiver and avoids contact on his or her
return.
• Ambivalent (resistant) attachment: infant becomes
anxious before the caregiver leaves, upset during his or her
absence, and both seeks and resists contact on his or her
return.
© McGraw Hill 21
Developmental Issues: Developing Attachments 3

Other research identified a fourth pattern:


• Disorganized-disoriented attachment: after separation
from the primary caregiver, the infant shows contradictory,
repetitious, or misdirected behaviors on the caregiver’s
return.
• The likelihood of disorganized attachment increases in the
presence of multiple risk factors.

© McGraw Hill 22
Developmental Issues: Developing Attachments 4

By the time babies are 1 year old, they have established a


characteristic style of attachment.
• Bowlby: attachment styles are the result of repeated
interactions with a caregiver.
• Secure attachment reflects Erikson’s concept of basic
trust; insecure attachment, mistrust.
Studies of attachment with methods other than the Strange
Situation suggest that:
• The tendency to use the mother as a secure base for
exploring the environment is universal; and
• Attachment may have a neurological basis.

© McGraw Hill 23
Developmental Issues: Developing Attachments 5

Attachment is a relational process.


• Child temperament and parenting interact.
Substitute caregivers can prompt anxiety in children.
• Separation anxiety: distress shown by someone, typically
an infant, when a familiar caregiver leaves.
• Stranger anxiety: wariness of strange people and places,
shown by some infants during the second half of the 1st
year.
• The quality and stability of substitute care is important.

© McGraw Hill 24
Developmental Issues: Developing Attachments 6

Attachment quality has long-term implications.


• Securely attached children have larger vocabularies, show
less stress in adapting to child care, and have more
positive interactions with peers.
• They also have higher levels of curiosity and self-
confidence.
• Secure attachment in infancy influences the quality of
attachment to a romantic partner in young adulthood.
The way adults recall early experience may influence the way
they respond to their own children.
• However, a cycle of insecure attachment can be broken
with intervention.
© McGraw Hill 25
Emotional Communication with Caregivers: Mutual
Regulation

Mutual regulation: process by which infant and caregiver


communicate emotional states to each other and respond
appropriately.
• Infants take an active part by sending behavioral signals.
• Some interactional synchrony may be expressed at a
biological level, as evidenced by heart rates.
Not surprisingly, there are links to later social behaviors.
• Children whose mothers were high in interactional
synchrony when young are more likely to later be better at
regulating their behavior.

© McGraw Hill 26
Social Referencing

Social referencing: understanding an ambiguous situation


by seeking another person’s perception of it.
• One person forms an understanding of how to act in an
unfamiliar situation by interpreting another person’s
perception of it.

Research has provided experimental evidence of social


referencing at 12 months.

As children age, they become less dependent on facial


expression and more dependent on language; and they
become pickier about whom to seek information from.

© McGraw Hill 27
Developmental Issues: The Emerging Sense of Self

Self-concept: sense of self; descriptive and evaluative


mental picture of one’s abilities and traits.

Between 4 and 10 months, infants develop a sense of


personal agency and self-coherence.

Their early perceptual discrimination—as when they pay


attention to their mirror image—may be the foundation of
conceptual self-awareness between 15 and 18 months.

© McGraw Hill 28
Developmental Issues: Development of Autonomy

Autonomy versus shame and doubt: Erikson’s second stage


in psychosocial development, in which children achieve a
balance between self-determination and control by others.
• In Erikson’s theory, occurs from about 18 months to 3 years.
• The virtue that emerges is will.
In the United States, toddlers often enjoy testing the notions
that they are individuals, have some control over their world,
and have new, exciting powers.
• This often shows itself as negativism—“No!”
• The terrible twos are not universal.

© McGraw Hill 29
Moral Development: Socialization and Internalization 1

Socialization: development of habits, skills, values, and


motives shared by responsible, productive members of a
society.

Internalization: during socialization, the process by which


children accept societal standards of conduct as their own.

Self-regulation: a child’s independent control of behavior to


conform to understood social expectations.

Conscience: internal standards of behavior, which produce


emotional discomfort when violated.

© McGraw Hill 30
Moral Development: Socialization and Internalization 2

Kochanska and colleagues:


• Some children show situational compliance, obedience in
response to a parent’s orders only in the presence of signs
of ongoing parental control.
• Other children seem to internalize their parents’ requests
more fully—they show committed compliance, a
wholehearted obedience without reminders or lapses.
• Receptive cooperation: Kochanska’s term for eager
willingness to cooperate harmoniously with a parent in daily
interactions, including routines, chores, hygiene, and play.

© McGraw Hill 31
Moral Development: Socialization and Internalization 3

Factors in the success of socialization:


• How parents go about the job of socializing a child and the
quality of the parent–child relationship;
• The child’s temperament; and

• Security of attachment.

Constructive conflict over misbehavior—with negotiation,


reasoning, and resolution—can help children develop moral
understanding.

© McGraw Hill 32
Relationships with Other Children: Siblings

Sibling relationships begin with the birth of a new baby and


continue to develop positively and negatively.

Although conflict is frequent, sibling rivalry is not the main


pattern of behavior.
• Affection, interest, companionship, and influence are also
prevalent.

Lessons and skills learned from interactions with siblings


carry over to relationships outside the home.
• Likewise, friendships can influence sibling relationships.

© McGraw Hill 33
Relationships with Other Children: Peers

Infants and toddlers show interest in people outside the


home, particularly people their own size.

Toddlers learn by imitating one another.

Preschoolers usually like to play with children of the same


age, sex, and gender.
• They also prefer prosocial playmates who are advanced in
theory of mind.

Some children are more sociable than others.


• Sociability is influenced by both temperament and
experience.

© McGraw Hill 34
Children of Working Parents: Effects of Maternal
Employment

Longitudinal data on 900 European American children showed


negative effects on cognitive development at 15 months to 3
years when mothers worked 30 or more hours a week by the
child’s 9th month.
• Maternal sensitivity, a high-quality home environment, and
high-quality child care lessened these negative effects.

Children in disadvantaged families showed fewer negative


cognitive effects than children in more advanced families.

One factor is the type of substitute care a child receives.

© McGraw Hill 35
Children of Working Parents: Early Child Care 1

About 61% of children under age 5 have some sort of regular


child care arrangement.
• About one-quarter are cared for in some form of organized
care facility.

Temperament and gender make a difference.


• Shy children and insecurely attached children experience
greater stress.
• Boys are more vulnerable to stress.

© McGraw Hill 36
Children of Working Parents: Early Child Care 2

Quality of care can be measured by structural characteristics


and process characteristics.
• Staff training.
• Ratio of children to caregivers.
• Warmth, sensitivity, and responsiveness of caregivers.
• Developmental appropriateness of activities.
The most important element is the caregiver.
• Consistency—low staff turnover—is also important.

© McGraw Hill 37
Maltreatment: Abuse and Neglect

Physical abuse: action taken deliberately to endanger


another person, involving potential bodily injury.

Neglect: failure to meet a dependent’s basic needs.

Sexual abuse: physically or psychologically harmful sexual


activity or any sexual activity involving a child and an older
person.

Emotional maltreatment: rejection, terrorization, isolation,


exploitation, degradation, ridicule, or failure to provide
emotional support, love, and affection.

© McGraw Hill 38
Maltreatment in Infancy and Toddlerhood

The highest rates of victimization and of death from


maltreatment are seen in children aged 3 and younger.

Babies who do not receive nurturance and affection or who


are neglected sometimes suffer from nonorganic failure to
thrive—a slowed or arrested physical growth, accompanied
by poor developmental and emotional functioning.
• Poverty is the single greatest risk factor.

Shaken baby syndrome: form of maltreatment in which


shaking an infant or toddler can cause brain damage,
paralysis, or death.

© McGraw Hill 39
Contributing Factors: An Ecological View 1

Abusive and neglectful parents and families:


• In 2017, about 78% of perpetrators were parents; slightly
over half were women.
• Maltreatment by parents is a symptom of extreme
disturbance in child rearing.
• Abuse may begin when a parent who is already in distress
tries to control a child but loses self-control.
• Abuse and neglect sometimes occur in the same families;
and substance abuse is a factor in approximately a quarter
of such cases.
• Sexual abuse often occurs along with other family
disturbances.
© McGraw Hill 40
Contributing Factors: An Ecological View 2

Cultural influences:
• Norms regarding child care impact the definition of neglect.
• Cultural values associated with higher rates of child
maltreatment include:
• Glorification of violence;
• Rigid gender roles; and
• Beliefs that diminish the status of the child within the parenting
relationship.

• Attitudes regarding corporal punishment are particularly


important.

© McGraw Hill 41
Helping Families in Trouble

State and local child protective services investigate reports of


maltreatment and determine what steps, if any, to take.

Availability of services is often limited.

Foster care removes a child from immediate danger, but the


situation is often unstable, it further alienates the child, the
care may be inadequate, and it may wind up being another
abusive situation.

Individuals who experienced foster care are more likely to


become homeless, to commit crimes, to become teenage
mothers, and to suffer mental or physical health problems.

© McGraw Hill 42
Long-Term Effects of Maltreatment

Consequences may be physical, emotional, cognitive, social,


or a combination.

Some abused children show remarkable resilience, however.


• Optimism, self-esteem, intelligence, creativity, humor, and
independence are protective factors.
• Another key factor may be the social support of a caring
adult.

© McGraw Hill 43
Research in Action: Postpartum Depression and Early
Development

Postpartum depression (PPD) includes symptoms of major


depressive disorder, experienced within 4 weeks of giving
birth, that interfere with maternal functioning.
• First time mothers may be especially at risk.

PPD has profoundly negative effects on mother-infant


interactions and is linked to long-term disruption in cognitive
and emotional outcomes.

Early intervention is essential.

© McGraw Hill 44
Window on the World: Are Struggles with Toddlers
Necessary?

The terrible twos do not appear to be universal.


In a cross-cultural study comparing Mayan families to
American families, researchers found striking differences in
the way siblings interacted.
Two factors appear to explain the differences:
• The age parents felt children can be held responsible; and
• The amount of direct parental supervision children
received.
The researchers suggest the terrible twos may be a phase
specific to societies that place individual freedom above the
needs of the group.
© McGraw Hill 45
Questions?

© McGraw Hill 46
End of Main Content

Because learning changes everything. ®

www.mheducation.com

© 2021 McGraw Hill. All rights reserved. Authorized only for instructor use in the classroom.
No reproduction or further distribution permitted without the prior written consent of McGraw Hill.

You might also like